Abstract
Impulsivity is strongly associated with substance use disorders (SUDs). Our review discusses impulsivity as an underlying vulnerability marker for SUDs, and treatment of co-occurring impulsivity in SUDs. Three factors should be considered for the complex relationship between impulsivity and a substance use disorder (SUD): (1) the trait effect of impulsivity, centering on decreased cognitive and response inhibition; (2) the state effect resulting from either acute or chronic substance use on brain structure and function; and (3) the genetic and environmental factors (e.g., age and sex) may influence impulsive behavior associated with SUDs. Both subjective and objective measures are used to assess impulsivity. Together, treatment developments (pharmacological, behavioral, and neurophysiological) should consider these clinically relevant dimensions assessed by a variety of measures, which have implications for treatment matching in individuals with SUD. Despite its heterogeneity, impulsivity is a marker associated with SUDs and may be understood as an imbalance of bottom-up and top-down neural systems. Further investigation of these relationships may lead to more effective SUD treatments.
Keywords: impulsivity, substance use disorders, treatment, tobacco, cannabis, alcohol
Graphical abstract:
Despite its heterogeneity, impulsivity is a marker associated with substance used disorders (SUDs) and may be understood as an imbalance of bottom-up and top-down neural systems. Our review discusses the multifaceted construct of impulsivity as an underlying vulnerability marker within the various stages of SUDs. We emphasize a transdiagnostic model for understanding impulsivity and addiction risk.
Introduction
Substance misuse occurs in over a quarter of a billion people worldwide, with 0.6% suffering from substance use disorders (SUDs)1. A significant challenge to understanding the etiology of this disorder relates to the complex interplay between environmental and genetic risk factors2. The behavioral and neurobiological relationships between impulsivity and addictive behaviors have been well established 3. Impulsivity is often equated as bottom-up control mechanisms being suppressed by automatic or reward-driven responses with diminished cognitive control to demands that may not be appropriate (disinhibition) 4,5. Moreover, impulsivity may be divided into four constructs: lack of meditation, lack of perseverance, sensation seeking, and urgency 6,7. Notably, sensation seeking has recently been recognized as a separate construct of impulsivity 8. Moreover, urgency has been recognized as having two traits, positive and negative 9. Positive urgency refers to the disposition to act rashly to extreme positive effect. Negative urgency refers to the disposition to act rashly to extreme negative effect 10. However, this review will use the Brewer and Potenza framework, thereby using the four constructs aforementioned and referring to urgency in general rather than specifically to positive or negative urgency.
Furthermore, brain injury and several mental illnesses may predispose individuals to disruptions in these inhibitory control (IC) mechanisms, resulting in impulsive behaviors 11,12. For instance, early stages of recreational drug use may be mediated by impulsive behavior 11. Regardless of an individuals’ awareness of the harms directly related to their drug habit (e.g., effects on health, finances, and interpersonal relationships), continued drug use and repeated failure of reduced intake or quit attempts, may also be mediated by deficient IC over the immediate reinforcing effects from drug use 11.
Many models have been used to link impulsive behavior to the prevalence of SUD, encompassing neurobiological mechanisms of causation versus risk. One explanation posits drug administration resulting in neurobiological and structural changes affecting behavioral self-control 13. The alternative explanation suggests deficits in impulsive control may have already been present prior to drug initiation, representing a vulnerability marker for SUDs 14. Neurobiological (e.g., dopaminergic (DAergic)) 15 and glutamatergic 16 neurocircuitry involved in reward-related learning), genetic, preclinical, and clinical studies have all extensively been conducted to try to disentangle the complicated relationship of initiation, continuation, addiction, and relapse to substances in relation to impulsive behavior 14. Ultimately, these different addiction phases have many clinical implications for treatment approaches (e.g., pharmacological, neurophysiological, and behavioral) 17,18.
Our review discusses the multifaceted construct of impulsivity as an underlying vulnerability marker within the various stages of SUD. We emphasize a transdiagnostic model for understanding impulsivity and addiction risk. This review is novel compared with previous reviews 11,19,20 since: (1) we review the literature on the development and efficacy of treatment options for the co-occurrence of impulsivity in SUD including pharmacological, neurophysiological, and behavioral approaches, and (2) we provide a theoretical framework of what dimension of impulsivity was measured in the various treatment trials for the co-occurrence of impulsivity and SUDs, thereby providing novel insights into which treatment options are most promising to pursue, while considering the interrelationship of trait, state and other effects, such as environmental and genetic factors (e.g., a transdiagnostic model).
Definitions of impulsivity and outcome measures
Impulsivity is a multidimensional construct, incorporating state and trait classifications, and a variety of associated behaviors 21–24. Despite agreement regarding the multifaceted perception of impulsivity, there is no consensus on classification 22,25,26. Further complicating its classification is the predisposition of impulsivity towards maladaptive, risky behavior relative to normal behavioral responses 21,22,26. Accordingly, measurements of impulsivity reflect the variability in definitions of the term and comprise various forms that range from self-report assessments to behavioral measures and electrophysiological analyses (Table 1) 21,25. Self-report relies upon an individual’s accurate recall of one’s own behavior, while behavioral measures are more objective 27. There is often little overlap between self-report and behavioral measures of impulsivity 28,29.
Table 1.
Summary of various subjective and objective measures of impulsivity.
Tasks | Purpose | Impulsive dimension/component |
---|---|---|
Subjective: self-report tasks | ||
The Barratt impulsiveness scale (BIS-11)38 | A 30-item questionnaire assessing three separable dispositions: attentional, motor, and non-planning impulsiveness | Impulsive trait: inhibitory control; accounts for lack of premeditation and perseverance impulsivity dimensions55 |
The Eysenck impulsiveness questionnaire (I7)215 | To assess personality traits of impulsivity, venturesomeness, and empathy | Impulsive trait |
The temperament and character inventory (TCI)216 | An inventory of personality traits based on four temperaments: novelty seeking, harm avoidance, reward dependence, and persistence | Impulsive trait |
The multidimensional personality questionnaire (MPQ)217 | A comprehensive assessment of personality traits (control versus impulsivity) encompassed of mostly 276 true-false items | Impulsive trait |
The UPPS impulsive behavior scale (IBS)41 | 59-item scale measuring different aspects of impulsive personality encompassing four traits: negative urgency, premeditation, lack of perseverance, and sensation seeking | Impulsive trait |
Objective: behavioral tasks | ||
The Kirby delay-discounting task (KDDT)134 | A 27-item questionnaire measuring temporal discounting, using small immediate monetary rewards versus larger delayed rewards | Impulsive state/choice: delay discounting |
The Iowa gambling task (IGT)218 | Task assessing decision making under risk and uncertainty using four virtual decks of cards on a computer screen, with contingencies are discovered by trial and error | Impulsive state/choice: impulsive decision making |
The Cambridge gambling task (CGT)219 | Rodent version of IGT, signaling the odds of winning | Impulsive state/choice: impulsive decision making |
The experiential discounting task (EDT)220 | Computerized task requiring participants experience choice consequences during a measurement period assessing motivation to earn or prevent loss of monetary reward | Impulsive state/choice: delay discounting |
The go/no-go task37 | Test of response inhibition with participants trained over multiple trials to make a particular response to a “go” signal, with some trials of “stop” signal presented prior to or simultaneously | Impulsive state/action: motor disinhibition |
The stop-signal reaction-time (SSRT) task36 | Test of behavioral inhibition/discrimination with a “stop” signal presented after the “go” signal | Impulsive state/action: motor disinhibition |
The balloon analogue risk task (BART)221 | Task designed to assess the risk propensity with balloon presentations on a computer screen that can be incrementally inflated while accumulating reward, with a constant probability of popping (loss of reward) | Impulsive state: impulsive choice: impulsive-decision making |
The oddball task87 | Analyzing a positive event-related potential (ERP), P300 mitigated through the presentations of sequences of repetitive stimuli infrequently disrupted by deviant stimulus | Impulsive state: inhibitory control |
The Stroop color and word test (SCWT)222 | Information processing approach to assess emotions examining response time of naming colors of negative emotional words | Impulsive state/action: cognitive disinhibition |
One common definition of impulsivity is the lack of behavioral inhibition leading to the tendency to act on impulse 21,25,26,30–32. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth edition (DSM-5) describes impulsivity as a lack of behavioral inhibition 33. Additionally, impulsivity has been considered a “failure of the inhibitory process” 32, as it involves implications in the frontostriatal circuitry leading to the dysfunction of top-down cognitive control 31. Motor (behavioral) impulsivity relates to the failure of motor inhibition (impulsive action) associated with dorsolateral prefrontal lobe activity 34 that is equivalent to response inhibition and often studied in preclinical models 35. Behavioral tasks involve terminating prepotent motor responses, using measures such as the stop signal reaction time (SST) task 36, the go/no-go task 37 as well as the continuous performance task (CPT).
Another common theme relates to decision-making lacking sensitivity to negative consequences and processing of long-term outcomes 21,25,26,30–32. The DSM-5 also describes impulsivity as dysfunctional decision making while incorporating a dimension of urgency and harmful behavior during emotionally charged situations 33. Some common self-report assessments include the Barratt impulsiveness scale (BIS-11) 23,38, Eysenck impulsiveness questionnaire (I7) 39, multidimensional personality questionnaire (MPQ) 40, and the UPPS impulsive behavior scale of impulsivity (IBS) 41. In terms of behavioral tasks, impulsive decision making is commonly measured by the delayed discounting of reward tasks, which comprises the favoring of smaller rewards in the near future compared with larger rewards in the extended future 21,42,43. Forms of this task involve the full permuted decision-making tasks 44,45 and the shorter Kirby monetary choice questionnaire 46. Impulsivity is associated with attentional dysfunction 21,25,26,30–32, and the inability to follow instructions 33. Recent findings suggest that these three domains—personality traits, discounting preferences, and response inhibition tasks—represent three conceptually related, but quantitatively distinct domains of impulsivity 47.
Neurobiology
The overlap between brain circuits and neurotransmitter systems involved in impulsivity and addiction risk 48 have provided a targeted engagement approach through which addiction risk may be remediated 49. This includes three neurobiological systems: (1) the regulatory system mediated by the medial and ventral prefrontal cortices; (2) the reward system via ventral striatum and midbrain DAergic system; and (3) the threat system via the amygdala 50 (Fig. 1). Regarding the four constructs of impulsivity, urgency (e.g., relating to the tendency of responding to negative emotions irrationally resulting in problematic outcomes7) has been associated with excessive recruitment of lateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) activity resulting in self-regulatory failure (e.g., substance misuse) 51. Lack of premeditation has been associated with decreased gray-matter volumes in the insula and putamen and postulated to relate to the efficacy of decision-making processes 52–54. Lack of perseverance or lack of conscientiousness is linked to impaired anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) function, and the left ventrolateral and left anterior prefrontal cortices, relating to risky behaviors 55. Finally, sensation-seeking has been associated with activation of regions related to motivation, arousal, and reinforcement such as the posterior medial orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and insula 56. Ultimately, inefficient control, strong reward and weak harm-avoidance signals have been proposed to contribute to substance use. This leads to an imbalance between the PFC top-down cognitive control systems and subcortical bottom-up incentive–reward system leading to risky behaviors such as drug experimentation 57,58.
