Skip to main content
Frontiers in Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Neuroscience
. 2019 Apr 3;13:322. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00322

Acute Social Defeat Stress Increases Sleep in Mice

Shinya Fujii 1, Mahesh K Kaushik 1, Xuzhao Zhou 1, Mustafa Korkutata 1,2, Michael Lazarus 1,*
PMCID: PMC6456680  PMID: 31001080

Abstract

Social conflict is a major source of stress in humans. Animals also experience social conflicts and cope with them by stress responses that facilitate arousal and activate sympathetic and neuroendocrine systems. The effect of acute social defeat (SoD) stress on the sleep/wake behavior of mice has been reported in several models based on a resident-intruder paradigm. However, the post-SoD stress sleep/wake effects vary between the studies and the contribution of specific effects in response to SoD or non-specific effects of the SoD procedure (e.g., sleep deprivation) is not well established. In this study, we established a mouse model of acute SoD stress based on strong aggressive mouse behavior toward unfamiliar intruders. In our model, we prevented severe attacks of resident mice on submissive intruder mice to minimize behavioral variations during SoD. In response to SoD, slow-wave sleep (SWS) strongly increased during 9 h. Although some sleep changes after SoD stress can be attributed to non-specific effects of the SoD procedure, most of the SWS increase is likely a specific response to SoD. Slow-wave activity was only enhanced for a short period after SoD and dissipated long before the SWS returned to baseline. Moreover, SoD evoked a strong corticosterone response that may indicate a high stress level in the intruder mice after SoD. Our SoD model may be useful for studying the mechanisms and functions of sleep in response to social stress.

Keywords: social defeat stress, slow-wave sleep, homeostatic sleep need, mouse model, slow-wave activity

Introduction

All living organisms respond to any external biological source of stress, i.e., perceived or actual threats, with a predictable biological pattern in an attempt to restore the internal homeostasis of the body (Chrousos, 2009). Acute stress typically involves transient responses of biological systems; however, prolonged activation of these initially beneficial reactions by excessive or chronically repeated stressors may lead to stress-related disorders. Social conflicts are a major source of stress for humans and play a major role in the pathogenesis of affective disorders like anxiety and depression (Charney and Manji, 2004; Huhman, 2006). Social conflicts also occur in animals (Toyoda, 2017). A social animal with the inability to dominate its opponent shows submissive behavior and accepts a lower social rank to avoid injury and death that could occur if the animal continues to act in an aggressive manner (Huhman, 2006). Rodent models of social defeat (SoD) stress are often based on a resident-intruder paradigm where the resident attempts to defend its home cage from the intruder who eventually subordinates itself to the unfamiliar territorial resident conspecific. Acute SoD stress in rats has been shown to increase the heart rate and body temperature by sympathetic stimulation and blood corticosterone levels by activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis (Koolhaas et al., 1997; Kataoka et al., 2014). These stress responses quickly dissipate and return to baseline levels within hours after the end of the stress period. SoD stress, however, also induces longer-lasting behavioral and physiologic changes. For example, several days may be required to normalize the circadian rhythmicity of body temperature, open-field behavior, and body mass growth after acute SoD (Meerlo et al., 1996a,b,c). Moreover, social animals are known to develop depression-like behaviors in response to chronically repeated SoD stress (Von Frijtag et al., 2000; van Kampen et al., 2002; Rygula et al., 2005; Huhman, 2006). For example, chronic SoD stress in mice induces anhedonia, anxiety, and social avoidance (Golden et al., 2011; Henriques-Alves and Queiroz, 2015). The anxiety and social avoidance behaviors in mice after chronic SoD stress can last several weeks (Berton et al., 2006; Tsankova et al., 2006; Krishnan et al., 2007). These long-lasting behavioral changes in animals that experience repeated SoD stress are commonly associated with alterations in gene expression patterns in the brain (Tsankova et al., 2007).

The sleep/wake cycle is regulated by multiple factors, including homeostasis, circadian rhythm, and external environment, that are integrated by specific neuronal circuits controlling the sleep/wake behavior (Weber and Dan, 2016; Eban-Rothschild et al., 2017). Stress promotes wakefulness and inhibits sleep during the period when enhanced arousal is necessary to cope with external challenges. Alterations in the sleep architecture are often observed after an acute stress exposure (Sanford et al., 2015). A sleep rebound may be induced in animals to compensate for the sleep loss during stressful situations, but it is believed that sleep alterations are not only a homeostatic response to the sleep loss but also created by the stress. The type of sleep that is induced and the extent to which sleep stages are enhanced are highly variable between the types of stress or even the study design. For example, acute immobilization or restraint stress is followed by a selective increase in rapid eye movement sleep (REMS) (Rampin et al., 1991; Meerlo et al., 2001). Moreover, mice experiencing escapable foot shock stress showed an increase in REMS, whereas mice treated with inescapable foot shock stress showed a decrease in REMS (Sanford et al., 2010). Sleep is considered to have an important role in coping with stressful situations (Goldstein and Walker, 2014).

The acute effect of SoD stress on sleep has been reported in several rodent models. Although slow-wave activity (SWA) is consistently enhanced during sleep after SoD stress in mice and rats (Meerlo et al., 1997; Meerlo and Turek, 2001; Kamphuis et al., 2015; Henderson et al., 2017), the extent to which sleep stages are promoted varies between SoD studies and protocols. Male C57BL/6j mice when defeated by aggressive mice of the same strain during a 1-h interaction period showed a strong increase of slow-wave sleep (SWS) (Meerlo and Turek, 2001), whereas only a small increase of the SWS amount was observed in another study using the same SoD procedure (Vaanholt et al., 2003). This difference may be explained by variations in the aggressive behavior of the resident mice or prior stress experiences of the intruder mice in the laboratory environment. When highly aggressive male CD-1 mice were used in another study for the SoD of C57BL/6j mice during a 5-min interaction period followed by a 20-min period of olfactory, visual and auditory contact between the resident and intruder mice, a SWS increase in the intruder mice was preceded by an increase of wakefulness (Henderson et al., 2017). A fixed interaction period likely leads to painful attacks of the CD-1 mouse on the intruder mouse and thus, the sleep-wake behavior of the intruder mice may also be affected by pain. Overall, the extent to which the SoD procedure (e.g., pain or sleep deprivation) contributes to the post-SoD stress sleep/wake effects is not well established.

