We appreciate the thoughtful response to our recent article.1 In particular, we were pleased to see other researchers dedicating their efforts to understanding suicidality in young children, a topic worthy of further investigation. Moreover, we were interested to hear of a replication of our findings in a similar sample of 330 young children, many of whom had experienced trauma, and we believe this further supports the importance of assessing suicidal cognitions and behaviors in young children. Although Dr. Scheeringa highlights a relatively smaller proportion of children endorsing suicidal thoughts in that sample, we believe that the presence of any suicidal thoughts in children younger than 7 years is alarming and worthy of further attention and investigation.
The letter raises an important concern about the distinction between suicidal cognitions that are out of the norm and developmentally appropriate curiosity about death. This is an extraordinarily relevant comment, particularly when examining suicidality in young children. Based on the procedures we used in our study, we believe that the measure of suicidal cognitions reflects non-normative and clinically relevant phenomena. To further clarify our basis for considering suicidal cognitions (as measured in this study) to be clinically relevant, we provide more detail about our psychiatric interviews and coding of suicidal cognitions and behaviors, and we describe unpublished data from a new sample of treatment-seeking young children with depression (3–6 years old) that also highlight the importance of assessing suicidal cognitions and behaviors in young children. Furthermore, we hope that this response and preliminary findings serve as a platform for continuing research that aims to further investigate the meaning and origins of suicidal thoughts and behaviors expressed by children this young.
A modified version of the Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) module of the Preschool-Age Psychiatric Assessment (PAPA)2 and the clinician-rated Preschool and Early-Childhood Functional Assessment Scale (PECFAS)3 were used to create our suicidal cognition and behavior (SI) variable. In this modified version of the MDD module of the PAPA, normative thoughts about death or dying were included as a subclinical code (e.g., 1 on a 0–3 scale), and extensive training was done on these distinctions. In addition, after being trained to reliability on the 2 interviews, a subsample of PAPA interviews was calibrated in an ongoing fidelity-and-review process with a master coder and the principal investigator (Luby). Therefore, we are quite confident that normative statements and behaviors related to death and/or dying were not deemed clinically significant and were not included in the creation of our SI variable.
Dr. Scheeringa also notes a seeming inconsistency in the association between SI and symptoms of psychopathology in our study.1 In particular, he draws attention to the fact that the SI variable was not related to concurrent symptoms of depression in children 3 to 7 years old. However, this analysis included a wide array of covariates, such as gender, family income-to-needs ratio, maternal psychopathology, and co-occurring disorders (e.g., attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, and anxiety). When these covariates are removed, depression during 3 to 7 years of age does demonstrate zero-order associations with SI (Table 2 in article), because 76% of young children in our sample who reported SI had a diagnosis of depression. Although we agree that the findings containing the covariates are unexpected, we also note that SI during 3 to 7 years of age was associated with externalizing disorders (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, and conduct disorder) during the same period. Because so little research is available to inform interpretations of these findings, we believe that SI during 3 to 7 years of age could serve as a nonspecific signal of distress that might not be related to any particular diagnosis but rather to other more general environmental stressors. Again, additional research is clearly needed to confirm these hypotheses and help determine what a young child’s suicidal cognitions and behaviors actually mean.
We fully agree with Dr. Scheeringa that the meaning of preschool-onset SI is unclear, yet we believe, based on our coding process, that the thoughts and behaviors observed in our study were not normative. We are currently designing an assessment that aims to address some of these critical questions on the meaning and risk of SI in young children. Specifically, it will be important for future research to assess the meaning behind SI by asking children specific and detailed questions about their understanding of death and dying and by investigating other key psychosocial correlates and exposures. For example, whether a family member has expressed SI or whether they are exposed to media in which SI is occurring will be crucial for future studies to assess.
Although it is not feasible for us to conduct these types of interviews with participants from our previously published data (which were based on parent report and clinician ratings of child SI), we intend to collect this information in the aforementioned sample of treatment-seeking children 3 to 6 years old. To date, 169 children have been enrolled in a treatment study for early-onset depression that is still ongoing. Each of these children and their primary caregivers completed a PAPA MDD module and a preschool version of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children (K-SADS)4 with clinicians trained to reliability. Using the parallel questions from the K-SADS-Early Childhood, an alarming 20.7% (n = 35) of preschoolers with depression whose caregivers sought participation in our treatment study met the clinical threshold for recurrent thoughts about death during the previous month, and 6.5% (n = 11) met the clinical threshold for suicidal ideation in the previous month. Four preschoolers engaged in suicidal gestures within the previous month, before enrollment in this study, including trying to choke himself or herself, placing robes/belts/ scarves/toys around his or her neck, and running into the street stating an intent to die and wishing to get hit by a car.
By using multiple assessment methods and asking children detailed follow-up questions about their understanding of death, we aim to address some of the difficulties inherent in this type of research and provide insight into the significant ambiguities that remain regarding the nature and course of preschool-onset SI.
Contributor Information
Diana J. Whalen, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis whalend@psychiatry.wustl.edu.
Andrew C. Belden, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis.
Joan L. Luby, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis.
Deanna Barch, Washington University School of Medicine and Washington University, St. Louis.
Katherine Dixon-Gordon, University of Massachusetts Amherst.
REFERENCES
- 1.Whalen DJ, Dixon-Gordon K, Belden AC, Barch D, Luby JL. Correlates and consequences of suicidal cognitions and behaviors in children ages 3 to 7 years. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015;54:926–937.e2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Egger HL, Ascher B, Angold A. The Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment: Version 1.4. Center for Developmental Epidemiology, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences. Durham, NC: Duke University Medical Center; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Hodges K PECFAS Self-Training Manual and Blank Scoring Form. Ann Arbor, MI: Author; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Gaffrey M, Luby J. Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for Early Childhood (K-SADS-EC). St. Louis: Washington University School of Medicine; 2012. [Google Scholar]