Figure 1.
Brain diagram to illustrate pathways leading to impulsive behaviors (adapted from Ref. 57). Three neurobiological systems including the control/regulatory, reward, and threat systems, mediated by the medial and ventral prefrontal cortices, the ventral striatum and midbrain dopaminergic system, and the amygdala, respectively, provide an overlapping pathway linking brain circuitries and neurotransmitter systems associated with addiction risk and impulsivity.
Recent models of addiction and impulsivity have focused on glutamatergic and gamma-aminobutyric acid-ergic (GABAergic) mechanisms in key structures (ACC), given their role in impulsivity, craving and drug seeking. Human studies have shown that elevated glutamate levels relating to an imbalance between synaptic and nonsynaptic levels are associated with dysregulation between the PFC and nucleus accumbens (NAcc) found in substance dependence 59,60. Glutamate levels in the dorsal ACC have also been associated with delay discounting (DD) in SUDs 61. Such findings support the potential of antiglutamatergic agents for the treatment of SUDs. In addition, reduced levels of inhibitory neurotransmitter GABA in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) 62 are associated with impulsivity 63. Increasing evidence supports the modulation of GABAergic systems for the treatment of SUDs and impulsive behaviors. For instance, GABA reuptake inhibitor tiagabine has shown to reduce cocaine use and control impulsive aggression 64. As such, these findings suggest possible functions of glutamatergic and GABAergic systems underlying comorbid impulsivity and addictive behaviors.
Another relevant neurotransmitter is dopamine (DA). The D2-like (D2) DA receptor is crucial for drug reinforcement 65. A core predisposition to addictive and impulsive behaviors is centered on a set of genes that promote feelings of well-being via DA release 65 from NAcc neurons through neurotransmitter interactions in the mesolimbic system. Furthermore, the reward cascade involves the release of serotonin resulting in hypothalamus stimulation of encephalin and inhibition of GABA at the substantia nigra, thus fine-tuning NAcc DA release. This has given rise to the hypothesis that genetic variation relating to DA may link impulsivity with addiction risk 66. A single nucleotide polymorphism (rs1800497) has been linked to addiction 67, impulsivity 68, and D2 receptor density 69; this variant is not in DRD2 gene itself, but is part of an evolutionarily conserved gene cluster on chromosome 11 that includes DRD2 and putatively functionally co-regulate DA neurotransmission 70. Support for the relevance of this gene cluster was present in a recent study of impulsivity in relation to this region, revealing two significantly associated haplotypes, with the association of one being driven by rs1800497 and the other being driven by a newly identified SNP (rs1079597) 71. Thus, dysfunction in the brain reward cascade caused by certain genetic variants may cause a hypo-DAergic drive that is behaviorally reflected in greater impulsivity and accordingly leads to greater drug-seeking behavior. Alcohol, cocaine, cannabis, and nicotine stimulate DA release, and thus putatively might remediate this drive.
There is controversy as to whether hyper- or hypoactivation of ventral striatal and DA functioning conveys addiction risk. Among adolescents, an imbalance of immature top-down and hyperactive bottom has been suggested to lead to increased susceptibility to SUD 58. In adolescents and adults, ventral striatal hypoactivation is linked to impulsivity and SUD,58 such as alcohol use disorder (AUD) 72, amongst those at risk 73, in youth 74, and in gambling 75. Reward deficiency syndrome has been proposed which postulates that hypo-DAergic activity results in decreased sensitivity to natural reinforcers, contributing to withdrawal and the perpetuation of drug use 76. Decreased DA activity may relate to D2 reductions in the anterior cingulate gyrus and OFC, thereby providing a mechanism by which DA disruptions lead to compulsive drug use 76. In rodent studies, reduced D2DR availability has been associated with trait-like impulsivity 63, while in stimulant use disorders striatal D2DR availability has been negatively correlated with impulsivity 77.
Another non-DA system potentially involved in impulsivity and SUDs includes norepinephrine. This neurotransmitter has been linked to impulsive behaviors and addictions mediating stimulant effects such as drug-seeking behavior 78. Moreover, clinical trials using adrenergic modulators have shown promise as substance cessation aids, with reduced use of cocaine 79. Finally, similar to glutamate and GABA, serotonin or 5-hydroxytryptamine (5-HT) levels have also been associated with SUDs. Specifically, clinical, animal and genetic studies have indicated that low levels of the 5-HT transmission have been linked to impulsive choices and addictions such as early onset alcoholism 78,80. Selective 5-HT receptor agonists and antagonists have demonstrated elevated and blocked impulsiveness, respectively57. Moreover, both human and animal trials have demonstrated that upregulated 5-HT receptor mechanisms contribute to the development of SUDs via dysregulated IC associated with impulsivity 81.
Does impulsivity contribute to addiction risk or vice versa?
There have been three well-accepted premises regarding impulsivity and SUDs: (1) impulsivity causes SUDs; (2) SUDs cause impulsivity; and (3) impulsivity is related to a third factor governing SUDs 3. The behavioral traits of impulsivity have been widely associated with SUDs and addictive behaviors (e.g., gambling). Notably, these traits vary throughout the life span, with enhanced impulsivity observed during adolescence coinciding with increased drug use 82.
Impulsivity contributes to SUD
Both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies have supported the idea of linking trait impulsivity to drug use. It has been widely recognized that deficient IC in SUDs includes a component of preexisting impulsivity that may predict initial substance use 83, the development of SUDs, risk for addiction 84, chronic use 85, relapse rates, 86 and treatment retention 87. In comparison with nonpsychiatric controls, higher levels of impulsivity have been found in individuals with SUD involving stimulant, opiate, and alcohol use 11, and greater discounting of delayed rewards has been found amongst individuals with SUD in tobacco 45, alcohol 88, cocaine 89, opiates 90, and methamphetamine 91. Similarly, increased impulsivity levels have been found amongst cannabis- 92, alcohol- 93, cocaine- 94, and opiate-dependent 95 individuals. Highly impulsive individuals with poor IC may be more sensitive to attention-grabbing properties of substance-related stimuli than those not using substances 96. Moreover, studies have found that discounting levels vary by a type of SUDs, with particularly cannabis, opiates and cocaine being associated with most impulsivity 90,92. Thus, impulsivity may potentially represent an endophenotype for SUDs and persist with symptom remissions, evidenced as heritable; conferring an increased risk of developing SUDs compared with the general population 97. A genetic basis of impulsivity is suggested given the early rs1800497 (DRD2/ANKK1) findings and putative reward deficiency syndrome.71 Heritable risk factors for SUDs involving inhibitory deficits prevalent in childhood disruptive behavior disorders, such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), and conduct disorder (CD) have also been suggested given these disorders are familial in nature, commonly comorbid with SUD, and present in early life 98. Ortal and colleagues also found that certain impulsivity constructs (e.g., disinhibition, impulsive choice, and sensation seeking) indexed via abnormal brain activity indicate shared neurophysiological deficits between ADHD and SUD 99. Moreover, several neurocognitive tests in clinical settings have consistently demonstrated deficits in impulsivity amongst individuals with SUD, indicative of preexisting PFC deficiencies 20,100. Advances in neuroanatomy and molecular pharmacology 101 as well as extending to imaging studies (e.g., positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies) have correlated cognitive activity in the brain to substance addictions. Dom and colleagues found that among 52 studies reviewed, most demonstrated significant deficits in decision making involving the OFC among individuals with SUDs 102. Several studies have found deficits in impulsivity indexed by the stop-signal reaction-time task within alcohol 103, cocaine 104,105, and methamphetamine 106 addictions.
With respect to the four categories mentioned of impulsivity, all have been linked to substance use disorders 107; a moderate to strong prediction has been found between negative urgency and lack of perseverance, respectively, and problematic substance use (e.g., alcohol), while the lack of premeditation and sensation seeking has been correlated with increased frequency of substance use. Nonetheless, it is important to note that various stages of addiction (acquisition, escalation, abstinence, relapse, and treatment), vary in the impact of impulsive behavior and its constituents 3.
In SUD stages, the acquisition phase, which involves the progression of initial drug use to the maintenance of use, animal models suggest that impulsivity may predate substance use 3. After screening for high or low measures of impulsivity, subsequent initiation of drug use has been monitored 3. For instance, a more rapid onset and greater cocaine self-administration were found in a high-impulsive group of rats compared with the low-impulsive group 108. Similarly, this same model has been translated to human behavior, with studies finding that impulsive choice to initiate substance use, and immediate euphoric effects of a drug, out-value future larger benefits, such as personal, educational, social, and economic success 109. Furthermore, initiation of substance-taking typically occurs during adolescence, which is a high-risk period for the development of SUD due to the immaturity of prefrontal cortical systems responsible for IC 110 as evidenced with fMRI scanning in young children, adolescents, and young adults 11.
Loss of control of drug use is another stage of addiction that posits impulsivity leading to SUD 3. No human studies have been conducted to support this premise; however, rat trials have demonstrated escalation of drug use in response to increased impulsivity, implicating that highly impulsive individuals may be more prone to accelerating drug use/SUD 111,112.
Levels of impulsivity have also been related to the stages of abstinence, relapse and treatment success. For instance, nicotine deprivation among smokers was found to increase the frequency at which smokers discounted delayed choices 113. One study also found that DD deficits amongst schizophrenia patients were linked with high rates of cigarette smoking and difficulty of maintaining abstinence 114. Similarly, chronic methamphetamine users who were abstinent for 5–7 days were found to have deficits in their response inhibition 106. Symptoms experienced via abstinence, such as increased levels of withdrawal 115 and craving, have also been suggested to be associated with self-reported impulsivity amongst many SUDs including alcohol 116, cocaine, and methamphetamine users.117 That is to say, those who are more impulsive tend to experience greater levels of craving during withdrawal, and a greater likelihood of relapse 3. Moreover, individuals with higher impulsivity scores based on questionnaires, have also been more likely to have poorer treatment retention for cocaine misuse, than those with lower impulsivity scores 118.
SUDs contribute to impulsivity.
It is also possible that substance use has a state effect on impulsivity 3. It has been repeatedly shown that psychoactive substance exposure (e.g., alcohol and cannabis) during adolescent and adulthood results in greater brain vulnerability to changes in white matter integrity, morphology, and activation during cognitive assessments 58. In particular, chronic neurobiological effects of drug self-administration may mediate structural change to the PFC, via direct neurotoxicity, cell death, or tissue shrinkage, resulting in a gradual attrition of behavioral self-control 13,119. Numerous studies have found that SUD alters performance in humans across several independent behavioral measures of impulsivity, relating to cognitive outcomes such as DD, behavioral inhibition, and lapses of attention, thus demonstrating that impulsivity may result from drug use 21. Moreover, both brain imaging and post-mortem studies have shown reduced regional brain volumes, gray (~20%) and white (~10%) matter densities in individuals with SUDs, such as alcohol, ecstasy, and opiates 120–122. Changes in gene expression as a consequence of reduced IC within SUD have also been found 11. Thus there has been a strong indication within human trials that drug use increases levels of impulsive behaviors, which in turn facilitates drug use 19. In contrast, although increased levels of impulsivity in humans have been found amongst those who use drugs, there is evidence of reduced drug use with increased impulsivity 123. However, one may argue that these results reflect dysregulated regions of the brain and subsequent cognitive processes given chronic drug use. Consistent with this assertion, animal trials have demonstrated SUD may cause impulsivity a short-term drug administration suggest the emergence of IC deficits 124,125. Ultimately, these findings depend on the dose, participant samples, and specific testing parameters in order to account for the various effects of SUDs on impulsivity. Taken together, drug use may impair IC resulting in functional consequences 21.
Impulsivity related to a third (independent) factor governing SUD.
Finally, the environment may contribute to impulsivity and SUDs 3. In both animal and human substance consumption, sex appears as a major factor with females exhibiting greater drug-seeking behavior than males 126,127. Men report more problems with SUDs compared with women; however, women are more likely to transition to continued misuse than men. Furthermore, preclinical models have shown that amongst females, drug self-administration, escalated drug intake, and higher reinstatement rates are more likely than in males 128–131. Clinical studies have also produced mixed results 132–134.
Another factor relating to impulsivity and SUDs is a hormonal status. The presence of estrogen, progesterone, and circulating gonadal hormones has been found to play a major role in SUD, with facilitated acquisitions of drug self-administration 135, escalation 136, reinstatement 137, and attenuating effects 136. For instance, higher levels of testosterone have been associated with greater levels of impulsivity, while another study has found the effect to be baseline dependent 138,139.
Risk of SUD and high levels of impulsivity have also been commonly associated with early life experiences such as prenatal drug exposure (e.g., alcohol) and impoverished rearing conditions (e.g., physical abuse) 140,141.
Epigenetic factors also contribute to impulsivity and addiction. In a longitudinal cohort, Wang et al. found that impulsivity mediated the relationship between family disorganization and subsequent alcohol use, specifically amongst individuals at low genetic risk based on polygenic risk scores for impulsivity 142.
Finally, other comorbidities such as ADHD, conduct disorders, oppositional deviant disorder, and other childhood behavior problems, may pose as risk factors for SUD through the role of impulsivity 99. Constructs of impulsivity such as sensation seeking and conduct problem symptoms prevalent among individuals with ADHD have also been suggested to predict the increased risk for SUD, including misuse of stimulants 143.
Trait factors in impulsivity as targets for SUD treatment development
There are three main approaches to assessing impulsive behavior. The first one focuses on translational between animal and human models 3. This includes targeting underlying processes involved in impulsivity and SUDs (i.e., working memory (WM) and attention) to validate behavioral measures of impulsivity 3. For instance, WM training can improve SUD outcomes 144,145. The second approach, behavioral measures are easy to administer in both species, thus requiring a modest amount of training, which may allow the ability to determine the efficacy of individualized treatments targeting impulsivity and SUD. 3 Finally, these tasks do not rely on retrospective measures or recalling of past events, but focus on the current state in the third approach. This would confirm the validity of assessing changes in impulsivity 3.
Genetic factors implicated in the development of SUD may also be a target for SUD treatment development. One sibling study found that impulsivity may be exacerbated with chronic substance use as evidenced by siblings of chronic stimulant users reporting significantly higher levels of trait impulsivity than controls 84. Furthermore, biological predisposition to impulsivity and SUD, relate to dysregulated function of the frontal control over corticolimbic circuitry contributed by DAergic projections from the ventral tegmentalVTA to the Nacc and also involves serotonergic, GABAergic, and glutamergic processes. As such targeting these various regions of the brain indirectly using medications or behavioral treatments may result in enhanced cognition via decision making thereby serving as effective treatment strategies 146. For instance, opioid receptor antagonists (naltrexone) and glutamatergic compounds (N-acetyl cysteine (NAC)) influence mesolimbic DA function indirectly and shown to target reward-seeking addictive behaviors 147,148.
Behavioral data also suggests that recognizing high impulsive behavior may predict the initiation and progression of SUD, thus early detection may serve as an effective means of treatment 146. Studies relating to patients with diagnoses that include aspects of impulsivity or related constructs, such as ADHD, have indicated an association with later developments of SUD 14,146. Finally, studying impulsivity in children and adolescents is important to consider, as it may help produce biological explanations of later developing SUD 58. Taken together, these trait factors offer several dimensions for the progressive treatment developments in SUDs, which have expanded to pharmacological, behavioral and neurophysiological mechanisms.
Treatment of co-occurring impulsivity and substance use disorders
There are strong overlaps in the neural circuitry and functional mechanisms between impulsivity traits and addiction 49, which has directed treatment approaches. However, one of the major difficulties when studying impulsivity and its relation to SUD is the inherent multiplicity of factors related to impulsivity. We review pharmacological (Table 2), behavioral (Table 3), and neuromodulation interventions.
Table 2.
Pharmacological treatments for impulsivity and SUD.
Reference | Sample | Drug | Design | Impulsivity measures | Impulsivity dimension | Treatment | Outcome |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
157 | Adults (n = 165) |
Tobacco | RCT | Personality profile: novelty seeking/hyperactive | Sensation seeking |
|
No differential effects between treatment groups based on personality profile |
158 | Adolescents (n = 115) |
Cannabis | RCT | BIS-11 | Lack of premeditation Lack of perseverance |
|
No effect of treatment group on substance use outcomes in highly impulsive individuals |
162 | Adult males (n = 160) |
Alcohol | RCT | TPQ Novelty seeking subscale | Sensation seeking |
|
Novelty seeking higher among dropouts. No differential effects between treatment groups based on novelty seeking |
163 | Adult males (n = 51) |
Alcohol | Open-label study | BIS-11, IMT, and SKIP | Lack of premeditation Lack of perseverance |
|
Naltrexone had no effect on alcohol use outcomes based on impulsivity |
166 | Adult males (n = 63) |
Alcohol | RCT | CPT, SST, and DRLR | Urgency (behavioral inhibition) |
|
TP group improved alcohol use outcomes which was associated with performance on behavioral
inhibition paradigm, CPT. TP group had higher improvement rates on CPT and stop-signal task. |
164 | n = 83 | Alcohol | RCT | BIS-11, State Impulsivity Questionnaire, SST, and DDT | Lack of premeditation Lack of perseverance |
|
Modafinil improved measures of state impulsivity but had no effect on behavioral measures of
impulsivity Modafinil prolonged time to relapse, increased abstinent days in participants with poor response inhibition at baseline, and reduced in participants with good response inhibition at baseline |
165 | n = 99 | Alcohol | RCT | BIS-11 | Lack of premeditation Lack of perseverance |
|
Apriprazole increased latency to consume drinks in individuals with high impulsivity |
174 | Adults (n = 75) |
Cocaine | RCT | BIS-11 and IGT | Lack of premeditation Lack of perseverance |
|
No difference between treatment groups or impulsivity at baseline CBT + CT effective for highly impulsive cocaine dependent patients |
175 | Adults (n = 65) |
Crack cocaine | RCT | BIS-11, SWCT, and SST | Lack of premeditation Lack of perseverance |
|
Modafinil did not appear to reduce measures of impulsivity in this population |
176 | Adults (n = 34) |
Cocaine | Open-label trial | BIS-11, IMPSS, Immediate/Delayed Memory Task, Go/Stop task, DD | Lack of premeditation Lack of perseverance |
1. D-amphetamine | D-amphetamine did not reduce impulsivity |
181 | n = 38 | Problem gamblers | RCT | EIQ Impulsiveness subscale | Sensation seeking |
|
Improvement in gambling severity by paroxetine was associated with changes in impulsiveness scores |
BIS-11, Barratt impulsiveness scale-11; BT, behavioral therapy; CBT, cognitive behavioral therapy; CM, contingency management; CPT, continuous performance pest; DD, delay discounting task; DRLR, differential reinforcement for low-rate responding; EIQ, Eysenck impulsiveness questionnaire; IGT, Iowa gambling task; IMPSS, Zuckerman’s Impulsive Sensation Seeking subscale; IMT, immediate memory task; RCT, randomized control trial; SKIP, single-key impulsivity paradigm; SST, stop-signal task; SCWT, Stroop color and word test; TP, topiramate; TPQ, tridimensional personality questionnaire.
Table 3.
Behavioral treatments for impulsivity and SUD.
Reference | Sample | Drug investigated | Design | Impulsivity measures | Impulsivity dimension | Treatment | Outcome |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
182 | Adolescents (n = 81) |
Tobacco | RCT | IMPSS | Sensation Seeking |
|
High impulsivity/sensation-seeking group showed greater reduction in cigarettes with the tobacco
educational control condition Low impulsivity/sensation-seeking group showed greater reduction in MET condition |
183 | Adolescents (n = 64) |
Tobacco | RCT | BIS-11 | Lack of premeditation Lack of perseverance |
|
CM was more effective at increasing abstinence for individuals high in impulsivity compared to CBT alone |
184 | Adults (n = 127) |
Cannabis | RCT | EDT | Lack of premeditation |
|
Pretreatment discount not associated with cannabis treatment outcomes Individuals in CM condition did not change discounting over time whereas those that did not receive CM increased their discounting |
185 | Adolescents (n = 2904) |
Cannabis | ClusterRCT | Reckless Behavior Questionnaire | Sensation Seeking | Personality-targeted interventions based on high-risk profile (anxiety sensitivity, hopelessness, impulsivity, and sensation seeking) | Impulsivity-targeted intervention did not reduce cannabis use outcomes Delayed onset of cannabis use in sensation-seeking targeted intervention |
186 | Young adults (n = 67) |
Alcohol | RCT | TPQ and IMPSS novelty-seeking subscale | Sensation Seeking |
|
High novelty-seeking group showed greater improvement in alcohol outcomes with the alcohol
educational control condition Low novelty-seeking group showed greater improvements in MET condition |
187 | Young adults (n = 207) |
Alcohol | RCT | UPPS | Urgency |
|
Negative urgency was positively associated with urge to drink in mindfulness intervention |
188 | Adults (n = 90) |
Cocaine and Alcohol | RCT | Monetary Choice Questionnaire | Lack of premeditation |
|
ATM intervention associated with less delay discounting and less cocaine use relative to control condition. Increases in delay discounting associated with decreased cocaine abstinence |
189 | Adults (n = 36) |
Cocaine | Prospective | DD | Lack of premeditation |
|
Delay discounting unrelated to abstinence in high-magnitude condition, decreased abstinence in low-magnitude condition |
144 | Adults (n = 50) |
Methamphetamine | RCT | BSI-11 | Lack of premeditation Lack of perseverance |
|
Impulsivity scores improved in CT group |
190 | Adults (n = 68) |
Heroin and Cocaine | RCT | TPQ Novelty-seeking subscale | Sensation Seeking |
|
No differences found between contingency treatment groups |
192 | Adults (n = 38) adults |
Methadone Maintenance/ Opioids and Cocaine | Pilot study | BIS-11 | Lack of premeditation Lack of perseverance |
|
3-S+ therapy group demonstrated reduced impulsivity and substance use |
191 | n=159 | Opioid | Second analysis | DD | Lack of premeditation | Buprenorphine + variations of voucher CM or standard counselling | All treatments equally promoted decreases in delay discounting |
ATM, advisor-teller monetary manager; BIS-11, Barratt impulsiveness scale-11; BT, behavioral therapy; CBT, cognitive behavioral therapy; CM, contingency management; CT, cognitive training; DD, delay-discounting task; EDT; experiential discounting task; IMPSS, Zuckerman’s Impulsive Sensation Seeking subscale; RCT, randomized control trial; WM, working memory; UPPS, urgency premeditation perseverance sensation-seeking positive impulsivity scale.
