Skip to main content
UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts logoLink to UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Apr 26.
Published in final edited form as: Pediatrics. 2009 Feb;123(2):e289–e296. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-1861

Binge pattern of alcohol consumption during pregnancy and childhood mental health outcomes: longitudinal population-based study

Kapil Sayal a,*, Jon Heron b, Jean Golding c, Rosa Alati d, George Davey Smith b, Ron Gray e, Alan Emond c
PMCID: PMC6485410  EMSID: EMS28757  PMID: 19171582

Abstract

Objective

Patterns of alcohol consumption during pregnancy such as episodes of binge drinking may be as important as average levels of consumption in conferring risk for later childhood mental health and learning problems. However, it can be difficult to distinguish risk resulting from episodic or regular background levels of drinking. This large study investigates whether patterns of alcohol consumption are independently associated with child mental health and cognitive outcomes, whether there are gender differences in risk and whether occasional episodes of higher levels of drinking carry any risk in the absence of regular daily drinking during pregnancy.

Methods

This prospective, population-based study used data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). We investigated the relationships between a binge pattern of alcohol use (consumption of ≥4 drinks in a day) in the second and third trimesters of pregnancy and childhood mental health problems at 47 and 81 months (n = 6355 and 5599 respectively). In a sub-group, we also investigated their relationships with child IQ at 49 months (n=924).

Results

After controlling for a range of prenatal and postnatal factors, any episodes of consuming ≥4 drinks in a day were independently associated with higher risks for mental health problems (especially hyperactivity/inattention) in girls at the age of 47 months and in both genders at 81 months. There was no association with IQ scores at 49 months after adjustment for confounders. The consumption of ≥4 drinks in a day continued to carry risk for mental health problems (especially hyperactivity/inattention) in the absence of regular daily drinking.

Conclusions

The consumption of ≥4 drinks in a day on an occasional basis during pregnancy may increase risk for child mental health problems in the absence of moderate daily levels of drinking. The main risks appear to relate to hyperactivity and inattention problems.

Keywords: pregnancy, prenatal alcohol exposure, alcohol drinking, fetal alcohol spectrum disorder, mental health problems, hyperactivity, IQ, ALSPAC


Heavy levels of alcohol consumption during pregnancy are associated with adverse effects such as fetal alcohol syndrome. In contrast, there are mixed findings as to whether moderate (1-2 drinks/day) average levels of alcohol consumption during pregnancy are associated with childhood learning and mental health problems.1-10 These discrepancies may reflect that intermittent episodes of heavier drinking such as binge episodes are obscured when investigating average levels. Drinking patterns may be as important as total amounts consumed in conferring risk for childhood behavioral and learning problems.11-16 However, these findings are also inconsistent which may reflect the choice and size of the sample, the timing and measurement of the exposure and outcomes, and limited measures of the range of potential confounders of the association between maternal drinking behavior and children's mental health.10,17-19

A recent systematic review has highlighted the need for research that distinguishes between adverse neurodevelopmental effects from intermittent binge drinking during pregnancy from those relating to regular background levels of drinking.20 For women, four drinks per occasion (equivalent to 5-6 UK units) is defined by the NIAAA as a ‘binge’ drinking pattern.21 Studies that have separated out women who binge drink and/or drink daily or heavily have been hindered by small sample sizes and possible selection bias.17,20 In a large representative population sample, we examine whether patterns of alcohol consumption during pregnancy are associated with child mental health and cognitive outcomes. Given our recent findings suggesting a possible greater vulnerability in girls from occasional drinking during pregnancy,22 we also investigate gender differences in risk and whether episodes of heavy drinking carry risk in the absence of regular daily drinking.

METHODS

Sample

The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) is a prospective population-based study in England.23 All pregnant women in the Avon area with an expected delivery date between April 1991 and December 1992 were invited to take part. Approximately 85% (n=14541) of eligible women participated. Participants were broadly representative of the local population of mothers with infants and comparable against national census data, although were slightly more likely to be Caucasian, married or cohabiting, and home owner-occupiers (further details at http://www.alspac.bris.ac.uk).22 Detailed information has been obtained at regular intervals during the pregnancy and since the birth. In addition, a 10% sub-sample of children (‘Children in Focus’) born within the last six months of the survey have been seen in a research clinic for more detailed assessments. Ethical approval was obtained from the Local Research Ethics and ALSPAC Ethics and Law Advisory Committees.

This paper is based on children from singleton births (to minimise clustering effects) alive at one year of age (n=13617). Through postal questionnaires, information on alcohol use at 18 weeks gestation was provided by 93% (n=12630) of mothers. Of these respondents, 65% (n=8240) provided additional information at 32 weeks gestation. This discrepancy reflects 3460 (27%) mothers who were not asked these questions in the initial version of the 32 week questionnaire and missing data in 930 (7%) mothers. There appeared to be no sampling bias involving mothers not asked these questions (table 1) but non-response was associated with earlier alcohol use and measured confounder variables (outlined below).

