Skip to main content
The Journal of Nutrition logoLink to The Journal of Nutrition
. 2009 Apr 29;139(6):1253S–1256S. doi: 10.3945/jn.108.097956

Fructose and Satiety

Timothy H Moran 1,*
PMCID: PMC6459054  PMID: 19403706

Abstract

A role for the increased intake of dietary fructose in general and high-fructose corn syrup (HFCS) in particular in the current obesity epidemic has been proposed. Consumed fructose and glucose have different rates of gastric emptying, are differentially absorbed from the gastrointestinal tract, result in different endocrine profiles, and have different metabolic fates, providing multiple opportunities for the 2 saccharides to differentially affect food intake. The consequences of fructose and glucose on eating have been studied under a variety of experimental situations in both model systems and man. The results have been inconsistent, and the particular findings appear to depend on the timing of saccharide administration or ingestion relative to a test meal situation, whether the saccharides are administered as pure sugars or as components of a dietary preload, and the overall volume of the preload. These factors rather than intrinsic differences in the saccharides' ability to induce satiety appear to carry many of the differential effects on food intake that have been found. On balance, the case for fructose being less satiating than glucose or HFCS being less satiating than sucrose is not compelling.

Introduction

The role of high-fructose corn syrup (HFCS) in the current obesity epidemic is uncertain. The timing of the increase in the prevalence of obesity coincides with the increased use of HFCS in the diet and especially in soft drinks and with an overall increase in daily energy consumption, leading to suggestions that this dietary shift may be contributing to the obesity epidemic (13). Yet, HFCS has been primarily substituted for sucrose in such beverages, and HFCS and sucrose are not substantially different in monosaccharide content (4).

Increased ingestion of HFCS-sweetened beverages could result in overall higher energy intake because of poor compensation for the ingestion of such palatable high-energy liquids. In support of such an increase, a direct evaluation of the effects of HFCS-sweetened soda on overall energy intake indicated that scheduled HFCS ingestion resulted in increased overall energy intake and body weight over the 3-wk study period (5). However, in this study, it was not clear whether this was a function of the increased fructose consumed or simply an outcome of the ingestion of a relatively high-energy sweetened beverage.

Chronic access to saccharide solutions in general has been associated with increased food intake and body weight gain in rodent studies. Kanarek and Orthen-Gambill (6) reported that 50 d access to 32% glucose, sucrose, or fructose resulted in excess energy intake, weight gain, and increased fat deposition in rats. Rats with fructose access gained the most weight, and both the sucrose and fructose groups had decreased ability to clear a glucose load at the end of the study. In a more recent study, Jurgens et al. (7) demonstrated that mice with access to a 15% fructose solution gained significantly more weight and had significantly higher percentages of body fat than mice with access to a 10% sucrose solution or to an aspartame-sweetened water. Interpretations from these later data are complicated by the different concentrations of sucrose and fructose and the absence of a glucose-only condition.

Differential endocrine consequences of fructose versus glucose ingestion

Glucose and fructose ingestion can result in very different endocrine and metabolic states (8). Teff et al. (9) have provided the most complete data available on 24-h plasma profiles of a variety of endocrine parameters following meals in which 30% of the total energy intake was in the form of free glucose or free fructose. Consistent with the known effects of glucose and fructose on insulin secretion, the high-glucose diet elevated plasma glucose and insulin significantly more than did the high-fructose diet. High glucose also resulted in higher leptin levels, which were maintained throughout most of the day, a finding likely secondary to the difference in glucose and insulin secretions. High glucose also resulted in greater postprandial decreases in plasma ghrelin levels. This overall profile of decreased elevations in the adiposity signals insulin and leptin and smaller postprandial decreases in the orexigenic peptide ghrelin has been suggested to have the potential to result in decreased satiety and increased food intake during long-term fructose consumption.

Palatability and long-term preferences

Although both the fat deposition and endocrine profile data suggest the possibility that fructose may be less satiating than glucose or other saccharides, the issue is whether fructose is actually overconsumed or is less effective at reducing intake of other dietary energy sources. Comparisons between fructose and glucose ingestion have been made on many levels. In rats, brief access tests in which satiety influences are minimized have demonstrated a small but consistent increased rate of ingestion of fructose relative to glucose suggestive of increased fructose palatability or preferences (10). Such a finding is consistent with results from measures of perceived sweetness of the sugars dating back to 1925 (11). However, longer ingestion tests that compared the overall ingestion of saccharide solutions provided a different profile. In tests in which saccharide solutions were always available, although such availability tended to produce greater weight gain overall, rats consumed more glucose, maltose, and sucrose relative to fructose, suggesting that factors other than relative sweetness or palatability were ultimately controlling intake (12).

Fructose and glucose satiating potential: gastrointestinal differences

Short-term effects of glucose and fructose on subsequent ingestion have been evaluated in a variety of testing situations. The results have been mixed, with some data suggesting differences, but others have found similar satiating potential for the 2 saccharides. The differences may relate to differences in the testing protocol or experimental subjects.

Their differential intestinal absorption provided the basis for examinations of the relative effects of glucose and fructose on rates of gastric emptying and subsequent food consumption in nonhuman primates. Glucose is mainly absorbed from the gastrointestinal tract by the sodium-dependent glucose transporter-1, whereas fructose is absorbed by a facilitated diffusion via glucose transporter-5 (13). Glucose transporter-2, a low-affinity transporter, plays some role with both fructose and glucose via facilitative diffusion (13). These differential transport mechanisms result in different rates of gastric emptying for fructose and glucose.

The gastric emptying of glucose solutions is characterized by a 2-phase process with the initial rapid rate of emptying affected primarily by the gastric volume and rate of stomach filling (14). This is followed by a slower linear phase of emptying that is sensitive to the glucose concentration such that emptying is slower with more concentrated solutions, allowing an equivalent rate of glucose delivery from the stomach to the intestine over a wide range of glucose concentrations (14). The gastric emptying of fructose is more rapid and less linear. Emptying does slow with increasing fructose concentration, but the overall rate of delivery of fructose from the stomach to the intestine is almost twice as rapid as with glucose (15).

These differences in the dynamics of gastric emptying were verified in monkeys using a dye dilution method and were then used as a test for the relevance of potential preabsorptive and absorptive events in the production of satiety (15). Rhesus monkeys received gastric preloads of glucose or fructose immediately before daily 4-h access, and the effects on cumulative food intake were monitored. Although the overall effects on 4-h intake did not differ, the dynamics of feeding through the 4-h period were differentially affected. Despite a more rapid delivery from the stomach to the intestine, fructose had less effect on food intake at early time points. Monkeys receiving fructose preloads had more rapid rates of consumption during the first 60 min of the test, suggesting the possibility that fructose is less satiating than glucose. However, this period of more rapid ingestion was followed by a period in which the ingestion rate was reduced relative to that following the glucose preloads. Overall, these data suggest differences in the satiating ability of fructose and glucose, differences that are related to temporal effect rather than to their overall ability to affect intake.

Differential time courses of the effects of fructose and glucose preloads on subsequent intake have also been found by other investigators in rodent studies. Warwick and Weingarten (16) examined the effects of glucose and fructose preloads on scheduled food intake when the preloads were administered at various intervals before the meal. In their hands, glucose and fructose resulted in equivalent suppression in intake at short intervals, but fructose had a greater suppressive effect than glucose as the intervals between the preload and meal were extended. They suggested that responses to preloads were differentially affected by test condition and the state of the animal and that there was no overall tendency for one saccharide to be more or less satiating than the other.

Satiety comparisons in human subjects

Comparisons of the ability of fructose and glucose to reduce test meal intake have also been studied in experiments with human subjects. Rodin and her colleagues (1719) carried out a series of experiments comparing the satiating effects of fructose and glucose preloads. Fifty-gram preloads of fructose and glucose had a differential effect on buffet test meal intake when administered 2 h before lunch. In these experiments, fructose reduced intake by 500 kcal (2092 kJ) more than did glucose and by 200 kcal (837 kJ) relative to a water preload, suggesting an overall appetite suppressive effect of fructose.

Follow-up studies demonstrated similar greater suppressive effects of fructose preloads in lean and obese subjects, and the authors suggested a role for plasma insulin in the differential effects: glucose loads elevated plasma glucose and insulin levels, whereas the fructose preload did not (18). Importantly, the difference in subsequent intake was lost when the fructose and glucose preloads were given as a part of a mixed breakfast meal rather than as single-nutrient constituents (20).

Guss et al. (21) examined the effects of glucose and fructose solutions on food intake and gastric emptying in nonobese women. In this study, the effects of 2 different concentrations of the saccharides (1% and 10%; total doses 5 and 50 g of sugar) were examined at 2 different intervals between the preload and the test meal. As in the nonhuman primate studies, fructose emptied from the stomach more rapidly than glucose as determined by the disappearance of a radioactive tracer from the stomach. In these studies, the effects of the preloads on subsequent intake were relatively small. In all cases the satiating efficiency (ratio of the drop in test meal intake to energy content of the preload) was <1. At the 30-min delay, both 10% fructose and 10% glucose significantly reduced intake relative to the more dilute solutions. At the 135-min interval, the 10% fructose but not the 10% glucose, significantly reduced intake relative to a water control, again suggesting differential effects of the 2 saccharides on the timing of satiating influences.

The idea of differential timing affecting the abilities of glucose and fructose to influence subsequent intake was directly assessed by Rodin in an experiment in which the preloads were given 38 min rather than 2 h before the test meal (17). The findings from the earlier experiments with a 2-h delay were replicated: fructose produced significantly greater reductions in test meal intake than did glucose. Guss et al. (21) have suggested that a remaining difference between the paradigms may explain the different results across studies. In the Rodin experiments, subjects were food deprived from the evening before, whereas in the Guss experiments, subjects consumed a standard breakfast.

The addition of glucose or fructose to a cereal preload produced a trend for differential timing of effects (22). Equicaloric cereal preloads containing either fructose or glucose resulted in equivalent effects on intake in a test meal scheduled 30 min following the preload, but there was a trend for a greater effect of the fructose on test meal intake 2 h later. The fructose preload resulted in 70.2% compensation, whereas the compensation in response to the glucose preload was only 42.5% at that time point.

Satiety effect of saccharide combinations

Recent studies have concentrated on comparisons between HFCS and sucrose or varying glucose-to-fructose ratios. These studies have produced mixed results. Work from Andersen et al. has demonstrated that 300-kcal (1255 kJ, 75 g of sugar) oral preloads have different effects on subsequent food intake depending on the relative glucose:fructose concentrations (23,24). In these experiments, 1 kcal/mL (4.18 MJ/L) liquid preloads containing varying mixtures of glucose and fructose or HFCS were consumed 80 min before a test meal of pizza. Overall, the 80% glucose:20% fructose and sucrose preloads produced the greatest effects on subsequent intake. The 20% glucose:80% fructose and the 35% glucose:65% fructose preloads were less satiating, and the 50% glucose:50% fructose and HFCS preloads produced intermediate effects. Plasma glucose, insulin, and ghrelin were also measured in these experiments. Consistent with prior data, solutions with the higher glucose concentrations had the greatest effect on plasma glucose and insulin (25). In contrast to the results from the Teff experiment, all solutions lowered plasma ghrelin except for the 80% glucose:20% fructose solution, suggesting that reductions in plasma ghrelin were not mediating the greater inhibitory feeding actions of the solutions with the higher glucose concentrations.

Other investigators have not found differences between sucrose and HFCS on subsequent food intake. Soenen and Westerterp-Plantenga (26) report that although there were time course differences in appetite ratings in response to sucrose and HFCS preloads, there were no differences in test meal intake or in plasma profiles of glucose, ghrelin, insulin, or glucagon-like peptide-1. Similarly, Drewnowski et al. (27) report no differences between sucrose- and HFCS-sweetened soft drinks on hunger and satiety profiles or test meal intake.

The disparity among the experimental results suggests that aspects of method may have a significant impact on the degree to which different energy-containing sweeteners affect food intake. The differences in timing of the preload relative to the test meal have already been discussed. With a few exceptions, it appears to be the case that glucose and fructose affect food intake with different dynamics. The inhibitory actions of fructose appear to be more long lasting. Potential reasons for the more lasting effects of fructose could depend on differences in gastric emptying or the rapid rise in plasma glucose and insulin in response to glucose preloads playing a short-term inhibitory role. In addition, the prolonged effects with fructose could be secondary to metabolic factors or, as raised by some investigators, a negative influence on intake caused by gastrointestinal malaise from the tendency for concentrated fructose solutions to draw water into the GI tract (23,28,29). Fructose absorption from the GI tract is improved in the presence of glucose (28), providing a potential explanation for why fructose effects appear to disappear when the fructose:glucose mixtures are used or when fructose is given as part of a mixed meal.

Other factors that may have influenced results are the overall energy content of the preloads and the volumes in which they were administered. Energy loads in human experiments ranged from a low of 214 kcal (895 kJ) to a high of 500 kcal (2092 kJ). The greater the energy load, the more likely it seems that differences may be found, as smaller energy preloads have little effect on subsequent ingestion. Effects of saccharide concentration across the various sugars have not been systematically studied.

The original question was: is fructose or HFCS less satiating than glucose or sucrose? Any differences seem to arise from experiments in which fructose is given alone or in relative concentrations that are not normally found in sweetened beverages or other foods. Thus, the relevance of such results to the consequences of consuming HFCS is unclear. Another issue of importance to the impact of increased fructose or HFCS consumption is whether results from short-term studies demonstrating different satiating potentials predict long-term regulatory consequences. Alterations in intake at one meal can produce compensatory changes in subsequent food intake such that the long-term consequences are difficult to predict (30). Only studies of effects at individual meals over the longer term would provide such information. Given the overall lack of consistency across experimental designs (Table 1), the questionable relevance of data using fructose at concentrations greater than normally consumed, and issues about generalization from individual assessments, the case for a lower satiating efficacy of fructose as a contributor to the current obesity epidemic is simply not compelling.

TABLE 1.

Summary of results from preload studies

Study (ref.) Design Mixed or Pure Subjects Result
Moran and McHugh (15) Preload Pure solutions Nonhuman primates Differential timing effects
Warwick and Weingarten (16) Preload Pure solutions Rats Differential timing effect
Rodin et al. (1720) Preload Pure solutions Humans Fructose > glucose
Guss et al. (21) Preload Pure solutions Humans Differential timing effects
Stewart et al. (22) Preload Mixed in cereal Humans Differential timing effects
Anderson et al. (2325) Preload Mixed fructose/glucose concentrations Humans Glucose > fructose
Soenen and Westerterp-Plantenga (26) Preload Sucrose vs. HFCS Humans No differences
Monsivais et al. (27) Preload Sucrose vs. HFCS Humans No differences

Other articles in this supplement include references (3140).

Footnotes

1

Published in a supplement to The Journal of Nutrition. Presented at the conference “The State-of-the-Science on Dietary Sweeteners Containing Fructose,” held in Beltsville, MD, March 18–19 2008. The conference was cosponsored by the Technical Committee on Carbohydrates of the International Life Sciences Institute North America (ILSI North America) and the USDA, Agricultural Research Service. The views expressed in these papers are not necessarily those of the USDA, the Agricultural Research Service, or the Supplement Coordinators. Supplement Coordinators for this supplement were David M. Klurfeld, USDA, Agricultural Research Service, and Molly Kretsch, USDA, Agricultural Research Service. Supplement Coordinator Disclosure: D. Klurfeld and M. Kretsch: no relationships to disclose.

Literature Cited

  • 1. Bray GA, Nielsen SJ, Popkin BM.. Consumption of high-fructose corn syrup in beverages may play a role in the epidemic of obesity. Am J Clin Nutr. 2004;79:537–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Elliott SS, Keim NL, Stern JS, Teff K, Havel PJ.. Fructose, weight gain, and the insulin resistance syndrome. Am J Clin Nutr. 2002;76:911–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Bray GA.. How bad is fructose? Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;86:895–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Forshee RA, Storey Ml, Allison DB, Glinsmann WH, Hein GL, Linebeack DR, Miller SA, Nichlas TA, Weaver GA, White JS.. A critical examination of the evidence relating high fructose corn syrup and weight gain. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr. 2007;47:561–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Tordoff MG, Alleva AM.. Effect of drinking soda sweetened with aspartame or high-fructose corn syrup on food intake and body weight. Am J Clin Nutr. 1990;51:963–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Kanarek RB, Orthen-Gambill N.. Differential effects of sucrose, fructose and glucose on carbohydrate-induced obesity in rats. J Nutr. 1982;112:1546–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Jurgens H, Haass W, Castaneda TR, Schurmann A, Koebnick C, Dombrowski F, Otto B, Nawrocki AR, Scherer PE, et al. . Consuming fructose-sweetened beverages increases body adiposity in mice. Obes Res. 2005;13:1146–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Stanhope KL, Friffen SC, Bair BR, Swarbrick MM, Keim NL, Havel PJ.. Twenty-four hour endocrine and metabolic profiles following consumption of high-fructose corn syrup-, sucrose-, fructose-, and glucose-sweetened beverages with meals. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;87:1194–203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Teff KL, Elliott SS, Tschop M, Kieffer TJ, Rader D, Heiman M, Townsend RR, Keim NL, D'Alessio D, Havel PJ.. Dietary fructose reduces circulating insulin and leptin, attenuates postprandial suppression of ghrelin, and increases triglycerides in women. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2004;89:2963–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Davis JD.. The effectiveness of some sugars in stimulating licking behavior in the rat. Physiol Behav. 1973;11:39–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Biester A, Wood MW, Wahlin CS.. Carbohydrate studies. I. The relative sweetness of pure sugars. Am J Physiol. 1925;73:387–95. [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Richter CP.. Six commom sugars as tools for the study of appetite for sugar. In: Weiffenbach JW. ed. Taste and Development. Bethesda: DHEW Publications; 1977, 387–398. [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Drozdowski LA, Thomson AB.. Intestinal sugar transport. World J Gastroenterol. 2006;12:1657–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Moran TH, Knipp S, Schwartz GJ.. Gastric and duodenal features of meals mediate controls of liquid gastric emptying during fill in rhesus monkeys. Am J Physiol. 1999;277:R1282–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Moran TH, McHugh PR.. Distinctions among three sugars in their effects on gastric emptying and satiety. Am J Physiol. 1981;241:R25–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Warwick ZS, Weingarten HP.. Dynamics of intake suppression after a preload: role of calories, volume, and macronutrients. Am J Physiol. 1994;266:R1314–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Rodin J.. Comparative effects of fructose, aspartame, glucose, and water preloads on calorie and macronutrient intake. Am J Clin Nutr. 1990;51:428–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Rodin J, Reed D, Jamner L.. Metabolic effects of fructose and glucose: implications for food intake. Am J Clin Nutr. 1988;47:683–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Spitzer L, Rodin J.. Effects of fructose and glucose preloads on subsequent food intake. Appetite. 1987;8:135–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Rodin J.. Effects of pure sugar vs. mixed starch fructose loads on food intake. Appetite. 1991;17:213–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Guss JL, Kissileff HR, Pi-Sunyer FX.. Effects of glucose and fructose solutions on food intake and gastric emptying in nonobese women. Am J Physiol. 1994;267:R1537–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Stewart SL, Black R, Wolever DM, Anderson GH.. The relationship between the glycemic response to breakfast cereals and subjective appetite and food intake. Nutr Res. 1997;17:1249–60. [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Akhavan T, Anderson GH.. Effects of glucose-to-fructose ratios in solutions on subjective satiety, food intake, and satiety hormones in young men. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;86:1354–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Anderson GH, Catherine NL, Woodend DM, Wolever TM.. Inverse association between the effect of carbohydrates on blood glucose and subsequent short-term food intake in young men. Am J Clin Nutr. 2002;76:1023–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Anderson GH, Woodend D.. Effect of glycemic carbohydrates on short-term satiety and food intake. Nutr Rev. 2003;61:S17–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Soenen S, Westerterp-Plantenga MS.. No differences in satiety or energy intake after high-fructose corn syrup, sucrose, or milk preloads. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;86:1586–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Monsivais P, Perrigue MM, Drewnowski A.. Sugars and satiety: does the type of sweetener make a difference? Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;86:116–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Rumessen JJ, Gudmand-Hoyer E.. Absorption capacity of fructose in healthy adults. Comparison with sucrose and its constituent monosaccharides. Gut. 1986;27:1161–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Henry RR, Crapo PA, Thorburn AW.. Current issues in fructose metabolism. Annu Rev Nutr. 1991;11:21–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. French S.. Effects of dietary fat and carbohydrate on appetite vary depending upon site and structure. Br J Nutr. 2004;92: Suppl 1:S23–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Jones JM.. Dietary Sweeteners containing fructose: overview of a workshop on the state of the science. J Nutr. 2009;139:1210S–3S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Borra ST, Bouchoux A.. Effects of science and the media on consumer perceptions about dietary sugars. J Nutr. 2009;139:1214S–8S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. White JS.. Misconceptions about high-fructose corn syrup: is it uniquely responsible for obesity, reactive dicarbonyl compounds, and advanced glycation endproducts? J Nutr. 2009;139:1219S–27S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Marriott BP, Cole N, Lee E.. National Estimates of Dietary Fructose Intake Increased from 1977 to 2004 in the United States. J Nutr. 2009;139:1228S–35S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Stanhope KL, Havel PJ.. Fructose consumption: considerations for future research on its effects on adipose distribution, lipid metabolism, and insulin sensitivity in humans. J Nutr. 2009;139:1236S–41S. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Angelopoulos TJ, Lowndes J, Zukley L, Melanson KJ, Nguyen V, Huffman A, Rippe JM.. The Effect of High-Fructose Corn Syrup Consumption on Triglycerides and Uric Acid. J Nutr. 2009;139:1242S–5S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Livesey G.. Fructose Ingestion: Dose-Dependent Responses in Health Research. J Nutr. 2009;139:1246S–52S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Schaefer EJ, Gleason JA, Dansinger ML.. Dietary Fructose and Glucose Differentially Affect Lipid and Glucose Homeostasis. J Nutr. 2009;139:1257S–62S. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. John P.. Bantle. Dietary Fructose and Metabolic Syndrome and Diabetes. J Nutr. 2009;139:1263S–8S. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Murphy SP.. The state of the science on dietary sweeteners containing fructose: summary and issues to be resolved. J Nutr. 2009;139:1269S–70S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Nutrition are provided here courtesy of American Society for Nutrition

RESOURCES