Pharmacological treatments
Tobacco and nicotine.
Preclinical studies have highlighted the effect that nicotine has on increasing impulsive action 149,150. This effect might be mediated by cholinergic receptors, specifically the nicotinic α4β2 receptors 151,152. Research studies using animal models have found that antagonists at this receptor reduce self-administration of nicotine and relapse behavior 153,154, indicating its potential as a treatment option with a dual effect on impulsivity and attenuating tobacco use in humans.
There have been two clinical studies in human subjects that have investigated pharmacological treatments targeting impulsivity in smokers. In a retrospective study exploring this topic, researchers looked at data from adolescents with ADHD and found that individuals that were taking methylphenidate exhibited lower rates of tobacco use when compared with non-medicated individuals 155. To follow up with these findings, a group of researchers conducted a trial of methylphenidate in current smokers with ADHD and found a reduction in ADHD symptoms, but no effect on tobacco use outcomes 156. These findings suggest that stimulant agents for ADHD may exert protective effects against later substance use in these patients 49. In another clinical study, researchers created a personality profile for the level of novelty seeking of each participant and assigned them to either a modified treatment condition targeting impulsivity with bupropion and tailored behavioral therapy or to a standard treatment group 157. The researchers found no differential response to either treatment condition for the novelty-seeking profile 157. As such, further studies conducted in humans should potentially investigate the role of cholinergic receptors on impulsivity and substance-related outcomes.
Cannabis.
One human study has examined pharmacological treatments for cannabis users with high impulsivity 158. The study was a clinical trial involving two treatment groups of contingency management (CM), but one including NAC administration158. They found no differences between the treatment groups, indicating the lack of efficiency of NAC on impulsive cannabis users 158. Preclinical animal studies have found an association between CB1 receptors and increases in impulsivity 159. Additionally, administering CB1 receptor antagonists, such as rimonabant, in animal models has been shown to reduce baseline impulsivity 160 and self-administration of several classes of substances 161. Further research in human models is warranted to investigate the role that the endocannabinoid system has on impulsivity and substance use as well as the potential to develop treatments targeting this system.
Alcohol.
Five studies have been conducted in humans to find a treatment for impulsivity and AUD 162–166. The first study compared 6 months of lithium, busiprone, or placebo on individuals with AUD 162. They found no difference between treatment groups, and that individuals high in the novelty-seeking trait were more likely to drop out of the study 162. Zorlu and colleagues conducted an open-label study of naltrexone for AUD and compared treated patients with naltrexone-naive patients and healthy controls 163. They found that naltrexone had no effect on alcohol use outcomes with impulsivity as a mediator 163. Rubio and colleagues 166 investigated the effects of topiramate compared with placebo and found that the treatment group significantly improved alcohol use outcomes, which were mediated by performance on an objective test of behavioral inhibition. They also found that the treatment group performed better on two behavioral tasks related to impulsivity across the study period 166. Another placebo-controlled trial investigated the effects of modafinil on impulsive drinkers with AUD and also found that the treatment group improved abstinence outcomes, which were associated with response inhibition 164. Modafinil was also found to improve self-reported measures of impulsivity but had no effect on behavioral measures of impulsivity 164.
Finally, Anton and colleagues 165 conducted a placebo-controlled trial with aripiprazole and brought participants into a laboratory bar paradigm at the end of the study, where they were asked to choose between an immediate drink or a delayed monetary reward. Aripiprazole reduced drinks consumed and increased the duration to drink in individuals rated high on impulsivity in the bar laboratory paradigm 165. Thus, there are promising pharmacological treatments for impulsivity in this population, but further research should be conducted to replicate these findings.
Stimulants.
In animal models, there has been evidence to demonstrate that pharmacological agents that modulate noradrenaline levels, such as selective noradrenaline reuptake inhibitors (NRIs) are effective at reducing impulsivity 167–171. Interestingly, the NRI atomoxetine has been found to inhibit cue-induced cocaine self-administration in mice 172,173. Three drug trials in human subjects have been conducted, all with a focus on improving impulsivity in cocaine users 174–176. Schmitz and colleagues 174 conducted an RCT comparing citalopram with cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) and CM to a placebo with CBT and CM. They found no differences between treatment groups, but that baseline impulsivity was predictive of better outcomes. Another RCT compared modafinil with CBT to CBT alone in crack cocaine users and found no improvement in impulsivity measures 175. Finally, an open-label trial was conducted with D-amphetamine in cocaine users, but they also found that the drug had no effect on impulsivity 176. No human studies have yet investigated the effects of NARIs in individuals with stimulant use disorders.
Opioids.
No human studies have been conducted on pharmacological treatments to improve impulsivity in opioid users. However, preclinical research has shown some promise in μ- and δ-opioid receptor antagonists, as they have been found to reduce drug self-administration and relapse behaviors 177–180.
Problem gambling.
A pilot study comparing paroxetine to placebo found that drug-related improvement in gambling severity was significantly associated with changes in impulsiveness scores 181.
Taken together, further research is needed on pharmacological agents that target impulsivity in individuals with SUDs. There are promising findings in preclinical research that show promise for drugs that target the cholinergic, noradrenergic, and opioid neurotransmitter systems for this population 153,154,167–173,177–180. Additionally, there has been convincing evidence that suggests the potential for prescribed amphetamines to prevent the onset of substance use disorders in individuals diagnosed with ADHD 49,155,156. Finally, the only positive findings in human drug trials that have been found to reduce impulsivity and improve substance use outcomes have been topiramate, modafinil, and aripiprazole for AUD 164–166 as well as paroxetine for problem gamblers 181.
Behavioral treatments
Tobacco.
Two treatment studies in smokers have demonstrated in the post-hoc analysis that baseline impulsivity levels may be predictive of poorer abstinence outcomes. These findings warrant the consideration of targeting impulsivity to improve outcomes in this population. Two studies have been conducted that focus on the effect of a behavioral treatment on impulsive individuals 182,183. Helstrom and colleagues 182 compared motivational enhancement therapy (MET) session with a tobacco educational control condition. They found unexpectedly that individuals high in impulsivity showed improved tobacco use outcomes in the educational control, indicating that MET may not be an effective strategy in impulsive individuals 182. Another study 183 found that CBT in combination with CM is more effective at improving abstinence rates in impulsive individuals compared with CBT alone. Further investigation is needed to solidify an effective treatment for impulsive smokers.
Cannabis.
Two studies have been conducted exploring behavioral treatments for cannabis users with high impulsivity traits. The first study was an RCT that included four conditions comparing CBT with a combination of two different CM strategies, which were CM with reinforcement for attendance, and CM with reinforcement for abstinence 184. They found that pretreatment impulsivity, using an objective delay discounting test (DDT), was not associated with cannabis use outcomes, but that both CM conditions prevented DD from worsening over time 184. The second study was a cluster-RCT that examined a personality-targeted intervention that was implemented in 21 secondary schools and was based on a four-faceted high-risk profile (anxiety, hopelessness, impulsivity, and sensation seeking) 185. They found that in students with a high sensation-seeking profile, the targeted intervention delayed the onset of cannabis use 185. Further clinical studies should be carried out to identify the psychological mechanism behind this association with impulsivity and cannabis use.
Alcohol.
Two RCTs have been conducted exploring behavioral treatment for impulsivity and AUDs. First, Feldstein and colleagues 186 compared motivational enhancement therapy (MET) to an alcohol educational control condition. They also found that individuals high in novelty seeking showed improved alcohol use outcomes in the educational control condition, and low novelty-seeking individuals fared better with MET 186. Second, another RCT was conducted examining the effects of a mindfulness intervention compared in AUD patients when considering several impulsivity traits 187. They found that negative urgency was associated with an increased urge to drink in the mindfulness intervention, indicating that mindfulness treatments may not be effective in highly impulsive drinkers 187. Thus far, there have been no effective behavioral treatments found for this population.
Stimulants.
Three studies have been conducted investigating behavioral treatments for impulsivity and stimulant use. Black and Rosen 144,188,189 conducted an RCT of the Advisor-Teller monetary manager (ATM) intervention compared with a control condition in cocaine users. ATM addresses money management problems in substance abuse and provides help from therapists with planning and monitoring budgets 188. They found that the ATM condition produced reductions in DD and cocaine use compared with the control condition and that these effects were association 188. Another prospective study compared CM with low- versus high-magnitude vouchers in cocaine users 189. They found that in individuals with high impulsivity had reduced abstinence rates in the low magnitude condition, but not in the high-magnitude condition, signifying that high magnitude CM might be more effective in impulsive cocaine users 189. Finally, Brooks and colleagues investigated the effects of WM cognitive training (CT) compared with treatment as usual in methamphetamine users and healthy controls 144. They found that the WM CT was effective at improving self-reported impulsivity scores 144. Stimulant users appear to be responsive to behavioral treatments targeting impulsivity, and further research should work to develop evidence-based treatments.
Opioids.
Three studies on behavioral treatments for impulsivity in opioid use disorders have been conducted 190,191. One of these studies involved three study conditions all including buprenorphine with CM in one of three conditions: (1) contingent reinforcement vouchers; (2) reduced value contingent reinforcement vouchers; and (3) non-contingent voucher 190. They found no differences in impulsivity across the treatment groups 190. In contrast, a secondary analysis reviewing the effects of two RCTs involving buprenorphine and CM on DD outcomes found that all treatments equally lead to reductions in DD 191. Finally, a group of researchers conducted a pilot study in substance-using patients enrolled in a methadone maintenance program which provided spiritual self-schema (3-S+) therapy compared with a standard care control condition 192. They found that the 3-S+ therapy group demonstrated reduced impulsivity and substance use, indicating the importance of incorporating elements of self-schema into mindfulness interventions in impulsive drug users 192. Opioid users appear to be responsive to behavioral treatments that aim to modify impulsivity, and further research should be conducted to develop treatments.
Overall, the behavioral treatment with the most supporting evidence for impulsive individuals with SUDs is CM, particularly with high-value rewards 184,189,190. There is little support to show that MET on its own provides any benefit to this population, and in two cases an education control condition has been shown to be more effective. The ATM188 and 3-S+ 192 are novel treatment paradigms that show promise for this population, but further research should be conducted across various SUDs.
Neuromodulation treatments
A review by Brevet-Aeby et al.24 suggested that non-invasive brain stimulation may lead to improvements in impulsivity in humans on dimensions of attention, planning, IC, risk taking, and DD after stimulation to prefrontal regions (e.g., DLPFC) 24. Several studies have found improvements in impulsive and risky behavior upon continuous theta burst stimulation (TBS) to DLPFC 193, transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the right inferior frontal gyrus (rIFG)194,195 and the DLPFC 196–200, as well as repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) over the dorsal frontomedial cortex 201, rIFG 202, DLPFC, and lateral PFC.203,204 In two studies analyzing cocaine use, rTMS and tDCS of the DLPFC led to decreased levels of measured impulsivity post-treatment 205,206. In additional two studies focusing on cigarette consumption, rTMS over the DLPFC also led to decreased impulsivity 207; however, tDCS over the same region did not show any changes in the second study 208. A study on cannabis use found that TBS over the DLPFC increased risky behavior 209 and another study looking at gambling disorder found neither rTMS nor TBS led to decreased impulsivity measures 210. From these results, regions implicated in impulsivity and targeted by certain neurophysiological treatments have spanned the frontal cortex, however, there lacks consistency in hemispheric localization and specificity of brain regions related to impulsivity. Despite the collection of studies analyzing general cognition in healthy individuals as described, neurophysiological treatments studies for substance use disorders that include assessment of impulsivity pre- and post-treatment are lacking. Moreover, a recent review of non-invasive neuromodulation techniques has highlighted the several gaps and high variability in the literature examining the effects of these methods to treat SUD 211, indicating the need for further research in this potentially promising area.
Implications of a transdiagnostic model for impulsivity and addiction risk
There are several primary measures included under the rubric of impulsivity (response inhibition, inattention, urgency, lack of premeditation and perseverance, and sensation seeking) 115. Impulsivity has been suggested as a trait vulnerability marker for addiction risk involving underlying brain circuits and neurotransmitter systems (e.g., DAergic), which may result in greater risk for SUD. Similarly, stage effects of SUD including the development/acquisition, escalation, abstinence, and relapse and treatment phases amongst both chronic and acute substance-using individuals, comprise state changes to specific brain regions resulting in declined cognitive abilities and increased impulsivity. Moreover, evidence suggests that chronic substance use (binge drinking) results in impairments of specific regions of the brain (PFC projection to the ACC and OFC) which contribute to an imbalance of craving–limbic drive and frontal cortical attention and executive function such as IC 212. Finally, other environmental and genetic factors may influence the initiation and progression of both SUD and impulsive behavior that further complicates this complex relationship 213. Understanding the interrelationships between these three components may lead to the development of targeted treatments (Fig. 2). Certain treatment modalities may target common neurotransmitter deficiencies present in dimensions of impulsive behavior and in SUDs. For instance, the dysfunctional reward pathway of the brain (centering on DLFPC) has been indicated in impulsivity and SUDs (e.g., cocaine craving) 205. Thus treatments such as rTMS, which have been found to activate these regions directly, may posit as an effective transdiagnostic model targeting the same underlying deficiencies in co-occurring impulsivity and SUD.
Figure 2.
Transdiagnostic model for addiction risk and targets for treatments. The interrelationship between trait effects, state effects and other environmental and genetic factors, as well as implicated neurotransmitter levels, influencing the initiation and progression of impulsivity and substance use disorders.
Conclusions
Taken together, there is increasing evidence supporting pharmacological and behavioral treatments for impulsivity in SUDs. However, further research is needed. Future pharmacological research should focus on investigating the neurotransmitter systems and pharmacological agents (e.g., topiramate, modafinil, and aripiprazole) that have been shown efficacious in preclinical studies. This research could better our understanding of the etiology of impulsivity in addiction and potentially provide tailored treatments for these individuals. Behavioral treatments should focus on novel therapies that target the root of the impulsivity trait to produce behavioral change in patients.
However, there are several gaps to address in future research. First, there is not always a consistent relationship between behavioral measures and self-report given the circumscribed definition of impulsivity, as well as an individual’s ability to report the cognitive processes underlying their behavior 3. The multiplicity of impulsivity measures besides the defined ones (e.g., urgency, sensation seeking, lack of perseverance, and lack of premeditation) provides an inconsistency amongst trials with conceptual/methodological heterogeneity. Second, there are individual differences on laboratory measures of impulsive choice and inhibition, indicating the importance of using several models to obtain convergent validity, and identifying how these behavioral measures relate, if at all 3. For instance, experimental conditions played a role in the determination of sex differences, with women discounting delayed hypothetical reinforcers at higher rates than men, while the reverse was found when real reinforces were offered 214. Third, studies will often assess impulsivity in the demographic assessments made at the beginning of the study, but fail to measure impulsivity after treatment. Thus a potential recommendation for future studies is to complete such measurements both pre- and post-study intervention. Moreover, measurement time frame should be considered given that some measures, like DDT, go/no-go, and SST are amenable to change but personality traits may have too broad a time window or be subject to demand characteristics.
It is not clear to what extent impulsivity is a result of chronic SUDs or a predisposing risk factor. Future studies should consider the multifaceted construct of impulsivity in parallel with the relationship of the various stages of addiction. This will ultimately provide a better understanding of the gaps that exist in understanding SUDS and impulsivity and for developing more effective treatments.
Acknowledgments
This manuscript was supported by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) predoctoral Graduate Fellowship (to K.K.), and operating Grants from the CIHR (MOP#115145) and National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA grant R21-DA-043949) to Dr. George.
Footnotes
Human and animal rights and informed consent
This article does not contain any studies with human or animal subjects performed by any of the authors.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests
References
- 1.WHO. World Drug Report. 2017; Available from: https://www.unodc.org/wdr2017/en/exsum.html.
- 2.Wong CC and Schumann G, Review. Genetics of addictions: strategies for addressing heterogeneity and polygenicity of substance use disorders. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 2008. 363(1507): p. 3213–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Perry JL and Carroll ME, The role of impulsive behavior in drug abuse. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2008. 200(1): p. 1–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Aron AR, The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive control. Neuroscientist, 2007. 13(3): p. 214–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Martel MM, Nigg JT, and Lucas RE, Trait Mechanisms in Youth with and without Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. J Res Pers, 2008. 42(4): p. 895–913. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Brewer JA and Potenza MN, The neurobiology and genetics of impulse control disorders: relationships to drug addictions. Biochem Pharmacol, 2008. 75(1): p. 63–75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Whiteside S and Lynam D, The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 2001. 30(4): p. 669–689. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Mitchell MR and Potenza MN, Addictions and Personality Traits: Impulsivity and Related Constructs. Curr Behav Neurosci Rep, 2014. 1(1): p. 1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Cyders MA and Smith GT, Emotion-based dispositions to rash action: positive and negative urgency. Psychol Bull, 2008. 134(6): p. 807–28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Smith GT and Cyders MA, Integrating affect and impulsivity: The role of positive and negative urgency in substance use risk. Drug Alcohol Depend, 2016. 163 Suppl 1: p. S3–S12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Verdejo-Garcia A, Lawrence AJ, and Clark L, Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 2008. 32(4): p. 777–810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kisa C, Yildirim SG, and Goka E, [Impulsivity and mental disorders]. Turk Psikiyatri Derg, 2005. 16(1): p. 46–54. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Goldstein RZ and Volkow ND, Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry, 2002. 159(10): p. 1642–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Kreek MJ, et al. , Genetic influences on impulsivity, risk taking, stress responsivity and vulnerability to drug abuse and addiction. Nat Neurosci, 2005. 8(11): p. 1450–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Hyman SE, Malenka RC, and Nestler EJ, Neural mechanisms of addiction: the role of reward-related learning and memory. Annu Rev Neurosci, 2006. 29: p. 565–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kalivas PW and Volkow ND, The neural basis of addiction: a pathology of motivation and choice. Am J Psychiatry, 2005. 162(8): p. 1403–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Evenden JL, Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 1999. 146(4): p. 348–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Tomko RL, Bountress KE, and Gray KM, Personalizing substance use treatment based on pre-treatment impulsivity and sensation seeking: A review. Drug Alcohol Depend, 2016. 167: p. 1–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.de Wit H, Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: a review of underlying processes. Addict Biol, 2009. 14(1): p. 22–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Stevens L, et al. , Impulsivity as a vulnerability factor for poor addiction treatment outcomes: a review of neurocognitive findings among individuals with substance use disorders. J Subst Abuse Treat, 2014. 47(1): p. 58–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.De Wit H, Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: a review of underlying processes. Addiction biology, 2009. 14(1): p. 22–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Evenden JL, Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology, 1999. 146(4): p. 348–361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Barratt ES, Impulsivity: Cognitive, behavioral, and psychophysiological correlates. Biological bases of sensation seeking, impulsivity, and anxiety, 1983. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Brevet-Aeby C, et al. , Prefrontal cortex and impulsivity: Interest of noninvasive brain stimulation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 2016. 71: p. 112–134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Moeller FG, et al. , Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. American journal of psychiatry, 2001. 158(11): p. 1783–1793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bakhshani N-M, Impulsivity: a predisposition toward risky behaviors. International journal of high risk behaviors & addiction, 2014. 3(2). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Dougherty DM, et al. , Laboratory behavioral measures of impulsivity. Behavior Research Methods, 2005. 37(1): p. 82–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Cyders MA and Coskunpinar A, Measurement of constructs using self-report and behavioral lab tasks: is there overlap in nomothetic span and construct representation for impulsivity? Clin Psychol Rev, 2011. 31(6): p. 965–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Reynolds B, et al. , Laboratory and self-report assessments of impulsive behavior in adolescent daily smokers and nonsmokers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 2007. 15(3): p. 264–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Reynolds B, et al. , Dimensions of impulsive behavior: Personality and behavioral measures. Personality and individual differences, 2006. 40(2): p. 305–315. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Dalley JW, Everitt BJ, and Robbins TW, Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron, 2011. 69(4): p. 680–694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Bari A and Robbins TW, Inhibition and impulsivity: behavioral and neural basis of response control. Progress in neurobiology, 2013. 108: p. 44–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Association AP, Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). 2013: American Psychiatric Pub. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Bevilacqua L and Goldman D, Genetics of impulsive behaviour. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 2013. 368(1615): p. 20120380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Bakhshani NM, Impulsivity: a predisposition toward risky behaviors. Int J High Risk Behav Addict, 2014. 3(2): p. e20428. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Logan GD, Schachar RJ, and Tannock R, Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychological Science, 1997. 8(1): p. 60–64. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Newman JP, Widom CS, and Nathan S, Passive avoidance in syndromes of disinhibition: psychopathy and extraversion. Journal of personality and social psychology, 1985. 48(5): p. 1316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Patton JH and Stanford MS, Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of clinical psychology, 1995. 51(6): p. 768–774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Eysenck SB, et al. , Age norms for impulsiveness, venturesomeness and empathy in adults. Personality and individual differences, 1985. 6(5): p. 613–619. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Patrick CJ, Curtin JJ, and Tellegen A, Development and validation of a brief form of the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Psychological assessment, 2002. 14(2): p. 150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Whiteside SP, et al. , Validation of the UPPS impulsive behaviour scale: a four-factor model of impulsivity. European Journal of Personality, 2005. 19(7): p. 559–574. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Richards JB, et al. , Delay or probability discounting in a model of impulsive behavior: effect of alcohol. Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 1999. 71(2): p. 121–143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Ainslie G, Specious reward: a behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychological bulletin, 1975. 82(4): p. 463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.MacKillop J, et al. , Divergent validity of measures of cognitive distortions, impulsivity, and time perspective in pathological gambling. J Gambl Stud, 2006. 22(3): p. 339–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Bickel WK, Odum AL, and Madden GJ, Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 1999. 146(4): p. 447–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Kirby KN and Petry NM, Heroin and cocaine abusers have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than alcoholics or non-drug-using controls. Addiction, 2004. 99(4): p. 461–471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.MacKillop J, et al. , The latent structure of impulsivity: impulsive choice, impulsive action, and impulsive personality traits. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2016. 233(18): p. 3361–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Jupp B and Dalley JW, Behavioral endophenotypes of drug addiction: Etiological insights from neuroimaging studies. Neuropharmacology, 2014. 76 Pt B: p. 487–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Jupp B and Dalley JW, Convergent pharmacological mechanisms in impulsivity and addiction: insights from rodent models. Br J Pharmacol, 2014. 171(20): p. 4729–66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Giedd JN, The teen brain: insights from neuroimaging. J Adolesc Health, 2008. 42(4): p. 335–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Chester DS, et al. , How do negative emotions impair self-control? A neural model of negative urgency. Neuroimage, 2016. 132: p. 43–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Mitchell MR and Potenza MN, Recent Insights into the Neurobiology of Impulsivity. Curr Addict Rep, 2014. 1(4): p. 309–319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Moreno-Lopez L, et al. , Trait impulsivity and prefrontal gray matter reductions in cocaine dependent individuals. Drug Alcohol Depend, 2012. 125(3): p. 208–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Bechara A and Van Der Linden M, Decision-making and impulse control after frontal lobe injuries. Curr Opin Neurol, 2005. 18(6): p. 734–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Rochat L, et al. , A multifactorial and integrative approach to impulsivity in neuropsychology: insights from the UPPS model of impulsivity. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol, 2018. 40(1): p. 45–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Joseph JE, et al. , Neural correlates of emotional reactivity in sensation seeking. Psychol Sci, 2009. 20(2): p. 215–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Dalley JW and Roiser JP, Dopamine, serotonin and impulsivity. Neuroscience, 2012. 215: p. 42–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Hammond CJ, Mayes LC, and Potenza MN, Neurobiology of adolescent substance use and addictive behaviors: treatment implications. Adolesc Med State Art Rev, 2014. 25(1): p. 15–32. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Schmaal L, et al. , N-acetylcysteine normalizes glutamate levels in cocaine-dependent patients: a randomized crossover magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Neuropsychopharmacology, 2012. 37(9): p. 2143–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Kalivas PW, The glutamate homeostasis hypothesis of addiction. Nat Rev Neurosci, 2009. 10(8): p. 561–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Schmaal L, et al. , The association between cingulate cortex glutamate concentration and delay discounting is mediated by resting state functional connectivity. Brain Behav, 2012. 2(5): p. 553–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Boy F, et al. , Dorsolateral prefrontal gamma-aminobutyric acid in men predicts individual differences in rash impulsivity. Biol Psychiatry, 2011. 70(9): p. 866–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Jupp B, et al. , Dopaminergic and GABA-ergic markers of impulsivity in rats: evidence for anatomical localisation in ventral striatum and prefrontal cortex. Eur J Neurosci, 2013. 37(9): p. 1519–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Gonzalez G, et al. , Clinical efficacy of gabapentin versus tiagabine for reducing cocaine use among cocaine dependent methadone-treated patients. Drug Alcohol Depend, 2007. 87(1): p. 1–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Blum K, et al. , Reward deficiency syndrome: a biogenetic model for the diagnosis and treatment of impulsive, addictive, and compulsive behaviors. J Psychoactive Drugs, 2000. 32 Suppl: p. i–iv, 1–112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.MacKillop J, Integrating behavioral economics and behavioral genetics: delayed reward discounting as an endophenotype for addictive disorders. J Exp Anal Behav, 2013. 99(1): p. 14–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Munafo MR, Matheson IJ, and Flint J, Association of the DRD2 gene Taq1A polymorphism and alcoholism: a meta-analysis of case-control studies and evidence of publication bias. Mol Psychiatry, 2007. 12(5): p. 454–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Eisenberg DT, et al. , Examining impulsivity as an endophenotype using a behavioral approach: a DRD2 TaqI A and DRD4 48-bp VNTR association study. Behav Brain Funct, 2007. 3: p. 2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Jonsson EG, et al. , Polymorphisms in the dopamine D2 receptor gene and their relationships to striatal dopamine receptor density of healthy volunteers. Mol Psychiatry, 1999. 4(3): p. 290–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Mota NR, et al. , Linking dopamine neurotransmission and neurogenesis: The evolutionary history of the NTAD (NCAM1-TTC12-ANKK1-DRD2) gene cluster. Genet Mol Biol, 2012. 35(4 (suppl)): p. 912–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.MacKillop J, et al. , Genetic influences on delay discounting in smokers: examination of a priori candidates and exploration of dopamine-related haplotypes. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2015. 232(20): p. 3731–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Beck A, et al. , Ventral striatal activation during reward anticipation correlates with impulsivity in alcoholics. Biol Psychiatry, 2009. 66(8): p. 734–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Andrews MM, et al. , Individuals family history positive for alcoholism show functional magnetic resonance imaging differences in reward sensitivity that are related to impulsivity factors. Biol Psychiatry, 2011. 69(7): p. 675–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Schneider S, et al. , Risk taking and the adolescent reward system: a potential common link to substance abuse. Am J Psychiatry, 2012. 169(1): p. 39–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Balodis IM, et al. , Diminished frontostriatal activity during processing of monetary rewards and losses in pathological gambling. Biol Psychiatry, 2012. 71(8): p. 749–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Blum K, et al. , The D2 dopamine receptor gene as a determinant of reward deficiency syndrome. J R Soc Med, 1996. 89(7): p. 396–400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Kohno M, et al. , Midbrain functional connectivity and ventral striatal dopamine D2-type receptors: link to impulsivity in methamphetamine users. Mol Psychiatry, 2016. 21(11): p. 1554–1560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Leeman RF and Potenza MN, Similarities and differences between pathological gambling and substance use disorders: a focus on impulsivity and compulsivity. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2012. 219(2): p. 469–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Sofuoglu M and Sewell RA, Norepinephrine and stimulant addiction. Addict Biol, 2009. 14(2): p. 119–29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Ratsma JE, Van Der Stelt O, and Gunning WB, Neurochemical markers of alcoholism vulnerability in humans. Alcohol Alcohol, 2002. 37(6): p. 522–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Schenk S and Aronsen D, Contribution of Impulsivity and Serotonin Receptor Neuroadaptations to the Development of an MDMA (‘Ecstasy’) Substance Use Disorder. Curr Top Behav Neurosci, 2017. 34: p. 17–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Spear LP, The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 2000. 24(4): p. 417–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Zernicke KA, et al. , The association between earlier age of first drink, disinhibited personality, and externalizing psychopathology in young adults. Addict Behav, 2010. 35(5): p. 414–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Ersche KD, et al. , Drug addiction endophenotypes: impulsive versus sensation-seeking personality traits. Biol Psychiatry, 2010. 68(8): p. 770–3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Meda SA, et al. , Investigating the behavioral and self-report constructs of impulsivity domains using principal component analysis. Behav Pharmacol, 2009. 20(5–6): p. 390–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Muller SE, et al. , Personality traits predict treatment outcome in alcohol-dependent patients. Neuropsychobiology, 2008. 57(4): p. 159–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Moeller FG, et al. , Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. Am J Psychiatry, 2001. 158(11): p. 1783–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Vuchinich RE and Simpson CA, Hyperbolic temporal discounting in social drinkers and problem drinkers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 1998. 6(3): p. 292–305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Coffey SF, et al. , Impulsivity and rapid discounting of delayed hypothetical rewards in cocaine-dependent individuals. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 2003. 11(1): p. 18–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Kirby KN and Petry NM, Heroin and cocaine abusers have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than alcoholics or non-drug-using controls. Addiction, 2004. 99(4): p. 461–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Hoffman WF, et al. , Neuropsychological function and delay discounting in methamphetamine-dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2006. 188(2): p. 162–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Johnson MW, et al. , Delay discounting in current and former marijuana-dependent individuals. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 2010. 18(1): p. 99–107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Cangemi S, et al. , Impulsiveness and time perception in alcohol dependent patients in alcoholic rehabilitation treatment. G Ital Med Lav Ergon, 2010. 32(3 Suppl B): p. B23–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Patkar AA, et al. , Pre-treatment measures of impulsivity, aggression and sensation seeking are associated with treatment outcome for African-American cocaine-dependent patients. J Addict Dis, 2004. 23(2): p. 109–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Nielsen DA, et al. , Former heroin addicts with or without a history of cocaine dependence are more impulsive than controls. Drug Alcohol Depend, 2012. 124(1–2): p. 113–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Field M and Cox WM, Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: a review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug Alcohol Depend, 2008. 97(1–2): p. 1–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Gottesman II and Gould TD, The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry, 2003. 160(4): p. 636–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Ivanov I, et al. , Inhibitory control deficits in childhood and risk for substance use disorders: a review. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse, 2008. 34(3): p. 239–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Ortal S, et al. , The Role of Different Aspects of Impulsivity as Independent Risk Factors for Substance Use Disorders in Patients with ADHD: A Review. Curr Drug Abuse Rev, 2015. 8(2): p. 119–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Rogers RD and Robbins TW, Investigating the neurocognitive deficits associated with chronic drug misuse. Curr Opin Neurobiol, 2001. 11(2): p. 250–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Gurevich EV and Joyce JN, Distribution of dopamine D3 receptor expressing neurons in the human forebrain: comparison with D2 receptor expressing neurons. Neuropsychopharmacology, 1999. 20(1): p. 60–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Dom G, et al. , Substance use disorders and the orbitofrontal cortex: systematic review of behavioural decision-making and neuroimaging studies. Br J Psychiatry, 2005. 187: p. 209–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Noel X, et al. , Alcohol cues increase cognitive impulsivity in individuals with alcoholism. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2007. 192(2): p. 291–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Hester R and Garavan H, Executive dysfunction in cocaine addiction: evidence for discordant frontal, cingulate, and cerebellar activity. J Neurosci, 2004. 24(49): p. 11017–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Fillmore MT and Rush CR, Impaired inhibitory control of behavior in chronic cocaine users. Drug Alcohol Depend, 2002. 66(3): p. 265–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Monterosso JR, et al. , Deficits in response inhibition associated with chronic methamphetamine abuse. Drug Alcohol Depend, 2005. 79(2): p. 273–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Berg JM, et al. , Parsing the heterogeneity of impulsivity: A meta-analytic review of the behavioral implications of the UPPS for psychopathology. Psychol Assess, 2015. 27(4): p. 1129–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Perry JL, Nelson SE, and Carroll ME, Impulsive choice as a predictor of acquisition of IV cocaine self- administration and reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior in male and female rats. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 2008. 16(2): p. 165–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.de Wit H and Richards JB, Dual determinants of drug use in humans: reward and impulsivity. Nebr Symp Motiv, 2004. 50: p. 19–55. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Chambers RA, Taylor JR, and Potenza MN, Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: a critical period of addiction vulnerability. Am J Psychiatry, 2003. 160(6): p. 1041–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Perry JL, et al. , Impulsivity (delay discounting) as a predictor of acquisition of IV cocaine self-administration in female rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2005. 178(2–3): p. 193–201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Belin D and Deroche-Gamonet V, Responses to novelty and vulnerability to cocaine addiction: contribution of a multi-symptomatic animal model. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med, 2012. 2(11). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Field M, et al. , Delay discounting and the behavioural economics of cigarette purchases in smokers: the effects of nicotine deprivation. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2006. 186(2): p. 255–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Wing VC, et al. , Effects of cigarette smoking status on delay discounting in schizophrenia and healthy controls. Addict Behav, 2012. 37(1): p. 67–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Loree AM, Lundahl LH, and Ledgerwood DM, Impulsivity as a predictor of treatment outcome in substance use disorders: review and synthesis. Drug Alcohol Rev, 2015. 34(2): p. 119–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Evren C, et al. , Relationship of relapse with impulsivity, novelty seeking and craving in male alcohol-dependent inpatients. Drug Alcohol Rev, 2012. 31(1): p. 81–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Roozen HG, et al. , The impact of craving and impulsivity on aggression in detoxified cocaine-dependent patients. J Subst Abuse Treat, 2011. 40(4): p. 414–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Moeller FG, et al. , The impact of impulsivity on cocaine use and retention in treatment. J Subst Abuse Treat, 2001. 21(4): p. 193–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Bechara A, Risky business: emotion, decision-making, and addiction. J Gambl Stud, 2003. 19(1): p. 23–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Cowan RL, et al. , Reduced cortical gray matter density in human MDMA (Ecstasy) users: a voxel-based morphometry study. Drug Alcohol Depend, 2003. 72(3): p. 225–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Chanraud S, et al. , Brain morphometry and cognitive performance in detoxified alcohol-dependents with preserved psychosocial functioning. Neuropsychopharmacology, 2007. 32(2): p. 429–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Lyoo IK, et al. , Prefrontal and temporal gray matter density decreases in opiate dependence. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2006. 184(2): p. 139–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Garavan H, Kaufman JN, and Hester R, Acute effects of cocaine on the neurobiology of cognitive control. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 2008. 363(1507): p. 3267–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Jentsch JD, et al. , Impairments of reversal learning and response perseveration after repeated, intermittent cocaine administrations to monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacology, 2002. 26(2): p. 183–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Ricaurte GA, Yuan J, and McCann UD, (+/−)3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (‘Ecstasy’)-induced serotonin neurotoxicity: studies in animals. Neuropsychobiology, 2000. 42(1): p. 5–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Lynch WJ, Sex differences in vulnerability to drug self-administration. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 2006. 14(1): p. 34–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Roth ME, Cosgrove KP, and Carroll ME, Sex differences in the vulnerability to drug abuse: a review of preclinical studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 2004. 28(6): p. 533–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Carroll ME, et al. , Acquisition of oral phencyclidine self-administration in rhesus monkeys: effect of sex. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2000. 149(4): p. 401–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Roth ME and Carroll ME, Sex differences in the escalation of intravenous cocaine intake following long- or short-access to cocaine self-administration. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 2004. 78(2): p. 199–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Lynch WJ, Arizzi MN, and Carroll ME, Effects of sex and the estrous cycle on regulation of intravenously self-administered cocaine in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2000. 152(2): p. 132–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Lynch WJ and Carroll ME, Reinstatement of cocaine self-administration in rats: sex differences. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2000. 148(2): p. 196–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Fillmore MT and Weafer J, Alcohol impairment of behavior in men and women. Addiction, 2004. 99(10): p. 1237–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Wallace CJ, The effects of delayed rewards, social pressure, and frustration on the responses of opiate addicts. NIDA Res Monogr, 1979(25): p. 6–25. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Kirby KN and Marakovic NN, Delay-discounting probabilistic rewards: Rates decrease as amounts increase. Psychon Bull Rev, 1996. 3(1): p. 100–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Jackson LR, Robinson TE, and Becker JB, Sex differences and hormonal influences on acquisition of cocaine self-administration in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology, 2006. 31(1): p. 129–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Larson EB, et al. , Effects of estrogen and progesterone on the escalation of cocaine self-administration in female rats during extended access. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 2007. 15(5): p. 461–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Larson EB, et al. , Effect of short- vs. long-term estrogen on reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior in female rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 2005. 82(1): p. 98–108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Takahashi T, et al. , Testosterone levels and discounting delayed monetary gains and losses in male humans. Neuro Endocrinol Lett, 2006. 27(4): p. 439–44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Svensson AI, Soderpalm B, and Engel JA, Gonadectomy enhances shock-induced behavioral inhibition in adult male rats: implications for impulsive behavior. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 2000. 65(4): p. 731–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Spear NE and Molina JC, Fetal or infantile exposure to ethanol promotes ethanol ingestion in adolescence and adulthood: a theoretical review. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 2005. 29(6): p. 909–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Olmstead MC, Animal models of drug addiction: Where do we go from here? Q J Exp Psychol (Hove), 2006. 59(4): p. 625–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Wang FL, et al. , Testing the Relations Among Family Disorganization, Delay Discounting, and Adolescent Alcohol Use: A Genetically Informed Study. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 2016. 40(4): p. 846–56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Van Eck K, Markle RS, and Flory K, Do conduct problems and sensation seeking moderate the association between ADHD and three types of stimulant use in a college population? Psychol Addict Behav, 2012. 26(4): p. 939–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Brooks SJ, et al. , The impact of cognitive training in substance use disorder: the effect of working memory training on impulse control in methamphetamine users. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2017. 234(12): p. 1911–1921. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Houben K, Wiers RW, and Jansen A, Getting a grip on drinking behavior: training working memory to reduce alcohol abuse. Psychol Sci, 2011. 22(7): p. 968–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Grant JE and Potenza MN (Eds), The Oxford handbook of impulse control disorders. 2011: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 147.Grant JE, Kim SW, and Odlaug BL, N-acetyl cysteine, a glutamate-modulating agent, in the treatment of pathological gambling: a pilot study. Biol Psychiatry, 2007. 62(6): p. 652–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Potenza MN, Biological contributions to addictions in adolescents and adults: prevention, treatment, and policy implications. J Adolesc Health, 2013. 52(2 Suppl 2): p. S22–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Blondel A, Sanger DJ, and Moser PC, Characterisation of the effects of nicotine in the five-choice serial reaction time task in rats: antagonist studies. Psychopharmacology, 2000. 149(3): p. 293–305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Kolokotroni KZ, Rodgers RJ, and Harrison AA, Acute nicotine increases both impulsive choice and behavioural disinhibition in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2011. 217(4): p. 455–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Tsutsui-Kimura I, et al. , Endogenous acetylcholine modulates impulsive action via alpha4beta2 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in rats. Eur J Pharmacol, 2010. 641(2–3): p. 148–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Xie X, et al. , Role of nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in the effects of cocaine-paired contextual stimuli on impulsive decision making in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2012. 223(3): p. 271–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Watkins SS, et al. , Blockade of nicotine self-administration with nicotinic antagonists in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 1999. 62(4): p. 743–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Liu X, et al. , Mecamylamine attenuates cue-induced reinstatement of nicotine-seeking behavior in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology, 2007. 32(3): p. 710–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Hammerness P, et al. , Do stimulants reduce the risk for cigarette smoking in youth with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder? A prospective, long-term, open-label study of extended-release methylphenidate. J Pediatr, 2013. 162(1): p. 22–7.e2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Covey LS, et al. , An exploration of site effects in a multisite trial of OROS-methylphenidate for smokers with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse, 2011. 37(5): p. 392–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157.Batra A, et al. , Multidimensional smoker profiles and their prediction of smoking following a pharmacobehavioral intervention. J Subst Abuse Treat, 2008. 35(1): p. 41–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Bentzley JP, Tomko RL, and Gray KM, Low Pretreatment Impulsivity and High Medication Adherence Increase the Odds of Abstinence in a Trial of N-Acetylcysteine in Adolescents with Cannabis Use Disorder. Journal of substance abuse treatment, 2016. 63: p. 72–77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Wiskerke J, et al. , On the Role of Cannabinoid CB1- and mu-Opioid Receptors in Motor Impulsivity. Front Pharmacol, 2012. 3: p. 108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160.Pattij T, et al. , Effects of the cannabinoid CB1 receptor antagonist rimonabant on distinct measures of impulsive behavior in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2007. 193(1): p. 85–96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161.Maldonado R, Robledo P, and Berrendero F, Endocannabinoid system and drug addiction: new insights from mutant mice approaches. Curr Opin Neurobiol, 2013. 23(4): p. 480–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162.Kravitz HM, et al. , Treatment attrition among alcohol-dependent men: is it related to novelty seeking personality traits? J Clin Psychopharmacol, 1999. 19(1): p. 51–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163.Zorlu N, et al. , One Week of Naltrexone Treatment Does Not Reduce Impulsivity During Inpatient Treatment of Alcohol-dependent Patients: An Open-label Study. Addictive Disorders & Their Treatment, 2016. 15(1): p. 25–33. [Google Scholar]
- 164.Joos L, et al. , Effect of modafinil on impulsivity and relapse in alcohol dependent patients: A randomized, placebo-controlled trial. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 2013. 23(8): p. 948–955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 165.Anton RF, et al. , Aripiprazole Suppression of Drinking in a Clinical Laboratory Paradigm: Influence of Impulsivity and Self-Control. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 2017. 41(7): p. 1370–1380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166.Rubio G, Martinez-Gras I, and Manzanares J, Modulation of impulsivity by topiramate: implications for the treatment of alcohol dependence. J Clin Psychopharmacol, 2009. 29(6): p. 584–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 167.Blondeau C and Dellu-Hagedorn F, Dimensional analysis of ADHD subtypes in rats. Biol Psychiatry, 2007. 61(12): p. 1340–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 168.Navarra R, et al. , Effects of atomoxetine and methylphenidate on attention and impulsivity in the 5-choice serial reaction time test. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry, 2008. 32(1): p. 34–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 169.Robinson ES, et al. , Similar effects of the selective noradrenaline reuptake inhibitor atomoxetine on three distinct forms of impulsivity in the rat. Neuropsychopharmacology, 2008. 33(5): p. 1028–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 170.Tsutsui-Kimura I, et al. , The effects of serotonin and/or noradrenaline reuptake inhibitors on impulsive-like action assessed by the three-choice serial reaction time task: a simple and valid model of impulsive action using rats. Behav Pharmacol, 2009. 20(5–6): p. 474–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 171.Fernando AB, et al. , Modulation of high impulsivity and attentional performance in rats by selective direct and indirect dopaminergic and noradrenergic receptor agonists. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 2012. 219(2): p. 341–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 172.Economidou D, Dalley JW, and Everitt BJ, Selective norepinephrine reuptake inhibition by atomoxetine prevents cue-induced heroin and cocaine seeking. Biol Psychiatry, 2011. 69(3): p. 266–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 173.Economidou D, et al. , High impulsivity predicts relapse to cocaine-seeking after punishment-induced abstinence. Biol Psychiatry, 2009. 65(10): p. 851–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 174.Schmitz JM, et al. , Baseline neurocognitive profiles differentiate abstainers and non-abstainers in a cocaine clinical trial. Journal of addictive diseases, 2009. 28(3): p. 250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 175.Mascha N, et al. , Impulsivity and attentional bias as predictors of modafinil treatment outcome for retention and drug use in crack-cocaine dependent patients: Results of a randomised controlled trial. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 2016. 30(7): p. 616–626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 176.Reed SC and Evans SM, The effects of oral d-amphetamine on impulsivity in smoked and intranasal cocaine users. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 2016. 163: p. 141–152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 177.Corrigall WA and Coen KM, Opiate antagonists reduce cocaine but not nicotine self-administration. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 1991. 104(2): p. 167–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 178.Ciccocioppo R, Martin-Fardon R, and Weiss F, Effect of selective blockade of mu(1) or delta opioid receptors on reinstatement of alcohol-seeking behavior by drug-associated stimuli in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology, 2002. 27(3): p. 391–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 179.Kiyatkin EA and Brown PL, Naloxone depresses cocaine self-administration and delays its initiation on the following day. Neuroreport, 2003. 14(2): p. 251–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 180.Spano MS, et al. , CB1 receptor agonist and heroin, but not cocaine, reinstate cannabinoid-seeking behaviour in the rat. Br J Pharmacol, 2004. 143(3): p. 343–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 181.Blanco C, et al. , A pilot study of impulsivity and compulsivity in pathological gambling. Psychiatry research, 2009. 167(1–2): p. 161–168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 182.Helstrom A, Hutchison K, and Bryan A, Motivational enhancement therapy for high-risk adolescent smokers. Addict Behav, 2007. 32(10): p. 2404–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 183.Morean ME, et al. , Contingency management improves smoking cessation treatment outcomes among highly impulsive adolescent smokers relative to cognitive behavioral therapy. Addict Behav, 2015. 42: p. 86–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 184.Peters EN, et al. , Delay Discounting in Adults Receiving Treatment for Marijuana Dependence. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 2013. 21(1): p. 46–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 185.Mahu IT, et al. , Can cannabis use be prevented by targeting personality risk in schools? Twenty-four-month outcome of the adventure trial on cannabis use: a cluster-randomized controlled trial. Addiction, 2015. 110(10): p. 1625–1633. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 186.Feldstein Ewing SW, et al. , Do genetic and individual risk factors moderate the efficacy of motivational enhancement therapy? Drinking outcomes with an emerging adult sample. Addict Biol, 2009. 14(3): p. 356–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 187.Vinci C, et al. , Effects of a brief mindfulness intervention on negative affect and urge to drink among college student drinkers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 2014. 59: p. 82–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 188.Black AC and Rosen MI, A money management-based substance use treatment increases valuation of future rewards. Addict Behav, 2011. 36(1–2): p. 125–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 189.Washio Y, et al. , Delay discounting is associated with treatment response among cocaine-dependent outpatients. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 2011. 19(3): p. 243–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 190.Helmus TC, et al. , Novelty seeking as a predictor of treatment retention for heroin dependent cocaine users. Drug Alcohol Depend, 2001. 61(3): p. 287–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 191.Landes RD, Christensen DR, and Bickel WK, Delay discounting decreases in those completing treatment for opioid dependence. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 2012. 20(4): p. 302–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 192.Margolin A, et al. , A preliminary study of spiritual self-schema (3-S+) therapy for reducing impulsivity in HIV-positive drug users. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 2007. 63(10): p. 979–999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 193.Cho SS, et al. , Continuous theta burst stimulation of right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex induces changes in impulsivity level. Brain Stimulation: Basic, Translational, and Clinical Research in Neuromodulation, 2010. 3(3): p. 170–176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 194.Jacobson L, Javitt DC, and Lavidor M, Activation of inhibition: diminishing impulsive behavior by direct current stimulation over the inferior frontal gyrus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2011. 23(11): p. 3380–3387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 195.Stramaccia DF, et al. , Assessing the effects of tDCS over a delayed response inhibition task by targeting the right inferior frontal gyrus and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Experimental brain research, 2015. 233(8): p. 2283–2290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 196.Fecteau S, et al. , Activation of prefrontal cortex by transcranial direct current stimulation reduces appetite for risk during ambiguous decision making. Journal of Neuroscience, 2007. 27(23): p. 6212–6218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 197.Fecteau S, et al. , Diminishing risk-taking behavior by modulating activity in the prefrontal cortex: a direct current stimulation study. Journal of Neuroscience, 2007. 27(46): p. 12500–12505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 198.Shen B, et al. , High-definition tDCS alters impulsivity in a baseline-dependent manner. Neuroimage, 2016. 143: p. 343–352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 199.Beeli G, et al. , Modulating presence and impulsiveness by external stimulation of the brain. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 2008. 4(1): p. 33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 200.Hecht D, Walsh V, and Lavidor M, Bi-frontal direct current stimulation affects delay discounting choices. Cognitive neuroscience, 2013. 4(1): p. 7–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 201.Ficarella SC and Battelli L, The critical role of the dorsal fronto-median cortex in voluntary action inhibition: A TMS study. Brain Stimulation: Basic, Translational, and Clinical Research in Neuromodulation, 2017. 10(3): p. 596–603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 202.Chambers CD, et al. , Executive “brake failure” following deactivation of human frontal lobe. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 2006. 18(3): p. 444–455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 203.Knoch D, et al. , Disruption of right prefrontal cortex by low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation induces risk-taking behavior. Journal of Neuroscience, 2006. 26(24): p. 6469–6472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 204.Figner B, et al. , Lateral prefrontal cortex and self-control in intertemporal choice. Nature neuroscience, 2010. 13(5): p. 538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 205.Politi E, et al. , Daily sessions of transcranial magnetic stimulation to the left prefrontal cortex gradually reduce cocaine craving. American Journal on Addictions, 2008. 17(4): p. 345–346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 206.Gorini A, et al. , Modulation of risky choices in recently abstinent dependent cocaine users: a transcranial direct-current stimulation study. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 2014. 8: p. 661. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 207.Sheffer CE, et al. , Neuromodulation of delay discounting, the reflection effect, and cigarette consumption. Journal of substance abuse treatment, 2013. 45(2): p. 206–214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 208.Fecteau S, et al. , Modulation of smoking and decision-making behaviors with transcranial direct current stimulation in tobacco smokers: a preliminary study. Drug & Alcohol Dependence, 2014. 140: p. 78–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 209.Boggio PS, et al. , Modulation of risk-taking in marijuana users by transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). Drug & Alcohol Dependence, 2010. 112(3): p. 220–225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 210.Zack M, et al. , Effects of high frequency repeated transcranial magnetic stimulation and continuous theta burst stimulation on gambling reinforcement, delay discounting, and stroop interference in men with pathological gambling. Brain Stimulation: Basic, Translational, and Clinical Research in Neuromodulation, 2016. 9(6): p. 867–875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 211.Coles AS, Kozak K, and George TP, A review of brain stimulation methods to treat substance use disorders. Am J Addict, 2018. 27(2): p. 71–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 212.Crews FT and Boettiger CA, Impulsivity, frontal lobes and risk for addiction. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 2009. 93(3): p. 237–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 213.Kozak K, Barr MS, and George TP, Traits and Biomarkers for Addiction Risk in Schizophrenia. Curr Addict Rep, 2017: p. 1–11.28357191 [Google Scholar]
- 214.Heyman GM and Gibb SP, Delay discounting in college cigarette chippers. Behav Pharmacol, 2006. 17(8): p. 669–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 215.Eysenck SB and Eysenck HJ, Impulsiveness and venturesomeness: their position in a dimensional system of personality description. Psychol Rep, 1978. 43(3 Pt 2): p. 1247–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 216.Cloninger CR, et al. , The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A guide to its development and use. 1994: p. 19–28. [Google Scholar]
- 217.Cain NM, Lukowitsky MR, and Wright AG, Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. The Encyclopedia of Clinical Psychology. [Google Scholar]
- 218.Bechara A, et al. , Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 1994. 50(1–3): p. 7–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 219.Clark L, et al. , The contributions of lesion laterality and lesion volume to decision-making impairment following frontal lobe damage. Neuropsychologia, 2003. 41(11): p. 1474–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 220.Reynolds B and Schiffbauer R, Measuring state changes in human delay discounting: an experiential discounting task. Behav Processes, 2004. 67(3): p. 343–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 221.Lejuez CW, et al. , Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2002. 8(2): p. 75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 222.Howieson DB, Lezak MD, and Loring DW, Orientation and attention: Neuropsychological assessment. 2004: Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]