Table 1.

Relationships between prenatal and child factors and availability of alcohol consumption data at 32 weeks

Data
available
n = 8240
Not
asked
n = 3460
Missing
data
n = 930
χ2
(2 d.f.)
p value
Prenatal Factors
Maternal age (≥35 years) 11% 11% 6% 19.64 <0.001
Any smoking 19%b 19% 35% 150.85 <0.001
Cannabis use 2% 2% 5% 28.70 <0.001
Illicit drug use 0.4% 0.5% 0.9% 3.60 0.166
Parity (≥ 1) 56% 54% 59% 5.92 0.052
Highest maternal education
(‘O’ level or above)
71% 71% 36% 27.57 <0.001
Own home 76% 75% 47% 298.55 <0.001
Currently married 77% 78% 56% 169.06 <0.001
Maternal depression 13% 15% 21% 49.18 <0.001
≥ 4 drinks at 14-18 weeks 16% 18% 23% 24.54 <0.001
Child Factors
Gestational age (≤36 weeks) 4% 5% 10% 48.28 <0.001
Ethnicity (non-white) 4% 5% 5% 3.28 0.194
Gender (male) 51% 52% 53% 0.77 0.679
Birth weight (kg) 3.44
(0.52)
3.42
(0.54)
3.28
(0.62)
a <0.001

% or mean (s.d.)

a

F statistic, F = 35.81

b

Daily frequency of smoking during pregnancy by mothers who provided alcohol consumption data at 32 weeks: 81.4% none, 3.6% 1-4, 4.7% 5-9, 4.4% 10-14, 2.7% 15-19, 2.1% 20-24, 0.5% 25-29, and 0.2% ≥30 times (data missing in 0.3%).

Measures

Exposure variables

Alcohol consumption during pregnancy was measured in two ways:

  • 1) Pattern of drinking (primary exposure variable) – At both 18 and 32 weeks gestation, the mother was asked the number of days in the previous four weeks on which she had consumed at least four units of alcohol. Examples specified that one drink was equivalent to one unit (8 grams) of alcohol. Response categories were 0, 1-2, 3-4, 5-10, or >10 days. For the analyses, we compared any days versus none.

  • 2) Frequency and Quantity of drinking (termed ‘frequency’ in the paper) - At 18 weeks, the mother was asked her frequency and amount of drinking during the previous two weeks or around the time she first felt the baby move. Response categories were never, <1 glass per week, ≥1 glass per week, 1-2 glasses a day, 3-9 glasses a day or ≥10 glasses a day. At 32 weeks, she was asked the amount she usually drinks per day at present. We defined daily drinking as an average of ≥1 drink a day at either time point.

Information on alcohol consumption at other times was also obtained:

  • 1) Pre-pregnancy - At 18 weeks gestation, the mother was asked about her usual drinking before the pregnancy. Response categories were the same as the frequency options described above.

  • 2) First trimester – This information was also collected at 18 weeks using the same frequency options. Based on our previous work, the groups consuming ≥1 glasses per week were combined.22

  • 3) Postnatal - When the child was aged 47 months (the primary outcome point), the mother was asked about her drinking during the previous week. Her maximum daily consumption was used to indicate postnatal episodic heavy drinking.

Outcome variables

  • 1) Child mental health was measured using the parent-completed Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) at 47 and 81 months.24 This widely used measure has been validated in a large community sample.25 It is a dimensional measure with sub-scales (0-10) for hyperactivity/inattention, conduct problems, emotional symptoms, and peer relationships. Our analyses focused on the total score (0-40), conduct problems and hyperactivity/inattention.

  • 2) Amongst the ‘Children in Focus’ group, cognitive outcomes were measured at 49 months using Intelligence Quotient (IQ) data from the Wechsler Pre-school and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI).26 The assessments were administered individually by trained psychology assistants who were blind to the exposure. Age-standardised scores were used to calculate performance, verbal and total IQ scores.

Confounder variables

Potential confounding factors associated with alcohol consumption and childhood mental health and learning outcomes that were measured in ALSPAC were included in the analyses. Maternal and socio-demographic variables obtained during pregnancy were categorised for analysis: maternal age (≤20, 21-34, or ≥35 years); parity (none or ≥1); highest level of maternal education (‘O’ levels or not); daily frequency of smoking at 18 weeks gestation (response categories were 0, 1-4, 5-9, 10-14, 15-19, 20-24, 25-29, and ≥30 times); and use of cannabis and other illicit drugs in pregnancy, home ownership and whether currently married (all yes/no). Maternal mental health was measured at 18 weeks gestation and when the child was 33 months old using the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS).27 High scores (>12) are highly associated with a diagnosis of a depressive disorder.28 Child factors included gestational age (≤36 or ≥37 weeks), birth weight, gender and ethnicity.

Analysis

The main focus was the associations between the consumption of ≥4 drinks in a day (exposure) at either 14-18 or 28-32 weeks gestation and child SDQ at 47 months. The sample consisted of 8240 children whose mothers provided information on alcohol use at both 18 and 32 weeks gestation. The analyses involved the following steps:

  • 1) We identified the relationships between the exposure and confounder variables (table 2). To check for possible selection bias at 47 months, we also examined whether SDQ response status was associated with the exposure and confounder variables.

  • 2) We explored the relationships between pre-pregnancy daily alcohol use and SDQ scores using multivariable linear regression analyses, adjusting for the confounder variables, to provide adjusted regression coefficients (as per previous investigations using the SDQ).29 These quantified the level of possible risk associated with background alcohol consumption as opposed to the risk from intra-uterine exposure.

  • 3) The univariable relationships between the main exposure (≥4 drinks in a day during pregnancy) and SDQ scores were examined before adjusting for the confounder variables to provide adjusted regression coefficients. Assessment was made for gender interaction within the unadjusted models before univariable and multivariable linear regression analyses were repeated separately for each gender (table 3).

  • 4) Three sets of sensitivity analyses were performed. First, given our previous findings, the analyses were repeated after adjusting for first trimester alcohol consumption.22 Second, as the child's birth weight might be on the causal pathway between prenatal alcohol exposure and neurodevelopmental problems, the analyses were repeated after omitting birth weight from the model. Third, the analyses were repeated after adjusting for risk factors in the postnatal environment (EPDS at 33 months and maximum alcohol consumption at 47 months).

  • 5) To examine whether episodes of heavier drinking carry risk in mothers who do not drink daily during pregnancy, the analyses in step 3 were repeated using a 4-level variable based on pattern (≥4 drinks in a day) and frequency (daily drinking) of drinking. We compared, in turn, three sub-groups of mothers (daily but not ≥4, ≥4 but not daily, ≥4 and daily) against the baseline group (not daily nor ≥4).

  • 6) Steps 2, 3, and 5 were repeated using the 81 month SDQ data to explore longer-term outcomes. Finally, linear regression analyses were repeated with the IQ data to examine neuro-developmental outcomes independent of parental report.

Table 2.

Correlates of consuming ≥4 drinks in a day during pregnancy

< 4 drinks
a day
n = 6259
≥ 4 drinks
a day
n = 1981
χ2
(1d.f.)
p
Prenatal Factors
Maternal age (≥35 years) 10% 11% 1.39 0.238
Any smoking 15% 30% 218.72 <0.001
Cannabis use 1% 4% 70.65 <0.001
Illicit drug use 0.3% 0.8% 7.99 0.005
Parity (≥ 1) 54% 61% 28.31 <0.001
Highest maternal education
(O level or above)
74% 63% 82.89 <0.001
Own home 78% 70% 62.66 <0.001
Currently married 79% 68% 101.51 <0.001
Maternal depression 11% 18% 62.83 <0.001
Child Factors
Gestational age (≤36 weeks) 5% 4% 0.00 0.951
Ethnicity (non-white) 5% 4% 0.02 0.881
Gender (male) 51% 52% 0.42 0.519
Birth weight (kg) 3.44 (0.52) 3.44 (0.54) a 0.804

% or mean (s.d.)

a

t-test, t = 0.25.

Table 3.

Relationships between drinking patterns and mean differences in 47 month SDQ

Unadjusted
(95% C.I.)
p Adjusted b
(95% C.I.)
p p for gender
interaction a
Whole sample
Conduct Problems
(0-10)
0.21 [0.13, 0.29] <0.001 0.06 [−0.03, 0.15] 0.161 0.184
Hyperactivity /
Inattention (0-10)
0.40 [0.27, 0.53] <0.001 0.25 [0.11, 0.40] 0.001 0.321
Total Problems
(0-40)
0.87 [0.61, 1.14] <0.001 0.46 [0.17, 0.74] 0.002 0.037
Boys
Conduct Problems
(0-10)
0.16 [0.04, 0.27] 0.007 0.00 [−0.12, 0.13] 0.894
Hyperactivity /
Inattention (0-10)
0.33 [0.14, 0.51] 0.001 0.18 [−0.02, 0.39] 0.079
Total Problems
(0-40)
0.59 [0.22, 0.97] 0.002 0.16 [−0.24, 0.57] 0.430
Girls
Conduct Problems
(0-10)
0.27 [0.15, 0.38] <0.001 0.13 [0.00, 0.25] 0.047
Hyperactivity /
Inattention (0-10)
0.46 [0.27, 0.65] <0.001 0.33 [0.13, 0.54] 0.002
Total Problems
(0-40)
1.15 [0.79, 1.52] <0.001 0.80 [0.40, 1.21] <0.001
a

in unadjusted model

b

Adjusted for: maternal age, parity, highest level of maternal education, daily frequency of smoking during the second trimester, use of cannabis and/or other illicit drugs in pregnancy, home ownership, whether currently married, high scores (>12) on the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale, and child gestational age, birth weight, gender and ethnicity.

RESULTS

Overall, 24% (1981/8240) of mothers reported at least one occasion during pregnancy of consuming ≥4 drinks in a day. Over half (56% and 57% at each time point) of these mothers consumed ≥4 drinks on only 1-2 days in the previous month. SDQs were available on 6355 children at 47 months and on 5599 at 81 months. Mothers who consumed ≥4 drinks were less likely than other mothers to provide SDQs at 47 months (74% vs. 78%; χ2=16.57, p<0.001) or 81 months (64% vs. 69%; χ2=23.94, p<0.001). Other maternal correlates of non-response at 47 months included younger age, higher parity, smoking, use of cannabis and other illicit drugs, depression, being unmarried, rented tenure, and lower level of education. However, amongst responders, SDQ score distributions were in keeping with expected population distributions (www.sdqinfo.com).

Relationships between alcohol consumption and outcomes

There was an interaction between child gender and alcohol exposure in relation to total problems at 47 months (table 3). In the univariable analyses, the consumption of ≥4 drinks was associated with behavioral and total problems in both genders (with effect sizes of up to 0.25 of a standard deviation). After adjustment for confounders, the associations between alcohol exposure and later problems were stronger in girls than in boys. The associations in girls remained in all three sensitivity analyses (omitting birth weight from the model and after adjustment for first trimester alcohol consumption and postnatal variables).

At 81 months, there were similar associations in the univariable analyses and no evidence of gender interactions (table 4). The associations persisted after adjustment for confounders. As pre-pregnancy daily alcohol consumption was associated with higher hyperactivity/inattention scores at 81 months (adjusted regression coefficients = 0.22, 95% C.I. 0.02 to 0.41; p=0.033), the multivariable analysis was repeated after adjusting for this. The consumption of ≥4 drinks in a day during pregnancy continued to show an association with hyperactivity/inattention scores (adjusted regression coefficients = 0.17, 0.00 to 0.33, p=0.044) and there was no longer an association with pre-pregnancy alcohol consumption, suggesting that intra-uterine exposure carried a risk for hyperactivity/inattention problems.

Table 4.

Relationships between drinking patterns and mean differences in 81 month SDQ

Unadjusted
(95% C.I.)
p Adjusted b
(95% C.I.)
p p for gender
interactiona
Conduct Problems
(0-10)
0.25 [0.16, 0.34] <0.001 0.12 [0.02, 0.22] 0.020 0.390
Hyperactivity /
Inattention (0-10)
0.37 [0.22, 0.51] <0.001 0.19 [0.04, 0.35] 0.017 0.671
Total Problems
(0-40)
0.78 [0.48, 1.07] <0.001 0.36 [0.04, 0.68] 0.026 0.594
a

in unadjusted model

b

Adjusted for: maternal age, parity, highest level of maternal education, daily frequency of smoking during the second trimester, use of cannabis and/or other illicit drugs in pregnancy, home ownership, whether currently married, high scores (>12) on the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale, and child gestational age, birth weight, gender and ethnicity.

IQ data were available on 924 children. Compared to the wider sample, maternal correlates of non-availability of IQ data included smoking (but not alcohol consumption), depression, being unmarried, rented tenure, and lower level of education. In the univariable analyses, the consumption of ≥4 drinks was associated with lower IQ scores (table 5). The effect sizes were similar to those involving the SDQ although the associations did not remain in the multivariable analyses.

Table 5.

Relationships between drinking patterns and mean differences in IQ

Unadjusted
(95% C.I.)
p Adjusted a
(95% C.I.)
p
Performance I.Q. −3.05 [−0.80, −5.30] 0.008 −1.92 [0.38, −4.22] 0.101
Verbal I.Q. −2.57 [−0.50, −4.64] 0.015 −1.54 [0.57, −3.64] 0.153
Total I.Q. −3.18 [−1.02, −5.35] 0.004 −1.96 [0.22, −4.14] 0.077
a

Adjusted for: maternal age, parity, highest level of maternal education, daily frequency of smoking during the second trimester, use of cannabis and/or other illicit drugs in pregnancy, home ownership, whether currently married, high scores (>12) on the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale, and child gestational age, birth weight, gender and ethnicity.

Relationships between patterns and frequency of drinking and outcomes

At either the 18 or 32 week time point, 9% (589/6563) of mothers reported drinking daily in the preceding month. When comparing the ‘≥4 but not daily’ sub-group with the baseline group (not daily nor ≥4), there was an association with higher hyperactivity/inattention scores at both time points after adjustment for confounders (table 6). There was also an association with greater total problems in girls at 47 months. These findings suggest that the risk from ≥4 drinks on one or more days during pregnancy was not contingent upon daily drinking. The ‘≥4 and daily’ sub-group was small which may have precluded the demonstration of associations although there was a consistent pattern at 81 months involving the largest regression coefficients when compared against the baseline group.

Table 6.

Relationships between drinking patterns and mean differences in SDQ scores in the presence or absence of daily drinking

Unadjusted
(95% C.I.)
p Adjusted
(95% C.I.)
p
47 months (Boys)
Conduct Problems
 Daily but not 4+ −0.06 [−0.37, 0.25] 0.708 −0.06 [−0.38, 0.27] 0.724
 4+ but not daily 0.20 [0.05, 0.36] 0.010 0.03 [−0.14, 0.19] 0.748
 4+ and daily 0.17 [−0.06, 0.41] 0.145 −0.01 [−0.27, 0.24] 0.931
Hyperactivity / Inattention
 Daily but not 4+ −0.10 [−0.62, 0.42] 0.711 0.00 [−0.54, 0.54] 0.998
 4+ but not daily 0.48 [0.23, 0.73] <0.001 0.34 [0.07, 0.61] 0.015
 4+ and daily 0.27 [−0.12, 0.65] 0.176 0.01 [−0.42, 0.43] 0.983
Total Problems
 Daily but not 4+ −0.85 [−1.39, 0.68] 0.502 −0.22 [−1.27, 0.84] 0.689
 4+ but not daily 0.77 [0.27, 1.28] 0.003 0.28 [−0.25, 0.81] 0.304
 4+ and daily 0.88 [0.12, 1.65] 0.024 0.24 [−0.59, 1.06] 0.577
47 months (Girls)
Conduct Problems
 Daily but not 4+ 0.00 [−0.27, 0.28] 0.988 0.03 [−0.26, 0.33] 0.820
 4+ but not daily 0.26 [0.11, 0.41] 0.001 0.12 [−0.05, 0.28] 0.158
 4+ and daily 0.15 [−0.08, 0.39] 0.204 0.05 [−0.21, 0.30] 0.710
Hyperactivity / Inattention
 Daily but not 4+ −0.05 [−0.51, 0.40] 0.826 0.09 [−0.40, 0.57] 0.717
 4+ but not daily 0.70 [0.46, 0.95] <0.001 0.53 [0.27, 0.80] <0.001
 4+ and daily −0.01 [−0.40, 0.38] 0.967 −0.14 [−0.57, 0.28] 0.505
Total Problems
 Daily but not 4+ −0.28 [−1.17, 0.60] 0.533 0.03 [−0.92, 0.97] 0.953
 4+ but not daily 1.52 [1.04, 1.99] <0.001 1.10 [0.58, 1.61] <0.001
 4+ and daily 0.23 [−0.53, 0.98] 0.557 −0.17 [−0.99, 0.65] 0.686
81 months
Conduct Problems
 Daily but not 4+ 0.09 [−0.13, 0.32] 0.421 0.03 [−0.21, 0.26] 0.837
 4+ but not daily 0.26 [0.14, 0.38] <0.001 0.12 [−0.01, 0.26] 0.064
 4+ and daily 0.27 [0.08, 0.45] 0.005 0.17 [−0.04, 0.37] 0.107
Hyperactivity / Inattention
 Daily but not 4+ −0.01 [−0.37, 0.36] 0.963 0.09 [−0.29, 0.46] 0.655
 4+ but not daily 0.44 [0.25, 0.64] <0.001 0.29 [0.08, 0.50] 0.007
 4+ and daily 0.54 [0.24, 0.84] <0.001 0.35 [0.03, 0.67] 0.032
Total Problems
 Daily but not 4+ 0.08 [−0.65, 0.81] 0.832 0.20 [−0.56, 0.96] 0.602
 4+ but not daily 0.80 [0.40, 1.19] <0.001 0.37 [−0.05, 0.79] 0.082
 4+ and daily 0.99 [0.39, 1.60] 0.001 0.56 [−0.07, 1.20] 0.083

Range is 0-10 except Total Problems (0-40)

DISCUSSION

After controlling for a range of prenatal and postnatal confounding factors, we found that the consumption of ≥4 drinks in a day during pregnancy was independently associated with greater risk of mental health problems in girls at the age of 47 months and in both genders at 81 months. The main risks involved hyperactivity and inattention problems. This episodic pattern of drinking carried risk for these outcomes in the absence of moderate levels of daily drinking.

The multivariable analyses adjusted for a wide range of factors that were potentially associated with the exposure and outcome. Sensitivity analyses suggested that the 47 month outcomes were not explained by factors in the postnatal environment (maternal depression and high levels of drinking). The combination of adverse outcomes demonstrated here reflects a mixed pattern that has been described as alcohol-related neuro-developmental disorder. Structural brain changes following prenatal alcohol exposure are consistent with possible difficulties with attention, learning and executive function.30 Our findings are in keeping with animal studies involving fetal exposure to peak levels of maternal blood alcohol with effects on neuro-development and hyperactivity.30-32

Findings are mixed about the association between prenatal alcohol exposure and ADHD.33,34 Although some research suggests a possible association, even after controlling for prenatal smoking and parental alcohol abuse or dependence, findings based on children-of-twins or differentially exposed sibling-pair designs contradict this.2,35-36 Conflicting findings might also reflect a gene-environment interaction.37 Relatively few studies have examined the relationship between binge drinking and childhood hyperactivity/inattention – some have not found an association and others have not separated out binge episodes from daily drinking.11,14,16

Findings involving binge drinking and cognitive outcomes are inconsistent, with some studies finding no effects or effects on non-verbal IQ.10,16-18 Other studies have reported persistent learning problems with particular risk from exposure to frequent binge episodes involving 5-6 drinks per occasion (40-60 grams of alcohol).8,11,13,15 After adjustment for confounders, we found no adverse effects on IQ related to four drinks per day (32 grams of alcohol). However, we found a persistent effect for hyperactivity/inattention that was not contingent upon lower IQ. Follow-up studies into adulthood have also found that episodic binge drinking during pregnancy is associated with later substance use and other psychiatric disorders.38,39

Gender differences

As in our previous investigation of the effects of occasional drinking in the first trimester, we found a greater adverse effect from episodic drinking for girls at 47 months.22 These associations remained after additionally adjusting for first trimester exposure. At 81 months, there was a similar pattern across both genders. It is not clear whether the possible mental health consequences of prenatal alcohol exposure occur earlier in girls or whether the effects are more readily demonstrable as there is less variation in SDQ scores. Animal studies also report gender differences in vulnerability to alcohol exposure40 and there is a need to better understand the pathways contributing to a possible earlier vulnerability in girls.

Methodological issues

A key strength of ALSPAC is the large dataset on a representative population with prospectively collected measures of alcohol consumption. A quarter of pregnant women reported a pattern of consuming ≥4 drinks in a day, an amount which might have been consistent with social drinking. Our prevalence figure probably underestimates this pattern of drinking as it reflects two self-reported snapshots covering eight weeks of pregnancy.41 We were unable to pinpoint any specific risk period and our findings may not generalise to binge drinking episodes at other times during pregnancy. It has been suggested that binge drinking prior to pregnancy recognition is associated with the greatest risk.8,11 This might also reflect greater prevalence of drinking at this time and increased power to demonstrate an effect. As sample attrition was greater amongst women who consumed ≥4 drinks, this may have led to an underestimate of the actual effect. In terms of measures, participants were asked the number of days rather than occasions on which they had consumed ≥4 drinks. This could have been interpreted as meaning ≥4 drinks over the course of a day rather than over a shorter time period, the latter exposing the fetus to higher peak alcohol levels. Despite adjustment for a large number of confounders relating to maternal, child, and socio-economic factors, there remains a possibility of residual confounding especially involving paternal risk factors and postnatal factors such as parental episodic binge drinking, family dysfunction, and parental mental health problems (including hyperactivity/inattention).

Clinical Implications

Although the adjusted regression coefficients and effects sizes were small, these were of a similar magnitude to other environmental risk factors for child mental health such as relative age within the academic school year and have implications at a population level.29 Health professionals monitoring pregnant women should ask about and be aware of patterns of alcohol consumption during pregnancy since these may differ from regular drinking levels. Taken together with our earlier findings on the effects of occasional drinking in the first trimester,22 there appear to be consistent effects on child mental health from both background low level alcohol consumption and occasional episodes of heavier drinking. By investigating the effects of episodic alcohol consumption beyond a particular threshold (in this case, ≥4 drinks) we highlight possible risks for women who might drink only occasionally but heavily. This has implications for health education and public health advice. For example, UK policy recommendations allow up to four drinks per week during pregnancy,42 a message that could be misinterpreted in line with the drinking patterns studied here. Our findings might reflect the possible adverse effects from a perceived ‘safe’ level of alcohol consumption and highlight the lack of a safe threshold.

Conclusions

Our findings suggest that occasional episodes of consuming ≥4 drinks per day during pregnancy can carry a risk for children's mental health, particularly hyperactivity and inattention problems. There is a possible earlier vulnerability in girls and the difficulties appear to persist over time. These findings require replication, especially in relation to binge drinking episodes at other periods during pregnancy.

Acknowledgements

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part, the midwives for help in recruiting them and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses. The UK Medical Research Council, the Wellcome Trust and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. We are grateful to Dr Ruth Little who was funded by NIAAA to advise on the questions asked relating to alcohol exposure. This publication is the work of the authors, and KS will serve as guarantor for the contents of this paper.

ABBREVIATIONS

ALSPAC

Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children

C.I.

Confidence Interval

EPDS

Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale

IQ

Intelligence Quotient

SDQ

Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire

REFERENCES

  • 1.Brown RT, Coles CD, Smith IE, Platzman KA, Erickson S, Falek A. Effects of prenatal alcohol exposure at school age, II: attention and behavior. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1991;13:369–376. doi: 10.1016/0892-0362(91)90085-b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.D'Onofrio BM, Van Hulle CA, Waldman ID, Rodgers JL, Rathouz PJ, Lahey BB. Causal inferences regarding prenatal alcohol exposure and childhood externalizing problems. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:1296–1304. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.11.1296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Goldschmidt L, Richardson GA, Stoffer DS, Geva D, Day NL. Prenatal alcohol exposure and academic achievement at age six: a nonlinear fit. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1996;20:763–770. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1996.tb01684.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Howell KK, Lynch ME, Platzman KA, Smith GH, Coles CD. Prenatal alcohol exposure and ability, academic achievement, and school functioning in adolescence: a longitudinal follow-up. J Pediatr Psychol. 2006;31:116–126. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsj029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Jacobson SW, Chiodo LM, Sokol RJ, Jacobson JL. Validity of maternal report of prenatal alcohol, cocaine, and smoking in relation to neurobehavioral outcome. Pediatrics. 2002;109:815–825. doi: 10.1542/peds.109.5.815. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Jacobson SW, Jacobson JL, Sokol RJ, Chiodo LM, Corobana R. Maternal age, alcohol abuse history, and quality of parenting as moderators of the effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on 7.5-year intellectual function. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2004;28:1732–1745. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000145691.81233.fa. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Streissguth AP, Barr HM, Sampson PD, Parrish-Johnson JC, Kirchner GL, Martin DC. Attention, distraction and reaction time at age 7 years and prenatal alcohol exposure. Neurobehav Toxicol Teratol. 1986;8:717–725. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Streissguth AP, Barr HM, Sampson PD. Moderate prenatal alcohol exposure: effects on child IQ and learning problems at age 7 1/2 years. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1990;14:662–669. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1990.tb01224.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Testa M, Quigley BM, Eiden RD. The effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on infant mental development: a meta-analytical review. Alcohol Alcohol. 2003;38:295–304. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agg087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Willford J, Leech S, Day N. Moderate prenatal alcohol exposure and cognitive status of children at age 10. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2006;30:1051–1059. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00119.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Carmichael Olson H, Streissguth AP, Sampson PD, Barr HM, Bookstein FL, Thiede K. Association of prenatal alcohol exposure with behavioral and learning problems in early adolescence. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36:1187–1194. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199709000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Goldschmidt L, Richardson GA, Cornelius MD, Day NL. Prenatal marijuana and alcohol exposure and academic achievement at age 10. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2004;26:521–532. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2004.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW, Sokol RJ, Ager JW., Jr Relation of maternal age and pattern of pregnancy drinking to functionally significant cognitive deficit in infancy. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1998;22:345–351. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1998.tb03659.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Mick E, Biederman J, Faraone SV, Sayer J, Kleinman S. Case-control study of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and maternal smoking, alcohol use, and drug use during pregnancy. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41:378–385. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200204000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Nordstrom Bailey B, Delaney-Black V, Covington CY, et al. Prenatal exposure to binge drinking and cognitive and behavioral outcomes at age 7 years. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2004;191:1037–1043. doi: 10.1016/j.ajog.2004.05.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.O'Callaghan FV, O'Callaghan M, Najman JM, Najman JM, Williams GM, Bor W. Prenatal alcohol exposure and attention, learning and intellectual ability at 14 years: A prospective longitudinal study. Early Hum Dev. 2007;83:115–123. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2006.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Nulman I, Rovet J, Kennedy D, et al. Binge alcohol consumption by non-alcohol-dependent women during pregnancy affects child behaviour, but not general intellectual functioning; a prospective controlled study. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2004;7:173–181. doi: 10.1007/s00737-004-0055-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Olsen J. Effects of moderate alcohol consumption during pregnancy on child development at 18 and 42 months. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1994;18:1109–1113. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1994.tb00089.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Sayal K. Alcohol consumption in pregnancy as a risk factor for later mental health problems. Evid Based Ment Health. 2007;10:98–100. doi: 10.1136/ebmh.10.4.98-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Henderson J, Kesmodel U, Gray R. Systematic review of the fetal effects of prenatal binge-drinking. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2007;61:1069–1073. doi: 10.1136/jech.2006.054213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.NIAAA National Advisory Council NIAAA council approves definition of binge drinking. 2004. pubs.niaaa.nih.gov/publications/Newsletter/winter2004/Newsletter_Number3.pdf.
  • 22.Sayal K, Heron J, Golding J, Emond A. Prenatal alcohol exposure and gender differences in childhood mental health problems: a longitudinal population-based study. Pediatrics. 2007;119:e426–434. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Golding J, Pembrey M, Jones R, ALSPAC Study Team ALSPAC: the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, 1—study methodology. Paed Perinatal Epidemiol. 2001;15:74–87. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-3016.2001.00325.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Goodman R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38:581–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Goodman R. Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. J Am Acad Child Adolescent Psychiatry. 2001;40:1337–1345. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Weschler D. Wechsler Pre-school and Primary Scale of Intelligence–Revised UK edition. The Psychological Corporation; Kent: 1990. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Cox J, Holden JM, Sagovsky R. Detection of postnatal depression: development of the 10-item Edinburgh postnatal depression scale. Br J Psychiatry. 1987;150:782–786. doi: 10.1192/bjp.150.6.782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Murray L, Carothers AD. The validation of the Edinburgh post-natal depression scale in a community sample. Br J Psychiatry. 1990;157:288–290. doi: 10.1192/bjp.157.2.288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Goodman R, Gledhill J, Ford T. Child psychiatric disorder and relative age within school year: cross sectional survey of large population sample. BMJ. 2003;327:472–475. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7413.472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Huizink AC, Mulder EJ. Maternal smoking, drinking or cannabis use during pregnancy and neurobehavioral and cognitive functioning in human offspring. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30:24–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.West JR, Goodlett CR, Bonthius DJ, Hamre KM, Marcussen BL. Cell population depletion associated with fetal alcohol brain damage: mechanisms of BAC-dependent cell loss. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1990;14:813–818. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1990.tb01820.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bonthius DJ, West JR. Alcohol-induced neuronal loss in developing rats: increased brain damage with binge exposure. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1990;14:107–118. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1990.tb00455.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Linnet KM, Dalsgaard S, Obel C, et al. Maternal lifestyle factors in pregnancy risk of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and associated behaviors: review of the current evidence. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:1028–1040. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Fryer SL, McGee CL, Matt GE, Riley EP, Mattson SN. Evaluation of psychopathological conditions in children with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Pediatrics. 2007;119:e733–741. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Knopik VS, Sparrow EP, Madden PA, et al. Contributions of parental alcoholism, prenatal substance exposure, and genetic transmission to child ADHD risk: a female twin study. Psychol Med. 2005;35:625–635. doi: 10.1017/s0033291704004155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Knopik VS, Heath AC, Jacob T, et al. Maternal alcohol use disorder and offspring ADHD: disentangling genetic and environmental effects using a children-of-twins design. Psychol Med. 2006;36:1461–1471. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706007884. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Brookes KJ, Mill J, Guindalini C, et al. A common haplotype of the dopamine transporter gene associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and interacting with maternal use of alcohol during pregnancy. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:74–81. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.1.74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Alati R, Al Mamun A, Williams GM, O'Callaghan M, Najman JM, Bor W. In utero alcohol exposure and prediction of alcohol disorders in early adulthood: a birth cohort study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:1009–1016. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.9.1009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Barr HM, Bookstein FL, O'Malley KD, Connor PD, Huggins JE, Streissguth AP. Binge drinking during pregnancy as a predictor of psychiatric disorders on the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV in young adult offspring. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:1061–1065. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.6.1061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kelly SJ, Goodlett CR, Hulsether SA, West JR. Impaired spatial navigation in adult female but not adult male rats exposed to alcohol during the brain growth spurt. Behav Brain Res. 1988;27:247–257. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(88)90121-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Kesmodel U, Frydenberg M. Binge drinking during pregnancy - is it possible to obtain valid information on a weekly basis? Am J Epidemiol. 2004;159:803–808. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwh095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Department of Health . The Pregnancy Book. Department of Health; London: 2007. Available at: http://www.dh.gov.uk/en/Publicationsandstatistics/Publications/PublicationsPolicyAndGuidance/DH_074920. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES