Abstract
Background
Mirror therapy is used to improve motor function after stroke. During mirror therapy, a mirror is placed in the patient's midsagittal plane, thus reflecting movements of the non‐paretic side as if it were the affected side.
Objectives
To summarise the effectiveness of mirror therapy for improving motor function, activities of daily living, pain and visuospatial neglect in patients after stroke.
Search methods
We searched the Cochrane Stroke Group's Trials Register (June 2011), the Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL) (The Cochrane Library 2011, Issue 2), MEDLINE (1950 to June 2011), EMBASE (1980 to June 2011), CINAHL (1982 to June 2011), AMED (1985 to June 2011), PsycINFO (1806 to June 2011) and PEDro (June 2011). We also handsearched relevant conference proceedings, trials and research registers, checked reference lists and contacted trialists, researchers and experts in our field of study.
Selection criteria
We included randomised controlled trials (RCTs) and randomised cross‐over trials comparing mirror therapy with any control intervention for patients after stroke.
Data collection and analysis
Two review authors independently selected trials based on the inclusion criteria, documented the methodological quality of studies and extracted data. We analysed the results as standardised mean differences (SMDs) for continuous variables.
Main results
We included 14 studies with a total of 567 participants that compared mirror therapy with other interventions. When compared with all other interventions, mirror therapy may have a significant effect on motor function (post‐intervention data: SMD 0.61; 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.22 to 1.0; P = 0.002; change scores: SMD 1.04; 95% CI 0.57 to 1.51; P < 0.0001). However, effects on motor function are influenced by the type of control intervention. Additionally, mirror therapy may improve activities of daily living (SMD 0.33; 95% CI 0.05 to 0.60; P = 0.02). We found a significant positive effect on pain (SMD ‐1.10; 95% CI ‐2.10 to ‐0.09; P = 0.03) which is influenced by patient population. We found limited evidence for improving visuospatial neglect (SMD 1.22; 95% CI 0.24 to 2.19; P = 0.01). The effects on motor function were stable at follow‐up assessment after six months.
Authors' conclusions
The results indicate evidence for the effectiveness of mirror therapy for improving upper extremity motor function, activities of daily living and pain, at least as an adjunct to normal rehabilitation for patients after stroke. Limitations are due to small sample sizes of most included studies, control interventions that are not used routinely in stroke rehabilitation and some methodological limitations of the studies.
Keywords: Humans, Recovery of Function, Stroke Rehabilitation, Activities of Daily Living, Exercise Movement Techniques, Exercise Movement Techniques/instrumentation, Exercise Movement Techniques/methods, Functional Laterality, Functional Laterality/physiology, Paresis, Paresis/etiology, Paresis/rehabilitation, Randomized Controlled Trials as Topic, Stroke, Stroke/complications
Mirror therapy for improving motor function after stroke
Paralysis of the arm or leg is common after stroke and frequently causes problems with activities of daily living such as walking, dressing or eating. Mirror therapy is a rehabilitation therapy in which a mirror is placed between the arms or legs so that the image of the non‐affected limb gives the illusion of normal movement in the affected limb. We found 14 relevant studies involving 567 participants. At the end of treatment, mirror therapy improved movement of the affected limb and the ability to carry out daily activities. Mirror therapy reduced pain after stroke, but only in patients with a complex regional pain syndrome. The beneficial effects on movement were maintained for six months, but not in all study groups. No adverse side effects were reported. Further research is needed with larger studies in natural clinical settings, and with a comparison of mirror therapy with more routine treatments.
Background
Description of the condition
Cerebrovascular diseases, taken together with ischaemic heart diseases, are the leading causes of death worldwide (WHO 2008). Each year approximately nine million people suffer a first‐ever stroke. Stroke is one of the leading causes of long‐term disability, particularly in high‐ and middle‐income countries (WHO 2008). Immediately after stroke onset, approximately 80% of survivors have an upper or lower limb motor impairment (Barker 1997; Jorgensen 1995; Nakayama 1994). Full upper limb function is achieved by nearly 80% of patients with mild paresis, but only by 20% of patients with severe paresis of the upper limb (Nakayama 1994). Of those patients with an initial plegic upper limb, only half regain some motor function in the paretic upper limb six months later (Kwakkel 2003). Two‐thirds of patients with lower limb impairment are not able to walk independently soon after their stroke, and after rehabilitation only half have independent walking function (Jorgensen 1995). The initial severity of upper and lower extremity paresis is one of the most important predictors of long‐term functional recovery after stroke (Hendricks 2002; Jorgensen 1995; Nakayama 1994), but variability is high, possibly influenced by therapeutic interventions.
Up to 50% of patients experience pain of the upper extremity during the first 12 months post‐stroke, especially shoulder pain and complex regional pain syndrome‐type I (CRPS‐type I) (Jönsson 2006; Kocabas 2007; Lundström 2009; Sackley 2008). Pain after stroke may restrict activities of daily living and reduce quality of life (Jönsson 2006; Lindgren 2007).
Additionally, about 40% of patients with an acute right hemispheric and 20% of patients with a left hemispheric stroke presented with a unilateral neglect (Ringman 2004). After three months a unilateral neglect was present in about 15% of patients with a right and 5% of patients with a left hemispheric stroke (Ringman 2004). Besides the spatial attention deficits, neglect is a negative factor for functional recovery (Farnè 2004; Katz 1999) and was found to be associated with a reduced health‐related quality of life (Franceschini 2010).
Therefore, effective training strategies to promote motor recovery and activities of daily living, reduce pain or visuospatial neglect or both are needed to reduce the burden of stroke.
Description of the intervention
Evidence suggests that effective therapeutic interventions for regaining motor function should potentially focus on the practice of functional tasks (Van Peppen 2004). However, task‐oriented training strategies, such as constraint‐induced movement therapy (French 2007; Liepert 1998; Miltner 1999b; Taub 1993), require some degree of voluntary movement, therefore they are not applicable for patients with severe paresis after stroke. Novel training strategies for this patient population use electromechanical training devices (Mehrholz 2007; Mehrholz 2008), electrical muscle stimulation (Urton 2007) or repetitive passive or assistive movement stimulation (Feys 2004; Platz 2005).
As an alternative treatment approach, mirror therapy has been proposed as potentially beneficial (Ramachandran 1994). In contrast to other interventions, which employ somatosensory input to assist motor recovery (Feys 2004), mirror therapy is based on visual stimulation. During mirror therapy, a mirror is placed in the patient's midsagittal plane, thus reflecting the non‐paretic side as if it were the affected side (Ramachandran 1995). By this setup, movements of the non‐paretic limb create the illusion of normal movements of the paretic limb. One of the possible advantages of mirror therapy is the relatively easy administration and the possibility for self‐administered home therapy for patients even with severe motor deficits. Mirror therapy was first described in Ramachandran 1995 and Ramachandran 1996 with the studies reporting the effects of mirror therapy on pain reduction in arm amputees. Furthermore, mirror therapy was claimed to alleviate hemiparesis after stroke (Ramachandran 1994). A pilot study confirmed the positive effects of mirror therapy on patients' movement ability in upper limb hemiparesis after stroke (Altschuler 1999).
Recently, some authors have described 'mirror‐like' video or computer graphic setups, where a video or computer graphic image of the moving limb is presented as if it were the opposite one (Adamovich 2009; Dohle 2004; Dohle 2011; Eng 2007; Gaggioli 2004; Morganti 2003).
How the intervention might work
The concept of mirror therapy has been substantiated neurophysiologically. Evidence suggests that the same cortical motor areas that are active during observation of movements are involved in the performance of the observed actions (Grèzes 2001). Movement mirroring (i.e. the inversion of the visual feedback) leads to an additional activation of the hemisphere contralateral to the perceived limb laterality (Dohle 2004; Matthys 2009; Shinoura 2008). In normal people, the mirror illusion may increase cortico‐muscular excitability (Fukumura 2007; Garry 2005). However, the precise mechanisms of the effect of mirror therapy in stroke patients remain speculative. As the visual image of the paretic limb is perceived similarly to the patients' own moving limb (Dohle 2004), the mirror illusion might prevent or reverse a learned non‐use of the paretic limb (Liepert 1995). Also, by modulation of the cortico‐muscular excitability, mirror therapy might directly stimulate motor recovery. Finally, mirror therapy was regarded as a variant of motor imagery training, which is based on repetitive imagination and mental rehearsal of motor tasks (Miltner 1998; Stevens 2003). Behavioural studies suggest that the experience of agency (the attribution of visual images of body parts as being controlled by oneself) relies on a tight temporal coupling of the visual feedback of active, but not passive movements (Longo 2009). Imaging studies suggest that mirrored computer graphic images are processed similarly to those of real movements (Adamovich 2009; Dohle 2011) as long as the temporal and spatial consistency with real movements does not fall below certain thresholds (Franck 2001). Thus, even technically generated images of a human moving limb can be integrated into the body scheme with the same sense of agency as during 'real' mirroring.
Regarding non‐motor symptoms, mirror therapy was found to be effective in reducing pain in patients with CRPS‐type I (McCabe 2003). The authors hypothesised that mirror therapy may normalise central sensory processing by providing a physiological image of the affected limb (McCabe 2003). Another study found significant effects of mirror therapy on reducing unilateral visuospatial neglect after stroke (Dohle 2009). The strong visual stimulus of watching self‐induced movements in the neglected hemifield was postulated to be responsible for this effect.
Why it is important to do this review
Recently, randomised controlled trials (RCTs) have been conducted to evaluate the effectiveness of mirror therapy after stroke (Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Dohle 2009; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yavuzer 2008). These trials however, employed different outcome measures and only had small study samples. Ezendam 2009 and Rothgangel 2011 published systematic reviews on the effectiveness of mirror therapy in different conditions. However, their search strategies were limited and the authors did not provide pooled analyses.
Objectives
The main purpose of this review is to summarise the effectiveness of mirror therapy compared with no treatment, placebo or sham therapy, or other treatments for improving motor function after stroke. Further, this review aims to assess the effects of mirror therapy on activities of daily living, pain and visuospatial neglect.
Methods
Criteria for considering studies for this review
Types of studies
We included RCTs and cross‐over RCTs comparing mirror therapy (provided by a mirror or a simultaneous video or virtual setup) with any other therapy modality, no therapy or sham therapy. If we included cross‐over RCTs, we only analysed the first period as a parallel group trial.
Types of participants
We included studies examining participants with a paresis of the upper or lower limb, or both, caused by stroke (all types, severity and stages of stroke) aged over 18 years. If we identified studies with mixed populations of patients with neurological conditions, we included those studies if separate data for stroke patients were available.
Types of interventions
Mirror therapy is defined as an intervention that uses a mirror to create a reflection of the non‐paretic upper or lower limb, thus giving the patient visual feedback of normal movement of the paretic limb. Using this setup, different variations in the experimental protocol are possible (Dohle 2005; Nakaten 2009). We included studies that used direct mirroring of movement of any regimen and variation, i.e. including video or virtual reality settings. However, we only included those studies where the regimen and delivery of mirror therapy could be identified.
The control arm of the study could include a no treatment group, usual or standard practice, or any other control treatment (i.e. placebo or sham therapy). We excluded studies where the influence of mirror therapy could not be isolated due to the comparison of different mirror therapy regimens or delivery. We contacted trialists if regimen or delivery (or both) of mirror therapy or the control intervention was unclear.
Types of outcome measures
We evaluated outcome measures post‐intervention (or change scores between pre‐ and post‐intervention measures) and at follow‐up after six months or longer.
Primary outcomes
The primary outcome was motor function. Due to the wide variety of outcome measures, we selected outcome measures to facilitate quantitative pooling. If more than one outcome measure was available we prioritised measures as follows.
-
Upper limb and hand function:
Fugl‐Meyer Assessment (Fugl‐Meyer 1975) ‐ upper limb or hand function or both;
Action Research Arm Test (Lyle 1981), Motor Assessment Scale (Carr 1985) ‐ upper limb and hand function or both; and
Wolf Motor Function Test (Wolf 2001), Brunnstrom Stages of the Upper Extremity (Brunnstrom 1966), Motricity Index (Demeurisse 1980) ‐ arm score.
-
Lower limb function:
Fugl‐Meyer Assessment ‐ lower limb function (Fugl‐Meyer 1975); and
Brunnstrom Stages of the Lower Extremity (Brunnstrom 1966).
-
Global motor function:
Motor Assessment Scale (Carr 1985), Rivermead Motor Assessment Scale (Collen 1991).
However, if these scales were not available, we accepted other measurements that evaluate motor function.
Secondary outcomes
Secondary outcomes included measures of activities of daily living (e.g. Functional Independence Measure (Keith 1987), Barthel Index (Mahoney 1965)), pain (Visual Analogue Scale or Numeric Rating Scale) and visuospatial neglect. We also searched for reported adverse effects (e.g. swelling).
Search methods for identification of studies
See the 'Specialised register' section in the Cochrane Stroke Group module.
Electronic searches
We searched the Cochrane Stroke Group's Trials Register, which was last searched by the Managing Editor in June 2011, the Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL) (The Cochrane Library 2011, Issue 2), MEDLINE (1950 to June 2011) (Appendix 1), EMBASE (1980 to June 2011) (Appendix 2), CINAHL (1982 to June 2011) (Appendix 3), AMED (1985 to June 2011), PsycINFO (1806 to June 2011) and the Physiotherapy Evidence Database (PEDro) (June 2011). We modified the MEDLINE and CINAHL search strategies for the other databases.
Searching other resources
In an effort to identify further published, unpublished and ongoing trials not available in the major databases we:
-
handsearched the following conference proceedings:
Deutsche Gesellschaft für Neurologie (2008, 2009);
Deutsche Gesellschaft für Neurorehabilitation (2000, 2001, 2003, 2005, 2007, 2009, 2010);
Deutsche Gesellschaft für Neurotraumatologie und klinische Neurorehabilitation (2005, 2007, 2009, 2010);
European Stroke Congress (2001 to 2009);
World Congress of Neurorehabilitation (1999, 2002, 2006, 2010);
World Congress of Physical Therapy (2003, 2007, 2011);
World Stroke Congress (2000, 2004, 2008, 2010);
screened reference lists of all relevant articles and books;
-
identified ongoing trials and research registers, including:
Current Controlled Trials (http://www.controlled‐trials.com/) (searched June 2011);
ClinicalTrials.gov (http://clinicaltrials.gov/) (searched June 2011);
Stroke Trials Registry (http://www.strokecenter.org/trials/) (searched June 2011);
International Clinical Trials Registry Platform (ICTRP) (http://www.who.int/ictrp/en/) (searched June 2011);
contacted trialists, experts, researchers and commercial companies (Reflex Pain Management Ltd) in our field of study to obtain information of unpublished studies and studies not available in the electronic databases;
searched OpenSIGLE ‐ System for Information on Grey Literature in Europe (http://www.opengrey.eu/) (searched June 2011); and
searched the REHABDATA database (http://www.naric.com/research/rehab/) (searched June 2011).
We did not impose any restrictions on language or publication status when deciding on including studies.
Data collection and analysis
Selection of studies
Two review authors (HT and CD) independently screened titles of the references identified from the electronic database searches and excluded obviously irrelevant references. We obtained abstracts or full texts or both of the remaining studies and used our inclusion criteria (types of studies, types of participants, types of interventions and outcome measures) to assess whether they were eligible for inclusion. We resolved disagreements by discussion. If the inclusion of a study was unclear due to missing information, we tried to contact the authors of the studies for further details.
Data extraction and management
Two review authors (HT and CD) independently extracted trial and outcome data of the included trials using a checklist. Because one author (CD) is principal investigator of an included trial, another author (JB) did the data extraction of this study. The checklists for data extraction contained:
methods of randomisation;
methods of concealment of allocation;
blinding;
use of an intention‐to‐treat (ITT) analysis (all participants initially randomised were included in the analysis as allocated to groups);
adverse events;
drop‐outs for all reasons;
imbalance of important prognostic factors;
participants (country, number of participants, age, gender, type of stroke, time since stroke onset to study entry);
inclusion and exclusion criteria;
details of interventions in treatment and control groups;
outcomes; and
time points of measurement.
We tried to establish all unclear characteristics of the studies by contacting the trial co‐ordinator or principal investigator. We checked the extracted data for agreement between authors and entered the data into Review Manager 5 (RevMan 5).
Assessment of risk of bias in included studies
We used the risk of bias assessment tool according to Chapter 8 of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011) to assess the adequacy of methods for sequence generation, concealment of allocation, ITT analysis, and blinding of assessors.
Furthermore, we used the PEDro scale, with 11 criteria, for methodological assessment of the included studies (Maher 2003). The PEDro scale assesses:
specified eligibility criteria;
random allocation;
concealed allocation;
similarity of baseline characteristics of the patients;
blinding of patients;
blinding of therapists;
blinding of assessors;
outcome data of at least 85% of participants of at least one key outcome;
ITT analysis;
between‐group statistical comparisons; and
point measures and measures of variability.
We scored each fulfilled criteria in the PEDro scale except the first one (specified eligibility criteria) with one point. Therefore, the maximum possible score was 10 points. Two review authors (HT and CD) independently assessed the PEDro scale of included studies. Because one author (CD) is principal investigator of an included trial, another author (JB) did the quality assessment of this study. We resolved disagreements in methodological assessment by consulting a third review author (MP or JB) and reached consensus through discussion. If an article did not contain information on any methodological criteria, we contacted the study authors for additional information. If no further information was available, we rated the criteria as 'unclear'.
Measures of treatment effect
The primary and secondary outcome variables of interest were continuous outcomes. We entered data of post‐intervention assessment and follow‐up assessment at six months as means and standard deviations (SDs) and calculated the standardised mean difference (SMD) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for each trial. We pooled data through calculation of the overall SMD and 95% CI. However, if post‐intervention data were not available we used changes between pre‐ and post‐assessment and summarised them in a separate analysis using the SMD and 95% CIs.
Unit of analysis issues
We considered randomised cross‐over trials prior to cross‐over and analysed only the first intervention phase.
Dealing with missing data
We contacted study authors if appropriate data for analysis were not adequately reported. If authors did not respond within one month after contact, we tried to get in contact with them at least one more time. We considered an ITT analysis as part of the risk of bias assessment.
Assessment of heterogeneity
We evaluated clinical heterogeneity through reported clinical and methodological diversity, variability of participants, interventions and outcomes in an additional table. The variability did not influence pooling trials. However, we used the I2 statistic to quantify heterogeneity (cut‐off 50%) for all comparisons. If we found substantial heterogeneity, we used a random‐effects model instead of a fixed‐effect model.
Assessment of reporting biases
We tried to minimise reporting bias through an extensive search of databases, handsearching of references lists and conference abstracts, and by contacting authors, trialists and experts in the field for other unpublished or ongoing trials. Furthermore, we conducted a sensitivity analysis, excluding studies of low methodological quality.
Data synthesis
Where possible, we conducted a pooled analysis of primary (motor function) and secondary (activities of daily living, pain, visuospatial neglect) outcomes as described above, using a random‐effects model instead of a fixed‐effect model if heterogeneity of the studies was high. We performed a subgroup analysis to establish the effectiveness relative to upper or lower extremity and type of control intervention. We also analysed subgroups by separating immediate and long‐term results of mirror therapy.
Sensitivity analysis
To test the robustness of the results we conducted a sensitivity analysis, removing studies that we assessed to be of lower or ambiguous methodological quality (all studies with total PEDro scores less than seven points, all studies without adequate methods of sequence generation, concealment of allocation, ITT analysis and blinded assessors). We also reanalysed the data by removing cross‐over RCTs.
Based on the inclusion criteria of the studies, we found two studies (Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b) that only included stroke patients with a diagnosis of CRPS‐type I as defined by Bruehl 1999. One could argue that this is a special stroke population selected by a prominent feature. Furthermore, studies reported positive effects of mirror therapy in populations with CRPS of different origin (Ezendam 2009). This fact may influence the effects of mirror therapy in stroke patients with this feature. Therefore, we performed a post‐hoc sensitivity analysis of the data without these two studies for all outcome measures.
Results
Description of studies
See: Characteristics of included studies, Characteristics of excluded studies, Characteristics of studies awaiting classification, Characteristics of ongoing studies and Table 6.
Table 1.
Patient characteristics of included studies
Study ID | Experimental: age, mean (SD) | Control: age, mean (SD) | Experimental: time post‐stroke | Control: time post‐stroke | Experimental: sex | Control: sex | Experimental: side paresis | Control: side paresis |
Acerra 2007 | 65.9 (11.5) years | 70.8 (5.4) years | 5.2 (3.4) days | 5.4 (2.7) days | 12 female, 8 male | 10 female, 10 male | 8 left, 12 right | 8 left, 12 right |
Altschuler 1999 | 55.8 (4.3) years | 60.2 (7.6) years | 1.85 (1.98) years | 7.72 (10.48) years | 2 female, 2 male | 2 female, 3 male | 1 left, 3 right | 0 left, 5 right |
Cacchio 2009a | 57.9 (9.9) years | 58.8 (9.4) years | 5.1 (2.5) months | 4.9 (2.8) months | 13 female, 11 male | 13 female, 11 male | 16 left, 8 right | 18 left, 6 right |
Cacchio 2009b | 63.4 (9.7) years | Control 1: 61.8 (8.4) years Control 2: 62.3 (9.2) years |
16.8 (6.6) months | Control 1: 14.9 (4.8) months Control 2: 15.4 (5.3) months |
4 female, 4 male | Control 1: 5 female, 3 male Control 2: 4 female, 4 male |
5 left, 3 right | Control 1: 5 left, 3 right Control 2: 5 left, 3 right |
Dohle 2009 | 54.9 (13.8) years | 58.0 (14.0) years | 26.2 (8.3) days | 27.8 (12.1) days | 5 female, 13 male | 5 female, 13 male | Not provided | Not provided |
Ietswaart 2011 | 69.3 (10.8) years | Control 1: 68.6 (16.3) years Control 2: 64.4 (15.9) years |
82.0 (55.0) days | Control 1: 90.8 (63.4) days Control 2: 80.5 (62.7) days |
18 female, 23 male | Control 1: 17 female, 22 male Control 2: 16 female, 25 male |
24 left, 17 right | Control 1: 23 left, 16 right Control 2: 22 left, 19 right |
Manton 2002 | Not provided | Not provided | Not provided | Not provided | Not provided | Not provided | Not provided | Not provided |
Michielsen 2011 | 55.3 (12.0) years | 58.7 (13.5) years | 4.7 (3.6) years | 4.5 (2.6) years | 13 female, 7 male | 7 female, 13 male | 6 dominant, 14 non‐dominant |
6 dominant, 14 non‐dominant |
Rothgangel 2004 | Experimental 1: 74.0 (12.5) years Experimental 2: 72.0 (15.3) years |
Control 1: 77.7 (4.9) years Control 2: 72.0 (15.3) years |
Median (range) Experimental 1: 12 (9 to 15) months Experimental 2: 7 (3 to 14) months |
Median (range) Control 1: 12 (5 to 18) months Control 2: 7 (5 to 24) months |
6 female, 2 male | 4 female, 4 male | Experimental 1: 2 left, 1 right Experimental 2: 3 left, 2 right |
Control 1: 1 left, 2 right Control 2: 2 left, 3 right |
Seok 2010 | 56.4 (14.8) years | 46.4 (21.5) years | 4.3 (1.6) months | 3.7 (1.9) months | 8 female, 11 male | 10 female, 11 male | 11 left, 8 right | 6 left, 14 right |
Sütbeyaz 2007 | 62.7 (9.7) years | 64.7 (7.7) years | 3.5 (1.3) months | 3.9 (1.9) months | 10 female, 10 male | 7 female, 13 male | 14 left, 6 right | 13 left, 7 right |
Tezuka 2006 | 64.6 (16.5) years | 63.7 (10.3) years | 38.3 (16.5) days | 29.7 (8.2) days | 4 female, 5 male | 5 female, 1 male | 4 left, 5 right | 2 left, 4 right |
Yavuzer 2008 | 63.2 (9.2) years | 63.3 (9.5) years | 5.4 (2.9) months | 5.5 (2.5) months | 8 female, 9 male | 9 female, 10 male | 10 left, 7 right | 11 left, 8 right |
Yun 2010 | 63.1 (7.8) years | 61.4 (8.7) years | 3.4 (1.6) weeks | 4.1 (1.8) weeks | 14 female, 6 male | 13 female, 7 male | 11 left, 9 right | 12 left, 8 right |
SD: standard deviation
Results of the search
We identified 19 studies from the search of the Cochrane Stroke Group's Trials Register. After excluding all duplicate references we identified a total of 1802 references from the other databases. Two review authors (HT and CD) identified 155 possible eligible trials. We excluded 140 studies. In the Excluded studies section, only those studies are mentioned that might in a superficial view appear to meet the eligibility criteria and those studies that were classified to be well known and likely to be considered relevant by some readers (Characteristics of excluded studies). There was insufficient information to determine inclusion eligibility for one trial (Amimoto 2008), but we failed to get in contact with the authors, therefore the study is listed as "awaiting classification" (see Characteristics of studies awaiting classification). Additionally, we identified four ongoing trials (see Characteristics of ongoing studies).
Included studies
Fourteen trials met the inclusion criteria of our review (Acerra 2007; Altschuler 1999; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Dohle 2009; Ietswaart 2011; Manton 2002; Michielsen 2011; Rothgangel 2004; Seok 2010; Sütbeyaz 2007; Tezuka 2006; Yavuzer 2008; Yun 2010) (see Characteristics of included studies). We found two separate reports of one study (Rothgangel 2004; Rothgangel 2007) and based our data extraction and analysis on their first publication (Rothgangel 2004). One study was only available as an abstract (Manton 2002) and we were not able to contact the authors of the study and therefore had insufficient data to include this trial in the pooled analysis.
Design
Twelve studies were RCTs with parallel group design (Acerra 2007; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Dohle 2009; Ietswaart 2011; Manton 2002; Michielsen 2011; Rothgangel 2004; Seok 2010; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yavuzer 2008; Yun 2010) and two studies used a cross‐over design with random allocation to the order of treatment (Altschuler 1999; Tezuka 2006). For the latter two studies, we only used data of the first intervention period for pooled analysis.
Sample Size
The 14 studies included a total of 567 participants. Individual sample sizes of identified trials ranged from nine (Altschuler 1999) to 121 (Ietswaart 2011). A detailed description of individual sample sizes can be found in the Characteristics of included studies section.
Participants
Detailed descriptions of patient characteristics are given in Table 6.
The mean age of participants in the included studies ranged from 51 years (Seok 2010) to 79 years (Rothgangel 2004). There were more participants with a hemiparesis of the left side (55%). There were more male (57%) than female (43%) participants. One study did not provide data of participants (Manton 2002). Nine studies included participants after their first‐ever stroke (Acerra 2007; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Dohle 2009; Michielsen 2011; Rothgangel 2004; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yavuzer 2008; Yun 2010). Mean time post‐stroke ranged between five days (Acerra 2007) and five years (Altschuler 1999). Four studies included participants in the acute or subacute phase after stroke (within three months post‐stroke) (Acerra 2007; Dohle 2009; Tezuka 2006; Yun 2010) and eight trials included the chronic phase (more than three months) (Altschuler 1999; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Manton 2002; Michielsen 2011; Rothgangel 2004; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yavuzer 2008). Two studies included participants within six months post‐stroke (Ietswaart 2011; Seok 2010). Ten studies provided information on the aetiology of strokes; four studies did not (Altschuler 1999; Ietswaart 2011; Manton 2002; Seok 2010). Among those patients with known aetiology, 83% had an ischaemic and 17% a haemorrhagic stroke.
Twelve studies provided information of the study setting: inpatient rehabilitation (Acerra 2007; Dohle 2009; Seok 2010; Sütbeyaz 2007; Tezuka 2006; Yavuzer 2008; Yun 2010); inpatient and outpatient rehabilitation (Cacchio 2009a); day hospital and inpatient rehabilitation (Rothgangel 2004); home setting (Manton 2002; Michielsen 2011); and inpatient hospital and home setting (Ietswaart 2011). The included studies were conducted in nine different countries.
Studies used the following inclusion criteria.
First‐ever diagnosed stroke (Acerra 2007; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Dohle 2009; Michielsen 2011; Rothgangel 2004; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yavuzer 2008; Yun 2010).
Diagnosed stroke (Altschuler 1999; Ietswaart 2011; Seok 2010; Tezuka 2006).
Diagnosis of CRPS‐type I (Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b).
Between 25 and 80 years of age (Dohle 2009).
Able to follow therapy instructions (Dohle 2009; Seok 2010; Yavuzer 2008).
Capable of participating in 30 minutes of daily therapy (Dohle 2009).
Knowledge of Dutch language (Michielsen 2011).
Ambulatory before stroke (Sütbeyaz 2007).
Brunnstrom score between III and V (Michielsen 2011), between II and V (Seok 2010) or between I and IV (Yavuzer 2008; Yun 2010) for the upper extremity; between I and III (Sütbeyaz 2007) for the lower extremity.
Minimal score of 1 on the Action Research Arm Test (Rothgangel 2004) or between 3 and 51 on the Action Research Arm Test (Ietswaart 2011).
Grade of hemiparesis of 6 or less points after Ueda's method (0 to 12 points) (Tezuka 2006).
Home dwelling status (Michielsen 2011).
Studies used the following exclusion criteria.
Major haemorrhagic changes, increased intracranial pressure and hemicraniectomy (Dohle 2009).
Major comorbidities (Acerra 2007) or serious uncontrolled medical conditions (Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Yun 2010).
Psychological diagnosis or cognitive impairment (e.g. severe dementia) that might interfere with study participation (Acerra 2007; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Ietswaart 2011; Seok 2010; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yavuzer 2008; Yun 2010) or higher brain dysfunction (Tezuka 2006).
Unco‐operative patients (Yun 2010).
Evidence of recent drug or alcohol abuse (Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Ietswaart 2011).
Impairment of vision or hearing or both (Acerra 2007; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Rothgangel 2004).
Global aphasia (Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b).
Severe aphasia (Ietswaart 2011; Seok 2010).
Severe neglect (Michielsen 2011; Rothgangel 2004).
Neglect, aphasia or apraxia (Yun 2010).
Inability to sit supported for less than one hour (Acerra 2007).
Other reasons for not being able to use the affected or unaffected limbs other than current stroke (Acerra 2007; Dohle 2009; Michielsen 2011).
Musculoskeletal or neurological damage of the unaffected upper extremity (Seok 2010).
Prior surgery to shoulder or neck or both (Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b).
Intra‐articular injection into the affected shoulder in the previous four months (Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b).
Presence of other explanation for pain (Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b).
Discharge from hospital within one week after admission (Ietswaart 2011).
No upper limb motor weakness (Ietswaart 2011).
Limited rehabilitation potential (Ietswaart 2011).
Modified Ashworth Scale of three or more points (Seok 2010).
Seven studies reported no drop‐outs during the intervention period (Acerra 2007; Altschuler 1999; Cacchio 2009b; Rothgangel 2004; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yavuzer 2008; Yun 2010), three trialists reported drop‐out rates of less than 15% (Cacchio 2009a; Ietswaart 2011; Michielsen 2011) and in two studies the drop‐out rate was above 15% (Dohle 2009; Tezuka 2006). In two studies the drop‐out rate was unclear (Manton 2002; Seok 2010).
A total of 15 participants dropped out in the experimental groups and 11 participants dropped out in the control groups. Reasons for dropping out during mirror therapy were:
death (Ietswaart 2011);
moving to another city (Cacchio 2009a);
changing hospital (Cacchio 2009a; Dohle 2009);
worsening medical condition (Dohle 2009; Ietswaart 2011; Michielsen 2011);
early discharge from rehabilitation (Dohle 2009);
withdrawal of consent (Dohle 2009; Ietswaart 2011; Michielsen 2011; Tezuka 2006); and
social issues (Michielsen 2011).
A detailed description of study characteristics can be found in the Characteristics of included studies section and in Table 6.
Interventions
Characteristics of interventions are summarised in Table 7. All included studies provided mirror therapy using a mirror or a mirror box in the midsagittal plane between the upper (Acerra 2007; Altschuler 1999; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Dohle 2009; Manton 2002; Michielsen 2011; Rothgangel 2004; Seok 2010; Tezuka 2006; Yavuzer 2008; Yun 2010) or lower limbs (Sütbeyaz 2007). Thus, the mirror reflected movements of the non‐affected side as if these movements were executed with the affected side.
Table 2.
Characteristics of interventions of included studies
Study ID | Extremity | Mirror therapy variation | Control intervention | Type of movements | Minutes per session | Sessions per week | Total duration (weeks) | Total amount of therapy (minutes) | Setting |
Acerra 2007 | Upper extremity | Bilateral activities | Bilateral activities; covered mirror | Functional motor tasks (i.e. with objects); motor co‐ordination tasks; sensory discrimination tasks; grip strength; active range of motion |
20 to 30 | 7 | 14 | 280 to 420 | Inpatient hospital |
Altschuler 1999 | Upper extremity | Bilateral activities | Bilateral activities; transparent plastic between limbs | Proximal and distal movements | 15, 2 times a day | 12 | 4 (first period) | 720 | Not stated |
Cacchio 2009a | Upper extremity | Activities of the unaffected limb | Activities of the unaffected limb; covered mirror | Flexion/extension of shoulder, elbow and wrist; prone/supination forearm | 30 first 2 weeks; 60 last 2 weeks |
5 | 4 | 900 | Inpatient and outpatient rehabilitation centre |
Cacchio 2009b | Upper extremity | Activities of the unaffected limb | Activities of the unaffected limb; covered mirror (control group 1); imagination of movements of the affected limb (control group 2) |
Flexion/extension of shoulder, elbow and wrist; prone/supination forearm | 30 | Daily | 4 | 840 | Inpatient and outpatient rehabilitation centre |
Dohle 2009 | Upper extremity | Bilateral activities | Bilateral activities; without mirror | Execution of arm, hand and finger postures | 30 | 5 | 6 | 900 | Inpatient rehabilitation centre |
Ietswaart 2011 | Upper extremity | Activities of the unaffected limb (additionally to a motor imagery intervention) | Control 1: mental rehearsal of non‐motor related images Control 2: standard care |
Elementary movements and patient selected movements | 10 | 1 to 2 | 4 | 60 | Hospital and home |
Manton 2002 | Upper extremity | Not stated | Not stated; transparent plastic between limbs | Not stated | Not stated | Not stated | 4 | Not stated | Home |
Michielsen 2011 | Upper extremity | Bilateral activities; once a week under supervision; 5 times a week at home | Bilateral activities; once a week under supervision, 5 times a week at home; without mirror | Exercises based on the Brunnstrom phases of motor recovery; functional tasks (i.e. with objects) | 60 | 1 (under supervision) + 5 (at home) | 6 | 2160 | Home |
Rothgangel 2004a | Upper extremity | Bilateral activities (hypotone muscles); unilateral activities (hypertone muscles) | Bilateral activities; without mirror | Gross motor arm and hand movements; functional activities (i.e. with objects); fine motor activities (i.e. with objects) | 30 | Total number of sessions: 17 | 5 | 510 | Outpatient centre |
Rothgangel 2004b | Upper extremity | Bilateral activities (hypotone muscles); unilateral activities (hypertone muscles) | Bilateral activities; without mirror | Gross motor arm and hand movements; functional activities (i.e. with objects); fine motor activities (i.e. with objects) | 30 | Total number of sessions: 37 | 5 | 1110 | Inpatient rehabilitation centre |
Seok 2010 | Upper extremity | Activities of the unaffected limb | No therapy | 5 movements of wrist and fingers, each 6 minutes | 30 | 5 | 4 | 500 | Inpatient rehabilitation centre |
Sütbeyaz 2007 | Lower extremity | Activities of the unaffected limb | Activities of the unaffected limb; covered mirror | Dorsiflexion movements of the ankle | 30 | 5 | 4 | 600 | Inpatient rehabilitation centre |
Tezuka 2006 | Upper extremity | Activities of the unaffected limb; affected limb passively moved by therapist | Activities of the unaffected limb; affected limb passively moved by therapist; without mirror | 13 kinds of movements, i.e. flexion/extension of wrist, pinching fingers, gripping ball | 10 to 15 | Daily | 4 (first period) | 280 to 420 | Inpatient rehabilitation centre |
Yavuzer 2008 | Upper extremity | Activities of the unaffected limb | Activities of the unaffected limb; covered mirror | Flexion/extension of wrist and fingers | 30 | 5 | 4 | 600 | Inpatient rehabilitation centre |
Yun 2010 | Upper extremity | Experimental 1: activities of the unaffected limb Experimental 2: activities of the unaffected limb and additionally neuromuscular electrical stimulation of the affected arm |
Neuromuscular electrical stimulation of finger and wrist extensors of the affected arm | Flexion/extension of wrist and fingers | 30 | 5 | 3 | 450 | Inpatient rehabilitation centre |
Two studies used a combination of mirror therapy and other interventions. Yun 2010 integrated a second intervention group, in which mirror therapy was combined with neuromuscular electrical stimulation. We combined both intervention groups of this study for analysis using raw data. Ietswaart 2011 used mirror therapy within a motor imagery intervention protocol. Mirror therapy was integrated in this study to evoke action stimulation and was used during less than 10% of the total intervention duration.
Mirror therapy was provided for one to two (Ietswaart 2011), five (Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Dohle 2009; Michielsen 2011; Seok 2010; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yavuzer 2008; Yun 2010) or seven (Acerra 2007; Cacchio 2009b; Tezuka 2006) days a week for two (Acerra 2007) to six weeks (Dohle 2009; Michielsen 2011). Each session lasted between 10 (Ietswaart 2011; Tezuka 2006) and 60 minutes (Cacchio 2009a; Michielsen 2011). For one study (Manton 2002) a detailed description of the interventions could not be identified.
Rothgangel 2004 included a total of 16 participants and randomised them to mirror therapy or bilateral arm training. However, six of the patients were treated in an outpatient rehabilitation centre, and 10 in an inpatient care facility, which led to a significant difference in treatment time: the outpatient group received 17 treatment sessions, 30 minutes each; the inpatient group received 37 treatment sessions, 30 minutes each. Because these two groups are considerably different in total treatment time, we decided to analyse them separately (outpatient group: Rothgangel 2004a and inpatient group: Rothgangel 2004b).
In five studies participants performed bilateral movements, moving the affected limb behind the mirror as best they could (Acerra 2007; Altschuler 1999; Dohle 2009; Michielsen 2011; Yavuzer 2008). In six studies patients only moved the unaffected side while looking in the mirror (Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Ietswaart 2011; Seok 2010; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yun 2010). In the study of Rothgangel 2004 patients with muscle hypotonia had to move the affected arm as best they could; patients with muscle hypertonia should only move the unaffected arm while looking into the mirror. In one study, a therapist passively moved the affected arm behind the mirror according to the movements of the unaffected one (Tezuka 2006).
Control interventions of all included studies were carried out with the same amount and frequency as the experimental intervention, except for one study (Seok 2010), where the control group received no additional intervention to standard rehabilitation. Ietswaart 2011 included two control groups, where the second control group received no additional treatment to normal care. Five studies used a form of sham therapy (Acerra 2007; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yavuzer 2008) where the reflecting side of the mirror was covered, or the nonreflecting side of the mirror was placed in the direction of the unaffected arm while practising. Yun 2010 included a control group with neuromuscular electrical stimulation but also with a covered mirror between limbs. Four studies provided interventions with an unrestricted view on the affected side using the same training as in the experimental groups but without a mirror (Dohle 2009; Michielsen 2011; Rothgangel 2004) or with a plexiglas between limbs (Altschuler 1999). In one study a therapist passively moved the affected arm according to the movements of the unaffected one but without a mirror between limbs (Tezuka 2006). Cacchio 2009b included a second control group, practising motor imagery tasks. We combined the two control groups of this study for overall analysis. As mentioned above, Ietswaart 2011 included two control groups, where the first control group received an attention‐placebo intervention. Patients in this group performed mental rehearsal that was not related to motor control. We combined both control groups of this study for analysis.
Based on the difference of using a covered mirror or no mirror (also transparent plexiglas), we performed a subgroup analysis differentiating the effects of both types of control intervention (covered mirror versus unrestricted view).
Outcome
The included studies used a number of different outcomes. A description of the outcome measures used can be found in Characteristics of included studies.
Primary outcome: motor function
For analysis of our primary outcome motor function we used the Fugl‐Meyer score of the upper extremity (Dohle 2009; Michielsen 2011; Yun 2010) or wrist and fingers (Tezuka 2006), the Action Research Arm Test (Ietswaart 2011; Rothgangel 2004), the Wolf Motor Function Test (functional ability) (Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b), the Motor Assessment Scale Item 7 (Acerra 2007) and the Brunnstrom stages of motor recovery lower extremity (Sütbeyaz 2007) or upper extremity and hand (combined using raw data) (Yavuzer 2008).
Secondary outcomes: activities of daily living, pain and visuospatial neglect
In our pooled analysis of the secondary outcome activities of daily living we used the Functional Independence Measure motor subscore (Dohle 2009; Sütbeyaz 2007) or self‐care subscore (Yavuzer 2008) and the Barthel Index (Ietswaart 2011). For the analysis of the secondary outcome of pain we included the measurement of pain at rest (Acerra 2007; Cacchio 2009b; Michielsen 2011) and during movement (Cacchio 2009a; Dohle 2009). The investigators used Numerical Rating Scales (Acerra 2007) between 0 and 10, Visual Analogue Scales between 0 and 10 (Cacchio 2009a) or between 0 mm and 100 mm (Cacchio 2009b; Michielsen 2011) or the pain section of the Fugl‐Meyer Assessment, normalised on the average score per item (0 to 2; 2 indicating no pain) (Dohle 2009).
Visuospatial neglect as an outcome was included in one study (Dohle 2009). The authors used a self‐defined five‐point neglect score based on the Behavioral Inattention Test (BIT) and the Test of Attentional Performance (TAP).
Follow‐up assessment
Seven studies provided follow‐up assessments of one month (Acerra 2007), five weeks (Rothgangel 2004), three months (Manton 2002) and six months (Cacchio 2009a; Michielsen 2011; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yavuzer 2008) after the intervention period. For analysis of sustained treatment effects for our primary outcome, we used only the data of follow‐up assessments after six months.
Adverse effects
Only one study explicitly reported the assessment of adverse effects (Acerra 2007).
Excluded studies
We excluded a total of 140 studies following consideration of abstracts, full texts or both (see: Characteristics of excluded studies section). Clinical studies with stroke patients employing computer graphic implementation of mirror therapy were generally not RCTs (Gaggioli 2009; Merians 2009).
Risk of bias in included studies
All details about the methodological quality of the included studies using the risk of bias assessment tool (Higgins 2011) and the PEDro Scale are provided in Characteristics of included studies, Figure 1, and Table 8.
Table 3.
PEDro score of included studies
Acerra 2007 | Altschuler 1999 | Cacchio 2009a | Cacchio 2009b | Dohle 2009 | Ietswaart 2011 | Manton 2002 | Michielsen 2011 | Rothgangel 2004 | Seok 2010 | Sütbeyaz 2007 | Tezuka 2006 | Yavuzer 2008 | Yun 2010 | |
Random allocation | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes |
Concealed allocation | Yes | Unclear | Unclear | Yes | Yes | Yes | Unclear | Yes | Yes | Unclear | Yes | No | Yes | No |
Baseline comparability | Yes | No | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Unclear | Yes | No | No | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes |
Blind participants | No | No | No | No | No | No | Unclear | No | No | No | No | No | No | No |
Blind therapists | No | No | No | No | No | No | Unclear | No | No | No | No | No | No | No |
Blind assessors | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Unclear | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | No |
Adequate follow‐up | Yes | No | Yes | Yes | No | Yes | Unclear | Yes | Yes | Unclear | Yes | No | Yes | Yes |
Intention‐to‐treat | Yes | No | Yes | Yes | No | Yes | Unclear | Yes | Yes | Unclear | No | No | No | No |
Between group comparisons | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Unclear | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes |
Point estimates and variability | Yes | No | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Unclear | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes |
Total PEDro score | 8/10 | 3/10 | 7/10 | 8/10 | 6/10 | 8/10 | 1/10 | 8/10 | 7/10 | 4/10 | 7/10 | 5/10 | 7/10 | 5/10 |
Figure 1.
Risk of bias summary: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item for each included study.
We emailed all trialists of the included studies to clarify some methodological or design issues, or both. Most trialists provided at least some of the requested information. We did not receive an answer to methodological issues for three trials (Altschuler 1999; Manton 2002; Seok 2010).
Two review authors (HT and CD) independently evaluated the methodological quality of the studies using the PEDro scale. One trial was rated by JB instead of CD. The review authors disagreed on the criteria of:
baseline comparability (Altschuler 1999; Dohle 2009);
adequate follow‐up assessment (Altschuler 1999); and
ITT analysis (Altschuler 1999; Dohle 2009; Michielsen 2011; Rothgangel 2004; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yavuzer 2008).
The assessing authors discussed all disagreements and resolved them by contacting another author or obtaining additional information through contact with the principal investigator of the study. Table 8 presents the ratings of each item and the total score of the PEDro scale of the included studies. In general, the quality of studies could be regarded as high. As it is not possible to blind patients and therapists to the intervention, the maximum possible total score is 8 out of 10 points. Three of the included studies reached the maximum possible score of eight points (Acerra 2007; Cacchio 2009b; Michielsen 2011). The study by Manton 2002 only reached a total score of one point due to incomplete information. The median possible PEDro score of all included studies was seven points.
Allocation
Two studies used a cross‐over design with random allocation to the order of treatment (Altschuler 1999; Tezuka 2006). We only analysed the first treatment period as a parallel group design in these two studies. Three studies used block randomisation methods (Cacchio 2009b; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yavuzer 2008 ). One study (Ietswaart 2011) based their randomised group allocation on different stratification factors. Another study randomly allocated ability matched pairs to treatment groups (Manton 2002). Eight studies used a concealment of allocation (Acerra 2007; Cacchio 2009b; Dohle 2009; Ietswaart 2011; Michielsen 2011; Rothgangel 2004; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yavuzer 2008).
Blinding
In all but two studies (Manton 2002; Yun 2010), at least the primary outcome measures were assessed by people blinded to group allocation.
Other potential sources of bias
The methods used for concealment of allocation are presented in the Characteristics of included studies tables. An ITT analysis was performed in six studies (Acerra 2007; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Ietswaart 2011; Michielsen 2011; Rothgangel 2004).
Information about the reasons for dropping out are presented in the Characteristics of included studies section. Only one of the included studies explicitly reported that they found no adverse effects (Acerra 2007).
Effects of interventions
We included 13 studies with a total of 506 participants in the analysis (Acerra 2007; Altschuler 1999; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Dohle 2009; Ietswaart 2011; Michielsen 2011; Rothgangel 2004; Seok 2010; Sütbeyaz 2007; Tezuka 2006; Yavuzer 2008; Yun 2010). Rothgangel 2004 provided data on two subgroups of patients. Because these two subgroups are considerably different in total treatment time, we analysed them separately (subgroup 1: Rothgangel 2004a and subgroup 2: Rothgangel 2004b). One included study was only available as an abstract and did not provide sufficient data for analysis (Manton 2002).
Comparison 1: Mirror therapy versus all other interventions
Outcome 1.1: Motor function at the end of the intervention phase
We included 13 studies in a pooled analysis on motor function after study end (Acerra 2007; Altschuler 1999; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Dohle 2009; Ietswaart 2011; Michielsen 2011; Rothgangel 2004; Seok 2010; Sütbeyaz 2007; Tezuka 2006; Yavuzer 2008; Yun 2010). As two studies (Altschuler 1999; Rothgangel 2004) only presented change scores between pre‐ and post‐assessment, we performed separate analyses for post‐assessment data and changes between pre‐ and post‐assessment, to control for possible differences in study effects.
We included eleven studies with a total of 234 participants in the intervention and 247 in the control groups in the post‐assessment data analysis (Acerra 2007; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Dohle 2009; Ietswaart 2011; Michielsen 2011; Seok 2010; Sütbeyaz 2007; Tezuka 2006; Yavuzer 2008; Yun 2010). Mirror therapy has a significant effect on motor function in patients after stroke compared with all other types of interventions (SMD 0.61; 95% CI 0.22 to 1.0; P = 0.002; I2 = 75%, random‐effects model) (Analysis 1.1).
Analysis 1.1.
Comparison 1 Mirror therapy versus all other interventions: primary and secondary outcomes, Outcome 1 Motor function at the end of intervention phase.
Nine studies with a total of 147 participants in the intervention and 136 in the control groups provided change scores between pre‐ and post‐assessment (Altschuler 1999; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Rothgangel 2004; Seok 2010), or we used raw data for analysis of change scores (Dohle 2009; Tezuka 2006; Yavuzer 2008; Yun 2010). In the analysis of change scores, we also found a significant effect of mirror therapy compared with all other interventions on motor function after stroke (SMD 1.04; 95% CI 0.57 to 1.51; P < 0.0001; I2 = 65%, random‐effects model) (Analysis 1.1).
Because the effects based on change scores might be overestimated, and only two studies with a total of 25 participants presented change scores (Altschuler 1999; Rothgangel 2004), we based all further analysis on studies that provided post‐intervention data.
Outcome 1.2: Activities of daily living at the end of the intervention phase
We included four studies in the analysis of the outcome of activities of daily living (Dohle 2009; Ietswaart 2011; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yavuzer 2008). These studies included 94 participants in the intervention and 123 in the control groups. Mirror therapy has a significant effect on activities of daily living for patients with stroke, compared with all other interventions (SMD 0.33; 95% CI 0.05 to 0.60; P = 0.02; I2 = 15%, fixed‐effect model) (Analysis 1.2).
Analysis 1.2.
Comparison 1 Mirror therapy versus all other interventions: primary and secondary outcomes, Outcome 2 Activities of daily living at the end of intervention phase.
Outcome 1.3: Pain at the end of the intervention phase
For analysing the effects of mirror therapy on pain at the end of the intervention, we included five studies presenting data on pain at rest or during movement (Acerra 2007; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Dohle 2009; Michielsen 2011). These five studies included 90 participants in the intervention and 98 in the control groups. Mirror therapy has a significant effect on pain reduction for patients after stroke, compared with all other interventions (SMD ‐1.10; 95% CI ‐2.10 to ‐0.09; P = 0.03; I2 = 89%, random‐effects model) (Analysis 1.3).
Analysis 1.3.
Comparison 1 Mirror therapy versus all other interventions: primary and secondary outcomes, Outcome 3 Pain at the end of intervention phase.
Outcome 1.4: Visuospatial neglect at the end of the intervention
One study reported outcome on visuospatial neglect (Dohle 2009). They presented data only on those patients who initially presented a visuospatial neglect (9 in the intervention and 11 in the control group). Based on these data, we found a significant effect of mirror therapy versus all other interventions on visuospatial neglect after stroke (SMD 1.22; 95% CI 0.24 to 2.19) (Analysis 1.4)
Analysis 1.4.
Comparison 1 Mirror therapy versus all other interventions: primary and secondary outcomes, Outcome 4 Visuospatial neglect at the end of intervention.
Outcome 1.5: Motor function at follow‐up after six months
Four studies provided data on motor function at a follow‐up period of six months (Cacchio 2009a; Michielsen 2011; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yavuzer 2008). These studies included 78 patients in the experimental and 79 in the control groups. At follow‐up after six months, mirror therapy had a significant, lasting effect on motor function in patients after stroke, compared with all other interventions (SMD 1.09; 95% CI 0.30 to 1.87; P = 0.007; I2 = 81%, random‐effects model) (Analysis 1.5).
Analysis 1.5.
Comparison 1 Mirror therapy versus all other interventions: primary and secondary outcomes, Outcome 5 Motor function at follow‐up after 6 months.
No adverse events of mirror therapy were reported.
Comparison 2: Subgroup analysis ‐ upper versus lower extremity
Outcome 2.1: Motor function at the end of the intervention phase
We performed a subgroup analysis for those studies examining mirror therapy for the upper extremity (subgroup 2.1.1) and lower extremity (subgroup 2.1.2) (Analysis 2.1). Thirteen studies examined mirror therapy for the upper extremity. Of these studies, we could include post‐intervention data of 10 studies with 194 participants in the experimental and 227 in the control groups (Acerra 2007; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Dohle 2009; Ietswaart 2011; Michielsen 2011; Seok 2010; Tezuka 2006; Yavuzer 2008; Yun 2010). We found a significant effect of mirror therapy on motor function of the upper extremity for patients after stroke compared to all other interventions (SMD 0.53; 95% CI 0.04 to 1.01; P = 0.03; I2 = 82%, random‐effects model) (Analysis 2.1).
Analysis 2.1.
Comparison 2 Subgroup analysis: upper versus lower extremity, Outcome 1 Motor function at the end of intervention.
One study with 20 participants in the experimental and control groups examined mirror therapy for the lower extremity (Sütbeyaz 2007). The effect of mirror therapy on motor function of the lower extremity for patients after stroke compared with all other interventions just reached significance (SMD 0.65; 95% CI 0.01 to 1.29; P = 0.05) (Analysis 2.1).
Comparison 3: Subgroup analysis ‐ sham intervention (covered mirror) versus other intervention (unrestricted view)
We found two different groups of control interventions. In all studies, participants in the control group performed the same movements as participants in the experimental groups. However, in one type of control intervention, the view on the affected side was obscured with a covered mirror, or with the non‐reflective side of the mirror (sham intervention). In the other type of control intervention, participants had an unrestricted view on both; the unaffected and the affected limb (other intervention). Because we believed that this may have influenced the effect of therapy, we performed a subgroup analysis, differentiating these two types of studies.
Outcome 3.1: Motor function at the end of the intervention phase
Six studies used a covered mirror in the control group (Acerra 2007; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yavuzer 2008; Yun 2010). These studies included 129 participants in the intervention and 111 in the control groups. For this subgroup we found a significant effect of mirror therapy on motor function after stroke (SMD 0.90; 95% CI 0.27 to 1.52; P = 0.005; I2 = 79%, random‐effects model). Five studies used no mirror, or a transparent plexiglas in the control groups, thus providing a view of both limbs (Altschuler 1999; Dohle 2009; Michielsen 2011; Rothgangel 2004; Tezuka 2006); we could analyse three of these studies. These studies included 47 participants in the experimental and 44 in the control groups. The effect of mirror therapy on motor function after stroke in these studies just reached significance (SMD 0.42; 95% CI 0.00 to 0.84; P = 0.05; I2 = 0%). However, the difference between subgroups was not statistically significant (P = 0.22) (Analysis 3.1).
Analysis 3.1.
Comparison 3 Subgroup analysis: sham intervention (covered mirror) versus other intervention (unrestricted view), Outcome 1 Motor function at the end of intervention phase.
Comparison 4: Sensitivity analysis by trial methodology
We tested the robustness of the results by analysing only RCTs and excluding randomised cross‐over trials, and by using specific methodological variables that could influence the observed treatment effects (PEDro total score > 6 points, concealment of allocation, blinding of assessors and ITT analysis) (Analysis 4.1).
Analysis 4.1.
Comparison 4 Sensitivity analysis by trial methodology, Outcome 1 Motor function at the end of intervention.
Outcome 4.1: Motor function at the end of the intervention phase
All studies without randomised cross‐over trials
We classified 12 studies as RCTs, of which we included 10 in a subgroup analysis of all studies without randomised cross‐over trials (Acerra 2007; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Dohle 2009; Ietswaart 2011; Michielsen 2011; Seok 2010; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yavuzer 2008; Yun 2010). The studies included 225 participants in the experimental and 241 in the control groups. Based on the analysis, mirror therapy has a significant effect on motor function in patients after stroke, compared to all other treatments (SMD 0.59; 95% CI 0.18 to 1.0; P = 0.005; I2 = 77%, random‐effects model) (Analysis 4.1).
All studies with a PEDro total score greater than 6 points
We classified eight studies as having more than six points in the PEDro scale, of which we could integrate seven in a pooled analysis (Acerra 2007; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Ietswaart 2011; Michielsen 2011; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yavuzer 2008). The studies included 148 participants in the experimental and 182 in the control groups. We found a significant effect of mirror therapy, compared with all other therapies for patients after stroke (SMD 0.81; 95% CI 0.27 to 1.36; P = 0.004; I2 = 81%, random‐effects model) (Analysis 4.1).
All studies with adequate sequence generation
We classified 10 studies as having an adequate method of sequence generation. We analysed nine studies with 202 participants in the intervention and 207 in the control groups (Acerra 2007; Dohle 2009; Ietswaart 2011; Michielsen 2011; Seok 2010; Sütbeyaz 2007; Tezuka 2006; Yavuzer 2008; Yun 2010). We found a significant effect of mirror therapy compared with all other therapies for patients after stroke (SMD 0.31; 95% CI 0.09 to 0.54; P = 0.007; I2 = 18%) (Analysis 4.1).
All studies with adequate concealed allocation
We classified seven studies as having used an adequate method of allocation concealment. We analysed six studies with 134 participants in the experimental and 160 in the control groups (Acerra 2007; Dohle 2009; Ietswaart 2011; Michielsen 2011; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yavuzer 2008). Based on the analysis, we found a significant effect of mirror therapy compared with all other therapies for patients after stroke (SMD 0.39; 95% CI 0.12 to 0.66; P = 0.005; I2 = 23%) (Analysis 4.1).
All studies with adequate intention‐to‐treat (ITT) analysis
We classified six studies as having used an adequate ITT analysis. Based on our analysis of five studies (Acerra 2007; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Ietswaart 2011; Michielsen 2011) with 111 participants in the experimental and 143 in the control groups with post‐intervention data, mirror therapy has a significant effect on motor function compared with all other interventions (SMD 0.91; 95% CI 0.12 to 1.71; P = 0.02; I2 = 87%, random‐effects model) (Analysis 4.1).
All studies with blinded assessors
Twelve studies used blinded assessors for at least the primary outcome. In this analysis we included 10 studies with 194 participants in the experimental and 227 in the control groups (Acerra 2007; Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b; Dohle 2009; Ietswaart 2011; Michielsen 2011; Seok 2010; Sütbeyaz 2007; Tezuka 2006; Yavuzer 2008). Mirror therapy has a significant positive effect on motor function compared with all other interventions (SMD 0.67; 95% CI 0.25 to 1.10; P = 0.002; I2 = 76%, random‐effects model) (Analysis 4.1).
Comparison 5: Post‐hoc sensitivity analysis ‐ removing studies that only included studies with complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS) after stroke
Two studies (Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b) only included patients after stroke with a diagnosis of CRPS‐type I which might have influenced the effects of the intervention. Thus, we performed a post‐hoc sensitivity analysis and removed studies that only included participants with CRPS after stroke.
Outcome 5.1: Motor function at the end of the intervention phase
We included 11 studies, of which we analysed nine (Acerra 2007; Dohle 2009; Ietswaart 2011; Michielsen 2011; Seok 2010; Sütbeyaz 2007; Tezuka 2006; Yavuzer 2008; Yun 2010). These nine studies included 202 participants in the intervention and 207 in the control groups. Excluding those studies that only included patients with CRPS led to a reduced, but still significant effect of mirror therapy on motor function for patients after stroke, compared with all other interventions (SMD 0.31; 95% CI 0.09 to 0.54; P = 0.0007; I2 = 18%) (Analysis 5.1).
Analysis 5.1.
Comparison 5 Post‐hoc sensitivity analysis removing studies that only included patients with CRPS after stroke, Outcome 1 Motor function at the end of intervention.
Outcome 5.2: Pain at the end of the intervention phase
After removing those two studies that only included patients with CRPS, we included three studies with 58 participants in the intervention and 58 in the control groups (Acerra 2007; Dohle 2009; Michielsen 2011). We found no significant effect on pain for mirror therapy compared with all other interventions in this subgroup (SMD ‐0.16; 95% CI ‐0.53 to 0.20; P = 0.38; I2 = 0%) (Analysis 5.2)
Analysis 5.2.
Comparison 5 Post‐hoc sensitivity analysis removing studies that only included patients with CRPS after stroke, Outcome 2 Pain at the end of intervention phase.
Outcome 5.3: Motor function at follow‐up after six months
We removed one study that included stroke patients with CRPS only. We analysed three studies with 54 participants in the experimental and 55 in the control group (Michielsen 2011; Sütbeyaz 2007; Yavuzer 2008). We found a reduced, but still significant effect of mirror therapy compared with all other interventions for motor function at follow‐up after six months (SMD 0.69; 95% CI 0.26 to 1.13; P = 0.002; I2 = 18%).
Discussion
Summary of main results
The main purpose of this review was to evaluate the effect of mirror therapy for improving motor function, activities of daily living and reducing pain and visuospatial neglect for patients after stroke. We included 14 studies (12 RCTs and two randomised cross‐over studies), with a total of 567 participants that compared mirror therapy with other interventions. We found evidence that mirror therapy may improve motor function, activities of daily living, pain and visuospatial neglect compared with all other interventions. Furthermore, the effects on motor function were stable at follow‐up assessment after six months. No adverse events of mirror therapy were reported.
Thirteen of the included studies evaluated the effect of mirror therapy on the upper extremity, and one study evaluated the effect of mirror therapy on the lower extremity. Mirror therapy was effective in improving motor function, both for the upper extremity and for the lower extremity. Based on a subgroup analysis, we found evidence that the effects might have been influenced by the type of control treatment: effects on motor function were robustly significant in those studies that compared mirror therapy with a sham intervention that uses a covered mirror, thus avoiding any view of the affected limb. Significance was just reached and the overall effect was smaller in studies that used unrestricted view (no mirror or a transparent plexiglas). It should be noted that Cacchio 2009a and Cacchio 2009b only included patients with a diagnosis of complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS)‐type I after stroke. By excluding these two studies in a sensitivity analysis however, the evidence that mirror therapy may improve motor function and motor function at six months follow‐up remained. However, no significant effect on pain was present after excluding the studies of stroke patients with a diagnosed CRPS‐type 1.
Quality of the evidence
We used several methodological domains (adequate sequence generation, adequate concealment of allocation, adequate ITT analysis and blinding of assessors) to assess the risk of bias in the included studies. We assessed four studies as having unclear sequence generation. Furthermore, we found six studies with no or unclear use of concealed allocation of participants to study groups, and eight studies with no or unclear use of an adequate ITT analysis. All but two analysed studies used blinded assessors. Additionally, we assessed the PEDro scale for evaluating the methodological quality of the studies (Maher 2003). The median of the PEDro scale total scores was seven points, indicating overall a high quality of studies. However, we classified six studies to have a PEDro score lower than seven points.
Some of the analysis showed significant heterogeneity. However, in all cases this was no longer present when leaving out those studies that included only patients with CRPS after stroke. However, we cannot exclude the possibility that other factors are responsible for the heterogeneity. Therefore, caution in the interpretation of the results is needed.
In order to test for potential biases through methodological issues, we performed a sensitivity analysis and excluded randomised cross‐over studies, studies with a total PEDro score below seven points, studies with unclear adequacy of sequence generation, studies with inadequate concealment of allocation, studies not providing an ITT analysis and studies that did not use assessors blinded to intervention. Based on that sensitivity analysis, the effects of mirror therapy on motor function of patients after stroke were robust. However, overall limitations of the included studies were small sample sizes of most studies, very limited inclusion of control groups that used other effective interventions for the upper or lower extremity in most studies and differences in therapy delivery between the studies (i.e. amount and frequency of the treatment period).
Potential biases in the review process
Through an extensive searching process, we are confident that we have identified all relevant studies in the field. However, a risk of publication bias towards a selection of positive results remains. Furthermore, there is a small possibility of additional (published or unpublished) studies that we did not identify. As stated above, there was heterogeneity between studies in terms of trial design (i.e. parallel group and cross‐over trials, duration of follow‐up and selection criteria for patients), characteristics of patients (i.e. severity of motor impairment and time since stroke onset) and characteristics of interventions (i.e. total amount of time of therapy, percentage of the intervention dedicated to mirror therapy only and therapy for upper or lower extremity). We also identified methodological limitations of studies. However, as stated above, a sensitivity analysis with respect to methodological limitations and patient characteristics revealed the robustness of the results across all stated potential confounding factors.
Agreements and disagreements with other studies or reviews
The results of this review are in line with the results of other reviews (Ezendam 2009; Rothgangel 2011). These reviews were systematic in terms of their method. However, they had more limited search strategies, only included studies that were published before 2009 and did not use a pooled analysis of identified studies. A narrative review also described positive effects of mirror therapy after stroke (Ramachandran 2009).
Potential benefit
We found that mirror therapy was effective in terms of improving motor function of the upper extremity, activities of daily living and pain for patients that suffered a stroke. No conclusion could be drawn in terms of visuospatial neglect because the results were based on only one study with a small population. The positive results for motor function were consistent with follow‐up assessment after six months. The results are limited because our subgroup analysis indicates evidence of a greater effect of mirror therapy on motor function when compared with a sham intervention (using a covered mirror) than when compared with other interventions (using unrestricted view). Therefore, the positive effects in this review at least indicate that mirror therapy as an adjunct to routine therapy can improve motor function for patients after stroke.
Mirror therapy did significantly reduce pain in patients after stroke. However, this result is mainly based on two studies that included only patients with CRPS‐type I after stroke and should not be generalised to an unselected stroke patient population.
One of the potential advantages of mirror therapy compared with other interventions may be due to the possibility of training by moving the unaffected arm or both arms while looking in the mirror. Therefore, even patients with severe paresis could practise on their own without a therapist. Furthermore, mirror therapy could be applied at home as evaluated in two studies (Manton 2002; Michielsen 2011). However, we were not able to test the effectiveness of this therapy regime explicitly due to limited data.
Authors' conclusions
The results of this review indicate that there is evidence for the effectiveness of mirror therapy for improving motor function for patients after stroke. The effects were more prominent and with a clear statistical significance when mirror therapy was compared with sham intervention. Compared with bilateral arm training with unrestricted view, the effects for mirror therapy only just reached significance. Therefore, mirror therapy could be applied as an additional intervention in the rehabilitation of patients after stroke, but no clear conclusion could be drawn if mirror therapy replaced other interventions for improving motor function of the arm. Additionally, we found evidence that mirror therapy may improve activities of daily living and visuospatial neglect, but the results must be interpreted with caution. Results for activities of daily living are based on only four studies. No clear implication could be drawn for visuospatial neglect, because results are based on only one study. Significant effects on pain are only present in studies that included only patients with a CRPS‐type 1 after stroke. Therefore, for this subgroup of patients, mirror therapy seems to be an effective intervention, both for improving motor function and reducing pain.
There is a need for well designed randomised controlled studies with large sample sizes in order to evaluate the effects of mirror therapy after stroke. Above all, further research should compare mirror therapy with other conventionally applied or newly developed and effective therapies. Additionally, further research should address specific questions due to the optimal dose, frequency and duration of mirror therapy, and should focus on outcomes in activities of daily living. Further research should also answer questions about the effects of mirror therapy according to the extent of motor impairment, and should even focus on patients with impairments other than motor impairments after stroke, such as pain and visuospatial neglect. Finally, it is important to update this review regularly in order to include studies that are ongoing at the time of publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank Brenda Thomas for her help with developing and running the search strategy and Hazel Fraser for providing us with relevant trials from the Cochrane Stroke Group Trials Register and giving us helpful support. We also thank Gaby Voigt for providing us with many helpful studies and Luara dos Santos for performing literature searches. We thank all authors and investigators who provided us with additional information and data on their studies. Finally, we owe thanks to the reviewers of the first version of this review who provided several helpful suggestions.
Appendices
Appendix 1. MEDLINE search strategy
MEDLINE (Ovid) 1950 to June 2011
1. cerebrovascular disorders/ or exp basal ganglia cerebrovascular disease/ or exp brain ischemia/ or exp carotid artery diseases/ or exp intracranial arterial diseases/ or exp intracranial arteriovenous malformations/ or exp "intracranial embolism and thrombosis"/ or exp intracranial hemorrhages/ or stroke/ or exp brain infarction/ 2. brain injuries/ or brain injury, chronic/ 3. (stroke$ or cva or poststroke or post‐stroke).tw. 4. (cerebrovasc$ or cerebral vascular).tw. 5. (cerebral or cerebellar or brain$ or vertebrobasilar).tw. 6. (infarct$ or isch?emi$ or thrombo$ or emboli$ or apoplexy).tw. 7. 5 and 6 8. (cerebral or brain or subarachnoid).tw. 9. (haemorrhage or hemorrhage or haematoma or hematoma or bleed$).tw. 10. 8 and 9 11. exp hemiplegia/ or exp paresis/ 12. (hempar$ or paretic or paresis or hemipleg$ or brain injur$).tw. 13. Gait Disorders, Neurologic/ 14. 1 or 2 or 3 or 4 or 7 or 10 or 11 or 12 or 13 15. exp Upper Extremity/ 16. (upper limb$ or upper extremit$ or arm or shoulder or hand or axilla or elbow$ or forearm$ or finger$ or wrist$).tw. 17. exp Lower Extremity/ 18. (lower limb$ or lower extremit$ or buttock$ or foot or feet or hip or hips or knee or knees or leg or legs or thigh$ or ankle$ or heel$ or toe or toes).tw. 19. 15 or 16 or 17 or 18 20. Illusions/ 21. (mirror$ or visual$ or virtual$).tw. 22. (visual adj5 (reflection or illusion or feedback or therapy)).tw. 23. ((limb$ or arm or leg) adj5 (reflect or reflection or illusion)).tw. 24. 20 or 21 or 22 or 23 25. 14 and 19 and 24
Appendix 2. EMBASE search strategy
1 exp basal ganglia cerebrovascular disease/ 2 cerebrovascular disorders/ 3 exp intracranial arterial diseases/ 4 exp intracranial arteriovenous malformations/ 5 exp "intracranial embolism and thrombosis"/ 6 exp intracranial hemorrhages/ 7 stroke/ 8 exp brain infarction/ 9 exp brain ischemia/ 10 exp carotid artery diseases/ 11 1 or 2 or 3 or 4 or 5 or 6 or 7 or 8 or 9 or 10 12 (brain injuries or brain injury, chronic).af. 13 (stroke$ or cva or poststroke or post‐stroke tw).af. 14 (cerebrovasc$ or cerebral vascular tw).af. 15 (cerebral or cerebellar or brain$ or vertebrobasilar tw).af. 16 (infarct$ or ish?emi$ or thrombo$ or emboli$ or apoplexy tw).af. 17 15 and 16 18 (cerebral or brain or subarachnoid tw).af. 19 (haemorrhage or hemorrhage or haematoma or hematoma or bleed$ tw).af. 20 18 and 19 21 exp hemiplegia/ or exp paresis/ 22 (hempar$ or paretic or paresis or hemipleg$ or brain injur$).tw. 23 Gait Disorders, Neurologic/ 24 11 or 12 or 13 or 14 or 17 or 20 or 21 or 22 or 23 25 exp Upper Extremity/ 26 (upper limb$ or upper extremit$ or arm or shoulder or hand or axilla or elbow$ or forearm$ or finger$ or wrist$).tw. 27 exp Lower Extremity/ 28 (lower lib$ or lower extremit$ or buttock$ or foot or feet or hip or hips or knee or knees or leg or legs or thigh$ or ankle$ or heel$ or toe or toes).tw. 29 25 or 26 or 27 or 28 30 Illusions/ 31 mirror$.tw. 32 (visual adj5 (refelction or illusion or feedback or therapy)).tw. 33 ((limb$ or arm or leg) adj5 (reflect or reflection or illusion)).tw. 34 30 or 31 or 32 or 33 35 24 and 29 and 34 (309)
Appendix 3. CINAHL search strategy
CINAHL (Ebsco) 1982 to June 2011
(MH "Cerebrovascular Disorders+") or (MH "stroke patients") or (MH "stroke units")
TI ( stroke or poststroke or post‐stroke or cerebrovasc* or cerebral vasc or cva) or AB ( stroke or poststroke or post‐stroke or cerebrovasc* or cerebral vasc or cva)
TI ( brain* or cerebr* or cerebell* or vertebrobasilar ) or AB ( brain* or cerebr* or cerebell* or vertebrobasilar )
TI ( ischemi* or ischaemi* or infarct* or thrombo* or emboli* or apoplexy* ) or AB ( ischemi* or ischaemi* or infarct* or thrombo* or emboli* or apoplexy* )
3 and 4
TI ( brain* or cerebr* or cerebell* or subarachnoid ) or AB ( brain* or cerebr* or cerebell* or subarachnoid )
TI ( haemorrhage* or hemorrhage* or haematoma* or hematoma* or bleed* ) or AB ( haemorrhage* or hemorrhage* or haematoma* or hematoma* or bleed* )
6 and 7
(MH "Hemiplegia")
TI ( hemipleg* or hemipar* or paresis or paretic or brain injur* ) or AB ( hemipleg* or hemipar* or paresis or paretic or brain injur* )
(MH "Brain Injuries")
1 or 2 or 5 or 8 or 9 or 10 or 11
(MH "Upper Extremity+")
TI ( upper limb* or upper extremit* or arm or shoulder or hand or axilla or elbow* or forearm* or finger* or wrist* ) or AB ( upper limb* or upper extremit* or arm or shoulder or hand or axilla or elbow* or forearm* or finger* or wrist* )
(MH "Lower Extremity+")
TI ( lower limb* or lower extremit* or buttock* or foot or feet or hip or hips or knee or knees or leg or legs or thigh* or ankle* or heel* or toe or toes ) or AB ( lower limb* or lower extremit* or buttock* or foot or feet or hip or hips or knee or knees or leg or legs or thigh* or ankle* or heel* or toe or toes )
13 or 14 or 15 or 16
(MH "Illusions+")
(MH "Reflection")
TI ( mirror* or video* or virtual* ) and AB ( mirror* or video* or virtual* )
TI ( reflect or reflection or illusion or visual feedback ) or AB ( reflect or reflection or illusion or visual feedback )
18 or 19 or 20 or 21
12 and 17 and 22
Data and analyses
Comparison 1.
Mirror therapy versus all other interventions: primary and secondary outcomes
Outcome or subgroup title | No. of studies | No. of participants | Statistical method | Effect size |
---|---|---|---|---|
1 Motor function at the end of intervention phase | 14 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | Subtotals only | |
1.1 All studies with post‐intervention data | 11 | 481 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.61 [0.22, 1.00] |
1.2 All studies with change scores | 10 | 283 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 1.04 [0.57, 1.51] |
2 Activities of daily living at the end of intervention phase | 4 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | Subtotals only | |
2.1 All studies | 4 | 217 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.33 [0.05, 0.60] |
3 Pain at the end of intervention phase | 5 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | Subtotals only | |
3.1 All studies | 5 | 188 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐1.10 [‐2.10, ‐0.09] |
4 Visuospatial neglect at the end of intervention | 1 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | Totals not selected | |
4.1 All studies | 1 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.0 [0.0, 0.0] | |
5 Motor function at follow‐up after 6 months | 4 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | Subtotals only | |
5.1 All studies | 4 | 157 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 1.09 [0.30, 1.87] |
Comparison 2.
Subgroup analysis: upper versus lower extremity
Outcome or subgroup title | No. of studies | No. of participants | Statistical method | Effect size |
---|---|---|---|---|
1 Motor function at the end of intervention | 11 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | Subtotals only | |
1.1 Mirror therapy for the upper extremity | 10 | 421 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.53 [0.04, 1.01] |
1.2 Mirror therapy for the lower extremity | 1 | 40 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.65 [0.01, 1.29] |
Comparison 3.
Subgroup analysis: sham intervention (covered mirror) versus other intervention (unrestricted view)
Outcome or subgroup title | No. of studies | No. of participants | Statistical method | Effect size |
---|---|---|---|---|
1 Motor function at the end of intervention phase | 9 | 331 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.75 [0.32, 1.19] |
1.1 Studies that used a covered mirror in the control group | 6 | 240 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.90 [0.27, 1.52] |
1.2 Studies that used unrestricted view in the control group | 3 | 91 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.42 [0.00, 0.84] |
Comparison 4.
Sensitivity analysis by trial methodology
Outcome or subgroup title | No. of studies | No. of participants | Statistical method | Effect size |
---|---|---|---|---|
1 Motor function at the end of intervention | 11 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | Subtotals only | |
1.1 All studies without randomised cross‐over trials | 10 | 466 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.59 [0.18, 1.00] |
1.2 All studies with a PEDro total score greater than 6 points | 7 | 330 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.81 [0.27, 1.36] |
1.3 All studies with adequate sequence generation | 9 | 409 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.31 [0.09, 0.54] |
1.4 All studies with adequate concealed allocation | 6 | 294 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.39 [0.12, 0.66] |
1.5 All studies with adequate intention‐to‐treat analysis | 5 | 254 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.91 [0.12, 1.71] |
1.6 All studies with blinded assessors | 10 | 421 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.67 [0.25, 1.10] |
Comparison 5.
Post‐hoc sensitivity analysis removing studies that only included patients with CRPS after stroke
Outcome or subgroup title | No. of studies | No. of participants | Statistical method | Effect size |
---|---|---|---|---|
1 Motor function at the end of intervention | 11 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | Subtotals only | |
1.1 All studies | 11 | 481 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.61 [0.22, 1.00] |
1.2 Without studies that only included patients with CRPS after stroke | 9 | 409 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.31 [0.09, 0.54] |
2 Pain at the end of intervention phase | 5 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | Subtotals only | |
2.1 All studies | 5 | 188 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐1.10 [‐2.10, ‐0.09] |
2.2 Without studies that only included patients with CRPS after stroke | 3 | 116 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.16 [‐0.53, 0.20] |
3 Motor function at follow‐up after 6 months | 4 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | Subtotals only | |
3.1 All studies | 4 | 157 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 1.09 [0.30, 1.87] |
3.2 Without studies that only included patients with CRPS after stroke | 3 | 109 | Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.69 [0.26, 1.13] |
Analysis 5.3.
Comparison 5 Post‐hoc sensitivity analysis removing studies that only included patients with CRPS after stroke, Outcome 3 Motor function at follow‐up after 6 months.
Differences between protocol and review
We added a further database for searching ongoing studies: International Clinical Trials Registry Platform (ICTRP).
We previously planed to perform a subgroup analysis comparing studies that included participants with different severities of motor impairment. Based on the baseline data for motor function we were not able to clearly differentiate studies based on this criteria, except in one study that only included participants with severe motor impairment (Dohle 2009). The other studies included participants with mixed severities of motor impairments. Due to these problems of differentiation, we decided not to do this subgroup analysis.
Two studies (Cacchio 2009a; Cacchio 2009b) only included patients after stroke with a diagnosis of CRPS‐type I, which might have influenced the effects of the intervention. Thus, we performed a post‐hoc sensitivity analysis by removing these studies; that was not planned in the protocol.
Characteristics of studies
Characteristics of included studies [ordered by study ID]
Acerra 2007
Methods | RCT | |
Participants | Country: Australia Sample size: 40 participants (20 in each group) Inclusion criteria: acute stroke (< 2 weeks) Exclusion criteria: previous stroke; vision or hearing impairment; acute trauma or impairment of the limbs; inability to sit for < 1 hour; MMSE < 22/30; major co‐morbidities |
|
Interventions | 2 arms:
1 and 2: 5 days a week, 20 to 30 minutes for 2 weeks; additional usual rehabilitation programme |
|
Outcomes | Outcomes were recorded at baseline, after 2 weeks of treatment and 1 month after treatment
|
|
Notes | Unpublished data We used means and SDs of Item 7 of the MAS, and combined the scores on pain intensity of shoulder and hand |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Computer generated random number sequence |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Generated list was used by an independent person for group allocation |
ITT analysis | Low risk | Results were analysed on an ITT basis |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) primary outcome | Low risk | Assessors were blinded to group allocation |
Altschuler 1999
Methods | Randomised cross‐over trial | |
Participants | Country: USA Sample size: 9 participants (9 in each group) Inclusion criteria: at least 6 months post‐stroke |
|
Interventions | 2 arms:
|
|
Outcomes | Outcomes were recorded at baseline, after 2, 4, 6 and 8 weeks
|
|
Notes | Data not included in the analysis | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Patients were randomly assigned (authors' statement) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not stated |
ITT analysis | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) primary outcome | Low risk | Assessors were blinded to group allocation |
Cacchio 2009a
Methods | RCT | |
Participants | Country: Italy Sample size: 48 participants (24 in each group; 6 dropped out post‐treatment, 3 more dropped out after 6 months) Inclusion criteria: hemiparesis after first ever ischaemic or haemorrhagic stroke; during first 6 months post‐stroke; diagnosed with CRPS‐type 1 with a VAS pain score > 4 cm Exclusion criteria: an intra‐articular injection into the affected shoulder during the previous 6 months or use of systemic corticosteroids during the previous 4 months; presence of another explanation of pain; prior surgery to shoulder or neck; serious uncontrolled medical conditions; global aphasia or cognitive impairments; visual impairments which might interfere with the aims of the study; evidence of recent alcohol or drug abuse; or severe depression |
|
Interventions | 2 arms: 4‐week conventional stroke rehabilitation programme and additional
1 and 2: 5 days a week, 30 minutes of therapy for the first 2 weeks; and 5 days a week, 60 minutes of therapy for the last 2 weeks |
|
Outcomes | Outcomes were recorded at baseline, 1 week after the intervention period and after 6 months
|
|
Notes | Published and unpublished data | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Patients were randomly allocated (authors' statement) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not stated |
ITT analysis | Low risk | Results were analysed on an ITT basis |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) primary outcome | Low risk | Assessors were blinded to group allocation |
Cacchio 2009b
Methods | RCT | |
Participants | Country: Italy Sample size: 24 Participants (8 in each group) Inclusion criteria: first ischaemic or haemorrhagic stroke (> 6 months); diagnosis of CRPS‐type 1 (pain VAS > 4 cm) Exclusion criteria: intra‐articular shoulder injection in the previous 6 months or systemic corticosteroid in the previous 4 months; another obvious explanation for pain; prior surgery to shoulder or neck region; serious uncontrolled medical conditions; global aphasia or cognitive impairments interfering with understanding instructions, motor testing and treatment; visual impairments interfering with aims of the study; evidence of recent alcohol or drug abuse; or severe depression |
|
Interventions | 3 arms:
1, 2 and 3: 5 days a week; 30 minutes of therapy for 4 weeks |
|
Outcomes | Outcomes were recorded at baseline and after the intervention period
|
|
Notes | Published and unpublished data; we only analysed the first intervention period (4 weeks); we summarised groups 2 and 3 to one control group | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Block randomisation; sequence generation method not stated |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | A therapist not involved in the treatments; opened sealed envelopes and assigned appointments according to treatment group (authors' statement) |
ITT analysis | Low risk | Results were analysed on an ITT basis |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) primary outcome | Low risk | Assessors were blinded to group allocation |
Dohle 2009
Methods | RCT | |
Participants | Country: Germany Sample size: 48 participants (24 in each group, 12 dropped out) Inclusion criteria: first ever ischaemic stroke in the territory of the middle cerebral artery; not more than 8 weeks post‐stroke; between 25 and 80 years old; able to follow therapy instructions; capable of participating in 30‐minute daily therapy sessions Exclusion criteria: experienced previous stroke; major haemorrhagic changes; increased intracranial pressure; hemicraniectomy or orthopedic, rheumatologic, or other diseases interfering with their ability to sit or to move either upper limb |
|
Interventions | 2 arms:
1 and 2: 5 days a week; 30 minutes of therapy for 6 weeks |
|
Outcomes | Outcomes were recorded at baseline and after the intervention
|
|
Notes | Published and unpublished data; we extracted the motor section of the FM‐UE (without reflex activity, 0 to 60) | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Sealed, numbered envelopes were created |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Sealed envelopes were broken after study inclusion |
ITT analysis | High risk | Drop‐outs were not included in analysis |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) primary outcome | Low risk | Assessors of primary outcome were blinded to group allocation |
Ietswaart 2011
Methods | RCT | |
Participants | Country: Scotland, UK Sample size: 121 participants (experimental: 41; control 1: 39; control 2: 41; 18 dropped out) Inclusion criteria: stroke in the prior 1 to 6 months; ARAT score 3 to 51; no evidence of alcohol or substance abuse; no severe cognitive deficits ‐ Mental Status Questionnaire > 6; no severe aphasia |
|
Interventions | 3 arms:
1 and 2: 3 days a week; 45 minutes of therapy for 4 weeks additional to standard; additional 8 sessions of 30 minutes unsupervised motor imagery 1 or non‐motor imagery 2 |
|
Outcomes | Outcomes were recorded at baseline and after 5 weeks:
|
|
Notes | Only 8% of the intervention included mirror therapy as defined in the review | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Automated randomisation procedure |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Patients were randomised using the automated randomisation procedure up to 1 week after baseline assessment |
ITT analysis | Low risk | For completeness an ITT analysis was performed which rendered very similar results |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) primary outcome | Low risk | Assessors were blinded to group allocation |
Manton 2002
Methods | RCT | |
Participants | Country: USA Sample size: 10 participants Inclusion criteria: 6 months or more post‐cerebrovascular accident |
|
Interventions | 2 arms:
1 and 2: 4 weeks |
|
Outcomes | Outcomes were recorded at pre‐treatment, mid‐treatment, post‐treatment and after 3 months
|
|
Notes | Abstract data only; not included in the analysis due to insufficient data | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Ability matched pairs were created and randomly assigned to groups |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not stated |
ITT analysis | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) primary outcome | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Michielsen 2011
Methods | RCT | |
Participants | Country: Netherlands Sample size: 40 participants (20 in each group,; 4 dropped out during intervention period, 4 more dropped out after 6 months) Inclusion criteria: knowledge of Dutch language, Brunnstrom score upper extremity between 3 and 5; home dwelling status; at least 1 year post‐stroke Exclusion criteria: neglect; co‐morbidities that influenced upper extremity usage; history of multiple strokes |
|
Interventions | 2 arms:
1 and 2: once a week physiotherapeutic supervision for 60 minutes; 5 times a week, 60 minutes of practice at home for 6 weeks |
|
Outcomes | Outcomes were recorded at baseline, post‐treatment and after 6 months
|
|
Notes | Published and unpublished data | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Computer generated random number sequence |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Patients received group allocation after baseline measurement |
ITT analysis | Low risk | Results were analysed on an ITT basis |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) primary outcome | Low risk | Assessors were blinded to group allocation |
Rothgangel 2004
Methods | RCT; 2 baseline subgroups | |
Participants | Country: Netherlands Sample size: 16 participants (6 in the outpatient centre group (Rothgangel 2004a), 10 in the inpatient rehabilitation group (Rothgangel 2004b) Inclusion criteria: first stroke in the territory of the middle cerebral artery; minimal 3 months post‐stroke; minimal score of 1 in the ARAT Exclusion criteria: bilateral stroke; severe neglect; severe visual impairments |
|
Interventions | 2 arms:
1 and 2: day hospital group (6 participants): 17 treatments during 5 weeks for 30 minutes each; inpatient rehabilitation group (10 participants): 37 treatments during 5 weeks for 30 minutes each |
|
Outcomes | Outcomes were recorded at baseline, in the middle of the treatment, after 5 weeks of treatment and 10 weeks after baseline
|
|
Notes | Due to sufficient differences in treatment intensity, we analysed both experimental and both control groups separately Significant differences in baseline characteristics (age, ARAT, patient‐specific problem scale) |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Computer generated random number sequence |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Patients received group allocation after baseline measurement |
ITT analysis | Low risk | All patients were analysed as allocated to groups. No drop‐outs |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) primary outcome | Low risk | Assessors were blinded to group allocation |
Rothgangel 2004a
Methods | RCT; subgroup: outpatient centre | |
Participants | Country: Netherlands Sample size: 6 participants (3 in each group) Inclusion criteria: first stroke in the territory of the middle cerebral artery; minimal 3 months post‐stroke; minimal score of 1 in the ARAT Exclusion criteria: bilateral stroke; severe neglect; severe visual impairments | |
Interventions | 2 arms:
1 and 2: 17 treatments during 5 weeks for 30 minutes each |
|
Outcomes | Outcomes were recorded at baseline, in the middle of the treatment, after 5 weeks of treatment and 10 weeks after baseline ARAT (0 to 57) Patient‐specific problem scale (0 to 100) | |
Notes | Significant differences in baseline characteristics: patients in the experimental group were younger and had a lower ARAT score |
Rothgangel 2004b
Methods | RCT; subgroup: inpatient rehabilitation | |
Participants | Country: Netherlands Sample size: 10 participants (5 in each group) Inclusion criteria: first stroke in the territory of the middle cerebral artery; minimal 3 months post‐stroke; minimal score of 1 in the ARAT Exclusion criteria: bilateral stroke; severe neglect; severe visual impairments | |
Interventions | 2 arms:
1 and 2: day hospital group (6 participants): 17 treatments during 5 weeks for 30 minutes each; inpatient rehabilitation group (10 participants): 37 treatments during 5 weeks for 30 minutes each |
|
Outcomes | Outcomes were recorded at baseline, in the middle of the treatment, after 5 weeks of treatment and 10 weeks after baseline ARAT (0 to 57) Patient‐specific problem scale (0 to 100) | |
Notes | Significant differences in baseline characteristics: patients in the experimental group were younger |
Seok 2010
Methods | RCT | |
Participants | Country: South Korea Sample size: 40 participants (19 in mirror therapy group, 21 in control group) Inclusion criteria: stroke within 6 months Exclusion criteria: not able to understand treatment instructions; communication difficulties due to aphasia; MMSE < 15 points; musculoskeletal or neurological damage of the unaffected upperextremity; modified Ashworth Scale of 3 or more points; Brunnstrom stage of recovery (arm) of 1 or more than 5 points |
|
Interventions | 2 arms:
1 and 2: 5 days a week, 30 minutes of therapy for 4 weeks |
|
Outcomes | Outcomes were recorded at baseline and after 4 weeks of treatment
|
|
Notes | Published data only, extracted in part on the basis of an unauthorised, automatic translation of the original publication in Korean Significant difference in MFT between groups at baseline measurement |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Computer generated random number sequence |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not stated |
ITT analysis | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) primary outcome | Low risk | Assessors were blinded to group allocation |
Sütbeyaz 2007
Methods | RCT | |
Participants | Country: Turkey Sample size: 40 participants (20 in each group; 7 dropped out at 6 months follow‐up) Inclusion criteria: first unilateral stroke during previous 12 months; a score of 1 or 2 in the Brunnstrom stages of lower extremity; ambulatory before stroke Exclusion criteria: severe cognitive disorders |
|
Interventions | 2 arms:
1 and 2: 5 days a week, 30 minutes of therapy for 4 weeks |
|
Outcomes | Outcomes were recorded at baseline, after 4 weeks and after 6 months
|
|
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Block randomisation, computer generated allocation of blocks |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | The physicians who assessed potential participants to determine eligibility did not know to which group the participants would be allocated |
ITT analysis | High risk | Drop‐outs were not included in analysis |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) primary outcome | Low risk | Assessors were blinded to group allocation |
Tezuka 2006
Methods | Randomised cross‐over trial | |
Participants | Country: Japan Sample size: 15 participants (9 in mirror therapy group; 6 dropped out, 4 during the first interval) Inclusion criteria: patients admitted or planned to be admitted to rehabilitation ward on the hospital due to post‐stroke hemiparesis; within 1 month post‐stroke; informed consent was obtained from the patient and their family Exclusion criteria: higher brain dysfunction |
|
Interventions | 2 arms:
1 and 2: 10 to 15 minutes per day for 4 weeks, followed by 4 weeks vice versa |
|
Outcomes | Outcomes were recorded at baseline and after 4 weeks of therapy
|
|
Notes | We only analysed the first intervention period of 4 weeks | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Computer generated allocation to groups |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | High risk | Stated by authors (unpublished information) |
ITT analysis | High risk | Stated by authors (unpublished information) |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) primary outcome | Low risk | Assessors were blinded to group allocation |
Yavuzer 2008
Methods | RCT | |
Participants | Country: Turkey Sample size: 40 participants (20 in each group; 4 dropped out at 6 months follow‐up) Inclusion criteria: first unilateral stroke during previous 12 months; a Brunnstrom stage between 1 and 4 of the upper extremity; able to understand and follow simple instructions Exclusion criteria: severe cognitive disorders (MMSE < 24) |
|
Interventions | 2 arms:
1 and 2: 5 days a week, 30 minutes of therapy for 4 weeks |
|
Outcomes | Outcomes were recorded at baseline, after 4 weeks and after 6 months
|
|
Notes | We combined the Brunnstrom stages of upper extremity and hand into one item using raw data | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Block randomisation, computer generated allocation of blocks |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | The physicians who assessed potential participants to determine eligibility did not know to which group the participants would be allocated |
ITT analysis | High risk | Drop‐outs were not included in the analysis |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) primary outcome | Low risk | Assessors were blinded to group allocation |
Yun 2010
Methods | RCT | |
Participants | Country: South Korea Sample size: 60 participants (20 in each of the 3 groups) Drop‐outs during intervention period: 0 Inclusion criteria: first unilateral stroke; Brunnstrom stage I‐IV; MMSE > 21 Exclusion criteria: not stated |
|
Interventions | 3 arms:
1,2 and 3: 5 days a week, 30 minutes of therapy for 3 weeks |
|
Outcomes | Outcomes were recorded at baseline and after 3 weeks of treatment
|
|
Notes | Parts of the study were published on the World Stroke Congress 2010. Metaanalysis based on unpublished data We combined group 1 and 3 for analysis using raw data |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Random sampling number table as stated by authors (unpublished information) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | High risk | Stated by authors (unpublished information) |
ITT analysis | High risk | Stated by authors (unpublished information) |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) primary outcome | High risk | assessors not blinded, Stated by authors (unpublished information) |
ADL: activities of daily living ARAT: Action Research Arm Test BI: Barthel Index CRPS‐type 1: complex regional pain syndrome ‐ type I FAC: Functional Ambulatory Categories FIM: Functional Independence Measure FM: Fugl‐Meyer Assessment FM‐UE: Fugl‐Meyer Assessment upper extremity ITT: intention‐to‐treat MAL: Motor Activity Log MAS: Motor Assessment Scale MFT: Manual Function Test MMSE: Mini Mental State Examination MMT: Manual Muscle Test NMES: neuromuscular electrical stimulation NRS: numeric rating scale QOM: quality of movement QST: quantitative sensory testing RCT: randomised controlled trial ROM: range of motion SD: standard deviation TS: Tardieu Scale VAS: visual analogue scale WMFT: Wolf Motor Function Test WMFT/FA: Wolf Motor Function Test ‐ functional ability WMFT/PT: Wolf Motor Function Test ‐ performance time
Characteristics of excluded studies [ordered by study ID]
Study | Reason for exclusion |
---|---|
Adamovich 2009 | Study on healthy people |
Altschuler 2005 | Study on healthy people |
Dohle 2009b | Study did not use motor function as primary outcome |
Eng 2007 | Protocol. Study is not an RCT |
Ezendam 2009 | Review |
Gaggioli 2009 | Study is not an RCT |
Garry 2005 | Study on normal people |
Grünert‐Plüss 2008 | Study is not an RCT |
Hamzei 2009 | Review |
Johnson 1999 | Study is not an RCT |
Krause 2007 | Study was not finished |
Merians 2009 | Study is not an RCT |
Michielsen 2011b | Study is not an RCT |
Miltner 1998 | Study is not an RCT. Method of randomisation was not adequate |
Miltner 1999 | Copy of Miltner 1998 |
Miltner 2000 | Study is not an RCT |
Miltner 2001 | Study is not an RCT |
Morganti 2003 | Protocol. Study is not an RCT |
Moseley 2004 | Study did not include patients after stroke |
Ramachandran 1999 | Study is not an RCT |
Ramachandran 2009 | Review |
Rothgangel 2007 | Publication of the results of Rothgangel 2004 |
Sathian 2000 | Study is not an RCT |
Sathian 2009 | Editorial |
Shinoura 2008 | Study is not an RCT |
Stevens 2003 | Study is not an RCT |
Stevens 2004 | Study is not an RCT |
Wanschura 2010 | Randomised controlled study on healthy people |
Zhu 2009 | Study is not an RCT. Method of randomisation was not adequate |
RCT: randomised controlled trial
Characteristics of studies awaiting assessment [ordered by study ID]
Amimoto 2008
Methods | Randomised cross‐over trial |
Participants | Country: Japan Sample size: 14 participants Inclusion criteria: 4 months and longer after stroke |
Interventions | 2 arms:
|
Outcomes |
|
Notes | We were not able to include this trial because of unclear outcome of motor function |
Characteristics of ongoing studies [ordered by study ID]
Dheeraj 2010
Trial name or title | Mirror therapy in unilateral neglect after stoke ‐ MUST |
Methods | RCT |
Participants | Country: India Inclusion criteria: all stroke patients within 48 hours of onset with thalamic and parietal lobe lesions will be included |
Interventions | Setting: stroke unit and College of Physiotherapy 2 arms:
1 and 2: 20 sessions, 1 to 2 hours each, for 4 weeks |
Outcomes | Outcomes will be assessed before treatment, after 4 weeks and 2 months follow‐up
|
Starting date | |
Contact information | Principal investigator: Dheeraj K, Christian Medical College and Hospital, Physiotherapy, Brown Road, Ludhiana, India Tel: +91 161 2229011 21 |
Notes | Estimated enrolment: 48 in each group |
DRKS00000732
Trial name or title | Mirror therapy as group intervention after stroke: a randomised controlled trial |
Methods | RCT |
Participants | Country: Germany Inclusion criteria: first supratentorial stroke within the previous 3 months; aged between 18 and 80 years; clinically diagnosed severe hemiparesis or hemiplegia of the distal upper limb with Medical Research Council grading of 0 or 1 of wrist and finger extensors Exclusion criteria: visual impairments that may limit the participation in mirror therapy; severe cognitive and/or language deficits which preclude participants from following instructions in the group training protocol; other neurological or musculoskeletal impairments of the upper extremity not due to stroke; severe neglect (head is not turned to the affected side due to instruction) |
Interventions | 3 arms:
1, 2 and 3: 20 sessions, 30 minutes each during 5 weeks |
Outcomes | Outcomes will be assessed before and after treatment, and 7 months after treatment
|
Starting date | April 2009 |
Contact information | Principal investigator: Holm Thieme, Schule für Physiotherapie (School for Physiotherapy), Erste Europäische Schule für Physiotherapie, Ergotherapie und Logopädie, Klinik Bavaria Kreischa, Kreischa, Germany Tel: +49 35206 64240 Email: holm.thieme@physiotherapie‐schule‐kreischa.de |
Notes | Estimated enrolment: 66 |
Trial name or title | Use of tendon vibration and mirror for the improvement of upper limb function and pain reduction |
Methods | RCT |
Participants | Country: Israel Inclusion criteria: stroke; 18 to 85 years of age; stroke onset between 1 month and 1 year ago; NIHSS 3 to 15 on study admission; affected upper limb function 10% to 90% on Fugl‐Meyer Scale; ability to understand instructions and to move the unaffected limb freely Exclusion criteria: severe cognitive impairment; severe aphasia; severe neglect that impairs ability to understand instructions or to execute tasks |
Interventions | 3 arms:
1, 2 and 3: 10 sessions, 30 minutes each |
Outcomes | Outcomes will be assessed after treatment and 3 months after treatment
|
Starting date | September 2009 |
Contact information | Principal Investigator: Elior Moreh, MD, Hadassah University Hospital, Jerusalem, Israel Email: elior@hadassah.org.il |
Notes | Estimated enrolment: 30 |
Thomas 2010
Trial name or title | Mirror arm exercises for stroke |
Methods | RCT |
Participants | Country: United Kingdom Inclusion criteria: first time stroke at least 1 week previously and inpatient in a stroke rehabilitation unit; no premorbid conditions limiting upper limb function; sufficient cognitive and communication skills to give consent (as judged by the clinical team); medically stable and able to participate in rehabilitation (as judged by the clinical team); upper limb weakness which limits activity (Motricity Index Upper Limb score less than 99) Exclusion criteria: unable to consent; not a first time stroke; previous condition limiting upper limb function; unable to participate in rehabilitation; no upper limb weakness |
Interventions | 2 arms:
1 and 2: 30 minutes a day for 4 weeks |
Outcomes | Outcomes will be assessed at baseline, immediately after the trial and 1 month after the trial
|
Starting date | 3 January 2011 |
Contact information | Principal Investigator: Miss Nessa Thomas, University of Salford, Centre for Rehabilitation and Human Performance Research, Allerton Building, Frederick Road, Salford, UK Email: n.thomas@salford.ac.uk |
Notes | Estimated enrolment: 83 |
ARAT: Action Resarch Arm Test FIM: Functional Independence Measure NIHSS: National Institutes of Health Stroke Scales RCT: randomised controlled trial
Contributions of authors
Holm Thieme (HT) and Christian Dohle (CD) were involved in all stages of the review and contributed to the conception and design of the review. Jan Mehrholz (JM) was involved in methodological planning and conducting the review, statistical analysis of outcome data and interpreting the results. Johann Behrens (JB) and Marcus Pohl (MP) were involved in extracting data, assessing the methodological quality of selected studies and interpreting the results. All authors approved the protocol and the final review.
Sources of support
Internal sources
Erste Europäische Schule für Physiotherapie, Ergotherapie und Logopädie, Klinik Bavaria Kreischa, Germany.
Klinik Bavaria Kreischa, Germany.
SRH Hochschule Gera, Germany.
Martin‐Luther‐Universität Halle‐Wittenberg, Germany, Not specified.
Median Klinik Berlin‐Kladow, Germany.
External sources
-
BMBF, Germany.
The research is funded by the Bundesministerium für Bildung, und Forschung (01KG1025).
Declarations of interest
Holm Thieme (HT) is principal investigator of an ongoing trial that may be relevant for the topic of this review. He has received and will receive honorarium for presentations and seminars on mirror therapy.
Christian Dohle (CD) is first author of one of the included studies on the effect of mirror therapy after stroke. He was not involved in checking this trial for eligibility, extracting data and assessing the methodological quality of the study. He has received and will receive honorarium for presentations and seminars on mirror therapy and is co‐author of a corresponding therapy manual (Nakaten 2009).
New
References
References to studies included in this review
- Acerra NE. Is early post‐stroke upper limb mirror therapy associated with improved sensation and motor recovery? A randomised‐controlled trial [PhD thesis] In: Sensorimotor Dysfunction in CRPS1 and Stroke: Characteristics, Prediction and Intervention. Brisbane, Australia: University of Queensland, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Altschuler EL, Wisdom SB, Stone L, Foster C, Galasko D, Llewellyn DM, et al. Rehabilitation of hemiparesis after stroke with a mirror. Lancet 1999;353(9169):2035‐6. [PUBMED: 10376620] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cacchio A, Blasis E, Blasis V, Santilli V, Spacca G. Mirror therapy in complex regional pain syndrome type 1 of the upper limb in stroke patients. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 2009;23(8):792‐9. [PUBMED: 19465507] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cacchio A, Blasis E, Necozione S, Orio F, Santilli V. Mirror therapy for chronic complex regional pain syndrome type 1 and stroke. New England Journal of Medicine2009; Vol. 361, issue 6:634‐6. [PUBMED: 19657134] [DOI] [PubMed]
- Dohle C, Pullen J, Nakaten A, Kust J, Rietz C, Karbe H. Mirror therapy promotes recovery from severe hemiparesis: a randomized controlled trial. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 2009;23(3):209‐17. [PUBMED: 19074686] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ietswaart M, Johnston M, Dijkerman HC, Joice S, Scott CL, MacWalter RS, et al. Mental practice with motor imagery in stroke recovery: randomized controlled trial of efficacy. Brain 2011;134:1373‐86. [PUBMED: 21515905] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manton JC, Hanson C. The effects of a new treatment for survivors of stroke six months or more post‐cerebrovascular accident. Physical Therapy 2002;82(5):(Abstract PL‐RR‐142‐F). [Google Scholar]
- Michielsen ME, Selles RW, Geest JN, Eckhardt M, Yavuzer G, Stam HJ, et al. Motor recovery and cortical reorganisation after mirror therapy in chronic stroke patients: a phase II randomized controlled trial. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 2011;25(3):223‐33. [DOI: 10.1177/1545968310385127; PUBMED: 21051765] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rothgangel AS, Morton AR, Hout JWE, Beurskens AJHM. Phantoms in the brain: mirror therapy in chronic stroke patients; a pilot study. Nederlands Tijdschrift voor Fysiotherapie 2004;114(2):36‐40. [ISSN: 0377‐208X] [Google Scholar]
- Rothgangel AS, Morton AR, Hout JWE, Beurskens AJHM. Phantoms in the brain: mirror therapy in chronic stroke patients; a pilot study. Nederlands Tijdschrift voor Fysiotherapie 2004;114(2):36‐40. [Google Scholar]
- Rothgangel AS, Morton AR, Hout JWE, Beurskens AJHM. Phantoms in the brain: mirror therapy in chronic stroke patients; a pilot study. Nederlands Tijdschrift voor Fysiotherapie 2004;114(2):36‐40. [Google Scholar]
- Seok H, Kim SH, Jang YW, Lee JB, Kim SW. Effect of mirror therapy on recovery of upper limb function and strength in subacute hemiplegia after stroke. Journal of Korean Academy of Rehabilitation Medicine 2010;34:508‐12. [Google Scholar]
- Sutbeyaz S, Yavuzer G, Sezer N, Koseoglu BF. Mirror therapy enhances lower‐extremity motor recovery and motor functioning after stroke: a randomized controlled trial. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 2007;88(5):555‐9. [PUBMED: 17466722] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tezuka Y, Fujiwara M, Kikuchi K, Ogawa S, Tokunaga N, Ichikawa A, et al. Effect of mirror therapy for patients with post‐stroke paralysis of upper limb: randomized cross‐over study. Journal of Japanese Physical Therapy Association 2006;33(2):62‐8. [Google Scholar]
- Yavuzer G, Selles R, Sezer N, Sutbeyaz S, Bussmann JB, Koseoglu F, et al. Mirror therapy improves hand function in subacute stroke: a randomized controlled trial. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 2008;89(3):393‐8. [PUBMED: 18295613] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yun G, Chun M‐H. Mirror therapy and NMES for hand rehabilitation in stroke patients. International Journal of Stroke 2010;5 Suppl 2:309‐10. [Google Scholar]
References to studies excluded from this review
- Adamovich SV, August K, Merians A, Tunik E. A virtual reality‐based system integrated with fmri to study neural mechanisms of action observation‐execution: a proof of concept study. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience 2009;27(3):209‐23. [PUBMED: 19531876] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Altschuler EL. Interaction of vision and movement via a mirror. Perception 2005;34:1153‐5. [PUBMED: 16245491] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dohle C, Kaick S, Görtner H, Schnellenbach I. [Kombination von funktioneller Elektrostimulation und Spiegeltherapie]. Gemeinsame Jahrestagung der DGNR/DGNKN. Berlin, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Eng K, Siekierka E, Pyk P, Chevrier E, Hauser Y, Cameirao M, et al. Interactive visuo‐motor therapy system for stroke rehabilitation. Medical and Biological Engineering and Computing 2007;45(9):901‐7. [PUBMED: 17687578] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ezendam D, Bongers RM, Jannink MJA. Systematic review of the effectiveness of mirror therapy in upper extremity function. Disability and Rehabilitation 2009;31:2135‐49. [PUBMED: 19903124] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaggioli A, Morganti F, Meneghini A, Pozzato I, Greggio G, Pigatto M, et al. Computer‐guided mental practice in neurorehabilitation. Studies in Health Technology and Informatics 2009;145:195‐208. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garry MI, Loftus A, Summers JJ. Mirror, mirror on the wall: viewing a mirror reflection of unilateral hand movements facilitates ipsilateral M1 excitability. Experimental Brain Research 2005;163(1):118‐22. [PUBMED: 15754176] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grünert‐Plüss N, Hufschmid U, Santschi L, Grünert J. Mirror therapy in hand rehabilitation: a review of the literature, the St Gallen protocol for mirror therapy and evaluation of a case series of 52 patients. British Journal of Hand Therapy 2008;13:4‐11. [ISSN: 1369‐9571] [Google Scholar]
- Hamzei F. Why mirror therapy?. Neurologie und Rehabilitation 2009;15(4):249‐51. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson K. Mirror therapy helps stroke patients recover: motion can be relearned when damaged limb mimics good one. Medical Tribune 1999;40:6. [Google Scholar]
- Krause H, Stephan KM, Dohle C, Dahncke O, Hömberg V. [EMG‐getriggerte Elektrostimulation in Kombination mit Spiegeltherapie zur Förderung der zerebralen Plastizität]. Gemeinsame Jahrestagung der DGNKN/DGNR. 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Merians AS, Tunik E, Fluet GG, Qiu Q, Adamovich SV. Innovative approaches to the rehabilitation of upper extremity hemiparesis using virtual environments. European Journal of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine 2009;45(1):123‐33. [PUBMED: 19158659] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michielsen ME, Smits M, Ribbers GM, Stam HJ, Bussmann JB, Selles RW. The neuronal correlates of mirror therapy: an fMRI study on mirror induced visual illusions in patients with stroke. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 2011;82(4):393‐8. [PUBMED: 20861065] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miltner R, Simon U, Netz J, Hömberg V. Motor imagery in the therapy of patients with central motor deficit [Bewegungsvorstellung in der Therapie von Patienten mit Hirninfarkt]. Funktionelle Bildgebung und Physiotherapie. Bad Honnef: Hippocampus Verlag, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Miltner R, Simon U, Netz J, Homberg V. Motor imagery in the therapy of patients with central motor deficit [Bewegungsvorstellung in der Therapie von Patienten mit Hirninfarkt]. Neurologie und Rehabilitation 1999;5(2):66‐72. [Google Scholar]
- Miltner R, Netz J, Hömber V. Cognitive therapy in sensorimotor disorders [Kognitive Therapie sensomotorischer Störungen]. Krankengymnastik 2000;6:954‐64. [Google Scholar]
- Miltner R. Cognitive therapy of sensorymotor dysfunction. Ergotherapie und Rehabilitation 2001;40(8):27‐9. [ISSN: 0942‐8623] [Google Scholar]
- Morganti F, Gaggioli A, Castelnuovo G, Bulla D, Vettorello M, Riva G. The use of technology‐supported mental imagery in neurological rehabilitation: a research protocol. Cyberpsychology and Behavior 2003;6(4):421‐7. [PUBMED: 14511455] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moseley GL. Graded motor imagery is effective for long‐standing complex regional pain syndrome: a randomised controlled trial. Pain 2004;108:192‐8. [PUBMED: 15109523] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramachandran VS, Altschuler EL, Stone L, Al‐Aboudi M, Schwartz E, Siva N. Can mirrors alleviate visual hemineglect?. Medical Hypotheses 1999;52(4):303‐5. [PUBMED: 10465667 ] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramachandran VS, Altschuler EL. The use of visual feedback, in particular mirror visual feedback, in restoring brain function. Brain 2009;132:1693‐710. [PUBMED: 19506071] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rothgangel A, Morton A, Hout JWE, Beurskens AJHM. Mirror therapy in the rehabilitation after stroke: effectiveness on upper limb functioning in chronic stroke patients [Spiegeltherapie in der Neurologischen Rehabilitation: Effektivität in Bezug auf die Arm‐ und Handfunktionen bei chronischen Schlaganfallpatienten]. Neurologie and Rehabilitation 2007;13(5):271‐6. [Google Scholar]
- Sathian K, Greenspan AI, Wolf SL. Doing it with mirrors: a case study of a novel approach to neurorehabilitation. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 2000;14(1):73‐6. [PUBMED: 11228952] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sathian K. Mirror, mirror, move my manu!. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 2009;23(3):207‐8. [PUBMED: 19240198] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shinoura N, Suzuki Y, Watanabe Y, Yamada R, Tabei Y, Saito K, et al. Mirror therapy activates outside of cerebellum and ipsilateral M1. NeuroRehabilitation 2008;23(3):245‐52. [PUBMED: 18560141] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens JA, Stoykov ME. Using motor imagery in the rehabilitation of hemiparesis. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 2003;84(7):1090‐2. [PUBMED: 12881842] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens JA, Stoykov ME. Simulation of bilateral movement training through mirror reflection: a case report demonstrating an occupational therapy technique for hemiparesis. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation 2004;11(1):59‐66. [PUBMED: 14872400] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wanschura JE. Combined behavioral and fMRI‐study to research cortical effects of a mirror therapy (Kombinierte Verhaltens‐ und fMRT‐ Studie zur Untersuchung kortikaler Effekte einer Spiegeltherapie) [PhD thesis]. Freiburg, Germany: Albert Ludwigs University, 2010. [http://www.freidok.uni‐freiburg.de/volltexte/7600/] [Google Scholar]
- Zhu L, Jia XH, Liu L, Wang SY, Yang SR, Song WQ. Efficacy of movement imagination on rehabilitation of hand function in patients with post‐stroke hemiplegia. Chinese Journal of Cerebrovascular Diseases 2009;6(9):451‐5. [Google Scholar]
References to studies awaiting assessment
- Amimoto K, Matsuda T, Watanabe S. The effect of mirror therapy on the lower limb function of chronic hemiplegic patients. International Journal of Stroke 2008;3 Suppl 1:336‐7 (Abstract PO02‐274). [Google Scholar]
References to ongoing studies
- Dheeraj KV, Arora R, Pandian JD. Therapy in unilateral neglect after stoke ‐ MUST. International Journal of Stroke 2010;5 Suppl 2:288. [Google Scholar]
- DRKS00000732. Mirror therapy as group intervention after stroke ‐ a randomised controlled trial [Spiegeltherapie als Gruppenintervention nach einem Schlaganfall ‐ eine randomisierte kontrollierte Studie]. www.germanctr.de (accessed 6 February 2011).
- NCT01010607. Use of tendon vibration and mirror for the improvement of upper limb function and pain reduction. clinicaltrials.gov/show/NCT01010607 (accessed 6 February 2011).
- Thomas N. Mirror arm exercises for stroke. http://www.controlled‐trials.com (accessed 6 February 2011).
Additional references
- Barker WH, Mullooly JP. Stroke in a defined elderly population, 1967‐1985. A less lethal and disabling but no less common disease. Stroke 1997;28(2):284‐90. [PUBMED: 9040676] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruehl S, Harden RN, Galer BS, Saltz S, Bertram M, Backonja M, et al. External validation of IASP diagnostic criteria for Complex Regional Pain Syndrome and proposed research diagnostic criteria. International Association for the Study of Pain. Pain 1999;81(1‐2):147‐54. [PUBMED: 10353502 ] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brunnstrom S. Motor testing procedures in hemiplegia: based on sequential recovery stages. Physical Therapy 1966;46:357‐75. [PUBMED: 590725] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carr J, Shepherd R. Investigation of a new Motor Assessment Scale. Physical Therapy 1985;65:175‐80. [PUBMED: 3969398] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collen FM, Wade DT, Robb GF, Bradshaw CM. The Rivermead Mobility Index: a further development of the Rivermead Motor Assessment. International Disability Studies 1991;13(2):50‐4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demeurisse G, Demol O, Robaye E. Motor evaluation in vascular hemiplegia. European Neurology 1980;19:382‐9. [PUBMED: 7439211] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dohle C, Kleiser R, Seitz RJ, Freund HJ. Body scheme gates visual processing. Journal of Neurophysiology 2004;91(5):2376‐9. [PUBMED: 14681333] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dohle C, Nakaten A, Püllen J, Rietz C, Karbe H. Basic mechanisms and application of mirror training [Grundlagen und Anwendung des Spiegeltrainings]. In: Minkwitz K, Scholz E editor(s). Standardisierte Therapieverfahren und Grundlagen des Lernens in der Neurologie. Idstein, Germany: Schulz‐Kirchner‐Verlag, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Dohle C, Stephan KM, Valvoda JT, Hosseiny O, Tellmann L, Kuhlen T, et al. Representation of virtual arm movements in precuneus. Experimental Brain Research 2011;208(4):543‐55. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-010-2503-0] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farnè A, Buxbaum LJ, Ferraro M, Frassinetti F, Whyte J, Veramonti T, et al. Patterns of spontaneous recovery of neglect and associated disorders in acute right brain‐damaged patients. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 2004;75(10):1401‐10. [PUBMED: 15377685] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feys H, Weerdt W, Verbeke G, Steck GC, Capiau C, Kiekens C, et al. Early and repetitive stimulation of the arm can substantially improve the long‐term outcome after stroke: a 5‐year follow‐up study of a randomized trial. Stroke 2004;35(4):924‐9. [PUBMED: 15001789] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franceschini M, Porta F, Agosti M, Massucci M, ICR2 group. Is health‐related quality of life of stroke patients influenced by neurological impairments at one year after stroke?. European Journal of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine 2010;46(3):389‐99. [PUBMED: 20927005] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franck N, Farrer C, Georgieff N, Marie‐Cardine M, Dalery J, d'Amato T, et al. Defective recognition of one's own actions in patients with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 2001;158(3):454‐9. [PUBMED: 11229988] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- French B, Thomas LH, Leathley MJ, Sutton CJ, McAdam J, Forster A, et al. Repetitive task training for improving functional ability after stroke. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2007, Issue 4. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD006073.pub2] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fugl‐Meyer AR, Jaasko L, Leyman IL, Olsson S, Steglind S. The post‐stroke hemiplegic patient. I. A method for evaluation of physical performance. Scandinavian Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine 1975;7:13‐31. [PUBMED: 1135616] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fukumura K, Sugawara K, Tanabe S, Ushiba J, Tomita Y. Influence of mirror therapy on human motor cortex. International Journal of Neuroscience 2007;117(7):1039‐48. [PUBMED: 17613113] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaggioli A, Morganti F, Walker R, Meneghini A, Alcaniz M, Lozano JA, et al. Training with computer‐supported motor imagery in post‐stroke rehabilitation. Cyberpsychology and Behavior 2004;7(3):327‐32. [PUBMED: 15257833] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grèzes J, Decety J. Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: a meta‐analysis. Human Brain Mapping 2001;12(1):1‐19. [PUBMED: 11198101 ] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hendricks HT, Limbeek J, Geurts AC, Zwarts MJ. Motor recovery after stroke: a systematic review of the literature. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 2002;83(11):1629‐37. [PUBMED: 12422337] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgins JPT, Altman DG, Sterne JAC (editors). Chapter 8: Assessing risk of bias in included studies. In: Higgins JPT, Green S (editors). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0 [updated March 2011]. The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011. Available from www.cochrane‐handbook.org.
- Jorgensen HS, Nakayama H, Raaschou HO, Olsen TS. Recovery of walking function in stroke patients: the Copenhagen Stroke Study. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 1995;76(1):27‐32. [PUBMED: 7811170] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jönsson AC, Lindgren I, Hallström B, Norrving B, Lindgren A. Prevalence and intensity of pain after stroke: a population based study focusing on patients' perspectives. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 2006;77(5):590‐5. [PUBMED: 16354737] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katz N, Hartman‐Maeir A, Ring H, Soroker N. Functional disability and rehabilitation outcome in right hemisphere damaged patients with and without unilateral spatial neglect. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 1999;80(4):379‐84. [PUBMED: 10206598] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keith RA, Granger CV, Hamilton BB, Sherwin FS. The functional independence measure: a new tool for rehabilitation. Advances in Clinical Rehabilitation 1987;1:6‐18. [PUBMED: 3503663] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kocabas H, Levendoglu F, Ozerbil OM, Yuruten B. Complex regional pain syndrome in stroke patients. International Journal of Rehabilitation Research 2007;30(1):33‐8. [PUBMED: 17293718] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kwakkel G, Kollen BJ, Grond J, Prevo AJ. Probability of regaining dexterity in the flaccid upper limb: impact of severity of paresis and time since onset in acute stroke. Stroke 2003;34(9):2181‐6. [PUBMED: 12907818] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liepert J, Tegenthoff M, Malin JP. Changes of cortical motor area size during immobilization. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 1995;97(6):382‐6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liepert J, Miltner WH, Bauder H, Sommer M, Dettmers C, Taub E, et al. Motor cortex plasticity during constraint‐induced movement therapy in stroke patients. Neuroscience Letters 1998;250(1):5‐8. [PUBMED: 9696052] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lindgren I, Jönsson AC, Norrving B, Lindgren A. Shoulder pain after stroke: a prospective population‐based study. Stroke 2007;38(2):343‐8. [PUBMED: 17185637] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Longo MR, Haggard P. Sense of agency primes manual motor responses. Perception 2009;38(1):69‐78. [PUBMED: 19323137] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lundström E, Smits A, Terént A, Borg J. Risk factors for stroke‐related pain 1 year after first‐ever stroke. European Journal of Neurology 2009;16(2):188‐93. [PUBMED: 19138338] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyle RC. A performance test for assessment of upper limb function in physical rehabilitation treatment and research. International Journal of Rehabilitation Research 1981;4:483‐92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maher CG, Sherrington C, Herbert RD, Moseley AM, Elkins M. Reliability of the PEDro scale for rating quality of randomized controlled trials. Physical Therapy 2003;83(8):713‐21. [PUBMED: 12882612] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mahoney FI, Barthel DW. Functional evaluation: the Barthel Index. Maryland State Medical Journal 1965;14:61‐5. [PUBMED: 14258950] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthys K, Smits M, Geest JN, Lugt A, Seurinck R, Stam HJ, et al. Mirror‐induced visual illusion of hand movements: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 2009;90(4):675‐81. [PUBMED: 19345786] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCabe CS, Haigh RC, Ring EF, Halligan PW, Wall PD, Blake DR. A controlled pilot study of the utility of mirror visual feedback in the treatment of complex regional pain syndrome (type 1). Rheumatology 2003;42(1):97‐101. [PUBMED: 12509620] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehrholz J, Werner C, Kugler J, Pohl M. Electromechanical‐assisted training for walking after stroke. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2007, Issue 4. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD006185.pub2] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehrholz J, Platz T, Kugler J, Pohl M. Electromechanical and robot‐assisted arm training for improving arm function and activities of daily living after stroke. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2008, Issue 4. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD006876.pub2] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miltner WH, Bauder H, Sommer M, Dettmers C, Taub E. Effects of constraint‐induced movement therapy on patients with chronic motor deficits after stroke: a replication. Stroke 1999;30(3):586‐92. [PUBMED: 10066856] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakaten A, Govers J, Dohle C. Spiegeltherapie in der Neurorehabilitation. Idstein, Germany: Schulz‐Kirchner‐Verlag, 2009. [ISBN‐13: 978‐3824806348] [Google Scholar]
- Nakayama H, Jorgensen HS, Raaschou HO, Olsen TS. Recovery of upper extremity function in stroke patients: the Copenhagen Stroke Study. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 1994;75(4):394‐8. [PUBMED: 8172497] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Platz T, Eickhof C, Kaick S, Engel U, Pinkowski C, Kalok S, et al. Impairment‐oriented training or Bobath therapy for severe arm paresis after stroke: a single‐blind, multicentre randomized controlled trial. Clinical Rehabilitation 2005;19(7):714‐24. [PUBMED: 16250190] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramachandran VS. Phantom limbs, neglect syndromes, repressed memory, and Freudian psychology. International Review of Neurobiology 1994;37:291‐333. [PUBMED: 7883483 ] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramachandran VS, Rogers‐Ramachandran D, Cobb S. Touching the phantom limb. Nature 1995;377(6549):489‐90. [PUBMED: 7566144 ] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramachandran VS, Rogers‐Ramachandran D. Synaesthesia in phantom limbs induced with mirrors. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 1996;263(1369):377‐86. [PUBMED: 8637922] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- The Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration. Review Manager (RevMan). Version 5.1. Copenhagen: The Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011.
- Ringman JM, Saver JL, Woolson RF, Clarke WR, Adams HP. Frequency, risk factors, anatomy, and course of unilateral neglect in an acute stroke cohort. Neurology 2004;63(3):468‐74. [PUBMED: 15304577] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rothgangel AS, Braun SM, Beurskens AJ, Seitz RJ, Wade DT. The clinical aspects of mirror therapy in rehabilitation: a systematic review of the literature. International Journal of Rehabilitation Research 2011;34(1):1‐13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sackley C, Brittle N, Patel S, Ellins J, Scott M, Wright C, et al. The prevalence of joint contractures, pressure sores, painful shoulder, other pain, falls, and depression in the year after a severely disabling stroke. Stroke 2008;39(12):3329‐34. [PUBMED: 18787199] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taub E, Miller NE, Novack TA, Cook EW 3rd, Fleming WC, Nepomuceno CS, et al. Technique to improve chronic motor deficit after stroke. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 1993;74(4):347‐54. [PUBMED: 8466415] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Urton ML, Kohia M, Davis J, Neill MR. Systematic literature review of treatment interventions for upper extremity hemiparesis following stroke. Occupational Therapy International 2007;14(1):11‐27. [PUBMED: 17623376] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peppen RP, Kwakkel G, Wood‐Dauphinee S, Hendriks HJ, Wees PJ, Dekker J. The impact of physical therapy on functional outcomes after stroke: what's the evidence?. Clinical Rehabilitation 2004;18(8):833‐62. [PUBMED: 15609840] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization. The global burden of disease: 2004 update. http://www.who.int/healthinfo/global_burden_disease/2004_report_update/en/index.html.
- Wolf SL, Catlin PA, Ellis M, Archer AL, Morgan B, Piacentino A. Assessing Wolf Motor Function Test as outcome measure for research in patients after stroke. Stroke 2001;32:1635‐9. [PUBMED: 11441212] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
References to other published versions of this review
- Thieme H, Mehrholz J, Pohl M, Dohle C. Mirror therapy for improving motor function after stroke. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2010, Issue 4. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD008449] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]