Skip to main content
Frontiers in Psychiatry logoLink to Frontiers in Psychiatry
. 2019 Apr 5;10:213. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00213

Improvement of Emotional Empathy and Cluster B Personality Disorder Symptoms Associated With Decreased Cocaine Use Severity

Matthias Vonmoos 1,*,, Christoph Eisenegger 2,†,, Oliver G Bosch 1,3, Katrin H Preller 1, Lea M Hulka 1, Markus Baumgartner 4, Erich Seifritz 3,5, Boris B Quednow 1,5,*,
PMCID: PMC6459943  PMID: 31024365

Abstract

Aims: Chronic cocaine users display impaired social cognitive abilities, reduced prosocial behavior, and pronounced cluster B personality disorder (PD) symptoms all contributing to their social dysfunctions in daily life. These social dysfunctions have been proposed as a major factor for maintenance and relapse of stimulant use disorders in general. However, little is known about the reversibility of social cognitive deficits and socially problematic personality facets when stimulant use is reduced or ceased. Therefore, we examined the relation between changing intensity of cocaine use and the development of sociocognitive functioning and cluster B PD symptomatology over the course of 1 year.

Methods: Social cognition, social decision-making, and cluster B PD symptoms were assessed in 38 cocaine users (19 with increased and 19 with decreased use) and 48 stimulant-naive healthy controls at baseline and at 1-year follow-up. Cocaine use severity was objectively determined by quantitative 6-month hair analyses. The categorization of the two cocaine user groups was based on a combination of absolute (± 0.5 ng/mg) and relative (± 10%) changes in the cocaine hair concentration between baseline and the 1-year follow-up. Social cognition was assessed using the Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET) and the Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition (MASC). A combined Distribution/Dictator Game was applied for assessing social decision-making. Cluster B PD symptoms were measured by a Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Disorders (SCID-II) PD questionnaire according to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV).

Results: Increased cocaine use was linked to worsened empathy, while decreased cocaine use went along with improved emotional empathy. Moreover, whereas decreased cocaine use was associated with reduced severity of self-reported cluster B PD symptoms, these symptoms remained largely stable in increasers. In contrast to a significant reduction of prosocial behavior at baseline in the combined cocaine user group, specifically decreasers were not statistically distinguishable from controls at the follow-up.

Conclusions: Sociocognitive deficits and cluster B PD symptoms of chronic cocaine users are adaptable over time as they covary with the increase or decrease in cocaine use. Hence, abstinence orientation and training of social cognition and interaction might improve social functioning, and should therefore be important therapeutic elements in cocaine addiction treatment.

Keywords: cocaine, stimulants, social cognition, empathy, Theory-of-Mind, social decision-making, cognition, personality disorder

Introduction

Neurocognitive deficits such as impaired attention, memory, and executive functions related to chronic cocaine use are well documented (13) and a risk factor for poor treatment outcomes (4, 5). While some studies investigated the linkage between these neurocognitive deficits and cocaine abstinence (6), only one study yet investigated the longitudinal relationship between cognitive impairments and changing cocaine use (7). In sum, these studies indicate that basal cognitive deficits in cocaine users seem to be largely drug-induced, remain stable during the first weeks of abstinence but likely improve after some months (8).

While nonsocial cognitive functions have been studied well during the last two decades, the systematic assessment of sociocognitive functioning in cocaine users has only recently emerged. Per definition, the concept of social cognition comprises not only abilities enabling the dynamic interaction with our social environments and include emotional and mental perspective-taking functions such as emotion recognition, emotional empathy (EE), and Theory-of-Mind, but also interactive abilities such as social decision-making (SDM), moral behavior, and social network behavior (9, 10). As daily-life social functioning strongly depends on intact social cognition and as the deteriorative impact of sociocognitive impairments on development, progress, and prognosis on other psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia is well known (11), a close relationship between sociocognitive functioning and the origin and course of stimulant use disorders has been proposed (1215). Accordingly, we previously demonstrated smaller social networks (16), reduced EE (16), altered SDM (17), stronger detachment from social norms (14), and impaired emotion recognition from voices (18) in recreational and dependent cocaine user groups. Moreover, dependent cocaine users made more errors than controls in a video-based Theory-of-Mind task, with recreational cocaine users performing intermediate between the two groups (16). Finally, cocaine users show also blunted neuronal responses to implicit and explicit forms of social reward (19, 20). Notably, all these studies were implemented with a cross-sectional design, but no study has investigated the longitudinal development of sociocognitive functioning so far. Thus, it is unclear if sociocognitive impairments are predisposed or drug-induced and if they are reversible upon prolonged abstinence or reduction of drug use.

As social cognition is the sum of those processes that allow individuals to interact in interpersonal contexts (21), disturbed sociocognitive functioning leads to aberrant social behavior and, in excessive forms, to deviant personality characteristics and impaired interpersonal functioning (22, 23). Notably, cocaine-addicted individuals show an increased risk for concurrent cluster B personality disorders (PDs), mainly of the antisocial and borderline types (24, 25). A cluster B PD comorbidity is largely influential for cocaine addiction severity and treatment outcomes including pronounced executive function deficits (26), more intense cocaine intake, lower rates of treatment applications, and decreased probability of cocaine addiction remission (27, 28). Additionally, it was demonstrated that impulsivity and gambling decision-making, which are both closely related to cluster B PD pathologies (22, 29), covary with changes in the intensity of cocaine use over 1 year (30). Nonetheless, the longitudinal relation between cocaine use intensity and cluster B PD symptomatology has also not been investigated to date.

In sum, only little is known about the temporal dynamics between cocaine use intensity and sociocognitive functioning. Hence, in order to investigate whether the described sociocognitive impairments and comorbid cluster B PD symptomatology in chronic cocaine users are modulated by the increase or decrease in cocaine abuse, we performed a longitudinal study with an interval of 1 year. Thereby, we compared 48 psychostimulant-naive controls with 19 cocaine users with decreased use (decreasers) and 19 cocaine users with increased use (increasers) after a 1-year interval. To objectively assess the severity and change in cocaine use and to control for co-use of other drugs, we performed quantitative hair and urine toxicology analyses at baseline and follow-up. Considering our previous results from the present sample that changes in basal cognitive functions and impulsivity clearly covary with cocaine use intensity over time (7, 30), we hypothesized that escalating cocaine use is also associated with aggravation of sociocognitive impairments and more cluster B PD symptoms within 1 year. Vice versa, we also expected that reduced cocaine use is linked to a reduction of sociocognitive deficits and cluster B PD symptomatology. To test these hypotheses, we expect significant time × group interactions specifically between decreasers and increasers. Given that at baseline cocaine users displayed significant alterations in EE, social network size, prosocial behavior in money distribution games, Theory-of-Mind, and cluster B PD symptoms (14, 16, 17), the longitudinal analysis was focused solely on these parameters.

Methods

Participants

From a baseline sample of 234 participants (96 healthy stimulant-naive controls, 138 cocaine users) (3, 16, 17), 48 healthy stimulant-naive controls and 38 chronic cocaine users were included in the present longitudinal study. This subsample has been published twice previously but with different outcome measures (7, 30). From the baseline sample, 102 participants could not be measured at the follow-up because of unavailability (i.e., not responding to the invitation, loss of interest, lack of time, death), 27 participants had to be excluded from the final analyses as hair analyses revealed drug use not allowed by our exclusion criteria (e.g., polysubstance use, change in drug preferences), and 19 cocaine users did not meet our cocaine use criteria [see also the cocaine user group assignment below; for further recruitment and selection details, please see Ref. (7)].

At baseline, general exclusion criteria were clinically significant somatic diseases, neurological disorders, head injuries, family history of schizophrenia/obsessive-compulsive disorder/bipolar disorder, or any medication affecting the central nervous system. Additional exclusion criteria for controls were Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV) axis I psychiatric disorders (excluding nicotine dependence) and regular illegal drug use (>15 lifetime occasions, except for recreational cannabis use). Additional exclusion criteria for cocaine users were a history of heroin use, polysubstance use, or DSM-IV axis I psychiatric disorders (except for cocaine, nicotine, cannabis, and alcohol abuse/dependence, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and a previous episode of an affective disorder). At baseline, inclusion criteria for cocaine users were cocaine use of >0.5 g per month, cocaine as primary drug, and an abstinence duration of <6 months. Participants were asked to abstain from illegal substances for at least 72 h and from alcohol for 24 h before the test sessions. Compliance with these instructions was controlled by urine screenings (semiquantitative enzyme multiplied immunoassay method). The study was approved by the Cantonal Ethics Committee of Zurich. All participants provided written informed-consent statements and were compensated for their participation.

Cocaine User Group Assignment

The categorization of the two cocaine user groups was based on changes of cocaine concentration in hair samples as determined by liquid chromatography–tandem mass spectrometry [for technical details, see Ref. (3)]. If possible, 6-cm hair samples were drawn covering the previous drug use of approximately 6 months. Cocaine users were categorized based on a combination of absolute (±0.5 ng/mg) and relative (>10% increase/decrease) changes in the hair concentration of cocainetotal between baseline and the 1-year follow-up (7, 30, 31). According to these criteria, cocaine users were divided into three equally sized groups: 19 cocaine increasers [mean ± SD: +30.4 ± 61.9 ng/mg (+297%), range: +0.5 to +268.5 ng/mg (+20% to +5,374%)], 19 cocaine decreasers [−10.6 ± 26.7 ng/mg (−72%), −116.9 to −0.6 ng/mg (−100% to −12%)]), and 19 users with a relatively low and stable cocaine use pattern who did not meet both criteria [−0.1 ± 0.5 ng/mg (−2%), −1.9 to +0.5 ng/mg (−100% to +720%)], and, thus, were not further analyzed in this study [for further details, see Ref. (7)].

Procedure

At baseline, self-reported drug use was assessed with a structured and standardized Interview for Psychotropic Drug Consumption (32), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms were assessed with the ADHD Self-Rating Scale (ADHD-SR) (33), and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders (SCID-I) (34) was carried out by trained psychologists.

The test battery was assessed at baseline and follow-up and included the Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET) (35) assessing EE, the Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition (MASC) (36) for the measurement of Theory-of-Mind, a Distribution/Dictator Game (37, 38) for the determination of prosocial behavior, the Social Network Questionnaire (SNQ) (39) measuring the social network size, and the SCID-II questionnaire (40) in order to ascertain cluster B PD symptoms. More detailed test descriptions published already in our previous work (16, 17) are given in Methods S1.

Statistical Analysis

Effect sizes and power analyses were calculated with G*Power 3.1 (41). As our previous analyses showed an effect size of pη2 = 0.12 (Cohen’s f = 0.37) and a power of 99% for the significant interaction in the domain of working memory between decreasers and increasers (two groups, p < .05, two measurements) for the present sample (7), we assumed a more conservative effect size of pη2 = 0.06 (f = 0.25) and calculated a still acceptable power of 86% for the detection of significant interactions in sociocognitive functions in the present sample.

In order to reduce data quantity [see also Ref. (17)], we computed an SDM composite score that was derived by averaging z-transformed payoffs for the other player in the Distribution and Dictator Game (payoffs B) according to the means and standard deviations of the control group. Because of a strong correlation of the explicit and implicit EE scores from the MET in the total sample (r = 0.86, p < .001), we further integrated both parameters by adding them up into a single MET EE score. The SCID-II Cluster B symptom score was calculated by summing up the dimensional values from histrionic, narcissistic, borderline, and antisocial PD.

Group differences in demographic data and drug use patterns were analyzed by means of Pearson’s chi-squared tests, analyses of variance (ANOVA), or independent Student’s t-tests. For the longitudinal analysis and in order to investigate group differences over all groups, we performed a multiple linear regression (forced entry) with the test score change values (Δ = t2 − t1) as dependent variables and four preselected independent variables: age, sex, ADHS-SR score, and dummy-coded (zero/one) group contrasts. The two demographic variables were included because previous findings suggest a linkage between advancing age and fairness in stimulant users (17) and due to known gender effects in social cognition/functioning (42, 43). Moreover, because ADHD has previously been linked to cognitive and sociocognitive performance in cocaine users (3, 16, 44), this variable was further included as a predictor into the regression model. To compare the groups, cocaine increasers acted as the reference group. To further analyze test score changes within the single groups (value t2 vs. value t1), we applied dependent Student’s t-tests (tdep). To compare the effect of changing cocaine use, we applied independent Student’s t-tests (tind) between controls and a combined cocaine user sample (CCU = increasers + decreasers) at baseline as well as between controls, cocaine increasers, and cocaine decreasers at the follow-up. Notably, at baseline, cocaine increasers and decreasers showed comparable baseline values in all reported test parameters (MET, MASC, SDM, SNQ, SCID-II Cluster B) differing only with very small effect sizes (tind(32–35) = 0.05–0.34, p = .99–.74, d = 0.00–0.11). In the test parameter analysis, frequency data were analyzed by the Fisher–Freeman–Halton Exact Test (FET) (45). To test for test–retest effects, we applied the Pearson product-moment correlation analyses. The confirmatory statistical comparisons were carried out on a significance level of p < .05 (two-tailed).

Results

Demographic characteristics and drug use: As shown before (7, 30), the three experimental groups did not significantly differ regarding age, sex distribution, verbal IQ, years of education, length of study interval ( Table 1 ), and socioeconomic status (Table S1). Still, cocaine-using groups showed significantly higher BDI and ADHD-SR sum scores than controls at baseline (7, 30). Whereas at baseline both cocaine user groups showed comparable cocaine use severity, the cocainetotal hair concentrations for increasers (∼3-fold increase) and decreasers (reduction by the factor 3.5) were significantly different at follow-up. Moreover, hair data revealed a clear preference for cocaine use compared to other illegal drugs. Finally, in both user groups, 8 of 19 participants sought psychiatric or psychological treatment during the study interval. The other cocaine users did not report any related treatment between baseline and follow-up.

Table 1.

Demographic data and pattern of cocaine use.

Baseline (t1) 1-year follow-up (t2)m
Controls (n = 48) Cocaine Increaser (n = 19) Cocaine Decreaser (n = 19) F/χ²/T df, dferr p Controls (n = 48) Cocaine Increaser (n = 19) Cocaine Decreaser (n = 19) F/χ²/T df, dferr p
Age, years 30.3 (8.9) 31.5 (9.4) 31.4 (8.3) .20a 2,83 .82
Sex (f/m) 16/32 3/16 5/14 2.11b 2 .35
Verbal IQ (MWT-B)d 107.6 (10.0) 102.9 (9.7) 103.8 (7.1) 2.20a 2,83 .12
Education, years 10.8 (1.8) 10.4 (1.8) 10.0 (1.5) 1.30a 2,83 .28
ADHD-SR score (0-22) 7.7 (5.2) 13.5 (9.4)** 14.1 (6.8)** 8.83a 2,83 <.001
ADHD DSM IV (y/n)e 0/48 4/15 3/16 7.02b 2 .03
Weeks between t1 and t2 58.2 (10.1) 59.3 (12.1) 61.9 (14.5) .69a 2,83 .50
BDI score (0–63) 3.5 (3.3) 7.3 (8.0)* 8.7 (6.5)** 7.53a 2,83 <.001
BDI depression (y/n)g 0/48 1/18 1/18 2.59b 2 .27
Cocaine
 Times per weekh 1.6 (1.8) 1.0 (1.3) 1.17c 36 .25 1.1 (0.8) 0.3 (0.3) 3.85c 36 <.001
 Grams per weekh 2.0 (2.5) 1.7 (2.3) .41c 36 .68 1.6 (2.5) 0.4 (0.4) 2.18c 36 .04
 Years of use 7.0 (5.5) 8.2 (5.4) .68c 36 .50 8.9 (5.4) 9.7 (5.2) .45c 36 .65
 Age of cocaine onset 24.5 (8.1) 23.1 (5.2) .61c 36 .54
 Max. dose (g/day)k 4.7 (4.4) 5.9 (6.4) .71c 36 .48 3.7 (2.5) 3.1 (2.8) .63c 36 .53
 Cumulative dose (g)k 1182 (1635) 3698 (8585) 1.25c 36 .22 91 (119) 49 (89) 1.25c 36 .22
 Last consumption (days) 18.5 (25.1) 16.8 (14.6) .29c 36 .77 7.0 (6.3) 81.4 (145.1) 2.23c 36 .03
 Cocaine craving (0–70)i 19.8 (9.5) 17.7 (7.2) .79c 36 .44 20.5 (10.8) 15.8 (6.2) 1.66c 36 .11
 Hair analysis (ng/mg)l
  Cocainetotal 10.3 (29.2) 14.9 (32.2) .46c 36 .65 40.7 (76.1) 4.2 (8.2) 2.08c 36 .05
  Cocaine 8.2 (23.3) 11.4 (23.9) .42c 36 .68 31.7 (56.5) 3.1 (5.9) 2.19c 36 .03
  Benzoylecgonine 1.9 (5.5) 3.1 (7.6) .58c 36 .56 8.3 (19.6) 1.0 (2.2) 1.62c 36 .11
  Cocaethylene 1.0 (2.8) 0.9 (2.8) .11c 36 .91 1.2 (2.1) 0.3 (1.0) 1.56c 36 .13
  Norcocainet 0.2 (0.5) 0.4 (0.8) .83c 36 .41 0.6 (1.4) 0.1 (0.1) 1.71c 36 .10
  Urine toxicology (n/p)k 48/0 14/5 16/3 .63b 1 .43 48/0 7/12 18/1 14.15b 1 <.001
Alcohol n
 Grams per weekh 119.9 (136.8) 169.4 (129.2) 155.3 (146.4) 1.07a 2,83 .35 104.3 (88.6) 259.7 (244.5)*** 127.4 (141.4)° 7.71a 2,83 <.001
 Years of use 13.3 (8.8) 13.7 (7.6) 12.0 (7.3) .23a 2,83 .79 14.0 (8.7) 14.8 (7.5) 12.6 (7.9) .34a 2,83 .71
Nicotine n
 Smoking (y/n)f 37/11 14/5 14/5 .13b 2 .94 40/8 15/4 13/6 1.83b 2 .40
 Cigarettes per dayh 8.7 (8.7) 12.8 (11.2) 9.5 (8.2) 1.38a 2,83 .26 8.2 (8.7) 13.4 (12.0) 8.2 (7.8) 2.31a 2,83 .11
 Years of use 9.3 (8.3) 10.4 (8.9) 12.7 (10.3) .95a 2,83 .39 10.5 (8.8) 12.5 (8.6) 12.6 (9.9) .56a 2,83 .57
Cannabis n
 Grams per weekh 0.6 (1.6) 3.3 (8.9) 1.2 (2.3) 2.38a 2,83 .10 0.5 (1.6) 2.1 (4.6) 1.1 (2.7) 2.28a 2,83 .11
 Years of use 4.5 (4.9) 9.5 (8.5)* 10.1 (9.7)* 5.92a 2,83 .004 4.6 (5.9) 10.5 (9.8)* 8.6 (9.7) 4.64a 2,83 .01
 Cumulative dose (grams) 980 (3985) 3199 (5899) 2606 (6359) 1.61a 2,83 .21 53.4 (180) 217.8 (526.5) 84.7 (189.6) 2.15a 2,83 .12
 Last consumption (days)j 39.3 (1.6);n = 22 10.0 (0.4);n = 14 25.4 (1.1);n = 12 2.19a 2,45 .12 36.5 (1.5);n = 22 9.7 (0.4);n = 13 50.8 (2.1);n = 10 1.20a 2,42 .31
 Urine toxicology (n/p)k 42/6 15/4 14/5 2.03b 2 .36 42/6 7/12 15/4 18.61b 2 <.001
Amphetamine n
 Grams per weekh 0.0 (0.1) 0.1 (0.1)** 0.0 (0.1) 5.18a 2,83 .008 0.0 (0.0) 0.1 (0.2)** 0.0 (0.1) 5.89a 2,83 .004
 Years of use 0.0 (0.0) 3.3 (4.0)*** 1.3 (3.1)° 13.73a 2,83 <.001 0.1 (0.5) 3.2 (4.9)** 2.7 (5.5)* 7.46a 2,83 .001
 Cumulative dose (grams) 0.0 (0.1) 56.0 (177.6)* 16.2 (35.9) 2.99a 2,83 .06 0.0 (0.1) 4.4 (8.9)** 1.4 (3.5) 6.47a 2,83 .002
 Last consumption (days)j 121.6 (5.1);n = 1 73.6 (3.1);n = 10 90.9 (3.8);n = 3 .29a 2,11 .75 17.5 (0.7);n = 1 35.7 (1.5);n = 8 99.8 (4.2);n = 4 1.48a 2,10 .27
 Hair analysis (ng/mg) 0.0 (0.0) 0.1 (0.2)* 0.0 (0.0) 4.35a 2,83 .02 0.0 (0.0) 0.1 (0.2) 0.1 (0.2) 2.89a 2,83 .06
MDMA n
 Tablets per weekh 0.0 (0.0) 0.0 (0.1)*** 0.0 (0.0)° 7.42a 2,83 .001 0.0 (0.0) 0.4 (0.9)** 0.0 (0.0)° 5.54a 2,83 .006
 Years of use 0.3 (1.0) 3.5 (4.5)*** 2.4 (4.6)* 8.42a 2,83 <.001 0.2 (1.4) 3.8 (5.5)** 3.2 (5.6)* 7.78a 2,83 <.001
 Cumulative dose (tablets) 1.3 (4.0) 108.8 (249.7)** 18.7 (46.2) 5.71a 2,83 .005 0.2 (0.8) 17.0 (49.3)* 2.8 (5.2) 3.67a 2,83 .03
 Last consumption (days)j 5.0 (0.2);n = 1 89.9 (3.7);n = 7 40.2 (1.7);n = 4 1.63a 2,9 .25 91.2 (3.8);n = 3 41.6 (1.7);n = 6 47.8 (2.0);n = 5 1.11a 2,11 .36
 Hair analysis (ng/mg) 0.0 (0.0) 0.3 (0.7) 0.4 (1.5) 2.23a 2,83 .11 0.0 (0.0) 0.5 (0.8)*** 0.1 (0.3) 7.87a 2,83 <.001
GHB n
 Cumulative dose (pipettes) 0.0 (0.0) 0.5 (0.7) 0.5 (1.7) 3.36a 2,83 .04 0.0 (0.0) 0.0 (0.0) 0.0 (0.0)
Hallucinogens n
 Cumulative dose (times) 0.9 (2.2) 27.9 (72.8)* 9.9 (22.9) 3.92a 2,83 .02 0.0 (0.0) 1.1 (1.6)*** 0.6 (1.5) 8.57a 2,83 <.001
Methlyphenidate n
 Cumulative dose (tablets) 0.0 (0.0) 20.2 (60.4)* 0.5 (2.3) 3.76a 2,83 .03 0.0 (0.1) 67.7 (239.5) 0.3 (0.6) 2.72a 2,83 .07
 Hair analysis (ng/mg) 0.0 (0.0) 0.0 (0.1) 0.0 (0.0) 1.80a 2,83 .17 0.0 (0.0) 0.1 (0.2)* 0.0 (0.0) 3.62a 2,83 .03

Means and standard deviations. Significant p values are shown in bold.

a

ANOVA (all groups, with significant Sidak post hoc test vs. control group: *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001; vs. cocaine increaser: p < .05).

b

χ²-test (all groups/cocaine users only) for frequency data.

c

Independent t-test (cocaine users only).

d

Verbal IQ was assessed by the Mehrfachwahl Wortschatz Intelligenztest (46).

e

ADHD-SR, ADHD self-rating scale (cutoff DSM-IV criteria) (33).

f

Smoking habits were assessed by the Fagerstroem Test of Nicotine Dependence (47).

g

BDI, Beck Depression Inventory (cutoff ≥ 18) (48).

h

Average use during the last 6 months.

i

Craving for cocaine was assessed by the Brief-CCQ (49).

j

Last consumption is averaged only for persons who used the drug in the last 6 months.

k

Urine toxicology (neg/pos) are based on the cutoff value for cocaine = 150 ng/ml and for tetrahydrocannabinol 50 ng/ml (50). The χ²-test for cocaine includes only cocaine users; the χ²-test for cannabis includes controls and cocaine users.

l

Hair samples were voluntary and data are missing for three controls.

m

Parameters at follow-up refer to the 1-year period between t1 and t2.

n

At baseline, average use during the last 6 months. Use frequency, duration of use, and cumulative doses are averaged within the total group.

Emotional empathy: The introduced predictors explained a significant amount of variance of the EE change scores in the multiple regression analysis [F(5,80) = 2.68, p < .05, R2 = .14; Table 2 ]. The strongest predictors were sex (β = −0.25, p < .05) and the group contrast cocaine increasers vs. decreasers (β = −0.25, p < .05; Figure 1 ). Post hoc analyses showed that controls and the combined cocaine user (CCU) group showed a nonsignificant difference in EE with a small to moderate effect size at baseline [tind(84) = 1.78, p = .08, d = 0.39]. Whereas in the 1-year interval, increasers slightly reduced their already hampered EE [tdep(18) = 1.19, p = .25 d = 0.27], and cocaine decreasers moderately improved their ability to respond empathically [tdep(18) = 1.80, p = .09, d = 0.41]. Notably, controls remained largely stable in EE [tdep(47) = 0.98, p = .33, d = 0.14]. Accordingly, at follow-up, controls differed significantly from the increaser group [tind(25) = 2.14, p < .05, d = 0.64], whereas the difference between controls and decreasers was strongly reduced [tind(65) = 0.40, p = .69, d = 0.11]. Finally, at follow-up, increasers and decreasers displayed a nonsignificant group difference of moderate effect size [tind(36) = 1.69, p = .10, d = 0.56].

Table 2.

Multiple regression analyses.

Emotional empathy
Δ MET Emotional empathy
Theory-of-Mind
Δ MASC total errors
Social interaction
Δ SDM composite score
Social network
Δ SNQ network size
Personality disorder
Δ SCID-II Cluster B
B SE β B SE β B SE β B SE β B SE β
Constant 1.55 1.14 −1.85 2.02 −0.26 0.45 −0.73 2.72 −1.01 2.80
Age 0.04 0.03 0.14 0.06 0.05 0.14 −0.01 0.01 −0.08 −0.04 0.06 −0.08 0.02 0.06 0.04
Sex −1.16 0.50 −0.25* −0.99 0.86 −0.12 0.09 0.20 0.05 −0.56 1.20 −0.05 −0.71 1.23 −0.06
ADHD-SR score −0.05 0.03 −0.16 −0.07 0.06 −0.14 0.00 0.01 −0.01 0.04 0.08 0.05 −0.24 0.08 −0.32**
Controls vs. cocaine increaser −0.26 0.59 −0.05 1.71 1.03 0.20 0.03 0.24 0.01 0.35 1.43 0.03 4.00 1.49 0.31**
Cocaine decreaser vs. cocaine increaser −1.30 0.66 −0.25* −0.51 1.17 −0.06 −0.31 0.27 −0.16 0.54 1.63 0.05 4.43 1.69 0.34**
R2 0.14 0.10 0.03 0.01 0.17
F 2.68* 1.67 0.57 0.22 3.25**

Multiple linear regression with the independent variables age, sex, ADHS-SR score, and dummy coded (zero, one) group variables. To compare the groups, cocaine increaser acted as the reference group. Dependent variables are change values (Δ = value at t2 − value at t1).

B, unstandardized regression coefficient; SE, unstandardized standard error; β, standardized beta.

(*)p < .10; *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Development of emotional empathy in cocaine increasers, decreasers, and stimulant-naive controls within 1 year. Mean emotional empathy sum scores and standard errors. At baseline, controls vs combined cocaine user (CCU) (= Ø of increaser and decreaser). Independent Student’s t-tests are shown if p < .10. (*)p < .10; *p < .05.

Theory-of-Mind: The applied multiple regression model could not predict the MASC total error change scores ( Table 2 ). At the phenomenological level, both cocaine user groups displayed small test–retest improvements [increasers: tdep(18) = 1.12, p = .28, d = 0.26; decreasers: tdep(17) = 0.60, p = .56, d = 0.14], while the control group showed pronounced improvements [tdep(47) = 4.68, p < .001, d = 0.68; Figure 2 ).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Development of the Theory-of-Mind in cocaine increasers, decreasers, and stimulant-naive controls within 1 year. Mean Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition (MASC) total errors and standard errors. At baseline, controls vs CCU (= Ø of increaser and decreaser). Independent Student’s t-tests are shown if p < .10. *p < .05.

Social interaction: The multiple regression model was also not able to predict the SDM composite change score ( Table 2 ). From the phenomenological perspective, controls and increasers acted less prosocial (giving less money to the opponent), while decreasers remained stable but, with that, came closer to the controls ( Figure 3 ). Exploratory post hoc analyses confirmed that controls and CCU significantly differed at baseline [tind(65) = 2.51, p < .05, d = 0.56]. At follow-up, controls and increasers still display a moderate group difference [tind(65) = 1.92, p = .06, d = 0.50], whereas the group difference between controls and decreasers was reduced to a small effect size [tind(64) = 0.98, p = .33, d = 0.26]. In addition, we analyzed behavioral changes between baseline and follow-up (more prosocial decisions, more self-serving decisions, similar decision) only in cocaine users and found that about two-thirds of the increasers (58% = 11/19) but only one-third of the decreasers (33% = 6/18) showed more self-serving decisions at follow-up (p = .40; FET; Figure S1).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Development of social interaction in cocaine increasers, decreasers, and stimulant-naive controls within 1 year. Mean social decision-making (SDM) composite z-scores and standard errors. At baseline, controls vs CCU (= Ø of increaser and decreaser). Independent Student’s t-tests are shown if p < .10. (*)p < .10; *p < .05.

Social network size: Regarding the SNQ total network size, the multiple regression model could again not substantially predict the change scores ( Table 2 , Figure 4 ). Interestingly, during the 1-year interval, all three groups reported a substantial and moderate social network reduction of about 2.5 contacts [controls: tdep(47) = 3.75, p < .001, d = 0.54; increasers: tdep(17) = 1.94, p = .70, d = 0.46; decreasers: tdep(17) = 3.09, p < .01, d = 0.73].

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Development of social network size in cocaine increasers, decreasers, and stimulant-naive controls within 1 year. Mean total network size and standard errors. At baseline, controls vs CCU (= Ø of increaser and decreaser). Independent Student’s t-tests are shown if p < .10. **p < .01.

Cluster B PD: The regression model significantly explained the variance in cluster B PD symptom change [F(5,77) = 3.25, p < .01, R2 = .17; Table 2 ]. This change score was best predicted by the ADHS-SR score (β = −0.32, p < .01) and the group contrasts cocaine increasers vs. decreasers (β = 0.34, p < .01) and cocaine increasers vs. controls (β = 0.31, p < .01). Importantly, the CCU group showed at baseline significantly more cluster B PD symptoms than the controls [tind(81) = 4.40, p < .001, d = 0.96; Figure 5 ]. Whereas controls [tdep(47) = 4.91, p < .001, d = 0.71] and decreasers [tdep(17) = 3.55, p < .01, d = 0.84] had significantly lower symptom scores after the 1-year interval period, the amount of symptoms for the increaser group remained largely stable [tdep(16) = 0.52, p = .61, d = 0.13]. Accordingly, at follow-up, controls differed strongly from the increasers [tind(19) = 4.70, p < .001, d = 1.58] and from the decreasers [tind(22) = 3.11, p < .01, d = 0.96]. Interestingly, already after 1 year of different cocaine use, increasers and decreasers displayed a moderate to strong group difference in cluster B PD symptoms at follow-up [tind(33) = 1.85, p = .07, d = 0.63].Of note, approximately three quarters of the decreasers (13/18) displayed lower cluster B PD scores, while more than half of the cocaine increasers (9/17) showed even more symptoms at follow-up (p < .05; FET; Figure S2).

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Development of Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Disorders (SCID-II) Cluster B in cocaine increasers, decreasers, and stimulant-naive controls within 1 year. Mean SCID-II Cluster B symptoms and standard errors. At baseline, controls vs CCU (= Ø of increaser and decreaser). Independent Student’s t-tests are shown if p < .10. (*)p < .10; **p < .01; ***p < .001.

Remarkably, the interaction effect on cluster B PD symptoms was mainly driven by changes in the narcissistic and borderline subscores and less by the histrionic and surprisingly also not by the antisocial subscore (see Figure S3a–d). Both the narcissistic and the borderline subscores revealed significant regression models (Table S2), but only the borderline subscore was significantly predicted by the group contrast cocaine increasers vs. decreasers (β = 0.40, p < .01). Compared to baseline, less symptoms occurred in controls [tdep(47) = 4.99, p < .001, d = 0.72] and decreasers [tdep(17) = 3.16, p < .01, d = 0.75] at follow-up, while symptoms remained stable in increasers [tdep(16) = 0.18, p = .86, d = 0.04], resulting in a strong group effect between increasers and decreasers [tind(33) = 2.57, p < .05, d = 0.87] at follow-up.

Change in alcohol use: As not only cocaine but also alcohol intake was increased in increasers (see Table 1 ), the change in alcohol consumption was considered in additional multiple regression models. However, alcohol change was not significant in any of the main regression models (p-values ranged from .222 to .659) shown in Table 2 , while the interaction effects and also the explained variances remained stable, indicating that changes in alcohol consumption have not impacted our main results.

Test–retest reliability: In the total sample, all dependent variables displayed acceptable to good test–retest reliabilities ( Table 3 ). Interestingly, in the SDM paradigm, controls and CCU differed significantly in their test–retest reliability (z = −3.25; p < .001): While in controls the SDM score showed hardly acceptable reliability (r = 0.48; p < .001), it was good in the cocaine users (r = 0.85; p < .001).

Table 3.

One-year test–retest reliability between baseline and 1-year follow-up in controls and cocaine users.

Controls (n = 48) Combined cocaine users (n = 38) Total sample (n = 86)
MET Emotional empathy score 0.66*** 0.79*** 0.74***
MASC total errors 0.67*** 0.60*** 0.63***
SDM composite score 0.48*** 0.85*** 0.70***
SNQ network size 0.75*** 0.81*** 0.80***
SCID-II Cluster B symptoms 0.68*** 0.74*** 0.77***

Pearson’s product-moment correlation. Significance level: ***p < .001.

Discussion

The present longitudinal study investigated the change of social cognition, social interaction, and socially relevant cluster B PD symptoms in healthy controls and relatively pure and non-help-seeking chronic cocaine users who clearly increased or decreased their cocaine consumption during a 1-year study interval. The most striking findings were that i) improved EE correlated with decreased cocaine consumption, whereas increased cocaine use severity was linked to less EE; and ii) cluster B PD symptom burden was lowered in decreasers, whereas increasers showed stable severity in these symptoms. Additionally, during the study interval, we found an approximation between controls and decreasers regarding their prosocial behavior, while the large gap between increasers and controls remained. Moreover, neither the Theory-of-Mind Task (MASC) nor the social network size showed interactions with changing cocaine use, indicating that mental perspective-taking (sometimes also interpreted as cognitive empathy) and the number of social contacts in the last months were not affected by changing drug use during the study interval.

Importantly, the present analysis of smaller (longitudinal) subgroups from our larger cross-sectional ZuCo2St sample published previously (14, 16, 17) still showed significantly reduced prosocial behavior, a smaller social network, and strongly elevated cluster B personality symptoms in the total group of cocaine users at baseline, indicating that these indicators of social functioning were robustly altered in this population. The EE score of the MET showed only a statistical trend between cocaine users and controls at baseline, but the present effect size (d = 0.39) was in the range of the previously reported effect sizes of the larger cross-sectional sample of recreational and dependent cocaine users (d = 0.39–0.64), suggesting rather a deficiency of power than a lack of reliability. This assumption is further supported by the fact that the MET EE score showed good test–retest reliability scores. Moreover, the MASC did not show any baseline group differences in the present subsample of cocaine users underscoring our previous conclusion that mainly very severe cocaine users with a putative ADHD comorbidity show disturbances in this task (16, 44).

While sociocognitive functions represent basic abilities in perspective-taking and interaction, more conventional psychopathology is aiming at the identification and quantification of symptoms in psychiatric disorders (51). As such, the research on the relationship between PDs and cocaine use is of special interest, as the differentiation between predispositions vs. drug-induced effects merges with the question if these pathologies are reversible or not. In our longitudinal investigation, we found that decreasers of cocaine consumption also significantly improved in cluster B PD symptoms during 1 year, whereas the increasers showed a stable PD symptom burden. This is insofar interesting as in both user groups 8 of 19 participants sought psychiatric treatment in the interval, but only decreasers improved in some social functions and socially relevant PD symptoms.

In general, PDs are defined as typical constellations of impaired subjective and behavioral traits that result in suffering of the affected individual and/or society (52). These personality traits are regarded as relatively stable across time and consistent across situations (diagnostically mandatory) (5356). Moreover, cluster B PDs show a higher stability over 12 to 18 years than the other clusters (57). However, studies also found considerable variability of PD symptoms across individuals over time (58, 59), questioning the trait-like character of the disorder. An early study showed changes in PD symptoms related to treatment in substance-dependent patients (60). Interestingly, clusters A and C profited most, while cluster B changes were only observed in patients with borderline PD. In patients with cocaine addiction, cluster B PDs are the most frequent and these patients have the most severe courses of illness including worst treatment outcomes (24, 2628, 61, 62). Therefore, cluster B PD symptoms are likely personality features that increase the risk for cocaine use and the development of an addiction. However, as seen in the present study, cluster B PD symptom load is nevertheless variable and reduction of consumption leads to a substantial improvement in these symptoms. Consequently, a reduction of cluster B PD symptom burden again increases likelihood of successful treatment, offering the patient an opportunity to leave the vicious circle of addiction.

The suggested consumption-dependent variability of social behavior as well as cluster B symptoms are well in line with our previous analyses from this sample that not only basal cognitive functions such as working memory but also self-reported impulsivity improve with a strong reduction of cocaine use, while they are worsened with increased cocaine consumption (7, 30). The present data and the previous analyses from this sample are also in accordance with our recent results from an independent longitudinal investigation showing that decreased cortical thickness (CT) of several regions within the prefrontal cortex of cocaine users can improve after a strong reduction of cocaine use, while sustained use went along with a further decrease in prefrontal CT during the study interval (63). Importantly, the cortical changes were correlated with cognitive changes, i.e., improved CT as associated with enhanced sustained attention (63). Thus, the overall pattern of change shown by longitudinal data supports our assumption that sociocognitive impairments of cocaine users are at least in part drug-induced and that neuroplastic changes in brain regions and neurotransmitter systems involved in social cognition, social interaction, and social reward processing contribute to a further decrease in social contact and social support leading to an increase in social isolation, aggression, and depressive symptoms. This ends in a further reduction of social reward resources, ongoing social withdrawal, and the establishment of cocaine as the main source of reward resulting in the maintenance of stimulant use and recurrent relapses (15).

While EE is more a perceptive social cognition ability, social decision-making (here assessed with a combined Distribution/Dictator Game) is a form of socially interactive behavior. In our previous cross-sectional analysis sample, cocaine users cared more about efficiency than about fairness compared to healthy controls at baseline (17). This was previously interpreted as predisposition of stimulant use (15), as such fairness preferences and severity of cocaine use were not correlated (17). Intriguingly, utilitarian and opportunistic attitudes assessed with the Machiavellianism Questionnaire (MACH-IV) were also increased in cocaine users compared to controls and were shown to be stable and independent of changing cocaine use (14). However, our data indicated a shift toward improved prosocial behavior in cocaine decreasers indicating space for enhancement potential by treatment. Conclusively, SDM deficits in cocaine users likely have both a trait and a state component, and it might be worse to specifically target the state component in therapy in order to improve the treatment outcome.

Limitations

When interpreting the present results, some limitations of our study have to be considered: i) The total sample size is moderate for a longitudinal analysis. Moreover, the test–retest reliabilities of the applied social cognition tasks and questionnaire have a broad range (in controls: r = 0.48–0.75; in cocaine users: r = 0.60–0.85; in the total sample: r = 0.63–0.80). As a consequence of both, the shown interaction effects are not very strong (in terms of p-values). However, to our knowledge, these are the only existing longitudinal samples of chronic cocaine users with objectively verified increasing and decreasing cocaine use (by hair testing). Moreover, the included individuals were preferably pure cocaine users with little axis-I psychiatric comorbidities. We therefore think that the carefully selected and homogeneous sample has nonetheless sufficient explanatory power. ii) In the context of our hypotheses, we attribute the changes in behavior to the changes in cocaine consumption. However, we cannot rule out if other changes in the lives of our cocaine users (e.g., positive or negative changes in their social environment) not assessed by our test battery may have impacted both drug use and social functioning. Future studies should therefore assess more information on the social life of cocaine users beyond simple parameters such as social network size (e.g., social media use).

Conclusions

The aim of this longitudinal study was to investigate whether cocaine use is associated with permanent or reversible alterations of social cognition and interaction as well as cluster B PD symptoms. We found that specific social dysfunctions and PD symptoms are variable over time as they seem to depend on variations in cocaine use. Thus, strong reduction of cocaine use within only 1 year seems to positively affect social dysfunctions that are assumed to be crucial factor in the maintenance of stimulant addiction (15, 64). From our perspective, the shown positive effects of reduced cocaine use clearly favor abstinence-orientated treatment approaches of cocaine addiction. Furthermore, having the strong impact of social cognitive abilities as well as prosocial behavior and attitudes on the patient–therapist relationships in mind (15), future developments in the psychotherapy of cocaine addictions should consider trainings specifically of social skills and cognitions in order to improve treatment outcome.

Ethics Statement

The study was approved by the Cantonal Ethics Committee of Zurich, Switzerland.

Author Contributions

BQ and MV had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. CE and BQ contributed to the study concept and design. CE, MV, OB, KP, LH, MB, and BQ contributed to the acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data. OB, CE, MV, and BQ contributed to the drafting of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content. MV and BQ conducted the statistical analysis. ES and BQ obtained funding. KP, LH, MV, and ES contributed to the administrative, technical, or material support. ES and BQ were in charge of supervision.

Funding

The study was supported by grants from the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF; grant Nos. PP00P1-123516/1 and PP00P1-146326/1) and the Olga Mayenfisch Foundation. The funders of the study did not influence the design and conduct of the study; the collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; the preparation, review, and approval of the manuscript; and the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This article is in tribute to our esteemed friend and colleague Prof. Christoph Eisenegger, who passed away so suddenly and unexpectedly during the time we worked on this article. We are grateful to Alex Gamma for the critical discussion of the present data. Moreover, we thank Daniela Jenni and Kathrin Küpeli for their excellent support.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00213/full#supplementary-material.

References

  • 1. Jovanovski D, Erb S, Zakzanis KK. Neurocognitive deficits in cocaine users: a quantitative review of the evidence. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol (2005) 27(2):189–204. 10.1080/13803390490515694 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Spronk DB, van Wel JH, Ramaekers JG, Verkes RJ. Characterizing the cognitive effects of cocaine: a comprehensive review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2013) 37(8):1838–59. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.07.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Vonmoos M, Hulka LM, Preller KH, Jenni D, Baumgartner MR, Stohler R, et al. Cognitive dysfunctions in recreational and dependent cocaine users: role of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, craving and early age at onset. Br J Psychiatry (2013) 203(1):35–43. 10.1192/bjp.bp.112.118091 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Aharonovich E, Nunes E, Hasin D. Cognitive impairment, retention and abstinence among cocaine abusers in cognitive-behavioral treatment. Drug Alcohol Depend (2003) 71(2):207–11. 10.1016/S0376-8716(03)00092-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Aharonovich E, Hasin DS, Brooks AC, Liu X, Bisaga A, Nunes EV. Cognitive deficits predict low treatment retention in cocaine dependent patients. Drug Alcohol Depend (2006) 81(3):313–22. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.08.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Potvin S, Stavro K, Rizkallah E, Pelletier J. Cocaine and cognition: a systematic quantitative review. J Addict Med (2014) 8(5):368–76. 10.1097/ADM.0000000000000066 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Vonmoos M, Hulka LM, Preller KH, Minder F, Baumgartner MR, Quednow BB. Cognitive impairment in cocaine users is drug-induced but partially reversible: evidence from a longitudinal study. Neuropsychopharmacology (2014) 39(9):2200–10. 10.1038/npp.2014.71 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Vonmoos M, Quednow BB. Cognitive dysfunctions in chronic cocaine users. In: Preedy VR. The neuroscience of cocaine. San Diego: Academic Press; (2017). p. 395–405. 10.1016/B978-0-12-803750-8.00040-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Rilling JK, Sanfey AG. The neuroscience of social decision-making. Annu Rev Psychol (2011) 62:23–48. 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131647 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Lieberman MD. Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. Annu Rev Psychol (2007) 58:259–89. 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085654 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Couture SM, Penn DL, Roberts DL. The functional significance of social cognition in schizophrenia: a review. Schizophr Bull (2006) 32 Suppl 1:S44–63. 10.1093/schbul/sbl029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Homer BD, Solomon TM, Moeller RW, Mascia A, DeRaleau L, Halkitis PN. Methamphetamine abuse and impairment of social functioning: a review of the underlying neurophysiological causes and behavioral implications. Psychol Bull (2008) 134(2):301–10. 10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.301 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Volkow ND, Baler RD, Goldstein RZ. Addiction: pulling at the neural threads of social behaviors. Neuron (2011) 69(4):599–602. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Quednow BB, Hulka LM, Preller KH, Baumgartner MR, Eisenegger C, Vonmoos M. Stable self-serving personality traits in recreational and dependent cocaine users. PLoS One (2017) 12(3):e0172853. 10.1371/journal.pone.0172853 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Quednow BB. Social cognition and interaction in stimulant use disorders. Curr Opin Behav Sci (2017) 13:55–62. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.10.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Preller KH, Hulka LM, Vonmoos M, Jenni D, Baumgartner MR, Seifritz E, et al. Impaired emotional empathy and related social network deficits in cocaine users. Addict Biol (2014) 19(3):452–66. 10.1111/adb.12070 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Hulka LM, Eisenegger C, Preller KH, Vonmoos M, Jenni D, Bendrick K, et al. Altered social and non-social decision-making in recreational and dependent cocaine users. Psychol Med (2014) 44(5):1015–28. 10.1017/S0033291713001839 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Hulka LM, Preller KH, Vonmoos M, Broicher SD, Quednow BB. Cocaine users manifest impaired prosodic and cross-modal emotion processing. Front Psychiatry (2013) 4:98. 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00098 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Preller KH, Herdener M, Schilbach L, Stampfli P, Hulka LM, Vonmoos M, et al. Functional changes of the reward system underlie blunted response to social gaze in cocaine users. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2014) 111(7):2842–7. 10.1073/pnas.1317090111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Tobler PN, Preller KH, Campbell-Meiklejohn DK, Kirschner M, Kraehenmann R, Stampfli P, et al. Shared neural basis of social and non-social reward deficits in chronic cocaine users. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci (2016) 11(6):1017–25. 10.1093/scan/nsw030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Frith CD, Frith U. Social cognition in humans. Curr Biol (2007) 17(16):R724–32. 10.1016/j.cub.2007.05.068 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Roepke S, Vater A, Preißler S, Heekeren HR, Dziobek I. Social cognition in borderline personality disorder. Front Neurosci (2012) 6:195. 10.3389/fnins.2012.00195 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Ruiz-Tagle A, Costanzo E, De Achával D, Guinjoan S. Social cognition in a clinical sample of personality disorder patients. Front Psychiatry (2015) 6:75. 10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00075 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Chen KW, Banducci AN, Guller L, Macatee RJ, Lavelle A, Daughters SB, et al. An examination of psychiatric comorbidities as a function of gender and substance type within an inpatient substance use treatment program. Drug Alcohol Depend (2011) 118(2–3):92–9. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.03.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Fernandez-Montalvo J, Lorea I. Comorbilidad de la adicción a la cocaína con los trastornos de la personalidad. An Sist Sanit Navar (2007) 30(2):225–31. 10.4321/S1137-66272007000300007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Albein-Urios N, Martinez-Gonzalez JM, Lozano-Rojas O, Verdejo-Garcia A. Executive functions in cocaine-dependent patients with Cluster B and Cluster C personality disorders. Neuropsychology (2014) 28(1):84–90. 10.1037/neu0000007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Ford JD, Gelernter J, DeVoe JS, Zhang W, Weiss RD, Brady K, et al. Association of psychiatric and substance use disorder comorbidity with cocaine dependence severity and treatment utilization in cocaine-dependent individuals. Drug Alcohol Depend (2009) 99(1–3):193–203. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.07.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Lopez-Quintero C, Hasin DS, de Los Cobos JP, Pines A, Wang S, Grant BF, et al. Probability and predictors of remission from life-time nicotine, alcohol, cannabis or cocaine dependence: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Addiction (2011) 106(3):657–69. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03194.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Bagby RM, Vachon DD, Bulmash E, Quilty LC. Personality disorders and pathological gambling: a review and re-examination of prevalence rates. J Pers Disord (2008) 22(2):191–207. 10.1521/pedi.2008.22.2.191 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Hulka LM, Vonmoos M, Preller KH, Baumgartner MR, Seifritz E, Gamma A, et al. Changes in cocaine consumption are associated with fluctuations in self-reported impulsivity and gambling decision-making. Psychol Med (2015) 45(14):3097–110. 10.1017/S0033291715001063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Hoelzle C, Scheufler F, Uhl M, Sachs H, Thieme D. Application of discriminant analysis to differentiate between incorporation of cocaine and its congeners into hair and contamination. Forensic Sci Int (2008) 176(1):13–8. 10.1016/j.forsciint.2007.07.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Quednow BB, Kuhn KU, Hoenig K, Maier W, Wagner M. Prepulse inhibition and habituation of acoustic startle response in male MDMA (‘ecstasy’) users, cannabis users, and healthy controls. Neuropsychopharmacology (2004) 29(5):982–90. 10.1038/sj.npp.1300396 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Roesler M, Retz W, Retz-Junginger P, Thome J, Supprian T, Nissen T, et al. Instrumente zur Diagnostik der Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-/Hyperaktivitätsstörung (ADHS) im Erwachsenenalter. Nervenarzt (2004) 75:888–95. 10.1007/s00115-003-1622-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. American Psychiatric Association (APA) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV. In: Washington D, editor. American Psychiatric Association (APA)., 4th ed Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (APA) (1994). [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Dziobek I, Rogers K, Fleck S, Bahnemann M, Heekeren HR, Wolf OT, et al. Dissociation of cognitive and emotional empathy in adults with Asperger syndrome using the Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET). J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38(3):464–73. 10.1007/s10803-007-0486-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Dziobek I, Fleck S, Kalbe E, Rogers K, Hassenstab J, Brand M, et al. Introducing MASC: a movie for the assessment of social cognition. J Autism Dev Disord (2006) 36(5):623–36. 10.1007/s10803-006-0107-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Charness G, Rabin M. Understanding social preferences with simple tests. Q J Econ (2002) 117(3):817–69. 10.1162/003355302760193904 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Engelmann D, Strobel M. Inequality aversion, efficiency, and maximin preferences in simple distribution experiments. Am Econ Rev (2004) 94(4):857–69. 10.1257/0002828042002741 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Linden M, Lischka A-M, Popien C, Golombek J. Der multidimensionale Sozialkontakt Kreis (MuSK)—ein Interviewverfahren zur Erfassung des sozialen Netzes in der klinischen Praxis. Z Med Psychol (2007) 16(3):135–43. [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Fydrich T, Renneberg B, Schmitz B, Wittchen H-U. SKID-II Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV, Achse II: Persönlichkeitsstörungen. In: [SCID-II Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV, Axis II: Personality Disorders]. Goettingen: Hogrefe; (1997). [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods (2007) 39(2):175–91. 10.3758/BF03193146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Christov-Moore L, Simpson EA, Coude G, Grigaityte K, Iacoboni M, Ferrari PF. Empathy: gender effects in brain and behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2014) 46 Pt 4:604–27. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.09.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Kret ME, De Gelder B. A review on sex differences in processing emotional signals. Neuropsychologia (2012) 50(7):1211–21. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.12.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Wunderli MD, Vonmoos M, Niedecker SM, Hulka LM, Preller KH, Baumgartner MR, et al. Cognitive and emotional impairments in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and cocaine use. Drug Alcohol Depend (2016) 163:92–9. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.03.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Freeman GH, Halton JH. Note on an exact treatment of contingency, goodness of fit and other problems of significance. Biometrika (1951) 38(1/2):141–9. 10.2307/2332323 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Lehrl S. Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest. MWT-B. 4th ed Balingen: Spitta; (1999). [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Heatherton TF, Kozlowski LT, Frecker RC, Fagerstrom KO. The Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence: a revision of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire. Br J Addict (1991) 86(9):1119–27. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01879.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Hautzinger M, Bailer M, Worall H, Keller F. Beck-Depressions-Inventar (BDI). Bearbeitung der deutschen Ausgabe. Testhandbuch. In: Beck Depression Inventory. Test manual., 2nd ed Bern, Göttingen, Toronto, Seattle: Huber; (1994). [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Sussner BD, Smelson DA, Rodrigues S, Kline A, Losonczy M, Ziedonis D. The validity and reliability of a brief measure of cocaine craving. Drug Alcohol Depend (2006) 83(3):233–7. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.11.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration Mandatory guidelines for federal workplace drug testing programs. Fed Regist (2008) 73(228):71858–907. [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Jaspers K. Allgemeine Psychopathologie. 9th ed Berlin, Heidelberg, New York: Springer-Verlag; (1973). [Google Scholar]
  • 52. American Psychiatric Association (APA) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (APA) (2013) 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Lenzenweger MF. Stability and change in personality disorder features: the longitudinal study of personality disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1999) 56(11):1009–15. 10.1001/archpsyc.56.11.1009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Hopwood CJ, Morey LC, Donnellan MB, Samuel DB, Grilo CM, McGlashan TH, et al. Ten-year rank-order stability of personality traits and disorders in a clinical sample. J Pers (2013) 81(3):335–44. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.00801.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Shea MT, Stout R, Gunderson J, Morey LC, Grilo CM, McGlashan T, et al. Short-term diagnostic stability of schizotypal, borderline, avoidant, and obsessive-compulsive personality disorders. Am J Psychiatry (2002) 159(12):2036–41. 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.12.2036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Grilo CM, Sanislow CA, Gunderson JG, Pagano ME, Yen S, Zanarini MC, et al. Two-year stability and change of schizotypal, borderline, avoidant, and obsessive-compulsive personality disorders. J Consult Clin Psychol (2004) 72(5):767–75. 10.1037/0022-006X.72.5.767 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Nestadt G, Di C, Samuels JF, Bienvenu OJ, Reti IM, Costa P, et al. The stability of DSM personality disorders over twelve to eighteen years. J Psychiatr Res (2010) 44(1):1–7. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.06.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Durbin CE, Klein DN. Ten-year stability of personality disorders among outpatients with mood disorders. J Abnorm Psychol (2006) 115(1):75–84. 10.1037/0021-843X.115.1.75 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Lenzenweger MF, Johnson MD, Willett JB. Individual growth curve analysis illuminates stability and change in personality disorder features: the longitudinal study of personality disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2004) 61(10):1015–24. 10.1001/archpsyc.61.10.1015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. de Groot MH, Franken IH, van der Meer CW, Hendriks VM. Stability and change in dimensional ratings of personality disorders in drug abuse patients during treatment. J Subst Abuse Treat (2003) 24(2):115–20. 10.1016/S0740-5472(02)00351-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Harro J. Neuropsychiatric adverse effects of amphetamine and methamphetamine. Int Rev Neurobiol (2015) 120:179–204. 10.1016/bs.irn.2015.02.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Rounsaville BJ. Treatment of cocaine dependence and depression. Biol Psychiatry (2004) 56(10):803–9. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.05.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Hirsiger S, Hänggi J, Germann J, Vonmoos M, Preller KH, Engeli EJE, et al. Longitudinal changes in cocaine intake and cognition are linked to cortical thickness adaptations in cocaine users. Neuroimage Clin (2019) 21:101652. 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101652 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Heilig M, Epstein DH, Nader MA, Shaham Y. Time to connect: bringing social context into addiction neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci (2016) 17(9):592–9. 10.1038/nrn.2016.67 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials


Articles from Frontiers in Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES