Abstract
Recent studies suggest that planned and corrective actions are recalibrated during some forms of motor adaptation. However, corrective (also known as reactive) movements in human locomotion are thought to simply reflect sudden environmental changes independently from sensorimotor recalibration. Thus, we asked whether corrective responses can indicate the motor system’s adapted state following prolonged exposure to a novel walking situation inducing sensorimotor adaptation. We recorded electromyographic (EMG) signals bilaterally on 15 leg muscles before, during, and after split-belts walking (i.e., novel walking situation), in which the legs move at different speeds. We exploited the rapid temporal dynamics of corrective responses upon unexpected speed transitions to isolate them from the overall motor output. We found that corrective muscle activity was structurally different following short versus long exposures to split-belts walking. Only after a long exposure, removal of the novel environment elicited corrective muscle patterns that matched those expected in response to a perturbation opposite to the one originally experienced. This indicated that individuals who recalibrated their motor system adopted split-belts environment as their new “normal” and transitioning back to the original walking environment causes subjects to react as if it was novel to them. Interestingly, this learning declined with age, but steady state modulation of muscle activity during split-belts walking did not, suggesting potentially different neural mechanisms underlying these motor patterns. Taken together, our results show that corrective motor commands reflect the adapted state of the motor system, which is less flexible as we age.
Keywords: EMG, locomotion, motor learning, split-belt walking
Significance Statement
We showed that corrective muscle activity elicited by sudden environmental transitions are revealing of the underlying recalibration process during sensorimotor adaptation. Our finding provides an alternate metric for quantifying the motor system’s recalibration other than aftereffects or postadaptation errors, which might not be clearly defined in some tasks. Notably, our novel approach enabled the identification of subject-specific motor learning not discernible from conventional kinematic aftereffects. This approach also revealed age-related decline on sensorimotor adaptation in a post hoc analysis, suggesting that older populations may have limited potential to correct their movements through error-based protocols simply given their age. Further, our detailed electromyographic (EMG) characterization provides valuable normative data of muscle activity informing our understanding of the therapeutic effect of split-belt walking.
Introduction
Humans adapt and learn new movements through interactions with the world, but there have been limited efforts investigating the modulation of muscle activity underlying this motor adaptation. For example, in locomotion, there have been several studies characterizing changes in kinematics (Reisman et al., 2005), kinetics (Ogawa et al., 2014; Sombric et al., 2018), metabolic cost (Finley et al., 2013; Sánchez et al., 2017), and the perception of movements (Vazquez et al., 2015) that are retained following walking on a split-belts treadmill that moves peoples’ legs at different speeds. However, the studies on muscle activity (Raja et al., 2012; Maclellan et al., 2014; Ogawa et al., 2014) do not provide a detailed characterization of what is adapted in muscle space during and after the novel split-belts situation. It is important to study the adaptation of muscle activity because it provides a distinct and more accurate representation of adjustments in neural commands than conventional biomechanical metrics. Namely, muscle activity is more directly reflective of neural control commands, whereas kinematics and kinetics arise from interactions between those commands and the environment (Collins et al., 2005), and the higher dimension of muscles compared to joints allows distinct muscle coordination to produce the same movements (Bernstein, 1967). Therefore, muscle recordings might reveal adjustments of motor commands (Ranganathan and Scheidt, 2016) that are obscured by solely characterizing changes in kinematic or kinetic variables.
Muscle activity can be adapted by adjusting planned and corrective motor commands that contribute distinctively to motor adaptation. Planned motor commands are predictive in nature and rely on internal models of the body and the environment (Wolpert et al., 1998). Conversely, corrective motor commands arise during movement execution through a transformation of current sensory information into appropriate actions (Jordan and Rumelhart, 1992; Bhushan and Shadmehr, 1999). While adaptation of planned movements is slow because it requires repeatedly experiencing a systematic perturbation to update internal models, motor adjustments through corrective actions are rapidly generated immediately after movement disturbances. For example, corrective actions could be strategic adjustments in subsequent movements, such as re-aiming the reaching direction (Morehead et al., 2015; McDougle et al., 2016) or foot landing in walking (Matthis et al., 2017). Corrective commands also include coordinated motor patterns rapidly generated by feedback processes transforming somatosensory input into actions counteracting external disturbances while the movement is ongoing (Chvatal et al., 2011; Crevecoeur and Scott, 2013). Some studies presume that rapid changes in motor commands following environmental transitions in walking are merely reactive in nature, reflecting differences in environment-specific walking requirements and distinct from the predictive control processes that are susceptible to sensorimotor recalibration (Reisman et al., 2005; Morton and Bastian, 2006; Yokoyama et al., 2018). Conversely, studies on volitional motor control suggest that internal models are available to processes generating both planned and corrective motor commands (Wagner and Smith, 2008; Yousif and Diedrichsen, 2012). In this study, we aim to determine the extent to which corrective responses are indicative of sensorimotor adaptation in locomotion.
We hypothesized that the structure (i.e., motor patterns across multiple muscles) of corrective responses on removing a novel environment will indicate the motor system’s recalibration. Specifically, that a recalibrated system would adopt the novel situation as a new “reference” such that deviations from it would induce corrective responses of the same structure as those elicited by other novel situations. This is innovative because sensorimotor recalibration is conventionally measured with aftereffects, rather than corrective responses. We defined corrective responses as rapid changes in electromyographic (EMG) activity on transitioning between walking environments or conditions (e.g., legs moving at same speeds vs different speeds). We tested our hypothesis by analyzing postadaptation corrective responses, which are those elicited on transitioning back to the original walking condition following split-belt walking. We considered that these corrective responses would merely reflect changes in the environment if they were numerically opposite to those originally observed when transitioning into the split condition. Conversely, they would be indicative of the motor system’s adapted state if their structure resembled that of corrective responses to transitioning de novo to the opposite split environment (i.e., opposite speed difference). This is based on the observation that removal of an altered environment facilitates learning of an opposite environment (Herzfeld et al., 2014). We further validated our hypothesis by contrasting the structure of corrective responses following short and long durations of the split environment. We expected that the hypothesized structural changes postadaptation would be exclusively present after the long, but not the short exposure to split-belt walking. Lastly, we demonstrated the utility of our approach with a post hoc analysis characterizing age-related decline in sensorimotor recalibration.
Materials and Methods
Subjects
A group of 16 healthy subjects of ages ranging between 46 and 78 years old (61 ± 9.9 years old, 10 female) participated in the study (Table 1). We selected an age rage similar to that of stroke survivors (Mozaffarian et al., 2016) because split-belts walking could be exploited to improve the gait of this clinical population (Reisman et al., 2013; Lewek et al., 2018). Therefore, there is an interest in characterizing the possible limitations in the adaptation of motor patterns solely due to the individuals’ age. Their movements and muscle activity were recorded before, during, and after walking on a split-belt treadmill. Subject S01 was excluded from our analyses because this individual failed to follow the instructions during the split-belts walking period, which likely compromised this subject’s motor adaptation. All subjects provided written informed consent before participating in the study, which was approved by the Institutional Review Board at our institution, and was in accordance to the declaration of Helsinki.
Table 1.
ID | Sex | Age (years old) | Mid speed (m/s) |
---|---|---|---|
S01 | F | 46 | 0.94 |
S02 | F | 51 | 1.02 |
S03 | F | 65 | 1.08 |
S04 | F | 58 | 0.9 |
S05 | M | 57 | 1.04 |
S06 | M | 52 | 1.05 |
S07 | M | 78 | 0.66 |
S08 | F | 52 | 1.16 |
S09 | M | 68 | 0.85 |
S10 | F | 62 | 0.98 |
S11 | M | 75 | 1.11 |
S12 | M | 57 | 0.99 |
S13 | M | 52 | 1.16 |
S14 | M | 64 | 1.25 |
S15 | M | 74 | 1.11 |
S16 | M | 49 | 1.08 |
Avg. | 10M/6F | 60 ± 9.9 | 1.02 ± 0.14 |
Bottom values indicate mean ± SD.
Experimental design
We assessed the adaptation and de-adaptation of muscle activity through the split-belts walking adaptation protocol illustrated in Figure 1A. Throughout the experiment, subjects were exposed to three different combinations of belt speeds (walking environments) on a split-belt treadmill (Bertec Corporation). Two of these environments were tied-belts walking, in which both belts moved at the same speed, at both a “medium” (self-selected) and a “slow” (33% slower) speed. The third was split-belts walking in which one belt moved at the same slow speed and the other at a “fast” (33% faster than medium) speed, such that the fast belt was moving at twice the speed of the slow one. All subjects experienced the three environments as six different conditions presented in the following order: slow walking (50 strides, tied, slow speed), mid walking (50 strides, tied, medium speed), short exposure (10 strides, split), baseline (150 strides, tied, medium speed), long exposure (900 strides in three blocks of 300, split), and washout (600 strides in two blocks of 300 or a single 600 block, tied, medium speed). The treadmill was started at the beginning and fully stopped at the end of each block, and speeds did not change while each block was ongoing. We compared muscle activity throughout the protocol to that of baseline walking, which was used as a reference. Slow walking was used to quantify speed-dependent modulation of muscle activity during regular treadmill walking. Short and long exposure were the only conditions where subjects were exposed to split-belts walking. We selected two exposure durations to dissociate the effect of sensorimotor recalibration from that of sudden changes in walking environment. Namely, the short exposure was limited to 10 strides because this is not sufficient to induce sensorimotor recalibration of internal models of walking (Roemmich and Bastian, 2015), whereas the long exposure was designed to have 900 strides to ensure muscle activity had reached a new steady state. The long exposure and washout conditions were divided into blocks to minimize fatigue and subjects were instructed not to step between blocks to prevent de-adaptation due to unrecorded steps.
The medium walking speeds were determined in a self-selected way to ensure subjects from all ages could complete the entire protocol. The self-selected speed was obtained by first averaging each subject’s speed when walking over ground in a 50-m hallway during a 6-min walking test (Rikli and Jones, 1998) and then subtracting 0.35 m/s, which resulted in a comfortable walking speed on a treadmill based on pilot data in older adults (>65 years). This resulted in medium walking speeds of 0.72 ± 0.26 m/s (mean ± SD) across the population. Medium walking speeds for each individual can be found in Table 1. The slow speed was defined as 66.6% of the medium speed, and the fast speed as 133.3% of the same. In this way, the average belt-speed during split-belts walking matched that of baseline and washout, and the belt-speed ratio during split-belts walking was 2:1. When walking in the split-belts environment, the dominant leg (self-reported leg used to kick a ball) was always walking faster. Thus, we refer to the dominant leg as the fast leg and the non-dominant one as the slow leg throughout the text.
Safety measures were designed such that participants from older ages could complete the study. First, all subjects wore a harness that only provided weight support in the event of falling but not during walking or standing. Also, subjects were told a few seconds in advance that they could hold on to a handrail (directly located in front of them) whenever a condition or block started or finished. Subjects were encouraged to let go of the handrail as soon as they felt comfortable doing so to minimize the effect of external support on muscle recordings. Finally, we monitored subjects’ heart-rate continuously and blood-pressure during the rest breaks to prevent over exertion in any of the participants.
Data analysis
Acquisition and pre-processing
We collected EMG signals, kinematics, and kinetic data to characterize subjects’ behavior. Surface EMG signals from 15 muscles on each leg were recorded for all subjects (for full list and abbreviations, see Table 2) at 2000 Hz using a Delsys Trigno System (Delsys Inc.). Signals were highpass-filtered to remove undesired movement artifacts and then rectified. We used a second order Butterworth filter (dual-pass) with a cutoff frequency of 30 Hz, which resulted in 80-dB/decade attenuation and zero-lag (Merletti and Parker, 2004). Unlike other studies (Torres-Oviedo and Ting, 2007), we did not apply a subsequent lowpass filter following rectification as we did not require the EMG envelope for our analysis (see EMG Parameters). Kinematic data were collected at 100 Hz with a passive motion analysis system (Vicon Motion Systems). Movements were recorded by placing reflective markers bilaterally on bony landmarks at the ankle (i.e., lateral malleolus) and the hip (i.e., greater trochanter). Ground reaction forces were recorded with an instrumented split-belt treadmill (Bertec Corporation) and sampled at 1000 Hz. Forces along the axis of gravity (Fz) were used to determine when the foot landed (i.e., heel-strike: Fz > 10N) or was lifted off the ground (i.e., toe-off: Fz < 10N).
Table 2.
Muscle name | Abbreviation |
---|---|
Tibialis anterior | TA |
Peroneus longus | PER |
Medial gastrocnemius | MG |
Lateral gastrocnemius | LG |
Soleus | SOL |
Biceps femoris | BF |
Semitendinosus | SMT |
Semimembranosus | SMB |
Rectus femoris | RF |
Vastus lateralis | VL |
Vastus medialis | VM |
Ilipsoas and sartorius | HIP |
Adductor magnus | ADM |
Gluteus medius | GLU |
Tensor fasciae latae | TFL |
EMG parameters
EMG activity was time-aligned, binned, and amplitude-normalized to allow for comparisons across epochs, muscles, and subjects (Fig. 1B, middle column). First, filtered EMG activity was divided in intervals of the gait cycle aligned to gait events to focus on changes in muscle activity within the gait cycle, rather than on changes due to differences in timing of the gait cycle across the distinct walking conditions (Dietz et al., 1994; Reisman et al., 2005). More specifically, we divided the gait cycle of each leg into four intervals according to well defined gait phases (Perry and Burnfield, 2010): first double-support (DS; from ipsilateral heel-strike to contralateral toe-off), single-stance (SINGLE; from contralateral toe-off to contralateral heel-strike), second double-support (DS; from contralateral heel-strike to ipsilateral toe-off), and swing (SWING; from ipsilateral toe-off to ipsilateral heel-strike). to achieve better temporal resolution, each of these four intervals were further divided. The DS phases were divided into two equal subintervals, and the SWING and SINGLE phases divided into four equal sub-intervals, yielding 12 intervals for each gait cycle. The average duration of these sub-intervals ranged between 75 and 120 ms. Specific timing for each interval throughout the different epochs of the study are presented in Table 3. Muscle activity amplitude was averaged in time for each of these subintervals for every stride and muscle resulting in 360 muscle activity variables per subject per stride cycle: 12 subintervals × 30 muscles. These variables were then amplitude-normalized to be able to compare measurements across subjects and muscles. For each subject and muscle, we first computed the mean activity for each subinterval across the last 40 strides (i.e., steady state) of baseline walking. Then each muscle’s activity was scaled and shifted in magnitude such that the least and most active subintervals for that subject and muscle during those last 40 strides of baseline took the values of 0% and 100%, respectively. The same scaling and shifting was used for activity during all conditions of our experiment so that muscle activity is represented as a percentage of the maximum baseline activity. Because of this, all muscle activity variables used in this study are dimensionless. This scaling allowed us to aggregate subjects and compare effect sizes across muscles even when recorded EMG amplitudes were very different because of sensor placement or underlying tissue properties. We present in Figure 1B sample traces of time-aligned and amplitude-normalized EMG activity for one sample muscle averaged across gait cycles for late baseline and early long exposure (Fig. 1B, middle column). The gray rows indicate the results of the time-binning for two experimental epochs: late baseline and early long exposure. The rows colored in red and blue represent the difference between these two epochs. The left column of Figure 1B shows the resulting time-binned and amplitude-normalized activity for all muscles averaged across subjects and strides during late baseline. The right column of Figure 1B shows the difference in time-binned and amplitude-normalized activity between early long exposure and late baseline. These changes in muscle activity on the introduction of the split-belts perturbation () was used to characterize the structure of corrective responses following the short exposure and long exposure of the split-belts condition (as explained in detail below, Characterizing corrective responses to sudden changes in the environment). This coordinated pattern of corrective responses () to the novel walking environment was very similar to balance-like responses previously reported (Torres-Oviedo and Ting, 2007; Chvatal and Ting, 2012; Safavynia and Ting, 2012; de Kam et al., 2017). For example, the activity of the low leg during SINGLE initially showed increased activity of anterior muscles [TA, quads (RF, VM, and VL), and hip flexors (HIP, ADM, TFL)] and reduced the activity of calf muscles (MG, LG, SOL), as has been observed in balance responses when the body falls backward (Tang and Woollacott, 1999; Chvatal et al., 2011; Chvatal and Ting, 2012).
Table 3.
Epoch | DS1 [FHS-STO] (ms) | Swing SLOW (ms) | DS2 [SHS-FTO] (ms) | Swing FAST (ms) |
---|---|---|---|---|
Baseline | 168 ± 12 | 393 ± 59 | 166 ± 29 | 383 ± 43 |
Early long | 177 ± 46 | 314 ± 64 | 135 ± 34 | 458 ± 128 |
Late long | 176 ± 21 | 329 ± 38 | 161 ± 30 | 473 ± 114 |
Early washout | 150 ± 35 | 358 ± 91 | 188 ± 32 | 372 ± 69 |
Late washout | 187 ± 24 | 405 ± 42 | 181 ± 33 | 394 ± 52 |
Intervals are presented in order of occurrence in the gait cycle, starting at the fast leg’s heel-strike (FHS). Median ± interquartile range values across participants.
Kinematic parameters
The adaptation of movements was characterized with step-length asymmetry, which is a metric known to change during and after split-belts walking (Reisman et al., 2005). We computed step-length asymmetry on each stride cycle by calculating the difference in step lengths (i.e., ankle to ankle distance at foot landing) for two consecutive steps taken with the fast and slow leg. This difference was normalized by the sum of step lengths to obtain a measure that was a proportion of each subjects’ step sizes. A zero step-length asymmetry value indicated that steps lengths were even, negative values indicated that the (non-dominant) leg walking on the slow belt was taking longer steps than the (dominant) one on the fast belt and vice versa for positive values. We also computed body displacement with respect to the foot in contact with the ground during the stance phase for each leg. This was done to interpret the changes in muscle activity on transitions between tied and split conditions. Body displacement was computed as the anterior-posterior distance between the middle of the hip markers (greater trochanter) and the ankle from ipsilateral heel-strike to contra-lateral heel-strike. Kinematic data were time-aligned to kinematic events (heel-strikes and toe-offs) in the same way as EMG data.
Epochs of interest
For our analysis, we considered behavioral measurements only at the beginning (“early”) and end (“late”) of each experimental condition (epochs). Early epochs were characterized by the average of five strides and late epochs by the average of 40 strides. Recordings during the very first and last stride of each condition were excluded to eliminate effects linked to the treadmill’s starting and stopping. For example, early long exposure consisted of the mean activity for strides 2–6, such that five strides were considered but the first one was excluded. For short exposure we used all available strides after discarding the first and last (a total of eight) instead of identifying separate early and late epochs.
Characterizing corrective responses to sudden changes in the environment
We proposed to study the structure of corrective responses to sudden changes in walking conditions (Fig. 2A). Corrective responses are defined to be the rapid changes in motor output immediately after a transition in the walking environment (i.e., within five steps after the speed transition is experienced). Per our definition, corrective motor responses include modulation of muscle activity that occurs at different latencies after a sudden change in the environment, which may or may not be voluntary (i.e., short-latency and long-latency reflexes, subsequent voluntary responses, and changes in strategy; Horak et al., 1990). We quantified corrective responses as the difference in EMG activity following changes in walking conditions, that is averaged EMG activity in the epoch after the transition (i.e., ) with respect to the epoch before it (i.e., , see description of epochs above). For example, in the baseline to long exposure transition, represents the average of the first five strides of long exposure and the average of the last 40 strides of baseline. Thus:
(1) |
We used the following subscripts to define the corrective responses (ΔEMG) to different environmental transitions: “on” and “off” indicate ΔEMG when the split environment is introduced or removed, respectively. “(+)” and “(–)” indicate the particular split environment that was introduced or removed. Specifically, (+) arbitrarily indicates that the belt under the dominant leg was moving faster than the one under the non-dominant leg (i.e., the environment used in this experiment) and (–) indicated the opposite situation (i.e., the non-dominant leg moving faster than the dominant one, not tested here). While all four combinations of on or off and (+) or (–) may happen, only three of them are relevant to our study: the introduction of the two split-belt environments “on (+)” and “on (–),” and the removal of the (+) environment: “off (+).” Lastly, we used “short” and “long” superscripts to distinguish between exposure durations to the novel split environment (i.e., 10 vs 900 strides, respectively), for off (+) transitions only.
To test our hypothesis, we analyzed the structure of corrective responses on the removal of a split-belts walking environment (; Fig. 2A). Specifically, we hypothesized that the structure of those responses may change because of sensorimotor recalibration (Fig. 2B). We analyzed the structure through the following regression model illustrated in Figure 2D:
(2) |
Where and represent the corrective responses observed when the (+) or the (–) environment are initially introduced, respectively (a cartoon of these responses is displayed in Fig. 2B).
On the long exposure (900 strides), when we expect subjects to experience sensorimotor recalibration, we considered two potential outcomes (Fig. 2C): solely environment-dependent (non-adaptive) corrective responses (O1), and adaptive corrective responses (O2). Under the first possibility, we expect corrective responses to the off transition would be numerically opposite to those on introduction (on) of the novel environment, as expected when switching back and forth between two environment-specific motor patterns (A to B vs B to A). Formally expressing O1:
(3) |
Under the second alternative (O2) the motor system learns that split-belts walking is the “new normal.” Consequently, removal of the (+) environment would be processed as a perturbation opposite to the one originally experienced [i.e., it would be equivalent to a on (–) transition]. This would be consistent with previous work reporting that the removal of altered environmental dynamics is in itself a perturbation (Herzfeld et al., 2014) and that corrective responses are adapted through experience to an altered environment even in the absence of feedback-specific learning opportunities (Wagner and Smith, 2008; Yousif and Diedrichsen, 2012). Formally expressing O2:
(4) |
In sum, our general regression model quantified concurrently the extent to which the structure of corrective responses postadaptation were environment-dependent (O1) and adaptive (O2) with and , respectively (Fig. 2D).
Note that β coefficients might be smaller than expected because of differences in the predicted structure or because magnitude differences between and . To dissociate the effect of magnitude from that of structure of corrective responses, we performed a secondary analysis in which we tested the alignment of either the vector and the vectors and by computing the cosine of the angles they form in the 360-dimensional space. This analysis is similar to the regression analysis, but is insensitive to differences in the magnitude of corrective responses. Under (O1), we expect to be aligned (cosine of 1) to , and under (O2), we expect it to be aligned to . All analyses were performed for both individual and group averaged patterns of activity.
As validation, we performed the same analyses following the short exposure (10 strides) to the (+) environment, when we expect subjects will not undergo sensorimotor recalibration. In this case, we expect corrective responses to be strictly consistent with the first alternative (O1; Fig. 2C, top).
Inferring corrective responses to an opposite transition
We did not directly measure to avoid exposing subjects to multiple environmental transitions. Instead, we infer these responses by exploiting the symmetry of the transition between the two legs. Assuming that leg dominance plays no role, the only difference between the (+) and (–) environments is which leg moves faster than the other. In other words, the environments are mirror images of one another. Thus, we expect the corrective responses to be the same as but transposing the activity for the two legs. That is, on a (–) transition, we expect to see in the dominant leg the same responses that are observed for the non-dominant leg in the (+) transition and vice versa. It is worth pointing out that our regression analyses assume that and muscle vectors are different from each other in muscle space (i.e., not colinear). Because of how we estimate , this implies that initial muscle responses () should not be anti-symmetric (i.e., same group of muscles increasing in a leg, decrease in the other one). This assumption was confirmed empirically, as the cosine of the angle formed by the two vectors was –0.13 ± 0.38 (median ± interquartile range across subjects).
Statistical analysis
Differences in muscle activity across epochs
Significant changes in muscle activity variables between any pair of epochs were determined by using the Wilcoxon signed-rank test (i.e., non-parametric analog of a paired t test) on each of the 360 muscle activity parameters. Effect sizes for the population were computed using median values across subjects since this is a measure less susceptible to outliers. All tests were two-tailed and the null hypothesis was that there was no difference in the activity between the two epochs being compared. To limit false discoveries given the large number of comparisons performed, we used the Benjamini–Krieger–Yekuteli two-stage procedure to control the false discovery rate (FDR; Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995; Benjamini et al., 2006; Kass et al., 2016). We set the acceptable FDR to be 0.05 (i.e., no more than 1 out of 20 significant findings will be false positives in expectation). Threshold (critical) p values from the FDR procedure are reported for each application. Solely for visualization purposes, we choose to indicate only the results that are significant after the FDR-controlling procedure and that correspond to an absolute effect size larger than 10% (i.e., 10% of the maximum baseline activity for that muscle, see the procedure for amplitude-normalization of muscle activity above). This was done to avoid showing statistically significant but small, and presumably meaningless, differences.
Structure of corrective responses
The linear regressions characterizing the structure of feedback-generated activity were performed using MATLAB’s fitlm function and computing (Pearson’s) R 2 values that were uncentered, given that our regression model did not include intercept terms. We compared the regressors obtained for data following the short and long exposure conditions using a two-tailed paired t test. We report p values, as well as mean changes and well as Cohen’s d for effect size.
Analysis of intersubject variability
We conducted post hoc regression analyses to determine if either age or walking speed could explain the large intersubject variability that we observed in the regression coefficients. We focused on these subject-specific features because they exhibited large ranges in our cohort that could have impacted our results. We also studied the association of age with the magnitude of muscle activity aftereffects, corrective responses and step-length asymmetry. Magnitude of muscle activity variables (aftereffects, corrective responses) was computed as the Euclidean norm of the relevant 360-dimensional vectors (e.g., for aftereffects, for on corrective responses). For all these analyses we used Spearman’s correlations, which is a non-parametric alternative to Pearson’s correlation, because it is more robust to potential outliers (Rousselet and Pernet, 2012). The correlation value (ρ) and the corresponding p value were presented. For the relation between regressors and age, Pearson’s correlations are also presented as a reference.
Data and code availability
All data and code used for this study are available at doi:10.6084/m9.figshare.c.4423772.
Results
Modulation of muscle activity during late long exposure is not a direct reflection of each leg’s walking speed
Muscle activity during late long exposure was not simply regulated as a function of the speed at which each leg moved in the split condition. Overall, we observed an anti-symmetric (i.e., similar magnitude but opposite sign) pattern of modulation of muscle activity of the two legs during late long exposure with respect to baseline (Fig. 3B). That is, we found that if a group of muscles increased activity on one side, the same group decreased activity on the other one. Interestingly, this opposing modulation across legs was not merely determined by the belt’s speed under each leg (i.e., ipsilateral walking speed), with all muscles on one leg increasing activity and all muscles on the other leg decreasing it. Should this have been the case, one would expect reduced activity of the slow leg with respect to baseline (medium speed) as seen in slow walking (Fig. 3A), and increased on the fast leg (Dietz et al., 1994; Den Otter et al., 2004). However, this type of modulation was only observed in distal muscles (MG, LG, SOL, PER, TA) and in the fast leg’s hip flexors (HIP, TFL, ADM), but not in most of the proximal muscles (SMB, SMT, BF, RF, VM, VL) and (ADM, TFL) of the slow leg, which increased in the leg walking slow and decreased in the leg walking fast. We found little similarities in the modulation of muscle activity between late long exposure and early washout. We indicate with black outlines in Figure 3B,C the muscles that do show the same modulation sign in both. This suggests that the muscle activity that gives rise to behavioral aftereffects cannot simply be inferred from the steady-state motor patterns observed in an altered environment. In sum, muscle activity for split-belts walking highlights the interlimb nature of locomotion because it exhibited an interaction between muscle group (i.e., distal vs proximal) and walking speed, rather than individual leg modulation based on speed.
Sensorimotor recalibration after prolonged exposure to split-belts walking is indicated by the structure of corrective responses
To quantitatively characterize the structure of corrective responses on removal of the novel split-belts environment, we used a regression model (; Fig. 4). If corrective responses were purely environment dependent (O1), we expect that muscle patterns on introduction and removal of the split condition would be numerically opposite (i.e., , thus ; Fig. 4A), whereas under the adaptive possibility (O2), it would be equivalent to experiencing the introduction of the opposite perturbation (i.e., , thus ; Fig. 4B).
For the transition following the long exposure period, our regression analyses were consistent with adaptive corrective responses (O2) when using both group averaged activity (CI for ; Fig. 4D, black dot), and muscle activity for each individual (median ± interquartile range for , and ; Fig. 5A, small magenta dots). Importantly, our results were not dependent on the number of strides used to quantify corrective responses. For example, similar results were obtained when using 15 strides, rather than five, but with slightly larger regression coefficients and R 2 (e.g., group averaged activity with 15 strides: CI for ). The same qualitative, although noisier, results are also obtained if we use a single stride for the analysis.
The adaptive nature of corrective responses was further supported with a secondary analysis that compared the structural similarities between and the regression factors and . Specifically, we computed the cosine of the angle between and each of the regression factors. We did this complementary analysis because it was not affected by the magnitude differences between corrective response to the on and off transitions (i.e., the magnitude of was 81.6% of the magnitude for individual subjects and 74.8% for group median data). We found that and were more aligned when using group median activity (cosine of 0.778) and individual data (median ± interquartile range: 0.521 ± 0.175) than and for group (cosine of 0.361) and individual data (median ± interquartile range: 0.19 ± 0.595).
Our regression and cosine-based results are qualitatively supported by the remarkable similarity between the and (Fig. 4B vs C), which was predicted under O2 (Fig. 4B, top half). In contrast, the adaptive nature of corrective responses could not be observed in the analysis of body motion (Fig. 4E). Specifically, the changes in body position during each leg’s stance phase (when the leg is in contact with the ground) were the same on removal of the (+) environment () and the body motion from the two alternatives considered ( and -).
Lastly, to confirm that these patterns of muscle activity arise from a learning-dependent process, and not simply due to removal of the split environment, we ran the same regressions on the split-to-tied transition following the short exposure condition (i.e., we used the exact same regression factors as in the analysis for the long exposure period). In this condition subjects did not have time to adapt, so we expected to observe changes in EMG activity consistent with environment-dependent transitions (O1). Indeed, that is what we found with our regression model applied to the transition after the short exposure (). This was true for both the group-averaged activity (CI for , p = 0.049, ; Fig. 4D, gray dot) and the activity of individual subjects (median ± interquartile range for , for , and for R 2 = 0.465 ± 0.224; data not shown). In addition, our cosine-based analysis returned values of 0.170 and 0.039 ± 0.336 for the group and individual vectors to , respectively, and 0.919 and 0.671 ± 0.166 for vectors to . In sum, we found a strong dissociation of the structure of corrective responses and exposure duration: following the short exposure condition, corrective muscle activity can be modeled as environment dependent, whereas following the long exposure the corrective responses are better explained by considering that they are adapted. Namely, comparison of the individual coefficients indicated that every subject has a higher βadapt and smaller βno-adapt after the Long than the short exposure, with a median change of 0.40 and –0.643 and Cohen’s d of 1.65 and –2.93, respectively (long vs short: βadapt: p = 1.87 × 10−6 and βno-adapt: p = 3.08 × 10−11). Consistently, every subject’s corrective responses were more aligned to and less to following the long exposure compared to the Short one, with a mean cosine change of 0.558 and –0.539, and corresponding Cohen’s d of 1.84 and –2.72, respectively (long vs short paired t test: p = 5.15 × 10−6 and p = 4.89 × 10−8). Taken together, we found that the structure of corrective muscle activity reflected the recalibration of the motor system during adaptation.
Corrective responses revealed age-related decline of sensorimotor adaptation
Regression analysis of intersubject variability revealed that older adults exhibited less adaptation of corrective responses. Namely, βadapt and βno-adapt were associated to subjects’ age (βadapt vs age: Spearman’s ρ = –0.52, p = 0.049, Pearson’s R 2 = 0.511, p = 0.00275 and βno-adapt vs age: ρ = 0.72, p = 0.0036, R 2 = 0.423, p = 0.0086; Fig. 5A) with older subjects showing smaller βadapt and larger βno-adapt. This indicates that corrective responses in older adults were less adapted (and more environment driven) compared to younger adults. We also observed that the magnitude of corrective responses was smaller in older adults (: , p = 0.0043; : , p = 0.016; Fig. 5B). These smaller responses in older individuals could make it more difficult to identify the structure of corrective responses because of the reduced signal-to-noise ratio, possibly leading to biased or noisy estimates of βadapt and βno-adapt. To discard this possibility, we correlated the R 2 of the fitted models for each individual with age. We found no effects of age (ρ = –0.51, R 2 = 0.25, p = 0.058; data not shown), meaning that the regression model applied to individual data captured comparable levels of variance regardless of subjects’ age. Taken together, we observed an age-mediated difference in the extent of adaptation of corrective muscle activity, suggesting an age-mediated decline of learning processes updating corrective responses. This limited learning capacity in older individuals was further supported by the negative association between age and the magnitude of EMG aftereffects (, p = 0.0001; Fig. 5E): the older the subjects, the smaller the aftereffects. This happened despite the similar magnitude of muscle activity during late long exposure across individuals ( vs age: ρ = –0.054, p = 0.85; Fig. 5C). Importantly, walking speed, which naturally alters muscle activity, was not associated to the magnitude of EMG aftereffects (p = 0.24), to the adaptation (speed vs βadapt, p = 0.81, speed vs βno-adapt, p = 0.61), or to the magnitude of corrective responses on off transition (p = 0.64). Interestingly, age dependency was not observed in the magnitude of step-length asymmetry aftereffects (ρ = –0.364, p = 0.182; Fig. 5D), which are conventionally used to characterize sensorimotor recalibration in locomotor. The partially distinct information characterized by muscle activity and step length asymmetry is further indicated by the poor relation between muscle activity aftereffects and kinematic aftereffects (ρ = 0.389, p = 0.152; data not shown). In sum, we observed an age-related effect in the structure of corrective responses following a long exposure of the split-belts condition and smaller EMG aftereffects in older adults.
Discussion
We showed that muscle activity patterns following sudden changes in environmental walking conditions are adapted with prolonged exposure to a new environment. Importantly, the structure of corrective activity on removing the novel split condition was similar to the muscle patterns expected in response to the introduction of a novel environment with dynamics opposite to the one originally experienced. We claim that these corrective responses postadaptation can be interpreted as a proxy for sensorimotor recalibration since the adaptive structural features were only observed when subjects had enough time to adopt the split pattern as their new reference gait. Interestingly, older subjects showed less adaptation of this corrective activity, leading to smaller aftereffects in muscle space. Our results are relevant not only because we identified a sensitive measure of individual sensorimotor adaptation, but also because we provided a valuable characterization of normative changes in muscle activity from split-belts walking, which has been suggested as a rehabilitation therapy.
Learning of a new steady-state locomotor pattern
Muscle activity modulation during late split-belts walking was mostly anti-symmetric, likely due to the uneven demands of the split environment, such as the distinct speed-specific propulsion force demands for each leg (Sombric et al., 2018). However, motor patterns were not simply adjusted based on ipsilateral speed demands, but activity of proximal muscles seemed influenced by the speed of the other leg’s belt. This highlighted the bilateral nature of locomotion (Maclellan et al., 2014) and contrasted with the idea that each leg is adapted independently, as observed in hybrid split-belts walking (i.e., one leg moving forward fast and the other leg moving backwards slowly; Choi and Bastian, 2007) possibly because of the peculiar demands of this hybrid task. In fact, most muscles increased activity when the split condition was introduced and plateaued at reduced activation levels, supporting the notion that initial stability demands result in higher activation levels that decreases as motor patterns become more efficient (Franklin et al., 2008; Finley et al., 2013; Huang and Ahmed, 2014). Notably, the learned (steady-state) modulation pattern did not resemble the aftereffects in muscle activity, except in a few muscles. This was to a certain degree unexpected given that kinematic aftereffects are thought to reflect a continuation of the adapted motor commands in the altered environment (Morton and Bastian, 2006; Malone et al., 2012; Ogawa et al., 2014). Plantarflexors (calf muscles) were the predominant muscles exhibiting similar activity before and after split-belts walking (only during double-support). Plantarflexor activity contributes to displacing the body and leg forward (Neptune et al., 2001), and consequently modulate step length (Neptune et al., 2008). Thus, continuation of plantarflexors’ anti-asymmetric activity postadaptation might lead to the known aftereffect on step length asymmetry following split-belts walking. To test this hypothesis, future work is needed with a different approach, such as musculoskeletal simulations (Steele et al., 2010; Song and Geyer, 2015), which will enable investigating the EMG-kinematic relation during and after split-belts walking. In sum, muscle activity in the split condition was anti-symmetric and only plantarflexors’ modulation during late split-belts walking continued on removal of the split environment.
Corrective activity postadaptation indicates the motor system’s adapted state
We demonstrated that the structure of corrective activity on removal of the novel split-belts environment indicated subjects’ adapted state and not just environmental changes. This was shown by the difference in the structure of corrective activity after identical environmental transitions following different exposure durations to the split environment. Corrective muscle activity following a long, but not a short, exposure period was equivalent to the modulation expected on the introduction of an environment opposite to the one subject experienced. This is consistent with the observation that removal of an altered environment facilitates meta-learning of an opposite environment (Herzfeld et al., 2014). Our results suggest that subjects have adopted the gait pattern of late split-belts walking as a new normal such that deviations from it are experience as perturbations. This interpretation is supported by studies indicating that subjects shift their perception of what constitutes symmetric walking (i.e., same speed for two legs) following adaptation to the split-belts environment (Vazquez et al., 2015; Statton et al., 2018). It is worth pointing out that the observed structure of corrective responses was not exactly what we expected under our adaptive hypothesis: both βadapt was smaller than 1, and βno-adapt was larger than 0. We interpret the non-zero βno-adapt as some amount of environment-based switching in corrective responses due to rapid strategic adjustments once subjects realize that they are no longer in the split condition. This is similar to re-aiming reaching direction when subjects notice that visuomotor rotations are removed (Morehead et al., 2015; McDougle et al., 2016). Moreover, the differences between the estimated and expected regressor values might be simply caused by methodological limitations. First, consider that the estimation of βadapt values relies on our assumption that can be inferred from . Thus, βadapt (but not βno-adapt) can be biased downwards by any asymmetries between the legs due to mismatched placement of the EMG sensors or temporally misaligned activity originated by unequal half-cycle durations across the legs. Second, any noise in the regression factors (i.e., and ) biases the estimates of the regressor coefficients toward 0. This is because noise in a high-dimensional space is more likely to lead to misalignment between these regression vectors and the vector characterizing corrective activity postadaptation (). Both of these problems are more acute when estimating βadapt and βno-adapt per subject, which have larger noise levels and where activity is more likely to be asymmetrically recorded. Consistently, the effect of noise can be reduced by using a larger number of strides to characterize muscle activity during early epochs. For example, similar results were obtained when using 15 strides (rather than five) to compute corrective activity, but with slightly larger regression coefficients and R 2. Despite these methodological limitations, we concluded that the structure of corrective activity postadaptation reflects the recalibration of the motor system during split-belts walking because overall corrective responses in the off transition were substantially more similar to the adaptive (O2) than environment-dependent (O1) corrective activity. Taken together our results suggests that the structure of corrective responses indicates the extent of sensorimotor recalibration at an individual level.
The adaptation of corrective responses suggests learning-dependent tuning of feedback activity
We believe that our results are evidence of the influence of predictive mechanisms on reactive control. We could not use conventional approaches to experimentally discern the adaptation of internal models for predictive (feedforward) control from online feedback corrections (Wagner and Smith, 2008; Yousif and Diedrichsen, 2012; Cluff and Scott, 2013; Crevecoeur and Scott, 2013) given the continuous nature of the walking task. We could only dissociate adjustments that are immediate from those that take multiple steps and presumably result from adaptation of internal models of the environment and/or body (Reisman et al., 2009; Torres-Oviedo and Bastian, 2010; Roemmich and Bastian, 2015). We concede that the corrective responses that we characterize include a variety of mechanisms that have different neural substrates, such as monosynaptic reflexes, longer loop responses, and voluntary movement corrections. However, we interpret the reported structural changes of these corrective responses as learning-dependent adjustments of feedback processes involving supraspinal mechanisms (i.e., longer loop responses). This interpretation is in accordance with previous reaching studies suggesting shared internal models between feedforward and feedback motor control (Wagner and Smith, 2008; Crevecoeur and Scott, 2013; Cluff and Scott, 2013). We discounted the possibility that split-belts walking modifies short-latency monosynaptic stretch reflexes since these can only change with extensive training (Thompson et al., 2009). Also, we reasoned that we are capturing little strategic adjustments (i.e., change in feedforward actions), such as goal-directed changes of foot landings (Matthis et al., 2017) or muscle activations passively driven by the environment. Namely, these kinds of modulation would predict more environment-based switching at the off (long) transition and we only observe little of that (as quantified by the small βno-adapt values). Taken together, our results provide further evidence that feedback control is influenced by the recalibration of processes involved in predictive motor control.
Age-related decline in the adaptive nature of feedback activity
Aging affects the adaptation of muscle activity after, but not during, split-belts walking, suggesting a possible dissociation between the update of feedback and feedforward activity. Motor learning was affected by age as indicated by the reduced magnitude of aftereffects and less mirroring in feedback activity postadaptation. It has been suggested that older subjects have noisier sensory information (Konczak et al., 2012), and thus, rely less on sensory input and more on predictive mechanisms (Wolpe et al., 2016). This would explain both the weaker feedback responses on environmental transitions and reduced recalibration of internal models (i.e., smaller βadapt) in older subjects. Despite the age-related modulation of aftereffects in muscle activity, this age dependency is not observed in kinematic aftereffects (Bruijn et al., 2012; Sombric et al., 2017). EMG signals may be more sensitive to the recalibration of internal models because they are a closer correlate of neural activity than kinematics. Further, the magnitude of muscle patterns during late long exposure, which we presume to be mostly feedforward-generated activity, was not affected with age. This dichotomy in age effects on feedback versus feedforward activity supports the partial dissociation of the adaptation of these two processes (Yousif and Diedrichsen, 2012). We conclude that age-related sensory decline might contribute to motor learning deficits in older adults, which is observed in the adaptation of corrective responses, but not in steady state motor patterns.
Study implications
Our findings suggest that corrective responses could be used as an alternate approach for quantifying sensorimotor recalibration. In discrete behaviors such as eye or arm movements, aftereffects are typically quantified by the initial error (e.g., the initial direction of a thrown dart after adaptation), since it reflects the newly acquired predictive model when the altered environment is removed. This is not easily quantifiable in walking because of its continuous nature, and because there is no explicit error measure. Our novel approach highlights that the recalibration of the predictive model can be quantified not only by the standard “aftereffect,” but by the subsequent corrective response elicited by an inadequate initial action. This corrective response is present in other motor adaptation tasks (e.g., reaching correction after starting with a wrong movement direction) where it may also be used as an indicator of the motor system’s implicit recalibration.
From a clinical perspective, split-belts walking can potentially correct gait asymmetry poststroke (Reisman et al., 2013), but little is known about the changes in muscle activity that can be expected from this task. Unlike previous work (Dietz et al., 1994; Raja et al., 2012; Ogawa et al., 2014), we provide a systematic characterization of healthy motor patterns during distinct phases of the gait cycle throughout the adaptation and de-adaptation process. These normative data allow for the assessment of adaptation deficits poststroke and early evaluation of the therapeutic limits of split-belts walking. Importantly, we show that intersubject learning capacities may be better captured through EMG-based measurements than conventional kinematic measurements. These measures suggest that learning might be limited in some patients simply because of their age. Lastly, our results indicate that aftereffects are strongly influenced by corrective responses to the sudden transition to tied-belts walking, rather than solely represent changes in feedforward circuits, which are arguably the true target of rehabilitation therapies. We conclude that the long-term rehabilitative potential of split-belts walking may not be well captured by solely measuring kinematic behavior immediately after adaptation.
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgements: We thank Reza Shadmehr, Adrian Haith, and Ryan Roemmich for their comments on earlier versions of this manuscript. We also thank the anonymous reviewers for their constructive criticism.
Synthesis
Reviewing Editor: Trevor Drew, University of Montreal
Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Sergiy Yakovenko, Matthew Tresch.
Reviewer #1
The authors put substantial effort to clarify the problems identified in the submitted study. It appears that about 80-90% of the original submission has been replaced. Many issues with the confusing terminology have been resolved. However, my main concern with the study design remains. Also, the new methodology lacks clarity, which is a similar problem to the previous submission.
MAJOR QUESTIONS
1) The claim in the study remains to be about the age-related decline in motor learning even though the study design was not set to test this hypothesis. The authors admit in the response letter that this was a post-hoc finding; yet, the study description does not reflect this. See, for example, the significance statement. In the current form, unfortunately, this constitutes a problem with the external validity in the design of this study.
The related problem is the logical motivation of this study. The idea that the changes in symmetric and asymmetric belt speeds would cause the recalibration of internal models is not supported by the cited literature. It appears that there is a mistaken equivalency the novel task adaptations and “the recalibration of internal models of walking”. The term was not used by the cited authors, and there is no indication that the cited manuscripts supported the modifications of body representations.
Thus, the use of the term “sensory recalibration” in the context of internal models is ambiguous. For example, the study of Roemmich and Bastian (2015) describes “different internal representations of the learning environment”, which is not the same as the concept of the internal model associated with the controlled plant.
2) The current hypothesis of this study, stated on lines 64-66, appears to be trivial. The principle that responses are going to be indicative of sensorimotor adaptations has been utilized for decades in multiple studies using catch trials and measurements of changes in EMG patterns.
The absence of a valid hypothesis constitutes a problem in the study design, related to question 1 above. Unfortunately, the current hypothesis is not a valid scientific hypothesis. See also, minor comment for ln210, which points to a rational-driven motivation. Altogether, the current submission appears to present conflicting descriptions.
3) The modified methodology that completely replaces the previous main assumptions of feedforward and feedback interactions appears to be unfinished. It uses confusing notations of 3 factors. The differences in ‘on’, ‘up’, ‘off’, ‘down’ in ‘short’ and ‘long’ ∂EMG combinations are not clearly described. The new diagram in Fig. 2 does not fully clarify these multiple combinations. What is the difference between ‘down on’ and ‘up off’? The lack of clarity in these descriptions and their statistical testing is a weakness of the current submission. It appears that there were multiple combinations for testing but only a subset of these relationship appears to be analyzed. For example, what is, e.g., ‘down_off_short’ and what happened to these analyses? The statistical implications of multiple tests related to this grouping are not described.
4) The statistical assumption of 10% in false positive rate. While it was not noted in the previous review, the choice of statistical significance is twice less stringent than the usual biological level of 5%. The choice is arbitrary in sciences but consistent within fields, and 5% rate is the regular level of significance expected in physiology. The reduced quality of comparisons is not standard and should be justified for a better understanding of the constraints in this study.
MINOR
Fig.1 DS probably stands for double stance, which is a part of STANCE. Please add a description of the abbreviation.
ln210: “We proposed to study the structure of corrective motor responses...” This was not the stated hypothesis.
Reviewer #2
I found the paper to be considerably clearer after the authors' revisions. They have done a very good job of clarifying the presentation and making the results more straightforward. I have only a few remaining issues.
Introduction: In response to the reviewers' comments, the authors dropped any mention of the issue of age related effects from the Introduction and placed the motivation for this analysis and consideration in the Methods. I don't want to send the authors in circles, but I found this a bit confusing as it seems much more straightforward to include in the Introduction.
Discussion: It might be worthwhile in the Discussion to include some consideration of the relationship between what the authors are describing here as ‘corrective’ responses and what people usually mean in terms of ‘aftereffects’ in the motor learning literature. I think it's fair to say that people focus on the initial error in movement when considering aftereffects - i.e. the initial direction of a reach, or the direction of a thrown dart after adaptation - since it reflects the newly acquired predictive model when the perturbation is removed. Although the authors' analysis will include this initial component, their analysis includes and I think highlights the subsequent corrective response that people make after realizing that their initial prediction is wrong. This corrective response is present in other motor learning tasks (e.g. the correction of the reach after the initial error) and reflects the aftereffect/predictive model, but is not usually highlighted. Because of the continuous nature of locomotion, it is difficult to directly see and quantify the standard ‘aftereffect’, but by examining the corrective response they can still gain insight into adaptation mechanisms. In my mind at least, I think this distinction might help clarify some aspects of the authors' study.
Also in the Discussion, I would recommend dropping the section on neural substrates (4.4). The present work really doesn't speak to this issue at all.
Figure 2 I confess that I didn't find the bottom panel of Figure 2 particularly helpful - it might be easier to only include the top panels.
Specific points
Line 31: ‘peoples’ ’
Line 36: This sentence took me some time to interpret - especially the emphasis on ‘passive’. Maybe it would be easier to say that muscles are more directly reflective of neural control commands, whereas kinematics and kinetics arise from interactions between those commands and the environment?
line 61: “...what impact it has on aftereffects.” It's not clear what ‘it’ is here. These last lines (from line 53-63) could be clearer - they're the main point of the paper.
Line 76-78: I'm not sure what ‘removing the motor pattern’ means here. This sentence could be clearer.
Line 135 ‘subjects’ ’
Line 261: ‘That is,..’?
Line 371: ‘slowly’
Figure 1B: The middle columns show the data for LG in the dominant and non-dominant legs in early-Long and Baseline conditions, with the colored squares underneath showing the difference between them. As I understand it, the data in the rightmost column should be these same differences, but shown for all muscles in each leg. However, the entries for LG in the non-dominant and dominant legs don't appear to match what's shown in the middle column. So maybe I'm missing something?
Figure 4: Maybe it would be helpful in the figure to label the two predicted and the observed EMG changes in the figure itself? It's explained in the text of course, but labels might make it easier to parse what's happening in the figure, which shows the key result of the study.
Comments from the Consultation that should also be taken into account
Reviewer#2
By and large. I like the age result but agree that it's posthoc and, even more critically, that it's not really motivated in the Introduction - though I think that was a change based on our previous comments. And I agree that the main result of the work is not motivated well by a clearly stated hypothesis in the manuscript, although I do think it's in there implicitly. This should be corrected.
I think the other reviewer's point (2) is the crux - whether showing that EMGs are a sensitive measure of adaptation in this paradigm is novel and/or interesting - or whether it constitutes a true hypothesis. I do think that there's utility in demonstrating this result for split-belt learning, especially as they are trying to emphasize examining the structure of corrections as opposed to the structure of aftereffects, which I think is novel. And I thought their analysis approach is novel and interesting.
I wasn't too concerned about the FDR - as I understood it they only use it for display purposes and it doesn't really affect the actual main results of environment vs. adaptation. They say that they could change it if the reviewers want. In fact I think they could get rid of it entirely and just plot the EMG summaries and it wouldn't really affect their results which is mainly for the regression analyses.
Reviewer #1
A reasonable rationale should be described.
There is a possible terminology mix-up in the description of what is a sensorimotor recalibration in a novel “task” and that in an “internal model. Although some may call this type of optimization of control signals to be a recalibration of the internal model, it is incorrect from the engineering perspective. Here, the control signals are optimized to the novel task, but the controlled plant and its internal model remains the same.
References
- Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y (1995) Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Series B Stat Methodol 57:289–300. 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Benjamini Y, Krieger A, Yekutieli D (2006) Adaptive linear step-up procedures that control the false discovery rate. Biometrika 93:491–507. 10.1093/biomet/93.3.491 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bernstein N (1967) The co-ordination and regulation of movements. Oxford: Pergamon Press. [Google Scholar]
- Bhushan N, Shadmehr R (1999) Computational nature of human adaptive control during learning of reaching movements in force fields. Biol Cybern 81:39–60. 10.1007/s004220050543 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruijn SM, Van Impe A, Duysens J, Swinnen SP (2012) Split-belt walking: adaptation differences between young and older adults. J Neurophysiol 108:1149–1157. 10.1152/jn.00018.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choi JT, Bastian AJ (2007) Adaptation reveals independent control networks for human walking. Nat Neurosci 10:1055–1062. 10.1038/nn1930 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chvatal SA, Ting LH (2012) Voluntary and reactive recruitment of locomotor muscle synergies during perturbed walking. J Neurosci 32:12237–12250. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6344-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chvatal SA, Torres-Oviedo G, Safavynia SA, Ting LH (2011) Common muscle synergies for control of center of mass and force in nonstepping and stepping postural behaviors. J Neurophysiol 106:999–1015. 10.1152/jn.00549.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cluff T, Scott SH (2013) Rapid feedback responses correlate with reach adaptation and properties of novel upper limb loads. J Neurosci 33:15903–15914. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0263-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins S, Ruina A, Tedrake R, Wisse M (2005) Efficient bipedal robots based on passive-dynamic walkers. Science 307:1082–1085. 10.1126/science.1107799 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crevecoeur F, Scott SH (2013) Priors engaged in long-latency responses to mechanical perturbations suggest a rapid update in state estimation. PLoS Comput Biol 9:e1003177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Kam D, Roelofs JM, Bruijnes AK, Geurts AC, Weerdesteyn V (2017) The next step in understanding impaired reactive balance control in people with stroke: the role of defective early automatic postural responses. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 31:708–716. 10.1177/1545968317718267 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Den Otter AR, Geurts ACH, Mulder T, Duysens J (2004) Speed related changes in muscle activity from normal to very slow walking speeds. Gait Posture 19:270–278. 10.1016/S0966-6362(03)00071-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dietz V, Zijlstra W, Duysens J (1994) Human neuronal interlimb coordination during split-belt locomotion. Gait Posture 101:513–520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finley JM, Bastian AJ, Gottschall J (2013) Learning to be economical: the energy cost of walking tracks motor adaptation. J Physiol 591:1081–1095. 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.245506 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franklin DW, Burdet E, Peng Tee K, Osu R, Chew CM, Milner TE, Kawato M (2008) CNS learns stable, accurate, and efficient movements using a simple algorithm. J Neurosci 28:11165–11173. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3099-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herzfeld DJ, Vaswani P, Marko MK, Shadmehr R (2014) A memory of errors in sensorimotor learning. Science 345:1349–1353. 10.1126/science.1253138 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horak FB, Nashner LM, Diener HC (1990) Postural strategies associated with somatosensory and vestibular loss. Exp Brain Res 82:167–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang HJ, Ahmed AA (2014) Reductions in muscle coactivation and metabolic cost during visuomotor adaptation. J Neurophysiol 112:2264–2274. 10.1152/jn.00014.2014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jordan MI, Rumelhart DE (1992) Forward models: supervised learning with a distal teacher. Cogn Sci 16:307–354. 10.1207/s15516709cog1603_1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kass RE, Caffo BS, Davidian M, Meng XL, Yu B, Reid N (2016) Ten simple rules for effective statistical practice. PLoS Comput Biol 12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konczak J, Sciutti A, Avanzino L, Squeri V, Gori M, Masia L, Abbruzzese G, Sandini G (2012) Parkinson’s disease accelerates age-related decline in haptic perception by altering somatosensory integration. Brain 135:3371–3379. 10.1093/brain/aws265 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewek MD, Braun CH, Wutzke C, Giuliani C (2018) The role of movement errors in modifying spatiotemporal gait asymmetry post stroke: a randomized controlled trial. Clin Rehabil 32:161–172. 10.1177/0269215517723056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maclellan MJ, Ivanenko YP, Massaad F, Bruijn SM, Duysens J, Lacquaniti F (2014) Muscle activation patterns are bilaterally linked during split-belt treadmill walking in humans. J Neurophysiol 111:1541–1552. 10.1152/jn.00437.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malone LA, Bastian AJ, Torres-Oviedo G (2012) How does the motor system correct for errors in time and space during locomotor adaptation? J Neurophysiol 108:672–683. 10.1152/jn.00391.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthis JS, Barton SL, Fajen BR (2017) The critical phase for visual control of human walking over complex terrain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 114:E6720–E6729. 10.1073/pnas.1611699114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDougle SD, Ivry RB, Taylor JA (2016) Taking aim at the cognitive side of learning in sensorimotor adaptation tasks. Trends Cogn Sci 20:535–544. 10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merletti R, Parker PPA (2004) Electromyography: physiology, engineering, and noninvasive applications. Piscataway, NJ: IEEE Press. [Google Scholar]
- Morehead JR, Qasim SE, Crossley MJ, Ivry R (2015) Savings upon re-aiming in visuomotor adaptation. J Neurosci 35:14386–14396. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1046-15.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morton SM, Bastian AJ (2006) Cerebellar contributions to locomotor adaptations during splitbelt treadmill walking. J Neurosci 26:9107–9116. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2622-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mozaffarian D, Benjamin EJ, Go AS, Arnett DK, Blaha MJ, Cushman M, Das SR, Ferranti SD, Després JP, Fullerton HJ, Howard VJ, Huffman MD, Isasi CR, Jiménez MC, Judd SE, Kissela BM, Lichtman JH, Lisabeth LD, Liu S, MacKey RH, et al. (2016) Heart disease and stroke statistics–2016 update: a report from the American Heart Association. Circulation 133:e38–e360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neptune RR, Kautz SA, Zajac FE (2001) Contributions of the individual ankle plantar flexors to support, forward progression and swing initiation during walking. . J Biomech 34:1387–1398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neptune RR, Sasaki K, Kautz SA (2008) The effect of walking speed on muscle function and mechanical energetics. Gait Posture 28:135–143. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.11.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ogawa T, Kawashima N, Ogata T, Nakazawa K (2014) Predictive control of ankle stiffness at heel contact is a key element of locomotor adaptation during split-belt treadmill walking in humans. J Neurophysiol 111:722–32. 10.1152/jn.00497.2012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perry J, Burnfield JM (2010) Gait analysis normal and pathological function, Ed 2 Thorofare, NJ: Slack Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Raja B, Neptune RR, Kautz SA (2012) Coordination of the non-paretic leg during hemiparetic gait: expected and novel compensatory patterns. Clin Biomech (Bristol, Avon) 27:1023–1030. 10.1016/j.clinbiomech.2012.08.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ranganathan R, Scheidt RA (2016) Organizing and reorganizing coordination patterns. In: Progress in motor control, pp 327–349. Cham: Springer. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reisman DS, Block HJ, Bastian AJ (2005) Interlimb coordination during locomotion: what can be adapted and stored? J Neurophysiol 94:2403–2415. 10.1152/jn.00089.2005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reisman DS, Wityk R, Silver K, Bastian AJ (2009) Split-belt treadmill adaptation transfers to overground walking in persons poststroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 23:735–744. 10.1177/1545968309332880 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reisman DS, McLean H, Keller J, Danks KA, Bastian AJ (2013) Repeated split-belt treadmill training improves poststroke step length asymmetry. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 27:460–468. 10.1177/1545968312474118 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rikli RE, Jones CJ (1998) The reliability and validity of a 6-minute walk test as a measure of physical endurance in older adults. J Aging Phys Act 6:363–375. 10.1123/japa.6.4.363 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Roemmich RT, Bastian AJ (2015) Two ways to save a newly learned motor pattern. J Neurophysiol 113:3519–3530. 10.1152/jn.00965.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rousselet GA, Pernet CR (2012) Improving standards in brain-behavior correlation analyses. Front Hum Neurosci 6:119. 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Safavynia SA, Ting LH (2012) Task-level feedback can explain temporal recruitment of spatially fixed muscle synergies throughout postural perturbations. J Neurophysiol 107:159–177. 10.1152/jn.00653.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sánchez N, Park S, Finley JM (2017) Evidence of energetic optimization during adaptation differs for metabolic, mechanical, and perceptual estimates of energetic cost. Sci Rep 7:7682. 10.1038/s41598-017-08147-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sombric CJ, Harker HM, Sparto PJ, Torres-Oviedo G (2017) Explicit action switching interferes with the context-specificity of motor memories in older adults. Fronti Aging Neurosci 9:40. 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sombric C, Calvert JS, Torres-Oviedo G (2018) Large propulsion demands increase locomotor learning at the expense of step length asymmetry. Front Physiol 10:60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Song S, Geyer H (2015) A neural circuitry that emphasizes spinal feedback generates diverse behaviours of human locomotion. J Physiol 593:3493–3511. 10.1113/JP270228 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Statton MA, Vazquez A, Morton SM, Vasudevan EVL, Bastian AJ (2018) Making sense of cerebellar contributions to perceptual and motor adaptation. Cerebellum 17:111–121. 10.1007/s12311-017-0879-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steele KM, Seth A, Hicks JL, Schwartz MS, Delp SL (2010) Muscle contributions to support and progression during single-limb stance in crouch gait. J Biomech 43:2099–2105. 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2010.04.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tang P, Woollacott MH (1999) Phase-dependent modulation of proximal and distal postural responses to slips in young and older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 54:M89–M102. 10.1093/gerona/54.2.M89 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson AK, Chen XY, Wolpaw JR (2009) Acquisition of a simple motor skill: task-dependent adaptation plus long-term change in the human soleus H-reflex. J Neurosci 29:5784–5792. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4326-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torres-Oviedo G, Ting LH (2007) Muscle synergies characterizing human postural responses. J Neurophysiol 98:2144–2156. 10.1152/jn.01360.2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torres-Oviedo G, Bastian AJ (2010) Seeing is believing: effects of visual contextual cues on learning and transfer of locomotor adaptation. J Neurosci 50:17015–17022. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4205-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vazquez A, Statton MA, Busgang SA, Bastian AJ (2015) Split-belt walking adaptation recalibrates sensorimotor estimates of leg speed, but not position or force. J Neurophysiol 114:3255–3267. 10.1152/jn.00302.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagner MJ, Smith MA (2008) Shared internal models for feedforward and feedback control. J Neurosci 28:10663–10673. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5479-07.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolpe N, Ingram JN, Tsvetanov KA, Geerligs L, Kievit RA, Henson RN, Wolpert DM, Rowe JB (2016) Ageing increases reliance on sensorimotor prediction through structural and functional differences in frontostriatal circuits. Nat Commun 7:1–11. 10.1038/ncomms13034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolpert DM, Miall RC, Kawato M (1998) Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends Cogn Sci 2:338–347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yokoyama H, Sato K, Ogawa T, Yamamoto SI, Nakazawa K, Kawashima N (2018) Characteristics of the gait adaptation process due to split-belt treadmill walking under a wide range of right-left speed ratios in humans. PLoS One 13:e0194875 10.1371/journal.pone.0194875 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yousif N, Diedrichsen J (2012) Structural learning in feedforward and feedback control. J Neurophysiol 108:2373–2382. 10.1152/jn.00315.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
All data and code used for this study are available at doi:10.6084/m9.figshare.c.4423772.