Abstract
The term “cerebral torque” refers to opposing right–left asymmetries of frontal and parieto-occipital regions. These are assumed to derive from a lateralized gradient of embryological development of the human brain. To establish the timing of its evolution, we computed and compared the torque, in terms of three principal features, namely petalia, shift, and bending of the inter-hemispheric fissure as well as the inter-hemispheric asymmetry of brain length, height and width for in vivo Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) scans of 91 human and 78 chimpanzee brains. We found that the cerebral torque is specific to the human brain and that its magnitude is independent of brain size and that it comprises features that are inter-related. These findings are consistent with the concept that a “punctuational” genetic change of relatively large effect introduced lateralization in the hominid lineage. The existence of the cerebral torque remains an unsolved mystery and the present study provides further support for this most prominent structural brain asymmetry being specific to the human brain. Establishing the genetic origins of the torque may, therefore, have relevance for a better understanding on human evolution, the organisation of the human brain, and, perhaps, also aspects of the neural basis of language.
Keywords: Cerebral torque, Asymmetry, Speciation, Chimpanzee, Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI)
Introduction
What neural structure has allowed humans to exceed other animals in “cognitive ability”? Two hypotheses have been proposed. First, relative to body size, the human brain is disproportionally larger than the brains of other primates (Rilling 2006; Striedter 2005). Second, whilst the total volume and surface area of the left and right cerebral hemispheres are highly conserved, the human brain typically exhibits an asymmetry in shape that is often referred to as a counter-clockwise torque (Toga and Thompson 2003). The shape asymmetry is proposed as a potential substrate for hemispheric specialization of function, including left hemisphere dominance for language. There have, however, been many reports of asymmetries in the chimpanzee brain; for example, in Broca’s (Cantalupo and Hopkins 2001), Wernicke’s (Gannon et al. 1998) and other areas (Gilissen and Hopkins 2013; Hopkins 2013) and which challenge the second view. Some authors suggested that chimpanzees share the patterns of asymmetry with humans, though to a lesser degree (Gomez-Robles et al. 2013). However, this classical Darwinian concept of gradual transitions over long periods of time cannot explain the gap in functional abilities, particularly relating to language and handedness between humans and chimpanzees. These issues require further investigation using state-of-the-art image analysis techniques.
At the level of individual brain structures, many studies have reported finding a significant asymmetry of the planum temporale in the human brain, with a prevalence ranging between 60 and 83% and a sizable magnitude, such that the left side is of the order one-third larger than the right side (Witelson and Kigar 1988), and which is also supported by micro-anatomical measures (Chance et al. 2006). In an influential study, Gannon et al. (1998) reported detecting a significant leftward asymmetry of the planum temporale in the chimpanzee brain using a technique in which pieces of thin plastic were cut to the size of this structure in fixed post-mortem brains. However, the asymmetric spacing of mini-columns that may account for the surface area asymmetry of planum temporale in human brains has been reported to be absent in chimpanzees (Buxhoeveden et al. 2001; Chance 2014). On the other hand, there is now little convincing evidence that the classic language region known as Broca’s area is asymmetric in either the human or the chimpanzee brain. In particular, based on a review of the literature, Witelson and Kigar (1988) concluded that “there is no evidence of a statistically larger left than right Broca’s region” in the human brains and which was supported by a more recent review of the literature by Keller et al. (2009a) who highlighted the large variation in the anatomical definitions and the inconsistency in methodology. These authors went on to perform a comparative study of Broca’s area in humans and Broca’s area homolog in chimpanzees using unbiased stereological techniques to measure volume on 3D MRI scans obtained in vivo using an identical protocol and did not detect a significant asymmetry for either species (Keller et al. 2009b). The absence of significant asymmetry of Broca’s area in chimpanzee brains has also been reported by Schenker et al. (2010) and Xiang et al. (2018), based on different measures. Thus, whether asymmetry is a principal difference between the human and chimpanzee brain is still to reach consensus. In the present study, we focus on the cerebral torque and add detailed measurements of three main features of the torque to the findings reported in Xiang et al. (2018).
With regard to the torque, an archetypical view of which is depicted in Fig. 2 of Toga and Thompson (2003). There are many reports of it being present in the human brain and which have used a wide range of measurement techniques on CT and MR images acquired both in vivo and in vitro (Bear et al. 1986; Watkins et al. 2001; Weinberger et al. 1982; Good et al. 2001; Barrick et al. 2005). The first study of the cerebral torque in non-human primates was by LeMay (1976). Based on the measurement from the photographs of 28 great ape brains (12 orangutans, 9 chimpanzees, and 7 gorillas), the authors reported that the width of the occipital pole was larger on the left in 11 brains and vice versa in 6 brains, and that the right occipital petalia was present in 11 ape brains and vice versa in two. In an in-vivo MRI study of 31 chimpanzees, Hopkins et al. (2008) applied the VBM technique to explore brain asymmetry and reported rightward asymmetries in the frontal region and leftward asymmetries in the posterior parietal and occipital lobes in this chimpanzee population, although, in the VBM approach, it is difficult to distinguish the asymmetry caused by a potential offset in the position of the cerebral hemispheres along the antero-posterior axis from the asymmetry in the left–right tissue distribution. In an analysis of landmarks on the CT scans of skull endocasts obtained for a sample of extant and fossil specimens including 89 hominins and 110 African great apes (including chimpanzees, gorillas, and bonobos), Balzeau et al. (2012) reported the existence of the petalia in great apes, and concluded that the petalia pattern is not human specific. However, inconsistent observation was reported by Holloway and De La Costelareymondie (1982). In a study of a large collection of 190 hominoid endocasts including specimens for 34 chimpanzees, 40 gorillas, and 41 bonobos, the authors did not find the torque to be present in great apes and concluded that “while true asymmetries of a cortical nature may exist in the extant pongids, their patterns and underlying evolutionary history have not been as strongly selected for”. In a recent in vivo MRI study, Xiang et al. (2018) performed a detailed analysis of 3D positional asymmetries of the surface of the human and chimpanzee brain. The torque and several notable local asymmetries, such as greater depth of the Superior Temporal Sulcus (STS) in the right cerebral hemisphere, were evident in the asymmetry maps of the human brain, but were not present in the chimpanzee brain.
In the present study, the analysis of Xiang et al. (2018) is extended to allow a more detailed study of brain torque from in vivo MRI scans of 91 human and 78 chimpanzee brains. Three aspects of the brain torque have been measured: (1) petalia whereby one cerebral hemisphere protrudes anteriorly and the other posteriorly in the antero-posterior direction, (2) shift whereby one cerebral hemisphere moves upwards anteriorly and downwards posteriorly relative to the other in the dorso-ventral direction, and (3) bending whereby the brain tissue in one hemisphere crosses the midline to displace tissue in the other hemisphere. We find that all three aspects of the torque are specific to the human brain and unrelated to brain size.
Materials and methods
Subjects
3D MRI brain scans of human subjects were acquired at the Edinburgh Imaging facility QMRI, University of Edinburgh, UK, and the Oxford Centre for Magnetic Resonance (OCMR), University of Oxford, UK. Approval was obtained separately at each site from the local Research Ethics Committee and subjects provided fully informed written consent prior to taking part. Altogether, 91 healthy subjects (mean age 33.5 years, 39 females, and 52 males) were included in the study. However, the handedness and the body weight of all the participants are not completely known. For the human subjects recruited in Edinburgh, MR images were obtained using a 3D MPRAGE sequence on a 3 T Verio MRI system (Siemens Medical Systems, Erlangen, Germany). Acquisition parameters were TR = 2300 ms, TE = 2.98 ms, TI = 900 ms, Flip angle = 9°, and FOV = 256 mm × 256 mm with an isotropic voxel resolution of 1 mm. For the human subjects recruited in Oxford, MR images were obtained using a 3D fast low-angle shot (FLASH) sequence on a Sonata 1.5 T MR system (Siemens Medical, Erlangen, Germany). Acquisition parameters were TR = 5400 ms, TE = 76 ms, Flip angle = 90°, 256 × 160 slice matrix comprising 208 contiguous slices with an isotropic voxel resolution of 1 mm. MRI scanning of the 78 chimpanzees (50 females and 28 males) which weighted an average of 64 kg was performed at Yerkes National Primate Research Centre (YNPRC) in Atlanta, Georgia, US. Chimpanzees were immobilized by ketamine injection (10 mg/kg) and subsequently anesthetized with propofol (40–60 mg/kg/h) before transportation to the MRI facility where they remained anesthetized (total time ~ 2 h) for the MR imaging and return to the home compound. Chimpanzees were scanned supine with a human head coil. T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid-acquisition gradient echo (MPRAGE) MR images were obtained using a Siemens 3 T Trio MR system. Acquisition parameters were TR = 2300 ms, TE = 4.4 ms, TI = 1100 ms, flip angle = 8, and FOV = 200 mm 9200 mm. The data matrix size was 320 × 320 with an isotropic voxel resolution of 0.6 mm.
Image processing
All MR images were pre-processed in FSL (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/) including skull strip, bias field correction, and brain normalization using 7 degrees of freedom transformations (i.e., 3 translations, 3 rotations, and 1 uniform scaling) without distorting the morphological shape of brains, and were analyzed using the standard FreeSurfer processing pipeline (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/), in which the surface-based module enables high-quality cerebral surface reconstruction from the brain volume data with subvoxel accuracy (Dale et al. 1999). The processing pipeline has been described in details elsewhere (Hopkins et al. 2017). Based on a method available in FreeSurfer (Schaer et al. 2008), the outer surface of the cerebrum was extracted from a smoothed cerebral hemisphere volume, on which the morphologic closing operation was applied to fill the sulci. The computation of brain dimensions and measurement of the cerebral torque was performed for this surface.
Despite the fact that the 3D MRI brain image has already been normalized to the MNI coordinate system in FSL, the low-dimensional linear registration used in the normalization step is often insufficient to align the inter-hemispheric fissure to the x = 0 plane that is commonly considered as the midline plane of the brain (Good et al. 2001; Kennedy et al. 1999; Lyttelton et al. 2009; Watkins et al. 2001). The deviation of the two planes is likely to affect the computation of brain asymmetry as well as the brain dimensions. To improve the accuracy of the measurements, a middle-sagittal plane (MSP) was computed based on the central portion of the brain where deviation is minimal. To achieve this, the MSP was estimated as the plane that best fits the vertices on the medial surface of the brain. The brain orientation was refined by rotating the brain surface with an angle between the plane x = 0 and the estimated MSP [Fig. 1, column 1, a more detailed description was given in Xiang et al. (2018)].
The dimensions (i.e., antero-posterior length, dorso-ventral height, and latero-medial width) of the brain as a whole and of the individual cerebral hemispheres were measured as the dimensions of the smallest orthogonal parallelepiped (i.e., so-called “bounding-box”) that could be constructed to enclose the smooth outer surface of each cerebral hemisphere with edges parallel to the three axes of the MNI coordinate system. The uniform scaling factor previously computed with FSL was applied to correct the measured dimensions to the true dimensions in native space. The ratios of the linear measures were also computed (i.e., length/width and height/width). The dimensional asymmetries were defined as the difference (i.e., L–R) of the linear measurements of length, height and width between the left and right cerebral hemispheres.
The frontal and occipital poles were computed as the most extreme points on each cerebral hemisphere along the antero-posterior axis and the description petalia refers to the respective displacements of the left and right frontal and occipital poles along this axis (Fig. 1, column 2) and shift as the corresponding displacements along the dorso-ventral axis (Fig. 1, column 3). Bending was computed as the angles between the best-fitting plane and plane x = 0 in the frontal and occipital regions. The procedure can be sub-divided into the following steps: (1) compute the surface normal and the angle θ between x-axis and the estimated normal at each cerebral surface vertex, (2) identify the vertices that belong to the medial cerebral surface as those associated with angles θ less than 40° (vertices on the lateral cerebral surface normally have larger angles, i.e., θ > 40°). From these vertices, identify those in the first (frontal region) and last quarters (occipital region) of the brain along the antero-posterior direction and (3) compute the least-squares planes that best fit the points belonging to the frontal and occipital regions, respectively, and compute the frontal and occipital bending as the angles between the normal of individual planes and the x-axis in associated regions (Fig. 1, column 4).
Statistical analysis
Multiple analysis of variance (MANOVA) was applied to determine potential species and sex differences in brain size. The brain dimensions (i.e., length, height, and width) are dependent variables and sex and species between group factors. For individual species, two-tailed one-sample t tests were applied to measure the asymmetry of each brain variable under the null hypothesis that the difference between the left and right hemispheres is equal to zero. The prevalence of the pattern of asymmetries was compared between species using a Chi-squared two-sample test. Findings were considered statistically significant at p < 0.01.
Results
The linear measurements of the brain of both human and chimpanzee brains are presented in Table 1. Based on the linear measurement of the whole cerebral surface, the human brain is significantly longer (95% CI for the effect of species is 62–66 mm), higher (95% CI is 41–44 mm), and wider (95% CI is 45–48 mm) than that of the chimpanzee brain by factors of 1.57, 1.57 and 1.54, respectively. Within each species, the ratio of length, height, and width is 1.31:0.88:1 in humans and 1.28:0.86:1 in chimpanzees, respectively. MANOVA [F(2,166) = 4.96, p = 0.008] based on the ratios between length and height to width and subsequent ANOVA confirmed relatively greater expansion in the antero-posterior [F(1,167) = 9.79, p = 0.002] and dorso-ventral [F(1,167) = 5.47, p = 0.02] directions compared to the lateral–medial direction in humans. Sex effects were present in both humans [F(3,87) = 24.22, p < 0.0005] and chimpanzees [F(3,74) = 7.16, p < 0.0005], although the normalized difference between the sexes is comparatively smaller in the chimpanzee than in the human brain.
Table 1.
(mm) | Left hemisphere | Right hemisphere | Whole cerebral surface | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Length | Height | Width | Length | Height | Width | Length | Height | Width | Length/Width | Height/Width | |
Humans | |||||||||||
All | 173.8 ± 7.5 | 116.0 ± 5.2 | 70.3 ± 3.2 | 172.9 ± 7.4 | 116.8 ± 5.1 | 70.0 ± 3.5 | 174.9 ± 7.6 | 117.7 ± 5.0 | 134.0 ± 5.6 | 1.31 | 0.88 |
Male | 177.0 ± 7.2 | 117.8 ± 4.9 | 71.9 ± 2.9 | 176.1 ± 7.0 | 118.5 ± 5.1 | 71.6 ± 2.9 | 178.2 ± 7.1 | 119.5 ± 4.7 | 137.1 ± 4.6 | 1.30 | 0.87 |
Female | 169.6 ± 5.6 | 113.7 ± 4.7 | 68.1 ± 2.2 | 168.7 ± 5.6 | 114.5 ± 4.3 | 67.4 ± 2.6 | 170.5 ± 5.7 | 115.3 ± 4.2 | 129.9 ± 3.9 | 1.31 | 0.89 |
Chimpanzees | |||||||||||
All | 110.4 ± 4.2 | 74.5 ± 3.6 | 45.1 ± 2.1 | 110.4 ± 4.2 | 74.4 ± 3.8 | 45.1 ± 2.1 | 111.1 ± 4.1 | 75.1 ± 3.7 | 87.2 ± 3.8 | 1.28 | 0.86 |
Male | 112.6 ± 4.5 | 75.8 ± 3.9 | 45.9 ± 1.9 | 112.4 ± 4.5 | 75.8 ± 4.1 | 45.8 ± 1.7 | 113.3 ± 4.4 | 76.4 ± 3.9 | 88.6 ± 3.3 | 1.28 | 0.86 |
Female | 109.2 ± 3.5 | 73.8 ± 3.3 | 44.6 ± 2.1 | 109.2 ± 3.6 | 73.6 ± 3.5 | 44.7 ± 2.2 | 109.9 ± 3.5 | 74.4 ± 3.4 | 86.4 ± 3.9 | 1.27 | 0.86 |
For each subject, the length, height and width of the whole brain and the two cerebral hemispheres were measured based on three bounding-boxes constructed independently for the relevant brain surfaces. For the human brain, the normalized difference in brain size between the sexes is between 3 and 5%, while, for the chimpanzee, the difference between the sexes is between 2 and 3%
The brain asymmetry measures are presented in Table 2 and the prevalence of the torque in each species can be found in Table 3. According to Table 2 (left panel), the left hemisphere of the human brain was significantly longer [t(90) = 4.77, p < 0.0005] and less high [t(90) = − 3.30, p = 0.001] than the right hemisphere; however, there was no significant global width asymmetry. For chimpanzees, no significant asymmetry was present in any of the dimensions.
Table 2.
Asymmetry (L–R) | Torque | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(mm) | Length | Height | Width | Petalia | Shift | Bending | |||
Anterior | Posterior | Anterior | Posterior | Anterior | Posterior | ||||
Humans | |||||||||
AVG | 0.92 ± 1.84 | − 0.73 ± 2.11 | 0.45 ± 2.74 | − 0.67 | − 1.58 | − 0.57 | − 1.30 | 0.04 | 3.63 |
t(90) | 4.77 | − 3.30 | 1.56 | − 4.94 | − 7.69 | − 0.89 | − 2.66 | 0.22 | 6.65 |
p (two-tail) | < 0.0005 | 0.001 | 0.123 | < 0.0005 | < 0.0005 | 0.38 | 0.01 | 0.82 | < 0.0005 |
Chimpanzees | |||||||||
AVG | 0.02 ± 1.38 | 0.11 ± 1.45 | − 0.03 ± 1.11 | − 0.18 | − 0.20 | − 0.77 | 0.25 | − 0.35 | − 0.39 |
t(77) | 0.14 | 0.69 | − 0.25 | − 1.80 | − 1.59 | − 1.62 | 0.65 | − 1.76 | − 1.14 |
p (two-tail) | 0.89 | 0.49 | 0.81 | 0.08 | 0.12 | 0.11 | 0.52 | 0.08 | 0.26 |
The aspects of cerebral asymmetry that proved to be significant with a significance level of 0.01 are highlighted in italics
Table 3.
Petalia | Shift | Bending | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Frontal | Frontal | Frontal | |||||||
LF (%) | RF (%) | LF (%) | RF (%) | LF (%) | RF (%) | ||||
Human | |||||||||
Occipital | |||||||||
LO | 20.88 | 60.44 | 81.32% | 23.08 | 39.56 | 62.64% | 14.29 | 7.69 | 21.98% |
RO | 9.89 | 8.79 | 18.68% | 20.88 | 16.48 | 37.36% | 30.77 | 47.25 | 78.02% |
30.77 | 69.23 | 43.96 | 56.04 | 45.05 | 54.95 | ||||
Chimpanzee | |||||||||
Occipital | |||||||||
LO | 21.79 | 30.77 | 52.56% | 19.23 | 24.36 | 43.59% | 23.08 | 34.62 | 57.69% |
RO | 17.95 | 29.49 | 47.44% | 21.79 | 34.62 | 56.41% | 16.67 | 25.64 | 42.31% |
39.74 | 60.26 | 41.03 | 58.97 | 39.74 | 60.26 |
Both the frontal and occipital petalia (see Table 2) were highly significant in humans [t(90) = − 4.94, p < 0.0005 and t(90) = − 7.69, p < 0.0005, respectively], whereas no significant asymmetry was found in either petalia in chimpanzees. According to the data distribution in Fig. 2, the combination of right-frontal and left-occipital petalia was most typical in humans (60%) consistent with the presence of the torque (Best 1988). In comparison, the configuration of the petalia was randomly distributed in chimpanzees (31%), which was significantly different to humans (Chi = 14.85, p < 0.0005). In addition, the left-occipital pole of the human brain was shifted significantly downward relative to the right [t(90) = − 2.66, p = 0.01], which was not the case in the chimpanzee brain. No significant relative shift can be found at the frontal pole in both species. With regard to bending, humans showed a significant rightward occipital bending t(90) = 6.65, p < 0.0005, which was not found in chimpanzees. By the Chi-squared test, the distribution of occipital bending differed significantly between humans and chimpanzees (Chi = 22.63, p < 0.0005). There was no significant sex effect found in asymmetries in either species.
Total intracranial volume (1.54 ± 0.23 dm3) computed using the FreeSurfer image analysis pipeline was significantly correlated with the product of length, height and width measures (r = 0.67, p < 0.0005). The correlations between asymmetries and this volume are, however, not significant (r = − 0.03, p = 0.77 in length, r = 0.02, p = 0.81 in height, r = − 0.02, p = 0.86 in width, r = − 0.08, p = 0.46 in frontal petalia, r = − 0.02, p = 0.83 in occipital petalia, r = 0.11, p = 0.29 in occipital bending, and r = 0.11, p = 0.28 in occipital shift). Pairwise Pearson’s correlations indicate that length asymmetry of the cerebral hemispheres in the human cohort is highly positively correlated with occipital petalia (r = 0.77, p < 0.0005) and negatively correlated with frontal petalia although with a lesser correction coefficient (r = − 0.25, p = 0.02). The occipital petalia was significantly more prominent than the frontal petalia t(90) = 4.77, p < 0.0005. In addition, a significant correlation was observed between length asymmetry and occipital bending (r = 0.57, p < 0.0005), and between occipital petalia and occipital bending (r = 0.63, p < 0.0005).
Discussion
Two main findings distinguish the human from the chimpanzee brain: (1) larger brain size with preferential expansion in the antero-posterior and dorso-ventral axes relative to the left–right axis and (2) the torque whereby the right-frontal and left-occipital poles are more prominent (i.e., petalia) than the contralateral poles; the left-occipital pole is shifted downward (i.e., shift) compared to the right and bends towards the right side (i.e., bending) and dimensional asymmetries whereby the left hemisphere is elongated and with reduced height compared to the right. The asymmetries are independent of brain size. To our knowledge, this is the first study to systemically examine the torque, particularly the “shift” and “bending”, in living human and great ape brains as well as the relationship of asymmetries to linear brain size.
Most previous linear measures of the irregularly shaped brain have necessarily been obtained on arbitrarily defined 2D sections (Turkheimer 1989). In the present study, all the brain measures were computed from the 3D brain surface obtained in vivo from 91 humans and 78 chimpanzees using MRI. The linear measurements of the brain of humans and chimpanzees were found to be highly correlated with brain volume (p < 0.0005), in line with the proposition that linear brain dimensions are simple and reliable indices of brain size (Gomori et al. 1984; Hamano et al. 1993; Reinard et al. 2015) on an evolutionary timescale.
The average body mass of the captive chimpanzee subjects in this study is 64 ± 15 kg (much heavier than the body mass of wild chimpanzee reported in Pusey et al. 2004, i.e., 39.0 kg for males and 31.3 kg for females). The weight of the human subjects was unfortunately not recorded, however the average weight of the European population is 71 kg (Walpole et al. 2012). Thus, according to the linear measurements, the human brain is disproportionately large relative to the chimpanzee with regard to body weight. The phylogenetic increase in brain size is allometric. Greater growth was found in the antero-posterior direction, which is the principal direction of neuroanatomical diversity in the central nervous system (Best 1988; Gilles et al. 1983). The greater growth in this direction accords with relatively speedy prenatal (Sakai et al. 2012) and prolonged postnatal (Owen 1859) growth. Because greater brain size is associated with more neural tissue, and thus greater processing capacity, an increase of brain size has been considered as the basis of the emergence of human intelligence (Jerison 1973; Pilbeam and Gould 1974; Tramo et al. 1998), tool making (Ko 2016), and language (Lenneberg 1967b). However, uniform change in size alone cannot account for the specific human ability to acquire language as brain size is substantially reduced in nanocephalic dwarfism (Lenneberg 1967a; Seckel 1960) without consistent impairment of the capacity for language. Nor have elephants evolved a linguistic competence parallel to or exceeding our own; nevertheless, the singularity of the elephant’s trunk has been discussed as a parallel to human language in evolutionary theory (Pinker 1994).
The primary finding of the study is the uniqueness of asymmetries in the human brain. Petalia were found to be significant in both frontal and occipital regions (p < 0.01) in the human brain, indicating an overall posterior sheer of the left hemisphere compared to the right. The pattern of right-frontal and left-occipital protrusion appears in 60% of the human population, lying in the range of frequencies reported by two studies of endocasts, namely 79% by Holloway and De La Costelareymondie (1982) and 44% by Balzeau et al. (2012). In contrast to humans, the results relating to great apes have been inconsistent. Holloway and De La Costelareymondie (1982) reported a prevalence of petalia of 12% in great apes and a significantly different petalia patterns between humans and great apes was confirmed by Chi-square statistics. Balzeau et al. (2012) observed a higher prevalence of right-frontal and left-occipital pattern of petalia in great apes at 35% that is comparable with the cases in humans (44%) and significant left-occipital petalia in both great apes (p < 0.05) and humans (p < 0.01). On this basis, the authors concluded that this species shared a similar pattern of asymmetries with humans. In our study, the prevalence of the petalia pattern in chimpanzee is closer to the latter study at 31%. However, according to t tests of directional asymmetry, neither the right-frontal (p < 0.0005) or left-occipital (p < 0.0005) petalia found in human cohort was significant in the chimpanzee cohort (p > 0.05), in addition to which the Chi-square test for difference in prevalence between human and chimpanzee points to the same conclusion as drawn by Holloway and De La Costelareymondie (1982) and Zilles et al. (1996) that petalia are not present in chimpanzees on a population level. In addition to petalia, a significant downward shift (p = 0.01) and rightward bending (p < 0.0005) of the occipital lobes was found in the human brain but not in the chimpanzee brain. The lack of directional bending in chimpanzee brains is in line with a study by Hou et al. (2018), in which the unique occipital bending in humans was found to be associated with asymmetry in Sylvian Fissure that is also unique to humans. The elongation and reduced height of the left hemisphere observed in only human brains correlates with occipital petalia and bending, and is compatible with the posterior extension of the temporo-occipital region and the greater length of the Sylvian Fissure in the left hemisphere (Cunningham 1892; Rubens et al. 1976). In the previous studies, the torque is revealed by corresponding asymmetries in the skull (Balzeau and Gilissen 2010; Balzeau et al. 2012) or in gross volumetric measures of frontal and occipital regions (Watkins et al. 2001; Weinberger et al. 1982) without separating different features of the torque from each other (Chance et al. 2005). In this study, a comparison is directly made between 3D MRI scans obtained in vivo using an identical protocol for groups of humans and chimpanzees with reasonable sample size. The automated analysis procedures avoid the subjective judgment of an operator who has knowledge of the hypothesis being tested. Furthermore, tests of variables that have not previously been examined are included in the present study. In particular, detailed measurements have been obtained for three prominent features of the cerebral torque (i.e., petalia, shift, and bending).
The non-significant correlations between brain size and asymmetries suggest that the factors influencing brain size and cerebral asymmetry may have independent phylogenetic origins. Thus, the absence of asymmetry in the chimpanzee brain in the present study is not related to smaller brain size. Instead, the absence of asymmetry is interpreted as reflecting a difference in the phylogenetic history since the separation of the species. This is supported by reports, from analysis of photographs, of length asymmetry of fetal and newborn brains (LeMay 1976) and corticospinal tract asymmetry in fetal and neonatal autopsy material (Yakovlev and Rakic 1966).
The mechanisms underlying the development of brain asymmetry are not well understood. Two theories have been proposed. Some (Crow 1993, 1994) have hypothesized that brain asymmetry appeared as a late and “punctuational” step in the human phylogeny, initiated by a genetic mutation and is a potential anatomical correlate of language. Others however, have proposed that asymmetry is simply an organizing principle “providing for more efficient programming through the allocation of different functions to the two cerebral hemispheres” as a result of increasing brain size (Corballis 2010). The fact that cerebral asymmetry is absent from the brain of the chimpanzee and is un-correlated with brain size is more readily reconciled with a saltational, i.e., relatively abrupt genetic origin (Annett 1985), in the hominid lineage than as a cross-specific adaptation of generalized mammalian neuro-development.
The existence of torque is consistent with the lateralized neuro-embryologic development model proposed by Best (1988), in which maturation of the brain occurs along a 3D diagonal growth vector running from ventral right-frontal motor and primary sensory areas to dorsal left-posterior and tertiary association areas. The more striking asymmetry in the posterior region than the anterior is in line with Best’s (1988) prediction that “the gross morphologic effect of earlier-emerging right-frontal-motor regions may become attenuated by the later, left-biased growth of the tertiary association cortex in the prefrontal region”. The between species difference in cerebral torque also appears to be greater in the occipital lobe. The symmetry of the chimpanzee brain suggests a difference in neuro-development compared to humans. Although consideration of its specific effects on brain function and cognition are beyond the scope of the current study, the unique existence of torque in the human brain points to a correlate to the high-level cognitive ability of human being.
There are two main limitations of the study. Firstly, the lack of complete handedness information in human participants restricts exploration of the association between handedness and asymmetries which was previously reported in (Zilles et al. 1996; LeMay 1976). However, in a recent study, Kong et al. (2018) did not find a significant relationship between brain asymmetry and handedness in a large human cohort of 17,141 healthy individuals. Secondly, despite the major advantages of being able to compare 3D MRI scans obtained in vivo for cohorts of humans and chimpanzees, it is to be acknowledged that the published studies of endocasts refer to natural populations with a wide geographic range and greater sample sizes as compared to the captive Yerkes chimpanzee group. The findings of the present study are, however, in line with the observations by Holloway and De La Costelareymondie (1982) based on endocasts of great apes including gorilla, bonobo, and chimpanzees. Besides, it is also worth mentioning that the measurements reported in the present study are not identical to, for example, measurements obtained using manual techniques such as caliper width and length (LeMay 1976), planimetric approaches for Region of Interest (ROI) analyses (Hopkins et al. 1998; Hopkins and Marino 2000), computer-based techniques such as voxel-based morphometry (VBM) (Hopkins et al. 2008), or analysis of landmarks on CT scans of endocasts (Balzeau and Gilissen 2010; Balzeau et al. 2012). In future work, it will be interesting to compare the application of the different methods to obtain measurements for the same database and which will provide a more detailed description of the phenomenon referred to as the torque.
In conclusion, we have shown that cerebral torque and asymmetries in the dimensions of the cerebral hemispheres are specific human attributes that are independent of brain size, and perhaps, a consequence of a “macro-mutation” during human evolution (Annett 1985; Crow 1993, 1994) that led to the development of a three-dimensional maturation gradient in the embryonic brain (Best 1988).
Acknowledgements
We thank Dr. William D. Hopkins for access to MRI scans of chimpanzees at the Yerkes National Primate Centre and Dr. Stephen Lawrie for access to MRI scans of humans at the University of Edinburgh and staff at the University of Oxford for help in scanning procedures and for access to the series of MRI scans from normal human subjects. This research was supported by the T.J. Crow Psychosis Research Trust.
Author contributions
XL devised the methods for analyzing cerebral asymmetry on 3D MR images, applied the methods to all data sets, conducted statistical analyses, and prepared Figures. TJC and NR devised and planned the project, co-ordinated analysis, and wrote the paper with XL. All authors reviewed and approved the paper.
Ethical approval
This manuscript is complied with Ethical Standards. All authors have been personally and actively involved in substantive work leading to the manuscript, and will hold themselves jointly and individually responsible for its content. Ethical approval was obtained from the local Research Ethics Committee where brain images were scanned.
Conflict of interest
We have no conflicts of interest to disclose and confirm that the manuscript is not under consideration for publication elsewhere.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- Annett M. Left, right, hand and brain: the right shift theory. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Balzeau A, Gilissen E. Endocranial shape asymmetries in pan paniscus, pan troglodytes and gorilla gorilla assessed via skull based landmark analysis. J Hum Evol. 2010;59:54–69. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2010.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balzeau A, Gilissen E, Grimaud-Herve D. Shared pattern of endocranial shape asymmetries among great apes, anatomically modern humans, and fossil hominins. PLoS One. 2012;7(1):e29581. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrick TR, Mackay CE, Prima S, Maes F, Vandermeulen D, Crow TJ, Roberts N. Automatic analysis of cerebral asymmetry: an exploratory study of the relationship between brain torque and planum temporale asymmetry. NeuroImage. 2005;24(3):678–691. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bear D, Schiff D, Saver J, Greenberg M, Freeman R. Quantitative analysis of cerebral asymmetries. Fronto-occipital correlation, sexual dimorphism and association with handedness. Arch Neurol. 1986;43(6):598–603. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1986.00520060060019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Best CT. The emergence of cerebral asymmetries in early human development: a literature review and a neuroembryological model. In: Molfese DL, Segalowitz SJ, editors. Brain lateralization in children. New York: Guilford Press; 1988. pp. 5–34. [Google Scholar]
- Buxhoeveden DP, Switala AE, Litaker M, Roy E, Casanova MF. Lateralization of minicolumns in human planum temporale is absent in nonhuman primate cortex. Brain Behav Evol. 2001;57:349–358. doi: 10.1159/000047253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cantalupo C, Hopkins WD. Asymmetric broca’s area in great apes. Nature. 2001;414(6863):505. doi: 10.1038/35107134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chance SA. The cortical microstructural basis of lateralized cognition: a review. Front Psychol. 2014;5:820. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00820. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chance SA, Esiri MM, Crow TJ. Macroscopic brain asymmetry is changed along the antero-posterior axis in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2005;74(2–3):163–170. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chance SA, Casanova MF, Switala AE, Crow TJ. Minicolumnar structure in Heschl’s gyrus and planum temporale: asymmetries in relation to sex and callosal fiber number. Neuroscience. 2006;143:1041–1050. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.08.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corballis MC. Handedness and cerebral asymmetry: an evolutionary perspective. In: Westerhausen KHaR., editor. The two halves of the brain: information processing in the cerebral hemispheres. Cambridge: The MIT press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Crow TJ. Sexual selection, machiavellian intelligence, and the origins of psychosis. Lancet. 1993;342(8871):594–598. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(93)91415-i. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crow TJ. The case for an x–y homologous determinant of cerebral asymmetry. Cytogenet Cell Genet. 1994;67:393–394. [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham DJ (1892) Contribution to the surface anatomy of the cerebral hemispheres. Cunningham Mem (R Ir Acad). 7(372)
- Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis I: segmentation and surface reconstruction. NeuroImage. 1999;9(2):179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gannon PJ, Holloway RL, Broadfield DC, Braun AR. Asymmetry of chimpanzee planum temporale: humanlike pattern of wernicke’s brain language area homolog. Science. 1998;279(5348):220–222. doi: 10.1126/science.279.5348.220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilissen EP, Hopkins WD. Asymmetries of the parietal operculum in chimpanzees (pan troglodytes) in relation to handedness for tool use. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23(2):411–422. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilles FH, Leviton A, Dooling EC. In: The developing human brain: growth and epidemiologic neuropathology. Dooling EC, Gilles FH, editors. Boston: John Wright, PSG, Inc; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Gomez-Robles A, Hopkins WD, Sherwood CC. Increased morphological asymmetry, evolvability and plasticity in human brain evolution. Proc Biol Sci. 2013;280(1761):20130575. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.0575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gomori JM, Steiner I, Melamed E, Cooper G. The assessment of changes in brain volume using combined linear measurements. A CT-scan study. Neuroradiology. 1984;26(1):21–24. doi: 10.1007/BF00328197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Good CD, Johnsrude I, Ashburner J, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS. Cerebral asymmetry and the effects of sex and handedness on brain structure: a voxel-based morphometric analysis of 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage. 2001;14(3):685–700. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamano K, Iwasaki N, Takeya T, Takita H. A comparative study of linear measurement of the brain and three-dimensional measurement of brain volume using CT scans. Pediatr Radiol. 1993;23(3):165–168. doi: 10.1007/BF02013822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holloway RL, De La Costelareymondie MC. Brain endocast asymmetry in pongids and hominids: some preliminary findings on the paleontology of cerebral dominance. Am J Phys Anthropol. 1982;58(1):101–110. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.1330580111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopkins WD. Neuroanatomical asymmetries and handedness in chimpanzees (pan troglodytes): a case for continuity in the evolution of hemispheric specialization. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2013;1288:17–35. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopkins WD, Marino L. Asymmetries in cerebral width in nonhuman primate brains as revealed by magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(4):493–499. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(99)00090-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopkins WD, Marino L, Rilling JK, MacGregor LA. Planum temporale asymmetries in great apes as revealed by magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) NeuroReport. 1998;9:2913–2918. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199808240-00043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopkins WD, Taglialatela JP, Meguerditchian A, Nir T, Schenker NM, Sherwood CC. Gray matter asymmetries in chimpanzees as revealed by voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage. 2008;42(2):491–497. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopkins WD, Li X, Crow T, Roberts N. Vertex- and atlas-based comparisons in measures of cortical thickness, gyrification and white matter volume between humans and chimpanzees. Brain Struct Funct. 2017;222(1):229–245. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016-1213-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hou L, Xiang L, Crow TJ, Leroy F, Rivièred D, Mangind J-F, Roberts N. Measurement of sylvian fissure asymmetry and occipital bending in humans and Pan troglodytes. Neuroimage (forthcoming) 2018 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jerison H. Evolution of the brain and intelligence. New York: Elsevier Inc; 1973. [Google Scholar]
- Keller SS, Crow T, Foundas A, Amunts K, Roberts N. Broca’s area: nomenclature, anatomy, typology and asymmetry. Brain Lang. 2009;109(1):29–48. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keller SS, Roberts N, Hopkins W. A comparative magnetic resonance imaging study of the anatomy, variability, and asymmetry of broca’s area in the human and chimpanzee brain. J Neurosci. 2009;29(46):14607–14616. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2892-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kennedy DN, O’Craven KM, Ticho BS, Goldstein AM, Makris N, Henson JW. Structural and functional brain asymmetries in human situs inversus totalis. Neurology. 1999;53(6):1260–1265. doi: 10.1212/wnl.53.6.1260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ko KH. Origins of human intelligence: the chain of tool-making and brain evolution. Anthropol Notebooks. 2016;22(1):5–22. [Google Scholar]
- Kong XZ, Mathias SR, Guadalupe T, Glahn DC, Franke B, Crivello F, Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Fisher SE, Thompson PM, Francks C. Mapping cortical brain asymmetry in 17,141 healthy individuals worldwide via the enigma consortium. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2018;115(22):E5154–E5163. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1718418115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeMay M. Morphological cerebral asymmetries of modern man, fossil man, and nonhuman primate. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1976;280:349–366. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1976.tb25499.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lenneberg EH. Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley; 1967. [Google Scholar]
- Lenneberg EH. The biological foundations of language. Hosp Pract. 1967;2(12):59–67. [Google Scholar]
- Lyttelton OC, Karama S, Ad-Dab’bagh Y, Zatorre RJ, Carbonell F, Worsley K, Evans AC. Positional and surface area asymmetry of the human cerebral cortex. Neuroimage. 2009;46(4):895–903. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Owen R. Contributions to the natural history of the anthropoid apes. no. Viii. On the external characters of the gorilla (trogloodytes gorilla, sav.) Trans Zool Soc. 1859;5:243–283. [Google Scholar]
- Pilbeam D, Gould SJ. Size and scaling in human evolution. Science. 1974;186(4167):892–901. doi: 10.1126/science.186.4167.892. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinker S. The language instinct. New York, NY, US: William Morrow & Co; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Pusey AE, Oehlert GW, Williams JM, Goodall J. Influence of ecological and social factors on body mass of wild chimpanzees. Int J Primatol. 2004;26:3–31. [Google Scholar]
- Reinard K, Basheer A, Phillips S, Snyder A, Agarwal A, Jafari-Khouzani K, Soltanian-Zadeh H, Schultz L, Aho T, Schwalb JM. Simple and reproducible linear measurements to determine ventricular enlargement in adults. Surg Neurol Int. 2015;6:59. doi: 10.4103/2152-7806.154777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rilling JK. Human and non-human primate brains: are they allometrically scaled versions of the same design? Evol Anthropol. 2006;15(2):65–77. [Google Scholar]
- Rubens AB, Mahowald MW, Hutton JT. Asymmetry of the lateral (sylvian) fissures in man. Neurology. 1976;26(7):620–624. doi: 10.1212/wnl.26.7.620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakai T, Hirata S, Fuwa K, Sugama K, Kusunoki K, Makishima H, Eguchi T, Yamada S, Ogihara N, Takeshita H. Fetal brain development in chimpanzees versus humans. Curr Biol. 2012;22(18):R791–R792. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.06.062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaer M, Cuadra MB, Tamarit L, Lazeyras F, Eliez S, Thiran JP. A surface-based approach to quantify local cortical gyrification. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2008;27(2):161–170. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2007.903576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schenker NM, Hopkins WD, Spocter MA, Garrison AR, Stimpson CD, Erwin JM, Sherwood CC. Broca’s area homologue in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): probabilistic mapping, asymmetry, and comparison to humans. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20(3):730–742. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seckel HPG. Bird-headed dwarfs: studies in developmental anthropology, including human proportions. Springfield: C Thomas; 1960. [Google Scholar]
- Striedter GF. Principles of brain evolution. Sunderland, MA, US: Sinauer Associates; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Toga AW, Thompson PM. Mapping brain asymmetry. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4(1):37–48. doi: 10.1038/nrn1009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tramo MJ, Loftus WC, Stukel TA, Green RL, Weaver JB, Gazzaniga MS. Brain size, head size, and intelligence quotient in monozygotic twins. Neurology. 1998;50(5):1246–1252. doi: 10.1212/wnl.50.5.1246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turkheimer E. Techniques of quantitative measurement of morphological structures of the central nervous system. In: Bigler ED, Yeo RA, Turkheimer E, editors. Neuropsychological function and brain imaging. US, Boston: Springer; 1989. pp. 47–64. [Google Scholar]
- Walpole SC, Prieto-Merino D, Edwards P, Cleland J, Stevens G, Roberts I. The weight of nations: an estimation of adult human biomass. BMC Public Health. 2012;12:439. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-12-439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watkins KE, Paus T, Lerch JP, Zijdenbos A, Collins DL, Neelin P, Taylor J, Worsley KJ, Evans AC. Structural asymmetries in the human brain: a voxel-based statistical analysis of 142 MRI scans. Cereb Cortex. 2001;11(9):868–877. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.9.868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinberger DR, Luchins DJ, Morihisa J, Wyatt RJ. Asymmetrical volumes of the right and left frontal and occipital regions of the human brain. Ann Neurol. 1982;11(1):97–100. doi: 10.1002/ana.410110118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Witelson SF, Kigar DL (1988) Asymmetry of brain function follows asymmetry in anatomical form: gross, microscopic, postmortem and imaging studies. In: Boller F, Grafman J (eds) Handbook of Neuropsychology. Elsevier, pp 111–142
- Xiang L, Crow TJ, Hopkins WD, Gong Q, Roberts N. Human torque is not present in chimpanzee brain. Neuroimage. 2018;165:285–293. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.10.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yakovlev PI, Rakic P. Patterns of decussation of bulbar pyramids and distribution of pyramidal tracts on two sides of the spinal cord. Trans Am Neurol Assoc. 1966;91:366–367. [Google Scholar]
- Zilles K, Dabringhaus A, Geyer S, Amunts K, Qu M, Schleicher A, Gilissen E, Schlaug G, Steinmetz H. Structural asymmetries in the human forebrain and the forebrain of non-human primates and rats. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1996;20(4):593–605. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(95)00072-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]