Abstract
People with Alzheimer's disease (AD) have characteristic problems navigating everyday environments. While patients may exhibit abnormal gait parameters, adaptive gait irregularities when navigating environments are little explored or understood. The aim of this study was to assess adaptive locomotor responses of AD subjects in a complex environment requiring spatial navigation. A controlled environment of three corridors was set up: straight (I), U-shaped (U) and dog-leg (S). Participants were asked to walk along corridors as part of a counterbalanced repeated-measures design. Three groups were studied: 11 people with posterior cortical atrophy (PCA), 10 with typical Alzheimer's disease (tAD) and 13 controls. Spatio-temporal gait parameters and position within the corridors were monitored with shoe-mounted inertial measurement units (IMUs). Hesitant steps were identified from statistical analysis of the distribution of step time data. Walking paths were generated from position data calculated by double integration of IMU acceleration. People with PCA and tAD had similar gait characteristics, having shorter steps and longer step times than controls. Hesitant steps tended to be clustered within certain regions of the walking paths. IMUs enabled identification of key gait characteristics in this clinical population (step time, length and step hesitancy) and environmental conditions (route complexity) modifying their expression.
Keywords: statistical analysis, diseases, gait analysis, biomedical measurement, patient monitoring
Keywords: walking paths, step hesitancy, route complexity, Alzheimer's disease navigating routes, abnormal gait parameters, gait irregularities, adaptive locomotor responses, spatial navigation, corridors, dog-leg, counterbalanced repeated-measures design, posterior cortical atrophy, shoe-mounted inertial measurement units, step time data, position data, Alzheimer's disease, gait characteristics, IMU acceleration, statistical analysi
1. Introduction
Early and common symptoms of Alzheimer's disease (AD) are problems in spatial navigation, greatly undermining autonomy. While such problems are often associated with deficits in memory and planning, the role of dementia-related visual processing impairments in limiting navigation, particularly in familiar environments, is often under-recognised [1]. In typical Alzheimer's disease (tAD), initial pathological changes in medial temporal regions particularly associated with episodic and spatial memory ultimately progress to posterior cortical regions, including those supporting visuospatial processing [2]. A neurodegenerative syndrome offering important insights into dementia-related visual impairment is posterior cortical atrophy (PCA) [3]. While PCA is most commonly caused by AD, in contrast to tAD, PCA patients demonstrate relatively well-preserved memory, particularly in early disease stages, but exhibit a range of complex visual deficits and environmental disorientation [4–7].
Gait function has also been shown to be impaired in dementia. Indeed, it has even been suggested that gait could be used as a biomarker of cognitive impairment and dementia syndromes [8]. Changes in step length, step time and walking speed have been observed in people with both cognitive impairment and dementia [9], and gait changes appear to be related to the stage of the disease [10–12]. Although a basic gait laboratory setting has been used to assess gait in tAD while also performing a cognitive task [13, 14], this does not replicate the daily navigational challenges of people with visual and/or cognitive problems. Perceptual factors (e.g. lighting, clutter) may have a disproportionate effect on functional performance in tAD, but situations that replicate these conditions are difficult to set up in a standard gait laboratory. Creating environments with walls and confined spaces makes it very difficult to use standard opto-electronic motion capture systems, which require a line-of-sight view from the motion detection camera to the subject.
Sensors attached directly to the participants would, in principle, enable assessment of spatio-temporal gait characteristics under any environment. Inertial measurement units (IMUs) comprise systems of orthogonal tri-axial accelerometers, gyroscopes and sometimes magnetometers, and can provide a portable means of measuring movement. IMUs have been used in studies of gait in healthy ageing [15, 16]. Their use in the assessment of gait in Parkinson's disease and dementia has recently been described [17]. IMUs can also provide an infrastructureless means of tracking the position of first responders within buildings in emergency situations [18], and for tracking people outdoors when GPS signals are not available [19]. IMUs therefore have the potential of being able to measure simultaneously both spatio-temporal characteristics of participants’ gait and their location.
This study is part of a more general investigation of gait and spatial navigation in people with dementia in a controlled real-world environment. It is proposed that people with PCA and tAD would exhibit inefficient adaptation of gait in response to local environments (corridor turns). In this present study, we investigate the feasibility of using IMUs to detect and localise hesitant behaviour.
2. Methods
2.1. Testing environment
The main platform of PAMELA consisted of 54 (9 × 6) configurable 1200 mm × 1200 mm modules. Wood boards of 55 mm in thickness were put on top of the concrete modules and then covered with dark-blue domestic carpet. The entire area of floor was gap-free and levelled. The platform was configured to provide three corridors with 2.1 m high walls using free-standing wooden panels (2100 mm height, 1200 mm width, 42 mm thickness) as walls. Walls were of good contrast to the floor carpet, consistent with residential design recommendations for individuals with sight loss and dementia [20]. The three corridors comprised: a 6 m straight (I) corridor, a 8.4 m U-shaped corridor with two 90° turns in the same direction and a 8.4 m ‘dog-leg’ (S) corridor with two 90° turns in opposite directions. These are illustrated schematically in Fig. 1, along with an overhead image of the set-up. The average ground illuminance was set at 190 lux, which is a typical indoor lighting level for homes.
2.2. Participants and procedures
Two groups of dementia patients were studied: 11 with PCA [mean age: 64.6 ± 5.6 years; male/female: 5/6; height (cm): 168.9 ± 6.5; mini mental state examination (MMSE) score: 18.4 ± 5.9], and 10 with tAD (mean age: 66.2 ± 5.0 years; male/female: 4/6; height: 167.9 ± 11.8; MMSE: 18.6 ± 4.9). These were compared with a control group (n = 13, mean age: 64.2 ± 4.1 years; male/female: 7/6; height: 171.2 ± 12.9); groups were well-matched on age, height and gender. PCA and tAD patients fulfilled consensus criteria for PCA and NIA-AA criteria for tAD, respectively. Exclusion criteria were a history of other neurological or major psychiatric diseases. Patients did not fulfil clinical criteria for dementia with Lewy bodies, Parkinson's disease, corticobasal degeneration or prion disease or exhibit associated clinical features (e.g. early visual hallucinations, parkinsonism, ataxia). Each participant had IMUs attached to each heel, as described above. Participants were asked to ‘keep walking until you reach the end of the corridor’. The start of each trial was verbally signalled by the experimenter (‘Start’) preceded by counting down from three. Trials ended when participants crossed the finishing line of the route (0.6 m from the end of each corridor). Participants walked down each of three route shapes in both directions (outward and return), resulting in a total of six trials for each participant. Data for the outward direction in one trial for one participant were rejected for technical reasons. Order of route and direction conditions were counterbalanced between participants to control for order effects. Ethical approval for the study was provided by the National Research Ethics Service London-Queen Square Ethics Committee and written informed consent was obtained from all participants.
2.3. Assessment of movement with IMUs
The measurement technique has been outlined previously [21]. IMUs (Xsens, The Netherlands) were attached to the heel of each foot. Local accelerations were converted to the laboratory frame with a 3D rotation matrix, using orthogonal acceleration data combined with roll, pitch and yaw values provided by the IMU software. Stance phases for each foot were identified from resultant acceleration values below 1 m/s2. Initial calculations of foot velocity were performed by integration of the orthogonal acceleration data. Sensor drift was then corrected using zero velocity updates applied to identified stance phases, and the corrected velocity was integrated to provide displacement in three orthogonal directions [18, 19]. A 2D rotation was then applied to the walking paths to align them with the laboratory orientation. Step length and step time were computed for each step during these walking tasks, together with position on the platform. Outlier steps were identified using a statistical method outlined below. For a summary of stages in outlier identification and localisation, see Fig. 2.
2.4. Statistical analyses
All statistical analyses were carried out using Stata V.14. Descriptive statistics for step times and step lengths were calculated by group for each of the corridor configurations (I-, U- and S-shaped), expressed as the mean and standard deviation of the within person means (ignoring journey direction).
To identify and remove outlying long step times (i.e. hesitant steps), an iterative procedure was used as follows. For each of the ‘group by route’ combinations, a three-level linear mixed model that included random person and (within person) journey effects was fitted in order to allow for correlations between repeated measurements from the same subject, and between measurements for the same journey. Outliers with long step times were defined as observations with a standardised residual greater than 3; these outliers were dropped and the model refitted and outlier removal repeated until no further outliers were identified. A similar process was used to identify outlier short step lengths (i.e. steps covering a short distance), defined as those with a standardised residual less than −3.
Having removed outliers, subsequent comparisons of person-specific means by group and by route shape were performed as follows. First, for a given route shape (I-, U- or S), comparisons between participant groups (PCA, tAD, controls) were made: (i) for mean step times using a generalised least squares linear regression that allowed for the apparent different variability of groups; and (ii) for mean step lengths using an ordinary least squares linear regression. In each case, pairwise comparisons were made only if a global test for a difference was statistically significant. Second, for a given participant group (PCA, tAD, controls), comparison across route shapes (I-, U- or S-corridor) were made: (iii) for mean step times and (iv) mean step lengths, using Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-ranks tests. For all study analyses, statistical significance was set at p < 0.05 (two-tailed test). There was no adjustment for multiple testing.
3. Results
3.1. Spatio-temporal gait parameters
Descriptive statistics of the spatio-temporal parameters of gait, excluding outliers as defined in terms of standardised residuals described in ‘Statistical analysis’ above, are shown in Table 1. Within the trials, there was some marked variability of step time and step length, particularly for the U- and S-corridors. This is illustrated in Fig. 3 for step time, in which individual step times for each participant are plotted; there are clearly a small number of outlier values. A total of 37 out of 2395 step times (1.5%) were considered outliers, 12/626 (1.9%), 17/863 (2.0%) and 8/906 (0.9%) for the straight, U- and S-shaped corridors, respectively (see Table 2 for group level outlier numbers for step times and lengths). Outliers were not included in the subsequent statistical analysis of group and route comparisons.
Step length, m | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
I-ROUTE | U-ROUTE | S-ROUTE | Step length comparison | |
controls | 0.738 (0.128) | 0.689 (0.132) | 0.688 (0.127) | U versus I: p = 0.003 |
S versus I: p = 0.005 | ||||
S versus U: p = 0.861 | ||||
PCA | 0.503* (0.101) | 0.405* (0.122) | 0.430* (0.111) | U versus I: p = 0.003 |
S versus I: p = 0.003 | ||||
S versus U: p = 0.131 | ||||
tAD | 0.548* (0.073) | 0.476* (0.143) | 0.482* (0.104) | U versus I: p = 0.013 |
S versus I: p = 0.013 | ||||
S versus U: p = 0.879 |
For each participant group, p-values are for Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-rank tests comparing route shapes. Asterisks denote group differences (versus controls: *p ≤ 0.001).
Step length | |||
---|---|---|---|
I-ROUTE | U-ROUTE | S-ROUTE | |
control | 4/191 (2.1%) | 0/237 (0%) | 1/258 (0.4%) |
PCA | 5/238 (2.1%) | 0/354 (0%) | 0/368 (0%) |
tAD | 7/197 (3.6%) | 0/272 (0%) | 0/280 (0%) |
Table 1.
Step time, s | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
I-ROUTE | U-ROUTE | S-ROUTE | Step time comparison | |
controls | 0.576 (0.052) | 0.588 (0.053) | 0.585 (0.060) | U versus I: p = 0.064 |
S versus I: p = 0.184 | ||||
S versus U: p = 0.780 | ||||
PCA | 0.691* (0.075) | 0.743* (0.090) | 0.724* (0.076) | U versus I: p = 0.016 |
S versus I: p = 0.062 | ||||
S versus U: p = 0.182 | ||||
tAD | 0.730* (0.122) | 0.803* (0.197) | 0.759* (0.153) | U versus I: p = 0.241 |
S versus I: p = 0.647 | ||||
S versus U: p = 0.386 |
Table 2.
Step time | |||
---|---|---|---|
I-ROUTE | U-ROUTE | S-ROUTE | |
control | 8/191 (4.2%) | 2/237 (0.8%) | 1/258 (0.4%) |
PCA | 2/238 (0.8%) | 9/354 (2.5%) | 5/368 (1.4%) |
tAD | 2/197 (1.0%) | 6/272 (2.2%) | 2/280 (0.7%) |
Comparison between groups for each individual route shape showed that mean step time was significantly longer and mean step length significantly shorter when comparing the PCA and tAD groups with controls (p = 0.001 or less), but no significant differences were observed when comparing PCA and tAD. When making comparisons between routes for each participant group, step length when walking along the straight corridor was significantly longer than step length walking along the other corridors for all three groups (p = 0.013 or less), but differences in step times were small and predominantly did not show any statistically significant differences (see Table 1).
3.2. Paths of routes during walking
Paths for all participants in the three groups are shown in Fig. 4, for walking in both the U- and S-shaped corridors. Observed qualitative differences between paths include sharper right-angled turns for patient groups compared to controls, seen particularly at the first corner. This type of behaviour is illustrated in two attached videos, showing animations of the walking paths for one control participant and one person with PCA, walking along the U-shaped corridor.
3.3. Hesitant steps
In Fig. 4, the positions of step times identified as outliers are shown. Clearly, there are some large step times of >2 s for the U-shaped corridor between 2 and 4 m of the individual patient trajectories. 8/139 step times in this region were outliers for the PCA group, and 6/108 step times were outliers for the tAD group. For the S-shaped corridor, 3/111 step times were outliers for the PCA group in the region from 4 to 6 m; for the tAD group there were no outlying step times in this section. For the I-shaped corridor there were very few outliers except for controls where 7/62 step times in 0–2 m region were outliers, all of which occurred very early in the route.
4. Discussion
Rigorous investigation of accessibility and mobility issues in people with physical and/or cognitive impairment should ideally be performed in environments that replicate the ‘real-world’ situations that people experience during activities of daily living. Such environments are difficult to set up in conventional motion capture laboratories using opto-electronic motion capture equipment. In this study, we have been able to create a simple scenario involving participants with different presentations of dementia navigating routes. Findings suggest that IMUs can be used in such environments to provide information on clinically relevant behaviour.
Gait was similar in both PCA and tAD groups. Even excluding outliers, participants with dementia had shorter step lengths and longer step times compared with controls. Differences in step times and length were observed when comparing different routes, in agreement with previous observations for adaptive gait in people with AD [22]. Although measures of gait variability in AD are frequently reported in the literature, the overall lengths of the paths walked by the participants were rather shorter than advised for a statistical description of the distribution of step time and step length data [23]. However, the aim of this study was to understand how deviations in step time and length related to specific locations within the tasks. As such, it was far more useful to identify outlier steps, which could then be considered indicators of hesitation. The criterion for defining an outlier step for an individual was based on that individual's step data, rather than using one fixed absolute value of step time as a threshold. In this way, disproportionately increased step times could be reliably identified and located for each individual.
The data show a consistent pattern of hesitation, determined by step time, for some dementia patients during the tasks. The clustering of hesitant steps at particular locations indicates that the approach of outlier detection may be more informative, as compared to a simple description of gait variability. Although it was possible to identify the location of hesitant steps, it is still uncertain what specific characteristics of that location induced this behaviour. The assessment of visually salient features within the visual field at these locations, combined with investigation of eye fixations could further identify problematic environmental characteristics, leading to the potential for design modifications in the built environment to improve mobility.
Tracking the position of the participants used the principle of dead reckoning, i.e. estimation of position from knowledge of initial position and subsequent movement from that position. However, during testing it was difficult to position some participants on a precisely defined mark at the start of each trial, and so this was not included in the protocol. The starting position varied within a square of ∼60 cm, and this position was not recorded. The plots in Fig. 4 show all participants starting from the same point (0,0). This difference between actual and ideal starting position explains some of the variation in the final positions for the complex corridors tasks. All paths are within the range of permissible paths allowed by the physical constraints of the walls. Although effects of sensor drift on walking paths are possible, we have taken steps to mitigate this using zero velocity update. A guide to the amount of drift in x- and y-positions can be obtained by assessment of drift in the vertical (z) direction, which was typically of the order of 5 cm when comparing the start and end of each trial.
Some of the people with dementia took abrupt turns at the corners. The primary importance of visuomotor control in anticipatory actions for adaptive walking is recognised [24, 25]. Controls were clearly able to anticipate the corners and walked with an efficient smooth path around the corners, but some participants in the dementia groups did not demonstrate the same anticipation. Either they were not able to efficiently interpret or act on the visual information, due to low visual orientation or visuomotor/visual processing impairments, or were unable to predict layouts of routes due to diminished topographical processing, spatial memory and/or executive function [26]. Future investigations might explore the relationship between gait dynamics, spatial memory and route learning abilities. Multi-target stepping tests have shown that young people fixate about three steps ahead, but older adults at risk of falling fixate around the immediate target [27].
It was not possible to use conventional techniques for tracking participants and monitoring gait, because the walls blocked any line-of-sight observation of participants. Overall, a total of 2395 steps were processed. In order to make data processing feasible for such a number of steps, a batch processing approach was used, in which a standard threshold of resultant acceleration was used to define swing and stance phases of gait. Although this may impact on the sensitivity and specificity of the processing of step data, the technique has the potential to be used extensively in the assessment of mobility in complex environments, where the portability of the technique and lack of constraints on the physical infrastructure are significant advantages. It is therefore particularly beneficial to be able to use standardised algorithms to process step data. In the context of research into dementia friendly environments, the effects of perceptual environmental variables, such as lighting, visual cues and clutter on patient navigation can be evaluated in a variety of scenarios [26].
The advantage of using the current sensor-based system to track position and measure gait is that no specific infrastructure is required to perform the measurements. The sensors are portable, and no modification of the local environment is necessary. Within the context of AD research, participant spatio-temporal data collection may be facilitated in environments such as day centre or residential care settings. More generally, the technique is applicable to study mobility in the built environment for people with both physical and cognitive impairment. In summary, this work demonstrates the feasibility of IMUs to be used to perform pedestrian navigation and analysis of spatio-temporal characteristics of gait simultaneously. Both patient groups differed from controls but PCA and tAD patients shared similar spatio-temporal gait characteristics. Identification of outlier step times from standardised residuals allowed the identification of locations where participants tended to hesitate.
5. Conclusion
It was feasible to use IMU sensor technology to assess how people with AD responded to environmental factors (route complexity) when navigating simulated indoor environments. We consider that such an approach is generally applicable to the investigation of mobility in the built environment.
6. Acknowledgments
The authors are grateful to Derrick Boampong, Giulia Barbareschi, Robin Serougne and Nikos Papadosifos for assistance with these measurements.
7. Funding and declarations of interest
This work was supported by an ESRC/NIHR grant no. ES/L001810/1 during this study. The Dementia Research Centre is supported by Alzheimer's Research UK, Brain Research Trust, and The Wolfson Foundation. KY is supported by the Alzheimer's Society. This work was also supported by the NIHR Queen Square Dementia Biomedical Research Unit, and the NIHR UCL/H Biomedical Research Centre.
8 References
- 1.Mendez M.F, Tomsak R.L, Remier B.: ‘Disorders of the visual system in Alzheimer's disease’, J. Clin. Neuroophthalmol., 1990, 10, (1), pp. 62–69 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Frisoni G.B., Fox N.C., Jack C.R., et al. : ‘The clinical use of structural MRI in Alzheimer disease’, Nat. Rev. Neurol., 2010, 6, (2), pp. 67–77 (doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2009.215) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Crutch S.J., Lehmann M., Schott J.M., et al. : ‘Posterior cortical atrophy’, Lancet Neurol., 2012, 11, pp. 170–178 (doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(11)70289-7) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Crutch S.J.: ‘Seeing why they cannot see: understanding the syndrome and causes of posterior cortical atrophy’, J. Neuropsychol., 2013, 8, pp. 157–170 (doi: 10.1111/jnp.12011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Shakespeare T.J., Kaski D., Yong K.X., et al. : ‘Abnormalities of fixation, saccade and pursuit in posterior cortical atrophy’, Brain, 2015, 138, (7), pp. 1976–1991 (doi: 10.1093/brain/awv103) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Tang-Wai D.F., Graff-Radford N.R., Boeve B.F., et al. : ‘Clinical, genetic, and neuropathologic characteristics of posterior cortical atrophy’, Neurology, 2004, 63, (7), pp. 1168–1174 (doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000140289.18472.15) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Gillebert C.R., Schaeverbeke J., Bastin C., et al. : ‘3D shape perception in PCA: a visual neuroscience perspective’, J. Neurosci., 2015, 35, (37), pp. 12673–12692 (doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3651-14.2015) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Montero-Odasso M.: ‘Gait as a biomarker of cognitive impairment and dementia syndromes’, Eur. J. Neurol., 2016, 23, (3), pp. 437–438 (doi: 10.1111/ene.12908) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Rosso A.L., Verghese J., Metti A.L., et al. : ‘Slowing gait and risk for cognitive impairment. The hippocampus as a shared neural substrate’, Neurology, 2017, 89, pp. 336–342 (doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004153) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Allali G., Annweiler C., Blumen H.M., et al. : ‘Gait phenotype from mild cognitive impairment to moderate dementia: results from the GOOD initiative’, Eur. J. Neurol., 2016, 23, (3), pp. 527–541 (doi: 10.1111/ene.12882) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Del Campo N., Payoux P., Djilali A., et al. : ‘Relationship of regional brain β-amyloid to gait speed’, Am. Acad. Neurol., 2016, 86, (1), pp. 36–43 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Castrillo A., Garcia Olmos L.M., Rodriguez F., et al. : ‘Gait disorder in a cohort of patients with mild and moderate Alzheimer's disease’, Am. J. Alzheimers Dis. Other Demen., 2015, 31, (3), pp. 257–262 (doi: 10.1177/1533317515603113) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Nascimbeni A., Caruso S., Salatino A., et al. : ‘Dual task-related gait changes in patients with mild cognitive impairment’, Funct. Neurol., 2015, 30, (1), pp. 59–65 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Cedervall Y., Halvorsen K., Åberg A.C.: ‘A longitudinal study of gait function and characteristics of gait disturbance in individuals with Alzheimer's disease’, Gait Posture, 2014, 39, pp. 1022–1027 (doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2013.12.026) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Monda M., Goldberg A., Smitham P., et al. : ‘Use of inertial measurement units to assess age-related changes in gait kinematics in an active population’, J. Ageing Phys. Activ., 2015, 23, pp. 18–23 (doi: 10.1123/JAPA.2012-0328) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.McCarthy I.D., Suzuki T., Tyler N., et al. : ‘Mobility in the built environment: age-related changes in gait characteristics when walking on complex terrain’, Healthy Ageing Res., 2016, 5, pp. 10–18 [Google Scholar]
- 17.Yoneyama M., Mitoma H., Sanjo N., et al. : ‘Ambulatory gait behavior in patients with dementia: a comparison with Parkinson's disease’, IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng., 2015, 24, (8), pp. 817–826 (doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2015.2477856) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Beauregard S.: ‘Infrastructureless pedestrian positioning’. PhD Thesis, University of Bremen, 2009 [Google Scholar]
- 19.Feliz R., Zalama E., Garcia-Bermejo J.G.: ‘Pedestrian tracking using inertial sensors’, J. Phys. Agents, 2009, 3, (1), pp. 35–43 [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bowes A., Dawson A., Greasley-Adams C., et al. : ‘Design of residential environments for people with dementia and sight loss: a structured literature review’, Br. J. Vis. Impair., 2016, 34, (3), pp. 225–237 (doi: 10.1177/0264619616653991) [Google Scholar]
- 21.McCarthy I.D., Yong K.X., Suzuki T., et al. : ‘Infrastructureless pedestrian navigation to assess the response of Alzheimer's patients to visual cues’. Proc. of the IET Conf. on Technologies for Active and Assisted Living, London, 2015, ISBN 978-1-78561-159-9, doi: 10.1049/ic.2015.0134 [Google Scholar]
- 22.Orcioli-Silva D., Simieli L., Barbieri F.A., et al. : ‘Adaptive walking in Alzheimer's disease’, Int. J. Alzheimer Dis., 2012, pp. 1–6, doi: 10.1155/2012/674589 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Gouelle A., Megrot F., Presado A., et al. : ‘The gait variability index: a new way to quantify fluctuation magnitude of spatiotemporal parameters during gait’, Gait Posture, 2013, 38, pp. 461–465 (doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2013.01.013) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Patla A.E.: ‘How is human gait controlled by vision?’, Ecol. Psychol., 1998, 10, pp. 287–302 (doi: 10.1080/10407413.1998.9652686) [Google Scholar]
- 25.Higuchi T.: ‘Visuomotor control of human adaptive locomotion: understanding the anticipatory nature’, Front. Psychol., 2013, 4, pp. 1–9, doi: 10.3389/fpsych.2013.00277 (doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00277) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Yong K.X., McCarthy I.D., Poole T., et al. : ‘Navigational cue effects in Alzheimer's disease and posterior cortical atrophy’, Ann. Clin. Transl. Neurol., 2018, 5, (6), pp. 697–709 (doi: 10.1002/acn3.566) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Yamada M., Higuchi T., Tanaka B., et al. : ‘Measurements of stepping accuracy in a multitarget stepping task as a potential indicator of fall risk in elderly individuals’, J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci., 2011, 66, pp. 994–1000 (doi: 10.1093/gerona/glr073) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]