Abstract
Emotion plays a crucial role, both in general human experience and in psychiatric illnesses. Despite the importance of emotion, the relative lack of objective methodologies to scientifically studying emotional phenomena limits our current understanding and thereby calls for the development of novel methodologies, such us the study of illustrative animal models. Analysis of Drosophila and other insects has unlocked new opportunities to elucidate the behavioral phenotypes of fundamentally emotional phenomena. Here we propose an integrative model of basic emotions based on observations of this animal model. The basic emotions are internal states that are modulated by neuromodulators, and these internal states are externally expressed as certain stereotypical behaviors, such as instinct, which is proposed as ancient mechanisms of survival. There are four kinds of basic emotions: happiness, sadness, fear, and anger, which are differentially associated with three core affects: reward (happiness), punishment (sadness), and stress (fear and anger). These core affects are analogous to the three primary colors (red, yellow, and blue) in that they are combined in various proportions to result in more complex “higher order” emotions, such as love and aesthetic emotion. We refer to our proposed model of emotions as called the “Three Primary Color Model of Basic Emotions.”
Keywords: basic emotions, core affection, monoamine, evolution, instinct, emotion flow, Drosophila
Introduction
Emotions are fundamental to human life (Kvajo, 2016); when expressed pathologically, psychiatric disorders of emotional regulation, such as depressive and bipolar disorders, are leading causes of medical disability. Despite the importance of emotion in human health and illness, scientists struggle to reach consensus on the constructs underlying emotional phenomena and experiences (LeDoux, 1995; LeDoux J., 2012). In addition, controversy abounds over the definitions of emotion, the number of discrete, fundamental emotional states that exist, and the degree to which different emotions have distinct neurophysiological signatures (LeDoux, 1995; Damasio, 1997). Insects, such as Drosophila, offer animal models for studying the mechanisms of fundamental emotional processes (Anderson and Adolphs, 2014; Hoopfer, 2016; Shpigler et al., 2017). It is important to use the powerful analytical tools available in invertebrate model organisms to understand the evolutionary origins and neurobiological underpinnings of emotions. Darwin (1876) found that insects use certain behaviors to express feelings homologous to human emotions, such as stridulation, which is the act of creating sounds by rubbing certain body parts together and can represent a range of various emotions in some insects. Darwin (1876) assumed that emotion-associated behavioral phenotypes are easily recognizable in many species, including insects (Anderson and Adolphs, 2014).
We have developed a theory of primary emotions using behavioral observations of Drosophila. Basic emotions are internal states induced by basical bodily changes, and can in turn induce genetically “hardwired” instinctual behaviors. They are highly conserved throughout evolution, and exhibit certain functional and adaptive properties that are shared across a wide phylogenetic range. For example, emotions such as fear and anger are thought to have evolved in response to fundamental life challenges and threats. Anderson and Adolphs (2014) suggested that these primary emotions (when combined) provide a framework for creating various types of secondary emotions, such that elements of primary emotions can be combined with the experience of other, higher order emotions that are more affected by specific learning and experience. Using this approach, primary emotions are observable in evolutionarily diverse organisms, allowing us to functionally “dissect” the mechanisms of the presumed associated internal emotional states and their externally manifest behaviors. There are many reports associating presumed fear and anger emotions with “fight or flight” behaviors in Drosophila (Kravitz and Fernandez, 2015). From analysis of these basic emotions and their associated behavioral phenotypes in animal models, we elucidate mechanisms of basic emotions in humans and propose to utilize this insight to define the mechanisms of disorders of emotional regulation.
Emotion Theories
During the last century, the two most widely accepted theories in affect studies are basic emotion theory and dimensional theory. However, these two theories have been contradictory to each other, and have been described as being in a “100 years war” against each other (Lindquist et al., 2013; Barrett and Russell, 2015). The difference lies in whether emotions are characterized as discrete entities or an independent dimension (Bestelmeyer et al., 2017). Here we give an integrative theory in which we propose that these two theories not necessarily contradictory.
Basic Emotion Theory
Basic emotion theory has been very influential for more than half a century, providing inspiration for interventions in psychopathology (Saarimaki et al., 2016; Celeghin et al., 2017; Williams, 2017; Hutto et al., 2018; Song and Hakoda, 2018; Vetter et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2018). Theories about basic emotions originated from ancient Greece and China (Russell, 2003). Current basic emotion theory started with Darwin (1872) and Ekman (2003), and later (Tomkins, 1962), subsequently followed by Ekman (1984), and Izard (1977), then by many current psychologists (Ortony and Turner, 1990; Panksepp, 2007; Scarantino and Griffiths, 2011; Gu et al., 2016, 2018; Saarimaki et al., 2016; Hutto et al., 2018). Basic emotion theory proposes that human beings have a limited number of emotions (e.g., fear, anger, joy, sadness) that are biologically and psychologically “basic” (Wilson-Mendenhall et al., 2013), each manifested in an organized recurring pattern of associated behavioral components (Ekman, 1992a; Russell, 2006). Izard (1977) argued that the basic emotions are preserved because their biological and social functions are essential in evolution and adaption; he further suggested that basic emotions have innate neural substrates and universal behavioral phenotypes (Shpigler et al., 2017). In a special issue of Emotion Review, several research psychologists outlined the latest thinking about each theoretical model of basic emotions (Plutchik, 1962; Ekman and Friesen, 1969; Ekman, 2003; Izard, 2010, 2011; Ekman and Cordaro, 2011; Levenson, 2011; Panksepp and Watt, 2011; Tracy and Randles, 2011).
Basic emotions evolved to handle fundamental life tasks, e.g., fear and anger can aid survival by influencing an organism to either flee for safety or fight to defend itself. The elements of basic emotions can be combined to form complex or compound emotions (Ekman, 1992b). Even though many psychologists have accepted the theory of basic emotions, there is no consensus about the precise number of basic emotions. Robert Plutchik proposed eight primary emotions: anger, fear, sadness, disgust, surprise, anticipation, trust and joy, and arranged them in a color wheel. Ekman proposed seven basic emotions: fear, anger, joy, sad, contempt, disgust, and surprise; but he changed to six basic emotions: fear, anger, joy, sadness, disgust, and surprise. However, a recent study found that disgust and anger shared similar wrinkled nose, and fear and surprise shared raised eyebrows (Jack et al., 2014). The differences between anger and disgust and the differences between fear and surprise, are thought to have developed later for social functions and not for survival per se (Mansourian et al., 2016). As such, Jack et al. (2014) proposed that we humans have four basic emotions: fear, anger, joy, and sad. Notably, other authors have also proposed fear, anger, joy, and sadness as four basic emotions (Gu et al., 2015, 2016; Wang and Pereira, 2016; Zheng et al., 2016). As Izard said: people need the category label of fear to explain flight to one another for safety, anger to explain the frustration of blocked goal responses, joy (or its equivalent) to explain the pride of achievement, and sadness to explain the experience of a life-changing loss (Izard, 2007).
Dimensional Theory of Emotion
Dimensional studies of emotions originated from Wundt (1897), later followed by Scholsberg (1954), who proposed that emotions can be defined by three-independent dimensions: pleasant-unpleasant, tension-relaxation, and excitation-calm. Later, many others found that the last two dimensions are actually overlapping. Ekman (1957) also proposed a pleasant-unpleasant and active-passive scale as sufficient to capture the difference among emotions. Then Russell’s (1980) invented the circumplex, and proposed that all emotions can be arranged in a circle controlled by two independent dimensions: hedonic (pleasure-displeasure) and arousal (rest-activated) (Figure 1, left) (Russell, 1980; Russell and Barrett, 1999; Posner et al., 2005; Barrett and Russell, 2015). The horizontal axis of the circumplex is hedonic and the vertical axis is arousal; accordingly, the different location of each emotion on the quadrant reflects varying amounts of hedonic and arousal properties (Figure 1; Posner et al., 2005).
Integration of Basic Emotion Theory and Dimensional Theory
Basic emotion approach differs from the dimensional approach in that the latter suggests that emotions are fundamentally the same, only differing in intensity or pleasantness (Ekman, 2003), while the former proposes that emotions are composed of limited number of basic emotions. Here we propose an integrative approach wherein basic emotions also differs in the intensity or pleasantness, like all other emotion. Therefore, basic emotion theory is not contradictory to dimensional theory. The dimensional approach proposes that every emotion has different amounts of hedonic and arousal value. The hedonic (pleasure-displeasure) and arousal (rest-activated) value, which can be called core affects (Russell, 2003; Barrett et al., 2007), are essential features of all emotions, including basic emotions (Gu et al., 2016). Therefore, basic emotions, like all other emotions can find their location in the circumplex.
The specificity of basic emotions on the circumplex is that they located on the axis of the dimensions, which might be the reason that they are “basic.” Happiness and sadness are on the opposite sides of the horizontal dimension, implying that they are a reflection of the hedonic value of the stimulus and unrelated to the safety value (Figure 1, left). Fear and anger are on the vertical axis, implying they are based on the safety value of the stimulus and independent upon the hedonic value of the stimulus. Because of their special location, the basic emotions (fear or anger) have “0” amounts of hedonic value, and the basic emotion of joy or sadness has “0” amounts of safety value. Therefore, the reason for basic emotions being “special” lies in that they only represent only one core affect, because of their specific location. Thus, we introduce a prerequisite conditions for basic emotions: Basic emotions should locate on the axis of the two emotional dimensions. Emotions located on the axis of the dimensions are basic emotions, which might suggest that we have four basic emotions: fear-anger, joy, and sadness. Complex emotions can also find their locations on the quadrant, such as love or aesthetic emotions.
Factors Affecting the Locations of Emotion in the Dimensional Plane
Ekman (1992b) said all emotions differ in the stimulation events, appraisals, behavioral response, and physiological responses. The locations of all emotions on the dimensions can be defined by these factors. According to the appraisal theory, emotions are internal states which are activated by stimulation events. Every stimulation event has two features: whether it is fit for our needs (hedonic value), and whether it happens as expected (arousal) (Figure 1, right). These two features correspond to the two core affects: the hedonic value that represents a physiological need, and the safety value represents the way the stimulus happens (Wang and Pereira, 2016; Zheng et al., 2016). The two-dimensional coordinate plane or the core affects also represents these values of the stimulus: whether it happens as expected, and whether it is fit for our needs (Gu et al., 2016). Lazarus distinguished two kinds of appraisals at a stressful stimulation (Figure 1, right) (Izard, 1977). The first is automatic, unconscious, fast-activating, is related to harm and threat, and induces fearful emotion to motivate avoidance and withdrawal, whereas the second appraisal (he named reappraisal) is conscious and associated with coping (Lazarus, 1999; Zheng et al., 2016). Fear and anger both result from unexpected stressful events, and while fear is associated with feelings of uncertainty, anger is associated with planning to cope with the stressful situation (Figure 1, right) (Moons et al., 2010). Ultimately, fear and anger depend on the manner the stimulation event occurs (Gu et al., 2016), or fear and anger are “twin” emotions, they are two sides of one coin (Gu et al., 2015), and they locate on the top of the vertical dimension.
The locations of emotions on the dimensional plane are also predicated by the behaviors they might induce. Emotion is an internal state, not a behavior (Baker, 2004). Emotion is a tendency of behavior (Roseman, 1984), because the emotion can be separated from the behavioral actions it induces. For example, we can block the actions associated with angry emotion. However, the emotion induced behaviors have two features: the direction and the tension of the behavior (LeDoux, 1998; LeDoux and Brown, 2017). These features can be reflected on the dimensions: hedonic value decides the approach/avoidance, the vertical dimensions decides the tension of the behavior (Wang and Pereira, 2016; Zheng et al., 2016). Thus, the locations of the emotions on the dimension can also be determined by the behaviors they might induce. Fear and anger can induce “flight or fight,” which have opposite directions: fear is in the negative direction (Certel et al., 2010), while fight is in the positive direction. Analogously, joy and sadness can induce approaching or avoidance, respectively (Arnott and Elwood, 2009).
Therefore, like what Lazard suggested, in face of a stressful situation, the individual will collect energy to cope with the situation with “fear or anger” emotions, and induce “fight or flight” actions. After coping with the situation, the individual will have an opportunity for reappraisal, which Lazarus named as emotion-focused coping (Izard, 1977). If the individual have successfully coped with the stressful situation, he/she will feel happy; otherwise, he will feel sad (Aldwin, 1994; Lazarus, 1999). Therefore, Frijida said, sadness is acceptance of the failure, without more efforts to fight (Frijda, 1986). In all, the two dimensions of emotions not only represent the two different feature of a stimulus (hedonic value and safety value), but also represent two features of the behaviors they induced: direction (approach or avoidance) and activation.
Evolutionary Aspects of Basic Emotion
Human emotions are poorly understood even though there is a long venerable tradition of research directly at understanding them. One problem is that physiologists and psychologists who study emotions do not necessarily look at emotion enough from an evolutionary standpoint (Ramachandran and Jalal, 2017). This is unfortunate because as Dobzhansky famously said, “Nothing in biology makes any sense except in the light of evolution” (Ramachandran and Jalal, 2017). However, comparative psychologists have also struggled with the problem of emotion because emotions cannot be directly observed or measured, and we have no access to the animals’ subjective experiences (Ramachandran and Jalal, 2017). Human being and some other animals (such as primates) can consciously know the subjective feelings (Kamitani and Tong, 2005; Kuppens et al., 2013); but other animals, who have no power of self-observation and cannot consciously know their internal states; they still have emotions (Perlovsky, 2016a). For the invertebrate animals, an emotion is just an internal state, which includes the need, drive, or tendency to a kind of behavior (Mesquita and Frijda, 2011).
We can only study emotions in the invertebrate animals depending on the animal’s behavior and physiology-emotional expression (Cosmides and John, 1995). Darwin (1872) was first to study the animal behaviors for emotions, and he suggested that even insects use certain behaviors to express feelings homologous to human emotions. In order to survive, animals need to sense their environment, evaluate the surrounding stimuli with their internal needs, and initiate appropriate behavioral responses (Kaun and Rothenfluh, 2017). Evolutionarily, emotional behaviors should first have direct survival value that was honed by natural selection, and these behaviors are adaptive responses to the environment that increase the chances of survival (John and Leda, 1990; Cosmides and John, 1995). The basic emotions were selected through evolution in order to promote the survivability of the species in their specific primitive environment (LeDoux J.E., 2012). Therefore, the basic emotion related behaviors are the naturally born instinct behaviors, which are evolutionarily adaptive (Ramachandran and Jalal, 2017). In addition, these basic emotion related behaviors are manifested as stereotypical behavioral phenotypes.
Basic Emotions Are Related to Instincts
Basic emotions are thought to be universal as they are related to the most basic needs of the body, or the bodily instinctual needs (Schoeller et al., 2018). These few emotions constitute the most ancient and noticeable emotions, while other emotions (complex emotions), such as aesthetic emotions, are related to higher needs of our human experiences (Perlovsky, 2012). Grossberg and Levine, 1987 proposed a famous theory about instinct, which considers that the instinct resembles “neural sensors that measure vital parameters important for survival” (Grossberg et al., 1987; Perlovsky, 2016c), “for example, a low blood glucose level specifies an instinctual need for food” (Schoeller et al., 2018). Therefore measurement of glucose level is a kind of neural sensor. Grossberg and Levine theory proposed that emotions are the neural signals that connect instinctual sensor with the conscious brain, to make the instinctual needs to be consciously known to the brain, to make the instinct conceptually recognized-understood (Fontanari et al., 2012). William James also proposed that feelings are derived from sensing the body states (Damasio and Carvalho, 2013). Therefore, emotion is a kind of internal neural activity, whose major function is to sense the bodily needs, and then motivates behaviors depending on the external stimulus (Schoeller et al., 2018). Therefore basic emotions indicate satisfaction of instincts (Fontanari et al., 2012).
Remarkably, studies in Drosophila confirmed these hypothesis. Recent studies reveal that a small number of specialized central brain neurons in Drosophila brain directly sense specific circulating macronutrients (Pool et al., 2014), monitor systemic energy balance and alter feeding probability based on internal nutritional state (Pool and Scott, 2014). In addition to internal nutrient sensors, the flies can distinguish sugars based solely on caloric content in the absence of sweet taste detection (Dus et al., 2011). Furthermore, there is accumulating evidence that all animals have the central sensors to directly detect the levels of circulating carbohydrates and amino acids (Hao et al., 2005; Domingos et al., 2013). These studies thus identify central brain mechanisms that sense availability of specific nutrients, convert it to a change in neuromodulator output, and promote or inhibit feeding (Pool and Scott, 2014).
The basic emotions are evoked by sensing the basic bodily instinctual needs, while the “complex” emotions, including love, aesthetic emotions are evoked by higher cognitive needs of human being. In Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs, physiological needs and safety needs might be directly related to basic emotions, while the other needs, such as the need for love, esteem and self-actualization, are related to feelings such as aesthetic emotions (Zheng et al., 2016). Kant (1951) said aesthetic emotions are related to need of knowledge in human being. Perlovsky also proposed that human beings have the special need for knowledge, which was named “knowledge instinct.” Aesthetic emotions are related to the knowledge needs, are related to learning, or understanding, and they are shown in many fields, such as mathematics, music, or even language (Perlovsky, 2015; Schoeller and Perlovsky, 2016). Aesthetic emotions can also be seen in creativity, and they are also related the NE and DA neuromodulator (Gu et al., 2018). In all, the emotions are evoked by these sensing for bodily needs, and underlined by the neuromodulator release, and will promote some behaviors.
Basic Emotions Are Primitive
Basic emotions are basic due to the fact that they fit for primary life needs, and are developed early phylogenetically and ontogenetically (Ekman, 2003). The idea that invertebrates exhibit basic forms of emotions is increasingly accepted (Arnott and Elwood, 2009). Animals spend a significant amount of time seeking and selecting food for eat, while striving to avoid being eaten (John and Leda, 1990; Cosmides and John, 1995). These two forms of needs, which can be called hedonic and safety values, are two equally important, independent needs (Zheng et al., 2016). In Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs, physiological needs were proposed to be superior to safety needs, with the idea that safety needs emerge only once physiological needs are satisfied (Maslow, 1948). However, an animal will not eat in a dangerous situation since the primary objective in such situations is assurance of safety (Zheng et al., 2016). Wild animals navigating the rich environments will face complicated situations with many uncertainties; accordingly, the evolution of an adaptive mechanism to first perform a safety check of the surroundings is critically important for its survival (Gu et al., 2016; Zheng et al., 2016). Maslow (1948) also said that “practically everything looks less important than safety, even sometimes the physiological needs which being satisfied, are now underestimated. A man may be characterized as living almost for safety alone” (Maslow, 1948). Therefore, in our previous paper, we proposed that safety needs might be more basic (Zheng et al., 2016). Anyway, physiological needs and safety needs represent two equally important dimensions of human needs, which are independent from each other. Darwin said “the fear emotion does not depend on experiences; instead it depends absolutely on heritage.” Exposure of laboratory rodents to a predator, such as a cat, elicits defensive behaviors even if they have never been exposed to cats before; therefore, such behaviors appear to be innate and not experiential, as Darwin posited. The fear emotion might be the most crucial emotion for survival via eliciting defensive responses, such as fighting, and thus has been preserved throughout evolution (Olsson and Phelps, 2007).
Basic Emotions Manifest as Stereotypical Behavioral Phenotypes
Emotion is a kind of internal drive, which can exert a powerful effect on behavioral choice (Pereira and Murthy, 2017). Basic emotion related internal drive can induce behaviors that are supported by genetically hardwired neural circuits and are critical for animals’ survival. Several major categories of innate behavior, such as feeding, reproduction, aggression, and sleep, are observed across animal species. As hierarchical systems comprised of behavioral subprograms, innate behaviors are not only robust but can also fluctuate in intensity and adapt to an organism’s internal and external contexts (Kim et al., 2017). Fear is an important hereditary gift that aids survival by protecting against dangerous situations (Becker, 1997). All animals have specific behaviors to defend against predators, and Darwin found that “even insects express fear, anger, jealousy and love, by their stridulation” (Darwin, 1998). At stressful situation, emotion anger develops after fear emotion disappear, and manifests as fighting to cope with stressful situations, which can be easily observed in invertebrates. For example, the stings of bees, spiders, flies, ants, and scorpions are powerful tools to protect the organisms from predators. In addition, fighting also serves in the acquisition and defense of vital resources, such as food, shelter, or access to mates, such as what Sturtevant reported the fighting behavior in Drosophila (Chen et al., 2002). In stressful situations in which two males are courting the same female, he wrote “in such cases they (males) may be seen to spread their wings, run at each other, and apparently butt heads. One of them soon gives up and runs away. If the other then runs at him again within the next few minutes he usually makes off without showing fight” (Chen et al., 2002; Kim et al., 2018). Although it is difficult to determine whether fighting behaviors in insects are manifestations of an internal emotional state of anger like in humans, the function of fighting behaviors of invertebrates is very similar to the defensive behaviors of mammals; e.g., biting, clawing, hissing, and arching the back are fighting behaviors by which mammals threaten dangers away. Furthermore, like mammals, emotions in invertebrates are transient internal states, such that anger in insects manifests after fear to cope with the stressful situation (Zheng et al., 2016). It is necessary for survival even in Drosophila, and fight-or-flight behaviors can be easily seen in Drosophila, the flies are ready to fight with wings up for threat at a stressful situation, and after a series of stereotypical fighting behaviors one fly admits failure and fly away (Darwin, 1872).
Basic Emotions Gain New Meanings Throughout Evolution
Despite the universal nature of basic emotions, new behaviors of emotions have emerged via evolution (Ekman, 1992b). Some emotion related behaviors might be learned, e.g., the stress-related emotions of fear and anger have also got new meanings over time. While the original meaning of fear entails life-threatening situations with fight or flight behaviors in response to predators, new meanings of fear are generalized to any situations that occur unexpectedly. In addition the original meaning of happiness is associated with pleasurable feelings and generates approach and consummator behaviors, eventually leading to behavioral reinforcement (Schultz et al., 1997), the new meaning of happiness also gained new meanings through evolution: whether the organism is able to cope successfully (Yurkovic et al., 2006).
Consistently, the neuromodulator dopamine (DA), is known to mediate unconditioned pleasure and reward from food, sex, and drugs, but recent findings suggest that DA is also involved in the anticipatory, preparatory, approach, and coping phases of reward behavior (Schultz et al., 1997; Sandoval and Seeley, 2017). This is consistent with Lazarus’s reappraisal theory about happiness and sadness, which posits that happy and sad emotions are related to the success or failure of coping with stressful situations, respectively (Lazarus, 1999). The happiness in insects parallels that of mammals in several aspects. In Drosophila, happy emotion is expressed as locomotive activity (Mansourian et al., 2016; Wang and Sokolowski, 2017), which also happens after successful coping. Depending on studies in Drosophila, Kim et al. (2018) proposed that winning is perceived as rewarding, while losing is aversive. Presentation of food promotes a state of elevated locomotive activity, which can be controlled by DA. Furthermore, during courtship, males extend their wings horizontally and vibrate them to generate a “song” that attracts females, and the optogenetic activation of specific brain interneurons that control the courtship song can lead to persistent singing for several minutes (Dickson, 2008; von Philipsborn et al., 2011; Inagaki et al., 2014). In contrast, Drosophila raise their wings vertically into the “wing-threat” position during antagonistic interactions with conspecific males, and after a series of stereotypical defensive behaviors, one fly lowers its wings to admit failure and retreats (Chen et al., 2002).
Biological Aspects of Emotions
Given that basic emotions evolved to handle fundamental life tasks, it would follow that there must be biological patterns that drive each emotion. Ekman (1992b) proposed that basic emotions have many characteristics that distinguish one emotion from another, such as universal signals, distinctive physiology, and automatic appraisal influenced by both ontogenetic and phylogenetic past. Therefore, basic emotions not only provide information through behavioral expression to conspecifics, but specific biological changes prepare the organism to respond differently to various states of emotion. However, many studies using fMRI failed to get consistent results about specific neural basis for specific basic emotions (Lindquist et al., 2012), which lead to many psychologists try to give up the basic emotion theory (Lindquist et al., 2013; Barrett and Russell, 2015). Here we review basic emotions in Drosophila, whose brain structure are totally different from that of humans, but the neuromodulator are similar among all these animals. Numerous studies have pointed to an important role for neuromodulators (e.g., DA, 5-HT, and NE) in emotional process (Pereira and Murthy, 2017). Neuromodulators are believed to control the internal states related to emotions, mood, and affects, and exert critical influences on emotion related behaviors (Watanabe et al., 2017). Therefore, we propose that emotion is an internal state, whose neural substrate is the neuromodulator (Kim et al., 2018). For example, norepinephrine is a potent modulator of brain-wide states such as arousal (Suver et al., 2012; Pereira and Murthy, 2017).
Neural Basis for Basic Emotions
Insects lack homologs of vertebrate forebrain structures involved in emotional processing, such as the nucleus accumbens (NAc), prefrontal cortex (PFC), amygdala, and hippocampus; however, insects have evolved structures such as the mushroom body and central complex that show many functional and anatomical similarities to the mammalian structures that mediate basic emotions (Lin A.C. et al., 2014; Lin S. et al., 2014). The monoamine system in invertebrates (Klemm et al., 1985) and non-mammalian vertebrates (Perry and Capaldo, 2011) has been implicated in stress by numerous studies. Comparative anatomical studies of the neurons releasing OA, DA, and 5-HT have shown striking similarities among different insect species (Konings et al., 1988; Dacks et al., 2005) and point to a stereotypic pattern of neurons that are widely distributed in the central nervous system (CNS) of flies. In Drosophila brain, there are about 100 octopaminergic neurons (Sinakevitch et al., 2005), which can be divided into two kinds of neurons: interneurons and efferent neurons. The efferent neurons project diffusely to almost all the neuropil structure in the brain (Sinakevitch et al., 2005). Like the NE in vertebrates, the counterpart of OA, some octopaminergic neurons have been prove to be implicated in aggression (Ramirez and Pearson, 1991; Schroter et al., 2007; Andrews et al., 2014). Similarly, there are approximately 40 5-HT neurons in the Drosophila brain, whose innervations are spread around the feeding apparatus and also the ring gland (Sitaraman et al., 2008). On the contrary, the dopaminergic neurons project to the mushroom bodies to control behaviors (Figure 2; Zhang et al., 2007). Similar to 5-HT, DA neurons are interneurons, which connect different regions of neuropil. DA neurons show hydroxylase-immunoreactivity, and locate in the central body, anterior protocerebrum and other scattered region (Han et al., 1996). A cluster of approximately 130 dopaminergic neurons that innervate the horizontal lobes of the mushroom body was implicated sugar reward (Burke et al., 2012; Liu et al., 2012; Figure 2).
Norepinephrine – Stress
The norepinephrine system of mammals influences various aspects of the animal’s life, including Drosophila, and they can modulate a variety of physiological processes and behaviors in response to stress. Stressful stimuli induce a metabolic and behavioral adaptation, leading to enhanced energy supply, increased muscle performance, increased sensory perception and a matched behavior. This so-called “fight or flight” response can be seen in both vertebrates and invertebrates. Cannon said: “The highest level of modulatory monoamine input occurs during “fight or flight” behavioral situations.”
Fighting is a primitive behavior and is regarded as one of the fundamental instincts behind innate animal behaviors by Konrad Lorenz and Nikolaas Tinbergen, the founders of modern neuroethology (Asahina, 2017). Fighting in Drosophila offers a unique opportunity for studying basic emotions because of its stereotypical actions and its easily identified genetic resources. The fighting behavior in Drosophila is activated by pheromones and internal states, such as hunger. Drosophila recognizes the sex, species and even the food quality by chemosensation (Wang et al., 2011; Clowney et al., 2015). For example, 11-cis-vaccenyl acetate is a specific male odor chemical, and has been shown to be involved in male-male fighting (Wang and Anderson, 2010; Liu et al., 2011). The yeast smell which might signify food quality can induce female aggression (Ueda et al., 2002; Nelson and Trainor, 2007; Asahina et al., 2014). Even though these cues can induce fighting, the internal status of the animals can affect the fighting too, for example, prior defeat can reduce fighting behavior in many animal species (Penn et al., 2010; Hammels et al., 2015). In addition to past fighting experience, fighting behavior can also be influenced by the outcome of other behaviors, including recent mating success, hunger levels, and sleepiness. For example, recent mating experience can increase fighting tendency (Yuan et al., 2014), whereas sleep deprivation can decrease the frequency of fighting (Kayser et al., 2015). Lorenz already found the importance of internal states in fighting and suggested that fighting threshold can be lowered by an aggressive drive (anger) (Asahina, 2017).
It might be due the fact that increase octopamine can modulate the flight-or-fight response by affecting chemosensation responses (Stevenson et al., 2005; Ramdya et al., 2015). Amines like dopamine, tyramine, octopamine, and serotonin, were among the first molecules to be implicated in Drosophila aggression, in large part because previous studies in lobsters and crickets suggested strongly that monoamine neuromodulators affect aggression (Asahina, 2017). It has been well-known that the function of NE system is to induce fight-or-flight behaviors and serve to help the organism cope with dangerous environment (Zheng et al., 2016). Following exposure to a threat, NE is released from sympathetic nervous system to the blood, and directly affects the heart rate, and triggers the glucose release. NE is also released from locus coeruleus in the central nervous system. LC neurons project very profusely to most regions of the brain, and can influence the whole brain (Yurkovic et al., 2006). Octopamine (OA) in insects is a neuroactive substance that has a chemical structure that closely resembles NE and can function as a neurohormone for basic emotion (Arnott and Elwood, 2009; Lovheim, 2012; Asahina, 2017). The activity of octopaminergic neurons was initially discovered in octopus’s salivary glands, and has since been shown to mediate stress-response in many invertebrate (Sinakevitch et al., 2005; Stevenson et al., 2005; Burke et al., 2012). In addition, OA has been shown to be involved in fighting and also in elevated flight in crickets (Gammie et al., 2005; Stevenson et al., 2005; Asahina, 2017). In male Drosophila, octopamine is necessary to maintain levels of aggression (Zhou et al., 2008). A null mutation in the gene that encodes tyramine-β-hydroxylase (TβH), which catalyzes the OA synthesis, can significantly reduce fighting and aggression (Andrews et al., 2014; Asahina, 2017), which suggest its role in maintenance of fighting (Andrews et al., 2014). Even though activation of octopaminergic neurons or exogenous administration of octopamine, the invertebrate counterpart of noradrenaline can activate aggression (Schretter et al., 2018), whether an octopaminergic signal is sufficient to elevate levels of aggression remains unclear. For example, even though OA plays a role in fighting or aggression, artificially administering OA or an OA-agonist results in mixed effects on aggression (Hoyer et al., 2008; Kayser et al., 2015). In addition, overexpression of TβH does not increase fighting (Hoyer et al., 2008), instead it induced conflicting outcomes (Certel et al., 2010; Kayser et al., 2015). This might be due the fact that OA and NE systems are the substrates for both fear and anger, or fight and flight behaviors. OA-dependent modulation of organs and tissues is mainly elicited through muscle action, especially in terms of its impact on the “fight or flight” response. Flight is also an important and critical behavior in flying insects. Here the “flight” means flying away, instead of the normal “fly.” Neuromodulation of insect “flight” has thus far been attributed primarily to biogenic amines (Brembs et al., 2007). However, there are few studies in Drosophila about the “flight” behavior (Shen, 2012). However, studies about the innervation pattern in the periphery also support the idea that the OA/TA system is crucial for insects to switch from a dormant to an excited state, by a positive modulation of muscle activity, heart rate, and energy supply, and a simultaneous negative modulation of physiological processes like sleep (Agrawal and Hasan, 2015; Pathak et al., 2105; Pauls et al., 2018).
Dopamine – Reward
Octopamine was historically considered to be the signal for reward in insects, only recently has dopamine been linked to motivated behavior and rewarding reinforcement in fruit flies (Burke et al., 2012). The hedonic hypothesis of DA was first proposed by Wise and posits that DA in the brain plays a critical role in the subjective pleasure associated with positive reward and that a reduction in DA results in a loss of pleasure (Bozarth and Wise, 1980; Gu et al., 2016). Afterward, many studies have proved the role of DA in reward signaling (Dougherty et al., 1999; Matthews et al., 2016), notably many pharmacological and behavioral studies have confirmed the important role of medial prefrontal DA system in reward behaviors (Keleman et al., 2012). In addition, studies of the mechanisms of some drugs of abuse also support the role of DA in reward system by increasing presynaptic release of DA and inhibiting DA reuptake, such as cocaine and amphetamine (Narita et al., 2008; Del Campo et al., 2011). In contrast, decreased striatum DA responses were reported in detoxified cocaine abusers. Together, these studies suggest an involvement of DA neurotransmission in the reward process (Matsumoto et al., 2016), and this hypothesis has significantly impacted theories of drug addiction, and motivation since it was first introduced.
However, recent DA studies have opened this theory to reexamination (Berridge and Kringelbach, 2015). The incentive salience hypothesis has recently been accepted, which suggests that the major function of DA is anticipatory, preparatory, approach, instead of unconditioned pleasures from food, sex, or drugs (Schultz et al., 1997; Sandoval and Seeley, 2017). Therefore, Hailan Hu proposed that happiness = reward minus predicted reward (Dickson, 2008; Hu, 2016; Matsumoto et al., 2016), which means surprise can enhance happiness (Lazarus, 1999). Similar in Drosophila, OA (surprise) was thought to be involved in reward in insects (Burke et al., 2012), including Drosophila (Zhang et al., 2007; Liu et al., 2017). It is found that OA can trigger activation of dopaminergic neurons (Burke et al., 2012; Kahnt et al., 2012). Analysis of the β-adrenergic-like OCTβ2R receptor suggest that this OA-dependent reinforcement requires an interaction with dopaminergic neurons that control appetitive motivation (Burke et al., 2012; Kahnt et al., 2012). These evidence suggests clear roles for DA in reward-related processes in invertebrates (Ma et al., 2016), including motivation behavior and nutritional valuation of reward (Arnott and Elwood, 2009).
Historically, DA has been suggested to be most prominently associated with reward and punishment (Liu et al., 2012), recent findings from Drosophila confirmed all these functions, as well as additional roles in the interplay between external sensation and internal states (Burke et al., 2012; Kaun and Rothenfluh, 2017). It is believed that drug addiction is due to the mechanism of drugs of abuse “hijacking” the dopaminergic “reward” circuit and thus these artificial rewards reinforce associated behaviors. Recent complicating dopaminergic involvement in addiction has been proved to modulate internal state of the animals (Kim et al., 2017). Many drugs of abuse, such as alcohol stimulate locomotion, can induce Drosophila hyper-locomotion, and PPM3 DA neurons in mushroom body has been suggested to be involved in changing the activity and arousal states of the flies (Kim et al., 2017). Different kinds of drugs with different stimulation salience can induce different dopaminergic activity in distinct DA neurons in the mushroom body.
Serotonin – Punishment
Although DA release mediates reward, inhibition of DA should induce punishment. However, evolution created a separate process for punishment, and while it may seem redundant, 5-HT has been known to play a critical role in punishment. It is found that approximately 90% of 5-HT is secreted by gut chromaffin cells in response to noxious food, thus inducing vomiting or diarrhea (Beyeler, 2016; Zheng et al., 2016). Some plants exploit this function of 5-HT by expediting passage of seed through the digestive system. In addition, some animals such as scorpion and wasp stings also use 5-HT to induce pain (Gu et al., 2016). Aversion is separated from reward in the evolutionarily lower animals (Beyeler, 2016). The nematode, Caenorhabditis elegans, can also be infected by pathogenic bacteria, even though it feeds on bacteria. Indeed, exposure to pathogenic bacteria can enhance the 5-HT release, which can induce negative reinforcing reflex in C. elegans. However, the function of 5-HT gets more complicated during evolution, for example, it is found that some 5-HT neurons are involved in the positive rewarding process (Liu et al., 2012; Ries et al., 2017). It is found that some 5-HT neurons in dorsal raphe nucleus fire consistently during acquisition of a variety of rewards, including sex (Isosaka et al., 2015). Some studies reported a correlation between 5-HT level and positive emotions in rodents (Kvajo, 2016). Therefore, it might be too complicated to differentiate the functions of DA and 5-HT in such highly evolutionary creatures such as mammals and human beings. As such, Drosophila offered a very good model to test the functions of DA or 5-HT in positive or aversive reinforcement (Zhang et al., 2016).
The emotions are internal states evoked by sensing for bodily instinctual needs, and underlined by neuromodulator release, and will promote some behaviors. In mammals, these neuromodulators are primarily monoamines, which are highly interconnected with a network of modulators and transmitters important for complex behaviors. Neuropeptides and monoamines are very often expressed in combination with each other and with neurotransmitters. Possible interactions between neuromodulators and neurotransmitters are interesting and important issues. A dozen neuromodulator systems in Drosophila have been implicated to date in basic emotion, including serotonin, octopamine, acetylcholine, glutamate, and GABA, and many neuropeptides, and many of these neuromodulator systems appear to be functionally conserved throughout evolution, including orthodox for mammalian peptidergic signals tachykinin, cholecystokinin, neuropeptide Y, Neuromedin U and insulin (Pool and Scott, 2014). However, work in Drosophila suggests that the rewarding drives are majorly gated through DA neurons, or DA plays a central role in creating the motivational drive underlying many behaviors (Kim et al., 2017). In male flies, dopaminergic neurons of the ventral nerve cord promote persistent copulation, and a subset of dopaminergic neurons innervating the mushroom body is required for persistent courtship. Similarly, even neuropeptide are shown to affect Drosophila aggression, such as neuropeptide F, a functional homolog of vertebrate neuropeptide Y. However, NPF-expressing neurons seem to play a more general role in modulating male behavioral patterns when potential competitors are present (Asahina, 2017). Unlike octopamine, which is clearly an important neuromodulator for aggression, NPF has also been regarded as neuromodulator of feeding behavior across animal species, and activation of NPF signaling mimics the hunger states. In addition, there is no specific population of NPF expression neurons, unlike the neurons for three monoamines (Asahina, 2017; Ryglewski et al., 2017).
Therefore, we propose that the monoamine neuromodulators underlie the three core affects of basic emotions; specifically, NE is related to the fight-or-flight responses at stressful events, DA is involved in reward, and 5-HT is related to punishment (Figure 3). Consistently, a paper in PNAS proposed that the use of Drosophila as a model for circuit dissection of internal states can promote behavioral changes associated with winning or losing after coping: Winning is perceived as rewarding, while losing is aversive (Kim et al., 2018).
Conclusion
In this review, we propose a framework for the evolutionary study of emotions based on behavioral observations of Drosophila. From analysis of molecules and neural systems to observational study of behaviors and social functions, the Drosophila model is a powerful tool to understand the evolutionary origin and neurobiological underpinnings of emotions (Anderson, 2016; Kim et al., 2018). The brain structure of Drosophila are totally different from that of humans, but they have similar neuromodulators and innate states (Kim et al., 2018). Numerous studies have pointed to an important role for neuromodulators (e.g., DA, 5-HT, and NE) in the emotional process (Pereira and Murthy, 2017). Neuromodulators are believed to control the internal states related to emotions, mood, and affects, and exert critical influences on emotion related behaviors (Watanabe et al., 2017).
Emotion Is an Innate State, Whose Neural Substrate Is the Neuromodulator Release
We demonstrated that basic emotions are primitive, internal states that have gained new meanings and new external behavioral expression via evolution in order to meet organisms’ biological, social, and functional needs (Ekman, 1992b; Anderson and Adolphs, 2014). Reward, punishment, and stress are the three most primitive features of the four basic emotions (happiness, sadness, fear, anger) and are driven by the three monoamine neuromodulators (DA-reward, 5-HT-punishment, NE-stress). These three monoamines are not only the substrates for the four basic emotions, but we posit that these monoamines combine in varying degrees to ultimately create various higher order emotions, much like the way different colors can be created from the three primary colors; we call this the “Three Primary Color Model of Basic Emotions.”
This paper establishes a new theory of emotion. A scientific theory in psychology, similar to those in physics, is its elegance and beauty in describing a vast area of knowledge from few basic principles, or use few fundamental principles to describe a vast area of knowledge (Perlovsky, 2016b). Traditional psychology is a “soft” science that does not develop models of the mind based on few principles, describing vast areas of knowledge, and making experimentally verifiable predictions (Perlovsky, 2016b). Here we introduced the very simple model for basic emotions, a very simple theory about emotions. It might be an oversimplification to categorize monoamine simply as an aggression-promoting neuromodulator, but we hope our hypothesis can help understand the basic emotion theory. For validation, detailed studies of the specific behavioral expressions of states of relative excess or deficit of the neurotransmitters 5-HT, NE, and DA may offer confirmatory observations supporting this model of emotions.
Author Contributions
FW and SG designed the manuscript. FW, JB, and JH wrote the manuscript. NP revised the manuscript.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Funding. This work was supported by a grant from Foundation of Humanities and Social Science in Ministry of Education in China (FW, 19YJAZH083).
References
- Agrawal T., Hasan G. (2015). Maturation of a central brain flight circuit in Drosophila requires Fz2/Ca(2)(+) signaling. eLife 2015:4. 10.7554/eLife.07046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aldwin C. M. (1994). Stress, Coping, and Development. New York, NY: Guildford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson D., Adolphs R. (2014). A framework for studying emotions across phylogeny. Cell 157 187–200. 10.1016/j.cell.2014.03.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson D. J. (2016). Circuit modules linking internal states and social behaviour in flies and mice. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 17 692–704. 10.1038/nrn.2016.125 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrews J. C., Fernandez M. P., Yu Q., Leary G. P., Leung A. K., Kavanaugh M. P., et al. (2014). Octopamine neuromodulation regulates Gr32a-linked aggression and courtship pathways in Drosophila males. PLoS Genet. 10:e1004356. 10.1371/journal.pgen.1004356 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnott G., Elwood R. (2009). Probing aggressive motivation in a cichilid fish. Biol. Lett. 5 762–764. 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0526 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asahina K. (2017). Neuromodulation and strategic action choice in drosophila aggression. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 40 51–75. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-072116-031240 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asahina K., Watanabe K., Duistermars B. J., Hoopfer E., Gonzalez C. R., Eyjolfsdottir E. A., et al. (2014). Tachykinin-expressing neurons control male-specific aggressive arousal in Drosophila. Cell 156 221–235. 10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker C. (2004). Behavioral Genetics: An Introduction to how Genes and Environments Interact Through Development to Shape Differences in Mood, Personality, and Intelligence. Washington DC: American Association for the Advancement of Science. [Google Scholar]
- Barrett L., Mesquita B., Ochsner K., Gross J. (2007). The experience of emotion. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58 373–403. 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085709 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett L., Russell J. (2015). They Psychological Construction of Emotion. New York: NY: Guildofrd Press. [Google Scholar]
- Becker G. (1997). The Gift of Fear: Survival Signals that Protect us from Violence. Boston, MA: Dell Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Berridge K. C., Kringelbach M. L. (2015). Pleasure systems in the brain. Neuron 86 646–664. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bestelmeyer P. E. G., Kotz S. A., Belin P. (2017). Effects of emotional valence and arousal on the voice perception network. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12 1351–1358. 10.1093/scan/nsx059 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beyeler A. (2016). Parsing reward from aversion. Science 354:558. 10.1126/science.aak9762 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bozarth M. A., Wise R. A. (1980). Electrolytic microinfusion transducer system: an alternative method of intracranial drug application. J. Neurosci. Methods 2 273–275. 10.1016/0165-0270(80)90016-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brembs B., Christiansen F., Pfluger H. J., Duch C. (2007). Flight initiation and maintenance deficits in flies with genetically altered biogenic amine levels. J. Neurosci. 27 11122–11131. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2704-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burke C. J., Huetteroth W., Owald D., Perisse E., Krashes M. J., Das G., et al. (2012). Layered reward signalling through octopamine and dopamine in Drosophila. Nature 492 433–437. 10.1038/nature11614 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Celeghin A., Diano M., Bagnis A., Viola M., Tamietto M. (2017). Basic emotions in human neuroscience: neuroimaging and beyond. Front. Psychol. 8:1432. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01432 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Certel S. J., Leung A., Lin C. Y., Perez P., Chiang A. S., Kravitz E. A. (2010). Octopamine neuromodulatory effects on a social behavior decision-making network in Drosophila males. PLoS One 5:e13248. 10.1371/journal.pone.0013248 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen S., Lee A., Bowens N., HUber R., AKravitz E. (2002). Fighting fruit flies: a model system for the study of aggression. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99 5664–5668. 10.1073/pnas.082102599 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clowney E. J., Iguchi S., Bussell J. J., Scheer E., Ruta V. (2015). Multimodal chemosensory circuits controlling male courtship in drosophila. Neuron 87 1036–1049. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.07.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cosmides L., John T. (1995). From Evolution to Adaptations to Behaviors: Toward an Integrated Evolutionary Psychology. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. [Google Scholar]
- Dacks A. M., Christensen T. A., Agricola H. J., Wollweber L., Hildebrand J. G. (2005). Octopamine-immunoreactive neurons in the brain and subesophageal ganglion of the hawkmoth Manduca sexta. J. Comp. Neurol. 488 255–268. 10.1002/cne.20556 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Damasio A., Carvalho G. B. (2013). The nature of feelings: evolutionary and neurobiological origins. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14 143–152. 10.1038/nrn3403 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Damasio A. R. (1997). Neuropsychology, towards a neuropathology of emotions and mood. Nature 386 769–770. 10.1038/386769a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Darwin C. (1872). The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals. Oxford: Oxford Press; 10.1037/10001-000 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Darwin C. (1876). The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex. New York, NY: D. Appleton and Company. [Google Scholar]
- Darwin C. (1998). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, 3rd Edn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Del Campo N., Chamberlain S. R., Sahakian B. J., Robbins T. W. (2011). The roles of dopamine and noradrenaline in the pathophysiology and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 69 e145–e157. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickson B. (2008). Wired for sex: the neurogiology of drosophila mating decisions. Science 322 904–909. 10.1126/science.1159276 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Domingos A. I., Sordillo A., Dietrich M. O., Liu Z. W., Tellez L. A., Vaynshteyn J., et al. (2013). Hypothalamic melanin concentrating hormone neurons communicate the nutrient value of sugar. eLife 2:e01462. 10.7554/eLife.01462 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dougherty D. D., Bonab A. A., Spencer T. J., Rauch S. L., Madras B. K., Fischman A. J. (1999). Dopamine transporter density in patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet 354 2132–2133. 10.1016/S0140-6736(99)04030-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dus M., Min S., Keene A. C., Lee G. Y., Suh G. S. (2011). Taste-independent detection of the caloric content of sugar in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108 11644–11649. 10.1073/pnas.1017096108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ekman P. (1957). A methodological discussion of nonverbal behavior. J. Psychol. 43 141–149. 10.1080/00223980.1957.9713059 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ekman P. (1984). “Expression and the nature of emotion,” in Approaches to Emotion, eds Scherer K., Ekman P. (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; ). [Google Scholar]
- Ekman P. (1992a). An arugment for basic emotions. Cogn. Emot. 6 169–200. 10.1080/02699939208411068 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ekman P. (1992b). Are there basic emotions? Psychol. Rev. 99 550–553. 10.1037/0033-295X.99.3.550 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ekman P. (2003). Emotions inside out. 130 years after Darwin’s “The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animal”. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1000 1–6. 10.1196/annals.1280.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ekman P., Cordaro D. (2011). What is meant by calling emotions basic. Emot. Rev. 3 364–370. 10.1177/1754073911410740 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ekman P., Friesen W. V. (1969). A tool for the analysis of motion picture film or video tape. Am. Psychol. 24 240–243. 10.1037/h0028327 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fontanari J. F., Bonniot-Cabanac M. C., Cabanac M., Perlovsky L. I. (2012). A structural model of emotions of cognitive dissonances. Neural Netw. 32 57–64. 10.1016/j.neunet.2012.04.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frijda N. H. (1986). The Emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Gammie S. C., Hasen N. S., Stevenson S. A., Bale T. L., D’Anna K. L. (2005). Elevated stress sensitivity in corticotropin-releasing factor receptor 2 deficient mice decreases maternal, but not intermale aggression. Behav. Brain Res. 160 169–177. 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.11.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grossberg S., Levine D., Schmajuk N. (1987). Predictive regulation of associative learning in a neural network by reinforcement and attentive feedback. Int. J. Neurol. 21-22 83–104. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grossberg S., Levine D. S. (1987). Neural dynamics of attentionally modulated Pavlovian conditioning: blocking, interstimulus interval, and secondary reinforcement. Appl. Opt. 26 5015–5030. 10.1364/AO.26.005015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gu S., Gao M., Yan Y., Wang F., Tang Y. Y., Huang J. H. (2018). The neural mechanism underlying cognitive and emotional processes in creativity. Front. Psychol. 9:1924. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01924 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gu S., Wang F., Yuan T., Guo B., Huang H. (2015). Differentiation of primary emotions through neuromodulators: review of literature. Int. J. Neurol. Res. 1 43–50. 10.17554/j.issn.2313-5611.2015.01.19 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gu S., Wang W., Wang F., Huang J. H. (2016). Neuromodulator and emotion biomarker for stress induced mental disorders. Neural Plast. 2016:2609128. 10.1155/2016/2609128 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammels C., Pishva E., De Vry J., van den Hove D. L., Prickaerts J., van Winkel R., et al. (2015). Defeat stress in rodents: from behavior to molecules. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 59 111–140. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.10.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han K. A., Millar N. S., Grotewiel M. S., Davis R. L. (1996). DAMB, a novel dopamine receptor expressed specifically in Drosophila mushroom bodies. Neuron 16 1127–1135. 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80139-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hao S., Sharp J. W., Ross-Inta C. M., McDaniel B. J., Anthony T. G., Wek R. C., et al. (2005). Uncharged tRNA and sensing of amino acid deficiency in mammalian piriform cortex. Science 307 1776–1778. 10.1126/science.1104882 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoopfer E. D. (2016). Neural control of aggression in Drosophila. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 38 109–118. 10.1016/j.conb.2016.04.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoyer S. C., Eckart A., Herrel A., Zars T., Fischer S. A., Hardie S. L., et al. (2008). Octopamine in male aggression of Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 18 159–167. 10.1016/j.cub.2007.12.052 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu H. (2016). Reward and aversion. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 39 297–324. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-070815-014106 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hutto D. D., Robertson I., Kirchhoff M. D. (2018). A new, better BET: rescuing and revising basic emotion theory. Front. Psychol. 9:1217. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01217 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Inagaki H., Jung Y., Hoopfer E., Wong A., Mishra N., Lin J., et al. (2014). Optogenetic control of freely behaving adult Drosophila using a red-shifted channelrhodopsin. Nat. Methods 11 325–332. 10.1038/nmeth.2765 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isosaka T., Matsuo T., Yamaguchi T., Funabiki K., Nakanishi S., Kobayakawa R., et al. (2015). Htr2a-expressing cells in the central amygdala control the hierarchy between innate and learned fear. Cell 163 1153–1164. 10.1016/j.cell.2015.10.047 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izard C. (2010). The many meanings/aspects of emotion:definitions, functions, activation, and regulation. Emot. Rev. 2 363–370. 10.1177/1754073910374661 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Izard C. (2011). Forms and functions of emotions: matters of emotion-cognition interaction. Emot. Rev. 3 371–378. 10.1177/1754073911410737 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Izard C. E. (1977). Human Emotions. New York, NY: Plenum Press; 10.1007/978-1-4899-2209-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Izard C. E. (2007). Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new paradigm. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2 260–280. 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00044.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jack R., Garrod O., Schyns P. (2014). Dynamic facial expressions of emotion transmit an evolving hierarchy of signals over time. Curr. Biol. 24 187–192. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.11.064 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- John T., Leda C. (1990). The past explains the present: emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethol. Sociobiol. 11 375–424. 10.1016/0162-3095(90)90017-Z [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kant I. (1951). Kant’s Critique of Judgement, trans. Bernard J. H. New York, NY: Hafner Publishing; (Original publication in 1892). [Google Scholar]
- Kahnt T., Park S. Q., Burke C. J., Tobler P. N. (2012). How glitter relates to gold: similarity-dependent reward prediction errors in the human striatum. J. Neurosci. 32 16521–16529. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2383-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamitani Y., Tong F. (2005). Decoding the visual and subjective contents of the human brain. Nat. Neurosci. 8 679–685. 10.1038/nn1444 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaun K. R., Rothenfluh A. (2017). Dopaminergic rules of engagement for memory in Drosophila. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 43 56–62. 10.1016/j.conb.2016.12.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kayser M. S., Mainwaring B., Yue Z., Sehgal A. (2015). Sleep deprivation suppresses aggression in Drosophila. eLife 4:e07643. 10.7554/eLife.07643 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keleman K., Vrontou E., Kruttner S., Yu J. Y., Kurtovic-Kozaric A., Dickson B. J. (2012). Dopamine neurons modulate pheromone responses in Drosophila courtship learning. Nature 489 145–149. 10.1038/nature11345 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim S. M., Su C. Y., Wang J. W. (2017). Neuromodulation of innate behaviors in drosophila. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 40 327–348. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-072116-031558 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim Y. K., Saver M., Simon J., Kent C. F., Shao L., Eddison M., et al. (2018). Repetitive aggressive encounters generate a long-lasting internal state in Drosophila melanogaster males. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115 1099–1104. 10.1073/pnas.1716612115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klemm N., Nassel D. R., Osborne N. N. (1985). Dopamine-beta-hydroxylase-like immunoreactive neurons in two insect species, Calliphora erythrocephala and Periplaneta americana. Histochemistry 83 159–164. 10.1007/BF00495147 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konings P. N., Vullings H. G., Geffard M., Buijs R. M., Diederen J. H., Jansen W. F. (1988). Immunocytochemical demonstration of octopamine-immunoreactive cells in the nervous system of Locusta migratoria and Schistocerca gregaria. Cell Tissue Res. 251 371–379. 10.1007/BF00215846 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kravitz E., Fernandez M. (2015). Aggression in Drosophila. Behav. Neurosci. 129 549–563. 10.1037/bne0000089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuppens P., Tuerlinckx F., Russell J. A., Barrett L. F. (2013). The relation between valence and arousal in subjective experience. Psychol. Bull. 139917–940. 10.1037/a0030811 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kvajo M. (2016). What we talk about when we talk about emotions. Cell 167 1443–1445. 10.1016/j.cell.2016.11.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lazarus R. S. (1999). Stress and Emotion: A New Synthesis. New York, NY: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- LeDoux J. (1998). Fear and the brain: where have we been, and where are we going? Biol. Psychiatry 44 1229–1238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeDoux J. E. (2012). Evolution of human emotion: a view through fear. Prog. Brain Res. 195 431–442. 10.1016/B978-0-444-53860-4.00021-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeDoux J. (2012). Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron 73 653–676. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.02.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeDoux J. E. (1995). Emotion: clues from the brain. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 46 209–235. 10.1146/annurev.ps.46.020195.001233 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeDoux J. E., Brown R. (2017). A higher-order theory of emotional consciousness. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114 E2016–E2025. 10.1073/pnas.1619316114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levenson R. W. (2011). Basic emotion questions. Emot. Rev. 3 379–386. 10.1177/1754073911410743 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lin A. C., Bygrave A. M., de Calignon A., Lee T., Miesenbock G. (2014). Sparse, decorrelated odor coding in the mushroom body enhances learned odor discrimination. Nat. Neurosci. 17 559–568. 10.1038/nn.3660 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin S., Owald D., Chandra V., Talbot C., Huetteroth W., Waddell S. (2014). Neural correlates of water reward in thirsty Drosophila. Nat. Neurosci. 17 1536–1542. 10.1038/nn.3827 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lindquist K., Wager T., Kober H., Bliss-Moreau E., Barrett L. (2012). The brain basis of emotion: a meta-analytic review. Behav. Brain Sci. 35 121–143. 10.1017/S0140525X11000446 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lindquist K. A., Siegel E. H., Quigley K. S., Barrett L. F. (2013). The hundred-year emotion war: are emotions natural kinds or psychological constructions? Comment on lench, flores, and bench (2011). Psychol. Bull. 139 255–263. 10.1037/a0029038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu C., Placais P. Y., Yamagata N., Pfeiffer B. D., Aso Y., Friedrich A. B., et al. (2012). A subset of dopamine neurons signals reward for odour memory in Drosophila. Nature 488 512–516. 10.1038/nature11304 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu Q., Tabuchi M., Liu S., Kodama L., Horiuchi W., Daniels J., et al. (2017). Branch-specific plasticity of a bifunctional dopamine circuit encodes protein hunger. Science 356 534–539. 10.1126/science.aal3245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu W., Liang X., Gong J., Yang Z., Zhang Y. H., Zhang J. X., et al. (2011). Social regulation of aggression by pheromonal activation of Or65a olfactory neurons in Drosophila. Nat. Neurosci. 14 896–902. 10.1038/nn.2836 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lovheim H. (2012). A new three-dimensional model for emotions and monoamine neurotransmitters. Med. Hypotheses 78 341–348. 10.1016/j.mehy.2011.11.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma Z., Stork T., Bergles D. E., Freeman M. R. (2016). Neuromodulators signal through astrocytes to alter neural circuit activity and behaviour. Nature 539 428–432. 10.1038/nature20145 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mansourian S., Corcoran J., Enjin A., Lofstedt C., Dacke M., Stensmyr M. (2016). Fecal-derived phenol induces egg-laying aversion in drosophila. Curr. Biol. 26 2762–2769. 10.1016/j.cub.2016.07.065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maslow A. H. (1948). Higher and lower needs. J. Psychol. 25 433–436. 10.1080/00223980.1948.9917386 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsumoto H., Tian J., Uchida N., Watabe-Uchida M. (2016). Midbrain dopamine neurons signal aversion in a reward-context-dependent manner. eLife 5:e17328. 10.7554/eLife.17328 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews G. A., Nieh E. H., Vander Weele C. M., Halbert S. A., Pradhan R. V., Yosafat A. S., et al. (2016). Dorsal raphe dopamine neurons represent the experience of social isolation. Cell 164 617–631. 10.1016/j.cell.2015.12.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mesquita B., Frijda N. H. (2011). An emotion perspective on emotion regulation. Cogn. Emot. 25 782–784. 10.1080/02699931.2011.586824 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moons W. G., Eisenberger N. I., Taylor S. E. (2010). Anger and fear responses to stress have different biological profiles. Brain Behav. Immun. 24 215–219. 10.1016/j.bbi.2009.08.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Narita M., Suzuki M., Kuzumaki N., Miyatake M., Suzuki T. (2008). Implication of activated astrocytes in the development of drug dependence: differences between methamphetamine and morphine. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1141 96–104. 10.1196/annals.1441.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson R. J., Trainor B. C. (2007). Neural mechanisms of aggression. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8 536–546. 10.1038/nrn2174 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olsson A., Phelps E. (2007). Social learning of fear. Nat. Neurosci. 10 1095–1102. 10.1038/nn1968 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ortony A., Turner T. J. (1990). What’s basic about basic emotions? Psychol. Rev. 97 315–331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panksepp J. (2007). Neurologizing the psychology of affects: how appraisal-based constructivism and basic emotion theory can coexist. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2 281–296. 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00045.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panksepp J., Watt D. (2011). What is basic about basic emotion? lasting lessons from affective neuroscience. Emot. Rev. 3 387–396. 10.1177/1754073911410741 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pathak T., Agrawal T., Richhariya S., Sadaf S., Hasan G. (2105). Store-operated calcium entry through orai is required for transcriptional maturation of the flight circuit in Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 35 13784–13799. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1680-15.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pauls D., Blechschmidt C., Frantzmann F., El Jundi B., Selcho M. (2018). A comprehensive anatomical map of the peripheral octopaminergic/tyraminergic system of Drosophila melanogaster. Sci. Rep. 8:15314. 10.1038/s41598-018-33686-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Penn J. K., Zito M. F., Kravitz E. A. (2010). A single social defeat reduces aggression in a highly aggressive strain of Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107 12682–12686. 10.1073/pnas.1007016107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pereira T. D., Murthy M. (2017). To fight or not to fight. Neuron 95 986–988. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.08.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perlovsky L. (2012). Emotions of “higher” cognition. Behav. Brain Sci. 35 157–158. 10.1017/S0140525X11001555 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perlovsky L. (2015). Aesthetic emotions goals: comment on “The quartet theory of human emotions: an integrative and neurofunctional model” by S. Koelsch et al. Phys. Life Rev. 13 80–82. 10.1016/j.plrev.2015.04.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perlovsky L. (2016a). Human consciousness is fundamental for perception and highest emotions. Behav. Brain Sci. 39:e191. 10.1017/S0140525X15002216 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perlovsky L. I. (2016b). Physics of the mind. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 10:84 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00084 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perlovsky L. (2016c). Scientific intuitions about the mind are wrong, misled by consciousness. Behav. Brain Sci. 39:e128. 10.1017/S0140525X15001624 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perry S. F., Capaldo A. (2011). The autonomic nervous system and chromaffin tissue: neuroendocrine regulation of catecholamine secretion in non-mammalian vertebrates. Auton. Neurosci. 165 54–66. 10.1016/j.autneu.2010.04.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plutchik R. (1962). The Emotions:Facts, Theories, and a New Model. New York, NY: Random House. [Google Scholar]
- Pool A. H., Kvello P., Mann K., Cheung S. K., Gordon M. D., Wang L., et al. (2014). Four GABAergic interneurons impose feeding restraint in Drosophila. Neuron 83 164–177. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pool A. H., Scott K. (2014). Feeding regulation in Drosophila. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 29 57–63. 10.1016/j.conb.2014.05.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Posner J., Russell J. A., Peterson B. S. (2005). The circumplex model of affect: an integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 17 715–734. 10.1017/S0954579405050340 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramachandran V. S., Jalal B. (2017). The evolutionary psychology of envy and jealousy. Front. Psychol. 8:1619. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01619 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramdya P., Lichocki P., Cruchet S., Frisch L., Tse W., Floreano D., et al. (2015). Mechanosensory interactions drive collective behaviour in Drosophila. Nature 519 233–236. 10.1038/nature14024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramirez J. M., Pearson K. G. (1991). Octopaminergic modulation of interneurons in the flight system of the locust. J. Neurophysiol. 66 1522–1537. 10.1152/jn.1991.66.5.1522 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ries A. S., Hermanns T., Poeck B., Strauss R. (2017). Serotonin modulates a depression-like state in Drosophila responsive to lithium treatment. Nat. Commun. 8:15738. 10.1038/ncomms15738 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roseman I. (1984). “Cognitive Determinants of Emotion: A Structural Theory,” in Review of Personality and Social Psychology, ed. Shaver P. (Beverly Hills, CA: Sage; ). [Google Scholar]
- Russell J. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychol. Rev. 110 145–172. 10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.145 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russell J., Barrett L. (1999). Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotions:dissecting the elephant. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 76 805–819. 10.1037/0022-3514.76.5.805 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russell J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 39 1161–1178. 10.1037/h0077714 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Russell J. A. (2006). Emotions are not modules. Can. J. Philos. 32 53–71. 10.1353/cjp.2007.0037 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ryglewski S., Duch C., Altenhein B. (2017). Tyramine actions on drosophila flight behavior are affected by a glial dehydrogenase/reductase. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 11:68. 10.3389/fnsys.2017.00068 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saarimaki H., Gotsopoulos A., Jaaskelainen I. P., Lampinen J., Vuilleumier P., Hari R., et al. (2016). Discrete neural signatures of basic emotions. Cereb. Cortex 26 2563–2573. 10.1093/cercor/bhv086 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandoval D., Seeley R. (2017). Physiology:gut feeling for food choice. Nature 542 302–303. 10.1038/nature21499 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scarantino A., Griffiths P. (2011). Dont give up on basic emotions. Emot. Rev. 3 444–454. 10.1177/1754073911410745 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Schoeller F., Perlovsky L. (2016). Aesthetic chills: knowledge-acquisition, meaning-making, and aesthetic emotions. Front. Psychol. 7:1093. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01093 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoeller F., Perlovsky L., Arseniev D. (2018). Physics of mind: experimental confirmations of theoretical predictions. Phys. Life Rev. 25 45–68. 10.1016/j.plrev.2017.11.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scholsberg H. (1954). Three dimensions of emotions. Psychol. Rev. 61 81–88. 10.1037/h0054570 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schretter C. E., Vielmetter J., Bartos I., Marka Z., Marka S., Argade S., et al. (2018). A gut microbial factor modulates locomotor behaviour in Drosophila. Nature 563 402–406. 10.1038/s41586-018-0634-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schroter U., Wilson S. L., Srinivasan M. V., Ibbotson M. R. (2007). The morphology, physiology and function of suboesophageal neck motor neurons in the honeybee. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 193 289–304. 10.1007/s00359-006-0182-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W., Dayan P., Montague P. R. (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275 1593–1599. 10.1126/science.275.5306.1593 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shen P. (2012). Analysis of Drosophila larval feeding response to quinine-adulterated food. Cold Spring Harb. Protoc. 2012:pdb.prot069336. 10.1101/pdb.prot069336 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shpigler H. Y., Saul M. C., Corona F., Block L., Cash Ahmed A., Zhao S. D., et al. (2017). Deep evolutionary conservation of autism-related genes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 14 9653–9658. 10.1073/pnas.1708127114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinakevitch I., Niwa M., Strausfeld N. J. (2005). Octopamine-like immunoreactivity in the honey bee and cockroach: comparable organization in the brain and subesophageal ganglion. J. Comp. Neurol. 488 233–254. 10.1002/cne.20572 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sitaraman D., Zars M., Lafrriere H., CHen Y., Sable-smith A., Kitamoto T., et al. (2008). Serotonin is necessary for place memory in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105 5579–5584. 10.1073/pnas.0710168105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Song Y., Hakoda Y. (2018). Selective impairment of basic emotion recognition in people with autism: discrimination thresholds for recognition of facial expressions of varying intensities. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 48 1886–1894. 10.1007/s10803-017-3428-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevenson P. A., Dyakonova V., Rillich J., Schildberger K. (2005). Octopamine and experience-dependent modulation of aggression in crickets. J. Neurosci. 25 1431–1441. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4258-04.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suver M. P., Mamiya A., Dickinson M. H. (2012). Octopamine neurons mediate flight-induced modulation of visual processing in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 22 2294–2302. 10.1016/j.cub.2012.10.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomkins S. S. (1962). Affect, Imagery, Consciousness: The Positive Affects, Vol. 1 New York, NY: Springer [Google Scholar]
- Tracy J., Randles D. (2011). Four models of basic emotions: a review of Elkman and cordaro, Izard, levenson, and panksepp and watt. Emot. Rev. 3 397–405. 10.1177/1754073911410747 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ueda H. M., Kato M., Saifuddin M., Tabe H., Yamaguchi K., Tanne K. (2002). Differences in the fatigue of masticatory and neck muscles between male and female. J. Oral Rehabil. 29 575–582. 10.1046/j.1365-2842.2002.00869.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vetter N. C., Drauschke M., Thieme J., Altgassen M. (2018). Adolescent basic facial emotion recognition is not influenced by puberty or own-age bias. Front. Psychol. 9:956. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00956 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Philipsborn A., Liu T., Yu J., Masser C., BIdaye S., Dickson B. (2011). Neuronal control of Drosophila courtship song. Neuron 69 509–522. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang F., Pan F., Shapiro L. A., Huang J. H. (2018). Stress induced neuroplasticity and mental disorders 2018. Neural Plast. 2018:5382537. 10.1155/2018/5382537 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang F., Pereira A. (2016). Neuromodulation, emotional feelings and affective disorders. Mens Sana Monogr. 14 5–29. 10.4103/0973-1229.154533 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang K., Guo Y., Wang F., Wang Z. (2011). Drosophila TRPA channel painless inhibits male-male courtship behavior through modulating olfactory sensation. PLoS One 6:e25890. 10.1371/journal.pone.0025890 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang L., Anderson D. J. (2010). Identification of an aggression-promoting pheromone and its receptor neurons in Drosophila. Nature 463 227–231. 10.1038/nature08678 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang S., Sokolowski M. (2017). Aggressive behaviors, food deprivation and the foraging gene. R. Soc. Open Sci. 4:170042. 10.1098/rsos.170042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watanabe K., Chiu H., Pfeiffer B. D., Wong A. M., Hoopfer E. D., Rubin G. M., et al. (2017). A circuit node that integrates convergent input from neuromodulatory and social behavior-promoting neurons to control aggression in Drosophila. Neuron 95 1112–1128.e7. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.08.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White K., Humphrey D., Hirth F. (2010). The dopaminergic system in the aging brain of Drosophila. Front. Neurosci. 4:205 10.3389/fnins.2010.00205 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams R. (2017). Anger as a basic emotion and its role in personality building and pathological growth: the neuroscientific, developmental and clinical perspectives. Front. Psychol. 8:1950. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01950 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson-Mendenhall C. D., Barrett L. F., Barsalou L. W. (2013). Neural evidence that human emotions share core affective properties. Psychol. Sci. 24 947–956. 10.1177/0956797612464242 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wundt W. M. (1897). Outlines of Psychology. New York, NY: Thoemmes Continuum press; 10.1037/12908-000 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Yuan Q., Song Y., Yang C. H., Jan L. Y., Jan Y. N. (2014). Female contact modulates male aggression via a sexually dimorphic GABAergic circuit in Drosophila. Nat. Neurosci. 17 81–88. 10.1038/nn.3581 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yurkovic A., Wang O., Basu A., Kravitz E. A. (2006). learning and memory associated with aggression in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103 17519–17524. 10.1073/pnas.0608211103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang J., Tan L., Ren Y., Liang J., Lin R., Feng Q., et al. (2016). Presynaptic excitation via GABAB receptors in habenula cholinergic neurons regulates fear memory expression. Cell 166 716–728. 10.1016/j.cell.2016.06.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang K., Guo J. Z., Peng Y., Xi W., Guo A. (2007). Dopamine-mushroom body circuit regulates saliency-based decision-making in Drosophila. Science 316 1901–1904. 10.1126/science.1137357 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zheng Z., Gu S., Lei Y., Lu S., Wang W., Li Y., et al. (2016). Safety needs mediate stressful events induced mental disorders. Neural Plast. 2016:8058093. 10.1155/2016/8058093 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou C., Rao Y., Rao Y. (2008). A subset of octopaminergic neurons are important for Drosophila aggression. Nat. Neurosci. 11 1059–1067. 10.1038/nn.2164 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]