Abstract
Background
Childhood apraxia of speech (CAS) affects a child's ability to produce sounds and syllables precisely and consistently, and to produce words and sentences with accuracy and correct speech rhythm. It is a rare condition, affecting only 0.1% of the general population. Consensus has been reached that three core features have diagnostic validity: (1) inconsistent error production on both consonants and vowels across repeated productions of syllables or words; (2) lengthened and impaired coarticulatory transitions between sounds and syllables; and (3) inappropriate prosody (ASHA 2007). A deficit in motor programming or planning is thought to underlie the condition. This means that children know what they would like to say but there is a breakdown in the ability to programme or plan the fine and rapid movements required to accurately produce speech. Children with CAS may also have impairments in one or more of the following areas: non‐speech oral motor function, dysarthria, language, phonological production impairment, phonemic awareness or metalinguistic skills and literacy, or combinations of these. High‐quality evidence from randomised controlled trials (RCTs) is lacking on interventions for CAS.
Objectives
To assess the efficacy of interventions targeting speech and language in children and adolescents with CAS as delivered by speech and language pathologists/therapists.
Search methods
We searched CENTRAL, MEDLINE, Embase, eight other databases and seven trial registers up to April 2017. We searched the reference lists of included reports and requested information on unpublished trials from authors of published studies and other experts as well as information groups in the areas of speech and language therapy/pathology and linguistics.
Selection criteria
RCTs and quasi‐RCTs of children aged 3 to 16 years with CAS diagnosed by a speech and language pathologist/therapist, grouped by treatment types.
Data collection and analysis
Two review authors (FL, AM) independently assessed titles and abstracts identified from the searches and obtained full‐text reports of all potentially relevant articles and assessed these for eligibility. The same two authors extracted data and conducted the 'Risk of bias' and GRADE assessments. One review author (EM) tabulated findings from excluded observational studies (Table 1).
Main results
This review includes only one RCT, funded by the Australian Research Council; the University of Sydney International Development Fund; Douglas and Lola Douglas Scholarship on Child and Adolescent Health; Nadia Verrall Memorial Scholarship; and a James Kentley Memorial Fellowship. This study recruited 26 children aged 4 to 12 years, with mild to moderate CAS of unknown cause, and compared two interventions: the Nuffield Dyspraxia Programme‐3 (NDP‐3); and the Rapid Syllable Transitions Treatment (ReST). Children were allocated randomly to one of the two treatments. Treatments were delivered intensively in one‐hour sessions, four days a week for three weeks, in a university clinic in Australia. Speech pathology students delivered the treatments in the English language. Outcomes were assessed before therapy, immediately after therapy, at one month and four months post‐therapy. Our review looked at one‐month post‐therapy outcomes only. A number of cases in each cohort had recommenced usual treatment by their speech and language pathologist between one month and four months post‐treatment (NDP‐3: 9/13 participants; ReST: 9/13 participants). Hence, maintenance of treatment effects to four months post‐treatment could not be analysed without significant potential bias, and thus this time point was not included for further analysis in this review.
We judged all core outcome domains to be low risk of bias. We downgraded the quality of the evidence by one level to moderate due to imprecision, given that only one RCT was identified.
Both the NDP‐3 and ReST therapies demonstrated improvement at one month post‐treatment. For three outcomes the effect was marginally greater for NDP‐3 than ReST: accuracy of production on treated words (NDP‐3 mean difference (MD) = 36.0, ReST MD = 33.9; absolute MD = 2.1 between groups); speech production consistency, measured by 25 real words repeated three times using the inconsistency subtest of the Diagnostic Evaluation of Articulation and Phonology (DEAP) test (NDP‐3 MD = 11.1, ReST MD = 10.9; absolute MD = 0.2 between groups); and accuracy of connected speech, assessed by imitated word accuracy in connected speech of at least three word combinations (NDP‐3 MD = 14.3, ReST MD = 11.5; absolute MD = 2.8 between groups). ReST (MD = 18.3) demonstrated a marginally greater effect than NDP‐3 (MD = 18.2) for accuracy of production on non‐treated words at one month post‐treatment (absolute MD = 0.1 between groups). The study did not assess the outcome of functional communication.
Authors' conclusions
There is limited evidence that, when delivered intensively, both NDP‐3 and ReST may effect improvement in word accuracy in 4‐ to 12‐year‐old children with CAS, measured by the accuracy of production on treated and non‐treated words, speech production consistency and the accuracy of connected speech. The study did not measure functional communication. No formal analyses were conducted to compare NDP‐3 and ReST by the original study authors, hence one treatment cannot be reliably advocated over the other. We are also unable to say whether either treatment is better than no treatment or treatment as usual. No evidence currently exists to support the effectiveness of other treatments for children aged 4 to 12 years with idiopathic CAS without other comorbid neurodevelopmental disorders. Further RCTs replicating this study would strengthen the evidence base. Similarly, further RCTs are needed of other interventions, in other age ranges and populations with CAS and with co‐occurring disorders.
Plain language summary
One well‐controlled study shows some evidence of effect of two interventions for childhood apraxia of speech (CAS)
Review question What treatments help to improve the speech and language of children and adolescents with childhood apraxia of speech (CAS).
Background Children with CAS find it difficult to produce sounds and syllables consistently and precisely in order to speak words and sentences with clarity and correct speech rhythm. As a result, children with CAS can be hard to understand with potential for negative impacts on school achievement and peer friendships. CAS affects around 0.1% of the general population. This review collates the research evidence to identify the most effective therapies for children with CAS.
Search date The evidence is current to 6 April 2017.
Study characteristics We found one study with 26 children aged 4 to 12 years with CAS. The children had mild to severe CAS without a known cause. Children were allocated randomly (using a method like coin tossing) to one of two treatments: the Nuffield Dyspraxia Programme ‐ Third Edition (NDP‐3); and the Rapid Syllable Transition treatment (ReST). Both therapies were delivered intensively in one‐hour sessions, four days a week for three weeks. The treatments were delivered by speech pathology students in a university clinic. Outcomes were assessed before therapy, immediately after therapy, at one month and four months post‐therapy. Our review looked at one‐month post‐therapy outcomes only.
Study funding sources The included study was funded by the Australian Research Council; the University of Sydney International Development Fund; Douglas & Lola Douglas Scholarship on Child and Adolescent Health; Nadia Verrall Memorial Scholarship; and a James Kentley Memorial Fellowship.
Key results Further studies replicating these findings would strengthen available evidence.
The study provides limited evidence that the NDP‐3 may improve the accuracy of production on treated items and the accuracy of connected speech. There is limited evidence that the NDP‐3 has a negligible effect on speech production consistency, and the ReST a negligible effect on accuracy of production on non‐treated words. The study did not measure functional communication.
Quality of the evidence The included study was a randomised controlled trial with an overall low risk of bias. We downgraded the quality of the evidence by one level to moderate, due to imprecision, given that only one RCT was identified.
Recommendations There is limited evidence that the NDP‐3 or ReST may be helpful for children with CAS of unknown origin, aged 4 to 12 years, without other co‐occurring conditions. We were not able to find out whether one of these treatment was better than the other, or whether either was better than no treatment or treatment as usual. There is currently no available evidence for other treatments.
Further RCTs — including studies comparing treatments to a no‐treatment (wait‐list) control group — would strengthen the evidence base. Further research is also needed for children with CAS and other disorders or diagnoses.
Summary of findings
for the main comparison.
Nuffield Dyspraxia Programme ‐ Third Edition (NDP‐3) versus Rapid Syllable Transition Treatment (ReST) for Childhood Apraxia of Speech | |||||
Patient or population: children aged 4 to 12 years with CAS of unknown cause Settings: University of Sydney Communication Disorders Treatment and Research Clinic Intervention: NDP‐3 Comparison: ReST | |||||
Outcomes | Summary of MD findings | Absolute MD | Number of participants (studies) | Quality of the evidence (GRADE) | Comments |
Primary outcomes | |||||
Accuracy of production on treated items Measured by: counting the number of real words produced correctly (/x) Follow‐up: pre‐intervention to 1 month post‐intervention |
NDP‐3 MD of 36.0 was greater than the ReST MD of 33.9 | 2.1 | 26 (1 trial) | ⊕⊕⊕⊝ Moderatea | — |
Accuracy of production on non‐treated items Measured by: counting the number of real words produced correctly (/x) Follow‐up: pre‐intervention to 1 month post‐intervention |
ReST MD of 18.3 was minimally greater than the NDP‐3 MD of 18.2 | 0.1 | 26 (1 trial) | ⊕⊕⊕⊝ Moderate a | — |
Secondary outcomes | |||||
Speech production consistency Measured by: calculating the number of inconsistent productions of 25 words produced 3 times using the DEAP inconsistency subtestb Follow‐up: pre‐intervention to 1 month post‐intervention |
NDP‐3 MD of 11.1 was greater than the ReST MD of 10.9 | 0.2 | 26 (1 trial) | ⊕⊕⊕⊝ Moderate a | — |
Accuracy of connected speech Measured by: counting the number of correct imitations of 3 word phrases (/x) Follow‐up: pre‐intervention to 1 month post‐intervention |
NDP‐3 MD of 14.3 was greater than the ReST MD of 11.5 | 2.8 | 26 (1 trial) | ⊕⊕⊕⊝ Moderate a | — |
GRADE Working Group grades of evidence High quality: we are very confident that the true effect lies close to that of the estimate of the effect. Moderate quality: we are moderately confident in the effect estimate; the true effect is likely to be close to the estimate of the effect, but there is a possibility that it is substantially different. Low quality: our confidence in the effect estimate is limited; the true effect may be substantially different from the estimate of the effect. Very low quality: we have very little confidence in the effect estimate; the true effect is likely to be substantially different from the estimate of effect. | |||||
CAS: childhood apraxia of speech; DEAP: Diagnostic Evaluation of Articulation and Phonology; MD: mean difference; NDP‐3: Nuffield Dyspraxia Programme ‐ Third Edition;ReST: Rapid Syllable Transition Treatment (ReST) for Childhood Apraxia of Speech |
aWe downgraded the quality of evidence by one level, to moderate, for imprecision, as there was only one study for comparison. bNote, a decrease in inconsistency is a positive outcome.
Background
Description of the condition
Childhood apraxia of speech (CAS) affects a child's ability to produce speech sounds and syllables in the right order, and to speak words and sentences with accuracy and correct speech rhythm. Over sixty years ago, Morley 1954 provided a seminal paper documenting a series of speech characteristics in children that resembled the speech production disorder of adults with acquired apraxia of speech, and the diagnosis of CAS was born. CAS is a rare condition, affecting only around 0.1% of the general population (Morley 1972; Yoss 1975). CAS is more prevalent within particular medical subgroups, however, and particularly penetrant in certain genetic syndromes (e.g. Fedorenko 2016; Mei 2017).
Historically, synonyms such as verbal dyspraxia and developmental apraxia of speech have been used. The most commonly used terms today are CAS and developmental verbal dyspraxia (DVD), with the latter used largely in the UK context (RCSLT 2011). We use the term CAS consistently throughout this review.
A deficit in motor programming or planning is thought to underlie CAS; that is, children know what they would like to say but there is a breakdown in the ability to programme or plan the required movements to accurately produce speech. The current approach to diagnosis of CAS is expert‐based perception of speech symptoms (Maas 2012a). There is consensus amongst speech and language pathologists (SLPs), also known as speech and language therapists (SLTs), that three core features of CAS have diagnostic validity: (1) inconsistent error production on both consonants and vowels across repeated productions of syllables or words; (2) lengthened and impaired coarticulatory transitions between sounds and syllables; and (3) inappropriate prosody (ASHA 2007).
In addition to the core features of CAS, children may also have co‐occurring impairments affecting non‐speech oral motor function, language, phonemic awareness/meta‐linguistics and literacy (ASHA 2007). Younger children typically present with more severe forms of the disorder, with improvement noted over time for both idiopathic CAS (Davis 2005; Jacks 2006) and individuals with CAS associated with genetic syndromes (Morgan 2017; Morgan 2018). It is not currently known how age, severity or underlying aetiology impact upon CAS treatment response or outcome.
There are no epidemiological data on the prevalence of CAS, although it occurs infrequently in comparison with other forms of developmental speech disorder such as articulation disorder and phonological disorder, which occur in around 3.5% of preschool children (Eadie 2015). A population‐based estimate suggests that CAS occurs in one child per 1000 (0.1%) (Morley 1972; Yoss 1975), and is found in 3.4% to 4.3% of the children referred to clinics for speech disorder management (Delaney 2004). The diagnosis of CAS can apply to children who have a specific impairment in speech with other neurodevelopmental functions relatively more preserved (e.g. borderline or typical non‐verbal cognition). Historically most cases were referred to as 'idiopathic', given limited aetiological knowledge of the condition (Morgan 2008). In recent times, however, novel insights have been gained into the genetic and neurobiological bases of CAS (Eising 2018). Variations in an increasing number of single genes have been associated with CAS (Eising 2018; Turner 2015), with the most replicated finding being disruption of the Forkhead box protein P2 or FOXP2 (Lai 2001; Morgan 2017; Vargha‐Khadem 2005). Beyond single gene causes, CAS has also been associated with copy number variant syndromes, such as 16p11.2 deletion syndrome (Fedorenko 2016; Mei 2017), Koolen de Vries Syndrome (Morgan 2018), 6q25.3 deletion syndrome (Peter 2017), 7q11.23 duplication syndrome (Velleman 2011), and other genetic conditions such as Floating Harbour syndrome (White 2010). Further to genetic causes, other medical conditions associated with CAS include metabolic disorders (e.g. galactosaemia; Shriberg 2011) or epilepsy disorders (e.g. Liégeois 2012). In relation to neurobiology or brain function, there is inconsistency as regards the key brain regions and networks disrupted in CAS, with neuroimaging studies reporting both cortical and subcortical anomalies (Liégeois 2012; Liégeois 2014; Liégeois 2016).
Description of the intervention
A range of CAS treatment approaches with differing theoretical standpoints have been reported. These studies are almost exclusively in the form of uncontrolled case studies or case series. Therapeutic approaches for CAS can be grouped into the following three areas.
Motor‐based approaches. These therapies are based on principles of motor learning (see Maas 2008 for a review); for example, traditional articulation‐based drill therapy (Velleman 1994), the Nuffield Dyspraxia Programme (Williams 2004), the Rapid Syllable Transitions Treatment (Ballard 2010), rate control therapy (Rosenthal 1994), the PROMPT System (Prompts for Restructuring Oral MuscularPhonetic Targets) (Chumpelik 1984; Dale 2013), melodic intonation therapy (Helfrich‐Miller 1994), adapted cueing technique (Klick 1985), and integral stimulation or dynamic temporal and tactile cueing (Maas 2012a; Strand 2006). Motor‐based therapy can also include non‐speech oro‐motor techniques; for example, oral form recognition training (Kingston 1987) and orofacial myofunctional therapy (Ray 2003). Motor‐based therapy can also be instrumentally based, such as delayed auditory feedback (Lozano 1978), electropalatography (Carter 2004; Lundeborg 2007), and ultrasound (Preston 2013).
Linguistic approaches. Linguistic therapies address language impairments that can co‐occur in children with CAS. Examples of linguistic approaches include programmes to address phonological speech production or awareness (McNeill 2009).
Multi‐modal communication approaches. These approaches seek to support verbal communication. Methods can address specific communication messages or features, such as Aided AAC (augmentative and alternative communication) Modelling (Binger 2007), or use of technological devices (Bornman 2001; Cumley 1999).
How the intervention might work
Below, we describe the ways in which the aforementioned approaches (described under Description of the intervention) might work.
Motor‐based approaches. These methods use principles of motor learning, such as emphasizing a high number of successful repetitions of a task, using stimuli with high complexity, and a period of teaching followed by practice where cues and feedback are faded. Such approaches are reported to facilitate maintenance and generalisation in children with CAS (Maas 2008; Maas 2014).
Linguistic approaches. These methods are focused on the semantics, phonology or grammar of language, and not on motor speech production per se. For example, a linguistic approach may include phonological contrast therapy, where children are taught how to abstract speech sound rules for the specific language(s) they speak (Dodd 2008). Another example of a linguistic approach is core vocabulary therapy, which focuses on shaping children's word approximations whilst expanding their expressive and receptive vocabulary (Crosbie 2005).
Multi‐modal communication approaches. These methods are used for children who are minimally verbal to help them communicate and reduce the frustration associated with their speech disability. Devices may include a computer, phone or tablet with applications to help children produce words, phrases and sentences. Other methods involve gesture, sign language or use of visual picture boards.
Why it is important to do this review
There is a need for clinicians and parents to be aware of the most efficacious treatments for children with CAS. To date, studies in the field are largely non‐RCT (randomised controlled trials), single case series or case‐control studies that are generally positive in stating improvements in speech post‐therapy across motor (e.g. Baas 2008; Ballard 2010; Edeal 2011; Hall 1989; Kadis 2014; McCabe 2014; Strand 2000; Strand 2006), linguistic (e.g. McNeill 2009a; McNeill 2009b; McNeill 2010; Stokes 2010; Zaretsky 2010), and multi‐modal communication approaches (e.g. Harris 1996; King 2013; Tierney 2016). Yet these non‐RCT studies are inherently biased in nature and there is a need in the field for a systematic evaluation of available evidence. This review identifies best available treatments for CAS. This is an update of a Cochrane Review first published in 2008 (Morgan 2008). The previous review revealed no available RCTs for review. The first RCT in this field was published in 2015, hence it was timely to provide an updated review.
Objectives
To assess the efficacy of interventions targeting speech and language in children and adolescents with CAS as delivered by speech and language pathologists/therapists.
Methods
Criteria for considering studies for this review
Types of studies
RCTs and quasi‐RCTs (e.g. studies in which participants are allocated to intervention groups on alternate days).
Types of participants
Children aged 3 to 16 years with a diagnosis of CAS made by a speech and language pathologist/therapist.
Types of interventions
See Description of the intervention section above.
Eligible control groups were no treatment control (e.g. wait‐list control), treatment as usual, or other treatment controls.
Types of outcome measures
Primary outcomes
Accuracy of production on treated or non‐treated* items (may be associated with motor‐based, linguistic or multi‐modal communication approaches noted under How the intervention might work)
A desirable outcome would have been an improvement in accuracy of speech or multi‐modal communication, while an undesirable outcome would have been deterioration from baseline.
*Non‐treated items are stimuli (e.g. syllables, words, phrases) that have not been practised by children during intervention sessions. They are a form of control whereby we are able to measure children's performance on 'treated' items (e.g. syllables, words, phrases the child has practised during speech sessions) and compare it with performance on 'non‐treated' items. In this way, we can quantify whether the child has 'generalised' their newly acquired speech skills, or improvement in speech, to non‐treated stimuli, or whether they have only improved on speech items practised during therapy.
Secondary outcomes
Speech production consistency across repeated words and syllables (may be associated with motor‐based, linguistic or multi‐modal communication approaches noted under How the intervention might work)
Accuracy of connected speech, including co‐articulation accuracy (e.g. syllable segregation, voice onset time; most commonly associated with motor‐based or linguistic approaches noted under How the intervention might work)
Functional communication (e.g. child‐ or parent‐based questionnaire; may be associated with motor‐based, linguistic or multi‐modal communication approaches noted under How the intervention might work)
A desirable outcome would have been an improvement on outcomes one to three, whilst an undesirable outcome would have been deterioration from baseline on outcomes one to three.
Outcome measurements were recorded before, immediately after and at longer‐term follow‐up.
Search methods for identification of studies
Electronic searches
Margaret Anderson, Cochrane Information Specialist for the Developmental, Psychosocial and Learning Problems Group, conducted the searches for this update in August 2011, June 2014 and April 2017. We searched the following list of sources which includes bibliographic databases, and international and national trials registers. We did not apply any date restrictions, but we only examined articles written in the English language. We report the search strategies for this update in Appendix 1. Earlier search strategies are in Appendix 2.
Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL; 2017, Issue 3) in the Cochrane Library, and which includes the Cochrane Developmental, Psychosocial and Learning Problems Specialized Register (searched 6 April 2017)
Ovid MEDLINE (1946 to March week 5 2017)
Ovid MEDLINE E‐Pub Ahead of Print (searched 6 April 2017)
Ovid MEDLINE In Process & Other Non‐indexed Citations (searched 6 April 2017)
Embase Ovid (1980 to 2017 week 15)
CINAHL EBSCOhost (Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature; 1937 to 10 April 2017)
PsycINFO Ovid (1806 to April week 1 2017)
PsycINFO EBSCOhost (1887 to 4 August 2011)
ERIC EBSCOhost (Education Resources Information Center; 1966 to 10 April 2017)
ERIC Proquest (Education Resources Information Center; 1966 to 6 June 2014)
Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (CDSR; 2017, Issue 4) part of the Cochrane Library
Database of Abstracts of Reviews of Effect (DARE; 2015, Issue 2) part of the Cochrane Library (not searched in previous version of review (Morgan 2008). Final issue published in 2015)
SpeechBITE (speechbite.com; searched 10 April 2017)
Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry (ANZCTR; www.anzctr.org.au/BasicSearch.aspx; searched 12 April 2017)
Chinese Clinical Trial Registry (ChiCTR; www.chictr.org.cn; searched 10 April 2017)
ClinicalTrials.gov (clinicaltrials.gov; searched 10 April 2017)
EU Clinical Trials Register (clinicaltrialsregister.eu; searched 10 April 2017)
ISRCTN Registry (www.isrctn.com; searched 10 April 2017)
Nederlands Trial Register (trialregister.nl/trialreg/admin; searched 10 April 2017)
World Health Organization International Clinical Trials Registry Platform (WHO ICTRP; www.who.int/ictrp/en; searched 10 April 2017)
Searching other resources
We searched the reference lists of included reports, and requested information on unpublished trials from authors of published studies and other experts, as well as information groups in the areas of speech and language therapy/pathology and linguistics.
Data collection and analysis
We were unable to use many of our preplanned methods (Morgan 2006), as only one study met the inclusion criteria (Criteria for considering studies for this review). This study was published in a peer‐reviewed journal and there are no other completed RCTs or quasi‐RCTs at this time, published or unpublished. See Appendix 3 and Morgan 2006.
Selection of studies
Two review authors (FL and AM) independently screened all titles and abstracts yielded by the search for eligibility. In cases of uncertainty over whether an abstract met the inclusion criteria, we obtained the full‐text report. Next, the same two reviewers independently evaluated each full‐text report against the inclusion criteria (Criteria for considering studies for this review). In the event of disagreement over inclusion of a particular paper, FL and AM reached consensus by re‐assessing the study against the inclusion criteria together. We present the results of our selection process in a PRISMA diagram; see Figure 1 (Moher 2009).
Data extraction and management
In addition to outcome data, we documented the following information using a data management form: participant details; setting (e.g. community clinic, school); type of intervention; length and frequency of intervention; professions involved; duration of impairment; level of severity; co‐morbidity; and assessment tools employed. We requested any information that was missing or unclear from the corresponding author (Dealing with missing data). AM independently extracted and entered the outcome data into Review Manager 5 (Review Manager 2014), and FL then independently evaluated the data and entries. AM and FL discussed any disagreements until they reached a consensus. EM entered further details of excluded studies into Table 2.
1. Excluded, low‐quality evidence from observational studies (case‐series, case control).
Study | Participants | Methodology/paper type | Intervention | Intervention approach | Intervention intensity and duration | Outcome measures | Treatment outcomes | Timing of outcome measures | Methodological considerations |
Baas 2008 | 1 male aged 12.8 years with CAS and charge syndrome | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case (AB) design) | Dynamic Temporal and Tactile Cueing | Motor | Phase I and II: sessions 4 × per week; Phase III: weekly therapy. Study over 25 months. Home practice not reported | Articulation accuracy on 2‐item scale for treated items; speech rate | Phase I (core vocabulary): change on 4/6 targets. Maintained at last probe. Phase II (core vocabulary): reached 100% accuracy for 3/5 words. Reduction of stereotypies. Phase III: decreased speech rate from 94 to 71 SPM |
Baseline and during treatment. No longer‐term follow‐up data | Lack of experimental control, multiple baselines, control, longer‐term follow‐up or generalisation data. Clinical file data used. No replication across participants. Assessors, participants, therapists not blinded |
Ballard 2010 | 3 siblings (2 males, 1 female) aged 7.8 and 10.10 years with CAS | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single subject multiple baseline design across behaviours and participants) | Rapid Syllable Transition Treatment (ReST) | Motor | 60‐minute sessions (100‐120 trials per session), 4 × per week for 12 sessions. Home practice not reported | Reading aloud 10 treated and 10 non‐treated non‐word strings; real word generalisation data; perceptual analysis of prosodic pattern and acoustic analysis using pairwise variability index | 3/3 had significant gains in treated items and generalisation to same level of treated complexity. 2/3 generalised to lower and higher complexity non‐word items. Minimal generalisation to real words | Baseline data taken at beginning of every 4th session and at 4 weeks post‐treatment | No long‐term follow‐up data. Limited participants for generalisation of outcomes. No blinding of assessors, participants or therapists. No stimulus generalisation measures |
Beathard 2008 | 1 female aged 3 years with CAS | Not quasi‐/RCT (Case description) | Music therapy | Other (alternative interventions) | 30‐minute sessions over 9 months. 24 sessions in total | Descriptive data only | Commenced non‐verbal. At end, had 11 phonemes in inventory | Pre‐treatment and post‐treatment. No follow‐up data | Lack of experimental control, multiple baselines or control data. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No replicable outcome measures. No statistical analysis. No blinding of assessors, participants or therapists. No follow‐up or generalisation data. Unclear which aspect of treatment provided outcomes or affect of maturation, schooling, etc. No replication across participants. No long‐term follow‐up data |
Binger 2007 | 2 males aged 4.2 and 4.4 years with CAS and language disorder | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case multiple baseline across participants) | Aided AAC Modeling | Augmentative and alternative communication | 15‐minute sessions, 1 to 3 × per week for 10 to 15 sessions | Frequency of use of multi‐symbol messages in play scenarios | Significantly more frequent use of multi‐symbol messages using aided AAC as well as different types of messages. Maintained and generalised gains. Increased participation | Baseline × 3, every 2nd treatment session, and at 2, 4 and 8 weeks post‐treatment | CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. Limited outcome measures. No blinding of assessors. No response generalisation data taken (only stimulus generalisation) |
Binger 2008 | 1 female (Latino) aged 3.4 years with CAS and suspected velocardiofacial syndrome | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case multiple baseline across participants) | Aided AAC Modeling | Augmentative and alternative communication | 10‐minute sessions, 1 to 3 × per week for 10 to 15 sessions | Frequency of use of multi‐symbol messages in play scenarios | Significantly more frequent use of multi‐symbol messages using aided AAC. Parental response to training excellent. Maintained and generalised gains | Baseline × 3, every 2nd treatment session, and at 2, 4 and 8 weeks post‐treatment | CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No blinding of assessors. No response generalisation data taken (only stimulus generalisation) |
Binger 2011 | 1 female aged 6 years with CAS and language disorder | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case multiple baseline across behaviours) | Aided AAC Modeling | Augmentative and alternative communication | 15‐minute sessions, 1 to 3 × per week for 10 to 15 sessions | Frequency of use of grammatical morphemes | Significantly more frequent use of grammatical morphemes using aided AAC. 2nd intervention period needed for 2/3 targets. Maintained gains | Baseline × 3, every treatment session, and 2, 4 and 8 weeks post‐treatment | CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No blinding of assessors. No response generalisation data taken (only stimulus generalisation) |
Bornman 2001 | 1 male aged 6.6 years with CAS, hemiplegia and seizures | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case (ABA) design) | Voice output devices (Macaw) | Augmentative and alternative communication | 60‐minute sessions for 2 sessions (training). Home practice focus | Frequency of appropriate responses to questions in structured discourse | Mother provided greater frequency and type of questions. Frequency of appropriate responses increased | 2 × baseline, 2 × practice period, 1 × post‐treatment, and 4 weeks post‐treatment | Lack of experimental control, multiple baselines or control data. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No statistical analysis. Limited outcome measures. No blinding of assessors. Unclear dosage of home practice. No generalisation data. No long‐term follow‐up data |
Carter 2004 | 1 male and 1 female aged 12 and 8 years respectively diagnosed with CAS. Additional 8 children (7 males) aged 4 to 7 years with persistent articulation errors | Not quasi‐/RCT (Case series ‐ single group study) | Electropalatography (EPG) on /t, d, k, g, s, z/ | Motor | 30‐minute sessions, 1 × per week for 10 weeks | Per cent consonants correct (PCC) and Probe Scoring System (PSS) on probe of 43 words | Significant difference noted for PSS for whole group. PCC scores improved in percentage | Pre‐treatment (baseline first session) and post‐treatment | Lack of experimental control, multiple baselines or control data. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No follow‐up or generalisation data. No blinding of assessors |
Culp 1989 | 1 female aged 8 years with CAS and intellectual disability | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case (ABA) design) | Partners in Augmentative Communication Training (PACT) | Augmentative and alternative communication | 30 to 90‐minute sessions daily after 3 days of intensive training. Home practice focus | Ratio of parent vs participant messages; ratio of successful/intelligible messages from child | Participant had greater frequency of messages compared to parent, and slightly higher frequency of successful measures (high baseline accuracy). Increased participation | Pre‐treatment and 2 months post‐treatment | Lack of experimental control, multiple baselines or control data. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No statistical analysis. Limited outcome measures. No blinding of assessors. No immediate post‐treatment data or generalisation data. No replication across participants |
Cumley 1999 | 2 females and 1 male aged 3.4, 8 and 12.9 years respectively, with CAS (2 with intellectual disability and 1 with submucous cleft) | Not quasi‐/RCT (3 case studies/reports) | Combined communication boards and voice output devices | Augmentative and alternative communication | 3.4‐year‐old: 2 to 3 × per week for 12 weeks 8‐year‐old: daily for 6 months 12‐year‐old: not reported |
3.4‐year‐old: MLU. 8‐year‐old: assessment of phonological processes; communication repairs. 12‐year‐old: description of functional communication |
3.4‐year old: minimal speech improvement, MLU increased to WNL 8‐year old: no change in speech, parent report of greater communication repairs, and less frustration 12‐year old: supplemented natural speech to initiate, maintain and repair communication |
Pre‐assessment and treatment descriptions | Lack of experimental control, multiple baselines or control data. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No statistical analysis. Limited outcome measures. No blinding of assessors. No immediate post‐treatment data or generalisation data. No replication across participants |
Dale 2013 | 3 males and 1 female aged 3.6 to 6 years diagnosed with CAS | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single subject (ABB or ABC) design) | Prompts for Restructuring Oral Muscular Phonetic Targets (PROMPT) ‐ full programme (FP) for 8 weeks versus PROMPT without tactile‐kinaesthetic‐proprioceptive cueing for 4 weeks and FP for 4 weeks | Motor | 50‐minute session, 2 × per week for 8 weeks | Trained words on probe, untrained words. Pre‐post testing on the DEAP, TOCS+, VMPAC focal motor and sequencing subtests and Vineland socialization scales | 2/4 improved on DEAP. 4/4 improved on TOCS+, VMPAC subtests and Vineland. All 4 showed greater improvement on easier targets and majority maintained to 3 months post‐treatment. Generalisation to untrained items noted | Probe words: baseline × 3, treatment × 4, post‐treatment, and 3 months post‐treatment | Lack of experimental control as control data changed and interpreted as generalisation but no other control used (e.g. multiple baselines). CAS diagnosis concerning prosody unclear. Blinded assessors for only some outcomes. No withdrawal period between treatment phases and participant differences made comparison between conditions difficult. All measures not taken at consistent times |
Edeal 2011 | 2 males aged 6.2 and 3.4 years with CAS (1 case with repaired cleft lip and palate and language disorder) | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case (AB) design) | Integral Stimulation (Dynamic Temporal and Tactile Cueing) | Motor | Varied across participants. 40‐minute sessions (15 minutes each condition plus probes). 1 case: 3 × per week for 11 weeks. 1 case: 2 × per week for 5 weeks | Probe data on targeted phonemes (articulation) in words for each participant. 1 phoneme targeted with high production frequency = 100 trials and another with moderate production frequency = 60 trials. Articulation and language sample taken at 2 weeks post‐treatment | Large effect sizes for high production frequency and moderate for moderate production frequency. Improvement in PCC and phoneme inventory post‐treatment. Some generalisation | Baseline × 3, each treatment session, and 1 probe post‐treatment | Lack of experimental control, multiple baselines or control data. No long‐term follow‐up data. No blinding of assessors. Accuracy based on if target phoneme was correct (including cognate pair substitution) not if whole word was correct |
Hall 1989 | 1 female aged 9 years with mild CAS (followed until 12 years) | Not quasi‐/RCT (Case study/report) | Articulation therapy, motor‐programming remedial model | Motor | 5 school semesters | Templin‐Darley Tests of Articulation | Remediation of all 31 items for /r/, /ɝ/ and /ɚ/ | Test completed each semester | Lack of experimental control, multiple baselines or control data. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No statistical analysis. Limited outcome measures. No blinding of assessors. No follow‐up data or generalisation data. No replication across participants. No stimulus generalisation measures |
Harris 1996 | 1 male aged 5 years with CAS and language disorder | Not quasi‐/RCT (Multiple baseline across discourse contexts) | Computer‐based AAC | Augmentative and alternative communication | 4‐minute sessions, 2 × per week for 22 sessions over 4 months | Frequency of noun/verb phrases in reciprocal book reading and structured discourse | Improvement in both contexts but more so in book reading than discourse. Some generalisation | Baseline, treatment, and withdrawal probes | CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No statistical analysis. Limited outcome measures. No follow‐up data. No blinding of assessors. No replication across participants |
Helfrich‐Miller 1994 | 3 children (2 males, 1 female) aged 2.9 to 8 years with CAS | Not quasi‐/RCT (Case study series) | Melodic Intonation therapy (MIT) | Linguistic and motor | Varied. 37 to 71 sessions | Varied. Description of skills, consonant inventories, sequencing error rates and intelligibility compared to typical development | Child 1: all consonants in inventory Child 2: spoke in complex sentences, poor intelligibility, and articulation errors present. Child 3: sequencing error rate dropped from 75% to 22%. 13/18 consonant sounds improved |
Pre‐ and post‐treatment | No experimental control. Lack of information on diagnosis of CAS. Primarily descriptive measures ‒ not reliable or tested using statistics. No control, maintenance or generalisation data |
Iuzzini 2010 | 4 children (2 males, 2 females) aged 3.7 to 6.10 years with CAS | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case design) | Stimulability (STP) and modified Core Vocabulary (mCVT) used concurrently | Linguistic and motor | 55‐minute sessions (10 minutes STP, 45 minutes mCVT), 2 × per week for 20 sessions. No home practice | Per cent phonemes correct, phonetic inventory and inconsistency | PCC increased on average 20% after combined therapy (range 9% to 32%). Inventory gained 5 phones on average (range 1 to 10). 3/4 had greater consistency on CSIP and ISP after therapy; 1 had greater inconsistency | Pre‐ and post‐ treatment | Poor experimental control as stable baseline not established, lack of control data. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No statistical analysis. No blinding of assessors. No immediate post‐treatment data or generalisation data |
Jaroma 1984 | 1 male aged 5.5 years with "some dyspraxic features" (CAS diagnosis not explicit) | Not quasi‐/RCT (Case study) | Sensory integrative therapy and speech therapy | Motor | Daily sessions for 2 months | (SP only) Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities | Test not completed post‐treatment. Observation of greater self‐monitoring and correction of speech | Pre‐treatment only | Lack of experimental control, multiple baselines or control data. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No statistical analysis. Limited outcome measures and no post‐treatment data. No blinding of assessors. No immediate post‐treatment data or generalisation data. No replication across participants. Lack of information on speech therapy provided |
Kadis 2014 | 14 children (9 males, 5 females) aged 3 to 6 years with diagnosed CAS (compared to 14 age‐matched controls) | Not quasi‐/RCT (Case series pre‐post design) | Prompts for Restructuring Oral Muscular Phonetic Targets (PROMPT) | Motor | 2 × per week for 8 weeks (16 sessions in total) | GFTA2, HCAPP, VMPAC, MRI |
Significant gains as a group for all speech measures | 1‐week pre‐treatment (baseline), 1‐week post‐treatment | CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. Age‐matched control group older than CAS group. Limited information on PROMPT targets selected for replication. No blinding of assessors. No stimulus generalisation measures |
King 2013 | 3 males aged 4.1, 5.8 and 8.6 years diagnosed with CAS. 1 of the 3 diagnosed with Opitz FG syndrome and another with PDD‐NOS | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single subject multiple baseline across participants design) | Integrated Multimodal Intervention (structured book reading, drill and play activities with AAC devices present and speech encouraged) | Augmentative and alternative communication | 1‐hour sessions, 2 × per week for 3 to 6 weeks | Category (e.g. vocalisation, AAC or both), type of word and accuracy targets. Case 1: final consonants. Case 2: initial /s/ clusters then /f/. Case 3: initial /s/ clusters |
Increases in vocalisations/spoken speech noted for 3/3. Speech accuracy improved on targets for 1/3 cases but all showed some generalisation to more accurate everyday speech | Baseline probes, probes every 2nd treatment session, 1‐month post‐treatment | Poor experimental control for case 1 and some change on control data noted. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No statistical analysis. Limited outcome measures. No blinding of assessors. No generalisation data. No long‐term treatment data |
Klick 1985 | 1 female aged 5.6 years with CAS | Not quasi‐/RCT (Case description) | Adapted Cueing Technique | Motor | 30 minutes of therapy per day for 6 months | Number of single words/utterances | From 2 to 4 words to 12 words and several carrier phrases. After 6 months began to produce novel sentences | Description of progress during treatment | Lack of experimental control, multiple baselines or control data. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No statistical analysis. Limited outcome measures. No blinding of assessors. No follow‐up or generalisation data. No replication across participants |
Krauss 1982 | 2 males aged 5 and 6 years diagnosed with CAS | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case (ABAA) design) | Concurrent Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) and traditional therapy (20% and 80% of sessions respectively) | Linguistic and motor | 2 × per week over 2‐month period | Pre‐ and post‐treatment gains on word‐morpheme usage, auditory comprehension, naming, describing function, sentence completion, imitation of word phrases and articulation. Tested using language sampling and Porch Index of Communicative Ability in Children | Significant gains were found in phrase length (MLU), picture naming, and verbal imitation tasks. Little change in articulation | Pre‐treatment, post‐traditional therapy, and post‐MIT therapy | Lack of experimental control, multiple baselines or control data. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No blinding of assessors. No immediate post‐treatment data or generalisation data. No long‐term follow‐up. There were no reliability data reported for language sample analysis, a subjective measure |
Lagasse 2012 | 2 males aged 5 and 6 years with suspected CAS | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case (AB) design) | Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) compared to 'traditional speech‐language therapy' | Linguistic and motor | Ongoing 1 × per week speech therapy (traditional articulation sessions) and 40‐minute MIT music sessions over 4 weeks (both treatments concurrent) | GFTA2; KLPA2 and speech production on stimulable sounds in 1‐ or 2‐syllable words | Case 1 made greater gains in MIT sessions (but only 2% gain). Case 2 made greater gains on traditional articulation therapy (15% gain) | Pre‐ and post‐ treatment | Lack of experimental control, multiple baselines or control data. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No statistical analysis. Limited outcome measures. No blinding of assessors. No follow‐up or generalisation data |
Lüke 2016 | 1 German‐speaking male aged 2.7 years with severe CAS | Single case design (A‐B design with 3 follow‐up assessments post‐treatment with some treatment sessions between assessments) | Speech Generating Devices ‒ fixed display (Gotalk 20+) and dynamic display (DynaVox V) | Augmentative and alternative communication | 45‐minute sessions × 50 treatment sessions. Treatment sessions 2 to 28 days apart | Means of communication (oral versus SGD), intelligibility of speech productions, consistency of speech productions, lexical development, and grammatical development | Significantly more communication initially with SGD than speech; significant increase in speech intelligibility; consistency (however reduced data in baseline period); amount of words used; and increased MLU and inflections after 8 to 9 sessions | Baseline × 3, every 2nd treatment session, and 2, 4 and 8 weeks post‐treatment | Lack of baseline data for consistency. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No blinding of assessors. No clear withdrawal phase after treatment with SGDs for control and no generalisation data |
Lundeborg 2007 | 1 female aged 5.1 years with CAS | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case cross‐over design) | Intra‐oral stimulation and electropalatography | Motor | 25‐minute sessions (5 minutes intra‐oral stim, 20 minutes EPG); daily at home, total of 195 sessions in 12 months | Per cent consonants correct, per cent phonemes correct, per cent words correct, intelligibility, visual deviancy | Significant treatment outcomes on all measures | Pre‐testing, A1 (baseline), B (intervention: oral stimulation therapy), A2 (withdrawal for 3 months), B (intervention: EPG), and A3 (follow‐up) | Cross‐over design, no control group or data taken to control for maturation. No replication across participants. No long‐term follow‐up or generalisation data taken |
Maas 2012a | 4 children (2 males, 2 females) aged 5.4 to 8.4 years with CAS (2 also with dysarthria and a third with language disorder); 3 also in Maas 2012b, as below | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case alternating treatments design with multiple baselines across behaviours over 2 phases) | Dynamic Temporal and Tactile Cueing (high versus moderate feedback frequency in cross‐over design) | Motor | 50‐minute sessions 3 × per week for 3 participants but 1 had 60‐minute sessions 2 × per week | Per cent accuracy on 2‐point scale of segmental and suprasegmental aspects of target words and phrases with 2 words | 2 responded better to low frequency feedback, 1 to high frequency feedback, and 1 to no condition. No generalisation effects | Weekly probes: 3 to 4 × baseline, 4 × treatment. Phase 1: 4 to 5 × withdrawal, 4 × treatment. Phase 2: 2 × withdrawal and 1 month post‐treatment | Small sample size with heterogeneity. Cross‐over conditions made comparison difficult regarding targets chosen. No control group. Effect sizes used not interpretable or comparable to others. Different doses across all participants. Treatment fidelity < 80%. No stimulus generalisation measures |
Maas 2012b | 4 children (2 males and 2 females) aged 5.0 to 7.9 years with CAS. 2 cases had additional dysarthria diagnoses. 1 other case had multiple co‐occurring disorders |
Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case alternating treatments design with multiple baselines across behaviours over 2 phases) | Dynamic Temporal and Tactile Cueing (random versus blocked practice compared in cross‐over design) | Motor | 2 × 4 week blocks of therapy | Per cent accuracy on 2‐point scale of segmental and suprasegmental aspects of entire target words and phrases with 2 words | 3/4 responded to both conditions. 2 responded relatively better to blocked practice, 1 to random practice, and 1 to no condition. 2/4 demonstrated generalization | Weekly probes: 3 to 4 × baseline, 4 × treatment. Phase 1: 4 to 5 × withdrawal, 4 × treatment. Phase 2: 2 × withdrawal and 1 month post‐treatment | Small sample size with heterogeneity. Cross‐over conditions made comparison difficult regarding targets chosen. No control group. Effect sizes used not interpretable or comparable to others. Treatment fidelity < 80%. No stimulus generalisation measures |
Martikainen 2011 | 1 female aged 4.7 years with CAS | Not quasi‐/RCT (Multiple baseline across behaviours ‐ cross‐over treatment design) | Combined Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) and Touch Cue Method (TCM) | Motor and linguistic | 3 sessions for 6 weeks for 18 sessions for MIT. 6 weeks no therapy. 3 sessions for 6 weeks for 18 sessions for TCM | Articulation accuracy: PVC, PCC. Also, overall word accuracy scores: PMLU, PWP, PWC. All calculated from responses to 46 picture cards |
1/5 measures significant post‐MIT (per cent vowels correct). Per cent consonants correct also reduced. 3/5 significant post‐TCM (PVC, PCC, PMLU). PVC, PCC and PMLU maintained. Greater changes for both therapies after withdrawal. PCC and PMLU only significant after MIT withdrawn |
Beginning and end of 6‐week baseline, beginning and end of both treatment phases, 12 weeks after TCM withdrawn | Lack of experimental control of other factors. Cross‐over design makes comparison of both treatments difficult as many changes only noted after withdrawal of MIT (accumulation effects). Limited outcome data. Lack of generalisation data No blinded assessors. No replication across participants |
Martin 2016 | 12 children (sex unknown) aged 3 to 10 years with CAS (11 with co‐occurring conditions) | Case series (pre and post design) | DuBard Association Method®. It is a multimodal, phonetic therapy which works from accurate sounds in isolation | Motor | Daily in small groups in a school programme for an 11‐month period | Articulation, mean length of utterance (MLU), and intelligibility on Arizona Articulation Proficiency Scale‐Third Revision (AAPS‐3) and perceptions of resilience judged by parents and SLPs | Significant changes in articulation, intelligibility and MLU, and some resilience measures over 2‐year period | Pre‐ and post‐ treatment | Lack of experimental control regarding maturation effects (despite using the Intervention Efficiency Index and Proportional Change Index) and lack of control of covariate, including other potential intervention over the same period. No control group. No follow‐up or generalisation data |
McCabe 2014 | 4 males aged 5.5 to 8.6 years with CAS. 2 children had additional auditory processing impairments | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case (AB) design with 1 month follow‐up) | Rapid Syllable Transition Treatment (ReST) | Motor | 60‐minute session, 4 × per week for 3 weeks (12 sessions in total). Minimum of 1200 trials per session | Articulation, prosodic and simultaneous articulation and prosodic accuracy on trained and untrained probe pseudo words; PCC, PVC and per cent lexical stress matches from connected speech; PPVT‐4 as control data | All 4 participants increased perceptual accuracy. 1/4 participants showed change in untreated items. All participants showed change in prosody (average prosody gain 58%, 3/4 in PVC and 2/4 in PCC; average gain 79%). Control data (receptive vocabulary on PPVT‐IV) changed minimally | Baseline × 2, probes in treatment × 2, 1 month follow‐up | There was no immediate post‐treatment data taken to determine treatment effects, the follow‐up data was 1 month post‐treatment and included a withdrawal phase. There was no statistical analysis of connected speech data. 1 participant reached ceiling. No blinding of assessors. No stimulus generalisation measures |
McNeill 2009a | 12 children (9 males, 3 females) aged 4.2 to 7.6 years with CAS | Not quasi‐/RCT (Case series design) | Integrated Phonological Awareness Intervention | Linguistic | 45‐minute session; 2 × per week for 6 weeks in 2 blocks with 6‐week withdrawal between blocks. Total of 245 sessions | Trained speech accuracy and phonological awareness accuracy on a probe. Generalisation‐ BTOPP and first trial of DEAP inconsistency subtest for PVC, PVC and inconsistency score. PIPA for 4‐year‐olds. TOPA for 5 to 7‐year‐olds. Burt Word Reading Test for non‐word reading and informal non‐word reading probe (Gillon 2000). Per cent grapheme correct score in spelling 10 words from DEAP inconsistency subtest | Speech: 9/12 children improved on trained items. Phonological awareness: 8/12 children improved in 1 or both intervention blocks. Generalisation for 8/12 on all measures except Burt Word Reading Test | Pre‐ and post‐treatment | Lack of experimental control, control group or control data. CAS diagnosis unclear regarding prosody. Limited information provided on each participant. Limited treatment phase data. No maintenance data. No blinding of assessors |
McNeill 2009b | 2 male identical twins aged 4.5 years with CAS (deletion at 10q21.2‐22.1) | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case design) | Integrated Phonological Awareness intervention | Linguistic | 45‐minute session; 2 × per week for 6 weeks in 2 blocks with 6‐week withdrawal between blocks. Total of 245 sessions | PPC, PVC on BTOPP, and DEAP inconsistency percentage. PIPA, PhonRep, Burt Word Reading, Non‐word Reading, Neale accuracy and comprehension | PCC and PVC improved at post‐treatment and follow‐up. Reduced inconsistency. Sound‐letter knowledge increased from 0 to 7 at post‐treatment. Reading WNL and spelling demonstrated use of strategies at final follow‐up | Pre‐ and post‐ treatment, and 6‐month follow‐up | Lack of experimental control, control group or control data. CAS diagnosis unclear regarding prosody. Limited information provided on each participant. Limited treatment phase data. No maintenance data. No blinding of assessors. No stimulus generalisation measures |
McNeill 2010 | 12 children (9 males, 3 females) aged 4.2 to 7.6 years diagnosed with CAS | Not quasi‐/RCT (12‐month follow‐up to 2009 case series) | Integrated Phonological Awareness intervention | Linguistic | As per McNeill 2009a | BBTOP and 1st trial of DEAP yielding PPC. PIPA for 4‐year‐olds & TOPA for 5 to 7‐year‐olds. Decoding measures (Burt Word Reading Test and Non‐word Reading Task) and spelling measures (probe of 10 words from the DEAP inconsistency subtest) were completed for participants at least 6 years of age at the beginning of the study. The NARA was administered for participants aged 5 years, 6 months and up | Significant difference for CAS group from pre‐ to post‐treatment on letter knowledge, non‐word reading probe, spelling, PCC, TOPA and Burt Non‐Word Reading. 3/7 improved on NARA to age‐appropriate level | 1‐year follow‐up to McNeill 2009a | 7/12 of original participants followed up. Whole group data ‒ case series. No control group or control data for experimental control or maturation effects |
Moriarty 2006 | 3 children (2 males, 1 female) aged 6.3, 6.10 and 7.3 years with CAS | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case multiple baseline design across behaviours) | Integrated Phonological Awareness Intervention | Linguistic | 45‐minute sessions 3 × per week for 3 weeks | PPC on probe, phoneme segmentation probe, phoneme manipulation probes, initial sound identification probes, letter‒sound knowledge subtest from the PIPA, non‐word reading tasks | 2/3 significantly increased PPC, 2/3 significantly improved phonological awareness skills on probes, letter‒sound knowledge, and non‐word reading. Limited transfer to untreated words | Baseline and post‐treatment (3 probes each) | Lack of control group and control data. CAS diagnosis unclear regarding prosody. Lack of multiple baseline data throughout treatment. No long‐term follow‐up. No blinding of assessors |
Namasivayam 2013 | 12 children (9 males, 3 females) aged 3 to 6 years with speech sound disorders | Not quasi‐/RCT (Case series pre‐post design) | Prompts for Restructuring Oral Muscular Phonetic Targets (PROMPT) | Motor | 45‐minute session 2 × per week for 8 weeks | GFTA2, HCAPP, VMPAC focal motor and sequencing subtests, Children's Speech Intelligibility Measure | Significant gains as a group for all speech measures | Baseline 1 week prior to treatment, and 1 week post‐treatment | Lack of experimental control, control group, multiple baseline or control data. No blinding of assessors. No blinding of assessors. No long‐term follow‐up |
Namasivayam 2015 | 37 children (28 males, 9 females) aged 2.6 to 4.5 years with CAS | Not quasi‐/RCT (pre‐postgroup design) | Motor Speech Treatment Protocol (MSTP) | Motor | Intense treatment group: 45‐minute session, 2 × per week × 10 weeks = 20 sessions. Less intense group: 45‐minute session, 1 × per week × 10 weeks = 10 sessions | GFTA‐2 sounds in words subtest; speech intelligibility using Children's Speech Intelligibility Measure (CSIM) at word level, and Beginner's Intelligibility Test (BIT) at sentence level. Functional Outcomes for Children Under Six (FOCUS) scale | Intense group had greater changes in articulation and functional communication compared to the less intense group with large effect sizes. Mixed results were found for intelligibility: at word‐level (CSIM), both the less intense and 1/2 intense groups made a significant and large change. At sentence level, 1/2 intense groups made a significant change | Pre‐ and post‐ treatment | No control group or control data. Participants were not directly randomised; however, no between‐group differences were found at baseline. There were missing data (dealt with using intention‐to‐treat analysis). No information on session trials was obtained, which is important for intensity calculations |
Preston 2013 | 6 males aged 9 to 15 years with CAS. 1 child had additional ADHD and another child had additional dysarthria | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case multiple baseline across behaviours across participants) | Ultrasound biofeedback (targeting articulation on clusters and CV or VC sequences of inaccurate phones) | Motor (instrumentally based) | 60 minute sessions, 2 × per week × 18 sessions (at least 150 trials per session) | Probe of whole‐word accuracy of treated and untreated items | U002 and U007 had significant gains on 2/4 treated combinations, U005 for 3/4, and U008, U009 and U012 had significant gains on all treated combinations. All exhibited some generalisation (target‐dependant). U005, U007, U008, U009, U012 demonstrated maintenance above pre‐treatment levels | Probes at baseline × 3, every treatment session, post‐treatment, and 2 months post‐treatment | No control group or comparison treatment. No blinding of assessors. Untreated items were not clearly selected as control or generalisation data with some showing change and others not |
Preston 2016 | 3 male children aged 11 to 13 years diagnosed with CAS and poor expressive language and phonological processing. 1 participant had additional flaccid dysarthria, ADHD, language and learning difficulties | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case multiple baseline across behaviours (syllable positions)) | Ultrasound biofeedback (using structured chaining and principles of motor learning) | Motor (instrumentally based) | 1 hour sessions × 14 sessions. Sessions 1 to 7 addressed target 1 and sessions 8 to 14 addressed target 2 with randomly assigned prosody or no prosody conditions | Treatment acquisition data, generalisation probe of untreated words, maintenance to 2 months post‐treatment | 2/3 participants acquired accurate articulation. 0/3 demonstrated generalisation or maintenance | 3 × baseline probes, midway therapy probe, post‐therapy probe (within 1 week after treatment), and 2‐month follow‐up | No control group. Greater within‐treatment probes and post‐treatment probes would have allowed for greater statistical analysis. No control data. No blinding of assessors. No stimulus generalisation measures |
Preston 2017 | 3 males aged 11 to 14 years with CAS | Not quasi/RCT (Single case (ABA) design) |
Ultrasound biofeedback (using structured chaining and principles of motor learning.) | Motor (Instrumentally based) | 2 × 1‐hour articulation treatment a day for 2 weeks. 16 hours of therapy in total. Over 100 trials per session | Treatment acquisition of /ɹ/, /s/ or /ʧ/. Generalisation to untrained items using a probe and sentence imitation task, and maintenance 1 to 3 weeks post‐treatment (audio‐samples submitted) | Case 1 had acquisition, generalisation, and maintenance of targets. Case 2 had some acquisition in the 2nd week of therapy and no generalisation and maintenance. Case 3 showed acquisition, limited generalisation to words and not phrases, and no maintenance | Probe conducted 1 × before treatment, at the end of the first week, and at the end of the second week (post‐treatment) | Lack of experimental control, multiple baselines or control data. No blinding of assessors. No long‐term follow‐up data. No stimulus generalisation measures |
Ray 2003 | 1 adult with CAS and class III malocclusion. Another 5 adults aged 18 to 23 years with persistent articulation disorders | Not quasi‐/RCT (Case series) | Orofacial myofunctional therapy | Motor (Instrumentally based) | 45‐minute session, 1 × per week for 6 weeks | Dworkin‐Culatta Oral Mechanism Examination for oral postures and intelligibility in single words, sentences, and spontaneous speech | All improved lips and tongue postures. 5/6 participants increased intelligibility. No improvement in intelligibility for person with DVD | Pre‐ and post‐treatment | Lack of experimental control, multiple baselines or control data. No treatment data or follow‐up reported. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No statistical analysis. Limited outcome measures. No blinding of assessors. No immediate post‐treatment data or generalisation data. No replication across participants |
Rosenbek 1974 | 1 female aged 9 years with CAS | Not quasi‐/RCT (Case study) | Intensive, systematic drill motor therapy | Motor | 22 sessions over 3 months | 20‐item probe of /r/ (target), ineligibility in spontaneous speech | /r/ improved from 0 to 20 correct in probe. Intelligibility judged by unfamilar listeners improved | Treatment sessions | Lack of experimental control, multiple baselines or control data. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No follow‐up data. Only ancedotal generalisation data. No statistical analysis. No reliability of judgments reported. No replication across participants |
Rosenthal 1994 | 4 children (3 males, 1 female) aged 10‐14 years diagnosed with CAS | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single subject (ABAB) design) | Rate Control Therapy | Linguistic and motor | 20‐minute session per reading passage. No further information available | Articulation accuracy (words read correctly) | Improved to 85% accuracy at 50% habitual rate and maintained in therapy as rate was slowly increased. Limited generalisation to conversation ‐ therapy implemented | Reading rate in 5‐minute intervals | Lack of control and follow‐up data. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No statistical analysis. No blinding of assessors. No stimulus generalisation measures. No report of data reliability |
Skelton 2014 | 3 children (2 males, 1 female) aged 4 to 6 years diagnosed with CAS | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case multiple baseline design across participants) | Concurrent treatment (using randomised variable practice) | Motor | Therapy until target sounds reached 80% accuracy. P1 had 26, P2 had 12 and P3 had 28 sessions. 2 × per week, 30 minutes per session and on average 100 to 115 trials per session | Per cent correct productions on /s, z, f, v/ trained targets during baseline and treatment; generalisation probes to untrained words and 3‐word phrases | All children reached 80% accuracy on target sounds. Moderate to large generalisation effects at word and 3‐word phrases levels (70% to 100% accuracy) | 3 × baseline probes, probes every 5 therapy sessions | No post‐treatment or follow‐up/maintenance data. No blinded assessors. No stimulus generalisation data. P3 continued regular school therapy during the study so could be a confounding factor. No stimulus generalisation measures |
Stokes 2010 | 1 male aged 7 years with residual CAS | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case (ABA) design) | Articulation with facilitative vowel contexts | Linguistic | 45‐ to 55‐minute session, 3 × per week for 3 weeks. 60+ trials per session. Home practice provided | Accuracy on 'sh' sound in word initial probe, 'tr' as control | Significant improvement in 'sh' articulation accuracy in trained and untrained words. No change in control words with 'tr' initial | Pre‐treatment, mid‐therapy × 2 (after sessions 3 and 6), post‐treatment, and maintenance (2 weeks post‐treatment) | Participant did not meet current CAS criteria. Lack of generalisation data beyond 'sh' sound. No blinded assessors. No replication across participants. No long‐term follow‐up data. No reliability of data reported |
Strand 2000 | 1 female aged 5 years with "severe motor planning deficits but no dysarthria" (CAS) | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case multiple baseline design) | Integral stimulation | Motor | 30‐ to 50‐minute session, 3 to 5 × per week (1 to 2 × per day) for 10 to 16 sessions. No home practice | Articulation accuracy ratings on a 2‐point scale | Improvement from 0.25 to 0.80 on 2‐point scale. 4/5 treatment stimuli achieved rating of 2/2 by end of therapy | Treated stimuli at start of each session, control stimuli twice a week | No statistical analysis. Limited outcome measures. No blinding of assessors. No follow‐up data or generalisation data. No replication across participants |
Strand 2006 | 4 males aged 5.5 to 6.1 years with CAS (2 with dysarthria and 1 with mild intellectual disability) | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case multiple baseline across participants) | Dynamic Temporal and Tactile Cueing | Motor | 30‐minute sessions, 2 × per day for 5 days a week for 38 to 50 sessions | Articulation accuracy on a 3‐point scale | Treatment gains for 3/4 participants maintained by 2/4 | Baseline × 4 (or more, staggered baseline), 20+ treatment probes | No follow‐up or generalisation data. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No statistical analysis. Limited outcome measures |
Thomas 2014 | 4 children (2 males, 2 females) aged 4.8 to 8 years with CAS | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case multiple baseline across participants and behaviours) | Rapid Syllable Transition Treatment (ReST) | Motor | 50 minute sessions 2 × per week for 6 weeks. 100 trials per session | Accuracy on imitated (a) treated words, (b) untreated pseudo words, (c) untreated real words and control words | Significant improvement on treated words and untreated real words. Significant improvement for 2/4 participants on untreated pseudo words. No change in control items | Baseline × 3 to 6, treatment × 3, and 1 day, 1 month and 4 months post‐treatment | Use of GFTA2 for control items. No stimulus generalisation data |
Thomas 2016 | 5 children (4 males, 1 female) aged 5 to 11 years with CAS (3 with mild or moderate receptive language disorder) | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case multiple baseline across participants) | Rapid Syllable Transition Treatment (ReST) | Motor (instrumentally based ‐ telehealth) | 60‐minute session, 4 times a week for 3 weeks (12 sessions in total). Minimum of 1200 trials per session | Accuracy on treated pseud‐word items, generalisation to untreated non‐words and real words, and control items (articulation of rhotics) on a probe; client/family satisfaction with telehealth treatment | 5/5 participants demonstrated significant change in treated items. 4/5 maintained gains to 4 months post‐treatment. 4/5 had significant generalisation to untrained non‐words and real words, and 1/5 demonstrated change in control data (articulation errors of rhotics or /s/). Families very satisfied and motivated by telehealth treatment | At least 3 baseline probes, 3 therapy probes (sessions 5, 9 and 1 day post‐treatment). Follow‐up at 1 week, 4 weeks & 4 months post‐treatment | Missing data for some participants at certain time points in Table 3. Problems with change in control data. Some internet issues (dropouts, port sound quality, etc.) were observed in 61% of sessions; however, significant outcomes were found. No stimulus generalisation data |
Tierney 2016 | 1 male aged 3 years with CAS and fine motor delay | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case design; descriptive) |
Multimodal therapy: Signed Exact English sign language, Sarah Rosenfeld Johnson's oro‐motor programme and Kaufman Speech Praxis Program | Augmentative and alternative communication | Clinic‐based sessions 45 minutes 1 to 2 × per week and home‐based sessions for 60 minutes 1 × per week | Language assessment; observations and Kaufman Speech Praxis Test; Verbal Motor Production Assessment for Children (VMPAC) | Receptive and expressive language consistently in average range but receptive relatively better than expressive language. By 3.6 years of age receptive and expressive language same level. Marked drooling and limited inventory and sequencing at 18 months, yet skills on Kaufman & VMPAC in average range at 3 years, 9 months. Discharged from therapy | Language assessment at 1.1 year, 3 years and 3.6 years. Kaufman test or observations at 1.6, 3 and 3.9 years. VMPAC at 3 years, 9 months | Lack of experimental control, multiple baselines or control data. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable regarding prosody and drooling. No statistical analysis. No blinding of assessors. No replication across participants. Limited repeated measures on same instrument. Participant had multiple therapies concurrently |
Vashdi 2013 | 1 male aged 14 years with severe CAS and limb/motor apraxia and obsessive compulsive disorder | Not quasi‐/RCT (Case study) | Verbal Motor Learning (with Dynamic Distal Stabilization Technique (DDST)) | Motor | 1 × 30‐minute clinic session and 6 × home practice sessions a week for 4 weeks | (1) Producing highest pitch using /I/ sound with and without DDST, to determine minimum and maximum frequency and length using Speech Analyser 1.5 (2) Imitation of 18 words to analyse word length, maximum loudness, maximum and minimum frequency |
Significant t‐test results for (1) increase in maximum frequency and length of pitch after DDST, no change in minimum frequency, and (2) decrease in word length (word said faster), maximum loudness, and maximum frequency | Pre‐ and post‐ treatment | Lack of experimental control, multiple baselines or control data. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No statistical analysis. Limited outcome measures. No blinding of assessors. No follow‐up or generalisation data. No replication across participants. Unclear data analysis procedures (unclear if they used visual analysis or perceptual analysis, and if they tested assumptions for the statistical analysis completed) |
Vashdi 2014 | 1 female aged 10 years with CAS and ASD | Not quasi‐/RCT (Case study) | Verbal Motor Learning (Initial Phoneme Cue (IPC) technique) | Motor | 2 × 1 hour sessions, 2 weeks apart (participant had initial therapy: 1‐hour session weekly for 1 year prior to this study) | Imitation accuracy of CVCV treated words either (a) with IPC or (b) without IPC | Imitation of CVCV was 0% to 22% accuracy and imitation with IPC was 96% to 100% accuracy | Pre‐ and post‐ treatment | Lack of experimental control, multiple baselines or control data. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No statistical analysis. Limited outcome measures. No statistical analysis. No blinding of assessors. No follow‐up or generalisation data. No replication across participants |
Yoss 1974 | 10 children (no information on gender reported) aged 6 to 11 years with moderate to severe DAS | Not quasi‐/RCT (Case descriptions/file audit) | School‐based intervention | Motor | 25 to 307 hours of therapy | Articulation, polysyllable words and connected speech in speech samples. Intelligibility rated on a 9‐point scale | Significant improvement on articulation. Minimal generalisation to polysyllable words and connected speech. Intelligibility improved by at least 0.5 points | Pre‐ and post‐ treatment | Lack of experimental control, multiple baselines or control data. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No statistical analysis. No blinding of assessors. No follow‐up data |
Zaretsky 2010 | 1 female aged 11.6 years with CAS, intellectual disability and language disorder | Not quasi‐/RCT (Single case design) | Phonological awareness (phoneme‒grapheme mapping, reading comprehension, 'Basics' programme). Speech ‐ PROMPT and Moving Across Syllables | Linguistic | Between 6.0 and 11.6 ongoing weekly treatments ‐ 1 hour × 1:1 sessions and PROMPT institute over summer | Per cent accuracy on phonological awareness and decoding | Improvement seen in phoneme‒grapheme mapping, segmentation and short vowel identification. Some improvement in decoding | Ongoing 1 × per week sessions from 6.0 to 11.6 years | Lack of experimental control, multiple baselines or control data. CAS diagnosis unclear and not replicable. No statistical analysis. Limited outcome measures. No blinding of assessors. No follow‐up or generalisation data. No replication across participants. Difficult to replicate measures and treatment used |
Participants: All participants are English speakers unless otherwise reported.
AOS: apraxia of speech; BBTOP: Bankson‐Bernthal Test of Phonology; CAS: childhood apraxia of speech; CSIP: consonant substitute inconsistency percentage; DAS: developmental apraxia of speech; DEAP: Diagnostic Evaluation of Articulation and Phonology; DVD: developmental verbal dyspraxia; GDD: global developmental delay; GFTA‐2: Goldman Fristoe Test of Articulation 2; HCAPP: Hodson Computerized Analysis of Phonological Patterns; ISP: inconsistency severity percentage; KLPA‐2: Khan‐Lewis Phonological Analysis, Second Edition; NARA: Neale Analysis of Reading Ability; PCC: percentage consonants correct; PDD‐NOS: pervasive developmental disorder ‐ not otherwise specified; PMLU: phonological mean length of utterance; PVC: percentage vowels correct; PWC: percentage words correct; PWP: proportion of whole‐word proximity; PIPA: Preschool and Primary Inventory of Phonological Awareness; RCT: randomised control trial; SSD: speech sound disorder; TOCS+: Test of Children's Speech Plus; TOPA: Test of Phonological Awareness; VMPAC: Verbal Motor Production Assessment for Children
Assessment of risk of bias in included studies
Two review authors (FL and AM) independently assessed the risk of bias within the one included study, using Cochrane's 'Risk of bias' tool (Higgins 2011a). Both review authors rated the risk of bias as low, high or unclear (uncertain), across each of the domains listed below. There were no cases of disagreement.
Sequence generation. Did the study describe the method used to generate the allocation sequence in sufficient detail to determine whether it produced comparable groups? In the review authors' judgment, was the sequence adequately generated?
Allocation concealment. Did the study describe the method used to conceal the allocation sequence in sufficient detail to assess whether intervention schedules could have been foreseen in advance of, or during, recruitment? In the review authors' judgment, was allocation adequately concealed?
Blinding of participants and personnel. Did the study describe any measures used to blind participants and personnel from knowledge of which intervention a given participant might have received? In the review authors' judgment, was knowledge of the allocated interventions adequately concealed from participants and relevant personnel during the study?
Blinding of outcome assessment. Did the study describe any measures used to blind outcome assessors from knowledge of which intervention a given participant might have received? In the review authors' judgment, was knowledge of the allocated interventions adequately concealed from all outcome assessors during the study?
Incomplete outcome data. Did the study report data on attrition and exclusions as well as the numbers involved (compared with total randomised), reasons for attrition/exclusion, and any re‐inclusions in analyses performed. In the review authors' judgment, did the study authors deal adequately with incomplete data?
Selective outcome reporting. Did the study make attempts to assess the possibility of selective outcome reporting? In the review authors' judgment, are reports of the study free of suggestion of selective outcome reporting determined by comparing the outcomes listed in the original study protocol with the final RCT publication?
Other sources of bias. Was the study apparently free of other problems that could put it at a high risk of bias? In the review authors' judgement, was the study free of other problems not covered by the domains above?
Measures of treatment effect
We were unable to conduct a meta‐analysis due to there being only one included study. We have archived our methods for use in future updates of this review (see Appendix 3; Morgan 2006).
Unit of analysis issues
For each outcome measure, we averaged the accuracy of production (e.g. number of correct items from total items elicited) across the group. Units were mean accuracy scores for each intervention group. See Appendix 3 for additional methods archived for use in future updates of this review.
Dealing with missing data
There were missing data for 1/26 participants in the Murray 2015 RCT, due to a participant withdrawing in the middle of treatment (see Appendix 3 and Morgan 2006).
Assessment of heterogeneity
We were unable to assess heterogeneity as only one study met the inclusion criteria (see Appendix 3 and Morgan 2006).
Assessment of reporting biases
We were unable to assess reporting biases due to there being only one included study (see Appendix 3 and Morgan 2006).
Data synthesis
We could not undertake a meta‐analysis as we included only one study in the review (see Appendix 3 and Morgan 2006).
Summary of findings
Using GRADEpro GDT (GRADEpro GDT 2015), we created a 'Summary of findings' table for the comparison: Nuffield Dyspraxia Programme ‐ Third Edition (NDP‐3) versus Rapid Syllable Transition Treatment (ReST) for Childhood Apraxia of Speech. In this table we report our primary (accuracy of production on treated and non‐treated items) and secondary (speech production consistency and accuracy of connected speech) outcomes for one month post‐treatment. We chose this time point as it is the most clinically salient time point. The time point immediately after therapy is not sufficient to determine whether the treatment effect was sustained. We did not examine the time point of four months post‐therapy because the number of participants in each group (NDP‐3: 9/13 participants; ReST: 9/13 participants) had returned to community SLP/SLT treatment between the one‐month and four‐month post‐therapy period and, as such, it would be difficult to delineate between a sustained treatment effect of the RCT versus the usual therapy re‐introduced. We also report in this table the quality ratings for each outcome as assessed by two review authors (AM and FL) using the GRADE approach (Schünemann 2017). They assigned ratings of high, moderate, low or very low quality, according to the presence of risk of bias (Risk of bias in included studies), indirectness of evidence, unexplained heterogeneity or inconsistency in results, imprecision of results and high probability of publication bias; they discussed any disagreements over the quality ratings until a consensus was reached.
Please see 'Table 1' for an overview of treatment effects for each outcome measure and GRADE assessment of the quality of the evidence.
Subgroup analysis and investigation of heterogeneity
We were unable to perform any subgroup analyses as we included only one study in the review. See Appendix 3 and Morgan 2006.
Sensitivity analysis
We were unable to perform a sensitivity analysis as we included only one study in the review. See Appendix 3 and Morgan 2006.
Results
Description of studies
Results of the search
We identified a total of 7978 records once duplicates were discarded. EM identified a further 16 records through handsearching. Of these 7994 titles and abstracts, we excluded 7895 as clearly irrelevant, and assessed the full texts of the remaining 99 reports against our inclusion criteria (Criteria for considering studies for this review). From these 99 reports, only one study met our inclusion criteria for this review (Included studies); we excluded the remaining 98 reports as irrelevant (see Excluded studies). We did not identify any non‐English abstracts for inclusion. Please see Figure 1.
Included studies
The one included study, Murray 2015, was an RCT that compared treatment effects for two interventions, each delivered intensively (one hour for four days a week for three weeks): the Nuffield Dyspraxia Programme ‐ Third Edition (NDP‐3; Williams 2004) and the Rapid Syllable Transition treatment (ReST; Ballard 2010). Twenty‐six children (13 allocated to each therapy group), aged 4 to 12 years (18 males) with CAS diagnosed by a SLP/SLT participated in the study, which took place at the University of Sydney Communication Disorders Treatment and Research Clinic. The primary outcomes were per cent accuracy on treated and untreated pseudo‐words and real words and phrases.
The research was funded (as published in the article) by: Douglas and Lola Douglas Scholarship on Child and Adolescent Health; Nadia Verrall Memorial 2010; and Postgraduate Student Scholarship 2011 through Speech Pathology Australia, James Kentley Memorial Scholarship, Postgraduate Research Support Schemes and Faculty of Health Sciences; University of Sydney International Development Program Fund, and the Australian Research Council Future Fellowship.
Please see the Characteristics of included studies table for further detail of the nature of these interventions.
Excluded studies
We excluded 98 full‐text reports. Of these, 29 studies were either not interventions (e.g. diagnostic studies), or did not include participants with CAS (e.g. focused on other speech disorders or adult‐acquired apraxia of speech). The remaining 69 excluded papers were CAS intervention studies but were not RCTs, and are tabulated in Characteristics of excluded studies tables. Further detail on the excluded CAS studies is provided in Table 2.
Risk of bias in included studies
We examined the one included study, Murray 2015, for risk of bias. We judged the study to be at low risk of bias for all domains except 'other sources of bias', which we judged to be at unclear risk of bias. Please see the 'Risk of bias' table (beneath the Characteristics of included studies table) for further detail on the basis of our decisions, and Figure 2 for a summary of ratings.
Effects of interventions
See: Table 1
We downgraded the quality of the evidence by one level to moderate due to imprecision, given that only one RCT was identified.
Primary outcome: accuracy of production
The Murray 2015 study compared the number of real words produced correctly (out of the total elicited words) at pre‐treatment with one month post‐treatment for treated and non‐treated items.
Treated items
The study authors reported that, compared to pre‐treatment baseline, the NDP‐3 MD of 36.0 was greater than the ReST MD of 33.9 at one month post‐treatment, with an absolute mean difference of 2.1 between groups.
Non‐treated items
The study authors reported that, compared to pre‐treatment baseline, the ReST MD of 18.3 was minimally greater than the NDP‐3 MD of 18.2 at one month post‐treatment with an absolute mean difference of 0.1 between groups.
Secondary outcomes
Speech production consistency
The Murray 2015 study compared treatment gains in speech production consistency (measured by 25 real words repeated three times using the inconsistency subtest of the Diagnostic Evaluation of Articulation and Phonology (DEAP) test (Dodd 2006)), at pre‐treatment with one month post‐treatment for treated items. The study authors reported that, compared to pre‐treatment baseline, the NDP‐3 MD of 11.1 was minimally greater than the ReST MD of 10.9 at one month post‐treatment, with an absolute mean difference of 0.2 between groups.
Accuracy of connected speech
The Murray 2015 study compared treatment gains in the accuracy of connected speech (as assessed by imitated word accuracy in connected speech of at least three word combinations), at pre‐treatment with one month post‐treatment for treated items. The study authors reported that, compared to pre‐treatment baseline, the NDP‐3 MD of 14.3 was greater than the ReST MD of 11.5 at one month post‐treatment, with an absolute mean difference of 2.8 between groups.
The study did not measure functional communication.
Discussion
Summary of main results
We sought to investigate the effectiveness of targeted speech and language interventions for children and young people, aged 3 to 16 years of age, with a diagnosis of CAS made by a speech and language pathologist/therapist. We found only one study, Murray 2015, which met our inclusion criteria (Criteria for considering studies for this review). This RCT recruited 26 children aged 4 to 12 years, and compared two interventions: the Nuffield Dyspraxia Programme‐3 (NDP‐3); and the Rapid Syllable Transitions Treatment (ReST). Treatments were delivered intensively in one‐hour sessions, four days a week for three weeks, in a university clinic in Australia. Speech pathology students delivered the treatments in the English language.
We considered all core domains to be at low risk of bias. Both the NDP‐3 and ReST therapies demonstrated improvement at one month post‐treatment. A number of cases in each cohort had recommenced usual treatment by their speech and language pathologist between one month and four months post‐treatment (NDP‐3: 9/13 participants; ReST: 9/13 participants). Hence we could not analyse maintenance of treatment effects to four months post‐treatment without significant potential bias, and so we did not include this time point for further analysis in this review.
Overall there is limited evidence that, when delivered intensively, both the NDP‐3 and ReST may effect improvement in word accuracy in 4‐ to 12‐year‐old children with CAS, measured by the accuracy of production on treated and non‐treated words, speech production consistency and the accuracy of connected speech. The study did not assess functional communication. We are unable to say whether either treatment is better than the other, or better than no treatment or treatment as usual. No evidence currently exists to support the effectiveness of other treatments for children aged 4 to 12 years with idiopathic CAS, without other comorbid neurodevelopmental disorders. No formal analyses were conducted to compare NDP‐3 and ReST by the original study authors, hence one treatment cannot be reliably advocated over the other. Further RCTs replicating this study would strengthen the evidence, which we currently rate as low using the GRADE evidence rating system (i.e. that 'further research is very likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate and is likely to change the estimate').
Further well‐controlled studies investigating the effectiveness of other treatments for CAS are also needed across other motor‐based therapies, and also across linguistic and multi‐modal approaches. As noted earlier in the Why it is important to do this review section, non‐RCT case series or case‐control studies examining motor, linguistic and multi‐modal interventions for CAS have described positive effects of intervention, but RCTs are required to strengthen the evidence base for these approaches. Further, there is also a need for trials that examine interventions for CAS compared to no treatment (e.g. wait‐list control group). A no‐treatment comparison is arguably difficult to achieve in this field however, given the typically severe presentation of speech disorder and reticence of parents or clinicians (or both) to withhold treatment from children. Finally, RCTs are also needed on populations with CAS and co‐occurring neurodevelopmental or medical disorders.
Cochrane Reviews are often criticised in the SLP/SLT field because they do not allow consideration of lower levels of evidence, such as case studies or case series, which are more commonly performed in the field. Recognising these concerns we have provided a summary of the observational studies of CAS interventions excluded from this review (see Table 2), to encourage future, rigorous and controlled investigation of the efficacy of these methods. The lack of RCT intervention data in the CAS field to date is reinforced by challenges of: (1) the low incidence of the disorder; (2) the lack of a universally applied diagnostic classification system; (3) a lack of understanding of the aetiology of CAS; and (4) the challenge of designing trials for children with co‐occurring clinical features (e.g. non‐verbal cognitive impairment) or disorders (e.g. intellectual disability, autism spectrum disorder).
Overall completeness and applicability of evidence
We identified only one small RCT for inclusion in this review, indicating that there is an urgent need for further RCTs in this field. The interventions examined are currently in use and therefore results are applicable to clinical practice.
Quality of the evidence
We considered the overall quality of the evidence to be moderate using the GRADE approach; see Table 1. We downgraded the quality of the evidence by one level to moderate, due to imprecision, given that only one RCT was identified.
Potential biases in the review process
We carefully managed potential conflicts of interest, as described below under Contributions of authors and Declarations of interest. There is a possible risk of language bias given that we only included studies written in English.
Agreements and disagreements with other studies or reviews
There are no other systematic reviews examining only RCT and quasi‐RCT evidence for efficacy of treatment for CAS.
Authors' conclusions
Implications for practice.
The present review concluded that there is only one RCT examining interventions for CAS in the literature to date, which requires replication. This study provides some evidence that the NDP‐3 may improve the accuracy of production on treated items (words) and connected speech, but limited evidence that the NDP‐3 improves speech production consistency or that the ReST improves accuracy of production on non‐treated words. The study did not measure functional communication.
There are a range of further therapies reported in the literature (Table 2), but the effectiveness of these interventions has not been rigorously examined; that is, other existing studies involve case study or case‐series investigations and not RCTs, limiting the ability to interpret and generalise findings to a broader population of children with CAS. At present the evidence supports the use of NDP‐3 or ReST intervention programmes for children with idiopathic CAS, aged 4 to 12 years, without other co‐occurring neurodevelopmental deficits. Further well‐controlled studies investigating the effectiveness of other treatments for CAS are urgently needed. There is a substantial range of treatments available for CAS; however, these require comparison with each other and to a no treatment (e.g. wait‐list control) group before their efficacy is rigorously demonstrated. Further trials are also needed that examine the efficacy of therapies for children with CAS with a range of co‐occurring neurodevelopmental impairments or diagnoses.
Implications for research.
There is a critical need for further rigorously controlled studies of treatment efficacy for CAS. Replication of the work by Murray 2015 is required. Further work should also rigorously examine other CAS treatments reported in the literature. RCTs and quasi‐RCTs are difficult to conduct given the heterogeneity of presentation of individuals with CAS, and due to the low incidence of the disorder. However, the work of Murray 2015 shows RCTs are possible.
Future studies may also investigate further therapy implementation variables to increase our understanding of treatment response in this population, in particular considering dose, delivery, uptake and context, with examples given below.
Duration, dose, delivery, uptake and intensity of treatment (e.g. intervention once a week over 12 weeks or three sessions over five weeks)
Response of particular subgroups of participants to treatment (e.g. subgroups based on age, genetic diagnosis, specific speech symptomatology), or dependent upon similarity of co‐occurring features (e.g. gross and fine motor or cognitive presentation)
Impact of timing of treatment (e.g. intervention at three years versus six years)
Effect of the administrator of treatment (e.g. clinician, parent, teacher's aide or even participant‐administered therapy for older children)
What's new
Date | Event | Description |
---|---|---|
29 August 2019 | Amended | Duplicate paragraph removed from the 'Main results' section of the Abstract and replaced with study results. |
History
Protocol first published: Issue 4, 2006 Review first published: Issue 3, 2008
Date | Event | Description |
---|---|---|
29 August 2017 | New search has been performed | The review was updated following a new search on 6 April 2017. |
29 August 2017 | New citation required and conclusions have changed | One new study included in review. |
4 September 2015 | Amended | Duplicate paragraph removed from the description of the intervention and reference error corrected in background section. |
13 May 2008 | Amended | Converted to new review format. |
12 May 2008 | Amended | Change of title from protocol stage ('developmental apraxia of speech') to 'childhood apraxia of speech' |
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to the current Cochrane Developmental, Psychosocial and Learning Problems group (CDPLPG: Joanne Wilson, Margaret Anderson and Geraldine Macdonald) for their support and guidance throughout the review process. Thank you also to the external reviewers. This work was supported by Victorian State Government Operational Infrastructure Support. The National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) of Australia provided further support to Angela Morgan (AM) in the form of a Career Development Fellowship (#607315) and Practitioner Fellowship (#1105008). An NHMRC Centre of Research Excellence in Speech and Language Neurobiology grant was awarded to AM and Frederique Liégeois (FL) (# 1116976), and an NHMRC Project grant was awarded to AM and FL (#1127144). FL's research is supported by the National Institute for Health Research Biomedical Research Centre at Great Ormond Street Hospital for Children NHS Foundation Trust and University College London (UK).
Disclaimer: the authors alone are responsible for the views expressed in this publication and they do not necessarily represent the official position, decisions, policy or views of the NHMRC or NHS.
Appendices
Appendix 1. Search strategies 2007 onwards
Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL), in the Cochrane Library, and which includes the Cochrane Developmental, Psychosocial and Learning Problems Specialised Register
Searched 6 April 2017 (172 records) Searched 6 June 2014 (103 records) Searched 4 August 2011 (62 records)
1MeSH descriptor: [Apraxias] explode all trees #2MeSH descriptor: [Speech Disorders] this term only #3dysprax* #4aprax* #5prax* #6(speech near/3 disorder*) #7(speech near/3 impair*) #8(speech near/3 problem*) #9(speech near/3 difficult*) #10voice near/3 disorder* #11voice near/3 impair* #12voice near/3 problem* #13voice near/3 difficult* #14vocal near/3 disorder* #15vocal near/3 impair* #16vocal near/3 problem* #17vocal near/3 difficult* #18communication near/3 disorder* #19communication near/3 impair* #20communication near/3 problem* #21communication near/3 difficult* #22#1 or #2 or #3 or #4 or #5 or #6 or #7 or #8 or #9 or #10 or #11 or #12 or #13 or #14 or #15 or #16 or #17 or #18 or #19 or #20 or #21 #23MeSH descriptor: [Adolescent] this term only #24MeSH descriptor: [Child] 1 tree(s) exploded #25(child* or girl* or boy* or pre next school* or pre‐school*) #26#23 or #24 or #25 #27#22 and #26 in Trials
MEDLINE Ovid
Searched 6 April 2017 (960 records) Searched 6 June 2014 (896 records) Searched 4 August 2011 (759 records)
1 exp Apraxias/ 2 Speech disorders/ 3 dysprax$.tw. 4 aprax$.tw. 5 prax$.tw. 6 (speech adj3 (disorder$ or impair$ or problem$ or difficult$)).tw. 7 ((voice or vocal) adj3 (disorder$ or impair$ or problem$ or difficult$)).tw. 8 (communication adj3 (disorder$ or impair$ or problem$ or difficult$)).tw. 9 or/1‐8 10 adolescent/ 11 exp Child/ 12 (adolescen$ or child$ or girl$ or boy$ or pre school$ or pre‐school$ or teen$).tw. 13 or/10‐12 14 speech therapy/ 15 language therapy/ 16 (therap$ or train$ or measur$ or assess$ or habilitat$ or rehabilitat$ or manage$ or assist$ or treat$ or remedia$ or augment$ or recover$ or intervent$).tw. 17 or/14‐16 18 9 and 13 and 17 19 limit 18 to yr="2007 ‐Current"20 limit 18 to ed=20110401‐20140529 21 limit 18 to ed=20140501‐20170324
MEDLINE Epub Ahead of Print Ovid
Searched 6 April 2017 (10 records)
1 dysprax$.tw. 2 aprax$.tw. 3 prax$.tw. 4 1 or 2 or 3 5 (speech$ or language$).tw. 6 4 and 5 7 (child$ or boy$ or girl$ or preschool$ or preschool$ or teen$ or adolesc$).tw. 8 6 and 7
MEDLINE In‐Process and Other Non‐Indexed Citations Ovid
Searched 6 April 2017 (30 records)
1 dysprax$.tw. 2 aprax$.tw. 3 prax$.tw. 4 1 or 2 or 3 5 (speech$ or language$).tw. 6 4 and 5 7 (child$ or boy$ or girl$ or preschool$ or preschool$ or teen$ or adolesc$).tw. 8 6 and 7
Embase Ovid
Searched 10 April 2017 (1237 records) Searched 6 June 2014 (1356 records) Searched 4 August 2011 (1011 records)
1 exp Apraxias/ 2 "apraxia of speech"/ 3 dysprax$.tw. 4 aprax$.tw. 5 prax$.tw. 6 (speech adj3 (disorder$ or impair$ or problem$ or difficult$)).tw. 7 ((voice or vocal) adj3 (disorder$ or impair$ or problem$ or difficult$)).tw. . 8 (communication adj3 (disorder$ or impair$ or problem$ or difficult$)).tw. 9 or/1‐8 10 adolescent/ 11 child/ or preschool child/ 12 (adolescen$ or child$ or girl$ or boy$ or pre school$ or pre‐school$ or teen$).tw. 13 or/10‐12 14 speech rehabilitation/ 15 speech therapy/ 16 (therap$ or train$ or manage$ or assist$ or measure$ or treat$ or assess$ or remedia$ or augment$ or recover$ or intervent$).tw. 17 or/14‐16 18 9 and 13 and 17 19 limit 18 to yr="2007 ‐Current" 20 limit 18 to yr="2011 ‐Current" 21 limit 18 to yr="2014 ‐Current"
CINAHL Plus EBSCOhost (Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature)
Searched 10 April 2017 (376 records) Searched 6 June 2014 (571 records) Searched 4 August 2011 (866 records)
S23 S17 AND S22 S22 EM 20140601‐ S21 S17 AND S20 S20 EM 20110401‐ S19 S17 and S18 S18 EM >=20070101 S17 S13 and S16 S16 S14 or S15 S15 (MH "Rehabilitation, Speech and Language") OR (MH "Speech Therapy+") OR (MH "Language Therapy") OR (MH "Voice Therapy") S14 (therap* or train* or rehabilitat* or manage* or assist* or measure* or treat* or assess* or remedia* or augment* or recover* or intervent*) S13 S9 and S12 S12 S10 or S11 S11 child* or girl* or boy* or pre school* or pre‐school* S10 (MH "Child") OR (MH "Child, Preschool") S9 S1 or S2 or S3 or S4 or S5 or S6 or S7 or S8 S8 (communication N3 disorder*) or (communication N3 impair*) or (communication N3 problem*) or (communication N3 difficult*) S7 (vocal N3 disorder*) or (vocal N3 impair*) or (vocal N3 problem*) or (vocal N3 difficult*) S6 (voice N3 disorder*) or (voice N3 impair*) or (voice N3 problem*) or (voice N3 difficult*) S5 (speech N3 disorder*) or (speech N3 impair) or (speech N3 problem*) or (speech N3 difficult*) S4 prax* S3 aprax* S2 dysprax* S1 (MH "Apraxia+")
PsycINFO Ovid
Searched 10 April 2017 (600 records) Searched 6 June 2014 (902 records)
1 apraxia/ 2 speech disorders/ 3 dysprax$.tw. 4 aprax$.tw. 5 prax$.tw. 6 (speech adj3 (disorder$ or impair$ or problem$ or difficult$)).tw. 7 ((voice or vocal) adj3 (disorder$ or impair$ or problem$ or difficult$)).tw. 8 (communication adj3 (disorder$ or impair$ or problem$ or difficult$)).tw. 9 or/1‐8 10 (adolescen$ or child$ or girl$ or boy$ or pre school$ or pre‐school$ or teen$).tw. 11 (adolescence 13 17 yrs or childhood birth 12 yrs or preschool age 2 5 yrs or school age 6 12 yrs).ag. 12 10 or 11 13 Speech Therapy/ 14 Language Therapy/ 15 Speech Language Pathology/ 16 intervention/ 17 Rehabilitation/ 18 (therap$ or train$ or measur$ or assess$ or rehabilitat$ or manage$ or assist$ or treat$ or remedia$ or augment$ or recover$ or intervent$).tw. 19 or/13‐18 20 9 and 12 and 19
PsycINFO EBSCOhost
Searched 4 August 2011 (2409 records)
S31 S11 and S15 and S30 S30 S17 or S18 or S19 or S20 or S21 or S22 or S23 or S24 or S25 or S26 or S27 or S28 or S29 S29 (evaluation N3 stud* or evaluation N3 research*) S28 (effectiveness N3 stud* or effectiveness N3 research*) S27 DE "Placebo" or DE "Evaluation" or DE "Program Evaluation" OR DE "Educational Program Evaluation" OR DE "Mental Health Program Evaluation" S26 (DE "Random Sampling" or DE "Clinical Trials") or (DE "Experiment Controls") S25 "cross over*" S24 crossover* S23 (tripl* N3 mask*) or (tripl* N3 blind*) S22 (trebl* N3 mask*) or (trebl* N3 blind*) S21 (doubl* N3 mask*) or (doubl* N3 blind*) S20 (singl* N3 mask*) or (singl* N3 blind*) S S19 (clinic* N3 trial*) or (control* N3 trial*) S18 (random* N3 allocat* ) or (random* N3 assign*) S17 randomis* or randomiz* S16 S12 and S15 S15 S13 or S14 S14 AG childhood Limiters ‐ Age Groups: Childhood (birth‐12 yrs) S13 (child* or girl* or boy* or pre school* or pre‐school*) S12 S10 and S11 S11 S1 or S2 or S3 or S4 or S5 or S6 or S7 or S8 or S9 S10 therap* or train* or rehabilitat* or manage* or assist* or measure* or treat* or assess* or remedia* or augment* or recover* S S9 (communication N3 disorder* ) or (communication N3 impair* ) or (communication N3 problem*) or (communication N3 difficult* ) S8 (vocal N3 disorder* ) or (vocal N3 impair* ) or (vocal N3 problem*) or (vocal N3 difficult* ) S7 (voice N3 disorder* ) or (voice N3 impair* ) or (voice N3 problem*) or (voice N3 difficult* ) S6 (speech N3 disorder* ) or (speech N3 impair* ) or (speech N3 problem*) or (speech N3 difficult* ) S5 prax* S4 aprax* S3 dysprax* S2 DE "Speech Disorders" S1 DE "Apraxia"
ERIC EBSCOhost (Education Resources Information Center)
Searched 10 April 2017 (293 records)
S1 DE "Speech Impairments" OR DE "Articulation Impairments" OR DE "Voice Disorders" S2 verbal apraxia of speech S3 aprax* S4 dysprax* S5 prax* N10 speech* S6 (speech n3 disorder*) S7 (speech n3 impair*) S8 (speech n3 problem*) S9 (speech n3 difficult*) S10 voice n3 disorder* S11 voice n3 impair* S12 voice n3 problem* S13 voice n3 difficult* S14 vocal n3 disorder* S15 vocal n3 impair* S16 vocal n3 problem* S17 vocal n3 difficult* S18 communication n3 disorder* S19 communication n3 impair* S20 communication n3 problem* S21 ommunication n3 problem* [Note: Input error. Correct in line 20] S22 communication n3 difficult* S23 S1 OR S2 OR S3 OR S4 OR S5 OR S6 OR S7 OR S8 OR S9 OR S10 OR S11 OR S12 OR S13 OR S14 OR S15 OR S16 OR S17 OR S18 OR S19 OR S20 OR S21 OR S22 S24 DE "Speech Improvement" OR DE "Speech Therapy" S25 (therap* or train* or rehabilitat* or manage* or assist* or measure* or treat* or assess* or remedia* or augment* or recover* or rehab*) S26 S24 OR S25 S27 S23 AND S26 S28 DE "Adolescents" OR DE "Early Adolescents" OR DE "Late Adolescents" S29 DE "Children" OR DE "Preadolescents" OR DE "Young Children" S30 (adolescen* or child* or girl* or boy* or pre school* or pre‐school* or teen*) S31 S28 OR S29 OR S30 S32 S27 AND S31 S33 YR 2014‐ S34 S32 AND S33 S35 YR 2017‐ S36 S32 AND S35
ERIC Proquest
Searched 6 June 2014 limited to publication year =2011‐2014 (379 records) Searched 4 August 2011 limited to publication year =2007‐2011 (321 records)
"((( (APRAX$.TI,AB.) OR (DYSPRAX$.TI,AB.) OR (PRAX$.TI,AB.) OR (( SPEECH NEAR ( DISORDER$1 OR IMPAIR$4 OR PROBLEM$1 OR DIFFICULT$3 ) ) .TI,AB.) OR (( ( VOICE OR VOCAL ) NEAR ( DISORDER$1 OR IMPAIR$4 OR PROBLEM$1 OR DIFFICULT$3 ) ) .TI,AB.) OR (COMMUNICATION NEAR ( DISORDER$1 OR IMPAIR$4 OR PROBLEM$1 OR DIFFICULT$3 )) ) .TI,AB.) AND (( CHILD$3 OR GIRL$1 OR BOY$1 OR PRE ADJ SCHOOL$ OR ADOLESCEN$3 OR TEEN$5 ) .TI,AB.)) AND ((SPEECH‐THERAPY.DE.) OR (INTERVENTION#.W..DE.) OR (( THERAP$4 OR TRAIN$3 OR REHABILITAT$3 OR assess$5 OR measur$4 OR MANAGE$4 OR ASSIST$3 OR TREAT$5 OR REMEDIA$4 OR AUGMENT$2 OR RECOVER$1 OR INTERVENTION$1 ) .TI,AB.))
Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (CDSR), part of the Cochrane Library
Searched 10 April 2017 (5 records)
#1MeSH descriptor: [Apraxias] explode all trees #2MeSH descriptor: [Speech Disorders] this term only #3dysprax*:ti #4aprax*:ti #5prax*:ti #6(speech near/3 disorder*):ti,ab #7(speech near/3 impair*):ti,ab #8(speech near/3 problem*):ti,ab #9(speech near/3 difficult*):ti,ab #10{or #1‐#9} #11MeSH descriptor: [Adolescent] this term only #12MeSH descriptor: [Child] 1 tree(s) exploded #13(child* or girl* or boy* or pre next school* or pre‐school*):ti,ab #14#11 or #12 or #13 #15#10 and #14 in Cochrane Reviews (Reviews and Protocols)
Database of Reviews of Effect (DARE), part of the Cochrane Library
Searched 10 April 2017 (8 records)
#1MeSH descriptor: [Apraxias] explode all trees #2MeSH descriptor: [Speech Disorders] this term only #3dysprax*:ti #4aprax*:ti #5prax*:ti #6(speech near/3 disorder*):ti,ab #7(speech near/3 impair*):ti,ab #8(speech near/3 problem*):ti,ab #9(speech near/3 difficult*):ti,ab #10{or #1‐#9} #11MeSH descriptor: [Adolescent] this term only #12MeSH descriptor: [Child] 1 tree(s) exploded #13(child* or girl* or boy* or pre next school* or pre‐school*):ti,ab #14#11 or #12 or #13 #15#10 and #14 in Other Reviews
SpeechBITE (speechbite.com)
Searched 10 April 2017 (27 records)
Basic search: "childhood apraxia"
Advanced search:
Practice Area: Apraxia / Dyspraxia Research Design: Randomised Controlled Trial
Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry (ANZCR; anzctr.org.au/BasicSearch.aspx)
Searched 10 April 2017 [5 records] Searched 20 June 2014 [2 records]
Advanced search
speech AND apraxia limited to children
Chinese Clinical Trial Registry (ChiCTR; www.chictr.org.cn/index.aspx)
Searched 10 April 2017 (0 records)
(childhood apraxia of speech) or (dyspraxia) or (apraxia), (child) AND (speech)
ClinicalTrials.gov (clinicaltrials.gov)
Searched 10 April 2017 (3 records) Searched 20 June 2014 (12 records)
Condition: apraxia OR dyspraxia Limited to children 0‐17
EU Clinical Trials Register (clinicaltrialsregister.eu)
Searched 10 April 2017 (0 records)
(childhood apraxia of speech) or (dyspraxia) or (apraxia), (child) AND (speech)
ISRCTN Registry (www.isrctn.com)
Searched 10 April 2017 (0 records)
(childhood apraxia of speech) or (dyspraxia) or (apraxia), (child) AND (speech)
Nederlands Trial Register (www.trialregister.nl/trialreg/index.asp)
Searched 10 April 2017 (0 records)
(childhood apraxia of speech) or (dyspraxia) or (apraxia), (child) AND (speech)
World Health Organization International Clinical Trials Registry Platform (WHO ICTRP; apps.who.int/trialsearch)
Searched 10 April 2017 (8 records) Searched 20 June 2014 (35 records) Searched 10 August 2011 (1 record)
Basic search: apraxia OR dyspraxia. Limited to clinical trials in children
Appendix 2. Search strategies up to 2007
Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL), in the Cochrane Library, and which includes the Cochrane Developmental, Psychosocial and Learning Problems Specialised Register
Searched 2016, Issue 4
#1 MeSH descriptor Apraxias explode all trees #2 dysprax* #3 aprax* #4 prax* 1007 #5 (speech near/3 disorder*) #6 (speech near/3 impair*) #7 (speech near/3 problem*) #8 (speech near/3 difficult*) #9 voice near/3 disorder* #10 voice near/3 impair* #11 voice near/3 problem* #12 voice near/3 difficult* #13 vocal near/3 disorder* #14 vocal near/3 impair* #15 vocal near/3 problem* #16 vocal near/3 difficult* #17 communication near/3 disorder* #18 communication near/3 impair* #19 communication near/3 problem* #20 communication near/3 difficult* #21 (#1 OR #2 OR #3 OR #4 OR #5 OR #6 OR #7 OR #8 OR #9 OR #10 OR #11 OR #12 OR #13 OR #14OR #15 OR #16 OR #17 OR #18 OR #19 OR #20) #22 (therap* or train* or rehabilitat* or manage* or assist* or measure* or treat* or assess* or remedia* or augment* or recover* or rehab*) #23 child near "MESH check words" #24 (child* or girl* or boy* or pre school* or pre‐school*) #25 (#23 OR #24) #26 (#21 AND #22 AND #25)
MEDLINE Ovid
Searched 1966 to January 2007
1 exp Apraxias/ 2 dysprax$.tw. 3 aprax$.tw. 4 prax$.tw. 5 (speech adj3 (disorder$ or impair$ or problem$ or difficult$)).tw. 6 ((voice or vocal) adj3 (disorder$ or impair$ or problem$ or difficult$)).tw. 7 (communication adj3 (disorder$ or impair$ or problem$ or difficult$)).tw. 8 or/1‐7 9 (therap$ or train$ or rehabilitat$ or manage$ assist$ or measure$ or treat$ or assess$ or remedia$ or augment$ or recover$ or rehab$).tw. 10 8 and 9 11 Child/ 12 (child$ or girl$ or boy$ or pre school$ or pre‐school$).tw. 13 or/11‐12 14 8 and 10 and 13 15 randomized controlled trial.pt. 16 controlled clinical trial.pt. 17 randomized controlled trials.sh. 18 random allocation.sh. 19 double blind method.sh. 20 single‐blind method.sh. 21 or/15‐20 22 (animals not human).sh. 23 21 not 22 (362564) 24 clinical trial.pt. 25 exp Clinical Trials/ 26 (clin$ adj25 trial$).ti,ab. 27 ((singl$ or doubl$ or trebl$ or tripl$) adj25 (blind$ or mask$)).ti,ab. 28 placebos.sh. 29 placebo$.ti,ab. 30 random$.ti,ab. 31 research design.sh. 32 or/24‐31 33 32 not 22 34 33 not 23 35 comparative study.sh. 36 exp Evaluation Studies/ 37 follow up studies.sh. 38 prospective studies.sh. 39 (control$ or prospectiv$ or volunteer$).ti,ab. 40 or/35‐39 41 40 not 22 42 41 not (23 or 34) 43 23 or 34 or 42 44 14 and 43
Embase Ovid
Searched 1980 to January 2007
1 exp Apraxias/ 2 dysprax$.tw. 3 aprax$.tw. 4 prax$.tw. 5 (speech adj3 (disorder$ or impair$ or problem$ or difficult$)).tw. 6 ((voice or vocal) adj3 (disorder$ or impair$ or problem$ or difficult$)).tw. 7 (communication adj3 (disorder$ or impair$ or problem$ or difficult$)).tw. 8 or/1‐7 9 (therap$ or train$ or rehabilitat$ or manage$ assist$ or measure$ or treat$ or assess$ or remedia$ or augment$ or recover$ or rehab$).tw. 10 Child/ 11 (child$ or girl$ or boy$ or pre school$ or pre‐school$).tw. 12 or/10‐11 13 clin$.tw. 14 trial$.tw. 15 (clin$ adj3 trial$).tw. 16 singl$.tw. 17 doubl$.tw. 18 trebl$.tw. 19 tripl$.tw. 20 blind$.tw. 21 mask$.tw. 22 ((singl$ or doubl$ or trebl$ or tripl$) adj3 (blind$ or mask$)).tw. 23 randomi$.tw. 24 random$.tw. 25 allocat$.tw. 26 assign$.tw. 27 (random$ adj3 (allocat$ or assign$)).tw. 28 crossover.tw. 29 28 or 27 or 23 or 22 or 15 30 exp Randomized Controlled Trial/ 31 exp Double Blind Procedure/ 32 exp Crossover Procedure/ 33 exp Single Blind Procedure/ 34 exp RANDOMIZATION/ 35 30 or 31 or 32 or 33 or 34 or 29 36 8 and 9 and 12 and 35
CINAHL Ovid
Searched 1982 to December 2006
1 exp Apraxias/ 2 dysprax$.tw. 3 aprax$.tw. 4 prax$.tw. 5 (speech adj3 (disorder$ or impair$ or problem$ or difficult$)).tw. 6 ((voice or vocal) adj3 (disorder$ or impair$ or problem$ or difficult$)).tw. 7 (communication adj3 (disorder$ or impair$ or problem$ or difficult$)).tw. 8 or/1‐7 9 (therap$ or train$ or rehabilitat$ or manage$ assist$ or measure$ or treat$ or assess$ or remedia$ or augment$ or recover$ or rehab$).tw. 10 Child/ 11 (child$ or girl$ or boy$ or pre school$ or pre‐school$).tw. 12 or/10‐11 13 randomi$.mp. [mp=title, subject heading word, abstract, instrumentation] 14 clin$.mp. [mp=title, subject heading word, abstract, instrumentation] 15 trial$.mp. [mp=title, subject heading word, abstract, instrumentation] 16 (clin$ adj3 trial$).mp. [mp=title, subject heading word, abstract, instrumentation] 17 singl$.mp. [mp=title, subject heading word, abstract, instrumentation] 18 doubl$.mp. [mp=title, subject heading word, abstract, instrumentation] 19 tripl$.mp. [mp=title, subject heading word, abstract, instrumentation] 20 trebl$.mp. [mp=title, subject heading word, abstract, instrumentation] 21 mask$.mp. [mp=title, subject heading word, abstract, instrumentation] 22 blind$.mp. [mp=title, subject heading word, abstract, instrumentation] 23 (17 or 18 or 19 or 20) and (21 or 22) 24 crossover.mp. [mp=title, subject heading word, abstract, instrumentation] 25 random$.mp. [mp=title, subject heading word, abstract, instrumentation] 26 allocate$.mp. [mp=title, subject heading word, abstract, instrumentation] 27 assign$.mp. [mp=title, subject heading word, abstract, instrumentation] 28 (random$ adj3 (allocate$ or assign$)).mp. 29 Random Assignment/ 30 exp Clinical Trials/ 31 exp Meta Analysis/ 32 28 or 24 or 23 or 16 or 13 or 29 or 30 or 31 33 8 and 9 and 12 and 32
PsycINFO SilverPlatter
Searched up to January 2007
#28 (((trial*) in TI) or ((randomly) in AB) or ((placebo) in AB) or ((randomized or randomised) in AB) or ("Clinical‐Trials" in MJ,MN)) and ((child* or girl* or boy* or pre school* or pre‐school*) and ((therap* or train* or rehabilitat* or manage* or assist* or measure* or treat* or assess* or remedia* or augment* or recover*) and ((communication near 3 difficult*) or (communication near 3 problem*) or (communication near 3 impair*) or (communication near 3 disorder*) or ((voice or vocal) near 3 (difficult*)) or ((voice or vocal) near 3 (problem*)) or ((voice or vocal) near 3 (impair*)) or ((voice or vocal) near 3 (disorder*)) or (speech near 3 difficult*) or (speech near 3 problem*) or (speech near 3 impair*) or (speech near 3 disorder*) or (prax*) or (aprax*) or (dysprax*) or ("Apraxia‐" in MJ,MN))))
ERIC Dialog Datastar (Education Resources Information Center)
Searched 1966 to January 2007
1 APRAX$.TI,AB. 2 DYSPRAX$.TI,AB. 3 PRAX$.TI,AB. 4 (SPEECH NEAR (DISORDER$ OR IMPAIR$ OR PROBLEM$ OR DIFFICULT$)).TI,AB. 5 ((VOICE OR VOCAL) NEAR (DISORDER$ OR IMPAIR$ OR PROBLEM$ OR DIFFICULT$)).TI,AB. 6 (COMMUNICATION NEAR (DISORDER$ OR IMPAIR$ OR PROBLEM$ OR DIFFICULT$)).TI,AB. 7 (1 OR 2 OR 3 OR 4 OR 5 OR 6).TI,AB. 8 (THERAP$ OR TRAIN$ OR REHABILITAT$ OR MANAGE$ OR ASSIST$ OR MEASURE$ OR TREAT$ OR ASSESS$OR REMEDIA$ OR AUGMENT$ ADJ RECOVER$).TI,AB. 9 (CHILD$ OR GIRL$ OR BOY$ OR PRE ADJ SCHOOL$ OR PRE‐SCHOOL$).TI,AB. 10 7.TI,AB. AND 8.TI,AB. AND 9.TI,AB. 11 (RANDOMISED OR RANDOMIZED).AB. 12 PLACEBO.AB. 13 RANDOMLY.AB. 14 TRIAL$.TI,AB. 15 11 OR 12 OR 13 OR 14 16 10 AND 15
Linguistics Abstracts Online
Searched 1985 to January 2007
Terms used:
dyspraxia AND child or children
OR
apraxia AND child or children
Appendix 3. Methods for future updates
Electronic searches
We will include non‐English language abstracts in any future updates of this review.
Measures of treatment effects
Binary data
We will analyse binary outcomes by calculating the risk ratio (RR) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs). Wherever necessary, we will contact original study authors for raw data.
Continuous data
To enable the combination of studies measuring the same outcome using different methods, we will report standardised mean difference (SMD) effect sizes with 95% CIs. For studies measuring the same outcome using the same measure, we will report mean difference (MD) effect sizes with 95% CIs. Wherever necessary, we will contact original study authors for raw data (e.g. where authors have only reported change from baseline data). We will transform and include skewed data where appropriate.
Unit‐of‐analysis issues
In future reviews, we will continue to consider the level at which randomisation occurred (i.e. in simple parallel‐group designs, as encountered in the included study here (Murray 2015), where participants were individually randomised to one of two intervention groups, and a measurement for each outcome from each participant was collected and analysed). However, if we encounter cluster‐randomised trials (i.e. where groups of individuals are randomised together to the same intervention), cross‐over trials or multiple observations of the same outcome (e.g. repeated measurements, recurring events. etc.), we will consult the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions for the latest recommendations on best management of unit‐of‐analysis issues (Higgins 2011b).
Dealing with missing data
If studies do not report intention‐to‐treat (ITT) analyses, we will contact the study authors and request the missing data. We will initially seek missing data via contact with the corresponding author. In regard to participant dropout, if the rate of attrition reaches a 30% threshold in an included study, we will conduct a sensitivity analysis and assess the impact of this attrition. If the impact is not significant, we will include the data. The maximum allowed difference in the dropout rate between the two groups that we will allow before we exclude an included study from a meta‐analysis is 10%.
Assessment of reporting biases
Where appropriate, we will use funnel plots to assess the possibility that study selection might be affected by bias, by investigating any relationship between effect size and study precision (closely related to sample size) (Morgan 2008). Such a relationship may be due to publication or related biases, to systematic differences between small and large studies, or to a statistical artefact of the chosen effect measure. We will use Egger's test to examine potential bias (Egger 1997).
Assessment of heterogeneity
We will estimate between‐study variance (τ²) using a random‐effects model and the inverse‐variance approach. We will use the random‐effects model because it is more conservative than the fixed‐effect model.
Data synthesis
We will only perform a meta‐analysis when studies employ similar interventions across the three intervention types (motor‐based, linguistic, multi‐modal communication). We will use a network meta‐analysis with a random‐effects model.
Characteristics of studies
Characteristics of included studies [ordered by study ID]
Murray 2015.
Methods | Parallel‐group randomised controlled trial | |
Participants |
Sample size: 26 children Dropouts/withdrawals: 1 child in the NDP‐3 group dropped out mid‐treatment yet was included in the analysis using intention‐to‐treat analysis Sex: 18 males, 8 females Mean age: 5 years and 6 months (SD = 25 months) Inclusion criteria
No information was collected on race, ethnicity or socioeconomic status |
|
Interventions |
Process
Participants were randomly assigned to 1 of the 2 treatments: ReST or NDP‐3. Concealed allocation was revealed after baseline assessment was completed. No significant differences between groups for any baseline variables (age, sex, primary or secondary outcome measures or CAS severity). Dose was controlled. Treatment was delivered for both ReST and NDP‐3 over 12 x 1‐hour sessions, scheduled 4 days/week for 3 weeks in school vacation time in January 2011 and January 2012, with a maximum of 10 participants per block. Treatments were provided as per intervention manuals and published protocol (Murray 2012). ReST sessions had an average of 100.4 production trials (SD = 0.9) and NDP‐3 had an average of 101.3 (SD = 1.2), with no significant difference in number of production trials between groups. Therapy was provided by student SLPs under the supervision of Murray and McCabe. Several days of training were provided for both treatments and in transcription and data collection until reaching inter‐rater reliability > 85%. Further detail on each treatment is provided below
|
|
Outcomes |
Timing of outcome assessment
Outcome assessments were conducted prior to treatment and within 1 week, 1 month and 4 months post‐treatment. No therapy was reported between study onset and 1 month post‐treatment yet over half the cohort resumed community SLP services between 1 and 4 months post‐treatment (ReST = 9, NDP‐3 = 9) Primary outcomes The primary outcomes included:
Outcomes were measured based on a 292‐item experimental probe of treated and untreated stimuli. 162 items from NDP‐3 assessment and 80 pseudo words from ReST treatment, and an additional 50 untreated 1‐, 2‐ and 3‐syllable real word stimuli were used to test for generalisation of treatment effects in both groups. The probe assessed impairment level speech outcomes for simultaneous accuracy for articulation and prosody. For further detail on scoring, see Murray 2015. Secondary outcomes A number of secondary measures of generalization were made to further explore potential differences in the treatments' effects
Comparisons 3 comparisons for each primary and secondary outcome measure were conducted
|
|
Notes |
Funding
Douglas and Lola Douglas Scholarship on Child and Adolescent Health; Speech Pathology Australia funded Nadia Verrall Memorial 2010 and Postgraduate Student Scholarship, James Kentley Memorial Scholarship, Postgraduate Research Support Schemes and Faculty of Health Sciences; University of Sydney International Development Program Fund; and Australian Research Council Future Fellowship Conflicts of interest: none known Study start date: January 2010 Study end date: July 2012 |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Clarification was sought from the corresponding author by phone who confirmed that each envelope had a note within it specifying the treatment condition to which the child was allocated (Murray 2015). The authors could not see through the envelopes. Envelopes were placed in a container and an independent person (corresponding author's husband) not involved in the study selected an envelope that was then given a participant number (P1, P2, etc.) until all participants were allocated to an arm of the study. Allocation was not revealed until after the pre‐treatment evaluation |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Clarification was sought from corresponding author (Murray 2015), who confirmed via email that envelopes were sequentially numbered based on the random order in which they were selected from a container (i.e. randomised and not based on any identifying variable). |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Low risk | SLP could not be blinded to type of intervention |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Blinded, independent assessors |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | No attrition |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Low risk | All outcome measures reported in the original protocol, Murray 2012, were reported. A lexical stress measure was added in final outcome ratings but not mentioned in protocol but this was an addition and not a failure to report |
Other bias | Unclear risk |
Qualified SLPs who had not seen the children previously conducted the 1 week, 1 month and 4 month post‐assessments. In some cases, final‐year undergraduate SLP students (4th‐year students) conducted post‐assessments. The same SLP or student SLP must not have seen/rated the children before. One researcher performed all of the pre‐assessments, including probes, before allocation was revealed |
CAS: childhood apraxia of speech;CELF‐IV: Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals ‐ Fourth Edition; CELF‐P2: Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals ‐ Preschool 2; DEAP: Diagnostic Evaluation of Articulation and Phonology; GFTA‐2: Goldman‐Fristoe Test of Articulation 2;NDP‐3: Nuffield Dyspraxia Programme ‐ Third Edition; ReST: Rapid Syllable Transitions Treatment; SD: standard deviation; SLP: speech language pathologist
Characteristics of excluded studies [ordered by study ID]
Study | Reason for exclusion |
---|---|
Baas 2008 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Ballard 2010 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT |
Beathard 2008 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Binger 2007 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study series) |
Binger 2008 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Binger 2011 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Bornman 2001 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Bose 2001 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study series) |
Carter 2004 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study series) |
Chappell 1973 | No experimental treatment data included in study |
Culp 1989 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (single case [ABA] design) |
Cumley 1999 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case series) |
Dale 2013 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case series) |
Daly 1972 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Dworkin 1988 | Study examined adult participant with AAOS |
Edeal 2011 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT |
Forrest 2001 | Study focuses on children with speech disorder, not specifically DAS. No experimental treatment data included in study |
Groenen 1996 | No experimental treatment data included in study |
Hadar 1984 | Study examined adult participant with AAOS |
Hall 1989 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Hall 1990 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (longitudinal case study) |
Harris 1996 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Hayden 2006 | Study uses a hypothetical treatment case only. No experimental treatment data |
Head 1975 | Study focuses on intervention for a group of participants with a range of speech disorders without dissociating between participants with subtypes of speech disorders. Does not report treatment efficacy specific to participants with DAS |
Helfrich‐Miller 1994 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study series) |
Iuzzini 2010 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Jaroma 1984 | Study does not specify whether child has diagnosis of DAS or only some features of dyspraxia |
Kadis 2014 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study series) |
Katz 2006 | Study examined adult participants with AAOS |
King 2013 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study series) |
Kingston 1987 | Study focused on articulation disorders, not specifically DAS |
Klick 1985 | No experimental treatment data included in study |
Krauss 1982 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Lagasse 2012 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Lozano 1978 | Study examined adult participant with AAOS |
Lundeborg 2007 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Lüke 2016 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Maas 2012a | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Maas 2012b | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Martikainen 2011 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT |
Martin 2016 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study series) |
McCabe 2014 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
McNeill 2009a | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case series) |
McNeill 2009b | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
McNeill 2010 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study series) |
Morgan Barry 1995 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study series) |
Moriarty 2006 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Namasivayam 2013 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study series) |
Namasivayam 2015 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (pre‐post group design) |
Preston 2013 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT |
Preston 2016 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Preston 2017 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Ray 2003 | Study examined adult participant with AAOS |
Richardson 2004 | Study focus on motor dyspraxia or developmental coordination disorder not apraxia of speech |
Rosenbek 1974 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Rosenthal 1994 | Study combined a number of treatment methods and grouped individuals. Could not determine individual participant outcomes related to specific treatment methods |
Skelton 2014 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Square 1994 | No experimental treatment data included in study |
Stokes 2010 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Strand 2000 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Strand 2006 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case series) |
Thomas 2014 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Thomas 2016 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Tierney 2016 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
Vashdi 2013 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT |
Vashdi 2014 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT |
Velleman 1994 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case series) |
Yoss 1974 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT |
Zaretsky 2010 | Not RCT or quasi‐RCT (case study) |
AAOS: acquired apraxia of speech. ABA: applied behaviour analysis DAS: developmental apraxia of speech. RCT: randomised controlled trial.
Differences between protocol and review
Changes between 2006 protocol and 2008 review
The title was changed from 'Intervention for developmental apraxia of speech' to 'Intervention for childhood apraxia of speech' to reflect current terminology (ASHA 2007).
Changes between 2006 protocol and 2017 review
Description of the intervention. We reclassified the types of interventions from 'perceptually‐based therapy' and 'instrumentally‐based biofeedback approaches' to 'motor‐based', 'linguistic‐based' and 'multi‐modal communication', to reflect more contemporaneous approaches in the field.
Criteria for considering studies for this review. We rewrote the inclusion criteria for studies to provide greater clarity around the specific types of interventions being targeted (i.e. interventions targeting speech and language); to specify that we would include studies comparing intervention to either no treatment (e.g. wait‐list) control as well as other interventions; and to specify that the CAS diagnosis had to have been made by an SLP/SLT
Types of outcome measures. We updated our outcome measures to reflect those used in current literature.
-
We increased the sensitivity of our search by adding additional search terms for the condition and intervention.
-
We added the following databases and trial registers to our electronic searches, to ensure our search was as comprehensive as possible:
Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews;
MEDLINE E‐Pub Ahead of Print and MEDLINE In‐Process and Other Non‐Indexed Citations, both of which are updated daily.
Database of Abstracts of Reviews of Effect (DARE); however, this was not searched in 2017, as DARE was last updated in 2015;
SpeechBITE;
Chinese Clinical Trial Registry (ChiCTR);
EU Clinical Trials Register;
ISRCTN Registry; and
Nederlands Trial Registry.
We did not search Linguistic Abstracts Online and Dissertation Abstracts because we judged these would not identify any unique studies not found in other databases.
Data collection and analysis. Some methodological sections involving meta‐analysis as reported in the original protocol, Morgan 2006, were not relevant in this review because only a single RCT was identified for inclusion. See Appendix 3 for further detail.
Dealing with missing data. Whilst not used in this version of the review, we have specified that in future updates of the review, if the rate of attrition reaches a 30% threshold in an included study, we will include the study in the review but not in the meta‐analysis. The maximum allowed difference in the dropout rate between the two groups will be 10% before a study included in the review is excluded from meta‐analysis. See Appendix 3.
Data synthesis > Summary of findings. We used the GRADE approach in this updated review to rate the quality of the evidence (Schünemann 2017). The GRADE system was not available when the original 2006 protocol (Morgan 2006), or 2008 review (Morgan 2008), were published.
Contributions of authors
Angela Morgan (AM; guarantor of the review), Frederique Liégeois (FL) and Elizabeth Murray (EM) contributed to drafts of the review. The authors developed the search strategy in concert with CDPLPG. AM and FL conducted study selection, study assessment, data extraction, data entry, and analysis. EM tabulated further detail on excluded studies in Table 2 and contributed to the Characteristics of included studies and Characteristics of excluded studies tables. AM and FL completed the first draft of the review. AM, FL and EM contributed to further drafts of the review. EM did not contribute to the study selection, risk of bias assessment, or extraction of data from this study due to potential for conflict of interest, given that EM was lead author of the included study.
Sources of support
Internal sources
None, Other.
External sources
-
National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC), Australia.
NHMRC Practitioner Fellowship (APP1105008) awarded to AM.
-
National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC), Australia.
NHMRC Centre of Research Excellence in Speech and Language Neurobiology (CRE‐SLANG) (APP1116976) awarded to AM and FL.
-
National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC), Australia.
NHMRC Project Grant (APP1127144) awarded to AM and FL.
Declarations of interest
Angela T Morgan (AM) — none known. Elizabeth Murray (EM) is an author of the included study, Murray 2015, and was not involved in selecting this study for inclusion, or extracting or reviewing data from this study. Study selection as well as data extraction and review was conducted by two independent authors — AM and FL. Frederique J Liégeois (FL) — none known.
Edited (no change to conclusions)
References
References to studies included in this review
Murray 2015 {published data only}
- Murray E, McCabe P, Ballard KJ. A randomized controlled trial for children with childhood apraxia of speech comparing rapid syllable transition treatment and the Nuffield Dyspraxia Programme ‐Third Edition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 2015;58(3):669‐86. [DOI: 10.1044/2015_JSLHR-S-13-0179; PUBMED: 25807891] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray E. (Faculty of Health Sciences, The University of Sydney, Lidcombe, Australia). [personal communication]. Conversation with: A Morgan (Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Melbourne, Australia) 13 August 2015.
References to studies excluded from this review
Baas 2008 {published data only}
- Baas BS, Strand EA, LM Elmer, Barbaresi WJ. Treatment of severe childhood apraxia of speech in a 12‐year‐old male with CHARGE association. Journal of Medical Speech‐language Pathology 2008;16(4):181‐90. [Google Scholar]
Ballard 2010 {published data only}
- Ballard KJ, Robin DA, McCabe P, McDonald J. A treatment for dysprosody in childhood apraxia of speech. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 2010;53(5):1227‐45. [DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0130); PUBMED: 20798323] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Beathard 2008 {published data only}
- Beathard B, Krout RE. A music therapy clinical case study of a girl with childhood apraxia of speech: finding Lily's voice. Arts in Psychotherapy 2008;35(2):107‐16. [DOI: 10.1016/j.aip.2008.01.004] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Binger 2007 {published data only}
- Binger C, Light J. The effect of aided AAC modeling on the expression of multi‐symbol messages by preschoolers who use AAC. Augmentative and Alternative Communication 2007;23(1):30‐43. [DOI: 10.1080/07434610600807470; PUBMED: 17364486] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Binger 2008 {published data only}
- Binger C, Light J. The morphology and syntax of individuals who use AAC: research review and implications for effective practice. Augmentative and Alternative Communications 2008;24(2):123‐38. [DOI: 10.1080/07434610701830587; PUBMED: 18465366] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Binger 2011 {published data only}
- Binger C, Maguire‐Marshall M, Kent‐Walsh J. Using aided AAC models, recasts, and contrastive targets to teach grammatical morphemes to children who use AAC. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 2011;54(1):160‐76. [DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0163); PUBMED: 20719874] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Bornman 2001 {published data only}
- Bornman J, Alant E, Meiring E. The use of a digital voice output device to facilitate language development in a child with developmental apraxia of speech: a case study. Disability and Rehabilitation 2001;23(14):623‐34. [PUBMED: 11697460] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Bose 2001 {published data only}
- Bose A, Square PA, Schlosser R, Lieshout P. Effects of PROMPT therapy on speech motor function in a person with aphasia and apraxia of speech. Aphasiology 2001;15(8):767‐85. [DOI: 10.1080/02687040143000186] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Carter 2004 {published data only}
- Carter P, Edwards S. EPG therapy for children with long‐standing speech disorders: predictions and outcomes. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 2004;18(6‐8):359‐72. [PUBMED: 15573477] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Chappell 1973 {published data only}
- Chappell GE. Childhood verbal apraxia and its treatment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 1973;38(3):362‐8. [PUBMED: 4721796] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Culp 1989 {published data only}
- Culp DM. Developmental apraxia and augmentative or alternative communication ‐ a case example. Augmentative and Alternative Communication 1989;5(1):27‐34. [DOI: 10.1080/07434618912331274936] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Cumley 1999 {published data only}
- Cumley GD, Swanson S. Augmentative and alternative communication options for children with developmental apraxia of speech: three case studies. Augmentative and Alternative Communication 1999;15(2):110‐25. [DOI: 10.1080/07434619912331278615] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Dale 2013 {published data only}
- Dale PS, Hayden DA. Treating speech subsystems in childhood apraxia of speech with tactual input: the PROMPT approach. American Journal of Speech‐language Pathology 2013;22(4):644‐61. [DOI: 10.1044/1058-0360(2013/12-0055); PUBMED: 23813194] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Daly 1972 {published data only}
- Daly DA, Cantrell RP, Cantrell ML, Aman LA. Structure speech therapy contingencies with an oral apraxic child. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 1972;37(1):22‐32. [DOI: 10.1044/jshd.3701.22] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Dworkin 1988 {published data only}
- Dworkin JP, Abkarian GG, John DF. Apraxia of speech: the effectiveness of a treatment regimen. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 1988;53(3):280‐94. [PUBMED: 3398481] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Edeal 2011 {published data only}
- Edeal DM, Gildersleeve‐Neumann CE. The importance of production frequency in therapy for childhood apraxia of speech. American Journal of Speech‐language Pathology 2011;20(2):95‐110. [DOI: 10.1044/1058-0360(2011/09-0005); PUBMED: 21330650] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Forrest 2001 {published data only}
- Forrest K, Elbert M. Treatment for phonologically disordered children with variable substitution patterns. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 2001;15(1‐2):41‐5. [DOI: 10.3109/02699200109167628; PUBMED: 21269096] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Groenen 1996 {published data only}
- Groenen P, Maassen B, Crul T, Thoonen G. The specific relation between perception and production errors for place of articulation in developmental apraxia of speech. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 1996;39(3):468‐82. [PUBMED: 8783127] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Hadar 1984 {published data only}
- Hadar U, Twiston‐Davies R, Steiner TJ, Rose FC. A psychomotor approach to improving speech by modulating suprasegmental control in motor dysphasia and articulatory apraxia. Advances in Neurology 1984;42:337‐51. [PUBMED: 6507181] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Hall 1989 {published data only}
- Hall PK. The occurrence of developmental apraxia of speech in a mild articulation disorder: a case study. Journal of Communication Disorders 1989;22(4):265‐76. [PUBMED: 2794108] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Hall 1990 {published data only}
- Hall PK, Hardy JC, LaVelle WE. A child with signs of developmental apraxia of speech with whom a palatal lift prosthesis was used to manage palatal dysfunction. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 1990;55(3):454‐60. [PUBMED: 2381187] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Harris 1996 {published data only}
- Harris L, Doyle ES, Haaf R. Language treatment approach for users of AAC: experimental single‐subject investigation. Augmentative and Alternative Communication (Baltimore, Md. : 1985) 1996;12(4):230‐43. [DOI: 10.1080/07434619612331277698] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Hayden 2006 {published data only}
- Hayden D. The PROMPT model: use and application for children with mixed phonological‐motor impairment. Advances in Speech Language Pathology 2006;8(3):265‐81. [DOI: 10.1080/14417040600861094] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Head 1975 {published data only}
- Head DG, Smith D. Speech remediation of children involved in two different physical education programs. Perceptual and Motor Skills 1975;40(1):261‐2. [DOI: 10.2466/pms.1975.40.1.261; PUBMED: 1118271] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Helfrich‐Miller 1994 {published data only}
- Helfrich‐Miller KR. A clinical perspective: melodic intonation therapy for developmental apraxia. Clinics in Communication Disorders 1994;4(3):175‐82. [PUBMED: 7994292] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Iuzzini 2010 {published data only}
- Iuzzini J, Forrest K. Evaluation of a combined treatment approach for childhood apraxia of speech. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 2010;24(4‐5):335‐45. [DOI: 10.3109/02699200903581083; PUBMED: 20345262] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Jaroma 1984 {published data only}
- Jaroma M, Danner P, Koivuniemi E. Sensory integrative therapy and speech therapy for improving the perceptual motor skills and speech articulation of a dyspractic boy. Folia Phoniatrica 1984;36(6):261‐6. [DOI: 10.1159/000265753] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Kadis 2014 {published data only}
- Kadis DS, Goshulak D, Namasivayam A, Pukonen M, Kroll R, Nil LF, et al. Cortical thickness in children receiving intensive therapy for idiopathic apraxia of speech. Brain Topography 2014;27(2):240‐7. [DOI: 10.1007/s10548-013-0308-8; PMC3921462; PUBMED: 23974724] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Katz 2006 {published data only}
- Katz WF, Bharadwaj SV, Stettler MP. Influences of electromagnetic articulography sensors on speech produced by healthy adults and individuals with aphasia and apraxia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 2006;49(3):645‐59. [DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/047); PUBMED: 16787902] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
King 2013 {published data only}
- King AM, Hengst JA, DeThorne LS. Severe speech sound disorders: an integrated multimodal intervention. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 2013;44(2):195‐210. [DOI: 10.1044/0161-1461(2012/12-0023); PUBMED: 23633644] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Kingston 1987 {published data only}
- Kingston LM, Rosenthal JB. Oral stereognosis in children with disordered articulation: measurement issues, and a treatment study. Australian Journal of Human Communication Disorders 1987;15(1):1‐14. [DOI: 10.3109/asl2.1987.15.issue-1.01] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Klick 1985 {published data only}
- Klick SL. Adapted cuing technique for use in treatment of dyspraxia. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 1985;16:256‐9. [DOI: 10.1044/0161-1461.1604.256] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Krauss 1982 {published data only}
- Krauss T, Galloway H. Melodic intonation therapy with language delayed apraxic children. Journal of Music Therapy 1982;19(2):102‐13. [DOI: 10.1093/jmt/19.2.102] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Lagasse 2012 {published data only}
- Lagasse B. Evaluation of melodic intonation therapy for developmental apraxia of speech. Music Therapy Perspectives 2012;30(1):49‐55. [DOI: 10.1093/mtp/30.1.49] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Lozano 1978 {published data only}
- Lozano RA, Dreyer DE. Some effects of delayed auditory feedback on dyspraxia of speech. Journal of Communication Disorders 1978;11(5):407‐15. [PUBMED: 730833] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Lüke 2016 {published data only}
- Lüke C. Impact of speech‐generating devices on the language development of a child with childhood apraxia of speech: a case study. Disability and Rehabilitation. Assistive Technology 2016;11(1):80‐8. [DOI: 10.3109/17483107.2014.913715; PUBMED: 24773213] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Lundeborg 2007 {published data only}
- Lundeborg I, McAllister A. Treatment with a combination of intra‐oral sensory stimulation and electropalatography in a child with severe developmental dyspraxia. Logopedics, Phoniatrics, Vocology 2007;32(2):71‐9. [DOI: 10.1080/14015430600852035; PUBMED: 17613788] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Maas 2012a {published data only}
- Maas E, Butalla CE, Farinella KA. Feedback frequency in treatment for childhood apraxia of speech. American Journal of Speech‐language Pathology 2012;21(3):239‐57. [DOI: 10.1044/1058-0360(2012/11-0119); PUBMED: 22442284] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Maas 2012b {published data only}
- Maas E, Farinella KA. Random versus blocked practice in treatment for childhood apraxia of speech. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 2012;55(2):561‐78. [DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2011/11-0120); PUBMED: 22207698] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Martikainen 2011 {published data only}
- Martikainen A‐L, Korpilahti P. Intervention for childhood apraxia of speech: a single‐case study. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 2011;27(1):9‐20. [DOI: 10.1177/0265659010369985] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Martin 2016 {published data only}
- Martin MK, Wright LE, Perry S, Cornett D, Schraeder M, Johnson JT. Children with developmental verbal dyspraxia: changes in articulation and perceived resilience with intensive multimodal intervention. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 2016;32(3):261‐75. [DOI: 10.1177/0265659015615924] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
McCabe 2014 {published data only}
- McCabe P, Macdonald‐D'Silva AG, Rees LJ, Ballard KJ, Arciuli J. Orthographically sensitive treatment for dysprosody in children with childhood apraxia of speech using ReST intervention. Developmental Neurorehabilitation 2014;17(2):137‐46. [DOI: 10.3109/17518423.2014.906002; PUBMED: 24694312] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
McNeill 2009a {published data only}
- McNeill BC, Gillon GT, Dodd B. Phonological awareness and early reading development in childhood apraxia of speech (CAS). International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders / Royal College of Speech & Language Therapists 2009;44(2):175‐92. [DOI: 10.1080/13682820801997353; PUBMED: 19234970] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
McNeill 2009b {published data only}
- McNeill BC, Gillon GT, Dodd B. A longitudinal case study of the effects of an integrated phonological awareness program for identical twin boys with childhood apraxia of speech (CAS). International Journal of Speech‐language Pathology 2009;11(6):482‐95. [DOI: 10.3109/17549500902842583; PUBMED: 21271925 ] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
McNeill 2010 {published data only}
- McNeill BC, Gillon GT, Dodd B. The longer term effects of an integrated phonological awareness intervention for children with childhood apraxia of speech. Asia Pacific Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing 2010;13(3):145‐61. [DOI: 10.1179/136132810805335074] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Morgan Barry 1995 {published data only}
- Morgan Barry R. The relationship between dysarthria and verbal dyspraxia in children: a comparative study using profiling and instrumental analyses. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 1995;9(4):277‐309. [DOI: 10.3109/02699209508985338] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Moriarty 2006 {published data only}
- Moriarty BC, Gillon GT. Phonological awareness intervention for children with childhood apraxia of speech. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders / Royal College of Speech & Language Therapists 2006;41(6):713‐34. [DOI: 10.1080/13682820600623960; PUBMED: 17079224] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Namasivayam 2013 {published data only}
- Namasivayam AK, Pukonen M, Goshulak D, Yu VY, Kadis DS, Kroll R, et al. Relationship between speech motor control and speech intelligibility in children with speech sound disorders. Journal of Communication Disorders 2013;46(3):264‐80. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2013.02.003; PUBMED: 23628222] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Namasivayam 2015 {published data only}
- Namasivayam A, Pukonen M, Hard J, Jahnke R, Kearney E, Kroll R, et al. Motor speech treatment protocol for developmental motor speech disorders. Developmental Neurorehabilitation 2015;18(5):296‐303. [DOI: 10.3109/17518423.2013.832431; PUBMED: 24088085] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Preston 2013 {published data only}
- Preston JL, Brick N, Landi N. Ultrasound biofeedback treatment for persisting childhood apraxia of speech. American Journal of Speech‐language Pathology 2013;22(4):627‐43. [DOI: 10.1044/1058-0360(2013/12-0139); PUBMED: 23813207] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Preston 2016 {published data only}
- Preston JL, Leece MC, Maas E. Intensive treatment with ultrasound visual feedback for speech sound errors in childhood apraxia. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 2016;10:440. [DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00440; PMC5003919; PUBMED: 27625603] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Preston 2017 {published data only}
- Preston JL, Leece MC, Maas E. Motor‐based treatment with and without ultrasound feedback for residual speech‐sound errors. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 2017;52(1):80‐94. [DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12259; PMC5156595; PUBMED: 27296780] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Ray 2003 {published data only}
- Ray J. Effects of orofacial myofunctional therapy on speech intelligibility in individuals with persistent articulatory impairments. International Journal of Orofacial Myology 2003;29:5‐14. [PUBMED: 14689652] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Richardson 2004 {published data only}
- Richardson AJ. Clinical trials of fatty acid treatment in ADHD, dyslexia, dyspraxia and the autistic spectrum. Prostaglandins, Leukotrienes, and Essential Fatty Acids 2004;70(4):383‐90. [DOI: 10.1016/j.plefa.2003.12.020; PUBMED: 15041031] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Rosenbek 1974 {published data only}
- Rosenbek J, Hansen R, Baughman CH, Lemme M. Treatment of developmental apraxia of speech: a case study. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 1974;5:13‐22. [DOI: 10.1044/0161-1461.0501.13] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Rosenthal 1994 {published data only}
- Rosenthal JB. Rate control therapy for developmental apraxia of speech. Clinics in Communication Disorders 1994;4(3):190‐200. [PUBMED: 7994294] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Skelton 2014 {published data only}
- Skelton SL, Hagopian AL. Using randomized variable practice in the treatment of childhood apraxia of speech. American Journal of Speech‐language Pathology 2014;23(4):599‐611. [DOI: 10.1044/2014_AJSLP-12-0169; MEDLINE: ] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Square 1994 {published data only}
- Square PA. Treatment approaches for developmental apraxia of speech. Clinics in Communication Disorders 1994;4(3):151‐61. [PUBMED: 7994290] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Stokes 2010 {published data only}
- Stokes SF, Griffiths R. The use of facilitative vowel contexts in the treatment of post‐alveolar fronting: a case study. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders / Royal College of Speech & Language Therapists 2010;45(3):368‐80. [DOI: 10.3109/13682820903094737; PUBMED: 20144008] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Strand 2000 {published data only}
- Strand EA, Debertine P. The efficacy of integral stimulation intervention with developmental apraxia of speech. Journal of Medical Speech‐language Pathology 2000;8(4):295‐300. [www.researchgate.net/publication/286964810_The_efficacy_of_integral_stimulation_intervention_with_developmental_apraxia_of_speech] [Google Scholar]
Strand 2006 {published data only}
- Strand EA, Stoeckel R, Baas B. Treatment of severe childhood apraxia of speech: a treatment efficacy study. Journal of Medical Speech‐language Pathology 2006;14(4):297‐307. [psycnet.apa.org/record/2006‐22884‐013] [Google Scholar]
Thomas 2014 {published data only}
- Thomas DC, McCabe P, Ballard KJ. Rapid Syllable Transitions (ReST) treatment for childhood apraxia of speech: the effect of lower dose‐frequency. Journal of Communication Disorders 2014;51:29‐42. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.06.004; PUBMED: 25052390] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Thomas 2016 {published data only}
- Thomas DC, McCabe P, Ballard KJ, Lincoln M. Telehealth delivery of Rapid Syllable Transitions (ReST) treatment for childhood apraxia of speech. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 2016;51(6):654‐71. [DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12238; PUBMED: 27161038] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Tierney 2016 {published data only}
- Tierney CD, Pitterle K, Kurtz M, Nakhla M, Todorow C. Bridging the gap between speech and language: using multimodal treatment in a child with apraxia. Pediatrics 2016;138(3):e20160007. [DOI: 10.1542/peds.2016-0007; PUBMED: 27492818] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Vashdi 2013 {published data only}
- Vashdi E. Using VML (verbal motor learning) method techniques in treatment of prosody disorder due to childhood apraxia of speech: a case study. International Journal of Child Health and Human Development 2013;6(2):255‐60. [www.novapublishers.com/catalog/product_info.php?products_id=52658] [Google Scholar]
Vashdi 2014 {published data only}
- Vashdi E. The influence of initial phoneme cue technique on word formation: a case study of a child with apraxia of speech and autism. International Journal of Child Health and Human Development 2014;7(2):197‐203. [www.novapublishers.com/catalog/product_info.php?products_id=53974] [Google Scholar]
Velleman 1994 {published data only}
- Velleman SL, Strand K. Developmental verbal dyspraxia. In: Bernthal JE, Bankson NW editor(s). Child Phonology: Characteristics, Assessment, and Intervention with Special Populations. New York (NY): Thieme, 1994:110‐39. [ISBN 0‐86577‐502‐8] [Google Scholar]
Yoss 1974 {published data only}
- Yoss KA, Darley FL. Developmental apraxia of speech in children with defective articulation. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 1974;17(3):399‐416. [PUBMED: 4421901] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Zaretsky 2010 {published data only}
- Zaretsky E, Velleman SL, Curro K. Through the magnifying glass: underlying literacy deficits and remediation potential in childhood apraxia of speech. International Journal of Speech‐language Pathology 2010;12(1):58‐68. [PUBMED: 20380250] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Additional references
ASHA 2007
- American Speech‐Language‐Hearing Association (ASHA). Technical Report. Childhood Apraxia of Speech: Ad Hoc Committee on Apraxia of Speech in Children. www.asha.org/policy/TR2007‐00278/ (accessed 20 April 2018).
Chumpelik 1984
- Chumpelik D. The PROMPT system of therapy: theoretical framework and applications for developmental apraxia of speech. Seminars in Speech and Language 1984;5(2):139‐56. [DOI: 10.1055/s-0028-1085172] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Crosbie 2005
- Crosbie S, Holm A, Dodd B. Intervention for children with severe speech disorder: a comparison of two approaches. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 2005;40(4):469‐71. [DOI: 10.1080/13682820500126049; PUBMED: 16195201] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Davis 2005
- Davis BL, Jacks A, Marquardt TP. Vowel patterns in developmental apraxia of speech: three longitudinal case studies. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 2005;19(4):249‐74. [DOI: 10.1080/02699200410001695367; PUBMED: 16019775] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Delaney 2004
- Delaney AL, Kent RD. Developmental profiles of children diagnosed with apraxia of speech. Annual Convention of the American Speech‐Language‐Hearing Association; 2004 Nov 18‐20; Philadelphia (PA). 2004.
Dodd 2006
- Dodd B, Hua Z, Crosbie S, Holm A, Ozanne A. DEAP: Diagnostic Evaluation of Articulation and Phonology. San Antonio (TX): PsychCorp of Harcourt Assessment, 2006. [Google Scholar]
Dodd 2008
- Dodd B, Crosbie S, McIntosh B, Holm A, Harvey C, Liddy M, et al. The impact of selecting difference contrasts in phonological therapy. International Journal of Speech‐Language Pathology 2008;10(5):334‐45. [DOI: 10.1080/14417040701732590; PUBMED: 20840033] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Eadie 2015
- Eadie P, Morgan A, Ukoumunne OC, Ttofari Eecen K, Wake M, Reilly S. Speech sound disorder at 4 years: prevalence, comorbidities, and predictors in a community cohort of children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 2015;57(6):578‐84. [DOI: 10.1111/dmcn.12635; PUBMED: 25403868] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Egger 1997
- Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta‐analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 1997;315(7109):629‐34. [DOI: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629; PMC2127453; PUBMED: 9310563] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Eising 2018
- Eising E, Carrion‐Castillo A, Vino A, Strand EA, Jakielski KJ, Scerri TS, et al. A set of regulatory genes co‐expressed in embryonic human brain is implicated in disrupted speech development. Molecular Psychiatry 2018 Feb 20 [Epub ahead of print]. [DOI: 10.1038/s41380-018-0020-x; PUBMED: 29463886] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Fedorenko 2016
- Fedorenko E, Morgan A, Murray E, Cardinaux A, Mei C, Tager‐Flusberg H, et al. A highly penetrant form of childhood apraxia of speech due to deletion of 16p11.2. European Journal of Human Genetics 2016;24(2):302‐6. [DOI: 10.1038/ejhg.2015.149; PMC4717199; PUBMED: 26173965] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Gillon 2000
- Gillon GT. The efficacy of phonological awareness intervention for children with spoken language impairment. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools 2000;31(2):126‐41. [DOI: 10.1044/0161-1461.3102.126; PUBMED: 27764385] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Goldman 2000
- Goldman R, Fristoe M. Goldman‐Fristoe Test of Articulation‒2. 2nd Edition. Minneapolis (MN): Pearson Assessments, 2000. [Google Scholar]
Gozzard 2004
- Gozzard H, Baker E, McCabe P. Single Word Test of Polysyllables. Unpublished manuscript 2004.
GRADEpro GDT 2015 [Computer program]
- McMaster University (developed by Evidence Prime). GRADEpro GDT. Version accessed prior to 30 April 2018. Hamilton (ON): McMaster University (developed by Evidence Prime), 2015.
Higgins 2011a
- Higgins JP, Altman DG, Gøtzsche PC, Jüni P, Moher D, Oxman AD, et al. The Cochrane Collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ 2011;343:d5928. [DOI: 10.1136/bmj.d5928; PMC3196245; PUBMED: 22008217] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Higgins 2011b
- Higgins JPT, Deeks JJ, Altman DG, editor(s). Chapter 16: Special topics in statistics. In: Higgins JP, Green S, editor(s). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0 (updated March 2011). The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011. Available from handbook.cochrane.org.
Jacks 2006
Lai 2001
- Lai CS, Fisher SE, Hurst JA, Vargha‐Khadem F, Monaco AP. A forkhead‐domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature 2001;413(6855):519‐23. [DOI: 10.1038/35097076; PUBMED: 11586359] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Liégeois 2012
- Liégeois FJ, Morgan AT. Neural bases of childhood speech disorders: lateralization and plasticity for speech functions during development. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 2012;36(1):439‐58. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.07.011; PUBMED: 21827785] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Liégeois 2014
- Liégeois F, Mayes A, Morgan A. Neural correlates of developmental speech and language disorders: evidence from neuroimaging. Current Developmental Disorders Reports 2014;1(3):215‐27. [DOI: 10.1007/s40474-014-0019-1; PUBMED: PMC4104164] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Liégeois 2016
- Liégeois FJ, Hildebrand MS, Bonthrone A, Turner SJ, Scheffer IE, Bahlo M, et al. Early neuroimaging markers of FOXP2 intragenic deletion. Scientific Reports 2016;6(35192):1‐9. [DOI: 10.1038/srep35192; PMC5062117; PUBMED: 27734906] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Maas 2008
- Maas E, Robin DA, Austermann Hula SN, Freedman SE, Wulf G, Ballard KJ, et al. Principles of motor learning in treatment of motor speech disorders. American Journal of Speech‐language Pathology 2008;17(3):277‐98. [DOI: 10.1044/1058-0360(2008/025); PUBMED: 18663111] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Maas 2014
- Maas E, Gildersleeve‐Neumann CE, Jakielski KJ, Stoeckel R. Motor‐based intervention protocols in treatment of childhood apraxia of speech (CAS). Current Developmental Disorders Reports 2014;1(3):197‐206. [DOI: 10.1007/s40474-014-0016-4; PMC4192721; PUBMED: 25313348] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
McNeill 2009
- McNeill BC, Gillon GT, Dodd B. Effectiveness of an integrated phonological awareness approach for children with childhood apraxia of speech (CAS). Child Language Teaching and Therapy 2009;25(3):341‐66. [DOI: 10.1177/0265659009339823] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Mei 2017
- Mei C, Fedorenko E, Amor DJ, Boys A, Hoeflin C, Carew P, et al. Speech and language phenotype in 16p11.2 deletion. European Journal of Human Genetics 2017 (in press). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Moher 2009
- Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta‐analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Medicine 2009;6(7):e1000097. [DOI: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097; PMC2707599; PUBMED: 19621072] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Morgan 2017
- Morgan A, Fisher SE, Scheffer IE, Hildebrand M. FOXP2‐related speech and language disorders. 2016 Jun 23 [updated 2017 Feb 2]. In: Adam MP, Ardinger HH, Pagon RA, Wallace SE, Bean LJH, Stephens K, et al. editor(s). GeneReviews®. Seattle (WA): University of Washington, 1993‐2018. [www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK368474/?report=reader#_NBK368474_pubdet_] [Google Scholar]
Morgan 2018
- Morgan AT, Haaften L, Hulst K, Edley C, Mei C, Yang Tan T, et al. Early speech development in Koolen de Vries Syndrome limited by oral praxis and hypotonia. European Journal of Human Genetics 2018; Vol. 26, issue 1:75‐84. [DOI: 10.1038/s41431-017-0035-9; PUBMED: 29225339] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Morley 1954
- Morley ME, Court D, Miller H. Developmental dysarthria. British Medical Journal 1954;1(4852):8‐10. [PMC2093079] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Morley 1972
- Morley ME. The Development and Disorders of Speech in Childhood. Baltimore (MD): Williams & Wilkins Co., 1972. [ISBN 0443008957] [Google Scholar]
Murray 2012
- Murray E, McCabe P, Ballard KJ. A comparison of two treatments for childhood apraxia of speech: methods and treatment protocol for a parallel group randomised control trial. BMC Pediatrics 2012;12:112. [DOI: 10.1186/1471-2431-12-112; ACTRN12612000744853; PMC3441276; PUBMED: 22863021] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Peter 2017
- Peter B, Lancaster H, Vose C, Fares A, Schrauwen I, Huentelman M. Two unrelated children with overlapping 6q25.3 deletions, motor speech disorders, and language delays. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A 2017;173(10):2659‐69. [DOI: 10.1002/ajmg.a.38385; PUBMED: 28767196] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
RCSLT 2011
- Royal College of Speech and Language Therapists (RCSLT). Developmental verbal dyspraxia policy statement. www.rcslt.org/speech_and_language_therapy/rcslt_position_papers (accessed prior to 21 March 2018).
Review Manager 2014 [Computer program]
- Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration. Review Manager 5 (RevMan 5). Version 5.3. Copenhagen: Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration, 2014.
Schünemann 2017
- Schünemann HJ, Oxman AD, Higgins JPT, Vist GE, Glasziou P, Akl E, et al. Chapter 11: Completing ‘Summary of findings’ tables and grading the confidence in or quality of the evidence. In: Higgins JPT, Churchill R, Chandler J, Cumpston MS, editor(s). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions version 5.2.0 (updated June 2017). Cochrane, 2017. Available from www.training.cochrane.org/handbook.
Shriberg 2010
- Shriberg LD, Fourakis M, Hall SD, Karlsson HB, Lohmeier HL, McSweeny JL, et al. Extensions to the Speech Disorders Classification System (SDCS). Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 2010;24(10):795‐824. [DOI: 10.3109/02699206.2010.503006; PMC2941221; PUBMED: 20831378] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Shriberg 2011
- Shriberg LD, Potter NL, Strand EA. Prevalence and phenotype of childhood apraxia of speech in youth with galactosemia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 2011;54(2):487‐519. [DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/10-0068); PMC3070858; PUBMED: 20966389] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Turner 2015
- Turner SJ, Mayes AK, Verhoeven A, Mandelstam SA, Morgan AT, Scheffer IE. GRIN2A: an aptly named gene for speech dysfunction. Neurology 2015;84(6):586‐93. [DOI: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001228; PMC4335991; PUBMED: 25596506] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Vargha‐Khadem 2005
- Vargha‐Khadem F, Gadian DG, Copp A, Mishkin M. FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience 2005;6(2):131‐8. [DOI: 10.1038/nrn1605; PUBMED: 15685218] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Velleman 2011
- Velleman SL, Mervis CB. Children with 7q11.23 Duplication Syndrome: speech, language, cognitive, and behavioral characteristics and their implications for intervention. Perspectives on Language Learning and Education 2011;18(3):108‐16. [DOI: 10.1044/lle18.3.108; PMC3383616; PUBMED: 22754604] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
White 2010
- White SM, Morgan A, Costa A, Lacombe D, Knight SJ, Houlston R, et al. The phenotype of Floating‐Harbor syndrome in 10 patients. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A 2010;152A(4):821‐9. [DOI: 10.1002/ajmg.a.33294; PUBMED: 20358590] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Williams 2004
- Williams P, Stephens H, editor(s). The Nuffield Centre Dyspraxia Programme. 3rd Edition. London (UK): The Nuffield Centre Dyspraxia Programme Ltd., 2004. [www.ndp3.org] [Google Scholar]
Williams 2010
- Williams P, Stephens H. The Nuffield Centre Dyspraxia Programme. In: Williams AL, McLeod S, McAuley RJ editor(s). Interventions for Speech Sound Disorders in Children. Baltimore (MD): Brookes Publishing Company, 2010:159‐77. [Google Scholar]
Yoss 1975
- Yoss KA. Developmental apraxia of speech in children: familial patterns and behavioral characteristics. American Speech and Hearing Association North Central Regional Conference, 1975 May 9; Minneapolis (MN). 1975.
References to other published versions of this review
Morgan 2006
- Morgan A, Vogel A. Intervention for developmental apraxia of speech. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2006, Issue 4. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD006278] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Morgan 2008
- Morgan AT, Vogel AP. Intervention for childhood apraxia of speech. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2008, Issue 3. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD006278.pub2; PUBMED: 18646142] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]