The paper by Green et al1 provides an extensive and in‐depth review of cognition in schizophrenia, supporting the argument that cognitive dysfunction is a core feature of the illness. However, the authors do not fully explore how knowledge about cognition can inform us about the nature and development of schizophrenia. Is cognitive dysfunction a cause, a consequence or a marker of illness?
Both schizophrenia and general cognitive ability are heritable, with a broad polygenic basis. Genome‐wide association studies have identified significant associations between intelligence and educational attainment, as a proxy for general cognitive abilities, and genes involved in central nervous system (CNS) development and synapse regulation. Some of these genes overlap with vulnerability genes for schizophrenia. These genetic associations are primarily negative (i.e., higher intelligence – lower schizophrenia risk), but some of them are bidirectional (i.e., higher intelligence – higher schizophrenia risk)2. Further analyses indicate a strong protective effect of intelligence on the risk for schizophrenia, and a smaller negative effect of schizophrenia (risk genes) on intelligence3.
Aspects of cognition are also impaired in relatives of people with schizophrenia, who take an intermediate position between their affected family member and healthy controls4. However, the vulnerability to schizophrenia does not appear to be based in an unlucky familial combination of cognitive and environmental risks. An intriguing registry‐based study indicates that schizophrenia risk is predicted by the individual's deviation from familial cognitive aptitude (i.e., what is expected from educational attainment and IQ in parents and siblings) and not by cognitive dysfunction per se. When cases are matched to controls by educational achievement or IQ, their relatives are found to have better cognitive aptitudes than the corresponding relatives of the controls. These findings point to the existence of a qualitatively different developmental impairment that is associated with schizophrenia risk5.
A central finding from genome‐wide association studies is the link between risk of schizophrenia and the immune system, in particular, the complement system. Studies have identified a new role for complement 4 (C4) in synaptic pruning. Synaptic pruning peaks during adolescence, and is essential for refinement of the CNS and maturation of cognitive abilities. Structurally different variants of C4 genes are associated with differences in C4 expression and with the risk of schizophrenia, supporting the notion that elevated complement activity leading to increased synaptic pruning is a risk factor for schizophrenia. A recent study using patient‐derived induced pluripotent stem cells found abnormalities in microglia‐like cells and synaptic structures, in addition to increased synaptic pruning in the neuronal cultures. Risk‐associated variants of the C4 genes were linked to increased complement uptake in synapses6. In line with this, there are indications of poorer memory function linked to increased predicted C4 expression, across patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls7.
Prospective studies of early cognitive development in children who later developed schizophrenia showed stable deficits in IQ, language, processing speed and executive functioning from infancy. Verbal deficits appear early and are relatively stable, while impairments in processing speed and executive functions increase during adolescence8. The widening gap towards healthy adolescence appears mainly to be based in a developmental lag rather than a loss of acquired functions. Studies on groups considered as clinical high‐risk (CHR) for psychosis also find significant cognitive dysfunctions. This is particularly the case for those in the CHR group who later experience transition to psychosis. There are, however, no direct indications of a cognitive decline from the prodrome/high‐risk state to the onset of the first episode9.
The main argument for the initial conceptualization of schizophrenia as a neurodegenerative disorder was the presence of cognitive dysfunction and a deteriorating clinical course. However, first episode studies do not find any associations between the duration of untreated psychosis and cognitive dysfunction. Prospective studies of cognitive trajectories from the first episode onwards also show significant cognitive stability, both in short‐ and long‐term. There are some indications of poorer cognitive development in patients with high illness activity during the first year of treatment, but of limited magnitude and balanced by findings of modest cognitive improvements in other subgroups10.
Taken together, our knowledge about cognition in the early phases of schizophrenia strongly supports the notion of a primarily neurodevelopmental basis for cognitive dysfunction. Cognitive problems may serve as additional stressors increasing psychosis risk, while other symptoms of the disorder may add to cognitive problems. However, current data indicate that cognitive dysfunction is neither a cause nor a consequence of the psychotic process but rather a biomarker of underlying neurodevelopmental problems.
This notion has important clinical implications: while specific treatments may improve one area of dysfunction (cognition or psychotic symptoms) in adults with schizophrenia, this may not translate to other areas. Preventing additional developmental lags in adolescents at high risk might be one of the most effective ways to prevent significant cognitive dysfunction.
References
- 1. Green MF, Horan WP, Lee J. World Psychiatry 2019;18:146‐61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Le Hellard S, Wang Y, Witoelar A et al. Schizophr Bull 2017;43:654‐64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Savage JE, Jansen PR, Stringer S et al. Nat Genet 2018;50:912‐9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. McCarthy NS, Badcock JC, Clark ML et al. Schizophr Bull 2018;44:908‐21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Kendler KS, Ohlsson H, Mezuk B et al. JAMA Psychiatry 2016;7:465‐71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Sellgren CM, Gracias J, Watmuff B et al. Nat Neurosci 2019;22:374‐85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Donohoe G, Holland J, Mothersill D et al. Psychol Med 2018;48:1608‐15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Mollon J, Reichenberg A. Psychol Med 2018;48:392‐403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Carrion RE, Walder DJ, Auther AM et al. J Psychiatr Res 2018;96:231‐8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Rund BR, Barder HE, Evensen J et al. Schizophr Bull 2016;42:87‐95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]