Abstract
High-trait anxiety is a risk factor for the development of affective disorders and has been associated with decreased cardiovascular and behavioral responsivity to acute stressors in humans that may increase the risk of developing cardiovascular disease. Although human neuroimaging studies of high-trait anxiety reveals dysregulation in primate cingulate areas 25 and 32 and the anterior hippocampus (aHipp) and rodent studies reveal the importance of aHipp glutamatergic hypofunction, the causal involvement of aHipp glutamate and its interaction with these areas in the primate brain is unknown. Accordingly, we correlated marmoset trait anxiety scores to their postmortem aHipp glutamate levels and showed that low glutamate in the right aHipp is associated with high-trait anxiety in marmosets. Moreover, pharmacologically increasing aHipp glutamate reduced anxiety levels in highly anxious marmosets in two uncertainty-based tests of anxiety: exposure to a human intruder with uncertain intent and unpredictable loud noise. In the human intruder test, increasing aHipp glutamate decreased anxiety by increasing approach to the intruder. In the unpredictable threat test, animals showed blunted behavioral and cardiovascular responsivity after control infusions, which was normalized by increasing aHipp glutamate. However, this aHipp-mediated anxiolytic effect was blocked by simultaneous pharmacological inactivation of area 25, but not area 32, areas which when inactivated independently reduced and had no effect on anxiety, respectively. These findings provide causal evidence in male and female primates that aHipp glutamatergic hypofunction and its regulation by area 25 contribute to the behavioral and cardiovascular symptoms of endogenous high-trait anxiety.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT High-trait anxiety predisposes sufferers to the development of anxiety and depression. Although neuroimaging of these disorders and rodent modeling implicate dysregulation in hippocampal glutamate and the subgenual/perigenual cingulate cortices (areas 25/32), the causal involvement of these structures in endogenous high-trait anxiety and their interaction are unknown. Here, we demonstrate that increased trait anxiety in marmoset monkeys correlates with reduced hippocampal glutamate and that increasing hippocampal glutamate release in high-trait-anxious monkeys normalizes the aberrant behavioral and cardiovascular responsivity to potential threats. This normalization was blocked by simultaneous inactivation of area 25, but not area 32. These findings provide casual evidence in primates that hippocampal glutamatergic hypofunction regulates endogenous high-trait anxiety and the hippocampal–area 25 circuit is a potential therapeutic target.
Keywords: anxiety, area 25, cardiovascular, glutamate, hippocampus, marmoset
Introduction
Trait anxiety refers to stable individual differences in how individuals respond to acutely stressful or threatening situations. High-trait-anxious individuals are predisposed to developing anxiety and depression (Weger and Sandi, 2018), are more likely to interpret ambiguous stimuli as threatening (Mathews et al., 1989), and show reduced motivation (Ginty et al., 2015). Physiologically, increased anxiety is normally associated with increases in the autonomic indices of fear in response to fearful stimuli. However, a principal diagnosis in the domain of “anxiety” covers a wide spectrum of anxiety-related and comorbid disorders and, although enhanced fear physiology is seen in specific phobia patients, there is increasing evidence that high chronic (trait) levels of anxiety and negative affect are associated with blunted cardiovascular responsivity and a failure to show the adaptive cardiovascular changes that characterize a healthy “fight-or-flight” response (Ginty and Conklin, 2011; Brindle et al., 2013; Lang et al., 2014, 2016; Souza et al., 2015). Moreover, the cardiovascular function of high-trait-anxious and negative individuals does not discriminate between acutely stressful and nonstressful conditions (Carroll et al., 2017), an adaptive failure that may contribute to the increased incidence of cardiovascular disease in patients suffering from depression and anxiety (Stapelberg et al., 2011, 2012).
Numerous models of high-trait anxiety have been developed to investigate the neurobiology of these disorders. One such model of rodent prenatal stress links a high-trait/depressive phenotype in the offspring with decreased depolarization-evoked glutamate release in the ventral hippocampus (vHipp) (Marrocco et al., 2012, 2014). Although the relevance of such models to endogenous high-trait anxiety is uncertain, early life stress is a known risk factor for affective disorders and there is growing evidence that reduced hippocampal glutamatergic function contributes to affective disorder pathophysiology. Certainly, hippocampal glutamatergic hypofunction correlates with depressive illness duration (Block et al., 2009; de Diego-Adeliño et al., 2013) and disrupted glutamatergic signaling pathways are associated with anxious/depressive behaviors in mice and humans (Tordera et al., 2007; Garcia-Garcia et al., 2009; Duric et al., 2013). However, these findings conflict with studies of nonanxious rodents in which reductions in vHipp glutamate function are anxiolytic in ethological tests of anxiety (Barkus et al., 2010) and the anxiolytic effects of hippocampal lesions in nonanxious primates (Chudasama et al., 2008, 2009). Therefore, there may be fundamental differences in how hippocampal glutamate contributes to healthy and high-trait-anxious/affective disorder states. The hippocampus modulates such behavior, in part, via a network of medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) brain regions including Brodmann's area 25 and area 32 and changes in anterior hippocampal (aHipp, the primate equivalent of the vHipp) connectivity with the mPFC are implicated in affective disorders in humans (Dickie et al., 2011; Hamilton et al., 2011; Treadway et al., 2015). Rodent studies also highlight the importance of vHipp–mPFC connectivity in the control of anxiety-related behavior (Adhikari et al., 2010; Padilla-Coreano et al., 2016). However, the contributions of hippocampal glutamate and hippocampal–mPFC circuitry in the behavioral and cardiovascular symptoms of endogenous high-trait anxiety remain unknown.
We have recently demonstrated the utility of a marmoset model for probing the behavioral and cardiovascular correlates of mPFC function and conditioned fear (Wallis et al., 2017) and described a subpopulation of marmosets with an endogenous high-trait anxiety phenotype that is associated with impaired discriminative Pavlovian conditioning, blunted amygdala serotonin function, and reduced dorsal anterior cingulate cortex volume (Shiba et al., 2014; Mikheenko et al., 2015). However, the contribution of hippocampal glutamate to this phenotype is unknown. Therefore, the first aim of the present study (Experiment 1) was to assess whether reduced postmortem hippocampal glutamate levels predicted heightened responsivity of a marmoset cohort to an unknown human. Having established a negative correlation between hippocampal glutamate and such responsivity, the second aim (Experiment 2) was to determine whether hippocampal glutamate and high-trait anxiety were causally related. Therefore, we used anatomically specific intracerebral infusions to increase presynaptic hippocampal glutamate release in high-trait-anxious monkeys and assessed its effectiveness in ameliorating the behavioral and cardiovascular correlates of negative emotion in two anxiety tests, the human intruder paradigm, which measures uncertainty-based behavior in the home cage, and an unpredictable threat test presented in an automated test apparatus, which measures real-time behavioral and cardiovascular responses. We then determined whether this amelioration used hippocampal–mPFC circuitry.
Materials and Methods
Animals
All of the marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) used in these studies were bred onsite at the University of Cambridge Marmoset Breeding Colony and were housed in male/female pairs (males were vasectomized). They were kept in a 12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 7:00 A.M., lights off at 7:00 P.M.) in a controlled environment of 22 ± 1°C and 50 ± 1% humidity. Their cages contained a variety of environmental enrichment aids including boxes to play in and suspended ladders, wooden branches, and ropes to climb and swing on. Animals were fed a varied diet including fruit, rusk, malt loaf, peanuts, eggs, sandwiches, and weekend treats and had ad libitum access to water. All procedures were performed in accordance with the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act of 1986 and the University of Cambridge Animal Welfare and Ethical Review Board.
Experiment 1
Hippocampal neurochemical quantification
To assess the contribution of hippocampal neurochemistry to the high-trait-anxious phenotype, stored hippocampal tissue from 12 marmosets (six males, six females) with a range of anxiety scores on the human intruder test (described below) was analyzed. Behavioral and cardiovascular data from these marmosets have previously been described previously (Shiba et al., 2014). After euthanasia, the brain was removed and the anterior two-thirds of the left and right hippocampus was visualized, immediately dissected out, and flash frozen into liquid nitrogen before storage at −80°C. For analysis, these tissue samples were thawed on ice and homogenized with 500 μl of Lysis Buffer NL from the Qiagen Qproteome Nuclear Protein Kit before 250 μl of this sample was analyzed using reverse-phase high-performance liquid chromatography (HPLC) and electrochemical detection for both monoamines and glutamate as described previously (Tsai et al., 1997; Clarke et al., 2004). Briefly, monoamines were separated on a C18 silica-based analytical column (10 × 4.6 mm ODS3; Hypersil, Phenomoex) using a mobile phase consisting of 13.6 g/L KH2PO4.H2O, 185 mg/L octane sulfonic acid, and 18% methanol, pH 2.75, delivered at 0.8 ml/min by a dual piston pump. Detection was achieved electrochemically using a dual electrode analytical cell and electrochemical detector with electrode 1 set at −150 mV and electrode 2 set at 180 mV with reference to a palladium reference electrode. For the measurement of glutamate, 6 μl of each sample was reacted with an equal volume of a derivatizing agent containing o-phthalaldehyde and β-mercaptoethanol for 2 min at room temperature. The derivatizing solution was prepared daily by diluting 1 ml of stock solution containing 27.5 mg o-phthalaldehyde, 1 ml of methanol, 5 μl of β-mercaptoethanol, and 9 ml of 0.4 m potassium tetraborate buffer, pH 10.4, in 3 ml of potassium tetraborate buffer. Glutamate was detected by HPLC and fluorescence detection (CMA 280) with excitation and emission wavelengths of 315–370 nm and 395–545 nm, respectively. Separation was achieved at 24°C using a Hypersil ODS 5 μm analytical column (80 × 4.6 mm; HPLC Technology) and a mobile phase consisting of 100 mm Na2HPO4 and 30% methanol, adjusted to pH 6.38 with orthophosphoric acid, which was delivered at 1 ml/min. Before use, the buffer was filtered through a 0.2 μm filter under vacuum. The HPLC system was calibrated using standards containing known amounts of monoamines and glutamate. Data were acquired online and the signals were integrated using Chromeleon software version 6.2 (Dionex). The resulting monoamine and glutamate levels of each animal were then compared with their anxiety scores.
Experiment 2
To determine whether increases in hippocampal glutamate activity could ameliorate the behavioral and cardiovascular responses associated with high-trait anxiety, we performed two behavioral experiments on marmosets that had been characterized as high anxious (described below). In Experiment 2a, we investigated the effects of selectively increasing aHipp glutamate levels on behavioral performance in the human intruder test in six animals (three males, three females). In Experiment 2b, in six animals (four of which also contributed to Experiment 2a; two females, four males), we investigated the behavioral and cardiovascular effects of selectively increasing aHipp glutamate levels in an unpredictable threat test and also investigated the contribution of mPFC regions 25 and 32 to these effects (Table 1).
Table 1.
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cannulae targets | aHipp/25/32 | aHipp/25/32 | aHipp | aHipp/25/32 | aHipp/25/32 | aHipp | aHipp/25/32 | aHipp/25/32 |
Human intruder test (Experiment 2a) | ||||||||
Saline 1 | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ||
aHipp LY/CGP | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ||
Saline 2 | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ||
Unpredictable threat test (Experiment 2b) | ||||||||
aHipp saline | ✓ (2) | ✓ (4) | ✓ (1) | ✓ (2) | ✓ (1) | ✓ (1) | ||
aHipp LY/CGP | ✓ (4) | ✓ (5) | ✓ (2) | ✓ (1) | ✓ (3) | |||
aHipp LY/CGP & musbac 25 | ✓ (1) | ✓ (1) | ✓ (3) | ✓ (2) | ||||
aHipp LY/CGP & musbac 32 | ✓ (6) | ✓ (3) | ✓ (4) | ✓ (4) | ||||
musbac 25 | ✓ (3) | ✓ (6) | ✓ (5) | ✓ (3) | ||||
musbac 32 | ✓ (5) | ✓ (2) | ✓ (6) | ✓ (2) | ✓ (4) | |||
No. of infusions | ||||||||
aHipp | 10 | 10 | 5 | 10 | 10 | 6 | 14 | 4 |
Area 25 | 3 | 4 | NA | 3 | 3 | NA | 8 | 3 |
Area 32 | 6 | 3 | NA | 2 | 4 | NA | 6 | 1 |
Checkmarks indicate that the subject took part in that phase of the study. For Experiment 2a, the numbers in parentheses indicate the order in which the infusions were received. Subjects 3 and 6 only had cannulae targeting the aHipp because they underwent prefrontal microdialysis (not reported here). Postmortem histology for subject 5 revealed that the cannulae were not located in areas 25 or 32 and histology for subject 7 revealed that the cannulae were not located in area 25; therefore, their data for these regions are excluded. However, the overall number of infusions that they received in these areas is included for completeness (including other drug combinations that are not reported here).
To allow the selective anatomical manipulation of either the aHipp and/or areas 25 and 32, all 10 animals in Experiment 2 underwent an aseptic surgical procedure to implant intracerebral cannulae targeting either the aHipp alone or the aHipp and areas 25 and 32. Of these, the six animals who also undertook Experiment 2B had another surgery to implant a telemetric blood pressure (BP) monitor into the descending aorta. Both surgeries were completed before the animals began any behavioral testing.
Cannulation surgery
Marmosets were premedicated with ketamine hydrochloride (Vetalar; 0.05 ml of a 100 mg solution, i.m.; GE Healthcare and Upjohn) before being given a long-lasting, nonsteroidal, anti-inflammatory analgesic (Carprieve; 0.03 ml of 50 mg/ml carprofen, s.c.; Pfizer,). The animals were intubated and maintained on 2.0–2.5% isoflurane in 0.3 L/min O2 and placed into a stereotaxic frame modified for the marmoset (David Kopf Instruments). Pulse-rate, O2 saturation, breathing rate, and CO2 saturation were all monitored by pulse oximetry and capnography (Microcap Handheld Capnograph; Oridion Capnography) and core body temperature was monitored by a rectal thermometer (TES-1319 K-type digital thermometer; TES Electrical Electronic). Cannulae (Plastics One) were implanted into area 25 [double 7-mm-long cannulae, 1 mm apart, anteroposterior (AP) + 14, lateromedial (LM ± 0.5), area 32 (double 2 mm long cannulae, 1 mm apart; AP +17; LM ± 0.5 at a 30° AP angle), and the aHipp (double 15-mm-long cannula, 1 mm apart, AP + 6, LM ± 5.75/7.75, ventral + 5) with coordinates adjusted where necessary in situ according to cortical depth (Roberts et al., 2007). Postoperatively, and when fully recovered, all monkeys were returned to their home cage and then received the analgesic meloxicam (0.1 ml of a 1.5 mg/ml oral suspension; Boehringer Ingelheim) for 3 d, after which they had at least a further 10 d recovery. Cannulae were cleaned every week (and caps and cannula blockers changed) to ensure that the cannula site remained free from infection.
Telemetry surgery
Animals were anesthetized as before, the descending aorta was visualized within the abdominal cavity, and the probe catheter of a telemetric BP transmitter (Data Sciences International) was implanted into the aorta as described previously (Braesicke et al., 2005). All monkeys received meloxicam as before in addition to prophylactic treatment with amoxicillin and clavulanic acid (Synulox; 50 mg/ml solution; Pfizer,) for 1 d before and 6 d after telemetry surgery.
Drug infusions
Approximately once a week before behavioral testing, animals received infusions of drug or vehicle down the cannulae. The use of chronically implanted cannulae and acute infusions allowed animals to act as their own controls and reduced experimental variation caused by intersubject differences. For all sterile central infusions, the marmoset was held gently in a researcher's hand, the caps and cannula blockers were removed from the guide, and the site was cleaned with 70% alcohol. A sterile injector (Plastics One) connected to a 2 μl gastight syringe in a syringe pump was inserted into the guide cannula for a 2 min drug infusion. Following the infusion, the injector was left in place for a further minute to allow the drug to diffuse before injector removal. Sterile cannula blockers and caps were replaced and the marmoset was returned to its home cage for the relevant pretreatment time. To increase glutamate in the hippocampus, 1 μl of a mixture of the mGlu2/3 receptor antagonist LY341495 (1 ng/μl; Tocris Bioscience) and the GABAB receptor antagonist CGP52432 (1 ng/μl; Tocris Bioscience), the LY/CGP cocktail, was bilaterally infused at a rate of 0.5 μl/min with a 15 min pretreatment time (Marrocco et al., 2012). These receptors both act to limit presynaptic glutamate release and their antagonism therefore acts to increase the amount of glutamate released. To inactivate areas 25 or 32, we used the GABAA agonist muscimol and the GABAB agonist baclofen (0.5 μl of 0.1 mm muscimol/1.0 mm baclofen, referred to here as “musbac”; both from Sigma-Aldrich) infusion at a rate of 0.25 μl/min and a pretreatment time of 25 min. Saline infusions acted as vehicle controls.
Human intruder test
Defining a high-anxiety trait.
At ∼2 years of age, high-trait-anxious marmosets were identified according to their performance on a human intruder test, a well-validated test of uncertainty-based anxiety in primates (Carey et al., 1992; Oler et al., 2009; Fox et al., 2010; Mikheenko et al., 2015). In the marmoset version of this test, the monkey is confined to the upper right hand quadrant of their cage away from their cage-mate and a video camera is focused on their quadrant to allow their behavior to be quantified after the test. After an 8 min habituation period, human (wearing an unfamiliar latex human face mask) enters the room, stands 30 cm away from the quadrant front, and maintains eye contact with the monkey for 2 min (Agustín-Pavón et al., 2012) (Figure 1). Because the intent of the unknown human is unclear, as he or she may provide treats or may try and hold/catch the monkey, the uncertainty tends to induce anxious behavior. There are many behaviors that can be analyzed in this paradigm, but one of the most relevant is the location of the animal within the cage. In contrast to normal animals, high-trait-anxious marmosets characteristically retreat from the human intruder to the back (increased average depth from intruder) and/or the top (increased average height from intruder) of the cage and therefore have a high score for time spent at the top/time spent at the back (TSAB) of the cage and a very low score for time spent at the front (TSAF) of the cage. In particular, TSAF has been shown to be sensitive to anxiolytics (Carey et al., 1992; Santangelo et al., 2016) and marmosets with a TSAF score of <9% (based on the lower confidence interval of the median of 63 naive marmosets tested on the human intruder) have been shown to display stable characteristic trait anxious behaviors and blunted serotonin signaling compared with marmosets that scored >25% TSAF (upper 95% confidence interval) (Mikheenko et al., 2015). Therefore, those animals with a TSAF score of <9% were defined as high anxious in this study.
Drug treatments.
To investigate whether increasing hippocampal glutamate release altered anxiety behaviors in the human intruder test, five high-trait-anxious animals performed this test on an additional three occasions a minimum of a week apart (four animals had cannulae in area 25, area 32, and the aHipp; two animals had cannulae in the aHipp only). The first and third tests were presented after aHipp saline treatment and the middle test was presented after aHipp LY/CGP treatment. This treatment order allowed us to determine whether any drug effects were being confounded by habituation that might occur upon repeated exposure to a human intruder. To help minimize habituation and to ensure that the intruder was novel in appearance on each occasion, the same intruder wore a different human rubber mask as a disguise for each test. These masks were counterbalanced between animals, covered the whole head and hair of the experimenter, and were close fitting so they did not compromise the ability to maintain eye contact.
Human intruder analysis
Behavior during the human intruder test was recorded and subsequently scored by an experimenter who was blinded to the drug infusion used. During the 2 min intruder period, the time (in seconds) that the animal spent in particular locations within the cage in terms of height (floor, low, medium, high, top of nest box, or average height) and depth (front, middle, back) was recorded as a percentage of the total time, alongside the number of jumps toward the intruder and the amount of time spent locomoting around the cage (in seconds). Additional measures included species specific behaviors namely, the number of seconds spent head and body bobbing (a marmoset behavior indicative of anxiety; Carey et al., 1992; Agustín-Pavón et al., 2011; Santangelo et al., 2016) and the number of various types of vocalizations based on differences in sound, length, and frequency range. These include the “tsik” call, a proactive alarm call used either singly or repetitively in mobbing situations in response to a potential intruder, “tse and egg” calls, which are both indicative of less proactive alarm/anxiety and are associated with vigilance behavior, and combinations of calls such as “tsik-egg” and “tse-egg,” which may serve to coordinate mobbing, vigilance, and anxiety between group members (Bezerra and Souto, 2008). All of these behavioral variables have previously been shown to contribute to an animal's overall response to a human intruder (Agustín-Pavón et al., 2012) and some have been shown to load onto a single factor that was calculated from an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) that was performed on human intruder test scores obtained as part of a screening procedure on 171 marmosets from the University of Cambridge Marmoset Breeding Colony. This EFA was used to predict the extent to which the different behaviors in the human intruder test are driven by any underlying latent variables. The EFA included: TSAF, TSAB, average height, proportion of time spent in locomotion, number of bobs, egg calls, tsik call, tsik-egg calls, and tse-egg calls. In total, nine factors were identified, but based on the point of inflection on a scree plot, a single factor was extracted accounting for 39.7% of the total variance in behavior (Table 2). The major behaviors that contributed to this factor were the percentage of TSAF, TSAB, the average height of the monkey within the cage, and the number of head and body bobs, tse-egg calls, and locomotion. The minor contributory behaviors were the number of tsik-egg and egg vocalizations, and the influence of the tsik calls was negligible. Specifically, animals that scored highly on this composite score spent more time at the back of the cage and high in the cage, indicating a greater avoidance of the human intruder. They also spent more time head and body bobbing, less time locomoting, and made fewer tsik vocalizations and more egg/tse-egg vocalizations, all signs of anxious behavior in marmosets (Carey et al., 1992; Bezerra and Souto, 2008; Agustín-Pavón et al., 2012; Santangelo et al., 2016). Therefore, the pattern in which the items cluster on this factor are all indicative of anxious behavior and suggest that these different behaviors are acting together to drive one underlying composite behavior that represents an animal's overall anxiety toward the human intruder. This composite anxiety score was therefore calculated for each of the high-anxious animals on each occasion that they performed the human intruder test in this study using z scores derived from their performance as a group.
Table 2.
Behavior | Proportional loading onto anxiety factor |
---|---|
Average height | 0.816 |
Bobs | 0.769 |
Time spent at back | 0.688 |
Tse egg calls | 0.417 |
Egg calls | 0.332 |
Tsik egg calls | 0.323 |
Tsik calls | −0.091 |
Locomotion | −0.568 |
Time spent at front | −0.790 |
The pattern in which these individual behaviors load onto this EFA factor suggests that it represents the marmosets' anxiety toward the intruder, with a high factor score representing an animal high up and at the back of cage far away from the intruder, remaining relatively still, and performing a lot of head bobs, indicative of high anxiety.
Unexpected threat paradigm
Behavioral testing using the unexpected threat paradigm took place within a custom-made, sound-attenuated testing chamber in a dark room. This apparatus enabled the measurement of real-time cardiovascular activity via a receiver situated directly under the animal. Animals were trained to enter a transparent Perspex carry box (l = 21.5, w = 19, h = 25 cm) with air holes and a removable clear Perspex door in which they were transported to the behavioral test apparatus. The Perspex carry box was placed inside the test chamber and the marmoset remained inside of this box at all times during testing. The test chamber was lit by a 3 W bulb (house light) located in the middle of the ceiling of the chamber and contained a computer-controlled speaker and a siren generator (120 dB; Biotronix) through which 75 dB auditory stimuli and a 117 dB siren (mildly aversive loud noise) could be played. The apparatus was controlled by the Whisker control system (Cardinal and Aitken, 2010) and in-house software. Three video cameras were positioned in the test chamber so that the activity of the animal within the Perspex box could be recorded by video software (Power Director; CyberLink) and observed directly by the experimenter during testing.
The aim of this test was to expose marmosets to a particular context with unpredictable threat (aversive loud noise) and then to determine their reactivity to a novel, unfamiliar cue presented within this unpredictable threat context. To investigate the effect of a variety of central brain manipulations on their reactivity to unfamiliar cues, a repeated block design was used involving a 5 d cycle of sessions per block. In each session, the animal was positioned inside the Perspex carry box within the testing apparatus and the house light was on. For the first 4 unpredictable threat days, animals were placed in the familiar environment of the testing box for 25 min and presented intermittently with an aversive sound (unconditioned stimulus, US) and an auditory cue. Each auditory cue was 20 s long, 75 dB, and presented 12 times with an intertrial interval (ITI) of 40–80 s. Each US was a 0.4 s, intrinsically aversive 117 dB siren (Agustín-Pavón et al., 2012) and was also presented 12 times per session with an ITI of 40–80 s. The threat of the aversive US was unpredictable because there was no temporal relationship between the auditory cue and the US, so the best predictor of the aversive noise was the context, namely the test apparatus. On the ambiguous cue probe (day 5), monkeys were presented with a novel, 20 s, 75 dB auditory cue presented 12 times with an ITI of 100–180 s within this same threatening context. No US was presented. All drug manipulations occurred on these ambiguous cue probe days. After the cue probe day, the next cycle then started with the novel auditory cue from the prior ambiguous cue probe day incorporated into the unpredictable threat training.
This paradigm therefore measures the response to an uncertain, novel stimulus in an environment associated with the unpredictable threat of a noise stressor (see Fig. 3A). However, because the meaning of the novel cue on the probe session is ambiguous, it is potentially threatening, a potential that is amplified by it being presented within an environment associated with the risk of unpredictable threat. By including an unpaired auditory cue on unpredictable threat sessions, it also becomes part of the context (although to a lesser extent than the testing apparatus) and prevents the marmosets learning that every novel auditory cue presentation (the probe days) is associated with no US presentation. Performing the drug manipulations on the cue probe day therefore allows the investigation of the components of this acute stress response in the absence of the noise stressor itself.
Unexpected threat analysis
For cardiovascular analysis, BP data were continuously transmitted by the implanted probe to a receiver (RPC-1; DSI) located beneath the testing apparatus. By comparison with an ambient pressure reference monitor (APR-1; DSI), a calibrated pressure output adapter (R11CPA; DSI) converted the absolute pressure into a gauge pressure in millimeters of mercury. The data were then collected, analyzed, and stored offline using an analog/digital converter (micro 1401; Cambridge Electronic Design) and data acquisition software (Spike2 version 7.01; Cambridge Electronic Design). Any outliers and recording failures in the data were removed (BP values >200 mmHg or <0 mmHg or other abnormal spikes). Data collection was reliable overall, but data gaps of <0.4 s were replaced by cubic spline interpolation and gaps of >0.4 s were treated as missing values. Systolic and diastolic BP events were extracted as local maxima and minima for each cardiac cycle and used to calculate the mean arterial pressure (MAP; diastolic BP + [1/3 (systolic BP − diastolic BP)]) and interbeat intervals (IBIs) used to calculate heart rate (HR) and HR variability (HRV) (the root-mean-square SD measure of the time difference between consecutive IBIs), for the 20 s baseline and 20 s aversive cue periods using custom-written code (RN Cardinal) and the HRV Toolkit (https://physionet.org/tutorials/hrv-toolkit/).
Because HRV is determined by the interval between heartbeats, it reflects how flexible the heart is in responding to internal or external stressors. Decreased HRV is associated with anxiety disorders and depression as well as cardiovascular disease and cardiovascular mortality. HRV reflects the balance between the parasympathetic (i.e., vagal) and sympathetic autonomic cardiac regulation and baroreflex activity and assesses the distinct contributions of vagal and sympathetic activity. Poincaré plots (plots of IBIj+1 as a function of IBIj) were created and the SD of the points perpendicular to the line of identity (SD1) and the SD of the points along the line of identity (SD2) (Toichi et al., 1997) were then used to derive indices of autonomic activity. There were the cardiac vagal index (CVI), which represents the parasympathetic component of cardiac activity, and the cardiac sympathetic index (CSI), which represents sympathetic activity as well as some parasympathetic and baroreflex activity (Toichi et al., 1997; but see Rahman et al., 2011). For each trial, the mean parameter of interest was calculated for the 20 s aversive cue presentation and for the immediately preceding 20 s baseline (BL) period (the last 20 s of the preceding intertrial interval). Therefore, during each cue presentation, the cue-directed HR was calculated as (HR during the cue) − (HR during BL). HRV was also assessed during each 20 s baseline and cue period. This was possible because the high resting HR of marmosets ensured that an adequate number of IBIs (∼100) were gathered across the two sampling periods (Toichi et al., 1997).
The normal healthy cardiovascular response to an ambiguous cue in a potentially threatening environment would be to show a heightened stress response to the potential threat, an increase in HR and CSI, and a withdrawal of CVI, leading to a transient reduction in HRV in preparation for fight-or-flight (de Geus et al., 1990; Boutcher and Stocker, 1996; Pagani et al., 1997). However, this cardiac responsivity is not seen in high-trait-anxious individuals in response to acute stress, who instead show no differences in cardiovascular function between the stress condition and the baseline. This has been referred to as “cardiovascular blunting” (Ginty and Conklin, 2011; Brindle et al., 2013; Levine et al., 2016; Carroll et al., 2017).
The behavior measured during the unexpected threat paradigm was vigilant scanning (VS), which is defined as a watchful scanning of surroundings accompanied by tense, vigilant body posture (Mikheenko et al., 2010; Agustín-Pavón et al., 2012). The time the animal spent engaged in this behavior during the 20 s cue period and 20 s BL period was scored. Cue-directed VS was calculated as the difference between these two (synonymous to cue-directed HR). Studies of healthy individuals indicate that an acute mild stress response is normally associated with an increases in cue-directed VS as the animal engages with the potential threat (Agustín-Pavón et al., 2012). However, high-trait-anxious individuals show deficits in motivational behaviors and task engagement that could be considered a “behavioral blunting” (Carroll et al., 2017).
A second person blinded to the conditions of the experiment scored a subset of the discrimination sessions. Interscorer reliability was high (R(72) = 0.76, p < 0.001).
Experimental design and statistical analysis
Glutamate levels from the 12 monkeys in Experiment 1 were correlated to their anxiety-related behaviors using parametric bivariate correlation analyses in SPSS version 22 (IBM) for both the left and right hippocampi. Human intruder test data from Experiment 2a were analyzed using repeated-measures ANOVA and post hoc paired (within-subjects) t tests in SPSS using a factor of “test” (saline 1, aHipp LY/CGP, or saline 2). Cardiovascular and behavioral data from the unpredictable threat paradigm (Experiment 2b) were analyzed in R version 3.2.2 (R Core Team, 2016) using repeated-measures ANOVA and post hoc paired (within-subjects) t tests from the lmerTest package and type III sums of squares with the Satterthwaite approximation for degrees of freedom. Factors included area (levels including aHipp, area 32, area 25, and combinations) and drug (including LY/CGP, saline, and musbac). Although MAP was analyzed throughout, it was highly variable and was not statistically affected by any of the manipulations used in the present study. MAP data are therefore not presented. In all cases n = 5 or above gives a high statistical power-to-subject ratio while minimizing the number of animals used.
Postmortem assessment of cannulae placement
Animals were premedicated with ketamine hydrochloride (Vetalar; 0.05 ml of a 100 mg solution, i.m.; GE Healthcare and Upjohn) before being euthanized with pentobarbitol sodium (Dolethal; 1 ml of a 200 mg/ml solution, i.v.; Merial Animal Health). Animals were then perfused transcardially with 500 ml of 0.1 m PBS, followed by 500 ml of 4% paraformaldehyde fixative solution over ∼15 min. The brain was removed and left in the 4% paraformaldehyde fixative solution overnight before being transferred to 30% sucrose solution for at least 48 h. Brains were then sectioned on a freezing microtome (coronal sections; 60 μm), mounted on slides, and stained with cresyl fast violet. The sections were viewed under a Leitz DMRD microscope (Leica Microsystems). The cannula locations for each animal were schematized onto drawings of standard marmoset brain coronal sections and composite diagrams were then made to illustrate the extent of overlap between animals.
Results
Experiment 1
Increased trait anxiety is associated with reduced glutamate levels within the primate right aHipp
To assess the contribution of aHipp neurochemistry to the trait-anxious phenotype, glutamate and monoamine levels within the right and left aHipp of marmosets (from Shiba et al., 2014) were compared with the composite anxiety score (as determined by factor analysis) and the individual behaviors displayed in response to an unknown human. Hierarchical linear regression revealed that right aHipp glutamate levels were negatively correlated with the composite anxiety score of the animals, with a higher composite anxiety score correlating with reduced aHipp glutamate (EFA score: R(12) = −0.601, p = 0.039).
Detailed inspection of correlations between right aHipp glutamate levels and individual performance variables revealed that reduced right aHipp glutamate was associated with the animals moving further away from the human intruder. Therefore, reduced glutamate was associated with an increased amount of TSAB and/or on the top of the nest box, the farthest location possible from the intruder (R(12) = −0.649, p = 0.022), and a trend toward an increase in average height (calculated from percentage time spent on the floor, low, middle, and high locations within the cage; R(12) = 0.533, p = 0.075). Reduced aHipp glutamate was also associated with increases in the amount of head and body bobbing, an anxiety-related behavior (R(12) = −0.641, p = 0.025), but no changes in the time spent at the front of the cage (R(12) = −0.430, p = 0.163), the amount of locomotion (R(13) = 0.384, p = 0.217), or the types of vocalizations made in response to the intruder (tse, R(12) = 0.209, p = 0.514; tse-egg, R(12) = 0.515 p = 0.087; tsik, R(12) = 0.280 p = 0.3; tsik-egg, R(12) = 0.309 p = 0.328; egg, R(12) = −0.026 p = 0.925).
These relationships were not seen with glutamate levels in the left aHipp because adding left aHipp glutamate to the regression model predicting EFA did not improve the fit (F(1,9) < 1, NS). The conclusions were also unchanged by analyzing according to the alternative sequence: left aHipp glutamate did not predict the EFA score (F(1,10) < 1, NS) but adding right aHipp glutamate to this model significantly improved the fit (F(1,9) = 5.8319, p = 0.039). Consistent with this, left aHipp glutamate did not correlate with average height (R(12) = −0.0.39 p = 0.904), time spent at front (R(12) = −0.123, p = 0.703), or back/top of nestbox (R(12) = −0.241 p = 0.450) bobbing (R(12) = −0.007 p = 0.982), any vocalizations (tse, R13 = −0.281, p = 0.376; tse-egg, R(13) = −0.435, p = 0.158; tsik, R(13) = 0.161 p = 0.616; tsik-egg, R(13) = 0.004 p = 0.3989; egg, R(13) = −0.102 p = 0.753) or the amount of the locomotion (R(12) = 0.149 p = 0.645).
Furthermore levels of DA, 5-HT and NA from both the left and right aHipp did not correlate with any of these individual behaviors or the composite anxiety score. However, both 5-HT and NA from the right aHipp, and DA from the left aHipp showed a positive correlation with the time spent at the back of the cage (5-HT, R(12) = 0.678 p = 0.015; NA, R(12) = 0.656 p = 0.021; DA, R(12) = 0.678 p = 0.015; Table 2). These findings indicate that while overall levels of trait anxiety can be selectively linked to reduction in right aHipp glutamate, other neurochemicals may also contribute to specific avoidance behaviors within this test.
Experiment 2A
Pharmacologically increasing presynaptic glutamate release within the aHipp reverses the high-anxiety phenotype on the human intruder test
Pharmacologically boosting aHipp glutamate levels in high-trait-anxious monkeys significantly altered their anxiety-related behavior in response to the human intruder, but did not affect all behaviors equally. Bilateral infusion of aHipp LY/CGP did decrease the composite anxiety score (EFA) indicative of a decrease in anxiety. However, there was also a tendency for EFA scores to show habituation between the first and second saline infusions (saline 1 and saline 2, respectively). Therefore, despite EFA scores correlating with right aHipp glutamate levels in Experiment 1 and showing a decrease after aHipp LY/CGP, its failure to return to saline 1 levels after saline 2 means that habituation effects cannot be ruled out (Fig. 2, Table 3). In contrast, the marked rise in the TSAF after aHipp LY/CGP was not accompanied by habituation between the first and second saline infusions, indicating that the animals experienced less anxiety and spent more time in close proximity to the human intruder after aHipp LY/CGP only. Performance variables of height, movements (jumps, bobs and locomotion), and call types were unaffected.
Table 3.
Dopamine | Serotonin | Noradrenaline | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
aHipp | L | R | L | R | L | R |
EFA score | R(12) = 0.332, p = 0.291 | R(12) = −0.072, p = 0.825 | R(12) = 0.026, p = 0.935 | R(12) = 0.271, p = 0.394 | R(12) = 0.021, p = 0.949 | R(12) = −0.127, p = 0.694 |
% in depth zones | ||||||
Front | R(12) = −0.362, p = 0.247 | R(12) = 0.227, p = 0.479 | R(12) = 0.016, p = 0.960 | R(12) = −0.362, p = 0.247 | R(12) = 0.053, p = 0.869 | R(12) = −0.239, p = 0.454 |
Middle | R(12) = 0.035, p = 0.913 | R(12) = −0.141, p = 0.661 | R(12) = 0.197, p = 0.540 | R(12) = 0.678, p = 0.015 | R(12) = 0.215, p = 0.503 | R(12) = 0.311, p = 0.325 |
Back | R(12) = 0.678, p = 0.015* | R(12) = 0.176, p = 0.585 | R(12) = 0.030, p = 0.927 | R(12) = 0.678, p = 0.015* | R(12) = −0.057, p = 0.860 | R(12) = 0.656, p = 0.021* |
% in height zones | ||||||
Floor | R(12) = −0.364, p = 0.245 | R(12) = −0.173, p = 0.590 | R(12) = −0.160, p = 0.619 | R(12) = −0.296, p = 0.350 | R(12) = −0.068, p = 0.833 | R(12) = 0.223, p = 0.485 |
Low | R(12) = −0.202, p = 0.530 | R(12) = −0.354, p = 0.259 | R(12) = −0.477, p = 0.117 | R(12) = −0.246, p = 0.441 | R(12) = −0.367, p = 0.240 | R(12) = 0.518, p = 0.085 |
Middle | R(12) = −0.341, p = 0.278 | R(12) = 0.473, p = 0.120 | R(12) = 0.073, p = 0.822 | R(12) = −0.059, p = 0.845 | R(12) = 0.005, p = 0.989 | R(12) = 0.127, p = 0.693 |
High | R(12) = 0.545, p = 0.067 | R(12) = 0.076, p = 0.814 | R(12) = 0.377, p = 0.227 | R(12) = 0.369, p = 0.237 | R(12) = 0.345, p = 0.272 | R(12) = −0.013, p = 0.968 |
Top of nest box | R(12) = −0.007, p = 0.982 | R(12) = −0.137, p = 0.671 | R(12) = −0.159, p = 0.622 | R(12) = −0.044, p = 0.891 | R(12) = −0.106, p = 0.606 | R(12) = −0.216, p = 0.499 |
Average height | R(12) = 0.328, p = 0.298 | R(12) = 0.014, p = 0.966 | R(12) = 0.091, p = 0.779 | R(12) = 0.220, p = 0.493 | R(12) = 0.032, p = 0.922 | R(12) = −0.281, p = 0.376 |
Movement | ||||||
Locomotion | R(12) = −0.364, p = 0.245 | R(12) = 0.041, p = 0.900 | R(12) = 0.049, p = 0.880 | R(12) = −0.224, p = 0.484 | R(12) = 0.097, p = 0.765 | R(12) = 0.183, p = 0.569 |
Jumps | R(12) = −0.0525, p = 0.080 | R(12) = 0.276, p = 0.385 | R(12) = 0.064, p = 0.842 | R(12) = −0.241, p = 0.450 | R(12) = 0.019, p = 0.952 | R(12) = −0.066, p = 0.838 |
Bobs | R(12) = 0.283, p = 0.373 | R(12) = −0.06, p = 0.854 | R(12) = 0.106, p = 0.742 | R(12) = 0.269, p = 0.397 | R(12) = 0.067, p = 0.837 | R(12) = −0.166, p = 0.605 |
Calls | ||||||
Tsik | R(12) = −0.420, p = 0.174 | R(12) = −0.053, p = 0.870 | R(12) = −5.098, p = 0.857 | R(12) = −0.054, p = 0.867 | R(12) = 0.–114, p = 0.725 | R(12) = 0.197, p = 0.540 |
Tsik-egg | R(12) = 0.376, p = 0.228 | R(12) = −0.079, p = 0.807 | R(12) = −0.098, p = 0.762 | R(12) = 0.525, p = 0.08 | R(12) = −0.209, p = 0.515 | R(12) = 0.261, p = 0.413 |
Tse | R(12) = −0.143, p = 0.657 | R(12) = 0.211, p = 0.510 | R(12) = −0.238, p = 0.438 | R(12) = −0.101, p = 0.755 | R(12) = −0.265, p = 0.406 | R(12) = −0.029, p = 0.928 |
Tse-egg | R(12) = −0.137, p = 0.672 | R(12) = 0.066, p = 0.839 | R(12) = −0.272, p = 0.393 | R(12) = −0.190, p = 0.555 | R(12) = −0.271, p = 0.294 | R(12) = −0.350, p = 0.265 |
Egg | R(12) = 0.381, p = 0.221 | R(12) = 0.266, p = 0.404 | R(12) = 0.034, p = 0.902 | R(12) = 0.04, p = 0.901 | R(12) = 0.047, p = 0.886 | R(12) = −0.357, p = 0.254 |
Neither right aHipp 5-HT and NA nor left aHipp DA correlated with the time spent at the back of the cage, suggesting that these neurochemicals may contribute to specific behaviors within the human intruder test.
* p < 0.05.
