Abstract
Mice are social animals hence group-housing of mice is preferred over individual housing. However, aggression in group-housed male mice under laboratory housing conditions is a well-known problem leading to serious health issues, including injury or death. Therefore, group-housed mice are frequently separated for welfare reasons. In this study, we investigated the effect of 3 different handling methods (tail, forceps, tube) in 2 different housing conditions (single vs. group) on the variance of aggression-associated parameters in male C57BL/6NCrl mice over 8 weeks. Blood glucose concentration, body weight, body temperature, plus number and severity of bite wounds and barbering intensity in group-housed mice were recorded. An assessment of nest complexity was also performed weekly. Feces were collected in week 3 and 7 for analysis of corticosterone metabolites. We also monitored the level of aggression by recording the behavior of group-housed animals after weekly cage cleaning. An open field test followed by a social novel object test, a light/dark box test, a hotplate and a resident-intruder test were performed at the end of the 8-week handling period. Post-mortem, we assessed organ weights. We found that forceps-handled mice, independent of the housing condition, had significantly higher levels of stress-induced-hyperthermia and enhanced aggression after cage cleaning, and they performed worse in the nest complexity test. In addition, handling male mice by the tail seems to be most effective to reduce aggressiveness after transferring animals into new cages, thereby representing an appropriate refinement.
Introduction
Before being used in experimental procedures, laboratory mice spend most of their lives in their home cages. According to European Union legislation, maintenance procedures for rodents must modify environment and handling to the behavioral and physiological needs of animals [1]. A major challenge in this context is that there is not much literature systematically addressing which measures and procedures cause stress or well-being, respectively, which is an essential prerequisite both for the well-being of experimental animals and reliable in vivo research [2].
Housing social animals in groups is required where applicable. However, group-housing of male mice is a concern in most animal facilities due to potential welfare concerns. Despite daily animal observations, aggression may not be adequately monitored since it can arise spontaneously resulting in pain, injury or death of the animals [3]. Besides affecting welfare and numbers of animals needed, aggression may result in serious problems with data validity [3].
While there are sophisticated refinement approaches with regard to housing conditions, such as by providing enrichment, it is necessary to thoroughly evaluate such measures, since additional cage equipment may also induce aggression as demonstrated by Howerton et al. [4]. Similarly, Marashi et al. showed negative effects of housing environment with regard to related physiological and immunological stress parameters [5]. Such findings indicate the importance of adequate awareness not only for experimental design, but also in regards to the animals’ history.
Besides housing, animal handling is a regular necessity in the animal facility. Handling is the most common procedure experienced by laboratory mice because it is necessary for routine husbandry (i.e. cage cleaning) and research procedures (i.e. injections or blood sampling). Due to pragmatic reasons, handling usually takes place in a context in which the animals anyhow experience stress, i.e. when disturbed in their inactive phase [6]. It is necessary to consider all potential interacting stressful factors for any kind of refinement program–handling being one of those. The distress of male laboratory mice is attributed to housing and experimental procedures for which handling is necessary, and handling related distress in the pre-experimental history of the animals represents aversive experiences and may exert long-lasting effects [7]. Conversely, animal distress may be reduced by certain procedures implemented in handling, thus taming their anxiety [8].
Bearing in mind that many neurobehavioral disorders are investigated in male mice [9], it is necessary to address how refinements of housing and handling may influence male aggressive responses. One paradox here is that males have been historically preferred over females due to the perception of “better” data quality due to less hormonal variability [9]. Whether aggression and dominance structures in males may affect inconsistency of data is mostly neglected. This is interesting, because males, but not females, housed in unisex groups demonstrate several behavioral [10] and physiological alterations [11].
In our study we studied the effects of routinely conducted handling by tail in the context of cage changing and compared this procedure with forceps and tube handling. As readout we focused on behavioral effects which were assessed in a behavioral test battery comprising tests for exploration (open field test), emotional states (light/dark box) and aggression (resident-intruder test). This was complemented by health monitoring (fur state) and assessment of clinical parameters, i.e. blood glucose assessment, physiological measures of stress, i.e. stress-induced hyperthermia, fecal corticosterone metabolites (FCM) as well as final organ weight determination. To analyze handling effects on animal welfare, we conducted a nest building assay (after Deacon et al. [12, 13]). To evaluate potential consequences of handling on agonistic behaviors, single-and group-housed males were examined with regard to data variability by looking at the standard deviation of means. We hypothesized that signs of stress and aggression should be reduced in both single- and group-housed mice if the handling condition is less stressful to the mice. Additionally, we hypothesized stress would lower the scores in the nest building test, the well-being parameter, in which we observed the mice’s nest building performance over time.
Materials and methods
Ethics statement
The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the German Animal Welfare Act and was approved by the Karlsruhe State Authority (permit number: G-154/17).
Animals
72 male C57BL/6NCrl mice (Charles River, Sulzfeld, Germany) arrived at the facility at the age of 3 weeks. The C57BL/6 strain is frequently used and one of the most common background strains for transgenic mouse models [14]. All mice were housed in Macrolon II cages (370cm2, Tecniplast, Milan, Italy), provided with aspen wood bedding (ABEDD LTE-001, Lab & Vet Service, Vienna, Austria) and a nestlet (Plexx B.V, AB Elst, The Netherlands). Tap water and food pellets (Rod 16-A LasVendi, Soest, Germany) were provided ad libitum. The animal room had a controlled temperature (21°C), photoperiod (reversed 12/12 h light/dark cycle: lights on between 21:00–09:00 h) and relative humidity (50–60%). The hygienic status was specific pathogen-free (SPF) according to Federation of European Laboratory Animal Science Association’s (FELASA) recommendations [15].
Out of all mice, 54 mice were divided into groups of 3 (18 cages) and the remaining 18 mice were housed individually for a total of 36 cages. Single (n = 18) and group-housed (n = 54) mice were arbitrarily allocated into three subgroups (transferring mice by picking them up by their tails with gloved hand using thumb and index finger (latex powder-free gloves, sempercare, premium by sempermed), single tail SH/ group tail GH, n = 6/18; by tube, single tube ST/ group tube GT, n = 6/18 or by forceps, single forceps SF/ group forceps GF, n = 6/18) according to the order they were unpacked. All animals were marked on the tail with a black waterproof marker (renewed weekly) and in addition by ear punches on arrival. In the group-housed condition only the mouse marked on arrival as number three was exposed to behavioral testing.
Interaction-partners for behavioral testing (sNO and RIT testing) were 8 weeks old male mice of the C3H/HeJRj inbred strain (Janvier, Laval, France), which arrived at the facility 1 week before being used for the resident-intruder test (RIT). This strain was selected because of its brown color (easier to distinguish from the black C57BL/6NCrl mice in the RIT) and due to their even tempers (personal communication with commercial breeders). They were housed under the same abiotic conditions as the experimental subjects, but in a separate room which had no reversed light/dark cycle (lights on between 09:00–21:00 h).
Experimental setup
Experiments and handling were conducted during the dark phase, as illustrated in Fig 1. Mice were handled 4 times a week by one female experimenter by the assigned handling method, i.e. tail handling, forceps handling (Mouse Holding Forceps with Replaceable Tips, FST, Heidelberg, Germany) or tube handling (polycarbonate mouse handling tube, Datesand Group, Manchester, United Kingdom). Mouse cages were opened and mice were lifted one by one via their allocated handling method, placed on the wire lid, and then returned to their home cage. As the first 2 weeks of handling served as acclimation, the data collected in this period were not used for statistical analysis.
Cage cleaning and the nest building test were performed weekly on the same day (except before the RIT). Immediately after transferring the group-housed mice into clean cages, their behavior was recorded by a camera (Ikegami Digital) for 20 min.
One day after cage cleaning, mice were weighed, their fur was examined for wounds or barbering signs according to a standardized score sheet (S1 Fig). Tail blood was taken by puncturing the vein with a cannula for glucose determination and body temperature was measured by gently inserting a rectal thermometer 1cm twice, 30 min apart, to determine stress-induced hyperthermia (SIH). To minimize circadian influences on behavior, the starting point in the order of mice being handled and the order of cages cleaned, recorded and measured was changed weekly. All animal care and testing were carried out by the same person. Six days after cage cleaning (weeks 3 and 7) feces were collected for FCM analysis.
To investigate potential handling-, and housing-induced aggression, one mouse (number 3) out of each cage was subjected to 4 behavioral tests at the age of 11–12 weeks: The open field test (OF) followed by a social novel-object test (sNO), light/dark box test (LDB), hotplate (HP) and RIT. On each testing day mice were transferred to the testing room 25 min before behavioral testing for acclimatization, and only one test was conducted sequentially per day.
Behavioral analysis
Nest building test
A standard nestlet (Plexx B.V, AB Elst, The Netherlands) and approximately 0.5 g of the old nesting material (to decrease aggression in male mice [3, 16]) was transferred into the clean cage at the day of cage change. The nest quality was analyzed after 5 and 24 h using a scoring system modified from Deacon [13]. Score 1: nestlet untouched, score 2: nestlet largely untouched (> 90% intact), score 3: nestlet mostly shredded (< 50% intact nestlet), score 4: identifiable, but flat nest (> 90% torn up), score 5: partial cup, score 6: full dome.
Behavioral scoring of aggression-associated parameters
Only group-housed mice were weekly recorded after cage cleaning for 20 min at 35 lux by a camera positioned above the home cages. We assessed the i) attack latency, ii) the first attacks’ duration and iii) the number of attacks from the video. In accordance to Garner et al. [17] an attack was defined as the rushing and leaping at a partner with bites and kicks.
Open field and social novel-object test
Mice were examined in an open field test (OFT) for measurement of locomotion, anxiety and exploration [18] for 2 days between 10.00 a.m. to 1.30 p.m. The animal was placed using its assigned handling method into a square, black OFT arena (50 x 50 cm2) placed on an infrared light surface illuminated with 25 lux from above. Up to 3 mice were tracked simultaneously in separate arenas for 10 min and filmed by a camera from above. ‘Velocity’, ‘distance moved’ and the time spent in defined areas of the OF, i.e. ‘center time’ were evaluated [19]. Subsequently a social novel-object (sNO) test was conducted. After 10 min of habituation to the empty OFT arena, an unfamiliar C3H mouse in a small grid-box (7 cm x 7 cm x 8 cm) was placed in the center of the arena (10 cm distance to walls), representing a social novel object. By evaluating the ‘latency’, ‘the time’ and ‘the number of approaches’ (an approach = mouse is nearing the grid box in close vicinity with head turned in the direction of the box) in the sNO, we measured exploration and social behaviors. Finally, number of fecal boli produced during the OFT as well as the sNO test was counted as an indicator of emotionality [20]. The videos were processed within Noldus EthoVision 4.0 (Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, The Netherlands).
Light/dark box test
A Light/dark box test was performed to examine anxiety-like behaviors. The mouse was placed into the dark compartment (22.5 x 22.5 cm2, approximately 1 lux) which was connected by an aperture with the light compartment (31.5 x 22.5 cm2, approximately 600 lux). Number and latency of exits into, as well as time spent in the lit compartment was recorded for 5 min by a camera positioned above the box as previously described [21]. An exit was defined as the placement of all four paws into the light chamber [22].
Hotplate test
To test a mouse’s reaction to thermal pain, it was placed in the center of a clear Plexiglas cylinder (20 cm diameter) atop a hot plate (Ugo Basile Hot/Cold Plate 35100, Ugo Basile, Gemonio, Italy) which was maintained at 53 ± 0.3°C. The time until the mouse first lifted its hind paw followed by clear paw flinching or licking movements was recorded and the animal was then removed from the plate. To avoid possible tissue damage the cut off time for an animal to react was set at 45 s [23].
Resident-intruder test
Using a RIT, mice were tested towards a stranger to monitor aggressive-like behaviors [24, 25]. 12 days prior to the test the home cage was not changed to maintain territoriality, which is strongly dependent on the presence of olfactory cues in the bedding [26]. The test began when the intruder (unfamiliar C3H male)) was placed in the resident cage. Interactions between the test mouse and the intruder were recorded (camera from above, 3 lux) for a period of 10 min.
Behavior was scored from video [17] (focusing only on the behavior of the tested mouse). The duration of threat behavior (tail rattling, thrust, mounting) and aggressive behavior (attack latency, boxing, aggressive bite, attack, fighting, chase) were measured. Mice that did not fight during the test were excluded from the statistical outcome. Severe injuries of the C3H mouse would have led to an immediate termination of the test.
Clinical parameters
Barbering and biting wounds were recorded throughout the study by means of a standardized score sheet (S1 Fig).
Body weight
The body weight of each mouse was measured once a week.
Blood glucose
Weekly blood glucose levels were measured prior to determination of body temperature. Mice were placed on a cage top using their assigned handling method. Blood samples were obtained from the tail vein by puncture. Assessment of blood glucose level was done by means of a glucometer (Medisana MediTouch 2, Promed GmbH, Germany).
Body temperature
Body temperature was measured as an indicator of stress-induced hyperthermia (SIH) by using a rectal thermometer (Testo 108, Testo SE & Co. KGaA, Lenzkirch, Germany + MLT1404 Rectal Probe, ADInstruments Ltd, Oxford, United Kingdom) which was inserted, after dipping it into a lubricant (Vidisic, Bausch + Lomb, Germany), for a length of 1 cm into the rectum of the mice at time point T1 (min) and time point T2 (min, T1 + 30 min). This required brief restraint by the tail: After being placed with their determined method on a wire top of an empty cage, their tail was hold up by the researchers’ hand to insert the rectal probe, hence all animals were touched on their tails. The difference, ΔT (= T2 –T1) is the measure for SIH, which is defined as a relative short lasting body temperature elevation in response to stress [27]. Only temperature differences at point ΔT of more than 0.5°C were defined as SIH.
Feces collection and FCM analysis
Stress hormone levels of mice were monitored non-invasively by measuring fecal corticosterone metabolites (FCM). Feces collection from the 3rd and 7th week was performed at day 6 after cage cleaning around 2 pm. Mice were placed individually in empty Macrolon Type II cages (370cm2, Tecniplast, Milan, Italy) for a maximum of 50 min. The sampling occurred in a limited time frame as corticosterone concentration has a circadian rhythm, so once the required number of fecal boli was reached (± 6 droppings), the mouse was returned to the home cage.
Feces were stored in polypropylene tubes at -20°C. FCM were extracted in accordance with Palme et al. [28]: Feces were dried for 1 h at a temperature of 70°C and homogenized. An aliquot of 0.05 g was extracted with 1 ml of 80% methanol and the extracts stored at -20°C. Later they were analyzed using a 5α-pregnane-3β,11β,21-triol-20-one enzyme immunoassay [29, 30].
Organ weights
Mice were deeply anesthetized and euthanized i.p. with ketamine 195 mg/kg body weight (Bremer Pharma GmbH, Warburg) and xylazine 30 mg/kg body weight (Ecuphar GmbH, Greifswald). Thymus, spleen, adrenal glands, seminal vesicles, and testes were dissected and weighed.
Statistical analysis
All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS software, version 25 for Mac (IBM). Parametric and non-parametric tests were utilized depending on data distribution (Q-Q plots or Shapiro-Wilk test). For parametric data, two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) were carried out with ‘housing’ and ‘handling’ as factors and the Tukey test as post hoc test. An intra-group comparison of the body weight and blood glucose was carried out by using a repeated measurement analysis, in which the factor ‘housing’ and ‘handling’ was added. Non-parametric data was analyzed by the chi-square statistics, Mann-Whitney-U-Test, Kruskal-Wallis-Test and the Friedman-Test. Additionally, we calculated the standard deviation of all values, serving as a measure of variation observed in the data. Statistical significance of variation was evaluated by a Levene test. P ≤ 0.05 was considered statistically significant. * indicates p ≤ 0.05, ** indicates p ≤ 0.01 and *** indicates p ≤ 0.001.
Results
Table 1 illustrates significant and non-significant findings regarding the factor ‘housing’ and ‘handling’.
Table 1. Statistical results concerning the factors ‘housing’ and ‘handling’.
Effects/ interactions | Post hoc test (Tukey) | Effect size (Cohen's d) | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Test | Parameter | Statistical test | Housing | Handling | Housing x handling | Hand vs. forceps | Hand vs. tube | Forceps vs. tube | Housing | Hand vs. forceps | Hand vs. tube | Forceps vs. tube | |
Clinical parameters | Body weight | weight, week 8 (g) | ANOVA | *p = 0.039, F = 4.43, df = 1 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | -0.58 | -0.07 | 0.02 | 0.09 |
Temperature difference | temperature difference, week 7 (°C) | ANOVA | *p = 0.046, F = 4.14, df = 1 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | 0.60 | -0.49 | -0.45 | 0.04 | |
temperature difference, week 8 (°C) | n.s. | **p = 0.009, F = 4.99, df = 2 |
n.s. | **p = 0.01 | n.s. | p = 0.059 | 0.11 | -0.82 | -0.21 | 0.67 | |||
Stress induced hyperthermia | temperature difference, week 8 (°C) | Chi-squared | n.s. | *p = 0.028 | *p = 0.019 | n.s. | *p = 0.019 | ||||||
Blood glucose | Blood glucose, week 8 (mmol/L) | ANOVA | n.s. | n.s. | **p = 0.005, F = 6.5, df = 2 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | -0.13 | 0.33 | 0.36 | 0.09 | |
Fecal corticosterone metabolites | corticosterone, week 7 (pg/well) | ANOVA | **p = 0.005,F = 8.98, df = 1 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | -1.00 | 0.20 | -0.50 | -0.56 | |
corticosterone, week 7 (ng/0.05 g feces) | **p = 0.005, F = 8.98, df = 1 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | -1.00 | 0.20 | -0.50 | -0.56 | |||
corticosterone, week 7–3 (pg/well) | ANOVA, repeated measurements | **p = 0.002, F = 10.79, df = 1 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | ||||||
corticosterone, week 7–3 (ng/0.05 g feces) | **p = 0.002, F = 10.77, df = 1 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | |||||||
Behavioral analysis | BS of aggression-associated parameters | cages showing aggression, week 5–8, attack yes/no | Chi squared | / | **p = 0.002 | **p = 0.004 | n.s. | *p = 0.02 | |||||
Nest-test | quality, week 3, 5 h scores | Mann-Whitney-U or Kruskal-Wallis | *p = 0.025 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | ||||||
quality, week 4, 5 h scores | **p = 0.01 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | ||||||||
quality, week 6, 24 h scores | **p = 0.01 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | ||||||||
quality, week 8, 24 h scores | *p = 0.044 | p = 0.061 | *p = 0.033 | n.s. | n.s. | ||||||||
quality, week 3–8, 5 h scores | ***single: p < 0.001 ***group: p < 0.001 | ***hand: p < 0.001 *forceps: p = 0.037 ***tube: p = 0.001 |
*SH p = 0.011 SF: n.s. *ST: p = 0.013 ***GH: p = 0.001 GF: n.s. GT: p = 0.068 |
||||||||||
quality, week 3–8, 24 h scores | *single: p = 0.027; group: p = 0.102 |
hand: p = 0.32; forceps: p = 0.145; *tube: p = 0.049 |
SH: n.s. SF: n.s. *ST: p = 0.043; GH: n.s. GF: n.s. GT: n.s. |
||||||||||
Openfield | distance to walls 0–10 min (cm) | ANOVA | **p = 0.006, F = 8.76, df = 1 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | -0.89 | -0.15 | 0.54 | 0.69 | |
time in center 0–10 min (s) | **p = 0.0011, F = 7.2, df = 1 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | 0.93 | 0.12 | -0.38 | -0.56 | |||
Social Novel-Object test | distance moved 0–5 min (cm) | ANOVA | n.s. | n.s. | *p = 0.035, F = 3.75, df = 2 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | -0.02 | 0.15 | 0.51 | 0.39 | |
velocity 0–5 min (cm/sec) | n.s. | n.s. | *p = 0.036 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | -0.05 | 0.15 | 0.50 | 0.37 | |||
time in center 0–10 min (s) | *p = 0.043, F = 4.4, df = 1 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | -0.70 | 0.15 | -0.03 | -0.15 | |||
Light/dark box test | exit latency (s) | Mann-Whitney-U or Kruskal-Wallis | n.s. | *p = 0.031 | n.s. | **p = 0.007 | n.s. | ||||||
tail rattling (s) | Mann-Whitney-U or Kruskal-Wallis | *p = 0.036 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | |||||||
aggressive bite (s) | *p = 0.043 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | ||||||||
attack (s) | *p = 0.024 | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | n.s. | ||||||||
fighting (s) | *p = 0.016 | n.s. | n.s. | *p = 0.038 | n.s. |
n.s. = no significance. Identification of overall effects of the factors 'handling' and 'housing' as well as interaction effects. Asterisks indicate the level of significance (* p ≤ 0.05; ** p ≤ 0.01; *** p ≤ 0.001). Data is split in 'clinical parameters' and 'behavioral analysis' for better overview. Non-parametric tested parameters are highlighted in grey. The effect size (Cohen’s d) was calculated for normal distributed data analyzed with ANOVA. Parameters not appearing in this table revealed non-significant results.
Barbering and biting wounds
No bite wounds were visible on external exam during the experiment. In week 8, 2 mice of a GH-cage showed hair loss on their heads, which we attribute to barbering behavior of the third mouse in the cage.
Nest building test
Immediate nest building improved over the course of the experiment (3th - 8th week) in both housing conditions (single housed: Chi2 (5) = 33.3, p < 0.001; group housed: Chi2 (5) = 29.4, p < 0.001) and in all animal cages. Significant results were detected for the groups SH (Chi2 (5) = 14.9, p = 0.011), ST (Chi2 (5) = 14.4, p = 0.013) and GH (Chi2 (5) = 21.5, p = 0.001) measured by the Friedman-test. When nest quality was assessed after 24 h, only singly-housed animals improved their nest building performance over the course of the experiment (Chi2 (5) = 12.6, p = 0.027). This only applies for singly-housed, tube-handled mice in a statistically significant way (Chi2 (5) = 11.4, p = 0.043).
24 h scoring in the 8th week, measured with a non-parametric-test for independent samples, revealed an overall effect for the factor ‘housing’ (z = -2.01, p = 0.044). Post hoc testing showed a significant difference (U = 35, p = 0.033) between tail-handled animals and forceps-handled animals (Fig 2A). No significant effects after 5 h in week 8 were detected.
Behavioral scoring of aggression-associated parameters
The first aggressive behavior after cage cleaning appeared in 8 weeks old group-housed mice handled with forceps (GF) in the 5th week. Using the chi squared test on proportions of cages displaying aggression there was a significant overall effect concerning the handling methods (Chi2 (2) = 12.09, p = 0.002), (Fig 2B). Tail-handled animals showed no aggression at all, 1 out of 6 tube-handled cages only once (7th week) and 2 out of 6 forceps-handled cages showed aggression from 5th and 6th week until end of recording.
Body weight
Animals gained weight over time regardless of housing condition. At the conclusion of the experiment, an effect for the factor ‘housing’ was seen. Singly-housed mice were significantly lighter than group-housed mice (F(1,70) = 4.43, p = 0.039). No differences in weight could be attributed to ‘handling’, however. (Fig 2C). Additionally, variation differences were investigated for both factors but were not significant.
Body temperature
There was a significant overall effect only in the 7th week for the factor ‘housing’ (Chi2 (1) = 5.04, p = 0.025) with singly-housed mice showing increased hyperthermia. In the 8th week, a significant difference in the factor ‘handling’ (Chi2 (2) = 7.13, p = 0.028) was noted. Post hoc testing revealed a significant difference between forceps-handled and tube-handled mice (Chi2 (1) = 5.49, p = 0.019) or tail (Chi2 (1) = 5.49, p = 0.019) (Fig 3A). Furthermore, forceps-handled males had the greatest variation in temperature difference (tail, 0.2 ± 0.8; forceps, 0.9 ± 1; tube, 0.3 ± 0.8; mean ± SD) confirmed by Levene testing (tail vs. forceps: p = 0.026; tube vs. forceps: p = 0.026).
Blood glucose
Neither housing nor handling influenced blood glucose levels overall. In the 8th week, the interaction between housing and handling was significant (F(2,36) = 6.5, p = 0.005). Tail-handling in singly-housed mice led to the lowest, whereas in group-housed animals this handling condition evoked the highest blood glucose levels. For forceps-handled mice the blood glucose levels under group- and single housing conditions were most similar. Tube-handling in singly-housed mice revealed the highest and in group-housed animals the lowest blood glucose levels.
Light/dark box test
Anxiety-like behaviors analyzed in the LDB were altered depending on handling conditions. This was especially evident in the ‘latency to the first exit’ (Chi2 (2) = 6.92, p = 0.031). Tail-handled mice entered the lit compartment significantly later compared to tube-handled mice with post hoc testing (U (1) = 26.5, p = 0.007), (Fig 3B).
Hotplate test
Mouse reaction time on the hotplate was not dependent on the handling condition (F(2,33) = 3.08, p = 0.059), (Fig 3C).
Open field and social novel-object test
Open field: Locomotion in the OF was significantly altered by the factor ‘housing’ concerning the parameters ‘distance to walls 0–10 min’ (F(1,34) = 8.76, p = 0.006) and the ‘time in center 0–10 min’ (F(1,34) = 7.2, p = 0.011). Group-housed mice compared to single-housed mice spent more time in the center, hence achieved greater distances to the walls (Fig 4A and 4B).
Social novel-object: Tukey post hoc testing revealed that tube-handled animals spent significantly more time (t = 2.47, p = 0.049) near the interaction partner compared to forceps-handled animals. Depending on the housing condition and the handling method mice differed in the first 5 min in their outcome in the parameters ‘distance moved’ (F(2,30) = 3.75, p = 0.035), ‘velocity’ (F(2,30) = 3.71, p = 0.036). Additionally, group-housed animals spent significantly more time in the center (F(1,34) = 4.4, p = 0.043) compared to singly-housed ones.
Fecal corticosterone metabolites (FCM)
Handling had no significant effect on FCM concentrations (ng/0.05 g feces) when analyzed with a one-way ANOVA after we calculated the changes relative to the absolute baseline value at week 3 (F(2,33) = 0.24, p = 0.791). The same applies for absolute values measured in week 3 (F(2,33) = 2.17, p = 0.13) or 7 (F(2,33) = 1.42, p = 0.256). A bifactorial ANOVA revealed significant differences between group and individual-housed animals (F(1,34) = 10.77, p = 0.002) (Fig 4C). No difference regarding absolute FCM values in week 3 (F(1,34) = 0.76, p = 0.392) could be observed, whereas in week 7 a significant difference existed (F(1,34) = 8.98, p = 0.005).
Resident-intruder test
Of the 36 mice (6 mice of each condition; as stated above only one defined mouse of the group-housed mice was participating in the behavioral tests) used in this study, 21 (58%) were aggressive towards an intruder male and included in the statistical analysis. Single mice showed in general more aggressive behavior when analyzed with a Mann-Whitney-U-Test for independent samples, reflected in detail by: significantly more events of tail rattling (U = 25, p = 0.036), number of aggressive bites’ (U = 26.5, p = 0.043) as well as attacks’ (U = 23, p = 0.024) and ‘fighting’ (U = 21.5, p = 0.016). Handling conditions did not influence these behaviors significantly. A high variation for forceps-handled animals’ data was notable for the parameter ‘fighting’ (tail, 24.7 ± 15.7; forceps, 58 ± 56.1; tube, 42.4 ± 13.3; mean ± SD), confirmed by the Levene test (tail vs. forceps: p = 0.007; forceps vs. tube: p = 0.004).
Organ weights
All organs were removed by the same animal technician. Neither the housing condition nor the handling procedure significantly affected mouse organ weights.
Discussion
Our results indicate that forceps handling has a negative impact on aggression-associated parameters. Forceps-handled mice had visible deficits in the well-being parameter represented by the nest building test and an increase in the number of attacks after cage cleaning, higher (stress) reaction in the stress induced hyperthermia and less pain perception on the hotplate. Contrary to our expectations, tube handling only affected anxiety-like behavior and offered no advantages over tail-handling in all other investigated parameters.
In the literature tunnel- or tube-handling is praised as a non-aversive method that reduces anxiety-like behavior as well as optimizes the reliability of behavioral testing in laboratory mice [8, 31–33]. In our study, the tube-handled mice were the fastest to exit the dark compartment in the LDB test, which indicates they experienced less anxiety, consistent with the previous findings [31, 34]. In contrast, the performance of mice picked up by the tail with a gloved hand was the poorest reinforcing the conclusion by Hurst et al. that this traditional way of mouse handling causes stress and anxiety [8]. Contrary to the outcomes reported in Gouveia et al. [31], the handling method did not have a strong influence on the general exploratory behavior when tested in the OF. In the sNO test, tube-handled animals did spend more time with the newly introduced interaction partner than those lifted by the tail. Hence, one may assume their curiousness/social drive to be greater than their anxiety. No further changes in behavior associated with tube-handling were observed. Nest building performance of tube-handled mice over the time did not differ from tail-handled animals after 5 h, whereas they performed better than forceps-handled animals. After 2 months of housing the scores after 24 h were not as good as in tail-handled animals, but still better than in forceps-handled mice. In stress-related parameters such as the SIH, tube- and tail-handled animals had similar results.
There are several possible explanations for the restricted outcome of the actual predicted advantage of tube-handling: First, for experimental testing, i.e. to gain blood, mice in our study were restrained once a week on a wire cage lid by hand, while Hurst and colleagues [8, 31] examined tube/tunnel-handling only via observation, so no restraint was applied to animals in Hurst et al.’s studies. In scientific research, most experiments do need to obtain more information from mice than behavior, therefore restraint is needed. This restraint, even if it was just performed once a week, may have had a greater influence than predicted.
Another explanation could be the difference in frequency and duration of handling in the different studies. Hurst and West handled CD-1, BALB/c and C57BL/6N mice only for 9 days and reported a significantly improved performance of non-aversive handled mice in behavioral testing [8, 31]. Novac et al. reported an absence of handling effects in their study handling CD-1 mice daily for 15 weeks [35], while we handled mice 4 times per week for 8 weeks and did see, contrary to them a handling-effect but not as large as shown by Hurst et al. [8]. It is known that mice may adapt to experimenter handling [36] thus, the effect of tube-handling in the present experiment should be in between the reported outcomes of the previous studies, which they are. Hence, we may see an effect of mice slightly habituated to handling.
The major consequences of handling were seen for forceps-handled animals. As predicted, group-housed, forceps-handled mice showed increased levels of aggression-related behavior. The number of attacks was significantly higher compared to tail- or tube-handled conspecifics. So far, no effects of forceps handling on aggressive behavior have been reported in the literature, consequently no previous data exist for us to compare our results. However, aggression is a welfare issue for mice and moreover creates additional variability [3, 37]. Interestingly the fecal corticosterone levels, which are thought to reflect stress experienced by the mice, did not show any difference between handling groups. These findings are in contrast with previous studies, which found changes in handling methods associated with changes in plasma corticosterone concentrations [38, 39].
We had expected forceps-handled mice to become more sensitive to pain, as this method is from an anthropocentric point of view the most painful one. Forceps-handling implied lifting the animals 4 times a week by forceps on the base of their tail and even if the distal part of the forceps is edged in plastic, it would seem to have an impact on pain perception. The results of this test, although not significant, were interesting as they contradicted our original hypothesis. Forceps-handled animals stayed the longest time on the plate, longer than mice handled with both other methods. An explanation might be a higher stress-induces analgesia (or hypoalgesia) in the forceps-handled mice, which is a well-known phenomenon [40]. As effective pain assessment is critical in terms of animal welfare and the reduction of variation within experimental group, an influence of other painful procedures on those which are conducted with the ongoing research experiment, should be determined [32]. Routine husbandry procedures therefore should not influence those outcomes.
We used rectal temperature to investigate the distress caused by different handling methods. The body temperature can be measured by a rectal thermometer [41, 42] but this method may increase mouse distress due to handling [43, 44]. Therefore, all animals were handled similarly for rectal temperature measurements. We hypothesized that a difference in the temperature measurement was dependent on the previous, 4 times a week conducted handling method, not on the brief restraint required for the measure. Our experiment demonstrated SIH was altered by handling conditions in forceps-handled mice in the last week of measurement: Their SIH was greater than in the other handling conditions, thus their stress-recovery within the 30 min after being handled was delayed. Though a consistent insertion of the probe is important (Bouwknecht et al recommend 2–2.5 cm [27]) we only inserted approximately 1 cm of the rectal probe as the mice were only 3 weeks old on arrival and we wanted to avoid potential injuries to the animal. As this may have led to unreliable measurements, further investigations of the SIH would be needed to verify our outcome.
The investigation of housing effects on mice is far more established than those of handling. In general, group housing is recommended over single housing [1] to maximize well-being of these social animals [45, 46]. However, separation of co-housed animals is a practical solution if aggression arises, even though it is known that male mice prefer each other’s company over individual-housing [47, 48]. Current research opinions differ concerning the consequences of housing configuration on stress-related parameters. Results of several studies indicate individual housing causes stress to mice resulting in stereotypy, nervousness, or handling difficulties [48]. Furthermore, single housing may affect the animals’ physiological stress response and hypersensitivity against toxins [47, 49]. Contrary to those results, Kamakura et al published in 2016 that single housing caused less stress to mice when compared with group housing [50], underlining their results by urinary corticosterone analysis. Urinary corticosterone levels were hereby decreased in individually-housed mice. Those results are supported by previous studies which led to the same findings [51, 52]. Kappel et al. recently published a review paper discussing whether it is in the best interest of male mice to be housed together in groups or alone [53], concluding that housing is highly context dependent.
The present study demonstrated several differences in animals based on housing condition. The body weights measured after 2 months clearly differed between group- and single-housed mice with single ones being lighter. Stress can influence the food intake and therefore the body weight, whereby some stressors lead to a decrease, but others have the opposite effect [54–57]. In our results, the weight differential seen could be due to nourishment competition in group-housed mice resulting in an increase in food intake compared to singly-housed mice, or singly-housed mice were stressed by isolation.
However, looking at the results of the fecal corticosterone metabolite analysis, no adverse effect of single housing is obvious. Instead increased FCM levels in group-housed mice were found. Corticosterone, a glucocorticoid which is released from the adrenal cortex in response to a stress stimulus, is a well-established stress marker in research [29, 52]. Even though it is most commonly detected in blood plasma, blood sampling itself as an invasive technique is known to be stressful, especially for small animals [29, 51]. Based on these facts we chose to collect fecal samples for measuring corticosterone metabolites instead, which is a non-invasive technique able to measure prior substantial stress [58]. The measured FCM levels in our singly-housed mice were decreased compared to group-housed animals perhaps indicating less stress compared to their group-housed conspecifics.
This conclusion can be underlined regarding the findings in the nest building test. Mice in both housing conditions improved their initial nest building performance during the course of the experiment. However, scoring after 24 h revealed a nest-building improvement only in singly-housed mice. As nest building behavior is an indicator of well-being in mice and reduced by stress [13, 59], group-housed mice seem to be more stressed in our study. To determine the position of an individual inside the dominance hierarchy of group-housed animals, aggressive interactions are an inevitable consequence, causing stress in animals involved in the agonistic encounter and bystanders. This could be an explanation for the previous results. However, the complexity of nests is of less impact in group-housed mice since they may compensate for lower nest qualities by physical contact [60]. Therefore the cold stress described by Gaskill et al. [60] could have led to respective differences in behavior since the impact of nest building in singly-housed mice is higher.
Looking at the data of the SIH, the single-housed mice experienced a single event of stress induced hyperthermia in the 10th week, which is not acutely evident anymore in the following week. SIH has been described in group- and single-housed conditions in several studies [27, 61, 62]. Due to our rectal temperature probe insertion of only 1 cm rather than the recommended 2–3 cm we could have caused some unreliable measurements.
In the RIT singly-housed mice showed more thrust- and aggressive behavior towards the intruder, whereas the distribution of attacking the intruder-males was equal for singly- and group-housed animals. Our results are therefore not consistent with previous findings which state that group-housed male mice are less likely to attack the intruder mouse [63]. We had to modify the standard protocol due to our experimental design; we could not house our residents individually in company with females before testing, as we wanted to examine the effect of handling conditions on behavior in different housing conditions. Additionally, our intruders were all housed singly to prevent a development of a social hierarchy, which might confound results by researchers selecting a mouse at different places in that cage’s dominance hierarchy, possibly influencing the resident’s reaction. In previous studies we consulted, intruder mice were always group-housed. The predicted outcome of singly-housed mice being more aggressive was only partially confirmed, as mice in both housing conditions did not differ in their attack latency, but singly-housed mice did show more aggressive behavior in total.
Another housing effect was detected in the OF, where group-housed mice exhibited fewer anxiety-related spatial movement patterns, supporting previous research on the effect of housing conditions on activity and anxiety behavior in mice [64]. In the sNO assay, the introduction of the caged male mouse, which allowed olfactory, acoustic and limited physical contact (but no biting), revealed no differences in behavior based on housing.
We used mice ordered from commercial breeders by age to study intermale aggression, therefore we probably mixed non-littermates, even if it is known that grouping littermates decreases aggression [21]. This approach was chosen to investigate aggressive behavior under most realistic conditions, because housing only littermates together in cages is mostly not achievable due to the high number of mice required in experiments.
Even though our aim was to study aggression in male mice, agonistic interactions among all males were generally low in our study. An explanation might be that we kept the mice in stable groups from weaning throughout the experiment. Another approach to explain the low aggression could be the way we investigated for biting wounds. Even though bite wounds are difficult to see through fur [65] we chose to use only visual examination as further handling of the mice would be an additional source of stress. It is likely we missed smaller injuries on the animals. Another explanatory approach might be found in the environment of our experimental setting. Only male mice were investigated and therefore they were kept in the experimental room without any female mice. Might the absence of females and consequently their pheromones might have led to the decreased levels of intra-cage aggression? The question of whether the presence of female mice influences male agonistic interactions has so far not been tested. A further investigation of this aspect, i.e. redoing the experiment with female mice in the husbandry room could be a valuable addition to the mouse aggression literature.
Conclusion
Our findings assessing the method of handling on aggression-associated parameters in C57BL/6NCrl mice do have important implications for common handling practices. Lifting mice up by their tails with forceps appears to stimulate aggressive behavior within groups of familiar adult mice more than lifting them up with fingers or in a tube. Consequently, this method influences behavioral assessments of aggression. Confirming the recommendations of Hurst et al. (18), tube-handling should be applied when minimization of anxiety in experimental mice is desired. Moreover, since group vs. single-housing significantly influenced some of our test results, these maintenance conditions should be considered carefully when planning or analyzing an experiment. The findings of the present study should be kept in mind when forceps-handling is the standard handling procedure (i.e. IVC cage changing), especially if the parameters being investigated in the course of an experiment are pain-, stress- or behavior-related.
Supporting information
Acknowledgments
We thank Charles River (Sulzfeld, Germany) and Janvier (Laval, France) for donating the mice used in this study. We thank members of our facility especially L. Walisch for helping us with tissue processing as well as the Institute of Medical Biometry and Informatics (IMBI), Heidelberg University, for their support.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
This project was supported in part by a grant of the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (FOR 2591, GZ: GA427/12-1) to P.G. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. There was no additional external or internal funding received for this study.
References
- 1.DIRECTIVE 2010/63/EU OF THE EUROPEAN PARLIAMENT AND OF THE COUNCIL of 22 September 2010 on the protection of animals used for scientific purposes, (2010).
- 2.Van Loo PL, Van Zutphen LF, Baumans V. Male management: Coping with aggression problems in male laboratory mice. Lab Anim. 2003;37(4):300–13. 10.1258/002367703322389870 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Weber EM, Dallaire JA, Gaskill BN, Pritchett-Corning KR, Garner JP. Aggression in group-housed laboratory mice: why can't we solve the problem? Lab Anim (NY). 2017;46(4):157–61. 10.1038/laban.1219 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Howerton CL, Garner JP, Mench JA. Effects of a running wheel-igloo enrichment on aggression, hierarchy linearity, and stereotypy in group-housed male CD-1 (ICR) mice. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 2008;115(1–2):90–103. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Marashi V, Barnekow A, Ossendorf E, Sachser N. Effects of different forms of environmental enrichment on behavioral, endocrinological, and immunological parameters in male mice. Horm Behav. 2003;43(2):281–92. Epub 2003/04/16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Benedetti F, Fresi F, Maccioni P, Smeraldi E. Behavioural sensitization to repeated sleep deprivation in a mice model of mania. Behavioural brain research. 2008;187(2):221–7. 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.09.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Lewejohann L, Zipser B, Sachser N. "Personality" in laboratory mice used for biomedical research: a way of understanding variability? Dev Psychobiol. 2011;53(6):624–30. Epub 2011/08/26. 10.1002/dev.20553 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Hurst JL, West RS. Taming anxiety in laboratory mice. Nat Methods. 2010;7(10):825–6. Epub 2010/09/14. 10.1038/nmeth.1500 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Palanza P, Parmigiani S. How does sex matter? Behavior, stress and animal models of neurobehavioral disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;76(Pt A):134–43. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Chourbaji S, Zacher C, Sanchis-Segura C, Spanagel R, Gass P. Social and structural housing conditions influence the development of a depressive-like phenotype in the learned helplessness paradigm in male mice. Behav Brain Res. 2005;164(1):100–6. Epub 2005/07/28. 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Bartolomucci A, Palanza P, Sacerdote P, Ceresini G, Chirieleison A, Panerai AE, et al. Individual housing induces altered immuno-endocrine responses to psychological stress in male mice. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2003;28(4):540–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Deacon RM. Assessing nest building in mice. Nat Protoc. 2006;1(3):1117–9. 10.1038/nprot.2006.170 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Deacon R. Assessing burrowing, nest construction, and hoarding in mice. J Vis Exp. 2012;(59):e2607 Epub 2012/01/20. 10.3791/2607 PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3369766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Bryant CD, Zhang NN, Sokoloff G, Fanselow MS, Ennes HS, Palmer AA, et al. Behavioral differences among C57BL/6 substrains: implications for transgenic and knockout studies. J Neurogenet. 2008;22(4):315–31. Epub 2008/12/17. 10.1080/01677060802357388 PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3697827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.rodents Fwgorogfhmo, rabbits, Mahler Convenor M, Berard M, Feinstein R, Gallagher A, et al. FELASA recommendations for the health monitoring of mouse, rat, hamster, guinea pig and rabbit colonies in breeding and experimental units. Lab Anim. 2014;48(3):178–92. Epub 2014/02/06. 10.1177/0023677213516312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Van Loo PL, Mol JA, Koolhaas JM, Van Zutphen BF, Baumans V. Modulation of aggression in male mice: influence of group size and cage size. Physiol Behav. 2001;72(5):675–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Joseph Garner BG, Cathleen Rodda, Cathleen Rodda, Aria Prater, Jon Klein, Hanno Würbel, Georgia Mason, Anna Olsson, Elin Weber, Jerome Geronimo. Mouse Ethogram Available from: mousebehavior.org/ethogram
- 18.Carola V, D'Olimpio F, Brunamonti E, Mangia F, Renzi P. Evaluation of the elevated plus-maze and open-field tests for the assessment of anxiety-related behaviour in inbred mice. Behav Brain Res. 2002;134(1–2):49–57. Epub 2002/08/23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Domanskyi A, Geissler C, Vinnikov IA, Alter H, Schober A, Vogt MA, et al. Pten ablation in adult dopaminergic neurons is neuroprotective in Parkinson's disease models. FASEB J. 2011;25(9):2898–910. Epub 2011/05/20. 10.1096/fj.11-181958 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Burgdorf J, Panksepp J, Brudzynski SM, Beinfeld MC, Cromwell HC, Kroes RA, et al. The effects of selective breeding for differential rates of 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations on emotional behavior in rats. Dev Psychobiol. 2009;51(1):34–46. Epub 2008/09/27. 10.1002/dev.20343 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Chourbaji S, Urani A, Inta I, Sanchis-Segura C, Brandwein C, Zink M, et al. IL-6 knockout mice exhibit resistance to stress-induced development of depression-like behaviors. Neurobiol Dis. 2006;23(3):587–94. 10.1016/j.nbd.2006.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Bourin M, Hascoët MJEjop. The mouse light/dark box test. 2003;463(1–3):55–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Chourbaji S, Brandwein C, Vogt MA, Dormann C, Hellweg R, Gass P. Nature vs. nurture: can enrichment rescue the behavioural phenotype of BDNF heterozygous mice? Behav Brain Res. 2008;192(2):254–8. Epub 2008/06/10. 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.04.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Malkesman O, Maayan R, Weizman A, Weller A. Aggressive behavior and HPA axis hormones after social isolation in adult rats of two different genetic animal models for depression. Behav Brain Res. 2006;175(2):408–14. Epub 2006/10/31. 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.09.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Floody OR, Pfaff DW. Aggressive behavior in female hamsters: the hormonal basis for fluctuations in female aggressiveness correlated with estrous state. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1977;91(3):443–64. Epub 1977/06/01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Harrington JE. Recognition of territorial boundaries by olfactory cues in mice (Mus musculus L.). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie. 1976;41(3):295–306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Adriaan Bouwknecht J, Olivier B, Paylor RE. The stress-induced hyperthermia paradigm as a physiological animal model for anxiety: a review of pharmacological and genetic studies in the mouse. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2007;31(1):41–59. Epub 2006/04/19. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.02.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Palme R, Touma C, Arias N, Dominchin MF, Lepschy M. Steroid extraction: get the best out of faecal samples. Wien Tierarztl Monatsschr. 2013;100(9–10):238–46. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Touma C, Sachser N, Mostl E, Palme R. Effects of sex and time of day on metabolism and excretion of corticosterone in urine and feces of mice. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2003;130(3):267–78. Epub 2003/02/28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Touma C, Palme R, Sachser N. Analyzing corticosterone metabolites in fecal samples of mice: a noninvasive technique to monitor stress hormones. Horm Behav. 2004;45(1):10–22. Epub 2004/01/22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Gouveia K, Hurst JL. Optimising reliability of mouse performance in behavioural testing: the major role of non-aversive handling. Sci Rep. 2017;7:44999 Epub 2017/03/23. 10.1038/srep44999 PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC5359560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Miller A, Kitson G, Skalkoyannis B, Leach M. The effect of isoflurane anaesthesia and buprenorphine on the mouse grimace scale and behaviour in CBA and DBA/2 mice. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2015;172:58–62. Epub 2016/03/05. 10.1016/j.applanim.2015.08.038 PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4768077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Nakamura Y, Suzuki K. Tunnel use facilitates handling of ICR mice and decreases experimental variation. J Vet Med Sci. 2018;80(6):886–92. Epub 2018/04/17. 10.1292/jvms.18-0044 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Gouveia K, Hurst JL. Reducing mouse anxiety during handling: effect of experience with handling tunnels. PLoS One. 2013;8(6):e66401 Epub 2013/07/11. 10.1371/journal.pone.0066401 PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3688777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Novak J, Bailoo JD, Melotti L, Rommen J, Wurbel H. An Exploration Based Cognitive Bias Test for Mice: Effects of Handling Method and Stereotypic Behaviour. PLoS One. 2015;10(7):e0130718 Epub 2015/07/15. 10.1371/journal.pone.0130718 PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4496074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Taylor K, Gordon N, Langley G, Higgins W. Estimates for worldwide laboratory animal use in 2005. Altern Lab Anim. 2008;36(3):327–42. Epub 2008/07/30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Sherwin CJAW. The influences of standard laboratory cages on rodents and the validity of research data. 2004;13(1):9–15. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Ghosal S, Nunley A, Mahbod P, Lewis AG, Smith EP, Tong J, et al. Mouse handling limits the impact of stress on metabolic endpoints. Physiol Behav. 2015;150:31–7. Epub 2015/06/17. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.06.021 PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4546855. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Ono M, Sasaki H, Nagasaki K, Torigoe D, Ichii O, Sasaki N, et al. Does the routine handling affect the phenotype of disease model mice? Jpn J Vet Res. 2016;64(4):265–71. Epub 2016/11/01. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Mogil JS, Sternberg WF, Balian H, Liebeskind JC, Sadowski BJP, behavior. Opioid and nonopioid swim stress-induced analgesia: a parametric analysis in mice. 1996;59(1):123–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Wong JP, Saravolac EG, Clement JG, Nagata LP. Development of a murine hypothermia model for study of respiratory tract influenza virus infection. Lab Anim Sci. 1997;47(2):143–7. Epub 1997/04/01. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Soothill JS, Morton DB, Ahmad A. The HID50 (hypothermia-inducing dose 50): an alternative to the LD50 for measurement of bacterial virulence. Int J Exp Pathol. 1992;73(1):95–8. Epub 1992/02/01. PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC2002468. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Toth LA. Defining the moribund condition as an experimental endpoint for animal research. ILAR J. 2000;41(2):72–9. Epub 2001/06/15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Warn PA, Brampton MW, Sharp A, Morrissey G, Steel N, Denning DW, et al. Infrared body temperature measurement of mice as an early predictor of death in experimental fungal infections. Lab Anim. 2003;37(2):126–31. Epub 2003/04/12. 10.1258/00236770360563769 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Kalliokoski O, Teilmann AC, Jacobsen KR, Abelson KS, Hau J. The lonely mouse—single housing affects serotonergic signaling integrity measured by 8-OH-DPAT-induced hypothermia in male mice. PLoS One. 2014;9(12):e111065 Epub 2014/12/02. 10.1371/journal.pone.0111065 PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4249803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Arakawa H. Ethological approach to social isolation effects in behavioral studies of laboratory rodents. Behav Brain Res. 2018;341:98–108. Epub 2017/12/31. 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.12.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Van Loo PL, Van de Weerd HA, Van Zutphen LF, Baumans V. Preference for social contact versus environmental enrichment in male laboratory mice. Lab Anim. 2004;38(2):178–88. Epub 2004/04/09. 10.1258/002367704322968867 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Van Loo PL, de Groot AC, Van Zutphen BF, Baumans V. Do male mice prefer or avoid each other's company? Influence of hierarchy, kinship, and familiarity. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science. 2001;4(2):91–103. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Ruis MA, te Brake JH, Engel B, Buist WG, Blokhuis HJ, Koolhaas JM. Adaptation to social isolation. Acute and long-term stress responses of growing gilts with different coping characteristics. Physiol Behav. 2001;73(4):541–51. Epub 2001/08/10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Kamakura R, Kovalainen M, Leppaluoto J, Herzig KH, Makela KA. The effects of group and single housing and automated animal monitoring on urinary corticosterone levels in male C57BL/6 mice. Physiol Rep. 2016;4(3). 10.14814/phy2.12703 PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4758932. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Hunt C, Hambly C. Faecal corticosterone concentrations indicate that separately housed male mice are not more stressed than group housed males. Physiol Behav. 2006;87(3):519–26. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.11.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Arndt SS, Laarakker MC, van Lith HA, van der Staay FJ, Gieling E, Salomons AR, et al. Individual housing of mice—impact on behaviour and stress responses. Physiol Behav. 2009;97(3–4):385–93. Epub 2009/03/24. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.03.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Kappel S, Hawkins P, Mendl MT. To Group or Not to Group? Good Practice for Housing Male Laboratory Mice. Animals (Basel). 2017;7(12). Epub 2017/12/01. 10.3390/ani7120088 PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC5742782. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Adam TC, Epel ES. Stress, eating and the reward system. Physiol Behav. 2007;91(4):449–58. Epub 2007/06/05. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Tamashiro KL, Hegeman MA, Nguyen MM, Melhorn SJ, Ma LY, Woods SC, et al. Dynamic body weight and body composition changes in response to subordination stress. Physiol Behav. 2007;91(4):440–8. Epub 2007/05/22. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.004 PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC1986729. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Tamashiro KL, Nguyen MM, Ostrander MM, Gardner SR, Ma LY, Woods SC, et al. Social stress and recovery: implications for body weight and body composition. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2007;293(5):R1864–74. Epub 2007/09/15. 10.1152/ajpregu.00371.2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Ulrich-Lai YM, Herman JP. Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(6):397–409. Epub 2009/05/27. 10.1038/nrn2647 PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4240627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Palme R, Rettenbacher S, Touma C, El-Bahr SM, Mostl E. Stress hormones in mammals and birds: comparative aspects regarding metabolism, excretion, and noninvasive measurement in fecal samples. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2005;1040:162–71. Epub 2005/05/14. 10.1196/annals.1327.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Jirkof P, Fleischmann T, Cesarovic N, Rettich A, Vogel J, Arras M. Assessment of postsurgical distress and pain in laboratory mice by nest complexity scoring. Lab Anim. 2013;47(3):153–61. Epub 2013/04/09. 10.1177/0023677213475603 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Gaskill BN, Rohr SA, Pajor EA, Lucas JR, Garner JPJAABS. Some like it hot: mouse temperature preferences in laboratory housing. 2009;116(2–4):279–85. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Veening JG, Bouwknecht JA, Joosten HJ, Dederen PJ, Zethof TJ, Groenink L, et al. Stress-induced hyperthermia in the mouse: c-fos expression, corticosterone and temperature changes. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2004;28(4):699–707. Epub 2004/07/28. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2004.05.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Van der Heyden JA, Zethof TJ, Olivier B. Stress-induced hyperthermia in singly housed mice. Physiol Behav. 1997;62(3):463–70. Epub 1997/09/01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Yang T, Yang CF, Chizari MD, Maheswaranathan N, Burke KJ, Borius M, et al. Social control of hypothalamus-mediated male aggression. Neuron. 2017;95(4):955–70. e4. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Voikar V, Polus A, Vasar E, Rauvala H. Long-term individual housing in C57BL/6J and DBA/2 mice: assessment of behavioral consequences. Genes Brain Behav. 2005;4(4):240–52. Epub 2005/06/01. 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2004.00106.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Gaskill BN, Stottler A, Pritchett-Corning KR, Wong LK, Geronimo J, Garner JPJAABS. He's getting under my skin! Comparing the sensitivity and specificity of dermal vs subcuticular lesions as a measure of aggression in mice. 2016;183:77–85. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.