In the present study, we developed a mouse model of SoD stress based on a resident-intruder paradigm to evoke sleep alterations in the intruder C57BL/6j mouse after acute SoD stress by the resident CD-1 mouse trained to display persistent aggression against the intruder mouse. To prevent painful attacks to the submissive intruder mouse, the intruder and resident mice were separated by a partition when the intruder mouse showed clear submissive behavior and was attacked by the resident mouse. We found that SWS strongly increased in response to SoD, whereas REMS was only moderately increased for a limited period several hours after the SoD session.

Materials and Methods

Animals

Male C57BL/6j mice (13–20 weeks old, and weighing 26–33 g), maintained at the International Institute of Integrative Sleep Medicine of the University of Tsukuba, were used in the experiments. Male CD-1 (retired breeders) mice were obtained from Japan SLC (Hamamatsu, Japan). The animals were housed in an insulated and soundproof recording chamber maintained at an ambient temperature of 23 ± 0.5°C with a relative humidity of 50 ± 5% and an automatically controlled 12 h light/12 h dark cycle (illumination intensity ≈ 100 lux). All animals had free access to food and water. This study was performed in strict accordance with the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the US National Institutes of Health (2011). Experimental protocols were in compliance with relevant Japanese and institutional laws and guidelines, and approved by the University of Tsukuba animal ethics committee (protocol #14-322). Every effort was made to minimize the number of animals used as well as any pain and discomfort experienced by the animals.

Stereotaxic Surgery for Placement of the EEG/EMG Electrodes

Mice were anesthetized with pentobarbital (50 mg/kg, intraperitoneal [i.p.]) and then placed in a stereotaxic apparatus. Electroencephalogram (EEG) and electromyogram (EMG) electrodes for polysomnographic recordings were chronically implanted in the mice (Oishi et al., 2016). The implant comprised two stainless steel screws (1 mm in diameter) that served as the EEG electrodes inserted through the skull above the cortex (anteroposterior, +1.0 mm; left-right, -1.5 mm from bregma or lambda) according to the atlas of Paxinos and Franklin (2004). Two insulated, stainless steel Teflon-coated wires, serving as the EMG electrodes, were placed bilaterally into both trapezius muscles. All electrodes were attached to a micro connector and fixed to the skull with dental cement.

Stress Procedures

The SoD stress protocol was designed based on the resident-intruder paradigm. To prepare aggressive resident mice, male CD-1 mice (body weight > 40 g) were singly housed and trained to display persistent aggression against male C57BL/6j mice by placing a male C57BL/6j mouse in the cage of a CD-1 mouse one or two times a week. During the training, the C57BL/6j mouse was immediately removed from the cage when attacked and defeated by the CD-1 mouse. Unsuitable CD-1 mice that showed little aggression or extreme violent behavior (i.e., mice that inflict serious injuries to their opponent) were excluded from the study. Only CD-1 mice showing aggression against C57BL/6j mice within 1 min were used in the experiments to ensure successful SoD. The C57BL/6j mice used for aggression training were not used for any SoD experiments.

For SoD stress, we used a transparent acrylic partition placed diagonally in a rectangular cage to separate the intruder and resident mice. The partition has a wire-mesh opening in the lower part for olfactory, visual, and auditory contact (Figure 1B,C). A SoD session lasted 60 min starting at zeitgeber time 11. The C57BL/6j mouse (intruder mouse) was placed behind the partition in the resident mouse’s cage. After 5 min, the partition was removed and re-inserted when the intruder mouse was attacked by the resident mouse and showed clear submissive behavior, including submissive posture, escaping, or freezing behavior. During one SoD session, the removal of the partition was repeated two, four, or eight times at 25-, 15-, or 7-min intervals, respectively. After the SoD session, the intruder mouse was returned to its home cage (Figure 1D).

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Protocol for social defeat (SoD) stress based on a resident-intruder paradigm. (A) Schedule of sleep recording and SoD session. (B) Cage used for SoD or EEG/EMG recording and partition for diagonal separation of the cage. (C) Cage with inserted partition. (D) Schematic diagram of the SoD procedure. The intruder mouse was placed behind the partition of the resident cage. The partition was repeatedly removed for multiple defeat experiences of the intruder mouse by the resident mouse during a 1-h period. After the SoD session, the intruder mouse was returned to the home cage for sleep recording. (E) Control experiments to differentiate between the specific effects of SoD and non-specific effects of the SoD procedure. The mouse was placed in the cage for 1 h under each condition.

The following experiments were also conducted to differentiate the specific effects of SoD and non-specific effects of the SoD procedure (Figure 1E): (1) C57BL/6j mice were placed in an unused cage with bedding, food pellets, and a partition for 60 min and sleep deprived by cage tapping (“Sleep deprivation” session). (2) C57BL/6j mice were placed in a cage with a partition previously used by a CD-1 mouse for more than 5 days (“No resident” session). (3) The intruder mouse was placed in the resident cage while the intruder and resident mice were separated by the partition for 60 min (“No contact” session). Sleep deprivation by cage tapping was not conducted during the “No resident” and “No contact” sessions.

Vigilance State Assessment Based on EEG/EMG Polygraphic Recordings

One week after surgery, the mice were individually housed in cages in an insulated soundproof recording chamber and connected to the EEG-EMG recording cables for 2–3 days of habituation before starting the polygraphic recordings. Before the SoD session, some mice were subjected to a “Sleep deprivation” or “No resident” session, followed by a “No contact” session. Sessions were run every 3 or 4 days. Baseline was recorded on the day prior to each session (Figure 1A). The EEG/EMG signals were amplified, filtered (EEG 0.5–30 Hz; EMG 20–200 Hz), digitized at a sampling rate of 128 Hz, and recorded using the data acquisition software SleepSign® (Kissei Comtec, Matsumoto, Japan). The vigilance states were classified offline in 10-s epochs of three stages, i.e., wakefulness, REMS, and SWS by SleepSign® (ver 3.4) according to standard criteria (Oishi et al., 2016). As a final step, the software-defined vigilance stage of each 10-s epoch was visually examined, and manually corrected when necessary. Spectral analysis of EEG by fast Fourier transform was performed, and the EEG power densities were calculated in the range of 0.5–25 Hz in 0.5-Hz bins. SWA during SWS was calculated based on EEG power in the range of 0.5–4 Hz. In a 3-hourly plot of SWA, the data were presented as percentages of the mean of the 12-h baseline SWS during the dark period.

Behavioral Analysis

Animal behaviors during “No resident” and “No contact” sessions were analyzed using video recordings. Vigilance states and behaviors during wakefulness were scored in 4-s epochs. Behaviors were scored as grooming, exploration (including ambulation, rearing, digging, and sniffing), consumption (eating and drinking), or quiet waking, when the behavior accounted for more than 50% of the epoch.

Blood Sampling and Corticosterone Measurement

Blood sampling was performed by cardiac puncture with the mice under deep isoflurane anesthesia immediately after the SoD sessions with 2, 4, or 8 defeats. Blood samples were obtained within 2 min, which is rapid enough to ensure that the stress imposed by the blood-sampling procedure did not affect the corticosterone levels in the plasma (Riley, 1960). For the control group, mouse blood was collected immediately after the “No resident” session. For basal corticosterone levels, blood was collected from undisturbed mice in their home cages at zeitgeber time 12. All the mice were used only once and not subjected to any behavioral tests. Blood samples were collected into EDTA-coated syringes and immediately centrifuged at 10,000 rpm for 15 min at 4°C. Plasma samples were collected and stored at -80°C until the assay was performed. Plasma corticosterone was measured in duplicate wells using the DetectX® ELISA kit (Arbor Assays, Ann Arbor, MI, United States). The mean of intra-assay coefficients of variations (n = 31) calculated from duplicate wells was 2.82%.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were carried out using GraphPad Prism software (GraphPad Software, La Jolla, CA, United States). All results are presented as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM). A paired two-tailed Student’s t-test was used for statistical comparisons between paired groups (Figure 2B,C, 3A, 4A–C,E and Tables 2, 4). Two-way repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by Bonferroni’s post hoc comparisons was used to analyze the sleep/wake profile (Figure 2A) and EEG power (Figure 4D,F). One-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s (Figure 2DF) or Dunnett’s (Figure 3B, 4E) post hoc comparisons were performed to compare groups of three or more. The Kruskal–Wallis test was used to analyze the data with significant variance heterogeneity assessed by Levene’s test (Figure 2D). In all cases, P < 0.05 was considered significant.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2

Slow-wave sleep (SWS) increased after social defeat (SoD) stress. (A) Time-course of SWS, rapid eye movement sleep (REMS), and wakefulness for 21 h after SoD sessions with two (n = 8), four (n = 8), or eight (n = 7) defeats. P < 0.05, ∗∗P < 0.01, and ∗∗∗P < 0.001 compared with baseline, assessed by two-way repeated measures ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s post hoc comparisons. (B) Time-course of SWS, REMS, and wakefulness for 3 h after SoD sessions. P < 0.05 compared with baseline, assessed by paired two-tailed Student’s t-test. (C) Total amounts of SWS, REMS, and wakefulness for 9 h after SoD sessions. P < 0.05, ∗∗P < 0.01, and ∗∗∗P < 0.001 compared with baseline, assessed by paired two-tailed Student’s t-test. (D) Changes in total amounts of SWS, REMS, and wakefulness for 9 h between baseline and after SoD sessions. P < 0.05, compared between groups, assessed by the Kruskal–Wallis test (SWS and wakefulness) or one-way ANOVA (REMS) followed by Bonferroni’s post hoc comparisons. (E) Deviation of changes in total amounts of SWS and wakefulness. The deviation was calculated by subtracting the mean from the experimental values of each animal for changes in the total amounts of SWS and wakefulness for 9 h in each condition. P < 0.05 and ∗∗P < 0.01, compared between groups, assessed by one-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s post hoc comparisons. (F) Blood plasma corticosterone levels from undisturbed mice (n = 6) at zeitgeber time 12 of the baseline day or mice after a “No resident” (n = 7) or SoD sessions (n = 6). P < 0.05, ∗∗P < 0.01, and ∗∗∗P < 0.001 compared between groups, assessed by one-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s post hoc comparisons. Data are presented as means ± SEM. NS, not significant.

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3

Social defeat (SoD) is essential for a strong slow-wave sleep (SWS) increase. (A) Total amounts of SWS, rapid eye movement sleep (REMS), and wakefulness for 9 h after a “Sleep deprivation” (n = 4), “No resident” (n = 4), “No contact” (n = 8), or SoD (n = 8) session with four defeats. P < 0.05, ∗∗P < 0.01, and ∗∗∗P < 0.001 compared with baseline, assessed by paired two-tailed Student’s t-test. (B) Changes in the total amounts of SWS, REMS, and wakefulness for 9 h between baseline and experimental days. P < 0.05, ∗∗P < 0.01, and ∗∗∗P < 0.001 compared with SoD, assessed by one-way ANOVA followed by Dunnett’s post hoc comparisons. Data are presented as means ± SEM. NS, not significant.

FIGURE 4.

FIGURE 4

Sleep architecture after social defeat (SoD) stress. (A–C) Episode numbers (A), mean episode durations (B), and numbers of stage transitions (C) for 9 h after SoD session with four defeats. P < 0.05 and ∗∗P < 0.01 compared with baseline, assessed by paired two-tailed Student’s t-test. (D) Power density of EEG during SWS for 30 min after SoD session with four defeats or onset of the dark phase on the baseline day. P < 0.05 compared with baseline, assessed by two-way repeated measures ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s post hoc comparisons. (E) Changes in the SWA (0.5–4 Hz) relative to the baseline for 30 min after a “Sleep deprivation,” “No resident,” “No contact,” or SoD session. P < 0.05 compared with baseline, assessed by paired two-tailed Student’s t-test, #P < 0.05 compared with SoD, assessed by one-way ANOVA followed by Dunnett’s post hoc comparisons. (F) Time-course of SWA changes after SoD session. ∗∗∗P < 0.001 compared with baseline, assessed by two-way repeated measures ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s post hoc comparisons.

Table 2.

Total amounts (minutes, mean ± SEM) of slow-wave sleep (SWS), rapid eye movement sleep (REMS), and wakefulness for 6, 9, 12, and 24 h after SoD sessions with two, four, or eight defeats.

SoD (2 times) n = 8 SoD (4 times) n = 8 SoD (8 times) n = 7
SWS (min)
6 h Baseline 95.8 ± 8.9 97.2 ± 6.1 106.1 ± 14.5
After session 153.9 ± 6.7*** 152.4 ± 5.0*** 154.9 ± 5.4**
9 h Baseline 179.5 ± 11.4 166.8 ± 6.3 187.8 ± 17.9
After session 249.6 ± 8.7** 258.0 ± 5.8*** 255.2 ± 8.9**
12 h Baseline 243.0 ± 12.6 221.5 ± 9.5 247.2 ± 18.3
After session 317.8 ± 10.8** 315.3 ± 11.5*** 325.1 ± 12.7***
24 h Baseline 655.0 ± 14.9 638.8 ± 12.3 663.6 ± 16.5
After session 712.3 ± 13.0** 725.3 ± 16.7*** 735.5 ± 14.3**
REMS (min)
6 h Baseline 4.0 ± 0.9 4.6 ± 1.0 7.3 ± 1.3
After session 4.5 ± 1.3 4.3 ± 1.2 2.4 ± 0.8*
9 h Baseline 10.7 ± 2.0 8.6 ± 1.7 16.0 ± 2.8
After session 13.5 ± 3.0 16.0 ± 1.6* 12.0 ± 1.8
12 h Baseline 14.3 ± 2.5 10.7 ± 1.8 21.1 ± 2.5
After session 18.8 ± 3.6 20.9 ± 1.5** 16.9 ± 2.0
24 h Baseline 70.9 ± 1.6 72.3 ± 3.8 77.3 ± 2.6
After session 73.6 ± 3.9 79.9 ± 2.6 69.8 ± 3.7
Wakefulness (min)
6 h Baseline 260.1 ± 9.3 258.3 ± 6.8 246.7 ± 15.4
After session 201.5 ± 7.8*** 203.3 ± 5.4*** 202.7 ± 4.8*
9 h Baseline 349.9 ± 12.0 364.6 ± 7.2 336.3 ± 20.0
After session 276.9 ± 11.4** 265.9 ± 6.4*** 272.7 ± 8.2**
12 h Baseline 462.7 ± 14.0 487.8 ± 10.6 451.7 ± 20.1
After session 383.4 ± 13.7** 383.9 ±12.0*** 378.0 ± 11.7**
24 h Baseline 714.1 ± 14.9 728.9 ± 13.7 699.0 ± 17.2
After session 654.1 ± 16.1** 634.9 ± 18.0** 634.7 ± 11.3**

P < 0.05, ∗∗P < 0.01, and ∗∗∗P < 0.001 compared with baseline, assessed by paired two-tailed Student’s t-test.

Table 4.

Total amounts (minutes, mean ± SEM) of slow-wave sleep (SWS), rapid eye movement sleep (REMS), and wakefulness for 6, 9, 12, and 24 h after “Sleep deprivation,” “No resident,” and “No contact” sessions.

Sleep deprivation n = 4 No resident n = 4 No contact n = 8
SWS (min)
6 h Baseline 94.4 ± 10.9 103.1 ± 9.9 93.7 ± 8.7
After session 132.0 ± 12.8** 118.7 ± 18.5 119.9 ± 7.0**
9 h Baseline 166.0 ± 12.9 186.7 ± 15.9 150.6 ± 12.2
After session 213.3 ± 10.7*** 201.7 ± 21.2 192.0 ± 11.5*
12 h Baseline 228.3 ± 19.9 229.3 ± 18.0 214.4 ± 12.6
After session 263.8 ± 23.8** 239.9 ± 17.3 242.8 ± 10.6
24 h Baseline 619.6 ± 25.0 616.7 ± 8.4 623.1 ± 10.7
After session 666.8 ± 37.3* 648.1 ± 13.7 653.9 ± 15.3
REMS (min)
6 h Baseline 3.8 ± 1.3 3.7 ± 1.2 4.8 ± 1.0
After session 8.6 ± 0.6 6.4 ± 1.9 7.0 ± 1.2
9 h Baseline 11.2 ± 1.9 8.0 ± 3.4 8.8 ± 1.6
After session 18.1 ± 2.6 13.2 ± 3.7 14.6 ± 2.1
12 h Baseline 15.6 ± 2.1 9.4 ± 3.7 12.7 ± 1.9
After session 21.6 ± 2.4 16.3 ± 3.3 17.8 ± 2.0
24 h Baseline 77.2 ± 3.0 61.2 ± 2.2 70.6 ± 2.8
After session 83.0 ± 3.3 74.7 ± 4.4 79.4 ± 3.9**
Wakefulness (min)
6 h Baseline 261.8 ± 11.5 253.2 ± 11.0 261.6 ± 9.0
After session 219.4 ± 12.4*** 235.0 ± 20.3 233.1 ± 7.7**
9 h Baseline 362.8 ± 13.7 345.3 ± 18.9 380.5 ± 12.8
After session 308.7 ± 9.8** 325.1 ± 24.9 333.4 ± 12.7*
12 h Baseline 476.1 ± 20.2 481.3 ± 21.4 492.9 ± 13.3
After session 434.6 ± 22.7** 463.8 ± 20.1 459.4 ± 11.6
24 h Baseline 743.2 ± 23.1 762.1 ± 6.3 746.3 ± 9.8
After session 690.2 ± 39.8 717.2 ± 16.6 706.7 ± 15.7*

P < 0.05, ∗∗P < 0.01, and ∗∗∗P < 0.001 compared with baseline, assessed by paired two-tailed Student’s

Results

SoD Stress Strongly Increases Sleep

We established a mouse model of sleep alterations after acute SoD stress based on a resident-intruder paradigm, whereby a C57BL/6j male mouse was introduced as an intruder to a resident CD-1 male mouse selected for its high level of aggression (Figure 1). To prevent injury to the intruder mouse, the intruder and resident mice were separated by a partition. During a 1-h SoD session, the partition was removed multiple times and reinserted every time when the intruder mouse was attacked and showed submissive behavior. After the SoD session, the intruder mouse was returned to the home cage and EEG and EMG of the intruder mice were recorded.

First, we investigated the extent to which sleep was affected by different numbers of defeats during a 1-h session. Intruder mice were exposed to two, four, or eight successive defeats. In each successive defeat, mice were in contact with the resident mice for 15.5 ± 1.5 s. We analyzed EEG and EMG recordings made after the SoD session or on the previous day (baseline). SWS strongly increased from the first 3 h after the sessions with two, four, and eight defeats. The strong increase in SWS lasted until 6 or 9 h after the sessions with two or four defeats, respectively. Accordingly, wakefulness was decreased during the corresponding periods. A REMS increase was observed only between 6 and 9 h after the SoD session with four successive defeats (Figure 2A and Table 1). When the first 3 h following the SoD sessions were analyzed in more detail, an increase in wakefulness only occurred during the first 10 min after the SoD sessions and SWS started to increase afterward (Figure 2B). Because most of the effects on sleep stages were observed within 9 h after the sessions, we compared the total amounts of 9 h following the SoD sessions (Figure 2C and Table 2). The total baseline amounts of the vigilance states during the same time period were not significantly different among groups [SWS: P = 0.4914, F(2,20) = 0.7364, REMS: P = 0.0761, F(2,20) = 2.938, Wake: P = 0.3578, F(2,20) = 1.083]. SWS significantly increased after SoD sessions, whereas wakefulness decreased accordingly. By contrast, REMS was only increased significantly after four defeats. Moreover, the calculated changes in the total amounts of SWS, REMS, and wakefulness between baseline and the post-SoD 9-h periods revealed a significant difference in REMS only between four and eight times (Figure 2D and Table 1). Because variance in SWS or wakefulness was highly significantly different among groups, we performed Bonferroni’s post hoc comparisons on the absolute deviations from the mean (Figure 2E). The deviations were significantly lower only in the intruder group with four defeats. Finally, we studied corticosterone responses in the intruder mice after SoD as an indicator of stress due to activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, the central stress response system (Mormède et al., 2007). Blood samples for measuring corticosterone levels were collected from undisturbed mice (to establish the baseline) and intruder mice immediately after the SoD sessions. A control session in a cage previously used by the resident mouse (“No resident”) was also conducted. SoD sessions significantly increased corticosterone levels and it was significantly stronger compared with “No resident” mice [P < 0.0001, F(4,26) = 24.5, one-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s post hoc comparisons, Figure 2F]. These results may indicate that SoD causes stress in mice. However, there was no significant difference between SoD sessions with different numbers of successive defeats. These results suggested that the SWS increase observed after SoD stress is mostly independent of the numbers of defeats.

Table 1.

Statistics of data shown in Figure 2A,B, 3B.

Two-way repeated measures ANOVA for Figure 2A
Main effect Interaction
SoD (2 times) SWS P = 0.0057, F(1,49) = 8.359 P < 0.0001, F(6,49) = 6.405
n = 8 REMS P = 0.9359, F(1,49) = 0.006532 P = 0.1535, F(6,49) = 1.65
Wakefulness P = 0.0124, F(1,49) = 6.744 P < 0.0001, F(6,49) = 5.89
SoD (4 times) SWS P < 0.0001, F(1,49) = 33.97 P < 0.0001, F(6,49) = 10.76
n = 8 REMS P = 0.2284, F(1,49) = 1.488 P < 0.0001, F(6,49) = 8.931
Wakefulness P < 0.0001, F(1,49) = 29.37 P < 0.0001, F(6,49) = 11.06
SoD (8 times) SWS P < 0.0001, F(1,42) = 20.49 P = 0.0007, F(6,42) = 4.931
n = 7 REMS P = 0.2984, F(1,42) = 1.109 P = 0.3936, F(6,42) = 1.074
Wakefulness P = 0.0005, F(1,42) = 14.38 P = 0.0049, F(6,42) = 3.698

One-way ANOVA or Kruskal–Wallis test for Figure 2D, 3B
One-way ANOVA or Kruskal–Wallis test Levene’s test

Figure 2D SWS P = 0.5716, H = 1.118 P = 0.0004, F(2,20) = 11.69
REMS P = 0.0481, F(2,20) = 3.546 P = 0.9844, F(2,20) = 0.01575
Wakefulness P = 0.2035, H = 3.184 P = 0.0025, F(2,20) = 8.239
Figure 3B SWS P = 0.0006, F(3,20) = 8.986 P = 0.0838, F(3,20) = 2.56
REMS P = 0.9563, F(3,20) = 0.1049 P = 0.8879, F(3,20) = 0.2106
Wakefulness P = 0.0026, F(3,20) = 6.713 P = 0.1492, F(3,20) = 1.981

Specific Effects of SoD Versus Non-specific Effects of the SoD Procedure

To differentiate between the specific effects of SoD and non-specific effects of the SoD procedure (e.g., novelty or sleep deprivation), the sleep/wake behavior of mice after SoD stress was compared with that following other conditions, including sleep deprivation in a clean unused cage (“Sleep deprivation”), a cage previously used by the resident mouse (“No resident”), or the presence of the resident mouse separated by a partition (“No contact”). Data of the control conditions were compared with those of four successive defeats, because SWS consolidation occurred with significantly less variation after this SoD procedure than the ones with two or eight defeats.

During the “No resident” or “No contact” session, the animals were spontaneously active and awake during most of the 1-h session (Table 3). “Sleep deprivation” and “No contact” sessions significantly increased SWS and decreased wakefulness during a 9-h period after the sessions (Figure 3A and Table 4); however, the SWS increase was smaller than that after SoD stress (Figure 3B). SWS was not changed after the “No resident” session (Figure 3A). REMS also increased after the SoD and control procedures; however, there was only a significant difference between the baseline and experimental periods after SoD stress (Figure 3A). These results suggest that a significant part of the sleep changes after SoD can be attributed to non-specific sleep deprivation during the SoD session, i.e., about half of the increase of SWS and all of the increase of REM sleep may be a homeostatic sleep response to non-specific sleep deprivation.

Table 3.

Percentage of animal behaviors (during wakefulness), slow-wave sleep (SWS), and rapid eye movement sleep (REMS) during the “No resident” and “No contact” sessions.

Behavior (%) No resident No contact
n = 4 n = 8
Quiet waking 2.0 ± 0.8 3.9 ± 2.8
Grooming 14.5 ± 2.4 7.0 ± 1.5
Exploration 70.4 ± 6.9 79.2 ± 6.6
Consumption 3.1 ± 1.0 5.0 ± 0.8
SWS 9.6 ± 6.8 4.9 ± 4.5
REMS 0.3 ± 0.3 0.0 ± 0.0

SoD Stress Increases the Number of SWS Episodes and Slow-Wave Activity

The sleep architecture and SWA were then analyzed after SoD session with four successive defeats. SoD stress significantly affected the episode number of SWS and wakefulness for 9 h after the SoD session (SWS: P = 0.0079, Wake: P = 0.0077, paired Student’s t-test, Figure 4A). The mean duration of wake episodes decreased by 48.4 ± 6.9% (P = 0.0035, paired Student’s t-test) compared with the baseline, whereas the duration of the SWS or REMS episodes was not significantly different or slightly increased, respectively (Figure 4B). The number of stage transitions from SWS to wake and wake to SWS was significantly increased (SWS to Wake: P = 0.0131, Wake to SWS: P = 0.0076, paired Student’s t-test), whereas other stage transitions were not affected by SoD stress (Figure 4C).

To assess whether EEG activity was altered by SoD stress, we compared the normalized EEG power spectrum of SWS for 30 min following the SoD session with baseline SWS. EEG activity was significantly increased in the frequency range of 1–4 Hz and decreased in the frequency range of 4.5–10 Hz during SWS [interaction P < 0.0001, F(49,350) = 25.47, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s post hoc comparisons, Figure 4D]. SWA in the frequency range of 0.5–4 Hz was then calculated during the same period and compared with the “Sleep deprivation,” “No resident,” and “No contact” conditions. SWA was significantly increased in “No resident,” “No contact,” and SoD mice, although the increase of SWA in the SoD mice was significantly stronger than that in the “No resident” mice, but not in the “No contact” mice [P = 0.0176, F(3,20) = 4.258, one-way ANOVA followed by Dunnett’s post hoc comparisons, Figure 4E]. Finally, we calculated the 3-h SWAs in intruder mice for 12 h following SoD stress and found that the first 3-h SWA was significantly higher than the baseline SWA on the previous day [main effect P = 0.0005, F(1,28) = 15.66, interaction P = 0.0002, F(3,28) = 8.991, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s post hoc comparisons, Figure 4F].

Discussion

The present study explored the effect of acute SoD stress on the sleep/wake cycle in mice. We observed that SoD stress strongly promoted SWS over 9 h. Corticosterone, as an indicator of stress, was significantly increased in the blood plasma by SoD, confirming that SoD induced stress in the mice. The increase in SWS was associated with more transitions from wakefulness to SWS without changing the mean episode duration of SWS.

The effect of acute SoD stress on subsequent sleep amount was reported in several studies. One study showed strong SWS increase in mice after acute SoD stress by interaction for 1 h with mice from the same strain (Meerlo and Turek, 2001). By contrast, another study using the same procedure showed little effect of SoD on the SWS amount (Vaanholt et al., 2003). Due to the lack of information about the number and duration of attacks of the resident mice against the intruder mice, it is hard to explain the difference between the studies. Moreover, it was also reported that wakefulness is increased in the first 3 h after SoD followed by a sleep increase during later hours (Henderson et al., 2017). Highly aggressive male CD-1 mice were used in this study for the SoD of C57BL/6 mice during a 5-min interaction period followed by a 20-min period of olfactory, visual, and auditory contact between the resident and intruder mice. The initial increase of wakefulness in this study may be attributed to repeated painful attacks of the CD-1 mice against smaller C57BL/6 mice. Alternatively, the durations of physical and sensory stress (25 min) may be too short to induce sleep. Although variations in the aggressive behavior of the resident mice may account to some extent for the differences in the sleep/wake responses between the studies, sleep architecture, SWA, and corticosterone responses in socially defeated mice may also be influenced by other factors. For example, laboratory environment and time-of-the-day when the conflict was introduced may affect sleep and corticosterone parameters. Moreover, prior stress experiences of the intruder mice such as early life stress during maternal care, fighting between littermates, or low social ranking in the litter can also be important factors for outcomes after SoD stress (Wang et al., 2014; Peña et al., 2017). We designed a SoD stress protocol to minimize behavioral variations during SoD. In our protocol, intruder mice were separated from aggressive resident mice (CD-1 mice) when they exhibited submissive behaviors rather than by setting a fixed duration of interaction with the resident mice, preventing repeated and painful attacks against the submissive mouse. Moreover, physical interaction was conducted two, four or eight times to maintain a high level of SoD stress in the intruders during the session. We found that acute SoD stress, introduced at the end of the light period, increases SWS shortly after the SoD session. Moreover, the comparison of the specific effects of SoD and non-specific effects of the SoD procedure showed that a large portion of the sleep changes after SoD stress can be attributed to a homeostatic response to sleep deprivation. On the other hand, about half of the SWS increase is likely a specific response to SoD.

We also observed a strong increase in SWA after the SoD session (Figure 4E,F), consistent with previous reports in mice and rats (Meerlo et al., 1997; Meerlo and Turek, 2001; Kamphuis et al., 2015; Henderson et al., 2017). An increase in SWA is considered the hallmark of homeostatic sleep need in response to sleep deprivation (Suzuki et al., 2013; Dispersyn et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2018). The SWA increase, however, was stronger after the SoD condition than under sleep-disrupting control conditions without a CD-1 mouse. Moreover, SWS was still enhanced long after the SWA returned to the baseline level. These observations suggest that SWA is also affected by social stress and at least part of the SWS in response to SoD is independent of SWA.

Moreover, we observed large variations in the SWS increase after the SoD sessions, especially the ones with two and eight successive defeats (Figure 2D,E). It is well known that group-housed male mice establish a social hierarchy (Wang et al., 2014). As we did not control the social ranking of the intruder mice before the SoD session, it is possible that the SWS response of the intruder mice is affected by their SoD resilience.

The neuronal mechanisms of the sleep response to SoD stress are unknown. The mesolimbic dopamine system plays an important role in the development of depression-related behaviors after repeated SoD stress (Russo and Nestler, 2013). The mesolimbic system comprises dopaminergic projections from the ventral tegmental area to the nucleus accumbens, an area critical for reward and motivation (Graybiel, 2008; Russo and Nestler, 2013), and is also connected to the medial prefrontal cortex, amygdala, and hippocampus, allowing the system to integrate cognition, emotion, and action (Floresco, 2015). We and others recently described the role of the mesolimbic system in the gating of sleep by motivated behavior (Eban-Rothschild et al., 2016; Oishi et al., 2017a,b; Luo et al., 2018). Therefore, it is plausible that the mesolimbic system also mediates sleep responses to SoD stress.

Acute stress responses induce behavioral, sympathetic and neuroendocrine changes in animals to facilitate a fight or flight response (Chrousos, 2009). Sympathetic and neuroendocrine responses, including the increase of body temperature and heart rate and secretion of noradrenaline, adrenaline, and corticosteroids, usually dissipate within hours after acute SoD in rodents (Koolhaas et al., 1997, 2017). Sleep is known to inhibit glucocorticoid secretion in healthy men (Weitzman et al., 1983), whereas REMS disinhibition and dysregulation of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis are hallmarks of depression and chronic stress (Nollet et al., 2019; Steiger and Pawlowski, 2019). The increased SWS after SoD stress in our study may facilitate the termination of stress responses. Moreover, SWS induction may also be beneficial for restoration of brain homeostasis after the stress response (Xie et al., 2013; Ding et al., 2016). By contrast, REMS is considered to have an important function in the processing of emotional memory of adverse experiences to promote emotional and mental recovery (Suchecki et al., 2012; Goldstein and Walker, 2014). Only a moderate REMS increase was observed during a period 6–9 h after SoD sessions with four defeats (Figure 2A). Most, if not all, of the REMS increase, however, may be attributed to non-specific sleep deprivation during the SoD session and thus, a beneficial effect of REMS after SoD remains unclear.

In summary, our study revealed strong SWS responses to acute SoD stress in submissive male mice attempting to appease their aggressive counterpart, when painful attacks on the submissive mice were prevented. Our SoD stress model may be useful for studying the mechanisms and functions of sleep in response to social stress.

Data Availability

All datasets generated for this study are included in the manuscript and/or the supplementary files.

Author Contributions

SF and ML designed the experiments. SF, MKK, XZ, and MK collected and analyzed the data. SF and ML wrote the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank all of the laboratory members for technical assistance, discussions, and comments.

Footnotes

Funding. This work was supported by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science [Grant in-Aid for Scientific Research B (Grant No. 17H02215) to ML]; the Japan Science and Technology Agency [CREST grant (Grant No. JPMJCR1655) to ML]; the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology (MEXT) of Japan [Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas “Living in Space” (Grant Nos. 15H05935, 15K21745, and 18H04966) and “WillDynamics” (Grant No. 17H06047) to ML]; the World Premier International Research Center Initiative (WPI) from MEXT (to ML); and a research grant of the Astellas Foundation for Research on Metabolic Disorders (to ML).

References

  1. Berton O., McClung C. A., DiLeone R. J., Krishnan V., Renthal W., Russo S. J., et al. (2006). Essential role of BDNF in the mesolimbic dopamine pathway in social defeat stress. Science 311 864–868. 10.1126/science.1120972 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Charney D. S., Manji H. K. (2004). Life stress, genes, and depression: multiple pathways lead to increased risk and new opportunities for intervention. Sci. STKE 2004:re5. 10.1126/stke.2252004re5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Chrousos G. P. (2009). Stress and disorders of the stress system. Nat. Rev. Endocrinol. 5 374–381. 10.1038/nrendo.2009.106 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ding F., O’Donnell J., Xu Q., Kang N., Goldman N., Nedergaard M. (2016). Changes in the composition of brain interstitial ions control the sleep-wake cycle. Science 352 550–555. 10.1126/science.aad4821 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Dispersyn G., Sauvet F., Gomez-Merino D., Ciret S., Drogou C., Leger D., et al. (2017). The homeostatic and circadian sleep recovery responses after total sleep deprivation in mice. J. Sleep Res. 26 531–538. 10.1111/jsr.12541 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Eban-Rothschild A., Giardino W. J., de Lecea L. (2017). To sleep or not to sleep: neuronal and ecological insights. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 44 132–138. 10.1016/j.conb.2017.04.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Eban-Rothschild A., Rothschild G., Giardino W. J., Jones J. R., de Lecea L. (2016). VTA dopaminergic neurons regulate ethologically relevant sleep–wake behaviors. Nat. Neurosci. 19 1356–1366. 10.1038/nn.4377 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Floresco S. B. (2015). The nucleus accumbens: an interface between cognition, emotion, and action. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 66 25–52. 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115159 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Golden S. A., Covington H. E., Berton O., Russo S. J. (2011). A standardized protocol for repeated social defeat stress in mice. Nat. Protoc. 6 1183–1191. 10.1038/nprot.2011.361 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Goldstein A. N., Walker M. P. (2014). The role of sleep in emotional brain function. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 10 679–708. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153716 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Graybiel A. M. (2008). Habits, rituals, and the evaluative brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 31 359–387. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112851 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Henderson F., Vialou V., El Mestikawy S., Fabre V. (2017). Effects of social defeat stress on sleep in mice. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 11:227. 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00227 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Henriques-Alves A. M., Queiroz C. M. (2015). Ethological evaluation of the effects of social defeat stress in mice: beyond the social interaction ratio. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:364. 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00364 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Huhman K. L. (2006). Social conflict models: can they inform us about human psychopathology? Horm. Behav. 50 640–646. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.06.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Kamphuis J., Lancel M., Koolhaas J. M., Meerlo P. (2015). Deep sleep after social stress: NREM sleep slow-wave activity is enhanced in both winners and losers of a conflict. Brain Behav. Immun. 47 149–154. 10.1016/j.bbi.2014.12.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Kataoka N., Hioki H., Kaneko T., Nakamura K. (2014). Psychological stress activates a dorsomedial hypothalamus-medullary raphe circuit driving brown adipose tissue thermogenesis and hyperthermia. Cell Metab. 20 346–358. 10.1016/j.cmet.2014.05.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Koolhaas J. M., de Boer S. F., Buwalda B., Meerlo P. (2017). Social stress models in rodents: towards enhanced validity. Neurobiol. Stress 6 104–112. 10.1016/j.ynstr.2016.09.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Koolhaas J. M., Meerlo P., De Boer S. F., Strubbe J. H., Bohus B. (1997). The temporal dynamics of the stress response. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 21 775–782. 10.1016/S0149-7634(96)00057-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Krishnan V., Han M.-H., Graham D. L., Berton O., Renthal W., Russo S. J., et al. (2007). Molecular adaptations underlying susceptibility and resistance to social defeat in brain reward regions. Cell 131 391–404. 10.1016/j.cell.2007.09.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Luo Y.-J., Li Y.-D., Wang L., Yang S.-R., Yuan X.-S., Wang J., et al. (2018). Nucleus accumbens controls wakefulness by a subpopulation of neurons expressing dopamine D1 receptors. Nat. Commun. 9:1576. 10.1038/s41467-018-03889-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Meerlo P., Easton A., Bergmann B. M., Turek F. W. (2001). Restraint increases prolactin and REM sleep in C57BL/6J mice but not in BALB/cJ mice. Am. J. Physiol. Reg. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 281 R846–R854. 10.1152/ajpregu.2001.281.3.R846 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Meerlo P., De Boer S. F., Koolhaas J. M., Daan S., Van Den Hoofdakker R. H. (1996a). Changes in daily rhythms of body temperature and activity after a single social defeat in rats. Physiol. Behav. 59 735–739. 10.1016/0031-9384(95)02182-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Meerlo P., Overkamp G. J., Benning M. A., Koolhaas J. M., Van Den Hoofdakker R. H. (1996b). Long-term changes in open field behaviour following a single social defeat in rats can be reversed by sleep deprivation. Physiol. Behav. 60 115–119. 10.1016/0031-9384(95)02271-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Meerlo P., Overkamp G. J., Daan S., Van Den Hoofdakker R. H., Koolhaas J. M. (1996c). Changes in behaviour and body weight following a single or double social defeat in rats. Stress 1 21–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Meerlo P., Pragt B. J., Daan S. (1997). Social stress induces high intensity sleep in rats. Neurosci. Lett. 225 41–44. 10.1016/S0304-3940(97)00180-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Meerlo P., Turek F. W. (2001). Effects of social stimuli on sleep in mice: non-rapid-eye-movement (NREM) sleep is promoted by aggressive interaction but not by sexual interaction. Brain Res. 907 84–92. 10.1016/S0006-8993(01)02603-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Mormède P., Andanson S., Aupérin B., Beerda B., Guémené D., Malmkvist J., et al. (2007). Exploration of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal function as a tool to evaluate animal welfare. Physiol. Behav. 92 317–339. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Nollet M., Hicks H., McCarthy A. P., Wu H., Möller-Levet C. S., Laing E. E., et al. (2019). REM sleep’s unique associations with corticosterone regulation, apoptotic pathways, and behavior in chronic stress in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116 2733–2742. 10.1073/pnas.1816456116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Oishi Y., Suzuki Y., Takahashi K., Yonezawa T., Kanda T., Takata Y., et al. (2017a). Activation of ventral tegmental area dopamine neurons produces wakefulness through dopamine D2-like receptors in mice. Brain Struct. Funct. 222 2907–2915. 10.1007/s00429-017-1365-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Oishi Y., Xu Q., Wang L., Zhang B.-J., Takahashi K., Takata Y., et al. (2017b). Slow-wave sleep is controlled by a subset of nucleus accumbens core neurons in mice. Nat. Commun. 8:734. 10.1038/s41467-017-00781-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Oishi Y., Takata Y., Taguchi Y., Kohtoh S., Urade Y., Lazarus M. (2016). Polygraphic recording procedure for measuring sleep in mice. J. Vis. Exp. 107: e53678. 10.3791/53678 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Paxinos G., Franklin K. B. J. (2004). The Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates 4th Edn. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  33. Peña C. J., Kronman H. G., Walker D. M., Cates H. M., Bagot R. C., Purushothaman I., et al. (2017). Early life stress confers lifelong stress susceptibility in mice via ventral tegmental area OTX2. Science 356 1185–1188. 10.1126/science.aan4491 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Rampin C., Cespuglio R., Chastrette N., Jouvet M. (1991). Immobilisation stress induces a paradoxical sleep rebound in rat. Neurosci. Lett. 126 113–118. 10.1016/0304-3940(91)90532-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Riley V. (1960). Adaptation of orbital bleeding technic to rapid serial blood studies. Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med. 104 751–754. 10.3181/00379727-104-25975 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Russo S. J., Nestler E. J. (2013). The brain reward circuitry in mood disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14 609–625. 10.1038/nrn3381 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Rygula R., Abumaria N., Flügge G., Fuchs E., Rüther E., Havemann-Reinecke U. (2005). Anhedonia and motivational deficits in rats: impact of chronic social stress. Behav. Brain Res. 162 127–134. 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.03.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Sanford L. D., Suchecki D., Meerlo P. (2015). Stress, arousal, and sleep. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 25 379–410. 10.1007/7854_2014_314 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Sanford L. D., Yang L., Wellman L. L., Liu X., Tang X. (2010). Differential effects of controllable and uncontrollable footshock stress on sleep in mice. Sleep 33 621–630. 10.1093/sleep/33.5.621 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Steiger A., Pawlowski M. (2019). Depression and sleep. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 20:607. 10.3390/ijms20030607 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Suchecki D., Tiba P. A., Machado R. B. (2012). REM sleep rebound as an adaptive response to stressful situations. Front. Neurol. 3:41. 10.3389/fneur.2012.00041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Suzuki A., Sinton C. M., Greene R. W., Yanagisawa M. (2013). Behavioral and biochemical dissociation of arousal and homeostatic sleep need influenced by prior wakeful experience in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110 10288–10293. 10.1073/pnas.1308295110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Toyoda A. (2017). Social defeat models in animal science: what we have learned from rodent models. Anim. Sci. J. 88 944–952. 10.1111/asj.12809 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Tsankova N., Renthal W., Kumar A., Nestler E. J. (2007). Epigenetic regulation in psychiatric disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8 355–367. 10.1038/nrn2132 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Tsankova N. M., Berton O., Renthal W., Kumar A., Neve R. L., Nestler E. J. (2006). Sustained hippocampal chromatin regulation in a mouse model of depression and antidepressant action. Nat. Neurosci. 9 519–525. 10.1038/nn1659 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Vaanholt L. M., Turek F. W., Meerlo P. (2003). Beta-endorphin modulates the acute response to a social conflict in male mice but does not play a role in stress-induced changes in sleep. Brain Res. 978 169–176. 10.1016/S0006-8993(03)02805-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. van Kampen M., Kramer M., Hiemke C., Flügge G., Fuchs E. (2002). The chronic psychosocial stress paradigm in male tree shrews: evaluation of a novel animal model for depressive disorders. Stress 5 37–46. 10.1080/102538902900012396 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Von Frijtag J. C., Reijmers L. G. J. E., Van der Harst J. E., Leus I. E., Van den Bos R., Spruijt B. M. (2000). Defeat followed by individual housing results in long-term impaired reward- and cognition-related behaviours in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 117 137–146. 10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00300-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Wang F., Kessels H. W., Hu H. (2014). The mouse that roared: neural mechanisms of social hierarchy. Trends Neurosci. 37 674–682. 10.1016/j.tins.2014.07.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Wang Z., Ma J., Miyoshi C., Li Y., Sato M., Ogawa Y., et al. (2018). Quantitative phosphoproteomic analysis of the molecular substrates of sleep need. Nature 558 435–439. 10.1038/s41586-018-0218-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Weber F., Dan Y. (2016). Circuit-based interrogation of sleep control. Nature 538 51–59. 10.1038/nature19773 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Weitzman E. D., Zimmerman J. C., Czeisler C. A., Ronda J. (1983). Cortisol secretion is inhibited during sleep in normal man. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 56 352–358. 10.1210/jcem-56-2-352 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Xie L., Kang H., Xu Q., Chen M. J., Liao Y., Thiyagarajan M., et al. (2013). Sleep drives metabolite clearance from the adult brain. Science 342 373–377. 10.1126/science.1241224 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

All datasets generated for this study are included in the manuscript and/or the supplementary files.


Articles from Frontiers in Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES