Abstract
Previous studies have demonstrated that migraine is associated with enhanced perception and altered cerebral processing of sensory stimuli. More recently, it has been suggested that this sensory hypersensitivity might reflect a more general enhanced response to aversive emotional stimuli. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging and emotional face stimuli (fearful, happy and sad faces), we compared whole-brain activation between 41 migraine patients without aura in interictal period and 49 healthy controls. Migraine patients showed increased neural activation to fearful faces compared to neutral faces in the right middle frontal gyrus and frontal pole relative to healthy controls. We also found that higher attack frequency in migraine patients was related to increased activation mainly in the right primary somatosensory cortex (corresponding to the face area) to fearful expressions and in the right dorsal striatal regions to happy faces. In both analyses, activation differences remained significant after controlling for anxiety and depressive symptoms. These findings indicate that enhanced response to emotional stimuli might explain the migraine trigger effect of psychosocial stressors that gradually leads to increased somatosensory response to emotional clues and thus contributes to the progression or chronification of migraine.
Keywords: Migraine, Headache chronification, fMRI, Emotion processing, Somatosensory cortex, Psychosocial stress
Highlights
-
•
First fMRI study to explore neural response to emotional faces in migraine patients
-
•
Migraine patients showed increased activation to fear in the right frontal regions
-
•
Migraine frequency was related to enhanced activation to fearful and happy faces
-
•
Activation in the right S1 and dorsal striatum was linked to migraine frequency
-
•
Sensitivity to emotional stimuli might have a role in triggering migraine
1. Introduction
Migraine headaches affect approximately 12% of the general population, with a lifetime incidence of 43% for females and 18% for males (Lipton et al., 2007; Stewart et al., 2008). Patients with migraine process and perceive sensory information differently than people without migraine (for a review, see Harriott and Schwedt, 2014; Schwedt et al., 2015). There is a large body of research supporting that during and between migraine attacks patients show enhanced perception and altered cerebral processing of somatosensory, visual, auditory, and olfactory stimuli. This hypersensitivity to sensory stimuli is specific to migraine and it has not been reported to the same extent in other headache or pain disorders. However, more recently, it has been proposed that migraine may be associated with a more general sensitivity to aversive/unpleasant stimuli (Wang et al., 2017; Wilcox et al., 2016). That is, patients with migraine display altered brain activation in response to negative emotional stimuli.
In support of this view, negative emotional events and emotional stress have been shown to play a significant role in precipitating or increasing migraine attacks (Andress-Rothrock et al., 2010; Kelman, 2007). In these studies, patients with migraine reported emotional stress as the most common trigger for their headache. Nevertheless, only a limited number of studies examined whether migraine patients display alterations in their processing of emotional stimuli, and they have found inconsistent results, with some showing enhanced reaction to all kinds of emotional stimuli and others demonstrating this response to negative emotional stimuli only.
For instance, based on event-related brain potential (ERP) studies (Buodo et al., 2011; Steppacher et al., 2016), there is evidence that adults and children with migraine exhibit enhanced reaction (indexed by larger late positive potential, LPP) to both positive and negative emotional stimuli (i.e., pictures depicting emotionally evocative scenes) selected from the International Affective Picture System database (IAPS; Lang et al., 2008). In addition, Andreatta et al. (2012) demonstrated that migraine patients showed enhanced N170 amplitudes toward angry faces and larger LPP toward happy faces (compared to neutral ones) than controls. These results raise the question if migraine patients are generally more sensitive to the high emotional relevance of a situation instead of its unpleasantness. It can be assumed that the cortical hyperresponsivity to sensory stimuli frequently demonstrated in migraine patients (Aurora and Wilkinson, 2007; de Tommaso et al., 2014) might also lead to intensified perceptions of emotional stimuli.
In contrast, according to recent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies, migraine patients exhibited increased brain response to negative IAPS stimuli, and no differences were found between patients and controls in response to positive IAPS pictures (Wang et al., 2017; Wilcox et al., 2016). These studies demonstrated increased activation to negative pictures in regions involved in visual, emotional and pain processing (such as amygdala, posterior cingulate, caudate, thalamus, Wilcox et al., 2016; visual cortex, cerebellum, Wang et al., 2017).
However, to our knowledge, no previous studies have employed fMRI to examine the neural response to face stimuli in migraine patients. In addition, research about the effect of migraine severity indices (e.g., disease duration, attack frequency) on the processing of emotional stimuli is still lacking. Given that previous studies have found association between brain structural and functional alterations and indices of disease severity in migraine (Hubbard et al., 2014; Mathur et al., 2016; Schwedt and Dodick, 2009), it is reasonable to believe that sensitivity to emotional stimuli may differ across patients as a function of migraine severity. It seems that migraine patients show alterations mainly in pain-related brain regions and these alterations are positively associated with longer disease duration and higher headache frequency.
The present study was therefore designed to investigate the neural response to emotional faces in episodic migraine without aura patients during the interictal period using fMRI. In this study, fearful, sad and happy faces were chosen because these expressions are unequivocal signals of negative and positive emotional states. Furthermore, we also aimed at evaluating the effects of different migraine indices (duration, frequency, pain intensity and impact of migraine headaches). In light of previous studies, we expected enhanced brain response to both negative and positive expressions in participants with migraine compared to healthy controls. Specifically, we hypothesized increased blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) response in regions related to emotional processing and pain (e.g., amygdala, prefrontal cortex, caudate, anterior cingulate cortex, insula; Cauda et al., 2012; Fusar-Poli et al., 2009; Lindquist et al., 2012; Peyron et al., 2000; Phan et al., 2004; Wilcox et al., 2016). We further hypothesized that these functional brain alterations in patients would be associated with migraine severity indices, and not explained by anxiety and depressive symptoms. These symptoms should be taken into account because both anxiety and depression are strongly associated with migraine (Louter et al., 2015; Peres et al., 2017), and these symptoms independently predict deficits in emotion processing, particularly in recognizing facial expressions (Bourke et al., 2010; Demenescu et al., 2010; Stuhrmann et al., 2011; Surcinelli et al., 2006). That is, both anxious and depressive symptoms are related to increased reactivity to negative stimuli, and depression is also associated with reduced reactivity to positive stimuli.
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Participants
Participants were recruited via university advertisements, newspaper articles and headache clinics. Of the 124 participants initially contacted, 94 participants (42 migraine patients, 52 healthy controls) aged 20 to 37 years met the inclusion criteria and agreed to participate in the study. Of these, two healthy controls had to be excluded for technical reasons and two participants, one from each group, for movement artifacts (criteria described below) resulting in a total of 90 participants (63 females, M age = 26.29, SD = 4.53).
The final sample consisted of 41 patients with episodic migraine without aura (33 females, 20–37 years old, M age = 27.00, SD = 4.92) and 49 healthy adult volunteers (30 females, 21–37 years old, M age = 25.69, SD = 4.13), matched for age and education. Both patients and controls were right-handed, as assessed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971), had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and no history of chronic, neurological (except migraine) or psychiatric disorder. To rule out the presence of mental disorders, participants were assessed using the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.; Sheehan et al., 1998) by senior neurologist and psychiatrist researchers.
Diagnosis of episodic migraine without aura was made by a headache specialist using the International Classification of Headache Disorders-III criteria (ICHD-III, beta version; Headache Classification Committee of the International Headache Society (IHS), 2013). No patients reported migraine attacks 48 h prior to the scan, and 24 h after the scan. They refrained from taking any analgesics 48 h before the scan session and did not take any prophylactic medicine during the last three months. Controls had no history of migraine, or other headache condition.
In accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, written informed consent forms were obtained from all subjects prior to participation in the study, and the study protocol was approved by the Scientific and Research Ethics Committee of the Medical Research Council (Hungary).
2.2. Clinical measures
Demographic data (age, sex, and educational level) and the following clinical features were collected from all migraine patients: 1) age at migraine onset; 2) number of years with migraine; 3) migraine frequency (average number of migraine per month); 4) estimated lifetime number of migraine attacks; 5) pain intensity of migraine attacks in the last 3 months (measured by a 0–10 numerical rating scale); 6) impact of migraine over the last 3 months (assessed by the Migraine Disability Assessment Scale, MIDAS; Stewart et al., 2000). A total MIDAS score was calculated for each patient by summing the number of missed days due to headache from work/school, household work, non-work (family, social, leisure) activity, and days with (at least 50%) reduced productivity over a 3-month period.
Depressive and anxiety symptoms were assessed using the trait version of State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-T; Spielberger et al., 1983) and the Zung Self-Rating Depression Scale (ZSDS; Zung, 1965). Both inventories consist of 20 items scored on a 4-point Likert scale (STAI-T: 1 = almost never to 4 = almost always; ZSDS: 1 = a little of the time to 4 = most of the time) and were completed prior to the MRI scan. STAI-T and ZSDS total scores demonstrated excellent internal consistency in the present study (Cronbach's alpha = .91 and .83, respectively).
2.3. Experimental task
An implicit facial expression recognition task was used to measure emotional processing (Szabó et al., 2017). In this task, participants categorized the sex of grey-scale photographs of happy, fearful, sad, and neutral faces. Stimuli consisted of six adult faces (three males and three females) taken from a standard set of pictures of facial affect (Ekman and Friesen, 1976). All faces were centred on a black background and were cropped to remove any non-facial features (e.g., hair, ears).
Faces were presented in blocks separated by three rest blocks where a white fixation cross appeared at the centre of the visual display for 20 s. Happy, fear and sad blocks (three blocks of each emotion) were presented in a pseudo-random order and interspersed with twelve neutral blocks (see Fig. 1). Each emotional block was 20 s long and comprised six faces. Faces were presented for 3000 ms (also in a pseudo-random order), followed by an interstimulus interval of 333 ms and 334 ms. The total duration of the task was 8 min.
Fig. 1.
Experimental paradigm (block design).
N, Neutral blocks; H, Happy blocks; S, Sad blocks; F, Fear blocks; R, Rest blocks.
E-Prime 2.0 software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Pittsburgh, USA) was used for stimuli presentation and data collection. Participants lay in the MRI scanner and viewed the visual stimuli on a screen via a mirror which was fixed to the head coil. To ensure that participants were attending to the task, they were asked to indicate the sex of the faces using a two-button response device. Accuracy and reaction times were monitored and recorded. Subjects were familiarized with the task before scanning on a laptop computer. The practice stimuli consisted of four neutral faces.
2.4. Data acquisition
Functional MRI data were acquired with a 3 T MRI scanner (Achieva 3 T, Philips Medical System) using a BOLD-sensitive T2*-weighted echo-planar imaging sequence (repetition time [TR] = 2500 ms, echo time [TE] = 30 ms, field of view [FOV] = 240 × 240 mm) with 3 mm × 3 mm in-plane resolution and contiguous 3-mm slices providing whole-brain coverage. A series of high-resolution anatomical images were also acquired during the first functional imaging session using a T1-weighted 3D TFE sequence with 1 × 1 × 1 mm resolution.
2.5. Data analysis
Demographic, clinical and behavioural data were analysed with SPSS version 23.0 software (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Independent t-tests were conducted to determine whether migraine patients and normal controls differed in age, anxiety and depressive symptoms, and accuracy rate and reaction times for each emotion. In addition, chi-square (χ2) and Fisher's exact tests were performed on gender and education level. The significance threshold was set at p < .05. Cronbach's alpha coefficients were calculated to assess the internal consistency of all self-report measures.
2.6. fMRI analysis
Imaging data processing and analysis were conducted using the Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM12) software package (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, Institute of Neurology, London, UK; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm12/) implemented in Matlab 2015b (Math Works, Natick, MA). Preprocessing steps included realignment, coregistration of the anatomical image to the mean functional volume, segmentation, normalization to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template, and spatial smoothing using an 8 mm full-width-at-half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel.
Data were monitored for motion outliers using the artifact detection toolbox (ART; http://www.nitrc.org/projects/artifact_detect/). Time points (volumes) in individuals' scans were identified as outliers if the global signal deviated more than three standard deviations from the mean or if scan-to-scan motion exceeded 1 mm deviation. Motion parameters and outliers were used as regressors of no interest in the individual-level analysis. Additionally, participants were excluded if >15% of volumes were marked as outliers. As noted above, two participants were excluded due to motion artifacts.
First-level analyses were computed for three contrasts using the general linear model (GLM) in SPM12: fear-neutral, happy-neutral, sad-neutral. The resulting contrast maps were then entered into the second-level analyses. Initially, one sample t-tests were performed to confirm the task-related activations in the whole brain (these results are reported in the Supplementary Material). After that two-sample t-tests were used to explore whether neural activation differed between migraine patients and healthy controls in response to each emotion. In addition, in the patient group, separate multiple regression analyses were conducted to examine the associations between BOLD responses to emotional faces and indices of migraine severity (i.e., migraine duration, migraine frequency, estimated lifetime number of migraine attacks, pain intensity of migraine attacks, impact of migraine), with migraine indices serving as covariates. Mean beta values were extracted from each significant cluster using MarsBar (Brett et al., 2002), and correlated with severity migraine indices using SPSS (these are reported in the Supplementary Material).
Age and sex were controlled for in each of the models (except in the case of task-related activations in all participants), because previous studies demonstrated sex and age effects on migraine indices and on structural and functional brain changes in migraine patients. More specifically, the prevalence of migraine increases with age, and it peaks at the age of 30 to 39 years (Vetvik and MacGregor, 2017). The clinical characteristics of migraine also change with age, especially in females (Bolay et al., 2015). In female patients longer duration and greater intensity of migraine attacks (Bolay et al., 2015; Vetvik and MacGregor, 2017), more dysfunctional connections (Liu et al., 2011), more pronounced structural changes and increased neural responses to noxious stimuli (Maleki et al., 2012b) have been found compared to male patients. Second-level analyses were repeated to investigate whether results remain the same after including anxiety and depressive symptoms as covariates of no interest (these results are reported in the Supplementary Material).
For all fMRI analyses, an initial threshold of p < .001 uncorrected for multiple comparison with a cluster size of ten voxels (k ≥ 10) was applied and results survived family-wise error correction at a cluster-level threshold of pFWE < .05 were reported. Peak activations of the significant clusters were identified anatomically using the Automated Anatomical Labelling atlas (aal; Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002). All statistical maps were visualized on the MNI 152 template brain provided in MRIcroGL (http://www.mccauslandcenter.sc.edu/mricrogl/).
3. Results
3.1. Participants' characteristics and behavioural results
The demographic and clinical characteristics of participants are summarized in Table 1. Age and education level were not significantly different between migraine patients and healthy controls, but there was a significant difference in sex ratio. Females predominated in the patient group (81%), which is in line with studies reporting higher prevalence of migraine in females compared to males (Buse et al., 2013; Lipton et al., 2007). In addition, there were no significant differences between groups in anxiety and depressive symptoms. Further details concerning the migraine patients are given in Table 1.
Table 1.
Demographic and clinical characteristics of migraine patients and healthy controls.
Patients (n = 41) |
Controls (n = 49) |
Test statistic |
p value |
Effect size |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean (SD) | Range | Mean (SD) | Range | ||||
Women | 33 (81%) | 30 (61%) | 3.994 | 0.047⁎ | 0.209 | ||
Age | 27.00 (4.92) | 20–37 | 25.69 (4.13) | 21–37 | 1.369 | 0.175 | 0.288 |
Highest education | 1.458 | 0.497 | 0.125 | ||||
High school | 15 (37%) | 24 (49%) | |||||
Graduate degree | 22 (54%) | 21 (43%) | |||||
Professional qualification | 4 (9%) | 4 (8%) | |||||
STAI-T | 32.76 (6.33) | 24–56 | 33.53 (9.45) | 22–72 | 0.878 | 0.382 | 0.096 |
ZSDS | 33.17 (5.19) | 23–43 | 32.69 (6.15) | 24–54 | 0.393 | 0.695 | 0.084 |
Migraine laterality | |||||||
Side-locked unilateral | |||||||
Right-sided | 5 (12%) | ||||||
Left-sided | 8 (20%) | ||||||
Side-shifting unilateral | 9 (22%) | ||||||
Bilateral | 19 (46%) | ||||||
Age at migraine onset | 15.15 (7.01) | 3–30 | |||||
Number of years with migraine | 11.76 (7.56) | 1–29 | |||||
Migraine frequency per month | 3.09 (2.99) | 1–12 | |||||
Estimated lifetime number of migraine attacks | 400.68 (517.99) | 13–2016 | |||||
Pain severity of migraine attacks (last three month, 0 to 10) |
5.72 (1.85) | 2–9 | |||||
MIDAS score (Total) | 10.81(10.84) | 0–45 |
Note. Data are expressed as mean ± standard deviation (SD) or as percentage (%). The p values are based on chi-square (χ2) or Fisher's exact tests for categorical data and independent-sample t-test for continuous data. Cramer's V and Cohen's d coefficients were used to measure effect sizes.
STAI-T, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory–Trait scale; ZSDS, Zung Self-Rating Depression Scale; MIDAS, Migraine Disability Assessment Scale. MIDAS scores were available for 37 patients.
p < 0.05.
The accuracy rate was high in the sex identification task. The mean accuracy level across all conditions was 99.19% (SD = 1.69). There were no significant differences between migraine patients and healthy subjects in accuracy (t(88) = 1.352, p = .180) and reaction times (neutral: t(88) = 1.545, p = .126; fear: t(88) = 1.476, p = .143; happy: t(88) = 0.992, p = .324; sad: t(88) = 0.723, p = .472).
3.2. fMRI results
3.2.1. Task-related activations
Task-related activations in all participants are reported in the Supplementary Material (see Table S1, Fig. S1).
3.2.2. Group differences in neural response to emotional faces
After controlling for sex and age, whole-brain analyses revealed that, compared to controls, migraine patients displayed significantly increased neural activation to fearful faces in one cluster. This included regions of the right middle frontal gyrus, right superior frontal gyrus, and right inferior frontal gyrus (see Table 2, Fig. 2). Importantly, when anxiety and depressive symptoms were controlled for, the cluster remained significant with activations located in the right middle frontal gyrus (see Supplementary Table S2). Brain activation during processing sad and happy facial expressions did not differ significantly between patients and controls.
Table 2.
Brain regions showing increased activation in migraine patients, compared to healthy controls, in response to fearful faces.
Cluster Size |
Cluster p (FWE) |
Region |
Peak Coordinates |
Peak T-value |
||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | ||||
145 | 0.014 | R Middle frontal gyrus | 39 | 32 | 32 | 4.56 |
R Superior frontal gyrus | 27 | 59 | 2 | 4.39 | ||
R Inferior frontal gyrus, pars triangularis | 48 | 35 | 26 | 4.38 | ||
R Middle frontal gyrus | 45 | 47 | 11 | 4.34 | ||
R Middle frontal gyrus, orbital part | 39 | 50 | -4 | 3.51 |
Note. The cluster is significant at pFWE = 0.05, corrected for multiple comparison. Coordinates are in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space.
R, right hemisphere.
Fig. 2.
Migraine patients displayed increased activation in response to fearful faces compared to healthy controls. The significant cluster is shown at pFWE = 0.05, corrected for multiple comparison. The key areas are primarily located in the right middle frontal gyrus. Coordinates are in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space.
3.2.3. Migraine severity and neural response to emotional faces
Associations between neural activation to emotional faces and indices of migraine severity (after controlling for sex and age) can be seen in Table 3. In the migraine patient group, migraine frequency and estimated lifetime number of migraine attacks were positively associated with two clusters of activation during processing fearful faces. There were increased activations in the right postcentral gyrus, right precentral gyrus and right inferior parietal lobule with increasing migraine frequency. Estimated lifetime number of migraine attacks were related to increased right angular gyrus and right postcentral gyrus activation. Furthermore, neural response to happy facial expressions showed significantly positive association with migraine frequency in one cluster covering areas of the right caudate nucleus and right putamen. Fig. 3 shows the significantly activated clusters. When Bonferroni correction was applied for the number of tests conducted (pFWE < .003), the association between migraine frequency and activation to fearful faces in the right postcentral gyrus cluster remained significant. Results of the correlations between migraine severity indices and cluster beta values are reported in Supplementary Fig. S2. After controlling for anxiety and depressive symptoms, activations remained significant in the right postcentral gyrus, precentral gyrus, angular gyrus, and the right caudate and putamen (see Supplementary Table S3).
Table 3.
Associations between migraine frequency, estimated lifetime number of migraine attacks and neural response to emotional faces in migraine patients.
Cluster Size |
Cluster p (FWE) |
Region |
Peak Coordinates |
Peak T-value |
||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | ||||
Migraine frequency per month | ||||||
Fear | ||||||
276 | 0.000 | R Postcentral gyrus | 51 | −16 | 32 | 5.66 |
R Postcentral gyrus | 57 | −16 | 41 | 5.32 | ||
R Postcentral gyrus | 57 | −13 | 29 | 5.24 | ||
R Precentral gyrus | 51 | −1 | 23 | 4.36 | ||
R Precentral gyrus | 48 | −4 | 29 | 4.35 | ||
R Postcentral gyrus | 57 | −10 | 21 | 4.19 | ||
R Postcentral gyrus | 60 | −13 | 17 | 3.92 | ||
59 | 0.043 | R Postcentral gyrus | 45 | −25 | 53 | 4.61 |
R Inferior parietal lobule | 42 | −37 | 50 | 4.01 | ||
Happy | ||||||
72 | 0.021 | R Caudate nucleus | 12 | 11 | −1 | 4.65 |
R Caudate nucleus | 12 | 20 | −4 | 4.32 | ||
R Caudate nucleus | 9 | 14 | 8 | 4.08 | ||
R Putamen | 24 | 5 | 5 | 3.79 | ||
R Putamen | 30 | 11 | −1 | 3.48 | ||
Estimated lifetime number of migraine attacks | ||||||
Fear | ||||||
94 | 0.006 | R Angular gyrus | 42 | −58 | 32 | 5.46 |
77 | 0.016 | R Postcentral gyrus | 45 | −28 | 50 | 4.39 |
R Postcentral gyrus | 54 | −16 | 44 | 4.21 |
Note. All clusters are significant at pFWE = 0.05, corrected for multiple comparison. Coordinates are in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space.
R, right hemisphere.
Fig. 3.
During exposure to fearful faces migraine frequency was associated with increased activation in the right postcentral gyrus and right inferior parietal lobule, and estimated lifetime number of migraine attacks were related to increased right postcentral gyrus and right angular gyrus activation. Increased neural response related to happy faces showed significant association with migraine frequency in the right caudate nucleus and right putamen. Significantly activated clusters are shown at pFWE = 0.05, corrected for multiple comparison. Coordinates are in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space.
Other indices of migraine severity (i.e., duration, pain intensity and impact of migraine headaches) were not associated with significant brain activations (neither positively, nor negatively) in response to emotional faces.
4. Discussion
In this study, we explored the neural processing of emotional faces in migraine without aura patients (during the interictal period) using functional MRI. Our results indicated that patients with episodic migraine exhibited increased neural activation to fearful faces in the right middle frontal gyrus and frontal pole compared to healthy controls. We further observed that depending on attack frequency, both fearful and happy facial expressions were related to enhanced brain activation in migraine patients.
The main finding in the present study was the significant neural activation in the right middle frontal gyrus in migraine patients relative to controls (which remained significant even after anxiety and depressive symptoms were controlled for). This brain region has been recognized as involved in the attentional network. More specifically, the right middle fontal gyrus has been proposed to contribute to both dorsal (top-down) and ventral (bottom-up) attention networks (Corbetta et al., 2008; Fox et al., 2006; He et al., 2007; Vossel et al., 2014). Considering the high priority of fearful expressions in capturing attentional resources (e.g., Ikeda et al., 2013; Phelps et al., 2006), it can be assumed that the activation of this region might be associated with the attention being paid to fearful faces in migraine patients. Several studies have demonstrated that fearful expressions, even though their emotional content is task-irrelevant, capture attention and interfere with the relevant task (Williams, 2006). In support of this view, previous studies have found that the right middle frontal gyrus and the right inferior frontal gyrus are involved in the detection of salient, behaviourally relevant but task-irrelevant stimuli (Doricchi et al., 2010; Shulman et al., 2009; Vossel et al., 2014).
Notably, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) spans over the middle and the superior frontal gyrus, and has been found to be involved in attentional processing of emotional stimuli, and appears to have a regulatory role in emotional responses (Jacob et al., 2014; Lindquist et al., 2012; Mondino et al., 2015; Phillips et al., 2003). In our study, increased activation was found in the posterior-dorsal subregion of the dlPFC which has been implicated in cognitive control related to stimulus processing and the selection of behaviourally relevant information (Cieslik et al., 2013). It is worth noting that the dlPFC has also been found to have increased pain-related activation in patients with migraine or chronic knee pain (Hiramatsu et al., 2014; Schwedt et al., 2014). The dlPFC is implicated in pain modulation, possibly due to its involvement in cognitive and attentional processes (Peyron et al., 2000; Seminowicz and Moayedi, 2017).
Of interest, our results revealed increased activation in migraine patients for the facial expression of fear. This sensitivity might be related to the perception of potential danger (threat detection) which is prioritized automatically (Turano et al., 2017; Williams, 2006; Williams et al., 2006). However, reaction times to fearful stimuli in migraine patients were comparable to those showed by healthy controls. This suggests that, although behavioural reaction to emotional stimuli can be similar in participants with and without migraine, neurological measures implicate different processing of emotional information.
In addition, we found significant association between neural response to fearful and happy faces and migraine severity indices (which remained significant even when the potential effect of anxiety and depressive symptoms was controlled for). Migraine attack frequency and estimated lifetime number of migraine attacks were related to increased activations mainly in the right postcentral gyrus to fearful faces. This region corresponds to the primary somatosensory (S1) cortex, and it is worth mentioning that activations were found in the face area of the S1 cortex (Kuehn et al., 2017; Moulton et al., 2009). In previous migraine studies, S1 has been associated with hyperresponsivity in the interictal state of migraine (Lang et al., 2004; Maleki et al., 2012a; Schwedt et al., 2014). Similar to our results, these studies also found that increased activation in the S1 cortex was linked to the frequency of migraine attacks. This is in line with the notion that the S1 is part of the ‘pain matrix’ regions and the functional/structural plasticity of the S1 cortex is closely related to pain chronification (Kim et al., 2017). Along this line, several studies have reported correlations between the extent of functional brain changes and indices of migraine severity (headache frequency or disease duration) (for a review, see Maniyar and Goadsby, 2013; Schwedt et al., 2015). This raises the possibility that migraine has a cumulative effect on brain function or the extent of underlying dysfunctions is connected to the risk of more debilitating migraine.
Finally, we provided evidence that higher attack frequency in migraine patients was associated with increased activation in dorsal striatal regions to happy faces. The processing of positive facial expressions evoked greater activation in two striatal subnuclei with increasing migraine frequency: the right putamen and the right caudate (predominantly the caudate head). The involvement of striatum in positive affect has been consistently reported in the literature. The best explored area in this field is probably the association of caudate nucleus and putamen with reward-related processing (Balleine et al., 2007; Delgado, 2007; Gerdes et al., 2010; Knutson and Cooper, 2005; Robinson et al., 2012). That is, increases in the activation of these nuclei, especially the head of the caudate nucleus, have been observed when participants are presented with rewarding and pleasant stimuli, such as social reward or monetary gain. Interestingly, these regions are frequently activated during studies of pain as well (Borsook et al., 2010). It seems that the putamen shows somatotopic activation to pain and contributes to the sensory aspects of pain-related processes (Bingel et al., 2004; Starr et al., 2011), while the caudate is part of the pain modulatory system (Freund et al., 2009, Freund et al.,2007). Notably, studies of migraine patients revealed abnormal caudate and putamen activation to painful stimuli, and altered resting state functional connectivity of the caudate with increasing migraine frequency (Maleki et al., 2011; Yuan et al., 2013).
To summarize our results, patients with migraine showed enhanced processing of fearful facial stimuli compared to controls. Wang et al. (2017) and Wilcox et al. (2016) also reported increased activation to negative pictures in migraine patients in regions involved in visual, emotional and pain processing, however, emotional processing was measured by IAPS pictures (Lang et al., 2008), and these studied did not investigate the effects of migraine severity. Our findings extend previous fMRI research by demonstrating that migraine headache frequency is associated with enhanced processing of aversive and positive emotional stimuli (fearful and happy facial expressions), with increased activation within the primary somatosensory and striatal regions. Importantly, activations occurred in the right hemisphere in migraine patients which is generally thought to be the dominant hemisphere for processing emotions (Borod et al., 1998; Killgore and Yurgelun-Todd, 2007), and based on the patients' characteristics (they had unilateral or bilateral headache episodes) this cannot be explained by the laterality of headache pain. Finally, these neural responses to the facial stimuli were not accounted for by depression and anxiety symptoms.
5. Limitations
There are certain limitations in the present study that should be acknowledged. First, our cross-sectional design precludes the examination of changes in migraine frequency over time which can have a potential sensitisation effect. Second, although groups were matched on age and education, there were more female participants in the patient than in the control group. However, given previous findings of sex-related structural and functional differences in migraine, sex was controlled for as a covariate of no interest across the analyses. It should be also emphasized that the indices of migraine severity were based on retrospective patient reports. While prior research suggests that patient estimations of headache frequency and duration are reasonably accurate, headache intensity appears more difficult to report, possibly due to the multidimensional nature of pain (Niere and Jerak, 2004).
Regarding our emotional faces task, comparing emotional stimuli to neutral ones is a widely used method to remove simple effect of visual perception (Sabatinelli et al., 2011), and the main effects of the task were in line with those reported in former studies (Fusar-Poli et al., 2009; Lindquist et al., 2012; Phan et al., 2004). However, it should be noted that because of the format of our facial expression recognition task, the rest condition could not be used as a baseline condition. Furthermore, this experimental paradigm was not able to differentiate between specific attentional processes (i.e., attentional engagement with and disengagement from emotional stimuli), and repeated stimuli presentation might have led to attenuation in neural responses. The possible differences in levels of arousal between fearful and sad facial stimuli should be also noted. It seems that fearful and sad faces are the same in terms of valence, but fearful faces are more arousing than sad faces, which can influence attention or perception (Johnsen et al., 1995; Mather and Sutherland, 2011). Thus, it is possible that fearful faces evoked higher levels of arousal in migraine patients which resulted in the differences in neural activation. Although previous studies measuring arousal (and valence) ratings of emotional pictures did not find differences between migraine patients and healthy controls (Andreatta et al., 2012; Buodo et al., 2011; Steppacher et al., 2016; Wilcox et al., 2016), obtaining subjective ratings of face stimuli during and after the task would have provided further insight into whether migraine patients experienced increased emotions. This could be tested by using angry facial expressions as well, which are also high arousing, and represent unpleasant and threatening social stimuli (Andreatta et al., 2012). Enhanced neural activations to happy faces were also observed in relation to migraine frequency. It seems that happy faces are processed more rapidly and accurately due to their unique facial features and positive valence (Calvo and Beltrán, 2013; Calvo and Lundqvist, 2008), and they might be more salient or arousing with increasing attack frequency.
Although hypersensitivity to sensory stimuli during and between headache attacks is specific to migraine, future imaging studies focusing on tension-type headache might be useful. There are some indications that patients with tension-type headache are more sensitive to negative affective states (anger, anxiety) while patients with migraine report both positive and negative emotional states as precipitants of their migraine attacks (Donias et al., 1991), which is in line with our results. Prospective studies are also needed to examine the possible link between changes in migraine frequency and corresponding changes in neural response patterns to emotional stimuli. Given that our study focused on episodic migraine, it would be also relevant to investigate patients with chronic migraine.
6. Conclusion
Based on the present and previous findings, it seems that migraine patients show enhanced response to emotional stimuli. Emotional cues encountered in everyday life can be considered as stressors, and it is well-known that stressors are potential triggers for migraine attacks. Our results suggest that the emotionally arousing stimuli, rather than unpleasantness only, might represent a possible trigger or precipitant of migraine headaches. These findings support the idea that with increasing frequency of attacks, migraine might become a more severe disease with greater central sensitivity.
Funding
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: the study was supported by the Hungarian Brain Research Program (Grant No. 2017–1.2.1-NKP-2017-00002); the Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungarian National Development Agency, Semmelweis University and the Hungarian Brain Research Program (Grant No. KTIA_NAP_13–2- 2015-0001) (MTA-SE-NAP B Genetic Brain Imaging Migraine Research Group); and the Hungarian Academy of Sciences (MTA-SE Neuropsychopharmacology and Neurochemistry Research Group).
Gyöngyi Kökönyei acknowledges the support of the Hungarian National Research, Development and Innovation Office (Grant No. FK128614). Lajos R. Kozák was supported by the Bolyai Research Fellowship Program of the Hungarian Academy of Sciences. Edina Szabó was supported by the ÚNKP-17-3-III-ELTE-346, Natália Kocsel was supported by the ÚNKP-18-3-III-ELTE-495, and Andrea Edit Édes was supported by the ÚNKP-17-3-IV-SE-3 New National Excellence Program of The Ministry of Human Capacities.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgement
The fMRI study was conducted as a groupwork at MR Research Center, Semmelweis University. The authors thank Ádám György Szabó, Krisztina Koósné Oláh, István Kóbor and Márk Folyovich for their contribution.
Author note
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Footnotes
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101790.
Contributor Information
Edina Szabó, Email: szabo.edina@ppk.elte.hu.
Attila Galambos, Email: galambos.attila@ppk.elte.hu.
Natália Kocsel, Email: kocsel.natalia@ppk.elte.hu.
Andrea Edit Édes, Email: edes.andrea@pharma.semmelweis-univ.hu.
Lajos Rudolf Kozák, Email: lkozak@mrkk.sote.hu.
György Bagdy, Email: bag13638@iif.hu.
Gyöngyi Kökönyei, Email: kokonyei.gyongyi@ppk.elte.hu, kokonyei.gyongyi@pharma.semmelweis-univ.hu.
Gabriella Juhász, Email: juhasz.gabriella@pharma.semmelweis-univ.hu.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
Supplementary material
References
- Andreatta M., Puschmann A.K., Sommer C., Weyers P., Pauli P., Mühlberger A. Altered processing of emotional stimuli in migraine: an event-related potential study. Cephalalgia. 2012;32:1101–1108. doi: 10.1177/0333102412459573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andress-Rothrock D., King W., Rothrock J. An analysis of migraine triggers in a clinic-based population. Headache. 2010;50:1366–1370. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2010.01753.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aurora S.K., Wilkinson F. The brain is hyperexcitable in migraine. Cephalalgia. 2007;27:1442–1453. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2982.2007.01502.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balleine B.W., Delgado M.R., Hikosaka O. The role of the dorsal striatum in reward and decision-making. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:8161–8165. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1554-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bingel U., Lorenz J., Glauche V., Knab R., Gläscher J., Weiller C., Büchel C. Somatotopic organization of human somatosensory cortices for pain: a single trial fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2004;23:224–232. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.05.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolay H., Ozge A., Saginc P., Orekici G., Uludüz D., Yalın O., Siva A., Bıçakçi Ş., Karakurum B., Öztürk M. Gender influences headache characteristics with increasing age in migraine patients. Cephalalgia. 2015;35:792–800. doi: 10.1177/0333102414559735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borod J.C., Cicero B.A., Obler L.K., Welkowitz J., Erhan H.M., Santschi C.…Whalen J.R. Right hemisphere emotional perception: evidence across multiple channels. Neuropsychology. 1998;12(3):446–458. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.12.3.446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borsook D., Upadhyay J., Chudler E.H., Becerra L. A key role of the basal ganglia in pain and analgesia - insights gained through human functional imaging. Mol. Pain. 2010;6 doi: 10.1186/1744-8069-6-27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bourke C., Douglas K., Porter R. Processing of facial emotion expression in major depression: a review. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry. 2010;44:681–696. doi: 10.3109/00048674.2010.496359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brett M., Anton J.-L., Valabregue R., Poline J.-P. Region of interest analysis using the MarsBar toolbox for SPM 99. NeuroImage. 2002;16:497. [Google Scholar]
- Buodo G., Sarlo M., Battistella P.A., Naccarella C., Palomba D. Event-related potentials to emotional stimuli in migrainous children. J. Child Neurol. 2011;26:1508–1515. doi: 10.1177/0883073811408905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buse D.C., Loder E.W., Gorman J.A., Stewart W.F., Reed M.L., Fanning K.M., Serrano D., Lipton R.B. Sex differences in the prevalence, symptoms, and associated features of migraine, probable migraine and other severe headache: results of the American migraine prevalence and prevention (AMPP) study. Headache. 2013;53:1278–1299. doi: 10.1111/head.12150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calvo M.G., Beltrán D. Recognition advantage of happy faces: tracing the neurocognitive processes. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51:2051–2061. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calvo M.G., Lundqvist D. Facial expressions of emotion (KDEF): identification under different display-duration conditions. Behav. Res. Methods. 2008;40:109–115. doi: 10.3758/brm.40.1.109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cauda F., Torta D.M.-E., Sacco K., Geda E., D'Agata F., Costa T., Duca S., Geminiani G., Amanzio M. Shared “core” areas between the pain and other task-related networks. PLoS One. 2012;7 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0041929. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cieslik E.C., Zilles K., Caspers S., Roski C., Kellermann T.S., Jakobs O., Langner R., Laird A.R., Fox P.T., Eickhoff S.B. Is there “one” DLPFC in cognitive action control? Evidence for heterogeneity from co-activation-based parcellation. Cereb. Cortex. 2013;23:2677–2689. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbetta M., Patel G., Shulman G.L. The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron. 2008;58:306–324. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Tommaso M., Ambrosini A., Brighina F., Coppola G., Perrotta A., Pierelli F., Sandrini G., Valeriani M., Marinazzo D., Stramaglia S., Schoenen J. Altered processing of sensory stimuli in patients with migraine. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 2014;10:144–155. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2014.14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delgado M.R. Reward-related responses in the human striatum. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2007;1104:70–88. doi: 10.1196/annals.1390.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demenescu L.R., Kortekaas R., den Boer J.A., Aleman A. Impaired attribution of emotion to facial expressions in anxiety and major depression. PLoS One. 2010;5 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donias S.H., Peioglou-Harmoussi S., Georgiadis G., Manos N. Differential emotional precipitation of migraine and tension-type headache attacks. Cephalalgia. 1991;11:47–52. doi: 10.1046/j.1468-2982.1991.1101047.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doricchi F., Macci E., Silvetti M., Macaluso E. Neural correlates of the spatial and expectancy components of endogenous and stimulus-driven orienting of attention in the posner task. Cereb. Cortex. 2010;20:1574–1585. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ekman P., Friesen W.V. Measuring facial movement. Environ. Psychol. Nonverbal Behav. 1976;1:56–75. [Google Scholar]
- Fox M.D., Corbetta M., Snyder A.Z., Vincent J.L., Raichle M.E. Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2006;103:10046–10051. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0604187103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freund W., Stuber G., Wunderlich A.P., Schmitz B. Cortical correlates of perception and suppression of electrically induced pain. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 2007;24:203–212. doi: 10.1080/08990220701723636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freund W., Klug R., Weber F., Stuber G., Schmitz B., Wunderlich A.P. Perception and suppression of thermally induced pain: a fMRI study. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 2009;26:1–10. doi: 10.1080/08990220902738243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fusar-Poli P., Placentino A., Carletti F., Landi P., Allen P., Surguladze S., Benedetti F., Abbamonte M., Gasparotti R., Barale F., Perez J., McGuire P., Politi P. Functional atlas of emotional faces processing: a voxel-based meta-analysis of 105 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. JPN. 2009;34:418–432. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerdes A.B.M., Wieser M.J., Muehlberger A., Weyers P., Alpers G.W., Plichta M.M., Breuer F., Pauli P. Brain activations to emotional pictures are differentially associated with valence and arousal ratings. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2010;4 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harriott A.M., Schwedt T.J. Migraine is associated with altered processing of sensory stimuli. Curr. Pain Headache Rep. 2014;18:2–7. doi: 10.1007/s11916-014-0458-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- He B.J., Snyder A.Z., Vincent J.L., Epstein A., Shulman G.L., Corbetta M. Breakdown of functional connectivity in frontoparietal networks underlies behavioral deficits in spatial neglect. Neuron. 2007;53:905–918. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Headache Classification Committee of the International Headache Society (IHS) The international classification of headache disorders, 3rd edition (beta version) Cephalalgia. 2013;33:629–808. doi: 10.1177/0333102413485658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hiramatsu T., Nakanishi K., Yoshimura S., Yoshino A., Adachi N., Okamoto Y., Yamawaki S., Ochi M. The dorsolateral prefrontal network is involved in pain perception in knee osteoarthritis patients. Neurosci. Lett. 2014;581:109–114. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.08.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hubbard C.S., Khan S.A., Keaser M.L., Mathur V.A., Goyal M., Seminowicz D.A. Altered brain structure and function correlate with disease severity and pain catastrophizing in migraine patients. eNeuro. 2014;1 doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0006-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ikeda K., Sugiura A., Hasegawa T. Fearful faces grab attention in the absence of late affective cortical responses. Psychophysiology. 2013;50:60–69. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01478.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacob H., Brück C., Domin M., Lotze M., Wildgruber D. I can't keep your face and voice out of my head: neural correlates of an attentional bias toward nonverbal emotional cues. Cereb. Cortex. 2014;24:1460–1473. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnsen B.H., Thayer J.F., Hugdahl K. Affective judgment of the Ekman faces: a dimensional approach. J. Psychophysiol. 1995;9:193–202. [Google Scholar]
- Kelman L. The triggers or precipitants of the acute migraine attack. Cephalalgia. 2007;27:394–402. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2982.2007.01303.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Killgore W.D.S., Yurgelun-Todd D.A. The right-hemisphere and valence hypotheses: could they both be right (and sometimes left)? Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2007;2(3):240–250. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim W., Kim S.K., Nabekura J. Functional and structural plasticity in the primary somatosensory cortex associated with chronic pain. J. Neurochem. 2017;141:499–506. doi: 10.1111/jnc.14012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knutson B., Cooper J.C. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of reward prediction. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2005;18:411–417. doi: 10.1097/01.wco.0000173463.24758.f6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuehn E., Dinse J., Jakobsen E., Long X., Schäfer A., Bazin P.-L., Villringer A., Sereno M.I., Margulies D.S. Body topography parcellates human sensory and motor cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2017;27:3790–3805. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lang E., Kaltenhäuser M., Neundörfer B., Seidler S. Hyperexcitability of the primary somatosensory cortex in migraine—a magnetoencephalographic study. Brain. 2004;127:2459–2469. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lang P.J., Bradley M.M., Cuthbert B.N. University of Florida. FL; Gainesville: 2008. International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical report A-8. [Google Scholar]
- Lindquist K.A., Wager T.D., Kober H., Bliss-Moreau E., Barrett L.F. The brain basis of emotion: a meta-analytic review. Behav. Brain Sci. 2012;35:121–143. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X11000446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipton R.B., Bigal M.E., Diamond M., Freitag F., Reed M.L., Stewart W.F., AMPP Advisory Group Migraine prevalence, disease burden, and the need for preventive therapy. Neurology. 2007;68:343–349. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000252808.97649.21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu J., Qin W., Nan J., Li J., Yuan K., Zhao L., Zeng F., Sun J., Yu D., Dong M., Liu P., von Deneen K.M., Gong Q., Liang F., Tian J. Gender-related differences in the dysfunctional resting networks of migraine suffers. PLoS One. 2011;6 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Louter M.A., Pijpers J.A., Wardenaar K.J., van Zwet E.W., van Hemert A.M., Zitman F.G., Ferrari M.D., Penninx B.W., Terwindt G.M. Symptom dimensions of affective disorders in migraine patients. J. Psychosom. Res. 2015;79:458–463. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2015.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maleki N., Becerra L., Nutile L., Pendse G., Brawn J., Bigal M., Burstein R., Borsook D. Migraine attacks the basal ganglia. Mol. Pain. 2011;7 doi: 10.1186/1744-8069-7-71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maleki N., Becerra L., Brawn J., Bigal M., Burstein R., Borsook D. Concurrent functional and structural cortical alterations in migraine. Cephalalgia. 2012;32:607–620. doi: 10.1177/0333102412445622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maleki N., Linnman C., Brawn J., Burstein R., Becerra L., Borsook D. Her versus his migraine: multiple sex differences in brain function and structure. Brain. 2012;135:2546–2559. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maniyar F.H., Goadsby P.J. Functional imaging in chronic migraine. Curr. Pain Headache Rep. 2013;17 doi: 10.1007/s11916-013-0333-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mather M., Sutherland M.R. Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2011;6:114–133. doi: 10.1177/1745691611400234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mathur V.A., Moayedi M., Keaser M.L., Khan S.A., Hubbard C.S., Goyal M., Seminowicz D.A. High frequency migraine is associated with lower acute pain sensitivity and abnormal insula activity related to migraine pain intensity, attack frequency, and pain catastrophizing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016;10 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mondino M., Thiffault F., Fecteau S. Does non-invasive brain stimulation applied over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex non-specifically influence mood and emotional processing in healthy individuals? Front. Cell. Neurosci. 2015;9 doi: 10.3389/fncel.2015.00399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moulton E.A., Pendse G., Morris S., Aiello-Lammens M., Becerra L., Borsook D. Segmentally arranged somatotopy within the face representation of human primary somatosensory cortex. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009;30:757–765. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20541. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niere K., Jerak A. Measurement of headache frequency, intensity and duration: comparison of patient report by questionnaire and headache diary. Physiother. Res. Int. 2004;9:149–156. doi: 10.1002/pri.318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oldfield R.C. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peres M.F.P., Mercante J.P.P., Tobo P.R., Kamei H., Bigal M.E. Anxiety and depression symptoms and migraine: a symptom-based approach research. J. Headache Pain. 2017;18 doi: 10.1186/s10194-017-0742-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peyron R., Laurent B., García-Larrea L. Functional imaging of brain responses to pain. A review and meta-analysis (2000) Neurophysiol. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2000;30:263–288. doi: 10.1016/s0987-7053(00)00227-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phan K.L., Wager T.D., Taylor S.F., Liberzon I. Functional neuroimaging studies of human emotions. CNS Spectr. 2004;9:258–266. doi: 10.1017/s1092852900009196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phelps E.A., Ling S., Carrasco M. Emotion facilitates perception and potentiates the perceptual benefits of attention. Psychol. Sci. 2006;17:292–299. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01701.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips M.L., Drevets W.C., Rauch S.L., Lane R. Neurobiology of emotion perception I: the neural basis of normal emotion perception. Biol. Psychiatry. 2003;54:504–514. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00168-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson J.L., Laird A.R., Glahn D.C., Blangero J., Sanghera M.K., Pessoa L., Fox P.M., Uecker A., Friehs G., Young K.A., Griffin J.L., Lovallo W.R., Fox P.T. The functional connectivity of the human caudate: an application of meta-analytic connectivity modeling with behavioral filtering. Neuroimage. 2012;60:117–129. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sabatinelli D., Fortune E.E., Li Q., Siddiqui A., Krafft C., Oliver W.T., Beck S., Jeffries J. Emotional perception: meta-analyses of face and natural scene processing. NeuroImage. 2011;54:2524–2533. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwedt T.J., Dodick D.W. Advanced neuroimaging of migraine. Lancet Neurol. 2009;8:560–568. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70107-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwedt T.J., Chong C.D., Chiang C.-C., Baxter L., Schlaggar B.L., Dodick D.W. Enhanced pain-induced activity of pain processing regions in a case-control study of episodic migraine. Cephalalgia. 2014;34:947–958. doi: 10.1177/0333102414526069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwedt T.J., Chiang C.-C., Chong C.D., Dodick D.W. Functional MRI of migraine. Lancet Neurol. 2015;14:81–91. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70193-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seminowicz D.A., Moayedi M. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in acute and chronic pain. J. Pain. 2017;18:1027–1035. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2017.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheehan D.V., Lecrubier Y., Sheehan K.H., Amorim P., Janavs J., Weiller E., Hergueta T., Baker R., Dunbar G.C. The mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 1998;59(Suppl. 20):22–33. (quiz 34-57) [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shulman G.L., Astafiev S.V., Franke D., Pope D.L.W., Snyder A.Z., McAvoy M.P., Corbetta M. Interaction of stimulus-driven reorienting and expectation in ventral and dorsal fronto-parietal and basal ganglia-cortical networks. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:4392–4407. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5609-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger, C.D., Gorsuch, R.L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P., Jacobs, G.A., 1983. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press., Palo Alto, CA.
- Starr C.J., Sawaki L., Wittenberg G.F., Burdette J.H., Oshiro Y., Quevedo A.S., McHaffie J.G., Coghill R.C. The contribution of the putamen to sensory aspects of pain: insights from structural connectivity and brain lesions. Brain. 2011;134:1987–2004. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steppacher I., Schindler S., Kissler J. Higher, faster, worse? An event-related potentials study of affective picture processing in migraine. Cephalalgia Int. J. Headache. 2016;36:249–257. doi: 10.1177/0333102415587705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stewart W.F., Lipton R.B., Kolodner K.B., Sawyer J., Lee C., Liberman J.N. Validity of the migraine disability assessment (MIDAS) score in comparison to a diary-based measure in a population sample of migraine sufferers. Pain. 2000;88:41–52. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00305-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stewart W.F., Wood C., Reed M.L., Roy J., Lipton R.B., AMPP Advisory Group Cumulative lifetime migraine incidence in women and men. Cephalalgia. 2008;28:1170–1178. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2982.2008.01666.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stuhrmann A., Suslow T., Dannlowski U. Facial emotion processing in major depression: a systematic review of neuroimaging findings. Biol. Mood Anxiety Disord. 2011;1:10. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-1-10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Surcinelli P., Codispoti M., Montebarocci O., Rossi N., Baldaro B. Facial emotion recognition in trait anxiety. J. Anxiety Disord. 2006;20:110–117. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szabó E., Kocsel N., Édes A., Pap D., Galambos A., Zsombók T., Szabó Á., Kozák L.R., Bagdy G., Juhász G., Kökönyei G. Callous-unemotional traits and neural responses to emotional faces in a community sample of young adults. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2017;111:312–317. [Google Scholar]
- Turano M.T., Lao J., Richoz A.-R., de Lissa P., Degosciu S.B.A., Viggiano M.P., Caldara R. Fear boosts the early neural coding of faces. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2017;12:1959–1971. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tzourio-Mazoyer N., Landeau B., Papathanassiou D., Crivello F., Etard O., Delcroix N., Mazoyer B., Joliot M. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage. 2002;15:273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vetvik K.G., MacGregor E.A. Sex differences in the epidemiology, clinical features, and pathophysiology of migraine. Lancet Neurol. 2017;16:76–87. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(16)30293-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vossel S., Geng J.J., Fink G.R. Dorsal and ventral attention systems: distinct neural circuits but collaborative roles. Neuroscientist. 2014;20:150–159. doi: 10.1177/1073858413494269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang M., Su J., Zhang J., Zhao Y., Yao Q., Zhang Q., Zhang H., Wang S., Li G.-F., Liu J.-R., Du X. Visual cortex and cerebellum hyperactivation during negative emotion picture stimuli in migraine patients. Sci. Rep. 2017;7 doi: 10.1038/srep41919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilcox S.L., Veggeberg R., Lemme J., Hodkinson D.J., Scrivani S., Burstein R., Becerra L., Borsook D. Increased functional activation of limbic brain regions during negative emotional processing in migraine. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016;10 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams L.M. An integrative neuroscience model of “significance” processing. J. Integr. Neurosci. 2006;5:1–47. doi: 10.1142/s0219635206001082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams L.M., Palmer D., Liddell B.J., Song L., Gordon E. The “when” and “where” of perceiving signals of threat versus non-threat. NeuroImage. 2006;31:458–467. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yuan K., Zhao Ling, Cheng P., Yu D., Zhao Limei, Dong T., Xing L., Bi Y., Yang X., von Deneen K.M., Liang F., Gong Q., Qin W., Tian J. Altered structure and resting-state functional connectivity of the basal ganglia in migraine patients without aura. J. Pain. 2013;14:836–844. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2013.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zung W.W. A self-rating depression scale. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1965;12:63–70. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1965.01720310065008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Supplementary material