Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Apr 9.
Published in final edited form as: Cogn Neurosci. 2012;3(3-4):250–259. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2012.697054

Repetition priming and repetition suppression: Multiple mechanisms in need of testing

Stephen J Gotts 1, Carson C Chow 2, Alex Martin 1
PMCID: PMC6454549  NIHMSID: NIHMS1011554  PMID: 24171755

Abstract

In our Discussion Paper, we reviewed four theoretical proposals that have the potential to link the neural and behavioral phenomena of Repetition Suppression and Repetition Priming. We argued that among these proposals, the Synchrony and Bayesian Explaining Away models appear to be the most promising in addressing existing data, and we articulated a series of predictions to distinguish between them. The commentaries have helped to clarify some of these predictions, have highlighted additional evidence supporting the Facilitation and Sharpening models, and have emphasized dissociations by repetition lag and brain location. Our reply addresses these issues in turn, and we argue that progress will require the testing of Repetition Suppression, changes in neural tuning, and changes in synchronization throughout the brain and over a variety of lags and task contexts.

Keywords: Repetition priming, Repetition suppression, Synchrony, Prediction, Bayesian

BAYESIAN EXPLAINING AWAY MODEL

Both Friston’s and Henson’s commentaries make the point that the Facilitation, Sharpening, Synchrony, and Bayesian Explaining Away models are not mutually exclusive. This is a point that we failed to clarify and that we fully endorse. The ideas are certainly mechanistically distinct, but they could all coexist with one another simultaneously, perhaps making separate contributions in explaining repetition priming. Efforts should be focused on assessing the contribution of any/all (none?) of these in a given experimental situation.

Friston’s commentary clarifies his positions on the experimental predictions that we articulated. He re--emphasizes his commitment to anti-symmetrical bottom-up and top-down interactions, while he is less enthusiastic about the relative timing predictions. Between-region anti-symmetry is the central claim of this model. It predicts that top-down causal interactions should be more negative after stimulus repetition and that repetition suppression in lower-level areas should be due to feedback from higher-level areas. Friston also stresses the presence of repetition-dependent changes in the feed-forward direction with stimulus repetition, although with an inverse valence to the feedback effects (implementing a negative feedback loop). We view these clarifications as quite reasonable but differ with Friston on other aspects of his argument. Friston claims that near-optimal perceptual inference lends support to a Bayesian brain hypothesis in which top-down/bottom-up interactions are anti-symmetrical. We believe that it is difficult in principle and practice to distinguish between near-optimal and satisfactory inference given a set of stimuli to be identified and tasks to be performed. Many neural network models demonstrate good performance over a range of learning problems. For example, the Boltzmann machine (e.g., Ackley, Hinton, & Sejnowski, 1985) utilizes a “contrastive” Hebbian algorithm to modify synaptic strengths as the model is exposed to a set of patterns to be associated. The learning algorithm, often heralded for its “biologically plausibility” (e.g., O’Reilly, 1998), leads this model to improve gradually with experience, develop similarity-based internal representations, and perform “linearly inseparable” mappings such as the XOR problem (e.g., Minsky & Papert, 1969). It does all of this while developing symmetrical weights between units in higher- and lower-level pools of units. Influential models such as Adaptive Resonance Theory (e.g., Grossberg, 1976), the Interactive Activation Model (McClelland & Rumelhart, 1981), and the Biased Competition Model (Desimone & Duncan, 1995) all predict a symmetrical coding scheme. These models exploit the flexible advantages of top-down, selective excitation in domains ranging from perception to working memory, visual attention/search, and imagery. It will be interesting to see if the Bayesian brain hypothesis can be extended into these domains using a more anti-symmetrical scheme. We would also note that attempts to test the anti-symmetrical property of Mumford’s (1992) Bayesian theory in single-cell recordings with monkeys have found support for feedback excitation rather than feedback inhibition (e.g., Lee & Mumford, 2003). This is not necessarily problematic for Friston’s proposal, because the cells encoding the conditional expectation of perceptual causes are distinct from those encoding prediction error. Nevertheless, we do not believe that it is self-evident that the brain behaves in its details as a Bayesian neural network model, at least one that relies on anti-symmetrical coupling in the feed-forward versus feedback directions.

Ewbank and Henson appear to take issue with our use of the label “Explaining Away” when referring to Friston’s Bayesian model, preferring instead “Predictive Coding”. Our rationale was simply to use a label that better distinguished the anti-symmetrical property in this model from the variety of models that utilize “prediction” in very different ways (e.g., Elman nets, Temporal Difference learning, forward models, etc.). Ewbank and Henson emphasize the difficulty in testing subtle predictions about brain connectivity using fMRI methods when the separate contributions of different cell types to the BOLD signal are unknown. We certainly agree that local estimates of the BOLD response in a given voxel will reflect an unknown mixture of various influences (a small fraction of which are neural). However, given the importance of the anti-symmetrical property to the Bayesian model articulated above, we think that it would be unwise to dispatch with this prediction prematurely. Causal modeling approaches that are capable of assessing directional influences between anatomically connected cortical regions (e.g., DCM, Grainger, etc.) should detect net inhibitory coupling in the feedback direction—even when local activity represents an average over different cell types that are present in unknown proportions. If the feedback is net excitatory, what would serve as the basis of repetition suppression? If the problem is the ability of causal modeling approaches to infer directional influences appropriately among interrelated variables, then this problem will apply in a similar manner to the analyses of experiments using alternative methods such as EEG/MEG (e.g., Kiebel, Garrido, Moran, & Friston, 2008). However, we agree that EEG/MEG studies of inter-areal interactions constitute a promising direction for future research.

FACILITATION AND SHARPENING MODELS: THE SHORT AND LONG OF IT

In his separate commentary, Henson makes the case that it is too soon to dismiss the Facilitation model. While he admits that supporting evidence from single-cell recordings has been lacking, he raises the possibility that accelerated neural responses may be commonplace in EEG/MEG. We concur with him about the basic puzzle: How is it that electrical/magnetic field data can become decoupled from spike data? This decoupling extends even to the basic latency of stimulus-evoked responses in microelectrode recordings from occipital areas in animals (firing-rate latencies ranging from 30–50ms, whereas field measurements often show onsets closer to 70–100ms). Our best guess for a resolution is that it involves some form of field cancellation of the earliest responses. In any case, accelerated responses at the single-cell level should be obtainable if the Facilitation model is to hold. Having said that, a very recent study (since the submission of our paper) has provided some more direct support for the Facilitation model, as well as the Sharpening model, in recordings from monkey inferior temporal cortex (Woloszyn & Sheinberg, 2012). This study involved an extensive training period of several months (like other studies providing support for Sharpening), but it still suggests that Facilitation may apply in some cases. We are therefore happy to concede the point to Henson that it is too early to dismiss the Facilitation model.

The Riesenhuber, Weiner and Grill-Spector, and McMahon commentaries all mention the issue of how repetition lag (short versus long) relates to the observation of proportional scaling versus Sharpening. Riesenhuber makes the case that evidence on long-lag repetitions and Sharpening is not mixed but paints a consistent picture, with proportional scaling effects limited to lags typically involved in fMRI rapid adaptation paradigms (repetitions separated by a few seconds). We agree that results from experiments employing very long lags (and/or practice durations) versus very short lags have been reliably associated with Sharpening and scaling, respectively (see also McMahon’s commentary). However, results for lags of an intermediate range (minutes or longer within a single testing session) do not fit cleanly into this picture. For example, Li et al. (1993) showed independent effects of short- and long-lag repetitions on single-cell firing rates in monkey inferior temporal cortex, with proportional scaling observed for long-lag repetitions (~ tens of minutes). As noted by Weiner and Grill-Spector, Weiner et al. (2010) found results in human fMRI for long lags that were consistent with proportional scaling in all but one of the regions that they examined (medial ventral temporal cortex). Given that these more intermediate lags are the ones involved in most repetition priming studies, the evidence supporting the involvement of Sharpening in repetition priming does indeed appear to be mixed. Even if we were to grant a larger role to Sharpening at these lags, additional assumptions would still be required to explain Repetition Priming. We concur with McMahon that the Synchrony model is well situated to explain priming at the shorter lags that tend to produce scaling, and it may participate at longer lags (and/or practice durations) as well.

DIFFERENT LOCATIONS DO NOT NECESSARILY IMPLY DIFFERENT MECHANISMS

The Weiner and Grill-Spector, Horner, and Wig commentaries all highlight the fact that studies of Repetition Suppression often report findings that vary by brain location. Weiner and Grill-Spector note the challenges facing the Synchrony model in explaining the region- and lag-dependent nature of Repetition Suppression in occipital and temporal brain regions. While no model can currently explain this range of data, we agree that this should be the goal. We would note that while synchrony is a mechanism at one level of description, it is also an emergent phenomenon with multiple possible underlying mechanisms that can apply differentially at different lags and potentially in different brain regions (e.g., spike-frequency adaptation and synaptic depression, electrical synapses between interneurons, spike-timing-dependent plasticity, etc.). Our current experimental focus is simply to detect whether synchronization is occurring in the appropriate experimental contexts and whether it is quantitatively related to the magnitude of repetition priming. In his commentary, Horner rightly makes the point that Repetition Suppression is most strongly related to priming in the prefrontal cortex and that this central issue should not be lost in the discussion. Wig counters, appropriately in our view, that just because occipital Repetition Suppression is more weakly related to repetition priming in certain tasks does not imply that it is irrelevant to priming magnitudes in all tasks. Would a task that emphasizes information represented in occipital areas (e.g., fine shape discriminations) yield a stronger association between occipital Repetition Suppression and priming (see also Martin & Gotts, 2005)? More generally, we would argue that Repetition Suppression effects that are dissociable by brain region or task do not necessarily imply qualitatively distinct lower-level mechanisms. Future experiments will need to clarify the region-and lag-dependence of Repetition Suppression, changes in neural tuning properties, as well as changes in Synchrony. One issue raised by Weiner and Grill-Spector that we would dispute is the exclusive role of high versus low frequency oscillations in local versus long-distance cortical interactions, respectively. Modulations of local synchrony can be in lower frequencies (theta, alpha, beta: E.g., Anderson et al., 2008; Gilbert et al., 2010; Gregoriou, Gotts, & Desimone, 2012) and modulations of long-distance synchrony can be in higher frequencies (gamma: E.g., Buschman & Miller, 2007; Gregoriou et al., 2009a).

