Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: Arch Sex Behav. 2019 Feb 12;48(4):1137–1158. doi: 10.1007/s10508-018-1333-1

Changing Trends in Substance Use and Sexual Risk Disparities among Sexual Minority Women as a Function of Sexual Identity, Behavior, and Attraction: Findings from the National Survey of Family Growth, 2002-2015

Margaret M Paschen-Wolff 1, Elizabeth Kelvin 2,3, Brooke E Wells 4, Aimee NC Campbell 5, Nicholas A Grosskopf 6,7, Christian Grov 2,7
PMCID: PMC6489449  NIHMSID: NIHMS1521460  PMID: 30756210

Abstract

Components of sexual minority (SM) status—including lesbian or bisexual identity, having same-sex partners, or same-sex attraction—individually predict substance use and sexual risk behavior disparities among women. Few studies have measured differing associations by sexual orientation components (identity, behavior, and attraction), particularly over time. Data were drawn from the 2002-2015 National Survey of Family Growth female sample (n = 31,222). Multivariable logistic regression (adjusted for age, race/ethnicity, education, marital/cohabitation status, survey cycle, and population-weighted) compared past-year sexual risk behavior, binge drinking, drug use, and sexually transmitted infection treatment among sexual minority women (SMW) versus sexual majority women (SMJW) by each sexual orientation component separately and by all components combined, and tested for effect modification by survey cycle. In multivariable models, SM identity, behavior, and attraction individually predicted significantly greater odds of risk behaviors. SM identity became non-significant in final adjusted models with all three orientation components; non-monosexual attraction and behavior continued to predict significantly elevated odds of risk behaviors, remaining associated with sexual risk behavior and drug use over time (attenuated in some cases). Trends in disparities over time between SMW vs. SMJW varied by sexual orientation indicator. In a shifting political and social context, research should include multidimensional sexual orientation constructs to accurately identify all SMW—especially those reporting non-monosexual behavior or attraction—and prioritize their health needs.

Keywords: sexual minority women, sexual orientation, bisexuality, non-monosexual, substance use, sexual risk behavior

INTRODUCTION

Sexual orientation is a multidimensional construct often comprised of sexual identity, sexual behavior, and sexual attraction (Badgett, 2009; Institute of Medicine, 2011). When compared to sexual majority women, sexual minority women—those who self-identify as lesbian, gay, or bisexual (LGB), or who report attraction to or sex with women—have indicated greater levels of substance use and abuse (Hughes, 2011; Lehavot & Simoni, 2011; Parsons, Kelly, & Wells, 2006) and sexual behavior associated with risk for sexually transmitted infections (STIs) and HIV (Goodenow, Szalacha, Robin, & Westheimer, 2008; Herrick, Matthews, & Garofalo, 2010; Lindley, Walsemann, & Carter, 2013).

Research regarding substance use and sexual risk among sexual minority women has varied in its operationalization of sexual orientation, from sexual identity (Hughes, Szalacha, & McNair, 2010; Parsons et al., 2006), to sexual behavior (Bolton & Sareen, 2011; Corliss, Grella, Mays, & Cochran, 2006; Eisenberg, 2001; Kerr, Ding, Burke, & Ott-Walter, 2015; Muzny, Sunesara, Martin, & Mena, 2011), to a combination of identity and behavior (Bauer, Jairam, & Baidoobonso, 2010; Goodenow et al., 2008; Kann, 2011), of identity and attraction (Corliss, Austin, Roberts, & Molnar, 2009), or of identity, behavior, and attraction (Herrick et al., 2010; McCabe, Hughes, Bostwick, West, & Boyd, 2009). Such variation is potentially problematic given that sexual identity, behavior, and attraction are often related but also likely distinct aspects of broader sexual orientation. For example, in the 2011-2013 cycle of the National Survey of Family Growth (NSFG), nearly 13% of females who identified as heterosexual had had same-sex partners and over 25% expressed attraction to women or uncertainty regarding their sexual attractions (Copen, Chandra, & Febo-Vazquez, 2016).

Observed differences in health outcomes among sexual minority vs. sexual majority populations have varied depending on how sexual orientation is measured (Wolff, Wells, Ventura-DiPersia, Renson, & Grov, 2017). Variable measurement of sexual orientation that does not capture the complexities of sexuality has obscured a full understanding of disparities in sexual risk and substance use among sexual minority women (Lindley et al., 2013; Wolff et al., 2017).

Sexual Minority Women and Sexual Behavior Related to STIs and HIV

Minimal research has examined STI/HIV-related sexual behavior among sexual minority women using multidimensional measures of sexual orientation, but studies that have done so have found variable health outcomes by different sexual orientation components. For example, one study looked at adolescent women’s engagement in HIV-related risk behaviors in terms of sexual identity and the sex of sex partners. Lesbian and bisexual identity, as well as a history of female sex partners, both independently predicted increased odds of having numerous sex partners. However, when sexual identity and the sex of sex partners were considered simultaneously, partner sex was a more salient indicator of having numerous partners, outweighing sexual identity (Goodenow et al., 2008). In a study that drew from Youth Risk Behavior Survey data, students who identified as gay or lesbian reported greater rates of previous sexual intercourse in conjunction with substance use compared to bisexually identified students in one survey site. However, when sex under the influence was measured in terms of sex of sex partners, findings were reversed: students with exclusively same-sex partners indicated lower levels of concurrent substance use and sexual intercourse compared to students who had had both male and female sex partners (Kann, 2011). Despite these variations in sexual risk behavior by sexual orientation component, some research has demonstrated that non-monosexual women—that is, women who identify as bisexual or have sex with or express attraction to both men and women—consistently report the highest rates of sexual risk compared to all other women (Everett, 2013; Muzny et al., 2011; Ripley, 2012).

Sexual Minority Women and Substance Use

Substance use outcomes among sexual minority women have also varied by sexual orientation component (Brewster & Tillman, 2012; Matthews, Blosnich, Farmer, & Adams, 2014; McCabe et al., 2009). For example, one study compared substance use and dependence among sexual minority vs. sexual majority adults in the U.S. using indicators of sexual identity, behavior, and attraction. When substance use outcomes were examined in terms of sexual identity, substance use and dependence were elevated for both lesbian and bisexually identified compared to heterosexually identified women. By contrast, when outcomes were examined in terms of sexual behavior, women who had had both male and female sex partners had significant disparities in susbtance use and dependence, but women who had only had female sex partners were no different from those with only male partners (McCabe et al., 2009). In a study of substance use among youth, sexual minority identity was significantly associated with substance use disparities when measured separately from attraction and behavior, but similar to the aforementioned study of HIV-related risk behaviors, identity was no longer significant when regression models also adjusted for sexual attraction and behavior (Brewster & Tillman, 2012). Despite variations by sexual orientation component, non-mono sexual women have also emerged with the greatest substance use disparities compared to other women (Przedworski, McAlpine, Karaca-Mandic, & VanKim, 2014).

Mechanisms of Risk among Sexual Minority Women in the Context of Policy Changes

Minority stress theory suggests that sexual minority individuals experience unique stressors associated with having a sexual minority status (Meyer, 2003). Research has documented a link between experiences of minority stress (e.g., stigma, discrimination, and internalized homophobia) and adverse health outcomes for sexual minority individuals, with stress spurring negative mental health processes (Hatzenbuehler, 2009; Meyer, 2003), substance use (Lehavot & Simoni, 2011), and sexual risk behavior (Goodenow et al., 2008) as potential resultant coping responses.

Some research suggests that sexual identity is a key predictor of health risk behaviors given that openly identifying as a sexual minority exposes individuals to greater likelihood of discrimination, stigma, and violence (McCabe et al., 2009). However, additional research purports that disclosing one’s sexual identity may be protective against health risk behaviors given that disclosure allows for greater access to social support (Juster, Smith, Ouellet, Sindi, & Lupien, 2013). Although some research has shown how different components of sexual orientation differentially predict health disparities for sexual minorities, research remains less conclusive on which sexual orientation components are the most relevant for understanding health disparities among sexual minority women.

Further confounding an understanding of the relationship between various sexual orientation components and health outcomes is how a rapidly evolving political and social climate in the U.S. may impact sexual minority health disparities. Research suggests that a policy environment more favorable to sexual minorities can mitigate minority stress (Hatzenbuehler, McLaughlin, Keyes, & Hasin, 2010; Pachankis et al., 2017). When discrimination and stigma have dissipated and legal protections against discrimination have created a sense of security for sexual minorities, risk behaviors that once functioned as stress coping mechanisms could have also declined (Hatzenbuehler et al., 2010). Between 2000 and 2009, policy changes reflected such growing support of sexual minorities (Movement Advancement Project, 2010). For example, support for permitting openly gay individuals to serve in the military and for gay and lesbian people to adopt children both increased by 21%. The number of states prohibiting sexual orientation-related discrimination increased from 12 to 22, the percent of Fortune 500 companies prohibiting job-based sexual orientation-related discrimination increased from 0.6% to 35%, and the passage of the 2009 Matthew Shepard and James Byrd, Jr. Hate Crimes Prevention Act marked the first U.S. federal law to increase punishment of hate crimes against sexual minorities (Movement Advancement Project, 2010). In 2015, the U.S. Supreme Court pronounced same-sex marriage legal at the federal level (Chappell, 2015).

Much of the existing literature examining the political influence on the health of sexual minorities has treated sexual minority individuals as a composite group without exploring differential effects for various sexual minority and gender subgroups. Most of these studies have measured sexual minority status in terms of sexual identity alone (Hatzenbuehler, 2009, 2011; Hatzenbuehler et al., 2010; Hatzenbuehler, Wieringa, & Keyes, 2011), despite sexual orientation being a multifaceted construct (Laumann, Gagnon, Michael, & Michaels, 1994) with only moderate correlations between sexual identity, behavior, and attraction (Brewster & Tillman, 2012). To our knowledge, only one study examining the impact of structural stigma on sexual minority health has also included sexual attraction and behavior as elements of sexual orientation (Pachankis et al., 2017). Furthermore, although recent research has examined changing trends in health disparities between sexual minorities and sexual majority individuals, these studies have focused on youth and have employed singular measures of sexual orientation (Fish, Watson, Porta, Russell, & Saewyc, 2017; Watson, Goodenow, Porta, Adjei, & Saewyc, 2017).

In addition to policy advances, the new millennium also ushered in increasing social acceptance of sexual minorities (Movement Advancement Project, 2010; Twenge, Sherman, & Wells, 2016). Between 2000 and 2009, the proportion of Americans viewing same-sex relationships as morally acceptable increased by 23% (Movement Advancement Project, 2010). According to the General Social Survey (GSS), the proportion of U.S. adults who perceived same-sex sexual behavior as “not wrong at all” increased from 11% in 1973 to 49% in 2014 (Twenge et al., 2016).

Despite increasingly positive attitudes toward same-sex sexual behavior and burgeoning policy protections for LGB individuals more broadly, research has not found growing positivity toward bisexually identified individuals (Dodge et al., 2016). Perceptions of same-sex relationships do not capture changes in attitudes toward non-monosexual individuals in same- vs. different-sex relationships. Moreover, policies more favorable to sexual minorities may not address the stigma and stereotyping of non-monosexual people that persist among both heterosexual and sexual minority communities (Brewster & Moradi, 2010; Dyar, Feinstein, & London, 2014; Wandrey, Mosack, & Moore, 2015). Because of such ongoing stigma, non-monosexual people may continue to lack community support (Ross et al., 2018) and to face limited social acceptance (Dyar, Feinstein, Schick, & Davila, 2017) regardless of policies that protect against discrimination at an institutional level.

Present Study

In order to understand how each component of sexual orientation contributes to health disparities, both among sexual minority women and compared to sexual majority women, multidimensional measurement of sexual orientation is critical (Wolff et al., 2017). This is especially true in the context of a rapidly shifting political and social environment that can influence the health of sexual minorities overtime (Hatzenbuehler et al., 2010). Specific attention to women who express non-monosexual identity, attraction, and/or sex with more than one sex or gender is also vital given that attitudes toward bisexually identified people have not advanced at the same pace as attitudes toward same-sex relationships more broadly (Dodge et al., 2016).

Using 2002-2015 data from the NSFG, we sought to examine the association of risk behaviors with women’s sexual minority status—with sexual orientation defined in terms of sexual identity, behavior, and attraction—and to explore whether these associations have changed over time as policies implemented during the same period of survey data collection became increasingly supportive of sexual minorities (Movement Advancement Project, 2010).

METHOD

Participants

Data for this study were drawn from the NSFG, a national survey weighted to be representative of the U.S. population age 15 to 44 (Lepkowski et al., 2013). The NSFG began collecting sexual orientation data in its 2002 sample (Groves, Mosher, Lepkowski, & Kirgis, 2009); thus, this study used a combined NSFG dataset from 2002 to 2015 (the most current dataset available at the time of data analysis). The NSFG is unique as one of the few national health surveys to collect data on sexual identity, behavior, and attraction (Wolff et al., 2017).

The NSFG 2002 (Cycle 6) sample included 7,643 female respondents (80% response rate) out of a total sample of 12,571. For the 2006-2010 cycle (continuous interviewing over the cycle), interviews were completed for 48 weeks of each year from June 2006 through June 2010. The sample included 12,279 female respondents (77.7% response rate) out of a total sample of 22,682. The 2011-2015 interviews occurred from September 2011 through September 2015 and included a sample of 11,300 female respondents (response rate 72.3%) out of a total sample of. 20,621 (National Center for Health Statistics, 2016a). Despite the change from 12-month to multi-year data interviewing cycles in the NSFG (Groves et al., 2009), the survey items of interest for the present study remained identical.

Measures

Sexual Orientation

The primary independent variables were the three aforementioned sexual orientation components: sexual identity, sexual attraction, and sexual behavior.

Sexual identity was assessed by responses to the question: “Do you think of yourself as: heterosexual or straight; (2) homosexual, gay, or lesbian; (3) bisexual; or (4) something else?” (Reference = heterosexual/straight). Given that “something else” was not available as a response option in the 2011-2015 survey cycle, we excluded those who identified as such from this analysis.

Sexual attraction was assessed by responses to the question: “People are different in their sexual attraction to other people. Which best describes your feelings? (1) only attracted to males; mostly attracted to males; (3) equally attracted to males and females; (4) mostly attracted to females; (5) only attracted to females; or (6) not sure.” We collapsed responses into a four-category variable: (1) only attracted to males; (2) only attracted to females; (3) attracted to both males and females; or (4) not sure (reference = only attracted to males). “Not sure” was retained for this analysis as a separate category within the four-item variable given that we could not ascertain whether these respondents were simply unsure of their sexual attraction, if they misunderstood the other response options, or if they would have indicated exclusive attraction to males, females, or both had they not been given the “not sure” response option.

Sexual behavior in this analysis was limited to measurement of past-year behavior with male and/or female partners (partner gender identity was not assessed) given that women’s sexual behavior may be fluid across the lifespan as their sexual desires and identity evolve (Diamond, 2003; Meyer & Wilson, 2009). Our sexual behavior variable included four categories: past-year sexual behavior (i.e., oral, vaginal, or anal sex) with (1) only male partners; (2) only female partners; (3) both male and female partners; and (4) no past-year partners (reference = only male partners).

Demographic Characteristics

Demographic characteristics included age in years, race/ethnicity (Hispanic, any race; Black/African-American, non-Hispanic; White, non-Hispanic; and any other race, non-Hispanic), education level (no high school degree to Bachelor’s degree or higher), and marital/cohabitation status (married to/living with versus not married to/not living with a partner). Participants were only directly asked about their marital/cohabitation status with an “opposite sex” partner. NSFG recorded voluntary responses about same-sex partnerships as comments but same-sex relationship status was not formally assessed nor was it integrated into the responses to the marital/cohabitation status question.

STI/HIV-Related Sexual Risk Behavior

We measured past-year STI/HIV-related sexual risk behavior as having engaged in any sexual risk behavior with male partners (again, partner gender identity was not assessed) in the past year, defined as any self-reported transactional sex, sex with male partners with high HIV risk (i.e., HIV-positive males, males who used injection drugs, males who had sex with males, or males who had other concurrent partners of any sex), having had five or more male partners in the past year, or any past-year condomless penile-vaginal intercourse with male partners. Given that the NSFG did not assess for it, we were unable to examine HIV and STI-related sexual risk behaviors with female partners. Any past-year condomless sex was assessed by the following question: “Thinking back over the past 12 months, would you say you used a condom with your partner for sexual intercourse [which referred to penile-vaginal intercourse]: (1) every time; (2) most of the time; (3) about half the time; (4) some of the time; (5) none of the time.” We collapsed responses into a dichotomous indicator for any past year condomless sex (responses two through five), which was included within the larger sexual risk behavior variable. Anal sex was not included given that the NSFG only asked about condom use at last, rather than any, occurrence of anal sex within the previous year.

STI Treatment

We defined STI treatment as report of having been treated for gonorrhea, chlamydia, herpes, or syphilis over the past year. This variable was used in place of a variable assessing for STI diagnoses because in the 2002 and 2006-2010 survey cycles, participants were only asked questions about whether they had received an actual STI diagnosis if they first responded “yes” to questions about whether they had been treated for an STI. As such, using the STI treatment variable avoided excluding participants who may have been diagnosed, but who did not receive treatment for STIs.

Recreational Drug Use

The NSFG assessed past-year recreational drug use in four separate questions using the following language: “During the last 12 months, how often have you: [Question 1] smoked marijuana? [Question 2] used cocaine? [Question 3] used crack? [Question 4] used crystal or meth, also known as tina, crack, or ice?” A fifth question asked, “During the last 12 months, how often have you shot up or injected drugs other than those prescribed to you? “ Response options ranged from “never” to “about once a day.” For this study, we measured recreational drug use in the past year as having used any of the aforementioned substances or engaging in non-prescription injection drug use at least once in the past 12 months. Although crystal methamphetamine use was not assessed in the 2002 sample, meth use was included in the drug use indicator given that all but 5 respondents in the 2006-2010 and 2011-2015 samples that had used crystal methamphetamine had also used other drugs.

Binge Drinking

Binge drinking was assessed through the following question: “During the last 12 months, how often did you have 4 or more drinks within a couple of hours? (1) never; (2) once or twice during the year; (3) several times during the year; (4) about once a month; (5) about once a week; (6) about once a day?” For the purposes of this analysis, we used the Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration definition of binge drinking (National Center on Alcohol Use and Alcoholism, n.d.), which is operationalized as (females) having four or more drinks on one occasion at least once a month (responses 4-6).

Data Analyses

Analyses were conducted on the merged 2002, 2006-2010, and 2011-2015 NSFG female datasets, which the NSFG maintains separately from the male datasets. Weights were adjusted to account for the different survey cycles (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2014).

We first describe the sample overall and stratified by each sexual orientation measure and with sexual orientation and health behaviors stratified by survey cycle. The Rao Scott modified chi square test was used to assess the significance of associations between sexual orientation indicators and categorical variables. Next, we examined the proportion of those within each sexual orientation group reporting each health outcome, stratified across survey cycles. Separate crude logistic regression models then explored the association of each of the three indicators of sexual orientation—sexual identity, sexual behavior, and sexual attraction—with each heath outcome.

Subsequently, four separate multivariable logistic regression models were run for each health outcome, with each model adjusted for age, race, ethnicity, education level, survey cycle, and marital/cohabitation status. The first set of regression models (Models A1 through A4) included sexual identity as the main predictor for each separate outcome, the second set (Models B1 through B4) included past-year sexual attraction as the main predictor, and the third set (Models Cl through C4) included sexual behavior. Finally, we ran an additional set of models (Models D1 through D4) that included all three measures of sexual orientation to look at their independent associations; a separate model was run for each of the four outcomes.

Chi square tests using Cramer’s V coefficients explored correlations between each indicator of sexual orientation to ensure that the indicators were not highly correlated and could be included in the same regression model. Sexual attraction and sexual identity—as well as identity and sexual behavior—were moderately correlated with Cramer’s V coefficients of 0.59 and 0.53 respectively; sexual behavior and sexual attraction had a low correlation with a Cramer’s v coefficient of 0.37.

We then ran the final adjusted models (Models D1 through D4) including all the aforementioned characteristics but leaving out marital/cohabitation status as a sensitivity analysis to see if the results differed without adjustment for this variable, which excluded marital/cohabitation with same-sex partners. The sensitivity analysis also addressed the fact that the large proportion of women reporting past-year sexual risk behavior was driven by the over 90% of married/cohabiting and about 82% of unmarried/not cohabiting respondents who reported condomless sex within the past year. Given that excluding marital/cohabitation status as a potential covariate had a minimal overall effect on the association between sexual minority status and the primary outcomes, the multivariable results include marital/cohabitation status as a covariate.

For Models D1 through D4 (the models that included all three measures of sexual orientation simultaneously), we then assessed whether the association between sexual minority status and each outcome differed by NSFG survey cycle by adding interaction terms for survey cycle by each of the three measures of sexual orientation, using separate interaction terms for each sexual orientation indicator. We were not able to examine interactions of sexual orientation by individual year of survey interview given that discrete interview years are not publicly available (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2017). If the p-value for any of the interaction terms was significant, final combined models were stratified on survey cycle to examine the direction of the effect modification. We report on trends in health outcomes among sexual minority vs. heteterosexual women and women attracted to/who had had sex with only males across survey cycles, focusing on where we observed significant interactions of sexual orientation indicators and survey cycle (Homma, Saewyc, & Zumbo, 2016).

Women who had not had sex with a male partner within the past year were excluded from models examining STI/HIV-related sexual risk behavior (Models A1 through D1). For the models exploring past-year STI treatment, given that only 21 lesbian identified women, 17 women exclusively attracted to females, and five who had sex only with women in the past year reported any past-year STI treatment, these groups were excluded from the adjusted models.

All analyses accounted for the complex sampling method and were weighted to the population. Analyses were performed in SAS 9.3 (SAS Institute, Cary NC) using the proc survey functions. Statistical significance was set at an alpha of .05 for regression models and an alpha of .10 for interaction terms, given that power decreases when examining effect modification (Marshall, 2007).

RESULTS

Table 1 displays demographic characteristics of the female U.S. population drawn from NSFG data, 2002 to 2015. The mean age was 29.68 years (SE = 0.09). About 41% of the sample was legally married to or living with an opposite-sex partner (40.93%) and the majority identified as non-Hispanic white (60.81%). Roughly a quarter of women fell within each category of education level. Most women reported a heterosexual identity (93.68%), being exclusively attracted to males (85.52%), and having had only male partners during the previous year (90.46%).

Table 1.

Demographic Characteristics, National Survey of Family Growth. 2002-2015. Females Aged 15-44 (N=31,222)a

Unweighted n (weighted %)/M age (SE)
Sexual Identity
 Heterosexual 38,217 (93.68)
 Gay/lesbian 508 (1.47)
 Bisexual 1,648 (4.99)
Sexual Attraction
 Only to Males 25,286 (85.52)
 Only to Females 315 (0.88)
 To Both 5,068 (15.62)
 Not sure 337 (0.99)
Sexual Behavior, Past Year
 Only Males 23,417 (90.46)
 Only Females 365 (1.26)
 Both 1,087 (3.54)
 No Sex Partners 1,495 (4.74)
Race & Ethnicity
 Hispanic (any race) 7,164 (17.96)
 Black, Non-Hispanic 6,485 (14.46)
 Other Race, Non-Hispanic 1,848 (6.78)
 White, Non-Hispanic 15,725 (60.81)
Education Level
 No High School 7657 (20.64)
 High School/GED 8,069 (25.05)
 Some College/Associate’s 8,864 (29.14)
 Bachelor’s or Higher 6,631 (25.18)
Married/living with partner 10,461 (40.93)
Age (continuous) 29.68 (0.09)
a

Total n may not add up to 100% due to missing responses.

Table 2 shows the overall proportion within sexual orientation category and engaging in past-year risk behaviors, as well as stratified by NSFG survey cycle. Overall, 87.50% of women who had sex with a male partner in the past year reported any STI/HIV-related sexual risk behavior during that time, but as noted, this was driven by the over 90% of married/cohabiting women who reported any past-year condomless sex with a male partner. Nearly 4% of women reported having received STI treatment within the previous year. Close to 15% of women reported binge drinking once a month or more within the past year; 16.37% indicated any past-year recreational drug use (including marijuana). From 2002 to 2011-2015, women were significantly less likely to report sexual risk behavior (89.43% in 2002 vs. 87.22% in 2011-2015, p = .001) and binge drinking (16.87% in 2002 vs. 12.44% in 2011-2015, p < .001) (Table 2). Despite these declines, health disparities largely persisted between sexual minority and sexual majority women regardless of how sexual orientation was measured (see Table 3). We further describe specific trends below in the context of effect modification.

Table 2.

Sexual Orientation & Past-Year Health Behaviors Stratified by National Survey of Family Growth Survey Cycles,2002-2015, Females Aged 15-44 (N = 31,222)a

2002 (n = 7,643) 2006-2010 (n = 12,279) 2011-2015 (n = 11,300)

Unweighted N (weighted %)/M age (SD) Unweighted n (weighted %) Unweighted n (weighted %) Unweighted n (weighted %) χ2 pb
  Sexual Orientation
Sexual Identity
 Heterosexual 38,217 (93.68) 6,780 (95.35) 11,275 (94.54) 10,162 (92.27) 43.87 < .001
 Gay/lesbian 508 (1.47) 100 (1.34) 197 (1.23) 211 (1.64) 4.20 .123
 Bisexual 1,648 (4.99) 258 (3.29) 591 (4.24) 799 (6.18) 54.21 < .001
Sexual Attraction
 Only to Males 25,286 (85.52) 6,483 (85.78) 9,960 (82.92) 8,843 (80.41) 42.52 < .001
 Only to Females 315 (0.88) 56 (0.67) 124 (0.77) 135 (1.07) 6.00 .050
 To Both 5,068 (15.62) 987 (12.75) 1,985 (15.55) 2,096 (17.30) 34.65 < .001
 Not sure 337 (0.99) 70 (0.80) 117 (0.76) 150 (1.22) 7.49 .023
Sexual Behavior, Past Year
 Only Males 23,417 (90.46) 5,925 (90.06) 9,208 (89.87) 8,284 (91.05) 3.43 .179
 Only Females 365 (1.26) 92 (1.33) 160 (1.11) 113 (1.31) .88 .645
 Both 1,087 (3.54) 222 (3.37) 439 (3.48) 426 (3.68) .67 .717
 No Sex Partners 1,495 (4.74) 410 (5.25) 733 (5.53) 352 (3.96) 13.41 .001
Health Behaviors, Past Year
Sexual risk behaviorc 19,628 (87.50) 4,918 (89.43) 7,576 (86.09) 7,134 (87.22) 14.37 .001
Received medication for STIs 1,504 (3.93) 310 (3.38) 591 (4.00) 603 (4.21) 5.89 .052
Binge drinking ≥ once a month 3,590 (14.81) 951 (16.87) 1,542 (16.95) 1,097 (12.44) 34.20 < .001
Any recreational drug use 5,571 (16.37) 1,308 (16.39) 2,220 (16.63) 2,043 (16.20) .24 .885
a

Total n may not add up to 100% due to missing responses.

b

Results based on Rao-Scott modified chi square tests.

c

Sexual risk behavior defined as: any self-reported past-year transactional sex with male partners, any sex with male partners with high HIV risk (i.e., HIV-positive males, males who use injection drugs, males who had sex with males, or males who had other concurrent partners of any sex), any sex with five or more male partners, or any condomless penile-vaginal intercourse with male partners.

Table 3.

Past-Year Health Behaviors by Sexual Orientation and National Survey of Family Growth Survey Cycles, 2002-2015, Females Aged 15-44 (N = 31,222) a

Sexual Orientation Unweighted N Health Behavior Unweighted n (weighted %) 2002 Unweighted n (weighted %) 2006-2010 Unweighted n (weighted %) 2011-2015 Unweighted n (weighted %) χ2 p b
STI/HIV-Related Sexual Risk Behavior c

Sexual Identity
 Heterosexual 38,217 18,103 (87.37) 4,462 (89.14) 7,082 (86.06) 6,559 (87.14) 11.40 .003
 Gay/lesbian 508 82 (78.32) 31 (89.14) 27 (80.53) 24 (62.98) 28.32 < .001
 Bisexual 1,648 1,093 (89.81) 172 (94.17) 401 (87.57) 520 (89.53) 4.90 .087
Sexual Attraction
 Only to Males 25,286 15,905 (87.23) 4,181 (89.48) 6,111 (85.35) 5,613 (86.96) 18.53 < .001
 Only to Females 315 47 (72.29) 14 (83.51) 17 (80.78) 16 (59.38) 292.96 < .001
 To Both 5,068 3,519 (89.07) 690 (89.19) 1,400 (90.04) 1,429 (88.50) 0.97 .615
 Not sure 337 141 (89.68) 28 (93.22) 47 (79.93) 66 (92.44) 11.21 .004
Sexual Behavior, Past Year
 Only Males 23,417 18,608 (87.39) 4,697 (89.22) 7,184 (86.00) 6,727 (87.17) 12.75 .002
 Only Females 365 0 (0.00) 0 (0.00) 0 (0.00) 0 (0.00) -- --
 Both 1,087 943 (90.46) 192 (93.44) 379 (88.25) 372 (90.13 3.00 .223
 No Sex Partners 1,495 0 (0.00) 0 (0.00) 0 (0.00) 0 (0.00) -- --

Received Medication for STIs

Sexual Identity
 Heterosexual 38,217 1,295 (3.73) 264 (3.20) 508 (3.75) 523 (4.02) 4.92 .086
 Gay/lesbian 508 21 (2.74) 2 (2.52) 11 (3.73) 8 (2.41) 1.15 .564
 Bisexual 1,648 144 (6.88) 15 (5.54) 63 (9.17) 66 (6.36) 3.04 .221
Sexual Attraction
 Only to Males 25,286 1,063 (3.44) 241 (3.10) 408 (3.31) 414 (3.73) 3.22 .200
 Only to Females 315 17 (4.12) 3 (4.54) 5 (4.00) 9 (4.02) 0.06 .968
 To Both 5,068 394 (6.38) 59 (5.16) 171 (7.81) 164 (6.16) 4.19 .123
 Not sure 337 27 (5.93) 6 (4.73) 7 (2.55) 14 (5.60) 5.12 .080
Sexual Behavior
 Only Males 23,417 1,236 (4.28) 260 (3.70) 490 (4.41) 486 (4.55) 4.48 .106
 Only Females 365 5 (1.50) 1 (1.69) 2 (0.81) 2 (1.74) 0.53 .767
 Both 1,087 158 (12.05) 20 (8.70) 73 (12.89) 65 (13.41) 2.19 .334
 No Sex Partners 1,495 37 (2.32) 15 (2.56) 13 (1.77) 9 (2.58) 0.42 .811

Any Recreational Drug Use

Sexual Identity
 Heterosexual 38,217 4,576 (14.83) 1,099 (15.46) 1,851 (15.11) 1,626 (14.32) 1.93 .380
 Gay/lesbian 508 171 (32.40) 21 (20.55) 75 (41.02) 75 (34.13) 12.22 .002
 Bisexual 1,648 723 (42.34) 116 (42.63) 270 (44.75) 337 (41.31) 0.82 .665
Sexual Attraction
 Only to Males 25,286 3,434 (12.46) 883 (13.37) 1,384 (12.38) 1.167 (11.94) 3.20 .203
 Only to Females 315 109 (34.12) 15 (21.97) 45 (38.91) 49 (36.50) 8.62 .013
 To Both 5,068 1,963 (36.11) 395 (36.66) 772 (38.68) 796 (34.55) 3.48 .176
 Not Sure 337 57 (18.90) 12 (19.32) 18 (12.54) 27 (21.03) 1.60 .448
Sexual Behavior
 Only Males 23,417 4,125 (15.89) 988 (15.72) 1,662 (16.54) 1,475 (15.61) 1.05 .591
 Only Females 365 113 (25.08) 23 (23.36) 58 (36.61) 32 (20.32) 6.22 .044
 Both 1,087 621 (55.42) 137 (61.45) 256 (56.83) 228 (51.36) 3.04 .218
 No Sex Partners 1,495 148 (9.64) 38 (9.17) 71 (8.52) 39 (10.95) 1.02 .600

Binge Drinking at Least Once a Month

Sexual Identity
 Heterosexual 38,217 3,075 (13.83) 797 (15.71) 1,369 (16.01) 909 (11.53) 30.92 < .001
 Gay/lesbian 508 102 (25.41) 21 (27.89) 39 (25.79) 42 (19.58) 10.10 .006
 Bisexual 1,648 323 (25.83) 62 (30.45) 121 (28.92) 140 (23.25) 4.33 .115
Sexual Attraction
 Only to Males 25,286 2,487 (12.92) 701 (15.10) 1,080 (14.74) 706 (10.57) 30.45 < .001
 Only to Females 315 71 (26.33) 15 (29.46) 28 (41.19) 28 (19.18) 14.05 .001
 To Both 5,068 984 (22.30) 218 (25.34) 420 (25.47) 346 (19.37) 10.33 .006
 Not Sure 337 43 (27.05) 15 (49.03) 13 (29.98) 15 (17.26) 33.90 < .001
Sexual Behavior
 Only Males 23,417 2,840 (14.40) 764 (16.45) 1,238 (16.75) 838 (11.90) 30.73 < .001
 Only Females 365 67 (23.17) 14 (13.93) 31 (25.16) 22 (27.50) 3.75 .153
 Both 1,087 335 (37.70) 74 (42.68) 148 (44.00) 113 (32.45) 6.63 .036
 No Sex Partners 1,495 89 (9.27) 24 (12.54 49 (10.87) 16 (5.64) 4.87 .088
a

Total N may not add up to 100% due to missing responses.

b

Results based on Rao-Scott modified chi square tests.

c

Sexual risk behavior defined as: any self-reported past-year transactional sex with male partners, any sex with male partners with high HIV risk (i.e., HIV-positive males, males who use injection drugs, males who had sex with males, or males who had other concurrent partners of any sex), any sex with five or more male partners, or any condomless penile-vaginal intercourse with male partners.

Logistic Regressions, Interactions, and Stratified Models

Table 4 shows results of unadjusted (crude) logistic regression models and results of the combined adjusted Models D1-D4. Table 5 shows interactions between each sexual orientation component and each NSFG survey cycle. Table 6 shows final adjusted models stratified by survey cycle.

Table 4.

Logistic Regression Models, National Survey of Family Growth, 2002-2015, Females Aged 15-44

Crude Associations Final Multivariable Models a
OR (95% CI) p AOR (95% CI) p
Past-Year STI/HIV-Related Sexual Risk Behaviors b (Final Multivariable Model D1, n = 22,140)

Sexual Identity n = 22,230
  Heterosexualc 1.00 1.00
  Gay/Lesbian 0.52 (0.26, 1.07) .076 0.57 (0.28, 1.17) .123
  Bisexual 1.28 (0.98, 1.67) .075 1.10 (0.80, 1.51) .548
Sexual Attraction n = 22,600
  Only to Males c 1.00 1.00
  Only to Females 0.38 (0.16, 0.92) .033 0.40 (0.14, 1.16) .093
  To Both 1.19 (1.02, 1.40) .030 1.26 (1.04, 1.53) .016
  Not Sure 1.27 (0.60, 2.71) .527 1.29 (0.54, 3.22) .588
Sexual Behavior, Past Year n = 22,531
  Only Males c 1.00 1.00
  Only Females d -- d
  Both 1.37 (1.04, 1.79) .024 1.75 (1.29, 2.37) < .001
  No Sex Partners d -- d
Survey Cycle n = 22,621
  2002c 1.00 1.00
  2006-2010 0.73 (0.63, 0.85) < .001 0.76 (0.66, 0.89) .001
  2011-2015 0.81 (0.69, 0.94) .007 0.84 (0.73, 0.98) .030
n = 22,621
Age (continuous) 1.05 (1.04, 1.05) < .001 1.04 (1.03, 1.04) < .001
Race & Ethnicity n = 22,621
  Hispanic, Any Race 0.82 (0.71, 0.94) .006 0.83 (0.71, 0.97) .018
  Black, Non-Hispanic 0.78 (0.68, 0.89) < .001 0.94 (0.81, 1.09) .423
  Other Race, Non-Hispanic 0.66 (0.53, 0.81) < .001 0.70 (0.56, 0.87) .001
  White, Non-Hispanic c 1.00 1.00
Education Level n = 22,621
  No High School 0.92 (0.78, 1.06) .231 1.47 (1.24, 1.73) < .001
  High School/GED 1.32 (1.11, 1.57) .002 1.70 (1.43, 2.01) < .001
  Some College/Associate’s 1.07 (0.94, 1.23) .314 1.39 (1.21, 1.59) .018
  Bachelor’s or Higher c 1.00 1.00
Married/Living with Partner n = 22,621
  Yes 2.05 (1.83, 2.30) < .001 2.24 (1.99, 2.53) < .001
  No c 1.00 1.00

Received Medication for STIs in the Past Year (Final Multivariable Model D2, n = 25,289)

Sexual Identity n = 30,339
  Heterosexualc 1.00 1.00
  Gay/Lesbian 0.73 (0.41, 1.28) .268 d
  Bisexual 1.91 (1.43, 2.53) < .001 0.78 (0.54, 1.15) .213
Sexual Attraction n = 30,971
  Only to Males c 1.00 1.00
  Only to Females 1.21 (0.67, 2.19) .535 d
  To Both 1.92 (1.58, 2.32) < .001 1.58 (1.24, 2.02) < .001
  Not Sure 1.77 (1.01, 3.10) .047 0.95 (0.47, 1.93) .888
Sexual Behavior, Past Year n = 26,342
  Only Males c 1.00 1.00
  Only Females 0.34 (0.11, 1.05) .060 d
  Both 3.06 (2.29, 4.10) < .001 1.77 (1.24, 2.52) .002
  No Sex Partners 0.53 (0.30, 0.93) .027 0.42 (0.23, 0.76) .004
Survey Cycle n = 31,040
  2002 c 1.00 1.00
  2006-2010 1.91 (1.00, 1.43) .057 1.18 (0.97, 1.43) .100
  2011-2015 1.26 (1.04, 1.51) .018 1.25 (1.01, 1.54) .042
n = 31,040
Age (continuous) 0.97 (0.96, 0.98) < .001 0.97 (0.96, 0.98) < .001
Race & Ethnicity n = 31,040
  Hispanic, Any Race 1.28 (1.02, 1.59) .031 1.15 (0.87, 1.49) .297
  Black, Non-Hispanic 2.21 (1.85, 2.65) < .001 1.72 (1.40, 2.10) < .001
  Other Race, Non-Hispanic 1.22 (0.81, 1.84) .349 1.26 (0.76, 2.11) .372
  White, Non-Hispanic c 1.00 1.00
Education Level n = 31,040
  No High School 1.95 (1.49, 2.56) < .001 1.75 (1.30, 2.37) < .001
  High School/GED 1.84 (1.41, 2.40) < .001 1.35 (1.03, 1.78) .029
  Some College/Associate’s 1.70 (1.27, 2.27) < .001 1.31 (0.98, 1.75) .065
  Bachelor’s or Higher c 1.00 1.00
Married/Living with Partner n = 31,040
  Yes 0.55 (0.46, 0.65) < .001 0.54 (0.45, 0.64) < .001
  No c 1.00 1.00

Any Past-Year Recreational Drug Use (Final Multivariable Model D3, n = 25,792)

Sexual Identity n = 30,323
  Heterosexual c 1.00 1.00
  Gay/Lesbian 2.75 (2.13, 3.54) < .001 0.81 (0.50, 1.31) .381
  Bisexual 4.22 (3.59, 4.95) < .001 1.14 (0.90, 1.43) .272
Sexual Attraction n = 30,956
  Only to Males c 1.00 1.00
  Only to Females 3.64 (2.62, 5.06) < .001 3.11 (1.84, 5.26) < .001
  To Both 3.97 (3.61, 4.38) < .001 2.91 (2.54, 3.34) < .001
  Not Sure 1.64 (1.01, 2.67) .048 2.03 (1.23, 3.34) .006
Sexual Behavior, Past Year n = 26,372
  Only Males c 1.00 1.00
  Only Females 1.77 (1.27, 2.47) .001 0.52 (0.34, 0.91) .004
  Both 6.58 (5.27, 8.21) < .001 2.21 (1.70, 2.88) < .001
  No Sex Partners 0.57 (0.34, 0.74) < .001 0.42 (0.31, 0.56) < .001
Survey Cycle n = 31,057
  2002 c 1.00 1.00
  2006-2010 1.02 (0.98, 1.16) .795 0.99 (0.86, 1.14) .903
  2011-2015 0.99 (0.87, 1.12) .834 0.91 (0.79, 1.05) .206
n = 31,057
Age (continuous) 0.94 (0.94, 0.95) < .001 0.94 (0.94, 0.95) < .001
Race & Ethnicity n = 31,057
  Hispanic, Any Race 0.67 (0.60, 0.76) < .001 0.57 (0.49, 0.66) < .001
  Black, Non-Hispanic 1.04 (0.94, 1.16) .445 0.78 (0.69, 0.88) < .001
  Other Race, Non-Hispanic 0.68 (0.54, 0.86) .001 0.76 (0.57, 1.01) .058
  White, Non-Hispanic c 1.00 1.00
Education Level n = 31,057
  No High School 2.00 (1.74, 2.29) < .001 1.44 (1.21, 1.71) < .001
  High School/GED 1.89 (1.67, 2.16) < .001 1.47 (1.26, 1.71) < .001
  Some College/Associate’s 2.00 (1.75, 2.28) < .001 1.52 (1.33, 1.74) < .001
  Bachelor’s or Higher c 1.00 1.00
Married/Living with Partner n = 31,057
  Yes 0.42 (0.38, 0.46) < .001 0.46 (0.41, 0.52) < .001
  No c 1.00 1.00

Any Past-Year Binge Drinking at Least Once a Month (Final Multivariable Model D4, n = 20,252)

Sexual Identity n = 22,663
  Heterosexualc 1.00 1.00
  Gay/Lesbian 2.12 (1.50, 3.00) < .001 1.06 (0.51, 2.19) .881
  Bisexual 2.17 (1.77, 2.65) < .001 1.09 (0.83, 1.42) .546
Sexual Attraction n = 23,012
  Only to Males c 1.00 1.00
  Only to Females 2.41 (1.58, 3.67) < .001 1.92 (0.90, 4.11) .093
  To Both 1.93 (1.70, 2.20) < .001 1.44 (1.20, 1.71) < .001
  Not Sure 2.50 (1.51, 4.13) < .001 1.71 (0.73, 4.00) .214
Sexual Behavior, Past Year n = 20,569
  Only Males c 1.00 1.00
  Only Females 1.79 (1.10, 2.93) .020 0.82 (0.34, 1.95) .649
  Both 3.60 (2.87, 4.52) < .001 2.03 (1.54, 2.66) < .001
  No Sex Partners 0.61 (0.42, 0.88) .008 0.44 (0.30, 0.66) < .001
Survey Cycle n = 23,061
  2002c 1.00 1.00
  2006-2010 1.01 (0.86, 1.17) 0.94 1.04 (0.89, 1.21) .622
  2011-2015 0.70 (0.60, 0.82) < .001 0.69 (0.59, 0.80) < .001
0.70 (0.60, 0.82) n = 23,061
Age (continuous) 0.96 (0.95, 0.96) < .001 0.97 (0.96, 0.97) < .001
Race & Ethnicity n = 23,061
  Hispanic, Any Race 0.88 (0.76, 1.03) .108 0.87 (0.74, 1.03) .103
  Black, Non-Hispanic 0.03 (0.72, 0.96) .013 0.64 (0.54, 0.76) < .001
  Other Race, Non-Hispanic 0.8 (0.54, 0.87) .002 0.70 (0.53, 0.92) .010
  White, Non-Hispanic c 1.00 1.00
Education Level n = 23,061
  No High School 1.71 (1.42, 2.05) < .001 1.10 (0.88, 1.38) .387
  High School/GED 1.93 (1.62, 2.30) < .001 1.51 (1.25, 1.83) < .001
  Some College/Associate’s 1.82 (1.54, 2.14) < .001 1.42 (1.20, 1.67) < .001
  Bachelor’s or Higher c 1.00 1.00
Married/Living with Partner n = 23,061
  Yes 0.49 (0.43, 0.54) < .001 0.49 (0.42, 0.56) < .001
  No c 1.00 1.00
a

All final multivariable models adjusted for age, race/ethnicity, education level, marital/cohabitation status, and survey cycle.

b

Sexual risk behavior defined as: any self-reported past-year transactional sex with male partners, any sex with male partners with high HIV risk (i.e., HIV-positive males, males who use injection drugs, males who had sex with males, or males who had other concurrent partners of any sex), any sex with five or more male partners, or any condomless penile-vaginal intercourse with male partners.

c

Reference category

d

Excluded due to inapplicability or small sample size.

Table 5.

Trends in Health Disparities among Sexual Minority Women in the National Survey of Family Growth, 2002-2015, Females Aged 15-44; Interactions between Sexual Orientation Components and Survey Cycles in Logistic Regression Models D1-D4

Interactions AOR (95% CI) p
Model D1 - Past-Year STI/HIV-Related Sexual Risk Behaviors a

Sexual Identity by Survey Cycle
   Heterosexual by 2002 b 1.00
   Lesbian by 2006-2010 0.56 (0.07, 4.39) .581
   Bisexual by 2006-2010 0.40 (0.14, 1.09) .074
   Lesbian by 2011-2015 0.25 (0.04, 1.61) .145
   Bisexual by 2011-2015 0.63 (0.24, 1.64) .340
Sexual Attraction by Survey Cycle
   Only to Males by 2002 b 1.00
   Only to Females by 2006-2010 2.65 (0.19, 37.39) .472
   To Both by 2006-2010 2.02 (1.31, 3.12) .002
   Not Sure by 2006-2010 0.49 (0.07, 3.56) .478
   Only to Females by 2011-2015 1.80 (0.16, 20.62) .638
   To Both by 2011-2015 1.27 (0.82, 1.97) .295
   Not Sure by 2011-2015 1.80 (0.28, 11.68) .537
Past-Year Sexual Behavior by Survey Cycle
   Only Males by 2002 b 1.00
   Both by 2006-2010 0.53 (0.21, 1.34) .180
   Both by 2011-2015 0.76 (0.32, 1.79) .527

Model D2 - Received Medication for STIs in the Past Year

Sexual Identity by Survey Cycle
   Heterosexual by 2002 b 1.00
   Bisexual by 2006-2010 1.33 (0.41, 4.31) .638
   Bisexual by 2011-2015 0.97 (0.31, 3.05) .954
Sexual Attraction by Survey Cycle
   Only to Males by 2002 b 1.00
   To Both by 2006-2010 1.44 (0.77, 2.70) .257
   Not Sure by 2006-2010 0.30 (0.06, 1.43) .133
   To Both by 2011-2015 1.01 (0.53, 1.93) .966
   Not Sure by 2011-2015 0.46 (0.11, 1.98) .297
Past-Year Sexual Behavior by Survey Cycle
   Only Males by 2002 b 1.00
   Both by 2006-2010 0.79 (0.26, 2.42) .687
   No Sex Partners by 2006-2010 0.60 (0.20, 1.74) .346
   Both by 2011-2015 1.22 (0.44, 3.39) .700
   No Sex Partners by 2011-2015 0.84 (0.23, 3.10) .798

Model D3 - Any Past-Year Recreational Drug Use

Sexual Identity by Survey Cycle
   Heterosexual by 2002 b 1.00
   Lesbian by 2006-2010 3.60 (1.16, 11.17) .028
   Bisexual by 2006-2010 1.40 (0.81, 2.45) .232
   Lesbian by 2011-2015 1.89 (0.61, 5.87) .269
   Bisexual by 2011-2015 2.22 (1.27, 3.87) .005
Sexual Attraction by Survey Cycle
   Only to Males by 2002 b 1.00
   Only to Females by 2006-2010 0.67 (0.18, 2.48) .554
   To Both by 2006-2010 1.17 (0.83, 1.65) .372
   Not Sure by 2006-2010 0.29 (0.06, 1.44) .133
   Only to Females by 2011-2015 1.76 (0.48, 6.40) .393
   To Both by 2011-2015 1.05 (0.75, 1.46) .788
   Not Sure by 2011-2015 0.47 (0.12, 1.88) .288
Past-Year Sexual Behavior by Survey Cycle
   Only Males by 2002 b 1.00
   Only Females by 2006-2010 0.73 (0.24, 2.26) .592
   Both by 2006-2010 0.53 (0.28, 1.00) .052
   No Sex Partners by 2006-2010 0.80 (0.46, 1.39) .420
   Only Females by 2011-2015 0.31 (0.10, 0.95) .042
   Both by 2011-2015 0.46 (0.25, 0.85) .015
   No Sex Partners by 2011-2015 1.19 (0.62, 2.27) .604

Model D4 - Any Past-Year Binge Drinking at Least Once a Month

Sexual Identity by Survey Cycle
   Heterosexual by 2002 b 1.00
   Lesbian by 2006-2010 1.25 (0.30, 5.25) .761
   Bisexual by 2006-2010 0.83 (0.46, 1.51) .546
   Lesbian by 2011-2015 0.30 (0.06, 1.61) .160
   Bisexual by 2011-2015 1.15 (0.61, 2.15) .670
Sexual Attraction by Survey Cycle
   Only to Males by 2002 b 1.00
   Only to Females by 2006-2010 0.59 (0.11, 3.13) .532
   To Both by 2006-2010 1.12 (0.76, 1.63) .568
   Not Sure by 2006-2010 0.32 (0.04, 2.57) .287
   Only to Females by 2011-2015 0.66 (0.11, 4.02) .649
   To Both by 2011-2015 1.08 (0.70, 1.66) .741
   Not Sure by 2011-2015 0.37 (0.06, 2.43) .300
Past-Year Sexual Behavior by Survey Cycle
   Only Males by 2002 b 1.00
   Only Females by 2006-2010 1.67 (0.46, 5.99) .435
   Both by 2006-2010 1.04 (0.53, 2.05) .908
   No Sex Partners by 2006-2010 0.80 (0.35, 1.84) .600
   Only Females by 2011-2015 6.40 (1.29, 31.83) .024
   Both by 2011-2015 0.84 (0.43, 1.62) .598
   No Sex Partners by 2011-2015 0.58 (0.21, 1.57) .287
a

Sexual risk behavior defined as: any self-reported past-year transactional sex with male partners, any sex with male partners with high HIV risk (i.e., HIV-positive males, males who use injection drugs, males who had sex with males, or males who had other concurrent partners of any sex), any sex with five or more male partners, or any condomless penile-vaginal intercourse with male partners.

b

Reference category

Table 6.

Final Multivariable Models (D1, D3, D4) Stratified by National Survey of Family Growth Survey Cycle, Females Aged 15-44

2002 (n = 5,126) 2006-2010 (n = 8,776) 2011-2015 (n =8,143)
AOR (95% CI) p AOR (95% CI) p AOR (95% CI) p
Past-Year STI/HIV-Related Sexual Risk Behaviors,a Stratified Model D1

Sexual Identity
  Heterosexual b 1.00 1.00 1.00
  Gay/Lesbian 1.04 (0.24, 4.54) .233 0.63 (0.14, 2.75) .537 0.27 (0.09, 0.84) .024
  Bisexual 1.69 (0.70, 4.06) .244 0.70 (0.41, 1.19) .192 1.17 (0.76, 1.82) .474
Sexual Attraction
  Only to Males b 1.00 1.00 1.00
  Only to Females 0.29 (0.04, 1.89)) .194 0.78 (0.12, 5.19) .793 0.52 (0.11, 2.38) .395
  To Both 0.90 (0.65, 1.25) .531 1.92 (1.43, 2.57) < .001 1.18 (0.87, 1.61) .285
  Not Sure 1.18 (0.30, 4.63) .817 0.58 (0.13, 2.560 .470 2.22 (0.59, 8.32) .238
Sexual Behavior, Past Year
  Only Males b 1.00 1.00 1.00
  Only Females c -- c -- c --
  Both 2.31 (1.08, 4.95) .032 1.31 (0.75, 2.27) .343 1.99 (1.30, 3.07) .002
  No Sex Partners c -- c -- c --
Age (continuous) 1.02 (1.01, 1.04) .012 1.04 (1.03, 1.05) < .001 1.04 (1.02, 1.05) < .001
Race & Ethnicity
  Hispanic, Any Race 0.92 (0.69, 1.24) .594 0.93 (0.73, 1.18) .558 0.77 (0.61, 0.97) .028
  Black, Non-Hispanic 1.03 (0.75, 1.40) .866 0.92 (0.71, 1.19) .528 0.90 (0.73, 1.12) .355
  Other Race, Non-Hispanic 0.84 (0.57, 1.23) .366 0.57 (0.38, 0.86) .007 0.74 (0.52, 1.04) .085
  White, Non-Hispanic b 1.00 1.00 1.00
Education Level
  No High School 1.39 (0.95, 2.03) .087 1.84 (1.44, 2.34) < .001 1.26 (0.98, 1.62) .067
  High School/GED 2.26 (1.60, 3.21) < .001 1.62 (1.29, 2.04) < .001 1.59 (1.20, 2.10) .001
  Some College/Associate’s 1.30 (0.99, 1.72) .062 1.31 (1.05, 1.63) .015 1.52 (1.24, 1.89) < .001
  Bachelor’s or Higher b 1.00 1.00 1.00
Married/Living with Partner
  Yes 1.73 (1.34, 2.25) < .001 2.26 (1.88, 2.72) < .001 2.51 (2.08, 3.03) < .001
  No b 1.00 1.00 1.00
2002 (n = 6,374) 2006-2010 (n = 10,408) 2011-2015 (n = 9,110)
AOR (95% CI) p AOR (95% CI) p AOR (95% CI) p

Sexual Identity
  Heterosexual b 1.00 1.00 1.00
  Gay/Lesbian 0.38 (0.18, 0.81) .126 1.45 (0.64, 1.34) .375 0.77 (0.34, 1.77) .537
  Bisexual 0.62 (0.39, 0.98) .041 0.96 (0.69, 1.35) .807 1.52 (1.08, 2.14) .017
Sexual Attraction
  Only to Males b 1.00 1.00 1.00
  Only to Females 3.00 (1.17, 7.08) .022 1.88 (0.75, 4.71) .175 4.71 (1.88, 11.80) .001
  To Both 2.74 (2.09, 3.61) < .001 3.16 (2.54, 3.93) < .001 2.78 (2.25, 3.44) < .001
  Not Sure 4.05 (1.09, 15.14) .037 1.17 (0.45, 3.03) .744 2.03 (1.06, 3.87) .033
Sexual Behavior, Past Year
  Only Males b 1.00 1.00 1.00
  Only Females 0.93 (0.36, 2.40) .885 0.69 (0.35, 1.35) .275 0.33 (0.17, 0.63) .001
  Both 3.91 (2.39, 6.40) < .001 2.15 (1.44, 3.20) < .001 1.81 (1.22, 2.69) .003
  No Sex Partners 0.39 (0.26, 0.60) < .001 0.31 (0.20, 0.47) < .001 0.54 (0.31, 0.94) .028
Age (continuous) 0.94 (0.93, 0.96) < .001 0.96 (0.95, 0.97) < .001 0.93 (0.92, 0.94) < .001
Race & Ethnicity
  Hispanic, Any Race 0.47 (0.36, 0.63) < .001 0.48 (0.38, 0.60) < .001 0.67 (0.54, 0.83) < .001
  Black, Non-Hispanic 0.62 (0.49, 0.77) < .001 0.74 (0.59, 0.94) .013 0.91 (0.76, 1.09) .316
  Other Race, Non-Hispanic 1.01 (0.51, 2.01) .976 0.85 (0.53, 1.36) .492 0.60 (0.42, 0.85) .004
  White, Non-Hispanic b 1.00 1.00 1.00
Education Level
  No High School 1.90 (1.33, 2.72) .001 1.52 (1.16, 2.00) .003 1.14 (0.86, 1.51) .363
  High School/GED 1.85 (1.39, 2.46) < .001 1.33 (1.04, 1.70) .022 1.39 (1.08, 1.79) .002
  Some College/Associate’s 1.61 (1.22, 2.12) .001 1.21 (0.98, 1.50) .071 1.67 (1.34, 2.07) < .001
  Bachelor’s or Higher b 1.00 1.00 1.00
Married/Living with Partner
  Yes 0.40 (0.31, 0.52) < .001 0.43 (0.36, 0.52) < .001 0.52 (0.43, 0.63) < .001
  No b 1.00 1.00 1.00
2002 (n = 4,840) 2006-2010 (n = 8,163) 2011-2015 (n = 7,249)
AOR (95% CI) p AOR (95% CI) p AOR (95% CI) p

Sexual Identity
  Heterosexual b 1.00 1.00 1.00
  Gay/Lesbian 1.59 (0.76, 3.34) .217 1.85 (0.51, 6.69) .350 0.44 (0.10, 1.93) .278
  Bisexual 1.01 (0.64, 1.58) .983 0.88 (0.59, 1.29) .503 1.23 (0.81, 1.85) .336
Sexual Attraction
  Only to Males b 1.00 1.00 1.00
  Only to Females 2.82 (0.80, 9.97) .108 1.88 (0.53, 6.68) .329 2.14 (0.54, 8.42) .278
  To Both 1.26 (0.92, 1.71) .153 1.43 (1.13, 1.81) .003 1.48 (1.08, 2.02) .014
  Not Sure 3.09 (0.81, 11.82) .099 1.34 (0.27, 6.62) .720 1.50 (0.48, 4.67) .481
Sexual Behavior, Past Year
  Only Males b 1.00 1.00 1.00
  Only Females 0.29 (0.13, 0.64) .002 0.49 (0.17, 1.39) .179 1.99 (0.52, 7.68) .315
  Both 2.02 (1.21, 3.37) .007 2.32 (1.48, 3.64) < .001 1.92 (1.25, 2.97) .003
  No Sex Partners 0.53 (0.28, 1.00) .048 0.45 (0.25, 0.79) .006 0.34 (0.15, 0.75) .008
Age (continuous) 0.96 (0.95, 0.98) < .001 0.96 (0.94, 0.97) < .001 0.98 (0.96, 0.99) .003
Race & Ethnicity
  Hispanic, Any Race 0.75 (0.55, 1.01) .057 0.69 (0.51, 0.93) .013 1.07 (0.83, 1.39) .593
  Black, Non-Hispanic 0.52 (0.40, 0.69) < .001 0.52 (0.39, 0.68) < .001 0.83 (0.62, 1.10) .197
  Other Race, Non-Hispanic 0.81 (0.49, 1.35) .422 0.47 (0.30, 0.73) .001 0.83 (0.56, 1.23) .352
  White, Non-Hispanic b 1.00 1.00 1.00
Education Level
  No High School 1.35 (0.91, 2.00) .140 0.95 (0.70, 1.27) .714 1.04 (0.67, 1.62) .851
  High School/GED 1.61 (1.12, 2.33) .011 1.32 (1.01, 1.72) .043 1.63 (1.18, 2.26) .004
  Some College/Associate’s 1.54 (1.14, 2.08) .005 1.26 (1.01, 1.58) .039 1.46 (1.09, 1.96) .012
  Bachelor’s or Higher b 1.00 1.00 1.00
Married/Living with Partner
  Yes 0.32 (0.25, 0.41) < .001 0.47 (0.38, 0.59) < .001 0.56 (0.44, 0.72) < .001
  No b 1.00 1.00 1.00
a

Sexual risk behavior defined as: any self-reported past-year transactional sex with male partners, any sex with male partners with high HIV risk (i.e., HIV-positive males, males who use injection drugs, males who had sex with males, or males who had other concurrent partners of any sex), any sex with five or more male partners, or any condomless penile-vaginal intercourse with male partners.

b

Reference category

c

Excluded from analyses due to inapplicability or small sample size.

Past-Year Sexual Behavior Related to STI/HIV Risk

This set of results describes findings from logistic regression models with past-year STI/HIV sexual risk behaviors as the main outcome. We first examined sexual risk behaviors by sexual orientation indicators in unadjusted models, and subsequently in three separate models (adjusted for age, race/ethnicity, education level, survey cycle, and marital/cohabitation status) by sexual identity (Model A1), attraction (Model B1), and behavior (Model C1). In Model D1, we then adjusted for all three sexual orientation indicators simultaneously. Below, we report on unadjusted models and the combined Model D1; see Supplemental tables for Models A1-C1.

In unadjusted logistic regression models (Table 4), women reporting women attracted to males and females (OR: 1.19, p = .030), as well as both male and female partners within the past year (OR: 1.37, p = .024) had significantly greater odds of past-year sexual risk behavior compared to women who only had male partners, while women reporting attraction only to females had 62% decreased odds of sexual risk behavior (OR: 0.38, p = .033). Sexual minority identity was not a significant predictor of elevated sexual risk behavior in unadjusted models (Table 4).

In Model D1, which included all three indicators of sexual orientation simultaneously, sexual identity remained non-significant, and attraction to both males and females (AOR: 1.26, p = .016) as well as having had partners of both sexes in the past year (AOR: 1.75, p = < .001) remained significantly associated with greater odds of sexual risk behavior (Table 4).

Next, we added interaction terms between each sexual orientation measure and survey cycle to Model D1. There was significant interaction between bisexual identity and survey cycle 2006-2010 (p = .074) and between attraction to both males and females and cycle 2006-2010 (p = .002) (Table 5). Because we found significant interactions, we stratified Model D1 by survey cycle to examine effect modification (Table 6).

In stratified models (see Table 6), bisexual compared to heterosexual identity was associated with 1.69 times the odds of sexual risk behavior (p = .244) in 2002 but a 30% decreased odds in 2006-2010 (p = .192). An odds ratio less than 1 for the interaction of bisexual identity and survey cycle 2006-2010 (AOR: 0.40; see Table 5) indicates that the disparity in sexual risk behavior between women who identified as bisexual and heterosexual narrowed from the 2002 to the 2006-2010 survey cycles (Homma et al., 2016). Reporting attraction to both males and females was associated with a non-significant decreased odds of sexual risk behavior in 2002 (AOR: 0.90, p = .531) but a significantly increased odds of risk behavior in the 2006-2010 cycle (AOR: 1.92, p < .001). An odds ratio greater than 1 for the interaction of attraction to both males and females and survey cycle 2006-2010 (AOR: 2.02; see Table 5) indicates that the disparity in sexual risk behavior between women attracted to both males and females compared to those exclusively attracted to males widened from 2002 to 2006-2010 (Homma et al., 2016). Figures 1 and 2 visually depict changes in the proportion of these groups that engaged in sexual risk behaviors, without adjustment for age, race/ethnicity, education level, or marital status.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

STI/HIV-Related Sexual Risk Behavior by Sexual Identity, National Survey of Family Growth, 2002-2015, Females Aged 15-44

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

STI/HIV-Related Sexual Risk Behavior by Sexual Attraction, National Survey of Family Growth, 2002-2015, Females Aged 15-44

Past-Year STI Treatment

Here, we report on findings from logistic regression models with past-year STI treatment as the main outcome. We first examined STI treatment by sexual orientation indicators in unadjusted models, and subsequently in three separate models for each sexual orientation indicator (Model A2: by sexual identity; Model B2: by sexual attraction; Model C2: by sexual behavior), adjusted for age, race/ethnicity, education level, survey cycle, and marital/cohabitation status. In Model D2, we then adjusted for all three sexual orientation indicators simultaneously. Below, we report on unadjusted models and the combined Model D2; see Supplemental tables for Models A2-C2.

In unadjusted models (see Table 4), non-monosexuality in terms of identity, behavior, and attraction were each associated with greater odds of STI treatment as compared to exclusive heterosexuality (bisexual identity OR: 1.91, p < .001; attraction to both males and females OR: 1.92,p < .001; sex with both male and female partners in the past year OR: 3.06, p < .001) (Table 4). In the model that combined all three indicators of sexual orientation (Model D2, Table 4), bisexual identity was no longer significantly associated with STI treatment in the past year, but being attracted to and having both male and female partners remained significant (AOR: AOR: 1.58, p < .001; 1.77, p = .002 respectively) (Table 4).

Next, we added interaction terms between each sexual orientation measure and survey cycle to Model D2. Since none of the interactions were significant, we did not stratify Model D2 by survey cycle.

Past-Year Drug Use

Next, we report on findings from logistic regressions with past-year drug use as the main outcome. We first examined drug use by sexual orientation indicators in unadjusted models, and subsequently in three separate models for each sexual orientation component (Model A3: by sexual identity; Model B3: by sexual attraction; Model C3: by sexual behavior) adjusted for age, race/ethnicity, education level, survey cycle, and marital/cohabitation status. In Model D3, we then adjusted for all three sexual orientation indicators simultaneously. Below, we report on unadjusted models and the combined Model D3; see Supplemental tables for Models A3-C3.

In unadjusted logistic regression models (see Table 4), sexual minority identity (lesbian OR: 2.75, p < .001; bisexual OR: 4.22, p < .001), attraction (only attracted to females OR: 3.64, p < .001; attracted to both males and females OR: 3.97, p < .001), and behavior (exclusively female past-year partners OR: 1.77, p = .001, both male and female past-year partners OR: 6.58, p < .001) were all significantly associated with increased odds of self-reported past-year drug use compared to sexual majority women. For each measure of sexual orientation, women with self-reported non-monosexual identity, attraction, and behavior had the greatest odds of past-year drug use compared to other groups.

In Model D3, which adjusted for all three measures of sexual orientation, sexual minority identity was no longer a significant predictor of drug use, while attraction exclusively to females (AOR: 3.11, p < .001) and attraction to both males and females (AOR: 2.91, p < .001), as well as having both male and female past-year sex partners (AOR: 2.21, p < .001) remained significantly associated with higher odds of drug use. Having exclusively female past-year sex partners, however, was associated with lower odds of drug use in this adjusted model, predicting a 48% decreased odds of past-year use compared to those with exclusively male past-year partners (p = .004) (Table 4).

Next, we added interaction terms between each sexual orientation measure and survey cycle to Model D3 (see Table 5). Because we found significant interaction between lesbian identity and survey cycle 2006-2010 (p = .028); bisexual identity and 2011-2015 (p = .005); having exclusively female past-year partners and cycle 2011-2015 (p = .042); having both male and female past-year partners in cycles 2010-2006 (p = .052) and 2011-2015 (p = .015) (Table 5), we then stratified Model D3 by survey cycle.

Lesbian identity predicted non-significant decreased odds of drug use in 2002 (AOR: 0.38, p = .126) but increased odds in 2006-2010 (AOR: 1.45, p = .375). A similar pattern was observed for bisexually identified women: Bisexual identity was associated with significantly decreased odds of drug use in 2002 (AOR: 0.62, p = .041) but increased odds in 2011-2015 (AOR: 1.52, p = .017). An odds ratio greater than 1 for interactions between lesbian identity and survey cycle 2006-2010 (AOR: 3.60) and for the interaction of bisexual identity and survey cycle 2011-2015 (AOR: 2.22, see Table 5 for interactions) demonstrates that the disparities in drug use among lesbian vs. heterosexual women widened from 2002 to 2006-2010, and among bisexual vs. heterosexual women from 2002 to 2011-2015 (Homma et al., 2016).

Having both male and female past-year sex partners was associated with significantly higher odds of past-year drug use relative to women with exclusively male past-year partners in all three survey cycles, but with the strength of the association decreasing over time (2002 AOR: 3.91, p < .001; 2006-2010 AOR: 2.15, p < .001; 2011-2015 AOR: 1.81, p = .003) (Table 6). Having exclusively female partners was associated with decreasing odds of drug use compared to women with exclusively male partners in each survey cycle, but the association was only significant in the 2011-2015 cycle (2002 AOR: 0.93 p = .885; 2006-2010 AOR: 0.69, p = .275; 2011-2015 AOR: 0.33, p = .001) (Table 6). Odds ratios less than 1 for the interactions between sex with male and female partners and survey cycles 2006-2010 (AOR: 0.53) and 2011-2015 (AOR: 0.46), and between exclusively female sex partners and 2011-2015 (AOR: 0.31; see Table 5 for interactions) indicate narrowing disparities in drug use among sexual minority women compared to women with exclusively male partners (Homma et al., 2016). See Figs. 3 and 4 for visual representations of these changing trends without adjustment for age, race/ethnicity, education level, or marital status.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Recreational Drug Use by Sexual Identity, National Survey of Family Growth, 2002-2015, Females Aged 15-44

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Drug Use by Sexual Behavior, National Survey of Family Growth, 2002-2015, Females Aged 15-44

Past-Year Binge Drinking Once a Month or More

Finally, we report on findings from logistic regression models with past-year binge drinking once a month or more as the main outcome. We first examined binge drinking by sexual orientation indicators in unadjusted models, and subsequently in three models adjusted for age, race/ethnicity, education level, survey cycle, and marital/cohabitation status: Model A4 examined past-year binge drinking by sexual identity, Model B4 by sexual attraction, and Model C4 by sexual behavior. In Model D4, we then adjusted for all three sexual orientation indicators simultaneously. Below, we report on unadjusted models and the combined Model D4; see Supplemental tables for Models A4-C4.

In the unadjusted logistic regression models (see Table 4) examining self-reported past-year binge drinking at least once a month, women reporting exclusive attraction to females had the greatest odds of past-year binge drinking when compared to women who reported exclusive attraction to males (OR: 2.41, p < .001; attraction to both OR: 1.93, p < .001); however, women with a bisexual identity (OR: 2.17, p < .001; lesbian identity OR: 2.12, p < .001) and with both male and female past-year partners (OR: 3.60,p < .001; exclusively female partners OR: 1.79,p = .020) had the highest odds of past-year binge drinking compared to heterosexually identified women and women who only had sex with male partners in the past year.

In Model D4 that included all three measures of sexual orientation, sexual minority identity was no longer significantly associated with self-reported binge drinking in the past year. Attraction to both males and females (AOR: 1.44, p < .001) and having had both male and female past-year partners (AOR: 2.03,p < .001) remained significant predictors of binge drinking compared to those with exclusively male past-year male partners.

When we added interaction terms between each sexual orientation measure and survey cycle to Model D4, we found significant interaction between having exclusively female past-year partners and survey cycle 2011-2015 (p = .024, see Table 5). Having only female partners (versus only male partners) predicted decreased odds of drinking in 2002 (AOR: 0.29, p = .002) and 2006-2010 (AOR: 0.49, p = .179) but was associated with (non-significant) greater odds of binge drinking in 2011-2015 (AOR: 1.99, p = .315). An odds ratio greater than 1 (AOR: 6.40) for the interaction of exclusively female partners and survey cycle 2011-2015 (see Table 5) shows that the disparity in binge drinking compared to those with exclusively male partners widened from 2002 to 2011-2015 (Homma et al., 2016) (see Fig. 5).

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Binge Drinking by Sexual Behavior, National Survey of Family Growth, 2002-2015, Females Aged 15-44

Results with Non-Monosexual Women as the Reference Group

Given that non-monosexual women generally presented with the highest levels of risk behavior, we re-ran the final models (D1-D4) with bisexual identity, attracted to both males and females, and past-year sex with both males and females as reference groups to assess if differences between non-monosexual and monosexual women were significant. In most cases, we found that non-monosexual women, regardless of how sexual orientation was measured, had significantly higher odds of risk behavior compared to both monosexual sexual minority (e.g., lesbian identified or exclusively female partners) and sexual majority women (e.g., heterosexual identified or exclusively male partners). See Supplemental Table 5 for full details.

DISCUSSION

We examined three dimensions of sexual orientation—sexual identity, behavior, and attraction—across three survey cycles of the NSFG. Concurrently accounting for sexual identity behavior, and attraction allowed for exploration of which sexual orientation components are independently associated with each health behavior relative to sexual majority women. The results of this study echo previous research showing that sexual minority orientation is associated with elevated odds of risky health behaviors among women, and that sexual attraction and behavior emerged as stronger predictors of health disparities between sexual minority and sexual majority women than sexual identity (Brewster & Tillman, 2012; Goodenow et al., 2008).

Similar to existing literature, non-monosexual women generally had the greatest disparities relative to other groups. Health disparities for women identifying as bisexual seemed to be explained by attraction to and sex with both males and females, rather than having a bisexual identity in and of itself. We suggest three hypotheses for this finding. First, women who identify as bisexual may have greater access to supportive communities as bisexual identity becomes increasingly common in the U.S. (England, Mishel, & Caudillo, 2016). Social support may protect against the deleterious effects of minority stress (Hatzenbuehler, 2009) among bisexual women (MacKay, Robinson, Pinder, & Ross, 2017). For women who express attraction to or have sex with both men and women without assuming a bisexual or other non-monosexual (e.g., queer; pansexual) identity, however, community connection may not be as readily available. Second, the association between both-sex attractions, as well as both-sex sexual partners and health risk behaviors may also be a result of concealment of a bisexual or other non-monosexual identity in anticipation of rejection and/or discrimination (Paul, Smith, Mohr, & Ross, 2014), which has been linked to decreased social support (Pachankis, 2007). Coming out as bisexual can decrease internalized bisexual stigma (Paul et al., 2014), but women who express bisexual attractions and behaviors without assuming a bisexual or other non-monosexual identity may not experience the health benefits of coming out. Finally, “internalized monosexism” (e.g., internalizing stereotypes that non-monosexuality is a temporary identity or indicative of promiscuity) (Dyar et al., 2017) may also contribute to greater health disparities among women expressing non-monosexual attraction and behavior (Goodenow et al., 2008). For non-monosexual people who do not divulge a bisexual identity, internalized monosexism appears to be particularly heightened (Dyar et al., 2017).

Changing Health Disparity Trends Across NSFG Survey Cycles

In models stratified by NSFG survey cycle, we found variable trends in health disparities depending on how sexual orientation was measured. For example, disparities in sexual risk behavior between bisexually and heterosexually identified women narrowed from the 2002 to the 2006-2010 survey cycles, but disparities between women attracted to males and females and those attracted exclusively to males widened from 2002 to 2006-2010. In models with drug use as the outcome, disparities widened between bisexually and heterosexually identified women, but narrowed between women with both male and female partners and those with only male partners from 2006-2010 to 2011-2015. Thus, the measure of sexual orientation used to assess trends across survey cycles impacts depiction of health disparities between sexual minority and sexual majority women.

Although researchers may determine at the data analysis phase that not all indicators of sexual orientation are relevant to the research questions at hand, we recommend that each component be made available within national health surveys to allow for more robust analyses of sexual orientation data and to provide health researchers with more options to explore which elements of sexual orientation are associated with health disparities among sexual minorities (Wolff et al., 2017), particularly women expressing non-monosexuality. We also suggest that sexual minority health disparity studies explore correlations between each component of sexual orientation to determine whether different indicators of orientation are measuring different constructs and thus should be examined separately (Wolff et al., 2017).

Although disparities dissipated for some groups over time, non-monosexuality generally remained associated with increased odds of health risks across survey cycles. Specifically, non-monosexual attraction remained associated with elevated odds of STI/HIV-related sexual risk and non-monosexual behavior with drug use across survey cycles. The potential for lack of community support, identity concealment, and internalized monosexism may be responsible for these enduring health risks. As recently reviewed by Ross et al. (2018), bisexual individuals continue to report a dearth of community support. As mentioned above, attitudes toward bisexuality have not improved over time as they have toward lesbian and gay identity (Dodge et al., 2016) and persistent monosexism appears to uphold a negative social climate for non- monosexual individuals (Dyar et al., 2017).

Limitations

Our findings should be understood in light of their limitations. First, we were limited to defining our variables in terms of the survey items at hand. Sexual attraction and behavior questions asked about attraction to and sex with “males” and “females” without considering individuals who may not identity as male or female or how respondents might express attraction in terms of partner gender identity rather than sex. Drug use measures did not assess for opioid (prescription or heroin) use, despite a growing epidemic in the United States during the administration of the NSFG survey cycles (Califf, Woodcock, & Ostroff, 2016). Survey questions also did not assess for substance use disorders. Although research has demonstrated that sex between female partners may facilitate STI transmission (Marrazzo & Gorgos, 2012; Ripley, 2012), the NSFG did not include items assessing for these behaviors. Our analyses were thus limited to examining women’s HIV/STI-related sexual risk with male partners. Additionally, the NSFG question on marital/cohabitation status does not formally assess same-sex partnerships. Since marriage has been shown to be protective against health risks such as substance use (Urquia, O’Campo, & Ray, 2013), future research might explore differences in health outcomes for married vs. unmarried sexual minority individuals.

Data were self-reported and thus subject to social desirability bias, particularly questions regarding potentially stigmatizing behavior like same-sex sexual behavior and substance use (Bauer et al., 2010). Despite a total sample of over 30,000 women, samples remained small for sexual minority women. This prevented a more in-depth analysis of risk behaviors by sexual minority status, and the inability to detect significant associations due to low statistical power (Type 2 error).

The results from this study should be applicable to the U.S. population given that the NSFG is a nationally representative sample; however, they are unlikely to apply to other populations that have different levels of acceptance or stigma associated with sexual minority status. NSFG data are not weighted on sexual orientation since there are no existing population-level data on this variable. Finally, we were unable to examine interactions of sexual orientation by NSFG interview year given that the NSFG does not have sufficient sample sizes to make valid statistical inferences for any 12-month period. Furthermore, the NSFG was not designed to be nationally representative for calendar years because of its continuous design (National Center for Health Statistics, 2016b). We were thus limited to more crude analyses involving interactions of sexual orientation components and NSFG survey cycles.

Despite these limitations, this study compared sexual minority and sexual majority women over time using measurement methods that include all three components of sexual orientation, which we feel is an improvement on previous methods that used only one definition of sexual minority status.

Conclusions

This investigation documented several important findings related to sexual minority women’s health outcomes and how those outcomes may vary over time. Our findings indicate that measurement methods matter when assessing associations between sexual orientation and health risk behaviors and future research should account for components of sexual behavior and attraction in addition to sexual identity in study design and instrumentation. Further, including all three constructs of sexual orientation in regression models exploring health risks among sexual minority women is needed to identify which groups have the greatest health disparities. To identify the specific mechanisms that contribute to elevated risks for non-monosexual women observed in multiple survey cycles, future research should investigate health outcomes separately among women who express non-monosexuality in terms of identity, behavior, and/or attraction rather than considering all sexual minority women as a composite group.

Multilevel interventions could be implemented to achieve health equity for all sexual minority women. For example, at the individual level, substance use disorder treatment programs for sexual minority women could address women’s experiences with stigma and discrimination and work with women to develop healthy coping mechanisms (Meyer, 2003). At a community level, tailored prevention strategies (e.g., social marketing campaigns) could address both the association between substance use and sexual risk behavior among sexual minority women, as well as the variations in sexual risk behavior by partner gender or sex and sexual identity. Finally, at the institutional and policy levels, the findings from this study suggest that policies and laws supporting sexual minorities should continue to be developed and monitored. Although some policies have resulted in improving acceptance of sexual minorities, the impact of a long history of homophobia, bigotry, and violence against sexual minorities may continue to contribute to ongoing adverse health outcomes among sexual minority women. Emerging research also suggests that the 2016 presidential election has again contributed to a hostile, discriminatory environment for sexual minorities (Veldhuis, Drabble, Riggle, Wootton, & Hughes, 2018). Policies and programs that support resiliency for all sexual minorities—especially non-monosexual women—are critical to ensure optimal health outcomes.

Supplementary Material

1

Acknowledgments

Funding: This study did not receive funding as the NSFG is a free and publicly-available data set.

This study did not receive funding as the NSFG is a free and publicly available data set. Dr. Margaret Paschen-Wolff is now supported by a training Grant (T32 MH019139; PI: Theodorus Sandfort, Ph.D.) from the National Institute of Mental Health at the HIV Center for Clinical and Behavioral Studies at the NY State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University (P30-MH43520; Center Principal Investigator: Robert Remien, Ph.D.).

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest: The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Ethical approval: All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent: Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Publisher's Disclaimer: Disclaimer: The findings and conclusions in the article are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official position of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.

REFERENCES

  1. Badgett M (2009). Best practices for asking questions about sexual orientation on surveys. Retrieved from http://williamsinstitute.law.ucla.edu/wp-content/uploads/SMART-FINAL-Nov-2009.pdf
  2. Bauer GR, Jairam JA, & Baidoobonso SM (2010). Sexual health, risk behaviors, and substance use in heterosexual-identified women with female sex partners: 2002 US National Survey of Family Growth. Sexually Transmitted Diseases, 37, 531–537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bolton S-L, & Sareen J (2011). Sexual orientation and its relation to mental disorders and suicide attempts: Findings from a nationally representative sample. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 56, 35–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Brewster KL, & Tillman KH (2012). Sexual orientation and substance use among adolescents and young adults. American Journal of Public Health, 102, 1168–1176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brewster M, & Moradi B (2010). Perceived experiences of anti-bisexual prejudice: Instrument development and evaluation. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 57, 451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Califf RM, Woodcock J, & Ostroff S (2016). A proactive response to prescription opioid abuse. New England Journal of Medicine, 374, 1480–1485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2014). Combining YRBS data across years and sites. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/healthvvouth/data/vrbs/pdf/vrbs_combining_data.pdf
  8. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2017). National Survey of Family Growth: Questionnaires, datasets, and related documentation. Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nsfg/nsfg_questionnaires.htm
  9. Chappell B (2015). Supreme Court declares same-sex marriage legal in all 50 states. Retrieved from http://www.npr.org/sections/thetwo-wav/2015/06/26/417717613/supreme-court-rules-all-states-must-allow-same-sex-marriages
  10. Copen CE, Chandra A, & Febo-Vazquez I (2016). Sexual behavior, sexual attraction, and sexual orientation among adults aged 18-44 in the United States: Data from the 2011-2013 National Survey of Family Growth. National Health Statistics Reports, 88, 1–14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Corliss HL, Austin SB, Roberts AL, & Molnar BE (2009). Sexual risk in “mostly heterosexual” young women: Influence of social support and caregiver mental health. Journal of Women’s Health, 18, 2005–2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Corliss HL, Grella CE, Mays VM, & Cochran SD (2006). Drug use, drug severity, and help-seeking behaviors of lesbian and bisexual women. Journal of Women’s Health, 15, 556–568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Diamond LM (2003). Was it a phase? Young women’s relinquishment of lesbian/bisexual identities over a 5-year period. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 352–364. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dodge B, Herbenick D, Friedman MR, Schick V, Fu T-CJ, Bostwick W, … Reece M. (2016). Attitudes toward bisexual men and women among a nationally representative probability sample of adults in the United States. PloS ONE, 11, e0164430. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0164430 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dyar C, Feinstein BA, & London B (2014). Dimensions of sexual identity and minority stress among bisexual women: The role of partner gender. Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity, 1, 441–451. [Google Scholar]
  16. Dyar C, Feinstein BA, Schick V, & Davila J (2017). Minority stress, sexual identity uncertainty, and partner gender decision making among nonmonosexual individuals. Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity, 4, 87–104. [Google Scholar]
  17. Eisenberg M (2001). Differences in sexual risk behaviors between college students with same-sex and opposite-sex experience: results from a national survey. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 30, 575–589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. England P, Mishel E, & Caudillo ML (2016). Increases in sex with same-sex partners and bisexual identity across cohorts of women (but not men). Sociological Science, 3, 951–970. [Google Scholar]
  19. Everett BG (2013). Sexual orientation disparities in sexually transmitted infections: examining the intersection between sexual identity and sexual behavior. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 225–236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fish JN, Watson RJ, Porta CM, Russell ST, & Saewyc EM (2017). Are alcohol - related disparities between sexual minority and heterosexual youth decreasing? Addiction, 112, 1931–1941. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Goodenow C, Szalacha LA, Robin LE, & Westheimer K (2008). Dimensions of sexual orientation and HIV-related risk among adolescent females: Evidence from a statewide survey. American Journal of Public Health, 98, 1051–1058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Groves R, Mosher M, Lepkowski J, & Kirgis N (2009). Planning and development of the continuous National Survey of Family Growth. Vital and Health Statistics, 1, 1–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hatzenbuehler ML (2009). How does sexual minority stigma “get under the skin”? A psychological mediation framework. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 707–730. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hatzenbuehler ML (2011). The social environment and suicide attempts in lesbian, gay, and bisexual youth. Pediatrics, 127, 896–903. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hatzenbuehler ML, McLaughlin KA, Keyes KM, & Hasin DS (2010). The impact of institutional discrimination on psychiatric disorders in lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations: A prospective study. American Journal of Public Health, 100, 452–459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hatzenbuehler ML, Wieringa NF, & Keyes KM (2011). Community-level determinants of tobacco use disparities in lesbian, gay, and bisexual youth: Results from a population- based study. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 165, 527–532. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Herrick AL, Matthews AK, & Garofalo R (2010). Health risk behaviors in an urban sample of young women who have sex with women. Journal of Lesbian Studies, 14, 80–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Homma Y, Saewyc E, & Zumbo BD (2016). Is it getting better? An analytical method to test trends in health disparities, with tobacco use among sexual minority vs. heterosexual youth as an example. International Journal for Equity in Health, 15, 79–87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hughes T (2011). Alcohol use and alcohol-related problems among sexual minority women. Alcoholism Treatment Quarterly, 29, 403–435. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hughes T, Szalacha LA, & McNair R (2010). Substance abuse and mental health disparities: Comparisons across sexual identity groups in a national sample of young Australian women. Social Science & Medicine, 71, 824–831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Institute of Medicine. (2011). The health of lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender people: Building a foundation for better understanding. Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences; [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Juster R-P, Smith NG, Ouellet E, Sindi S, & Lupien SJ (2013). Sexual orientation and disclosure in relation to psychiatric symptoms, diurnal cortisol, and allostatic load. Psychosomatic Medicine, 75, 103–116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kann L (2011). Sexual identity, sex of sexual contacts, and health-risk behaviors among students in grades 9-12: Youth risk behavior surveillance, selected sites, United States, 2001-2009. MMWR Surveillance Summaries, 60, 1–133. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kerr D, Ding K, Burke A, & Ott-Walter K (2015). An alcohol, tobacco, and other drug use comparison of lesbian, bisexual, and heterosexual undergraduate women. Substance Use & Misuse, 50, 340–349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Laumann EO, Gagnon JH, Michael RT, & Michaels S (1994). The social organization of sexuality: Sexual practices in the United States. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  36. Lehavot K, & Simoni JM (2011). The impact of minority stress on mental health and substance use among sexual minority women. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79, 159–170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Lepkowski JM, Mosher WD, Groves RM, West BT, Wagner J, & Gu H (2013). Responsive design, weighting, and variance estimation in the 2006-2010 National Survey of Family Growth. Vital and Health Statistics, 2, 1–42. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lindley LL, Walsemann KM, & Carter JW (2013). Invisible and at risk: STDs among young adult sexual minority women in the United States. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 45, 66–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. MacKay J, Robinson M, Pinder S, & Ross LE (2017). A grounded theory of bisexual individuals’ experiences of help seeking. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 87, 52–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Marrazzo JM, & Gorgos LM (2012). Emerging sexual health issues among women who have sex with women. Current Infectious Disease Reports, 14, 204–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Marshall SW (2007). Power for tests of interaction: Effect of raising the Type I error rate. Epidemiologic Perspectives & Innovations, 4, 1–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Matthews DD, Blosnich JR, Farmer GW, & Adams BJ (2014). Operational definitions of sexual orientation and estimates of adolescent health risk behaviors. LGBT Health, 1, 42–49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. McCabe SE, Hughes TL, Bostwick WB, West BT, & Boyd CJ (2009). Sexual orientation, substance use behaviors and substance dependence in the United States. Addiction, 104, 1333–1345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Meyer IH (2003). Prejudice, social stress, and mental health in lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations: conceptual issues and research evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 674–697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Meyer IH, & Wilson PA (2009). Sampling lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 56, 23–31. [Google Scholar]
  46. Movement Advancement Project. (2010). A decade of progress on LGBT rights. Retrieved from http://www.lgbtmap.org/policv-and-issue-analvsis/a-decade-of-progress-on-lgbt-rights
  47. Muzny CA, Sunesara IR, Martin DH, & Mena LA (2011). Sexually transmitted infections and risk behaviors among African American women who have sex with women: Does sex with men make a difference? Sexually Transmitted Diseases, 38, 1118–1125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. National Center for Health Statistics. (2016a). 2013-2015 National Survey of Family Growth: Public Use Data and Documentation. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nsfg/nsfg_2013_2015_puf.htm
  49. National Center for Health Statistics. (2016b). 2013-2015 NSFG User’s Guide Appendix 2: SAS and STATA guidelines for common file manipulations. Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/nsfg/nsfg_2013-2015_ug_app2_filemanipulations_rev.pdf
  50. National Center on Alcohol Use and Alcoholism. (n.d.). Drinking levels defined. Retrieved from https://www.niaaa.nih.gov/alcohol-health/overview-alcohol-consumption/moderate-binge-drinking
  51. Pachankis JE (2007). The psychological implications of concealing a stigma: A cognitive- affective-behavioral model. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 328–345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Pachankis JE, Hatzenbuehler ML, Mirandola M, Weatherburn P, Berg RC, Marcus U, & Schmidt AJ (2017). The geography of sexual orientation: Structural stigma and sexual attraction, behavior, and identity among men who have sex with men across 38 European countries. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 46, 1491–1502. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Parsons JT, Kelly BC, & Wells BE (2006). Differences in club drug use between heterosexual and lesbian/bisexual females. Addictive Behaviors, 31, 2344–2349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Paul R, Smith NG, Mohr JJ, & Ross LE (2014). Measuring dimensions of bisexual identity: Initial development of the Bisexual Identity Inventory. Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity, 1, 452–460. [Google Scholar]
  55. Przedworski JM, Me Alpine DD, Karaca-Mandic P, & VanKim NA (2014). Health and health risks among sexual minority women: An examination of 3 subgroups. American Journal of Public Health, 104, 1045–1047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Ripley V (2012). An invisible issue: Women who have sex with women may be more at risk of infection than realised. Nursing Standard, 26, 22–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Ross LE, Salway T, Tarasoff LA, MacKay JM, Hawkins BW, & Fehr CP (2018). Prevalence of depression and anxiety among bisexual people compared to gay, lesbian, and heterosexual individuals: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Sex Research, 55, 435–456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Twenge JM, Sherman RA, & Wells BE (2016). Changes in American adults’ reported same-sex sexual experiences and attitudes, 1973-2014. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45, 1713–1730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Urquia ML, O’Campo PJ, & Ray JG (2013). Marital status, duration of cohabitation, and psychosocial well-being among childbearing women: a Canadian nationwide survey. American Journal of Public Health, 103, e8–el5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Veldhuis CB, Drabble L, Riggle ED, Wootton AR, & Hughes TL (2018). “We won’t go back into the closet now without one hell of a fight”: Effects of the 2016 presidential election on sexual minority women’s and gender minorities’ stigma-related concerns. Sexuality Research and Social Policy, 15, 12–24. [Google Scholar]
  61. Wandrey RL, Mosack KE, & Moore EM (2015). Coming out to family and friends as bisexually identified young adult women: A discussion of homophobia, biphobia, and heteronormativity. Journal of Bisexuality, 15, 204–229. [Google Scholar]
  62. Watson RJ, Goodenow C, Porta C, Adjei J, & Saewyc E (2017). Substance use among sexual minorities: Has it actually gotten better? Substance Use & Misuse, 0, 1–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Wolff M, Wells B, Ventura-DiPersia C, Renson A, & Grov C (2017). Measuring sexual orientation: A review and critique of US data collection efforts and Implications for health policy. The Journal of Sex Research, 54, 507–531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1

RESOURCES