Table 4.
aHipp Saline 1 | aHipp LY/CGP | aHipp Saline 2 | |
---|---|---|---|
EFA score* | 0.798 ± 0.179 | 0.06 ± 0.121* | 0.161 ± 0.066 |
% in depth zones | |||
Front* | 0.957 ± 0.957 | 20.55 ± 4.766* | 7.853 ± 4.572* |
Middle | 40.327 ± 16.098 | 60.089 ± 10.811 | 60.704 ± 11.866 |
Back | 32.877 ± 9.965 | 16.878 ± 7.839 | 18.990 ± 7.537 |
% in height zones | |||
Floor | 0.00 ± 0.00 | 10.136 ± 8.180 | 10.106 ± 6.613 |
Low | 0.00 ± 0.00 | 3.7 ± 3.7 | 4.331 ± 2.842 |
Middle | 2.302 ± 2.302 | 16.935 ± 7.995 | 8.904 ± 3.967 |
High | 71.681 ± 8.089 | 66.738 ± 16.030 | 64.119 ± 10.841 |
Top of nest box | 19.717 ± 7.8 | 1.738 ± 1.738 | 4.414 ± 2.328 |
Average height | 68.758 ± 3.697 | 58.79 ± 7.503 | 55.5 ± 5.593 |
Movement | |||
Locomotion | 2.003 ± 0.832 | 3.208 ± 0.999 | 3.623 ± 1.119 |
Jumps | 1.667 ± 0.667 | 2.5 ± 1.057 | 3.5 ± 1.057 |
Bobs | 30.333 ± 8.313 | 19.333 ± 5.714 | 19.0 ± 7.546 |
Calls | |||
Tsik | 3.833 ± 3.637 | 4.0 ± 2.696 | 1.0 ± 0.516 |
Tsik-egg | 4.667 ± 3.721 | 4.0 ± 3.235 | 6.667 ± 5.690 |
Tse | 2.0 ± 1.095 | 0.667 ± 0.333 | 0.833 ± 0.477 |
Tse-egg | 6.667 ± 2.951 | 2.833 ± 2.242 | 4.00 ± 1.949 |
Egg | 4.823 ± 2.926 | 1.667 ± 0.333 | 4.0 ± 1.770 |
Shown are mean scores for all behaviors analyzed during the human intruder test after each presentation of the test (saline 1, LY/CGP, and saline 2). Data are presented as mean ± SEM.
*p < 0.05. Asterisks in column 1 indicate a main overall effect of the measure; asterisks in columns 2–4 indicate significance compared with the preceding test.
Repeated-measures ANOVA of the composite anxiety score revealed a main effect of test (saline 1, LY/CGP, saline 2) and a significant decrease in anxiety score between saline 1 and aHipp LY/CGP, but no difference between aHipp LY/CGP and saline 2 indicative of habituation (main effect of test, F(2,10) = 7.427, p = 0.011; first saline vs LY/CGP, t(5) = 2.572, p = 0.042; second saline vs LY/CGP, t(5) = 1.197 p = 0.285; Table 3). Repeated-measures analysis of all three depth measures (front, middle, back) also indicated that depth was sensitive to aHipp glutamate levels, but this was not due to alterations in time spent in the middle or back (no main effects of test3: middle, F(2,10) = 1.98, p = 0.189; back, F(2,10) = 2.239, p = 0.157) and was solely due to a main effect of TSAF. Importantly, this effect was not due to habituation because TSAF was relatively low following both saline infusions, but was markedly increased after aHipp LY/CGP (effect of test3, F(2,10) = 8.465, p = 0.007; first saline vs LY/CGP, t(5) = 3.759, p = 0.013; second saline vs LY/CGP, t(5) = 3.256, p = 0.023). No other performance variables of movement, height, or vocalizations were affected (locomotion, F(2,10) = 1.17, p = 0.348; jumps F(2,10) = 1.027, p = 0.393; or bobs F(2,10) = 2.9, p = 0.101; height5 × test3, F(10,50) = 1.69, p = 0.108; or vocalizations6 × test3, F(10,50) = 1.776, p = 0.09).
Therefore, enhancement of glutamate release in the aHipp specifically ameliorates the high-trait anxiety phenotype by increasing the approach to a human intruder.
Experiment 2B
High-trait-anxious marmosets display low cardiovascular responsivity to the presentation of an ambiguous cue in a threatening environment
To assess the contribution of aHipp–mPFC circuitry to anxiety, we first assessed the response of high-trait-anxious animals to a novel auditory cue in a context that has previously been associated with the threat of an unpredictable, aversive loud noise (Mikheenko et al., 2010) (Figure 3A).
Because the cardiovascular responsivity of high-trait-anxious humans is blunted, we first determined whether the cardiovascular and VS response to the novel cue changed compared with the preceding baseline (cue − baseline). High-trait-anxious animals did show a slight increase in VS after bilateral aHipp saline infusion, but like high-trait-anxious humans, they failed to show any cardiovascular alterations because cue-directed HR, HRV, CSI, and CVI were all close to zero (Mikheenko et al., 2010) (one-sample t tests compared with no change: VS, t(5) = 3.744, p = 0.013; HR, t(5) = 0.888, p = 0.415; HRV t(5) = 1.078, p = 0.330; CSI, t(5) = 0.182, p = 0.863; CVI, t(5) = 1.043, p = 0.345; Fig. 4A). Within-subjects correlation analysis of these changes in HR and VS revealed that those animals that showed the least HR responsivity also showed the smallest changes in VS (F(1,65) = 19.001, p < 0.0001; Fig. 5), indicating a relationship between cardiovascular responsivity and behavioral engagement that we have previously shown to be independent of alterations in arousal or locomotion (Mikheenko et al., 2010; Wallis et al., 2017).
aHipp LY/CGP increases cardiovascular responsivity to the presentation of an ambiguous cue in a threatening environment
Compared with the nonresponsiveness of the cardiovascular system to the novel auditory cue after a saline infusion, a marked increase in responsiveness was observed following aHipp LY/CGP infusions. A two-way ANOVA with factors of drug (saline or aHipp LY/CGP) and cue presentations (1–12) revealed that aHipp LY/CGP increased cue-directed HR (main effects of drug2, F(1,107.67) = 11.25, p = 0.001 and drug2 × cue12, F(11,103.22) = 2.65, p = 0.005, Fig. 4). Similar analyses also revealed an increase in cue-directed CSI (main effects of drug2, F(1,108) = 4.954, p = 0.0281, and drug2 × cue12, F(11,101.35) = 2.63, p = 0.005), a decrease in cue-directed HRV (main effect of drug2 only, F(1,107.9) = 4.956, p = 0.028) and no change in CVI (no main effect of drug F(1,107.9) = 1.446, p = 0.23, drug2 × cue12, F < 1, NS).
In summary, high-trait-anxious animals show a marked lack of cardiovascular responsivity in response to threatening cues, but increasing aHipp presynaptic glutamate release significantly increased both the cardiovascular and behavioral responsivity.
Inactivation of area 25 blocks the ability of aHipp LY/CGP to increase cardiovascular responsivity and VS
To determine whether the increases in cardiovascular responsivity induced by aHipp LY/CGP engage aHipp-area 25 circuitry, aHipp LY/CGP infusions were combined with simultaneous inactivation of area 25 with 0.5 μl of musbac.
This simultaneous inactivation was found to reduce the impact of aHipp LY/CGP on HR, VS, HRV, and CSI. Therefore, repeated-measures ANOVA with factors of manipulation (25 musbac, aHipp saline, aHipp LY/CGP, and aHipp LY/CGP + 25 musbac) and cue presentations (1–12) revealed changes in HR, VS, HRV, and CSI, but not CVI (HR, main effect of manipulation4 F(3,16.7) = 8.98, p = 1.491 × 10−05; VS, main effect of manipulation4 F(3,177.46) = 4.82, p = 0.002; HRV, main effect of manipulation4 F(3,180) = 3.4, p = 0.017; CSI, a trending effect of manipulation4 F(3,148.22) = 2.269, p = 0.08, and a manipulation4 × cue number12 interaction F(33,173) = 1.59, p = 0.02; CVI, F(3,180) = 0.69, p = 0.55).
Post hoc analysis revealed that simultaneous aHipp LY/CGP + area 25 musbac infusions reversed some of the cue-directed changes that were induced by aHipp LY/CGP alone. Specifically, the increases in HR that were seen after aHipp LY/CGP were abolished to below control levels, with the effects of aHipp LY/CGP + area 25 musbac no longer different from aHipp saline (t(3) = 1.8, p = 0.157). Similarly, the increases in VS and CSI seen after aHipp LY/CGP were returned to saline levels after aHipp LY/CGP + area 25 musbac (VS, aHipp saline vs aHipp LY/CGP + area 25 musbac t(3) = 0.7, p = 0.49, aHipp LY/CGP vs aHipp LY/CGP + area 25 musbac t(2) = 1.45, p = 0.285. CSI, aHipp saline vs aHipp LY/CGP + area 25 musbac t(3) = 1.5, p = 0.21, aHipp LY/CGP vs aHipp LY/CGP + area 25 musbac t(2) = 6.43, p = 0.02). Although, numerically, the combined aHipp LY/CGP + area 25 musbac did reverse the reductions in cue-directed HRV induced by aHipp LY/CGP, these did not reach significance due to high variability (aHipp saline vs aHipp LY/CGP + area 25 musbac t(2) = 1.5, p = 0.27, aHipp LY/CGP vs aHipp LY/CGP + area 25 musbac t(2) = 1.2, p = 0.27).
For control purposes, we also investigated the effects of area 25 inactivation alone. However, in contrast to the ability of area 25 inactivation to block the enhanced responsivity induced by aHipp LY/CGP, area 25 inactivation on its own also enhanced responsivity in a similar manner to aHipp LY/CGP. Therefore, compared with saline, area 25 inactivation increased measures of both cue-directed HR and VS (HR, t(3) = 4.03, p = 0.027; VS, t(3) = 4.03, p = 0.027; Fig. 4B). HRV, CSI, and CVI were not affected by area 25 inactivation (t < 1, NS). Because these HR and VS findings are very similar to those seen after aHipp LY/CGP, the fact that both measures do not differ from saline after the combined aHipp LY/CGP + area 25 musbac infusions indicates that the combined manipulation not only abolishes the independent effects of aHipp LY/CGP manipulations alone, but also abolishes the effects of 25 inactivation alone. This indicates reciprocal communication between these structures in the behavioral and cardiovascular regulation of high-trait anxiety.
Area 25 activity is therefore an important determinant of aHipp LY/CGP's ability to alter the behavioral and cardiovascular indices of high-trait anxiety in response to an ambiguous cue and highlights the importance of reciprocal aHipp-area 25 connectivity in this phenotype.
Inactivation of area 32 does not alter the effects of aHipp LY/CGP
To determine the contribution of aHipp-area 32 circuitry to the effects of aHipp LY/CGP, we also combined aHipp LY/CGP infusions with simultaneous inactivation of area 32. However, unlike area 25, area 32 inactivation did not modulate the effects of aHipp LY/CGP. Repeated-measures ANOVA of the area 32 manipulations with factors of manipulation (32 musbac, aHipp saline, aHipp LY/CGP, and aHipp LY/CGP + 32 musbac) and cue number (1–12) revealed no main effects of manipulation4 on cue-directed HRV or CVI (HRV, F(3,180) = 1.6, p = 0.18; CVI, F(3,179.3) = 0.7, p = 0.55). Although similar analysis did reveal main effects of manipulation on both cue-directed HR and cue-directed VS (HR, F(3,190.97) = 4.1, p = 0.007; VS, F(3,189.4) = 4.36, p = 0.005) and CSI approaching significance (F(3,175.7) = 1.6, p = 0.055), these effects were not due to a modulatory role of area 32 or to the effects of simultaneous aHipp LY/CGP + area 32 inactivation, but rather to the effects of aHipp LY/CGP alone.
Therefore, although aHipp LY/CGP + area 32 inactivation did slightly reduce the increases in cue-directed HR seen with aHipp LY/CGP alone, this effect was not significant (aHipp LY/CGP vs aHipp LY/CGP + area 32 musbac, t(3) = 1.4, p = 0.234; Fig. 4C). Similarly, aHipp LY/CGP + area 32 musbac did not alter the effect of aHipp LY/CGP on cue-directed VS (t < 1, NS, aHipp saline vs area 32 musbac, t < 1, NS; aHipp LY/CGP vs aHipp LY/CGP + area 32 musbac, t(3) = 1.53, p = 0.223), CSI (aHipp saline vs area 32 musbac, t <1, NS; or aHipp LY/CGP vs aHipp LY/CGP +area 32 musbac, t(2) = 1.89, p = 0.199), or CVI (aHipp saline vs area 32 musbac, t < 1, NS, aHipp LY/CGP vs aHipp LY/CGP +area 32 musbac, t < 1, NS). Control inactivations of area 32 alone revealed no effects on cue-directed HR, VS, CSI (all t < 1, NS), HRV, or CVI (t > 1, NS). Therefore, activity within area 32 does notaffect the ability of aHipp LY/CGP to increase the behavioral and cardiovascular indices of high-trait anxiety in response to an ambiguous cue.
Discussion
We have demonstrated that high-trait anxiety in marmosets is correlated with right, but not left, aHipp glutamate levels, with reduced presynaptic glutamate in the right aHipp being associated with increased anxiety. Pharmacologically elevating aHipp presynaptic glutamate release with aHipp LY/CGP in a cohort of high-trait-anxious marmosets reduced their behavioral measures of anxiety in response to an unknown human. Glutamatergic elevations also increased the responsivity of the previously unresponsive cardiovascular systems of these high-trait-anxious animals to unpredictable threat cues and simultaneously increased their vigilant scanning responses to the same cues. Investigation into the specific pathways between the aHipp and mPFC that may underlie these cardiovascular and behavioral effects in high-trait-anxious animals revealed that simultaneous inactivation of area 25, but not area 32, abolished the effects of elevating aHipp glutamate. Therefore, we provide causal evidence in primates to support the hypothesis that aHipp glutamatergic hypofunction contributes to specific aspects of the behavioral and autonomic correlates of high-trait anxiety via an aHipp–area 25 pathway.
High-trait-anxious marmosets in the current study showed a marked lack of cardiovascular responsivity to a threatening cue and similarly low levels of VS. These findings contrast with the increases in HR and/or BP and VS that normally signal increased fear in nonanxious animals during aversive fear conditioning and in high-anxious marmosets responding in a similar, but less aversive, unpredictable threat paradigm (Mikheenko et al., 2010; Shiba et al., 2014; Wallis et al., 2017). Although one interpretation of these findings is that the paradigm was not aversive enough to cause a fear response, this is unlikely given the known aversive impact of the noise cue used (Wallis et al., 2017). A more likely interpretation is based on the findings in high-trait-anxious or depressed humans, who show blunted cardiovascular responsivity to such stressors and reduced behavioral engagement that is most extreme in those individuals who show the greatest cardiovascular blunting (Ginty et al., 2015). Therefore, the lack of cardiovascular responsivity to the threatening cues in the high-trait-anxious marmosets may be equivalent to the cardiovascular blunting seen in high-trait-anxious humans, although for confirmation, a direct comparison with low-anxious animals is required. Certainly, however, we have reported previously a reduction in BP, reminiscent of the blunted response seen here, in a subset of high-anxious marmosets following repeated exposure to aversive Pavlovian-conditioned stimuli (Shiba et al., 2014), an effect not seen in low-anxious marmosets that displayed the expected elevation in cardiovascular activity to an aversive Pavlovian-conditioned stimulus. Furthermore, like humans, the high-trait-anxious marmosets with the smallest HR changes also showed the smallest VS changes in Experiment 2, so the low levels of VS seen in the present study may reflect decreased behavioral engagement with the cue. These findings therefore most likely indicate that the high-trait-anxious monkeys display a similar blunted phenotype to high-trait-anxious humans and that this phenotype is linked to reduced hippocampal glutamate.
Hippocampal glutamatergic hypofunction has previously been linked to affective illness severity (de Diego-Adeliño et al., 2013). However, despite strong evidence that a hippocampal network mediates stressor-evoked behavioral and cardiovascular activity, few studies have investigated the role of the hippocampus in cardiovascular responsivity (Neafsey et al., 1993; Bär et al., 2016; Kuntze et al., 2016; Ajayi et al., 2018). Here, we show that increasing aHipp glutamate with aHipp LY/CGP causes corresponding increases in the HR and VS response to a threatening cue, along with increases in CSI and decreased HRV. These are the components of the fight-or-flight response that are normally shown in nonanxious animals responding to threat and suggest that aHipp LY/CGP has had an anxiolytic effect by normalizing the previously blunted responses. This is supported by the anxiolytic effects of aHipp LY/CGP in the human intruder test. Here, aHipp LY/CGP increased the time animals spent at the front of the cage and reduced the time spent deep in the cage, consistent with an increased approach to, and engagement with, the potentially threatening intruder. Therefore, both the increased VS in the unexpected threat paradigm and the increased approach behavior in the human intruder test may reflect increased engagement with the potential threat (be it aversive cue or human intruder). Together, these findings indicate that elevation of aHipp glutamate can ameliorate the high-trait-anxiety phenotype, in part by returning cardiovascular and behavioral reactivity to the healthy, nonblunted state (via a paradoxical increase in the fight-or-flight response). Glutamate within the aHipp is clearly an important regulator of the cardiovascular and behavioral correlates of high-trait anxiety.
Given that our data associated elevated trait anxiety with reductions of right aHipp glutamate only, we could speculate that these alterations are due to the effect of increasing glutamate within the right and not the left aHipp despite our use of bilateral aHipp LY/CGP infusions. Our manipulation data and human neuroimaging data are inconclusive regarding the laterality of the hippocampal abnormalities that are apparent in the affective disorders (Malykhin and Coupland, 2015) and no hemispheric differences were seen in the hippocampal response to early life stress (maternal deprivation) in marmosets (Law et al., 2009). However, some neuroimaging studies do report a stronger relationship between right aHipp neurochemistry and the affective disorders compared with the left hippocampus (de Diego-Adeliño et al., 2013). Furthermore, brief, repeated exposure to a novel environment during the early life of rodents has been shown to preferentially alter the synaptic plasticity and volume of the right, not left, hippocampus (Verstynen et al., 2001; Tang et al., 2008). These rodent changes were small (and equivalent changes may have been masked by the lower group sizes in the marmoset study), but nevertheless suggest the right hippocampus may be particularly important for translating the effects of early life experiences into adult temperament.
The hippocampus is connected to many other limbic brain regions through which it can enact these effects. These regions include the amygdala and the mPFC, which have been extensively studied in rodent models of anxiety and fear behavior, but less so with respect to cardiovascular regulation. Certainly, rodent studies of threat regulation emphasize a tripartite mPFC–vHipp–amygdala circuit (Sierra-Mercado et al., 2011; Padilla-Coreano et al., 2016) and stronger amygdala–hippocampal network activity has been associated with higher levels of human trait anxiety (Kirkby et al., 2018). Although we have not investigated the contribution of the amygdala in this study, we have previously shown that levels of 5-HT are reduced in the amygdala of high-anxious marmosets (Mikheenko et al., 2015), and the aHipp sends double-projecting neurons to both the mPFC and the basal amygdala through which both regions can be modulated simultaneously (Kim and Cho, 2017).
Communication specifically between the vHipp and the mPFC is vital for the behavioral correlates of threat conditioning in rodents, and studies have primarily emphasized the role of vHipp connectivity with the prelimbic (PL), rather than the infralimbic (IL), subregion (LeDoux, 2000; Adhikari et al., 2010; Sierra-Mercado et al., 2011; Sotres-Bayon et al., 2012; Padilla-Coreano et al., 2016). Because anatomical and connectivity studies suggest that rodent IL and PL are homologous to primate areas 25 and 32, respectively (Vertes, 2006; Vogt and Paxinos, 2014; Heilbronner et al., 2016), these rodent studies predict the importance of primate–area 32 connectivity for threat regulation. Despite this, the current data highlight the importance of aHipp–area 25 connectivity for the regulation of the behavioral and cardiovascular indices of cue-directed high-trait anxiety and provide causal support for human neuroimaging studies highlighting the importance of aHipp-area 25 connectivity in the regulation of negative emotional behavior (Dickie et al., 2011; Hamilton et al., 2011; Treadway et al., 2015). One possible explanation for these differences is that hippocampal glutamate differentially regulates these mPFC subregions in healthy and high-trait-anxious states. This would also explain why v/aHipp lesions and glutamate antagonism in nonanxious rodents and primates are anxiolytic (Chudasama et al., 2008, 2009; Barkus et al., 2010), yet increasing v/aHipp glutamate in high-trait-anxious rats and marmosets is also anxiolytic (Marrocco et al., 2012; present study). However, confirmation of this hypothesis requires the testing of nonanxious animals on the current paradigm for comparison with the high-anxious cohort. Alternatively, the rodent IL and PL may have different functions from primate areas 25 and 32. Recent behavioral findings in marmosets showed that inactivation of areas 25 and 32 had opposite autonomic and behavioral effects on fear discrimination and extinction to these putative rodent homologs, indicating uncertainty over the functional analogy of IL/25 and PL/32 (Wallis et al., 2017). Inactivation of the IL also has no effect on cardiovascular control during baseline or stressful conditions, whereas area 25 inactivation causes significant bradycardia (Müller-Ribeiro et al., 2012; Wallis et al., 2017). Nevertheless, vHipp–IL circuitry in the rat has been implicated in cardiovascular regulation because electrical stimulation of the vHipp in anesthetized rodents is only able to elicit a depression of cardiovascular activity (decreased HR) if the IL is intact (Ruit and Neafsey, 1990; Gianaros et al., 2004). However, it should be noted that, in both species, the hippocampus appears to connect primarily with the mPFC regions in which inactivation reduces fear.
Finally, inactivations of areas 25 and 32 alone had distinct effects. Inactivation of area 25 caused similar increases in HR and VS to aHipp LY/CGP, which suggests that area 25 inactivation is also anxiolytic in this paradigm. This replicates previous findings in which area 25 inactivation reduced the behavioral and cardiovascular indices of cued fear in healthy marmosets (Wallis et al., 2017; but see Gianaros et al., 2004; Critchley et al., 2011). The findings that area 25 inactivation and aHipp LY/CGP have the same effects independently, but their simultaneous infusion blocks these effects, suggests functional dependence of these two structures in the regulation of high-trait anxiety. In contrast, area 32 inactivation had no effect on the cardiovascular and VS responses induced by the novel cue, and the combined aHipp LY/CGP + area 32 inactivation also failed to modulate the anxiolytic effects of aHipp LY/CGP alone. This diverges from the increase in threat responses seen after area 32 inactivation (with GABA agonists) during cued fear conditioning in nonanxious marmosets (Wallis et al., 2017) and the positive relationship between perigenual GABA levels and human trait anxiety (Delli Pizzi et al., 2016). It is also different from the increase in HR and foreboding, or negative bias, that is seen after perigenual ACC stimulation in nonanxious epileptic patients and macaques, respectively, which may have preferentially activated GABAergic interneurons (Amemori and Graybiel, 2012; Parvizi et al., 2013). However, it may not be possible to detect further increases in defensive behaviors in response to certain or uncertain threat in high-trait-anxious animals, which may already be displaying maximal levels of this behavior. Clearly, the differing contributions of areas 25 and 32 to the modulation of threat responses has implications when considering treatment, particularly when considering GABAergic medications such as benzodiazepines (Bruijn et al., 2001).
To conclude, we have shown that aHipp glutamatergic hypofunction is a key predictor of a high-trait-anxiety phenotype in marmoset monkeys and that, by pharmacologically normalizing such hypofunction, we can also normalize the abnormal behavioral and cardiovascular blunting associated with this phenotype. We provide evidence that an aHipp–area 25 circuit, but not an aHipp–area 32 circuit underlies this specific anxiolytic effect and highlight the aHipp–area 25 circuit as a potential therapeutic target. Together, these findings demonstrate the importance of investigating both the behavioral and autonomic aspects of emotional responding and suggest that investigating cardiovascular function in rodents may be a valuable adjunct when comparing rodent and primate mPFC function.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the Medical Research Council (MRC Career Development Award RG62920 to H.F.C.) and was performed within the Behavioral and Clinical Neuroscience Institute (BCNI), which is supported by a consortium award from the Wellcome Trust and the MRC. J.L.Z. was supported by the National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (Brazil). C.U.W. and L.A. were supported by MRC studentships to the BCNI. A.S., G.C., and Y.S. were supported by MRC Programme Grant MR/M023990/1 to A.C.R. S.Q. is supported by studentship from the Malaysian Public Service Department
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Adhikari A, Topiwala MA, Gordon JA (2010) Synchronized activity between the ventral hippocampus and the medial prefrontal cortex during anxiety. Neuron 65:257–269. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.12.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agustín-Pavón C, Braesicke K, Shiba Y, Santangelo AM, Mikheenko Y, Cockroft G, Asma F, Clarke H, Man MS, Roberts AC (2012) Lesions of ventrolateral prefrontal or anterior orbitofrontal cortex in primates heighten negative emotion. Biol Psychiatry 72:266–272. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.03.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agustín-Pavón C, Parkinson J, Man MS, Roberts AC (2011) Contribution of the amygdala, but not orbitofrontal or medial prefrontal cortices, to the expression of flavour preferences in marmoset monkeys. Eur J Neurosci 34:1006–1017. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07813.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ajayi IE, McGovern AE, Driessen AK, Kerr NF, Mills PC, Mazzone SB (2018) Hippocampal modulation of cardiorespiratory function. Respir Physiol Neurobiol 252–253:18–27. 10.1016/j.resp.2018.03.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amemori K, Graybiel AM (2012) Localized microstimulation of primate pregenual cingulate cortex induces negative decision-making. Nat Neurosci 15:776–785. 10.1038/nn.3088 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bär KJ, Herbsleb M, Schumann A, de la Cruz F, Gabriel HW, Wagner G (2016) Hippocampal-brainstem connectivity associated with vagal modulation after an intense exercise intervention in healthy men. Front Neurosci 10:145. 10.3389/fnins.2016.00145 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barkus C, McHugh SB, Sprengel R, Seeburg PH, Rawlins JN, Bannerman DM (2010) Hippocampal NMDA receptors and anxiety: at the interface between cognition and emotion. Eur J Pharmacol 626:49–56. 10.1016/j.ejphar.2009.10.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bezerra BM, Souto A (2008) Structure and usage of the vocal repertoire of Callithrix jacchus. Int J Primatol 29:671–701. 10.1007/s10764-008-9250-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Block W, Träber F, von Widdern O, Metten M, Schild H, Maier W, Zobel A, Jessen F (2009) Proton MR spectroscopy of the hippocampus at 3 T in patients with unipolar major depressive disorder: correlates and predictors of treatment response. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 12:415–422. 10.1017/S1461145708009516 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boutcher SH, Stocker D (1996) Cardiovascular response of young and older males to mental challenge. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 51:P261–P267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braesicke K, Parkinson JA, Reekie Y, Man MS, Hopewell L, Pears A, Crofts H, Schnell CR, Roberts AC (2005) Autonomic arousal in an appetitive context in primates: a behavioural and neural analysis. Eur J Neurosci 21:1733–1740. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03987.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brindle RC, Ginty AT, Conklin SM (2013) Is the association between depression and blunted cardiovascular stress reactions mediated by perceptions of stress? Int J Psychophysiol 90:66–72. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruijn JA, Moleman P, van den Broek WW, Mulder PG (2001) Trait anxiety and the effect of a single high dose of diazepam in unipolar depression. J Psychiatr Res 35:331–337. 10.1016/S0022-3956(01)00035-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burman KJ, Rosa MG (2009) Architectural subdivisions of medial and orbital frontal cortices in the marmoset monkey (Callithrix jacchus). J Comp Neurol 514:11–29. 10.1002/cne.21976 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardinal RN, Aitken MR (2010) Whisker: a client-server high-performance multimedia research control system. Behav Res Methods 42:1059–1071. 10.3758/BRM.42.4.1059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carey GJ, Costall B, Domeney AM, Jones DN, Naylor RJ (1992) Behavioural effects of anxiogenic agents in the common marmoset. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 42:143–153. 10.1016/0091-3057(92)90458-R [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carroll D, Ginty AT, Whittaker AC, Lovallo WR, de Rooij SR (2017) The behavioural, cognitive, and neural corollaries of blunted cardiovascular and cortisol reactions to acute psychological stress. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 77:74–86. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chudasama Y, Wright KS, Murray EA (2008) Hippocampal lesions in Rhesus monkeys disrupt emotional responses but not reinforcer devaluation effects. Biol Psychiatry 63:1084–1091. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.11.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chudasama Y, Izquierdo A, Murray EA (2009) Distinct contributions of the amygdala and hippocampus to fear expression. Eur J Neurosci 30:2327–2337. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.07012.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clarke HF, Dalley JW, Crofts HS, Robbins TW, Roberts AC (2004) Cognitive inflexibility after prefrontal serotonin depletion. Science 304:878–880. 10.1126/science.1094987 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Critchley HD, Nagai Y, Gray MA, Mathias CJ (2011) Dissecting axes of autonomic control in humans: Insights from neuroimaging. Auton Neurosci 161:34–42. 10.1016/j.autneu.2010.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Diego-Adeliño J, Portella MJ, Gómez-Ansón B, López-Moruelo O, Serra-Blasco M, Vives Y, Puigdemont D, Pérez-Egea R, Álvarez E, Pérez V (2013) Hippocampal abnormalities of glutamate/glutamine, N-acetylaspartate and choline in patients with depression are related to past illness burden. J Psychiatry Neurosci 38:107–116. 10.1503/jpn.110185 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Geus EJ, van Doornen LJ, de Visser DC, Orlebeke JF (1990) Existing and training induced differences in aerobic fitness: their relationship to physiological response patterns during different types of stress. Psychophysiology 27:457–478. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1990.tb02343.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delli Pizzi S, Padulo C, Brancucci A, Bubbico G, Edden RA, Ferretti A, Franciotti R, Manippa V, Marzoli D, Onofrj M, Sepede G, Tartaro A, Tommasi L, Puglisi-Allegra S, Bonanni L (2016) GABA content within the ventromedial prefrontal cortex is related to trait anxiety. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 11:758–766. 10.1093/scan/nsv155 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickie EW, Brunet A, Akerib V, Armony JL (2011) Neural correlates of recovery from post-traumatic stress disorder: a longitudinal fMRI investigation of memory encoding. Neuropsychologia 49:1771–1778. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duric V, Banasr M, Stockmeier CA, Simen AA, Newton SS, Overholser JC, Jurjus GJ, Dieter L, Duman RS (2013) Altered expression of synapse and glutamate related genes in post-mortem hippocampus of depressed subjects. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 16:69–82. 10.1017/S1461145712000016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox AS, Shelton SE, Oakes TR, Converse AK, Davidson RJ, Kalin NH (2010) Orbitofrontal cortex lesions alter anxiety-related activity in the primate bed nucleus of stria terminalis. J Neurosci 30:7023–7027. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5952-09.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garcia-Garcia AL, Elizalde N, Matrov D, Harro J, Wojcik SM, Venzala E, Ramírez MJ, Del Rio J, Tordera RM (2009) Increased vulnerability to depressive-like behavior of mice with decreased expression of VGLUT1. Biol Psychiatry 66:275–282. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gianaros PJ, Van Der Veen FM, Jennings JR (2004) Regional cerebral blood flow correlates with heart period and high-frequency heart period variability during working-memory tasks: Implications for the cortical and subcortical regulation of cardiac autonomic activity. Psychophysiology 41:521–530. 10.1111/1469-8986.2004.00179.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ginty AT, Conklin SM (2011) High perceived stress in relation to life events is associated with blunted cardiac reactivity. Biol Psychol 86:383–385. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ginty AT, Brindle RC, Carroll D (2015) Cardiac stress reactions and perseverance: Diminished reactivity is associated with study non-completion. Biol Psychol 109:200–205. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.06.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton JP, Chen G, Thomason ME, Schwartz ME, Gotlib IH (2011) Investigating neural primacy in major depressive disorder: multivariate Granger causality analysis of resting-state fMRI time-series data. Mol Psychiatry 16:763–772. 10.1038/mp.2010.46 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heilbronner SR, Rodriguez-Romaguera J, Quirk GJ, Groenewegen HJ, Haber SN (2016) Circuit-based corticostriatal homologies between rat and primate. Biol Psychiatry 80:509–521. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.05.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim WB, Cho JH (2017) Synaptic targeting of double-projecting ventral CA1 hippocampal neurons to the medial prefrontal cortex and basal amygdala. J Neurosci 37:4868–4882. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3579-16.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirkby LA, Luongo FJ, Lee MB, Nahum M, Van Vleet TM, Rao VR, Dawes HE, Chang EF, Sohal VS (2018) An amygdala-hippocampus subnetwork that encodes variation in human mood. Cell 175:1688–1700.e14. 10.1016/j.cell.2018.10.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuntze LB, Ferreira-Junior NC, Lagatta DC, Resstel LB (2016) Ventral hippocampus modulates bradycardic response to peripheral chemoreflex activation in awake rats. Exp Physiol 101:482–493. 10.1113/EP085393 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lang PJ, McTeague LM, Bradley MM (2014) Pathological anxiety and function/dysfunction in the brain's fear/defense circuitry. Restor Neurol Neurosci 32:63–77. 10.3233/RNN-139012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lang PJ, McTeague LM, Bradley MM (2016) RDoC, DSM, and the reflex physiology of fear: a biodimensional analysis of the anxiety disorders spectrum. Psychophysiology 53:336–347. 10.1111/psyp.12462 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Law AJ, Pei Q, Walker M, Gordon-Andrews H, Weickert CS, Feldon J, Pryce CR, Harrison PJ (2009) Early parental deprivation in the marmoset monkey produces long-term changes in hippocampal expression of genes involved in synaptic plasticity and implicated in mood disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 34:1381–1394. 10.1038/npp.2008.106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeDoux JE. (2000) Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 23:155–184. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levine JC, Fleming R, Piedmont JI, Cain SM, Chen WJ (2016) Heart rate variability and generalized anxiety disorder during laboratory-induced worry and aversive imagery. J Affect Disord 205:207–215. 10.1016/j.jad.2016.07.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malykhin NV, Coupland NJ (2015) Hippocampal neuroplasticity in major depressive disorder. Neuroscience 309:200–213. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.04.047 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marrocco J, Mairesse J, Ngomba RT, Silletti V, Van Camp G, Bouwalerh H, Summa M, Pittaluga A, Nicoletti F, Maccari S, Morley-Fletcher S (2012) Anxiety-like behavior of prenatally stressed rats is associated with a selective reduction of glutamate release in the ventral hippocampus. J Neurosci 32:17143–17154. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1040-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marrocco J, Reynaert ML, Gatta E, Gabriel C, Mocaër E, Di Prisco S, Merega E, Pittaluga A, Nicoletti F, Maccari S, Morley-Fletcher S, Mairesse J (2014) The effects of antidepressant treatment in prenatally stressed rats support the glutamatergic hypothesis of stress-related disorders. J Neurosci 34:2015–2024. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4131-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mathews A, Richards A, Eysenck M (1989) Interpretation of homophones related to threat in anxiety states. J Abnorm Psychol 98:31–34. 10.1037/0021-843X.98.1.31 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mikheenko Y, Man MS, Braesicke K, Johns ME, Hill G, Agustín-Pavón C, Roberts AC (2010) Autonomic, behavioral, and neural analyses of mild conditioned negative affect in marmosets. Behav Neurosci 124:192–203. 10.1037/a0018868 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mikheenko Y, Shiba Y, Sawiak S, Braesicke K, Cockcroft G, Clarke H, Roberts AC (2015) Serotonergic, brain volume and attentional correlates of trait anxiety in primates. Neuropsychopharmacology 40:1395–1404. 10.1038/npp.2014.324 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller-Ribeiro FC, Zaretsky DV, Zaretskaia MV, Santos RA, DiMicco JA, Fontes MA (2012) Contribution of infralimbic cortex in the cardiovascular response to acute stress. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 303:R639–R650. 10.1152/ajpregu.00573.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neafsey EJ, Terreberry KM, Hurley KM, Ruit KG, Frysztak RJ (1993) Anterior cingulate cortex in rodents: connections, visceral control functions, and implications for emotion. In: Neurobiology of cingulate cortex and limbic thalamus: a comprehensive handbook (Vogt BA, Gabriel M, eds), pp 206–233. Boston: Birkhäuser. [Google Scholar]
- Oler JA, Fox AS, Shelton SE, Christian BT, Murali D, Oakes TR, Davidson RJ, Kalin NH (2009) Serotonin transporter availability in the amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis predicts anxious temperament and brain glucose metabolic activity. J Neurosci 29:9961–9966. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0795-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Padilla-Coreano N, Bolkan SS, Pierce GM, Blackman DR, Hardin WD, Garcia-Garcia AL, Spellman TJ, Gordon JA (2016) Direct ventral hippocampal-prefrontal input is required for anxiety-related neural activity and behavior. Neuron 89:857–866. 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pagani M, Montano N, Porta A, Malliani A, Abboud FM, Birkett C, Somers VK (1997) Relationship between spectral components of cardiovascular variabilities and direct measures of muscle sympathetic nerve activity in humans. Circulation 95:1441–1448. 10.1161/01.CIR.95.6.1441 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parvizi J, Rangarajan V, Shirer WR, Desai N, Greicius MD (2013) The will to persevere induced by electrical stimulation of the human cingulate gyrus. Neuron 80:1359–1367. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team (2016) R: A Language and Environment For Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
- Rahman F, Pechnik S, Gross D, Sewell L, Goldstein DS (2011) Low frequency power of heart rate variability reflects baroreflex function, not cardiac sympathetic innervation. Clin Auton Res 21:133–141. 10.1007/s10286-010-0098-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts AC, Tomic DL, Parkinson CH, Roeling TA, Cutter DJ, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ (2007) Forebrain connectivity of the prefrontal cortex in the marmoset monkey (Callithrix jacchus): an anterograde and retrograde tract-tracing study. J Comp Neurol 502:86–112. 10.1002/cne.21300 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruit KG, Neafsey EJ (1990) Hippocampal input to a “visceral motor” corticobulbar pathway: an anatomical and electrophysiological study in the rat. Exp Brain Res 82:606–616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santangelo AM, Ito M, Shiba Y, Clarke HF, Schut EH, Cockcroft G, Ferguson-Smith AC, Roberts AC (2016) Novel primate model of serotonin transporter genetic polymorphisms associated with gene expression, anxiety and sensitivity to antidepressants. Neuropsychopharmacology 41:2366–2376. 10.1038/npp.2016.41 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shiba Y, Santangelo AM, Braesicke K, Agustín-Pavón C, Cockcroft G, Haggard M, Roberts AC (2014) Individual differences in behavioral and cardiovascular reactivity to emotive stimuli and their relationship to cognitive flexibility in a primate model of trait anxiety. Front Behav Neurosci 8:137. 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00137 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sierra-Mercado D, Padilla-Coreano N, Quirk GJ (2011) Dissociable roles of prelimbic and infralimbic cortices, ventral hippocampus, and basolateral amygdala in the expression and extinction of conditioned fear. Neuropsychopharmacology 36:529–538. 10.1038/npp.2010.184 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sotres-Bayon F, Sierra-Mercado D, Pardilla-Delgado E, Quirk GJ (2012) Gating of fear in prelimbic cortex by hippocampal and amygdala inputs. Neuron 76:804–812. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Souza GG, Mendonça-de-Souza AC, Duarte AF, Fischer NL, Souza WF, Coutinho ES, Figueira I, Volchan E (2015) Blunted cardiac reactivity to psychological stress associated with higher trait anxiety: a study in peacekeepers. BMC Neurosci 16:81. 10.1186/s12868-015-0216-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stapelberg NJ, Neumann DL, Shum DH, McConnell H, Hamilton-Craig I (2011) A topographical map of the causal network of mechanisms underlying the relationship between major depressive disorder and coronary heart disease. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 45:351–369. 10.3109/00048674.2011.570427 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stapelberg NJ, Hamilton-Craig I, Neumann DL, Shum DH, McConnell H (2012) Mind and heart: heart rate variability in major depressive disorder and coronary heart disease - a review and recommendations. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 46:946–957. 10.1177/0004867412444624 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tang AC, Zou B, Reeb BC, Connor JA (2008) An epigenetic induction of a right-shift in hippocampal asymmetry: Selectivity for short- and long-term potentiation but not post-tetanic potentiation. Hippocampus 18:5–10. 10.1002/hipo.20370 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toichi M, Sugiura T, Murai T, Sengoku A (1997) A new method of assessing cardiac autonomic function and its comparison with spectral analysis and coefficient of variation of R-R interval. J Auton Nerv Syst 62:79–84. 10.1016/S0165-1838(96)00112-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tordera RM, Totterdell S, Wojcik SM, Brose N, Elizalde N, Lasheras B, Del Rio J (2007) Enhanced anxiety, depressive-like behaviour and impaired recognition memory in mice with reduced expression of the vesicular glutamate transporter 1 (VGLUT1). Eur J Neurosci 25:281–290. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05259.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Treadway MT, Waskom ML, Dillon DG, Holmes AJ, Park MTM, Chakravarty MM, Dutra SJ, Polli FE, Iosifescu DV, Fava M, Gabrieli JDE, Pizzagalli DA (2015) Illness progression, recent stress, and morphometry of hippocampal subfields and medial prefrontal cortex in major depression. Biol Psychiatry 77:285–294. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.06.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsai TH, Matthews K, Dalley JW (1997) Determination of glutamate in rat brain microdialysates by microbore liquid chromatography with electrochemical detection. J Liq Chrom Rel Technol 20:3039–3047. 10.1080/10826079708006579 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Verstynen T, Tierney R, Urbanski T, Tang A (2001) Neonatal novelty exposure modulates hippocampal volumetric asymmetry in the rat. Neuroreport 12:3019–3022. 10.1097/00001756-200110080-00008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vertes RP. (2006) Interactions among the medial prefrontal cortex, hippocampus and midline thalamus in emotional and cognitive processing in the rat. Neuroscience 142:1–20. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.06.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogt BA, Paxinos G (2014) Cytoarchitecture of mouse and rat cingulate cortex with human homologies. Brain Struct Funct 219:185–192. 10.1007/s00429-012-0493-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallis CU, Cardinal RN, Alexander L, Roberts AC, Clarke HF (2017) Opposing roles of primate areas 25 and 32 and their putative rodent homologs in the regulation of negative emotion. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 114:E4075–E4084. 10.1073/pnas.1620115114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weger M, Sandi C (2018) High anxiety trait: A vulnerable phenotype for stress-induced depression. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 87:27–37. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.01.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- West EA, DesJardin JT, Gale K, Malkova L (2011) Transient inactivation of orbitofrontal cortex blocks reinforcer devaluation in macaques. J Neurosci 31:15128–15135. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3295-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]