NEGATIVE PRIMING AND OTHER PARADIGMS

In the final commentary, Dyson and Alain argue that our proposal has failed to consider the influences of task, time, and context on repetition priming. They cite evidence from EEG/ERP studies in the auditory modality, noting conflicting evidence from negative priming, perceptual learning of speech tokens, and sensory gating. Some of the differences with our literature review may involve genuine differences between visual and auditory modalities. However, we would reiterate the difficulty of using scalp EEG/ERP measurements to rule out a proposal cast at the level of underlying neural sources. Too many ambiguities are present. Results from paradigms such as negative priming that involve multiple simultaneous stimuli and additional processes (selective attention) may also not be directly comparable to simple identification paradigms with sequentially presented single stimuli.

The Commentaries offered in response to our Discussion Paper highlight the importance and interest in uncovering the mechanism(s) linking Repetition Suppression to one of nature’s most powerful learning phenomena, Priming. We again thank our colleagues for their thoughtful and thought-provoking comments on our proposal.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the authors for their commentaries and excellent feedback on our Discussion Paper. A number of important clarifications and issues have been raised, to which we provide some brief responses.

The preparation of this paper was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health, NIH, Division of Intramural Research.

Footnotes

This work was authored as part of the Contributor’s official duties as an Employee of the United States Government and is therefore a work of the United States Government. In accordance with 17 U.S.C. 105, no copyright protection is available for such works under U.S. Law.

References from the Discussion Paper, the Commentaries, and the Reply

  1. Abbott LF, Varela JA, Sen K, & Nelson SB (1997). Synaptic depression and cortical gain control. Science, 275, 220–224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ackley DH, Hinton GE, & Sejnowski TJ (1985). A learning algorithm for Boltzmann machines. Cognitive Science, 9, 147–169. [Google Scholar]
  3. Ahmed B, Allison JD, Douglas RJ, Martin KAC, & Whitteridge D (1998). Estimates of the net excitatory currents evoked by visual stimulation of identified neurons in cat visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 8, 462–476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Aiello LC, & Wheeler P (1995). The expensive tissue hypothesis: The brain and digestive system in human and primate evolution. Current Anthropology, 36, 199–221. [Google Scholar]
  5. Alain C, Campeanu S, & Tremblay K (2009). Changes in sensory evoked responses coincide with rapid improvement in speech identification performance. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 392–403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Allman JM (1990). The origin of the neocortex. Seminars in the Neurosciences, 2, 257–262. [Google Scholar]
  7. Anderson B, Mruczek RE, Kawasaki K, & Sheinberg D (2008). Effects of familiarity on neural activity in monkey inferior temporal lobe. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 2540–2552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Anderson JR (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  9. Andresen DR, Vinberg J, & Grill-Spector K (2009). The representation of object viewpoint in human visual cortex. NeuroImage, 45, 522–536. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Baker CI, Behrmann M, & Olson CR (2002). Impact of learning on representation of parts and wholes in monkey inferotemporal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 5(11), 1210–1216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bandettini PA, & Cox RW (2000). Event-related fMRI contrast when using constant interstimulus interval: Theory and experiment. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 43, 540–548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Barton JJS, Fox CJ, Sekunova A, & Iaria G (2010). Encoding in the visual word form area: An fMRI adaptation study of words versus handwriting. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(8), 1649–1661. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21286 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bastos AM, Usrey WM, Adams RA, Mangun GR, Fries P, & Friston KJ (under review). Canonical microcircuits for predictive coding. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  14. Bazhenov M, Stopfer M, Sejnowski TJ, & Laurent G (2005). Fast odor learning improves reliability of odor responses in the locust antennal lobe. Neuron, 46, 483–492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Becker S, Moscovitch M, Behrmann M, & Joordens S (1997). Long-term semantic priming: A computational account and empirical evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1059–1082. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bedny M, McGill M, & Thompson-Schill SL (2008). Semantic adaptation and competition during word comprehension. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 2574–2585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Ben-David BM, Campeanu S, Tremblay K, & Alain C (2011). Auditory evoked potentials dissociate rapid perceptual learning from task repetition without learning. Psychophysiology, 48, 797–807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bentin S, & Peled BS (1990). The contribution of task-related factors to ERP repetition effects at short and long lags. Memory & Cognition, 18, 359–366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Bi GQ, & Poo MM (1998). Synaptic modifications in cultured hippocampal neurons: Dependence on spike timing, synaptic strength, and postsynaptic cell type. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 10464–10472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Biederman I, & Cooper EE (1991). Evidence for complete translational and reflectional invariance in visual object recognition. Perception, 20, 585–593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Biederman I, & Cooper EE (1992). Size invariance in visual object priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 121–133. [Google Scholar]
  22. Boynton GM, & Finney EM, (2003). Orientation-specific adaptation in human visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 8781–8787. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Buffalo EA, Fries P, Landman R, Buschman TJ, & Desimone R (2011). Laminar differences in gamma and alpha coherence in the ventral stream. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108 (27), 11262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Buschman TJ, & Miller EK (2007). Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science, 315, 1860–1862. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Canolty RT, Edwards E, Dalal SS, Soltani M, Nagarajan SS, Kirsch HE, et al. (2006). High gamma power is phase-locked to theta oscillations in human neocortex. Science, 313, 1626–1628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Cant JS, Large ME, McCall L, & Goodale MA (2008). Independent processing of form, colour, and texture in object perception. Perception, 37, 57–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Cardin V, Friston KJ, & Zeki S (2011). Top-down modulations in the visual form pathway revealed with dynamic causal modeling. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 550–562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Cave CB (1997). Very long-lasting priming in picture naming. Psychological Science, 8, 322–325. [Google Scholar]
  29. Cave CB, Bost PR, & Cobb RE (1996). Effects of color and pattern on implicit and explicit picture memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 639–653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Cave CB, & Squire LR (1992). Intact and long lasting repetition priming in amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 509–520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Chawla D, Lumer ED, & Friston KJ (1999). The relationship between synchronization among neuronal populations and their mean activity levels. Neural Computation, 11(6), 1389–411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Cheyne D, Bostan AC, Gaetz W, & Pang EW (2007). Event-related beamforming: A robust method for presurgical functional mapping using MEG. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118, 1691–1704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Collins AM, & Loftus EF (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428. [Google Scholar]
  34. Constanti A, & Sim JA (1987). Calcium-dependent potassium conductance in guinea-pig olfactory cortex neurones invitro. Journal of Physiology (London), 387, 173–194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Dayan P, Hinton GE, Neal RM, & Zemel RS (1995). The Helmholtz machine. Neural Computation, 7, 889–904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. D’Esposito M, Zarahn E, & Aguirre GK (1999). Event-related functional MRI: Implications for cognitive psychology? Psychological Bulletin, 125, 155–164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. De Baene W, Ons B, Wagemans J, & Vogels R (2008). Effects of category learning on the stimulus selectivity of macaque inferior temporal neurons. Learning and Memory, 15(9), 717–727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. De Baene W, & Vogels R (2010). Effects of adaptation on the stimulus selectivity of macaque inferior temporal spiking activity and local field potentials. Cerebral Cortex, 20(9), 2145–2165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Desimone R (1996). Neural mechanisms for visual memory and their role in attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93, 13494–13499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Desimone R, & Duncan J (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193–222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Dobbins IG, Schnyer DM, Verfaellie M, & Schacter DL (2004). Cortical activity reductions during repetition priming can result from rapid response learning. Nature, 428, 316–319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Doya K (1999). What are the computations of the cerebellum, the basal ganglia and the cerebral cortex? Neural Networks, 12(7–8), 961–974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Eger E, Henson RN, Driver J, & Dolan RJ (2007). Mechanisms of top-down facilitation in perception of visual objects studied by fMRI. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 2123–2133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Egner T, Monti JM, & Summerfield C (2010). Expectation and surprise determine neural population responses in ventral visual stream. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 16601–16608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Engel AK, Fries P, & Singer W (2001). Dynamic predictions: Oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 704–716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Ewbank M, & Henson RN (2012). Explaining away repetition effects via predictive coding. Cognitive Neuroscience, 3(3–4), 239–240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Ewbank MP, Henson R, Rowe J, Stoyanova R, & Calder A (in press). Different neural mechanisms within occipitotemporal cortex underlie repetition suppression across same and different-size faces. Cerebral Cortex. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Ewbank MP, Lawson RP, Henson RN, Rowe JB, Passamonti L, & Calder AJ (2011). Changes in “top-down” connectivity underlie repetition suppression in the ventral visual pathway. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 5635–5642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Fairhall SL, Anzellotti S, Pajtas PE, & Caramazza A (2011). Concordance between perceptual and categorical repetition effects in the ventral visual stream. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106, 398–408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Fang F, Murray SO, Kersten D, & He S (2005). Orientation-tuned fMRI adaptation in human visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 94, 4188–4195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Feldman H, & Friston KJ (2010). Attention, uncertainty, and free-energy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Fiebach CJ, Gruber T, & Supp GG (2005). Neuronal mechanisms of repetition priming in occipitotemporal cortex: Spatiotemporal evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging and electroencephalography. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 3414–3422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Fox MD, & Raichle ME (2007). Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 700–711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Freedman DJ, Riesenhuber M, Poggio T, & Miller EK (2006). Experience-dependent sharpening of visual shape selectivity in inferior temporal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 16(11), 1631–1644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Fries P (2005). A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: Neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 474–480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Friston K (2005). A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 360, 815–836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Friston K (2008). Hierarchical models in the brain. PLoS Computational Biology, 4(11), e1000211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Friston K (2012). Predictive coding, precision and synchrony. Cognitive Neuroscience, 3(3–4), 238–239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Friston KJ, Buechel C, Fink GR, Morris J, Rolls E, & Dolan RJ (1997). Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. NeuroImage, 6, 218–229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Friston KJ, Harrison L, & Penny W (2003). Dynamic causal modeling. NeuroImage, 19, 1273–1302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Gagnepain P, Chételat G, Landeau B, Dayan J, Eustache F, & Lebreton K (2008). Spoken word memory traces within the human auditory cortex revealed by repetition priming and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 5281–5289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Garrido MI, Kilner JM, Stephan KE, & Friston KJ(2009). The mismatch negativity: A review of underlying mechanisms. Clinical Neurophysiology, 120(3), 453–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Georgopoulos AP, Schwartz AB, & Kettner RE (1986). Neuronal population coding of movement direction. Science, 233, 1416–1419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Ghuman AS, Bar M, Dobbins IG, & Schnyer DM (2008). The effects of priming on frontal-temporal communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(24), 8405–8409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Gilbert JR, Gotts SJ, Carver FW, & Martin A (2010). Object repetition leads to local increases in the temporal coordination of neural responses. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Gold BT, Balota DA, Kirchhoff BA, & Buckner RL (2005). Common and dissociable activation patterns associated with controlled semantic and phonological processing: Evidence from fMRI adaptation. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1438–1450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Gotts SJ (2003). Mechanisms underlying enhanced processing efficiency in neural systems. Pittsburgh, PA: Carnegie Mellon University Press. [Google Scholar]
  68. Gotts SJ, Chow CC, & Martin A (2012). Repetition priming and repetition suppression: A case for enhanced efficiency through neural synchronization. Cognitive Neuroscience, 3(3–4), 227–237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Gotts SJ, Milleville SC, Bellgowan PSF, & Martin A (2011). Broad and narrow conceptual tuning in the human frontal lobes. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 477–491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Graf P, Squire LR, & Mandler G (1984). The information that amnesic patients do not forget. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 164–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Gregoriou GG, Gotts SJ, & Desimone R (2012). Cell-type-specific synchronization of neural activity in FEF with V4 during attention. Neuron, 73, 581–594. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Gregoriou GG, Gotts SJ, Zhou H, & Desimone R (2009a). High-frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attention. Science, 324, 1207–1210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Gregoriou GG, Gotts SJ, Zhou H, & Desimone R (2009b). Long-range neural coupling through synchronization with attention. Progress in Brain Research, 176, 35–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Gregory RL (1980). Perceptions as hypotheses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B, 290, 181–197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Grill-Spector K, Henson R, & Martin A (2006). Repetition and the brain: Neural models of stimulus-specific effects. Trends in Cognitive Science, 10, 14–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Grill-Spector K, & Malach R (2001). fMR-adaptation: A tool for studying the functional properties of human cortical neurons. Acta Psychologica, 107, 293–321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Grossberg S (1976). Adaptive pattern classification and universal recoding: II. Feedback, expectation, olfaction, and illusions. Biological Cybernetics, 23, 187–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Gruber T, & Muller MM (2005). Oscillatory brain activity dissociates between associative stimulus content in a repetition priming task in the human EEG. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 109–116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Hamburger M, & Slowiaczek LM (1998). Repetition priming and experimental context effects. American Journal of Psychology, 111, 1–31. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Hansen BJ, & Dragoi V (2011). Adaptation-induced synchronization in laminar cortical circuits. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 10720–10725. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. He BJ, Zempel JM, Snyder AZ, & Raichle ME (2010). The temporal structures and functional significance of scale-free brain activity. Neuron, 66, 353–369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Henson R (2003). Neuroimaging studies of priming. Progress in Neurobiology, 70(1), 53–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Henson R (2012). Repetition accelerates neural dynamics: Indefense of facilitation models. Cognitive Neuroscience, 3 (3–4), 240–241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Henson RN, & Gagnepain P (2010). Predictive, interactive multiple memory systems. Hippocampus, 20, 1315–1326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Henson RN, Goshen-Gottstein Y, Ganel T, Otten LJ, Quayle A, & Rugg MD (2003). Electrophysiological and haemodynamic correlates of face perception, recognition and priming. Cerebral Cortex, 13, 793–805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Henson RN, Price CJ, Rugg MD, Turner R, & Friston KJ (2002). Detecting latency differences in event-related BOLD responses: Application to words versus nonwords and initial versus repeated face presentations. NeuroImage, 15, 83–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Henson RN, Rylands A, Ross E, Vuilleumier P, & Rugg MD (2004). The effect of repetition lag on electrophysiological and haemodynamic correlates of visual object priming. NeuroImage, 21, 1674–1689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Henson RN, Wakeman DG, Litvak V, & Friston KJ (2011). A parametric empirical Bayesian framework for the EEG/MEG inverse problem: Generative models for multisubject and multimodal integration. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 76, 1–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Henson R, Wakeman D, Phillips C, & Rowe J (2012). Effective connectivity between OFA and FFA during face perception: DCM of evoked MEG, EEG and fMRI responses. Abstract accepted for Human Brain Mapping 2012. [Google Scholar]
  90. Horner AJ, & Henson RN (2008). Priming, response learning and repetition suppression. Neuropsychologia, 46(7), 1979–1991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Horner AJ, & Henson RN (2009). Bindings between stimuli and multiple response codes dominate long-lag repetition priming in speeded classification tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(3), 757–779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Horner AJ, & Henson RN (2012). Incongruent abstract stimulus-response bindings result in response interference: FMRI and EEG evidence from visual object classification priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(3), 760–773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. James TW, & Gauthier I (2006). Repetition-induced changes in BOLD response reflect accumulation of neural activity. Human Brain Mapping, 27, 37–45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. James TW, Humphrey GK, Gati JS, Menon RS, & Goodale MA (2000). The effects of visual object priming on brain activation before and after recognition. Current Biology, 10, 1017–1024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Jiang X, Bradley E, Rini RA, Zeffiro T, VanMeter J, & Riesenhuber M (2007). Categorization training results in shape—and category-selective human neural plasticity. Neuron, 53(6), 891–903. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Jiang X, Rosen E, Zeffiro T, VanMeter J, Blanz V, & Riesenhuber M (2006). Evaluation of a shape-based model of human face discrimination using FMRI and behavioral techniques. Neuron, 50(1), 159–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Jiang Y, Haxby JV, Martin A, Ungerleider LG, & Parasuraman R (2000). Complementary neural mechanisms for tracking items in human working memory. Science, 287, 643–646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Kaliukhovich DA, & Vogels R (2011). Stimulus repetition probability does not affect repetition suppression in macaque inferior temporal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 21(7), 1547–1558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Kellogg RT, Newcombe C, Kammer D, & Schmitt K (1996). Attention in direct and indirect memory tasks with short- and long-term probes. American Journal of Psychology, 109, 205–217. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Kiebel SJ, Garrido MI, Moran RJ, & Friston KJ (2008). Dynamic causal modelling for EEG and MEG. Cognitive Neurodynamics, 2, 121–136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Kiefer M (2005). Repetition priming modulates category-related effects on event-related potentials: Further evidence for multiple cortical semantic systems. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 199–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Kisley MA, Noecher TL, & Guinther PM (2004). Comparison of sensory gating to mismatch negativity and self-reported perceptual phenomena in healthy adults. Psychophysiology, 41, 604–612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Koester J, & Siegelbaum SA (2000). Membrane potential In Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, & Jessell TM (Eds.), Principles of neural science (4th ed, pp. 125–139). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
  104. Konen CS, & Kastner S (2008). The hierarchically organized neural systems for object information in human visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 224–231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Koutstaal W, Wagner AD, Rotte M, Maril A, Buckner RL, & Schacter DL (2001). Perceptual specificity in visual object priming: Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for a laterality difference in fusiform cortex. Neuropsychologia, 39(2), 184–199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Kraskov A, Quiroga RQ, Reddy L, Fried I, & Koch C (2007). Local field potentials and spikes in the human medial temporal lobe are selective to category. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 479–492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Krekelberg B, Boynton GM, & van Wezel RJ (2006). Adaptation: From single cells to BOLD signals. Trends in Neurosciences, 29(5), 250–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Larsson J, & Smith AT (2012). fMRI repetition suppression: Neuronal adaptation or stimulus expectation? Cerebral Cortex, 22, 567–576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Lee TS, & Mumford D (2003). Hierarchical Bayesian inference in the visual cortex. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 20, 1434–1448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Lewicki MS, & Sejnowski TJ (1996). Bayesian unsupervised learning of higher order structure. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, 9, 529–535. [Google Scholar]
  111. Li L, Miller EK, & Desimone R (1993). The representation of stimulus familiarity in anterior inferior temporal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 69(6), 1918–1929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Liebe S, Hoerzer GM, Logothetis NK, & Rainer G (2012). Theta coupling between V4 and prefrontal cortex predicts visual short-term memory performance. Nature Neuroscience, 15(3), 456–462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Logan GD (1990). Repetition priming and automaticity: Common underlying mechanisms? Cognitive Psychology, 22, 1–35. [Google Scholar]
  114. Luck SJ, Chelazzi L, Hillyard SA, & Desimone R (1997). Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 77, 24–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Madison DV, & Nicoll RA (1984). Control of the repetitive discharge of rat CA1 pyramidal neurones invitro. Journal of Physiology (London), 354, 319–331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Mahon BZ, Milleville SC, Negri GAL, Rumiati RI, Alfonso C, & Martin A (2007). Action-related properties shape object representations in the ventral stream. Neuron, 55, 507–520. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Markram H, Lubke J, Frotscher M, & Sakmann B (1997). Regulation of synaptic efficacy by coincidence of postsynaptic APs and EPSPs. Science, 275, 213–215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Martin A, & Gotts SJ (2005). Making the causal link: Frontal cortex activity and repetition priming. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1134–1135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Mayr S, Niedeggen M, Buchner A, & Pietrowsky R (2003). ERP correlates of auditory negative priming. Cognition, 90, B11–B21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. McClelland JL, McNaughton BL, & O’Reilly RC (1995). Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological Review, 102, 419–457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. McClelland JL, & Rumelhart DE (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375–407. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. McClelland JL, & Rumelhart DE (1985). Distributed memory and the representation of general and specific information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 159–188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. McKone E (1995). Short-term implicit memory for words and nonwords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 1108–1126. [Google Scholar]
  124. McKone E (1998). The decay of short-term implicit memory: Unpacking lag. Memory & Cognition, 26, 1173–1186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. McMahon DB, & Olson CR (2007). Repetition suppression in monkey inferotemporal cortex: Relation to behavioral priming. Journal of Neurophysiology, 97, 3532–3543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Meyer T, & Olson CR (2011). Statistical learning of visual transitions in monkey inferotemporal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(48), 19401–19406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Miller EK, Gochin PM, & Gross CG (1991). Habituation-like decrease in the responses of neurons in inferior temporal cortex of the macaque. Visual Neuroscience, 7, 357–362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Miller EK, Li L, & Desimone R (1993). Activity of neurons in anterior inferior temporal cortex during a short-term memory task. Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 1460–1478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Minsky M, & Papert S (1969). Perceptrons. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  130. Mirpour K, & Esteky H (2009). State-dependent effects of stimulus presentation duration on the temporal dynamics of neural responses in the inferotemporal cortex of macaque monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102(3), 1790–1800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Mitchell DB (2006). Nonconscious priming after 17 years: Invulnerable implicit memory? Psychological Science, 17, 925–929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Mondor TA, Breau LM, & Milliken B (1998). Inhibitory processes in auditory selective attention: Evidence of location-based and frequency-based inhibition of return. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 296–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Mumford D (1992). On the computational architecture of the neocortex. II. The role of cortico-cortical loops. Biological Cybernetics, 66, 241–251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Newsome WT, Britten KH, & Movshon JA (1989). Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision. Nature, 341, 52–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Norman KA, & O’Reilly RC (2003). Modeling hippo-campal and neocortical contributions to recognition memory: A complementary learning-systems approach. Psychological Review, 110, 611–646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Olichney JM, Van Petten C, Paller KA, Salmon DP, Iragui VJ, & Kutas M (2000). Word repetition in amnesia: Electrophysiological measures of impaired and spared memory. Brain, 123, 1948–1963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. O’Reilly RC (1998). Six principles for biologically based computational models of cortical cognition. Trends in Cognition Science, 2, 455–462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Ostergaard AL (1998). The effects on priming of word frequency, number of repetitions, and delay depend on the magnitude of priming. Memory & Cognition, 26, 40–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Paller KA, & Gross M (1998). Brain potentials associated with perceptual priming vs explicit remembering during the repetition of visual word-form. Neuropsychologia, 36, 559–571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Pearl J (1988). Probabilistic reasoning in intelligent systems: Networks of plausible inference. San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann. [Google Scholar]
  141. Pedreira C, Mormann F, Kraskov A, Cerf M, Fried I, Koch C, et al. (2010). Responses of human medial temporal lobe neurons are modulated by stimulus repetition. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103, 97–107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Peissig JJ, Singer J, Kawasaki K, & Sheinberg DL (2007). Effects of long-term object familiarity on event-related potentials in the monkey. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 1323–1334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Piazza M, Izard V, Pinel P, Le Bihan D, & Dehaene S (2004). Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the human intraparietal sulcus. Neuron, 44, 547–555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Puce A, Allison T, & McCarthy G (1999). Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. III. Effects of top-down processing on face-specific potentials. Cerebral Cortex, 9, 445–458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Race EA, Badre D, & Wagner AD (2010). Multiple forms of learning yield temporally distinct electrophysio-logical repetition effects. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 1726–1738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Race EA, Shanker S, & Wagner AD (2009). Neural priming in human frontal cortex: Multiple forms of learning reduce demands on the prefrontal executive system. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1766–1781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Raichle ME, & Mintun MA (2006). Brain work and brain imaging. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 29, 449–476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Rainer G, Asaad WF, & Miller EK (1998). Selective representation of relevant information by neurons in the primate prefrontal cortex. Nature, 393, 577–579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Rainer G, & Miller EK (2000). Effects of visual experience on the representation of objects in the prefrontal cortex. Neuron, 27, 179–189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Rao RP, & Ballard DH (1999). Predictive coding in the visual cortex: A functional interpretation of some extra-classicalreceptive-field effects. Nature neuroscience, 2(1), 79–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Reyes AD (2003). Synchrony-dependent propagation of firing rate in iteratively constructed networks invitro. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 593–599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Roediger HL (1990). Implicit memory retention without remembering. American Psychologist, 45, 1043–1056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Rollenhagen JE, & Olson CR (2005). Low-frequency oscillations arising from competitive interactions between visual stimuli in macaque inferotemporal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 94(5), 3368–3387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Rowe JB, Hughes LE, Barker RA, & Owen AM (2010). Dynamic causal modelling of effective connectivity from fMRI: Are results reproducible and sensitive to Parkinson’s disease and its treatment? Neuroimage, 52, 1015–1026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Rugg MD, Brovedani P, & Doyle MC (1992). Modulation of event-related potentials (ERPs) by word repetition in a task with inconsistent mapping between repetition and response. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 84, 521–531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Rugg MD, Mark RE, Gilchrist J, & Roberts RC (1997). ERP repetition effects in indirect and direct tasks: Effects of age and interitem lag. Psychophysiology, 34, 572–586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Salinas E, & Sejnowski TJ (2000). Impact of correlated synaptic input on output firing rate and variability in simple neuronal models. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 6193–6209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Salinas E, & Sejnowski TJ (2001). Correlated neuronal activity and the flow of neural information. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 539–550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Sayres R, & Grill-Spector K (2006). Object-selective cortex exhibits performance-independent repetition suppression. Journal of Neurophysiology, 95, 995–1007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Schacter DL (1987). Implicit memory—History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 13(3), 501–518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Schacter DL, & Buckner RL (1998). Priming and the brain. Neuron, 20, 185–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Schacter DL, Dobbins IG, & Schnyer DM (2004). Specificity of priming: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 5, 853–862. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Schall JD, & Hanes DP (1993). Neural basis of saccade target selection in frontal eye field during visual search. Nature, 366, 467–469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Schendan HE, & Kutas M (2003). Time course of processes and representations supporting visual object identification and memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 111–135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Scott LS, Tanaka JW, Sheinberg DL, & Curran T (2006). A reevaluation of the electrophysiological correlates of expert object processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1453–1465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Sjöstrom PJ, Turrigiano GG, & Nelson SB (2001). Rate, timing, and cooperativity jointly determine cortical synaptic plasticity. Neuron, 32, 1149–1164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Squire LR (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99, 195–231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Srinivas K (1996). Size and reflection effects in priming: A test of transfer-appropriate processing. Memory & Cognition, 24, 441–452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Stark CE, & McClelland JL (2000). Repetition priming of words, pseudowords, and nonwords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 945–972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Stopfer M, Bhagavan S, Smith BH, & Laurent G (1997). Impaired odour discrimination on desynchronization of odour-encoding neural assemblies. Nature, 390, 70–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Stopfer M, & Laurent G (1999). Short-term memory in olfactory network dynamics. Nature, 402, 664–648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. Summerfield C, Trittschuh EH, Monti JM, Mesulam MM, & Egner T (2008). Neural repetition suppression reflects fulfilled perceptual expectations. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 1004–1006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Summerfield C, Wyart V, Johnen VM, & de Gardelle V (2011). Human scalp electroencephalography reveals that repetition suppression varies with expectation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 67. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  174. Swick D (1998). Effects of prefrontal lesions on lexical processing and repetition priming: An ERP study. Cognitive Brain Research, 7, 143–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  175. Szymanski KF, & MacLeod CM (1996). Manipulation of attention at study affects an explicit but not an implicit test of memory. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 5, 165–175. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  176. Todorovic A, van Ede F, Maris E, & de Lange FP (2011). Prior expectation mediates neural adaptation to repeated sounds in the auditory cortex: An MEG study. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 9118–9123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  177. Tsodyks MV, & Markram H (1997). The neural code between cortical pyramidal neurons depends on neuro-transmitter release probability. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 94, 719–723. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. Tulving E, & Schacter DL (1990). Priming and human memory systems. Science, 247, 301–306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. van Turennout M, Bielamowicz L, & Martin A (2003). Modulation of neural activity during object naming: Effects of time and practice. Cerebral Cortex, 13, 381–391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. van Turennout M, Ellmore T, & Martin A (2000). Long-lasting cortical plasticity in the object naming system. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1329–1334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  181. Varela JA, Song S, Turrigiano GG, & Nelson SB (1999). Differential depression at excitatory and inhibitory synapses in visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 4293–4304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  182. Verhoef B-E, Kayaert G, Franko E, Vangeneugden J, & Vogels R (2008). Stimulus similarity-contingent neural adaptation can be time and cortical area dependent. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 10631–10640. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  183. von Stein A, Chiang C, & Konig P (2000). Top-down processing mediated by interareal synchronization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97, 14748–14753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  184. Voss JL, & Paller KA (2008). Brain substrates of implicit and explicit memory: The importance of concurrently acquired neural signals of both memory types. Neuropsychologia, 46, 3021–3029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  185. Wang Y, Iliescu BF, Ma J, Josić K, & Dragoi V (2011). Adaptive changes in neuronal synchronization in macaque V4. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 13204–13213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  186. Warrington EK, & Weiskrantz L (1974). The effect of prior learning on subsequent retention in amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia, 12, 419–428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  187. Weiner KS, Sayres R, Vinberg J, & Grill-Spector K (2010). fMRI-adaptation and category selectivity in human ventral temporal cortex: Regional differences across time scales. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103, 3349–3365. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  188. Wig GS, Buckner RL, & Schacter DL (2009). Repetition priming influences distinct brain systems: Evidence from task-evoked data and resting-state correlations. Journal of Neurophysiology, 101(5), 2632–2648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  189. Wig GS, Grafton ST, Demos KE, & Kelley WM (2005). Reductions in neural activity underlie behavioral components of repetition priming. Nature Neuroscience, 8(9), 1228–1233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  190. Wiggs CL, & Martin A (1998). Properties and mechanisms of perceptual priming. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8, 227–233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  191. Wiggs CL, Martin A, & Sunderland T (1997). Monitoring frequency of occurrence without awareness: Evidence from patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19(2), 235–244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  192. Woloszyn L, & Sheinberg DL (2012). Effects of long-term visual experience on responses of distinct classes of single units in inferior temporal cortex. Neuron, 74, 193–205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  193. Xu Y, Turk-Browne NB, & Chun MM (2007). Dissociating task performance from fMRI repetition attenuation in ventral visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 5981–5985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES