Skip to main content
HHS Author Manuscripts logoLink to HHS Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Apr 30.
Published in final edited form as: Alter. 2016 October-December;10(4):345–360. doi: 10.1016/j.alter.2016.09.003

Field testing a draft version of the UNICEF/Washington Group Module on child functioning and disability. Background, methodology and preliminary findings from Cameroon and India

Test de la version provisoire du Module UNICEF/Washington Group sur le fonctionnement et le handicap des enfants. Contexte, méthodologie et premiers résultats au Cameroun et en Inde

Islay Mactaggart a,*, Claudia Cappa b, Hannah Kuper a, Mitchell Loeb c, Sarah Polack a
PMCID: PMC6488935  NIHMSID: NIHMS1019554  PMID: 31049115

Abstract

Background.

Global child disability data are generally non-comparable, comprising different tools, methodologies and disability definitions. UNICEF and The Washington Group on Disability Statistics (WG) have developed a new tool on child functioning and disability to address this need.

Aims.

The aim of this paper is to describe the development of the new module, and to present an independent field test of the draft module in two contrasting settings.

Methods.

UNICEF and the WG developed a parent-reported survey module to identify children aged 2–17 years with functional difficulties in population-based surveys through: review of existing documentation, consultation with experts and cognitive testing. A field test of the draft module was undertaken in Cameroon and India within a population-based survey. Functional limitation in each of 14 domains was scored on a scale comprising “no difficulty”, “some difficulty”, “a lot of difficulty” and “cannot do”.

Results.

In all, 1713 children in Cameroon and 1101 children in India were assessed. Sixty-four percent of children in Cameroon and 35% of children in India were reported to have at least some difficulty in one or more domain. The proportion reported to have either “a lot of difficulty” or “cannot do” was 9% in Cameroon and 4% in India. There were no significant differences in reported functional difficulties by sex but children aged 2–4 were reported to have fewer functional difficulties of any kind compared with older children in both countries.

Conclusion.

Comparable estimates were generated between the two countries, providing an initial overview of the tool’s outputs. The continued development of this standardised questionnaire for the collection of robust and reliable data on child disability is essential.

Keywords: Child disability, Functioning, Measurement, Field testing

1. Introduction: measuring child disability

Global, national and sub-national population-based data on child disability have historically differed in methodology and rigour, forestalling comparison between countries and over time. A recent global review of child disability datasets by Cappa, Petrowski, & Njelesani (2015) summarised the heterogeneity of available data, much of which predated or otherwise dissented from the prevailing bio-psycho-social conceptualisation of disability as per the International Classification of Disability, Functioning and Health (ICF) and the United Nations Convention on the Rights of Persons with Disabilities (UNCRPD) (Cappa et al., 2015; The United Nations, 2006; World Health Organization, 2001).

The most frequently cited tool for child disability measurement in population-based data collection efforts is the Ten Questions (TQ) Tool for children aged 2–9 years, used or adapted in a handful of studies of childhood disability in various Low- and Middle-Income Countries (LMICs), and integrated into the third round of the United Nations Children’s Fund (UNICEF) Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey Guidelines (Couper, 2002; Hartley & Wirz, 2002; Khan et al., 2009; Muga, 2003; UNICEF, 2008). The TQ documents caregiver-reported health conditions, impairments and activity limitations experienced by children, but has recognised limitations including dichotomous response options, validation only amongst younger children, and low sensitivity for specific impairments (Durkin, Hasan, & Hasan, 1995). Alternative caregiver-reported tools for children have also been developed, including the Rapid Assessment of Disability (RAD) child module and the World Health Organisation Disability Assessment Schedule (WHODAS 2.0) child module, but these have not been validated or widely used (Centre for Eye Research Australia and Nossal Institute for Global Health, 2013; Scorza et al., 2013).

Assessment of disability in children is particularly complicated given the continuum of development experienced throughout childhood. Whilst progression against developmental milestones has been shown to significantly predict developmental outcome in a number of settings, cultural variation can lead to low transferability of milestone-based tools, the majority of which were not developed in LMICs (Brothers, Glascoe, & Robertshaw, 2008; Scherzer, Chhagan, Kauchali, & Susser, 2012).

The dearth of quality evidence on the prevalence and spectrum of child disability reflective of the prevailing framework curtails efforts to advocate, monitor and evaluate disability-inclusive policy and programmes, particularly in the emerging post 2015 agenda. Advancing a consistent definition of disability for both data collection and data disaggregation is therefore urgent.

This paper has two aims:

  • to describe the development of a new survey module by UNICEF and the Washington Group on Disability Statistics (commonly known as the Washington Group) to meet this need;

  • to present the results of a field test of the draft version of this module in Cameroon and India, comparing findings across the two sites.

2. Development of the UNICEF/Washington Group Extended Set on child functioning and disability

Responding to the lack of agreed tools and methodologies for the assessment of child disability, UNICEF and the Washington Group have developed a parent-reported survey module to identify children with functional difficulties in population-based surveys.

The UNCRPD definition of disability was operationalized in the design of the module, and the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health for Children and Youth (ICF-CY) was selected as a conceptual framework for question development (World Health Organization, 2007).

The specific purpose of the module is to identify children with functional difficulties that may place them at a greater risk of experiencing limited participation than children without functional difficulties, as a proxy for equalization of opportunity.

Existing documentation relating to the measurement of childhood disability was collated and analysed to determine appropriate ICF-CY domains for inclusion (Cappa et al., 2015). Extensive consultations with international specialists in child development (paediatricians, developmental psychologists, speech therapists etc.) were also undertaken to further refine the draft question set.

The module focuses on children aged 2–17. Whilst the importance of early detection and intervention for children with functional difficulties is recognised, below the age of two the development process is rapid and varied, can be subjective and culturally influenced, and may not represent the presence of functional limitation. Age-range specific variations of some questions were developed to account for the continuum of development across childhood.

Following established Washington Group validation procedures, the module underwent extensive cognitive testing between 2012 and 2014 in India, Belize, Oman, Montenegro, and USA (Madans et al., 2004; Crialesi, De Palma, & Loeb, 2015).

The remainder of this paper presents the results of an independent field test of a draft version of the module as part of two surveys of disability in Cameroon (2013) and India (2014). Since its application in this research, the module has undergone several modifications and revisions.

3. Methods

Two population-based surveys of disability including people of all ages were conducted by the International Centre for Evidence in Disability (ICED) at the London School of Hygiene & Tropical Medicine, in Cameroon (2013) and India (2014). Representatives from both UNICEF and the Washington Group participated in the study’s advisory committee but were not members of the study team. The aim of the overall study was to develop a comprehensive population-based disability survey methodology (using both self-reported functional limitations and objective tools to measure clinical impairment) compatible with the ICF, and to explore the relationship between different components of disability within this framework.

The remainder of this manuscript focuses on the use and results of the draft UNICEF/Washington Group module on child functioning and disability to determine reported functional limitations in children aged 2–17 years.

The sample-size was calculated using a conservative expected all-age prevalence of moderate/severe hearing, vision and physical impairment of 4% (World Health Organization, 2011). A minimum all-age sample-size of 4056 per country was calculated, assuming 20% precision, 95% confidence, a design effect of 1.5 and 20% non-response.

We used a two-stage sampling procedure; fifty-one clusters of eighty people were selected using probability-proportionate-to-size sampling, using the most recent census for the sampling frame. Within clusters, households were selected using compact segment sampling. Each enumerated child aged 2–17 was assessed using a draft version of the UNICEF/Washington Group module on child functioning and disability.

In this draft version, 14 functional domains (D1-D14), separated into “Basic Function” and “Complex Function, Emotion and Participation” domains, were assessed. Domains are coded as follows in Table 1. Age-relevant variations were included for some domains (e.g. playing) and certain domains were included only for children aged 5–17. The draft version of the module used in the study is available upon request.

Table 1.

Module domains.

Basic function domains Complex function, emotion and participation domains
D1 Seeing D9 Feeling worried/sada
D2 Hearing D10 Controlling behaviour
D3 Walking D11 Completing a taska
D4 Self-carea D12 Accepting changea
D5 Understanding D13 Getting along with other childrena
D6 Being understood D14 Playing
D7 Learning
D8 Rememberinga
a

Children 5–17 only.

Functional limitation in 12 of the 14 domains were reported on a 4-point scale: “no difficulty”, “some difficulty”, “a lot of difficulty” and “cannot do at all”. The response categories for the remaining two domains (controlling behaviour and anxiety/sadness) were “the same or less”, “more” and “a lot more”. Usage of glasses and hearing aids were also included. To standardize proxy-respondent responses to generally accepted stages of child development, where appropriate, questions were prefaced with the clause “compared with children of the same age…”.

Parents or adult primary caregivers reported for children under the age of 9 or unable to communicate independently. Children aged 9 and above were interviewed directly where feasible and appropriate.

In India, a single question “do you consider yourself [age 9–17]/your child [age 2–8] to have a disability?” was included for comparison purposes.

Three survey teams per country received 10 days training on disability awareness, project protocols and tools, ethics and practice interviewing. Teams consisted of 3 clinical team members, 5 field assistants, and 2 interviewers. Field assistants in each team were responsible for completing the UNICEF/Washington Group module.

Ethical Approval for the study was granted by:

  • the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine;

  • Cameroon National Ethics Committee for Research in Human Health;

  • Cameroon Baptist Convention Health Board Institutional Review Board;

  • Public Health Foundation of India Institutional Ethics Committee;

  • Government of India Health Ministry Screening Committee.

Basic medicines (vitamins, anti-inflammatories, ear and eye drops) were distributed by clinical team members as needed, and all participants with unmet health needs were referred to relevant and available health, rehabilitation or educational services.

Caregivers of all children aged 2–17 were read an information sheet about the study and given the opportunity to ask questions. If they agreed to participate, written/finger print consent was taken from the caregiver and assent was provided by children aged 9 and above. Caregivers were required to remain present throughout the interview process.

In both settings, the questionnaires were cognitively tested for context relevance and adapted accordingly. The module was translated into Telegu in India and verbally translated into Pigin English in Cameroon, using a phrase sheet of appropriate phonetic translations.

Data was double-entered into Microsoft Access, corrected for inconsistencies between entries using the EpiInfo Data Compare utility and merged in STATA 12.0 for analysis. The svy command was used to derive prevalence estimates with 95% confidence intervals (CI) at different functional thresholds (“no difficulty” in any, “some difficulty” in at least one domain, “some difficulty” in at least two domains, “a lot of difficulty” in at least one domain and “cannot do” in at least one domain), accounting for the cluster sampling design. Domain-specific analyses are reported as “at least some difficulty” and “at least a lot of difficulty”, given small numbers. Associations between reported limitations (both aggregate domains and for each specific domain), age group and gender were assessed using a Chi2 test of association. Pearson’s correlation coefficient analysis (r) was computed to assess pairwise relationships between endorsed domains.

4. Results

4.1. Overall ressults

Findings use the age grouping 2–4, 5–8, 9–12 and 13–17 years to accommodate age-range specific questions, and are presented as the minimum level of limitation endorsed. “Reported” refers to both caregiver report for children age 2–8 and self-report for children 9–17.

Table 2 presents the study cohort descriptors. A total of 1713 children aged 2–17 were assessed in Cameroon, and 1101 in India. Fifty percent of the study children were male in Cameroon and 52% in India.

Table 2.

Cohort descriptors.

Cameroon
India
Male
Female
Total
Male
Female
Total
n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%) n (100%)
2–4 166 (19.6) 200 (23.2)  366 (21.4) 113 (19.7) 120 (22.8)  233 (21.2)
5–8 270 (31.8) 237 (27.4)  507 (29.6) 163 (28.4) 140 (26.6)  303 (27.5)
9–12 222 (26.2) 226 (26.2)  448 (26.2) 138 (24.0) 135 (25.7)  273 (24.8)
13–17 191 (22.5) 201 (23.3)  392 (22.9) 161 (28.0) 131 (24.9)  292 (26.5)
Total 849 (100) 864 (100) 1713 (100) 575 (100) 526 (100) 1101 (100)

Aggregate domain endorsement across all domains and stratified by domain type (basic versus complex) are presented in Table 3. Two thirds of the sample in Cameroon reported at least some difficulty in at least one domain (63.9%, 95% CI 60.0–67.6), compared with one third in India (34.9%, 30.8–39.2). Prevalence declined with increasing reported difficulty in both samples. In Cameroon, 42.0% (38.0–46.0) reported some difficulty in any two domains, 8.9% (7.1–11.2) reported a lot of difficulty in any one domain and 0.7% (0.4–1.2) reported inability to do any one domain. In India 19.8% (16.5–23.6) reported some difficulty in any two domains, 3.5% (2.3–5.1) a lot of difficulty in any one, and 0.9 (0.5–1.7) inability to do any one. This trend repeated when disaggregated by basic versus complex domains in both countries, with higher proportions identified in Cameroon at the level of some difficulty in one, or two domains (basic or complex) than India, but similar, much lower prevalence estimates at the higher levels of difficulty.

Table 3.

Aggregate domain endorsement in Cameroon and India.

Cameroon, n = 1713 % (95% CI) India, n = 1101 % (95% CI)
No difficulty in any domain 36.1 (32.4–40.1) 65.1 (60.8–69.2)
All domains
 At least some difficulty in one domain 63.9 (60.0–67.6) 34.9 (30.8–39.2)
 At least some difficulty in two domains 42.0 (38.0–46.0) 19.8 (16.5–23.6)
 At least a lot of difficulty in one domain  8.9 (7.1–11.2)  3.5 (2.3–5.1)
 At least cannot do in one domain  0.7 (0.4–1.2)  0.9 (0.5–1.7)
Basic domains
 At least some difficulty in one domain 43.7 (39.8–47.8) 28.0 (24.1–32.2)
 At least some difficulty in two domains 19.7 (17.0–22.7) 12.3 (9.7–15.4)
 At least a lot of difficulty in one domain  2.5 (1.8–3.5)  2.0 (1.2–3.4)
 At least cannot do in one domain  0.4 (0.1–0.9)  0.7 (0.4–1.4)
Complex domains
 At least some difficulty in one domain 43.1 (39.7–46.5) 17.3 (14.0–21.1)
 At least some difficulty in two domains 18.3 (16.1–20.8)  7.1 (5.3–9.4)
 At least a lot of difficulty in one domain  7.4 (5.7–9.5)  2.3 (1.5–3.5)
 At least cannot do in one domain  0.6 (0.3–1.0)  0.5 (0.2–1.3)

Table 4 presents overall endorsement of basic and complex domains disaggregated by age. Children in the youngest age group (2–4) were least likely to have any difficulties in any domains in both countries (p < 0.001). In Cameroon, age group was strongly associated with reporting some difficulty in one or more basic or complex domain (p < 0.001), but there was no clear trend by age. In India, age group was associated with reporting some difficulty in one or more complex domain (p < 0.01) only. Reporting a lot of difficulty or higher in any basic or complex domain was not significantly different by age group in either country. There was no statistical difference by sex in overall endorsement of basic and complex domains in either country (data not shown).

Table 4.

Aggregate domain endorsement in Cameroon and India.

2 to 4
5 to 8
9 to 12
13 to 17
χ2 p
n % n % n % n %
Cameroon
 No difficulty in any domain 193 52.7 172 33.9 139 31.0 115 29.3 57.7 < 0.001
 Basic Domains
  At least some difficulty in one domain 101 27.6 207 40.8 229 51.1 213 54.3 68.3 < 0.001
  At least some difficulty in two domains  40 10.9  79 15.6 118 26.3 101 25.8 44.8 < 0.001
  At least a lot of difficulty in one domain   7  1.9  13  2.6  11  2.5  12  3.1  1.0 0.8
  At least cannot do in one domain   1  0.3   5  1.0   0   0   0   0  8.9 < 0.05
 Complex domains
  At least some difficulty in one domain  81 22.1 257 50.7 209 46.7 191 48.7 84.9 < 0.001
  At least some difficulty in two domains   4  1.1 124 24.5 103 23.0  83 21.2 94.0 < 0.001
  At least a lot of difficulty in one domain  27  7.4  44  8.7  31  6.9  24  6.1  2.3 0.5
  At least cannot do in one domain   0  0   8  1.6   1  0.2   1  0.3 12.5 < 0.01
India
 No difficulty in any domain 169 72.5 193 63.7 173 63.4 182 62.3  7.3 0.06
 Basic domains
  At least some difficulty in one domain  55 23.6  82 27.1  80 29.3  91  31.2  4.0 0.3
  At least some difficulty in two domains  25 10.7  46 15.2  33 12.1  31 10.6  3.7 0.3
  At least a lot of difficulty in one domain   5  2.2   5  1.7   6  2.2   6  2.1  0.3 1.0
  At least cannot do in one domain   4  1.7   1  0.3   1  0.4   2  0.7  4.3 0.2
 Complex domains
  At least some difficulty in one domain  22  9.4  59 19.5  53 19.4  56 19.2 12.7 < 0.01
  At least some difficulty in two domains   4  1.7  25  8.3  21  7.7  28 10.0 13.8 < 0.01
  At least a lot of difficulty in one domain   6  2.6   7  2.3   6  2.2   6  2.1  0.2 1.0
  At least cannot do in one domain   1  0.4   1  0.3   2  0.7   1  0.3  0.7 0.9

Table 5 presents the overall proportion of children in Cameroon and India reporting at least some difficulty and at least a lot of difficulty by specific domain.

Table 5.

Proportion endorsing each domain–Cameroon and India.

Cameroon
India
At least some difficulty
A least a lot of difficulty
At least some difficulty
At least a lot of difficulty
n % n % n % n %
Basic domains
 2–17
  Seeing  99  5.8  6 0.4  46  4.2  3 0.3
  Hearing 130  7.6  6 0.4  38  3.5  5 0.5
  Walking  93  5.4 13 0.8  39  3.5  9 0.8
  Understanding  86  5.0  6 0.4  84  7.6 10 0.9
  Being understood  83  4.9  7 0.4  77  7.0  8 0.7
  Learning 357 20.8 11 0.6 125 11.4 10 0.9
 5–17a
  Remembering 388 28.8 15 1.1 151 17.4  7 0.8
  Self-care  79  5.9  4 0.3  33  3.8  6 0.7
 2–17
  Controlling behaviourb 397 23.2 55 3.2 118 10.7 11 1.0
  Playing  69  4.0 11 0.6  54  4.9 12 1.1
 5–17a
  Feeling worried/sadb 270 20.0 46 3.4  59  6.8  7 0.8
  Completing task 253 18.8 22 1.6  71  8.2  8 1.0
  Accepting change 305 22.6 27 2.0  49  5.6  9 1.0
  Getting along with other children  59  4.4  5 0.4  39  4.5  9 1.0
a

Question valid age 5–17 only, denominator reflects this.

b

Maximum option is “a lot more”, not “cannot” for this question.

4.2. Results by functional domain in Cameroon

In Cameroon, the most commonly reported limitations in basic domains at the some or greater difficulty level were learning and remembering (20.8% and 28.8% of children 2–17 and 5–17 respectively, Table 5). Common difficulties in other basic functional domains were at least some difficulty hearing (7.6%) or seeing (5.8%).

The most common complex domains in which at least some difficulty was reported were controlling behaviour (23.2% of children 2–17), accepting change (22.6% of children 5–17) and feeling worried/sad (20.0% of children 5–17). Less than 1% of the sample in Cameroon reported a lot of difficulty or higher in any basic domain, with the exception of remembering (1.1% of 5–17 year olds). Amongst complex domains, 3.2% of children 2–17 reported a lot of difficulty or higher controlling their behaviour, 3.4% of children aged 5–17 reported a lot of difficulty or higher with worrying or feeling sad, and 2.0% of children aged 5–17 reported a lot of difficulty or higher in accepting change. Less than 2% reported a lot of difficulty or more in completing tasks (1.6%), playing (0.6%) or getting along with other children. There were no statistically significant differences by sex in any specific domain at any degree of difficulty (data not shown here).

Table 6 (overleaf) stratifies the proportion of children in Cameroon and India reporting at least some difficulty in each basic and complex domain by age group. In Cameroon, reporting at least some difficulty in seeing and hearing (2–17 years), and remembering and feeling worried/sad (5–17 years) were positively associated with age group (p < 0.001–p < 0.01), whilst being understood (2–17 years), self-care and completing a task (both age 5–17) were negatively associated (p < 0.01). Walking, learning and playing (all 2–17 years) were all associated with age (p < 0.05), but with no directional trend. There were no significant differences by age in the proportions of children reported to experience a lot of difficulty or inability to complete any basic domain or complex domains, although cell sizes were very small (data not shown here).

Table 6.

Proportion endorsing at least Some Difficulty in Cameroon and India.

Cameroon
India
2 to 4
5 to 8
9 to 12
13 to 17
χ2 p 2 to 4
5 to 8
9 to 12
13 to 17
χ2 p
n % n % n % n % n % n % n % n %
Basic domains
 2–17
  Seeing  6 1.6 17  3.4  33  7.4  43 11.0 38.5 < 0.001 3 1.3  3  1.0 15  5.5 25  8.6 27.8 < 0.001
  Hearing  7 1.9 28  5.5  45 10.0  50 12.8 38.7 < 0.001 3 1.3 12  4.0 14  5.1  9  3.1  5.9 0.1
  Walking 17 4.6 19  3.8  24  5.4  33  8.4 10.1 < 0.05 15 6.4  4  1.3  8  2.9 12  4.1 10.7 < 0.05
  Understanding 28 7.7 23  4.5  21  4.7  14  3.6  7.4 0.06 26 11.2 26  8.6 17  6.2 15  5.1  7.8 < 0.05
  Being understood 30 8.2 22  4.3  17  3.8  14  3.6 11.7 < 0.01 23 9.9 23  7.6 16  5.9 15  5.1  5.2 0.2
  Learning 69 18.9 84 16.6 115 25.7  89 22.7 13.6 < 0.01 39 16.7 34 11.2 23  8.4 29  9.9  9.6 < 0.05
 5–17 onlya
  Remembering 112 22.1 140 31.3 136 34.7 19.1 < 0.001 53 17.5 46 16.8 52 17.8  0.1 1.0
  Self-care  60 11.8  16  3.6  3  0.8 55.5 < 0.001 19  6.3  8  2.9  6  2.1  8.1 < 0.05
Complex domains
 2–17
  Controlling behaviour 92 25.1 118 23.3 108 24.1  79 20.2  3.0 0.4 16 6.9 32 10.6 33 12.1 37 12.7  5.3 0.2
  Playing 15 4.1  21  4.1  9  2.0  24  6.1  9.2 < 0.05 14 6.0 12  4.0 14  5.1 14  4.8  1.2 0.7
 5–17 onlya
  Feeling worried/sadb  84 16.6  85 19.0 101 25.8 12.1 < 0.01 18  6.0 18  6.6 23  7.9  0.9 0.6
  Completing task 132 26.0  72 16.1  49 12.5 29.8 < 0.001 26  8.6 18  6.6 27  9.2  1.4 0.5
  Accepting change 122 24.1 102 22.8  81 20.7  1.5 0.5 20  6.6  9  3.3 20  6.8  4.1 0.1
  Getting along with other children  24  4.7  15  3.3  20  5.1  1.8 0.4 13  4.3 11  4.0 15  5.1  0.4 0.8
a

Questions valid age 5–17 only, denominator reflects this.

b

Maximum option is “a lot more”, not “cannot” for this question.

4.3. Results by functional domain in India

Basic domains related to cognition were the most frequently reported in India (some or greater difficulty in understanding: 7.6%, being understood: 7.0%, learning: 11.4% and remembering: 17.4%), whilst less than five percent reported some or greater difficulty in the remaining basic domains (seeing: 4.2%, hearing: 3.5%, walking: 3.5% and self-care: 3.8%), as shown in Table 5. Commonly reported complex domains included 10.7% of children aged 2–17 reporting at least some difficulty in controlling behaviour, and amongst those aged 5–17, 8.2% reporting at least some difficulty completing tasks and 6.8% feeling worried or sad. Less than two percent of children reported a lot of difficulty or greater in any basic or complex domain.

There were generally no clear differences by sex in reporting some or greater difficulty in different domains (data not shown here), and fewer associations between specific domains and age group than in Cameroon (Table 6). Some or greater difficulty understanding (2–17) or with self-care (5–17) were negatively associated with age (p < 0.05), whilst seeing, walking and learning (all 2–17) were associated but showed no trend.

4.4. Relationship between endorsed domains

Appendix 1 presents the pairwise correlation matrix between domains for Cameroon endorsed at the level of some difficulty or higher. No strong, significant relationships (r > 0.7, p < 0.05) were identified between any two domains. A moderate positive relationship was identified between understanding and being understood (r = 0.55, p < 0.05). Weak but significant positive relationships were also identified between remembering and learning, and between completion of a task and accepting change (both r = 0.36, p < 0.05).

In India, the only strong and statistically significant pairwise relationship at the level of some or greater difficulty was between understanding and being understood (r = 0.77, p < 0.05), presented in Appendix 2. Four pairs demonstrated moderate positive relationships, and 20 pairs demonstrated weak but positive relationships. In particular, at least some difficulty playing was significantly associated (all p < 0.05) with at least some difficulty in the domains of self-care, understanding, being understood, learning, worrying/feeling sad, controlling behaviour, completing tasks, accepting change and getting along with others. Similarly, at least some difficulty in getting along with others was significantly associated with at least some difficulty in self-care, being understood, worrying/feeling sad, completing tasks and accepting change.

4.5. Single question on disability

In the Indian sample, 2.5% answered “yes” to the question “do you consider yourself [age 9–17]/your child [age 2–8] to have a disability” (Table 7). Less than one percent of those who did not report any difficulties in any domain answered affirmatively, compared with 6% of those who reported some or greater difficulty in any domain, and 44.7% of those who reported a lot or greater difficulty in any domain.

Table 7.

Single question responses in India.

n Yes
No
n % n %
No difficulty in any domain  717  4  0.6  713 99.4
Some or greater difficulty in one or more domain  384 23  6.0  361 94.0
A lot or greater difficulty in one or more domain   38 17 44.7   21 55.3
Total 1713 27  2.5 1074 97.6

5. Discussion

5.1. Aggregate domain endorsement

Two thirds of the sample in Cameroon reported at least some difficulty in at least one domain, compared with one third in India. Just under half of the sample reported some difficulty in any basic (43.7%) or complex (43.1%) domain in Cameroon, compared with 28.0% and 17.3% respectively in India. However, at the higher threshold of a lot of difficulty or greater, less than three percent reported difficulties in any basic domain in either country (2.5% in Cameroon and 2.0% in India), and less than ten percent in any complex domain (7.4% and 2.3% respectively). At the highest level cannot do, less than one percent in either country reported difficulties in either basic or complex domains. Gender does not appear to be related to functional limitations as endorsed in this study in either country. However, children in the youngest age group (2–4) were least likely to report any difficulties in any domain in either country, albeit with no discernible trend at the aggregate level.

As already discussed, there are limited available data with which to compare these findings. The Cameroon MICS (2006) estimated a prevalence of 13.9% amongst children 2–9 in North West Cameroon using the Ten Questions (TQ) tool (Institut National de la Statistique, 2012). The tool was used in the study without a second-stage assessment, which is estimated to lead to 300% overestimation of serious disability (Durkin et al., 1995). Data from the Indian Census (2011) estimates disability amongst children 0–9 in India of 1.6% at the national level (Government of India Ministry of Home Affairs, 2011). The census used a disability screener (“Is this person mentally/physically disabled”) followed by a list of 8 types of disability; a method previously shown to underestimate the proportion of people with disabilities (Mont, 2007; Office of the Registrar General and Census Commissioner, 2013).

The large variation between the all-age proportion of children reported to have “some” difficulty between Cameroon and India may suggest different cultural interpretations of the term, reinforcing the importance of contextual translation. In addition, lowest agreement between countries was found in the 9–12 age group, who were the youngest group to self-report. This may indicate that this age group is not consistent in self-reporting, adding to debate on proxy versus self-report in children.

The all-age aggregate domain similarity between countries at the higher thresholds of “a lot of difficulty” and “cannot do” may suggest that whilst “some difficulty” may be reported in relation to regular variation in the child’s general development, this is distinct to reporting a perceived substantial limitation at the level of “a lot of difficulty” or “cannot do”.

5.2. Endorsement by domain

Domain-specific analyses provide further insight. In both countries, reporting some difficulty in basic domains related to cognition (learning, remembering, understanding, being understood) was higher (up to 30%) than sensory or mobility domains, or self-care (less than ten percent) in both countries. Some or greater difficulty in complex domains were generally endorsed more frequently than basic domains, ranging between 4% and 24% in Cameroon, and 5% and 11% in India. Most commonly these were controlling behaviour, accepting change and feeling worried or sad. It will be important to further probe whether these responses are related to functional limitation or natural variations in child development appropriate to age.

No individual domains (basic or complex) in either country were endorsed at the level of a lot or greater difficulty by more than five percent of the respective samples, and the majority were endorsed by less than one percent.

5.3. Endorsement by age group

Further breakdown of domain endorsement by age group shows numerous directional and non-directional associations between reporting some or greater difficulty and age group.

A significantly higher proportion of young children aged 2–4 were reported to experience at least some difficulties in domains related to milestones of early childhood development (walking, understanding, being understood, learning) than older children. Gladstone et al. (2010) examined the reliability in Malawi of several tools developed in High Income settings to assess early childhood development. The study found that items related to gross motor or language development milestones were generally reliable, but items related to social skills showed poor reliability in a Malawian setting (Gladstone et al., 2010).

Older children were also more likely to report “some” difficulties in seeing, hearing and remembering than younger children (whose caregivers reported on their behalf) in Cameroon. Similarly, older children were also more likely to report some difficulty seeing in India. Previous studies have shown that caregivers may experience difficulty identifying sensory impairments in children at an early age, which may account for the comparatively lower proportions of parental-reported younger children reported to experience sensory limitations (Omondi, Ogol, Otieno, & Macharia, 2007; Rahi, Cumberland, & Peckham, 2010).

Further, the reliance on proxy report adds a second dimension of complexity given potential for mis-reporting by caregivers (Eiser & Morse, 2001). Older children (who self-reported) were significantly more likely to report feeling worried/sad than younger children (reported for by proxy) in Cameroon. A systematic review of the relationship between parental and child self-reported health-related quality of life (HRQoL) found greater agreement on “observable” functioning such as physical functioning and symptoms of somatic distress, and lower agreement in domains related to emotional or social HRQoL issues (Eiser & Morse, 2001). Further work is needed to validate whether parental report on emotional and behavioural domains are acceptably in agreement with the perspective of the child.

5.4. Pairwise Domain Relationships

Limited pairwise correlations were identified in either country. Namely, with the exception of a strong and predictable correlation between at least some difficulty in both understanding and being understood in India (and none in Cameroon), there were no strong statistically significant pairwise relationships between domains to justify combination algorithms of reporting some difficulty in two or more domains as a threshold for prevalence estimations.

5.5. Comparison with a Single Question

The inclusion of a single question on disability perception in India further emphasises the need for the UNICEF/Washington Group module. Less than 2.5% of the overall sample responded affirmatively to this question, including less than half of those who reported a lot of difficulty or greater in one or more domain. This shows that not only does a single question lead to under report of significant limitations in functioning in children, but also the clear difference between reporting some difficulty in functioning (6% of who also responded yes to the singe question) and a lot of difficulty/inability to complete (44.7%).

5.6. Strengths and Limitations

The study sample in Cameroon and India of a combined 2814 children aged 2–17 represents one of the first field tests of the draft UNICEF/Washington Group module on child functioning and disability. This data is urgently needed to respond to calls for a consistent method for child disability data collection and disaggregation.

A potential limitation of the study is that all children aged 9 and above who were able to communicate independently were interviewed directly in the presence of a primary caregiver, whist children aged 2–8 or unable to communicate independently were interviewed via adult proxy report. Consequently, the findings are part parental-reported and part self-reported by children, which limit comparability between age groups.

In addition, further comprehensive field tests by UNICEF and the Washington Group, including comparison of findings against established tools, determination of response distributions, module behaviour tests (e.g. non-response rates and sample-size calculations) were completed in mid-2015. These results and the final version of the module will be presented in a separate publication, and may not be fully reflected in the results presented here.

5.7. Implications for further research

The very high and moderately high proportions of children reported to have at least “some” difficulty in one or more domain in Cameroon and India respectively warrants further investigation, to identify whether the threshold accurately identifies functional limitation or generates false positives. In particular, future research should clarify whether this issue was compounded by translation errors in Cameroon, or whether this is a common finding. The issue is further complicated by the use of a proxy respondent, given previous findings on the capacity of proxies to gauge limitations in emotional or social domains. Field testing of the final module will include probing questions to address the nuances of a parental report of “some” difficulty, so as to address this issue. Additionally, a forthcoming research study conducted by the Question Design Research Laboratory (QDRL) at the US National Center for Health Statistics (NCHS) will compare self and proxy respondents to the UNICEF/Washington Group module on child functioning and disability for a sample of youth aged 15–17 years of age and their parent.

The final round of field testing will further progress the capacity of the UNICEF/Washington Group module to adequately and accurately identify children at risk of experiencing limited participation and consequently the availability of appropriate methodologies for generating child disability statistics.

6. Conclusions

During this field test of the draft UNICEF/Washington Group module on child functioning and disability, comparable overall estimates were generated at the “a lot of difficulty” and “cannot do” thresholds across the two study settings, although an absence of similar prevailing studies prevents us from validating or rejecting these estimates. It is hoped that this endeavour to produce a standardised questionnaire will greatly advance the collection of child disability data statistics in providing a robust and reliable methodology for the determination of disability status among children in survey settings.

Appendix 1. Relationship between domains in India–Pearson’s Correlation Coefficient (r)

D1 r 1
p
D2 r 0.0849a 1
p 0.0048
D3 r 0.1319a 0.0984a 1
p 0 0.0011
D4 r 0.1230a 0.1127a 0.1969a 1
p 0 0.0002 0
D5 r 0.0939a 0.0956a 0.2781a 0.2705a 1
p 0.0018 0.0015 0 0
D6 r 0.0851a 0.0847a 0.2557a 0.2651a 0.7663a 1
p 0.0047 0.0049 0 0 0
D7 r 0.0684a 0.0421 0.1947a 0.2225a 0.4687a 0.4182a 1
p 0.0233 0.1625 0 0 0 0
D8 r 0.1015a 0.0982a 0.0807a 0.2551a 0.2136a 0.2220a 0.3317a 1
p 0.0007 0.0011 0.0074 0 0 0 0
D9 r 0.0914a 0.0655a 0.1508a 0.2420a 0.2203a 0.2194a 0.2200a 0.2803a 1
p 0.0024 0.0298 0 0 0 0 0 0
D10 r 0.1331a 0.0632a 0.2195a 0.2146a 0.2875a 0.2849a 0.2647a 0.3228a 0.2435a 1
p 0 0.0361 0 0 0 0 0 0 0
D11 r 0.0745a 0.0719a 0.0897a 0.2792a 0.2728a 0.3050a 0.2441a 0.2931a 0.2495a 0.4709a 1
p 0.0134 0.017 0.0029 0 0 0 0 0 0 0
D12 r 0.1090a 0.0316 0.0778a 0.2979a 0.2366a 0.2862a 0.1727a 0.2725a 0.3202a 0.3951a 0.5889a 1
p 0.0003 0.2951 0.0098 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0
D13 r 0.1319a 0.0445 0.0962a 0.3409a 0.2966a 0.3135a 0.2567a 0.2950a 0.4781a 0.2989a 0.4098a 0.5067a 1
p 0 0.1399 0.0014 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0
D14 r 0.0787a 0.1183a 0.2750a 0.3054a 0.4100a 0.3665a 0.3297a 0.2273a 0.3755a 0.3020a 0.3513a 0.4201a 0.6163a 1
p 0.009 0.0001 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0
D1 D2 D3 D4 D5 D6 D7 D8 D9 D10 D11 D12 D13 D14

Key: D1 Seeing, D2 Hearing, D3 Walking, D4 Self-Care, D5 Understanding, D6 Being Understood, D7 Learning, D8 Remembering, D9 Feeling worried/sad, D10 Controlling Behaviour, D11 Completing a Task, D12 Accepting Change, D13 Getting along with other children, D14 Playing.

a

Indicate that r is statistically significant at the 5% significance level.

Appendix 2. Relationship between domains in Cameroon–Pearson’s Correlation Coefficient (r)

D1 r 1
p
D2 r 0.0896a
0.0002
D3 r 0.029 0.0675a 1
p 0.2305 0.0052
D4 r −0.0187 0.0421 0.0824a 1
p 0.4399 0.0816 0.0006
D5 r 0.0003 0.0855a 0.0393 0.1151a 1
p 0.9887 0.0004 0.104 0
D6 r 0.014 0.0996a 0.0419 0.1059a 0.5457a 1
p 0.5621 0 0.0828 0 0
D7 r 0.0084 0.1243a 0.0800a 0.0791a 0.2309a 0.1921a 1
p 0.7275 0 0.0009 0.0011 0 0
D8 r 0.0572a 0.1451a 0.0673a 0.1137a 0.1055a 0.0987a 0.3610a 1
p 0.0178 0 0.0053 0 0 0 0
D9 r 0.0835a 0.0740a 0.0646a 0.1020a 0.0679a 0.0652a 0.1143a 0.2369a 1
p 0.0005 0.0022 0.0075 0 0.0049 0.007 0 0
D10 r 0.0478a 0.0359 0.0088 0.0573a 0.1018a 0.1080a 0.1508a 0.1457a 0.1010a 1
p 0.0481 0.1375 0.7149 0.0176 0 0 0 0 0
D11 r −0.0255 0.005 0.0382 0.1752a 0.0625a 0.0517a 0.1388a 0.1796a 0.0795a 0.1769a 1
p 0.2908 0.8372 0.1137 0 0.0096 0.0325 0 0 0.001 0
D12 r 0.0221 0.0222 0.0366 0.1960a 0.0747a 0.0087 0.2045a 0.2658a 0.2226a 0.2326a 0.3611a 1
p 0.3616 0.3584 0.1295 0 0.002 0.7195 0 0 0 0 0
D13 r 0.0493a 0.0184 0.0819a 0.0958a 0.1325a 0.1065a 0.1080a 0.1272a 0.1102a 0.1390a 0.1109a 0.1966a 1
p 0.0415 0.4465 0.0007 0.0001 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0
D14 r 0.0511a 0.0086 0.0164 0.0824a 0.1025a 0.1474a 0.1434a 0.0736a 0.0487a 0.1267a 0.1239a 0.1220a 0.2544a 1
p 0.0346 0.7233 0.4968 0.0006 0 0 0 0.0023 0.0439 0 0 0 0
D1 D2 D3 D4 D5 D6 D7 D8 D9 D10 D11 D12 D13 D14

Key: D1 Seeing, D2 Hearing, D3 Walking, D4 Self-Care, D5 Understanding, D6 Being Understood, D7 Learning, D8 Remembering, D9 Feeling worried/sad, D10 Controlling Behaviour, D11 Completing a Task, D12 Accepting Change, D13 Getting along with other children, D14 Playing.

a

Indicates that r is statistically significant at the 5% significance level.

Footnotes

Disclosure of interest

The authors declare that they have no competing interest.

References

  1. Brothers KB, Glascoe FP, & Robertshaw NS (2008). PEDS: developmental milestones—an accurate brief tool for surveillance and screening. Clinical Pediatrics, 47(3), 271–279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Cappa C, Petrowski N, & Njelesani J (2015). Navigating the landscape of child disability measurement: A review of available data collection instruments. European Journal of Disability Research/Revue européenne de recherche sur le handicap, 9(4), 317–330. [Google Scholar]
  3. Centre for Eye Research Australia and Nossal Institute for Global Health. (2013). Rapid assessment of disability toolkit Melbourne, Australia. [Google Scholar]
  4. Couper J (2002). Prevalence of childhood disability in rural KwaZulu-Natal. South African Medical Journal. Suid-Afrikaanse Tydskrif Vir Geneeskunde, 92(7), 549–552. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Crialesi R, De Palma E, & Loeb M (2015). The WG-UNICEF module on child functioning and disability: Review of the work. In Paper presented at the 15th Meeting of the Washington Group on Disability Statistics, Copenhagen, Denmark: (Retrieved 18.04.16; from http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/washington_group/wg_meeting15.htm) [Google Scholar]
  6. Durkin MS, Hasan ZM, & Hasan KZ (1995). The ten questions screen for childhood disabilities: Its uses and limitations in Pakistan. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health, 49(4), 431–436. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Eiser C, & Morse R (2001). Can parents rate their child’s health-related quality of life? Results of a systematic review. Quality of Life Research, 10(4), 347–357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Gladstone M, Lancaster GA, Umar E, Nyirenda M, Kayira E, van den Broek NR, et al. (2010). The Malawi Developmental Assessment Tool (MDAT): The creation, validation, and reliability of a tool to assess child development in rural African settings. PLoS Med, 7(5), e1000273 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000273 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Government of India Ministry of Home Affairs. (2011). Census of India (Retrieved 12.12.13; from http://www.censusindia.gov.in/2011census)
  10. Hartley SD, & Wirz SL (2002). Development of a ‘communication disability model’ and its implication on service delivery in low-income countries. Social Science & Medicine, 54(10), 1543–1557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Institut National de la Statistique. (2012). Enquête démographique et de santé et à indicateurs multiples 2011 Cameroun: Ministère de la Santé Publique Yaoundé. [Google Scholar]
  12. Khan NZ, Ferdous S, Islam R, Sultana A, Durkin M, & McConachie H (2009). Behaviour problems in young children in rural Bangladesh. Journal of Tropical Pediatrics, 55(3), 177–182. 10.1093/tropej/fmn108 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Madans JH, Altman BM, Rasch E, Mbogoni M, Synneborn M, Jeremiah B, et al. (2004). Washington Group Position Paper proposed purpose of an internationally comparable general disability measure (Available from: http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/washington_group/WG_purpose_paper.pdf)
  14. Mont D (2007). Measuring health and disability. The Lancet, 369(9573), 1658–1663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Muga E (2003). Screening for disability in a community: The ‘ten questions’ screen for children, in Bondo, Kenya. African Health Sciences, 3(1), 33–39. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Office of the Registrar General Census Commissioner. (2013). Census of India 2011 data on disability (Available from http://www.languageinindia.com/jan2014/disabilityinindia2011data.pdf [Accessed 20.02.15])
  17. Omondi D, Ogol C, Otieno S, & Macharia I (2007). Parental awareness of hearing impairment in their school-going children and healthcare seeking behaviour in Kisumu district, Kenya. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 71(3), 415–423. 10.1016/j.ijporl.2006.11.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Rahi JS, Cumberland PM, & Peckham CS (2010). Improving detection of blindness in childhood: The British Childhood Vision Impairment study. Pediatrics, 126(4), e895–e903. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Scherzer AL, Chhagan M, Kauchali S, & Susser E (2012). Global perspective on early diagnosis and intervention for children with developmental delays and disabilities. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 54(12), 1079–1084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Scorza P, Stevenson A, Canino G, Mushashi C, Kanyanganzi F, Munyanah M, et al. (2013). Validation of the “World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule for Children, WHODAS-Child” in Rwanda. PLoS ONE, 8(3) 10.1371/journal.pone.0057725 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. The United Nations. (2006). Convention of the rights of persons with disabilities and optional protocol New York: United Nations. [Google Scholar]
  22. UNICEF. (2008). Monitoring child disability in developing countries New York, USA: United Nations Children’s Fund Division of Policy and Practice. [Google Scholar]
  23. World Health Organization. (2001). International classification of functioning, disability and health (Retrieved 28th September 2011; from http://www.who.int/classification/icf/en)
  24. World Health Organization. (2007). International classification of functioning. Disability and health children and youth version Switzerland. [Google Scholar]
  25. World Health Organization. (2011). World report on disability Geneva: World Health Organization. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES