Skip to main content
PLOS Computational Biology logoLink to PLOS Computational Biology
. 2019 Apr 24;15(4):e1006975. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006975

A kinetic model for Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor mediated spike timing-dependent LTP

Sergio M G Solinas 1,2, Elke Edelmann 3,4, Volkmar Leßmann 3,4,#, Michele Migliore 1,*,#
Editor: Joseph Ayers5
PMCID: PMC6502438  PMID: 31017891

Abstract

Across the mammalian nervous system, neurotrophins control synaptic plasticity, neuromodulation, and neuronal growth. The neurotrophin Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF) is known to promote structural and functional synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus, the cerebral cortex, and many other brain areas. In recent years, a wealth of data has been accumulated revealing the paramount importance of BDNF for neuronal function. BDNF signaling gives rise to multiple complex signaling pathways that mediate neuronal survival and differentiation during development, and formation of new memories. These different roles of BDNF for neuronal function have essential consequences if BDNF signaling in the brain is reduced. Thus, BDNF knock-out mice or mice that are deficient in BDNF receptor signaling via TrkB and p75 receptors show deficits in neuronal development, synaptic plasticity, and memory formation. Accordingly, BDNF signaling dysfunctions are associated with many neurological and neurodegenerative conditions including Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s disease. However, despite the widespread implications of BDNF-dependent signaling in synaptic plasticity in healthy and pathological conditions, the interplay of the involved different biochemical pathways at the synaptic level remained mostly unknown. In this paper, we investigated the role of BDNF/TrkB signaling in spike-timing dependent plasticity (STDP) in rodent hippocampus CA1 pyramidal cells, by implementing the first subcellular model of BDNF regulated, spike timing-dependent long-term potentiation (t-LTP). The model is based on previously published experimental findings on STDP and accounts for the observed magnitude, time course, stimulation pattern and BDNF-dependence of t-LTP. It allows interpreting the main experimental findings concerning specific biomolecular processes, and it can be expanded to take into account more detailed biochemical reactions. The results point out a few predictions on how to enhance LTP induction in such a way to rescue or improve cognitive functions under pathological conditions.

Author summary

Storing memory traces in the brain is essential for learning and memory formation, and it occurs through synaptic plasticity processes. Timing-dependent Long-Term Potentiation (t-LTP) is a physiologically relevant type of synaptic plasticity that results from the repeated sequential firing of action potentials (APs) in pre- and postsynaptic neurons. T-LTP is observed during learning in vivo and is a cellular correlate of memory formation. T-LTP can be elicited by different patterns of combined pre- and postsynaptic activity that recruit distinct synaptic growth processes underlying t-LTP. The protein Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF) is released at synapses and mediates synaptic plasticity in response to specific patterns of t-LTP stimulation in the theta frequency band, while other patterns mediate BDNF-independent t-LTP. Here, we developed a realistic computational model that accounts for our previously published experimental results of BDNF-independent 1:1 t-LTP (70 repeats of pairing 1 presynaptic with 1 postsynaptic AP) and BDNF-dependent 1:4 t-LTP (25 repeats of pairing 1 presynaptic with 4 postsynaptic APs). The model explains the magnitude and time course of both t-LTP forms and allows predicting t-LTP properties that result from altered BDNF turnover. Since BDNF levels are decreased in demented patients, understanding the function of BDNF in memory processes is important to counteract neurodegenerative diseases.

Introduction

Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF) is a member of the protein family of mammalian neurotrophins, further comprising nerve growth factor, neurotrophin 3 and neurotrophin 4/5. Neurotrophins are well known across the animal kingdom to support survival, ontogenetic development, differentiation, and stability of neurons in the entire nervous system[1,2]. In the mature nervous system, BDNF, in particular, serves additional roles by regulating functional and structural synaptic plasticity (reviewed e.g. in [1,35]).

In recent years, a wealth of data has been accumulated on the many roles of BDNF in regulating synaptic plasticity at glutamatergic and GABAergic synapses, e.g. in hippocampus, neocortex, amygdala, and cerebellum, unraveling signaling pathways of unprecedented complexity [1,610]. However, despite this well-established role of BDNF as a central activity-dependent mediator (i.e. switching on biochemical pathways that induce and maintain enhanced synaptic transmission [1113]) and modulator (i.e., facilitating synaptic changes that are mediated by other signaling pathways [3,1417]) of synaptic plasticity, the interplay between intra- and extracellular signaling pathways [18,19] that regulate and fine-tune BDNF-dependent synaptic changes is not well understood.

The overall picture is rather complex. BDNF consists of a protein homodimer that is generated exclusively in glutamatergic neurons from two identical peptide chains held together by noncovalent interactions. The precursor protein, pre-proBDNF, is sequestered into the endoplasmic reticulum, where the pre-sequence is cleaved off, yielding proBDNF. Intracellularly, proBDNF can be cleaved (by protein convertases, PCs and furin) into mature BDNF (mBDNF) and BDNF pro-peptide. All three BDNF species are thought to be assembled into secretory vesicles that are transported to the plasma membrane in soma, dendrites, and axons, where they release their content via Ca2+-dependent exocytosis [20].

Following secretion, remaining proBDNF can be cleaved by extracellular proteases (e.g. plasmin and matrix metalloproteinases). This is an important functional step since at this point it is determined whether mBDNF or proBDNF dependent signaling cascades are activated at a synapse. Because proBDNF and mBDNF activate signaling cascades that partially antagonize each other, the importance of knowing the exact identity of released BDNF can hardly be overestimated. While mBDNF preferentially binds to the tyrosine-kinase receptor B (TrkB) and, among other functions, supports LTP, proBDNF preferably docks to the p75 receptor, which mediates long-term depression (LTD) [8].

The complexity of BDNF control over neuronal growth, plasticity, and modulation, makes it difficult to carry out experimental studies to fully understand BDNF-dependent processes. Computational modeling can significantly help to untangle the interplay of these processes but, despite the widespread implications of BDNF signaling in structural and functional neuromodulation during normal and pathological physiological conditions, a biologically realistic model of how BDNF signaling instructs these changes is still missing. Except for a very recent example of a model of a positive BDNF feedback loop, to take into account experiments on inhibitory avoidance training [21], to the best of our knowledge there are no published models available that address BDNF-dependent pathways.

In this paper, we set out to investigate BDNF-dependent synaptic mechanisms, by implementing the first kinetic model of the central BDNF-dependent subcellular pathways underlying spike timing-dependent Long-Term Potentiation (t-LTP) at hippocampal synapses. For this purpose, we focused on TrkB-dependent processes at hippocampal Schaffer collateral to CA1 pyramidal cell synapses, for which extensive experimental work is available that can be used to constrain the parameter values [11,13]. We show that the model can capture the main experimental findings by using a minimal set of subcellular pathways, with which we can make specific predictions on how to enhance LTP induction in such a way to rescue or improve cognitive functions under pathological conditions.

Results

Experimental findings

As a reference for our model, we considered the data from [11]. In the paper, the authors described t-LTP elicited in hippocampal pyramidal CA1 neurons by repeatedly pairing, with different delays (Δt), a single stimulation of the Schaffer Collaterals with one, two or four postsynaptic action potentials elicited at 200 Hz. These induction protocols are hereof designated 1:1, 1:2, and 1:4 t-LTP. The paper highlights important properties of the mechanisms underlying t-LTP. Their main results are summarized in Fig 1A, where the EPSP slope recorded in whole-cell patch clamp mode is plotted as a function of time. On average, the expression of t-LTP was relatively delayed, and it took approximately 30 min to reach its maximum expression (Fig 1A, data reproduced from Fig. 1B of [11]). The increase in synaptic strength after both 1:1 and 1:4 t-LTP was graded with time. Assuming that an individual synapse switches to a potentiated state following an all-or-none change [22,23], this progressive increase in the overall t-LTP observed at the soma suggests a distribution of transition times for different spines, driven by the time course of the processes underlying t-LTP induction and expression. For both protocols, a Δt>15 ms did not result in a significant t-LTP (Fig 1B), being consistent with other experimental findings [24,25]. As commonly expected, only short positive delays between pre- and postsynaptic stimulation are efficient to produce timing-dependent LTP, while longer delays reduce t-LTP magnitudes. In the experimental study that forms the basis of our model [11] a significant reduction of t-LTP was observed with positive time delays between 15–25 ms. It should be stressed that there was a rather large variability in the overall potentiation (i.e. in time course and magnitude) observed in recordings from individual cells, as demonstrated by the six typical cases of recording from different cells reported in Fig 1C. As will be discussed later, this finding is important for a better understanding of the interplay among the different processes underlying the induction of plasticity at each synaptic contact.

Fig 1. t-LTP expression in whole cell current clamp recordings.

Fig 1

A) Average EPSP slope as a function of time (-10 to 0 min: baseline control). The 70x 1:1 t-LTP protocol (blue diamonds, n = 16) or the 25x 1:4 t-LTP protocol (red circles, n = 19) were executed at t = 0 min; in all cases average values (±sem) were calculated (bin width: 2 min), for cells with Δt = 5 − 10 ms. B) Change of EPSP slopes in individual neurons 30 min after t-LTP induction for different Δt between pre-synaptic activation and post-synaptic action potentials (70x 1:1 t-LTP n = 56, 25x 1:4 t-LTP n = 55; color coding as in panel A; data redrawn from Edelmann et al., 2015 (Fig 1B). The insets show typical somatic recordings of synaptic responses before (green) and after 1:1 t-LTP (blue) or 1:4 t-LTP (red). C) Typical examples of individual EPSP slope recordings following a conditioning period after a 10 min long baseline recording (upper panels paradigm 1:1, lower panels paradigm 1:4).

Additional properties of t-LTP are summarized in Table 1 and suggest that, in all cases, t-LTP induction was found to be postsynaptic and NMDA receptor-dependent. Instead, expression was found to be pre-synaptic for the 70x 1:1 protocol, post-synaptic for the 25x 1:4 protocol, and mixed for the 50x 1:2 protocol. The pre- or post-synaptic expression of t-LTP was experimentally determined by i) analyzing synaptic responses to short latency (50 ms) paired pulses inducing pre-synaptic short term plasticity (i.e. paired-pulse facilitation), ii) by infusing an inhibitor of AMPA receptor insertion into the postsynaptic membrane via the recording pipette solution, iii) by testing the AMPA/NMDAR current ratio, and iv) by using analysis of the coefficient of variation of EPSPs pre- vs. post LTP induction (see Fig.2 in [11]). E.g. pre-synaptic 1:1 t-LTP changes the glutamate release probability of release and therefore changes the temporal dynamics of short-term plasticity. Conversely, the post-synaptically expressed 1:4 t-LTP does not change short-term plasticity, but rather changes postsynaptic AMPA/NMDAR current ratio and depends on incorporation of new AMPA receptors into the postsynaptic membrane (all respective data shown in Fig.2 of [11]). Of note, the 1:1 t-LTP protocol was composed of 1 EPSP paired with 1 backpropagating action potential (bAP), whereas the 1:4 t-LTP protocol was composed of 1 EPSP paired with 4 (instead of 1) bAPs. Thus the 1:1 t-LTP protocol can be considered as being included (i.e. being a part of) in the 1:4 t-LTP protocol. One might thus expect that the mechanisms triggered by the 1:1 t-LTP protocol should also be activated by the 1:4 t-LTP protocol, but this was not experimentally observed [11].

Table 1. Summary of experimental results taken from Edelmann et al.[11].

Property 1:1 t-LTP 1:2 t-LTP 1:4 t-LTP
Location of t-LTP expression Pre Mixed Post*
Location of t-LTP induction Post Post Post
Number of repeated stimuli 70–100 50 25–35
Stimulus repetition frequency 0.5 Hz 0.5 Hz 0.5 Hz
Test frequency§ 0.05 Hz 0.05 Hz 0.05 Hz
NMDA receptor-dependent Yes Yes Yes
BDNF-dependent No Partial Yes
Slow expression (30 min) Yes Yes Yes
t-LTP Occlusion by 1:1 protocol -- n.d. No

n.d. = data not available.

* The paired pulse ratio (PPR) change induced by the 1:4 t-LTP protocol was small and not significant, but the PPR change distribution reveals occasional contamination by presynaptic expression.

This frequency was used during t-LTP induction, each stimulus elicited either 1 EPSP paired with 1 bAP or 1 EPSP paired with 4 bAPs.

§ This frequency was used during the phase of t-LTP expression, each stimulus elicited a single EPSP.

Other experimental suggestions that could be used to further constrain the model implementation: (i) An increase in postsynaptic intracellular calcium, [Ca2+]i, was necessary to initiate a complex chain of biochemical reactions leading to the vesicular release of BDNF [3]; this process has stochastic dynamics that are ~10 times slower than glutamate release which resulted in a large variability of the time course of postsynaptic BDNF release with respect to the triggering event of a transient [Ca2+]i elevation [26]. (ii) The postsynaptic BDNF release could last from a few seconds up to approximately 300 s ([20], supplementary Fig. 5 in [11]). (iii) There is no 1:1 (i.e. pre-synaptic) t-LTP expressed following 1:4 t-LTP stimulation [11]; this result may imply the existence of an additional mechanism, triggered by the 1:4 t-LTP protocol which is able to block the induction of 1:1 t-LTP. In summary, these experimental observations form a useful set of properties that give specific indications on what the model must be able to reproduce to be considered a reasonable representation of the many biochemical pathways that can be involved.

The model

In agreement with experimental suggestions [22,23], the model was based on the assumption that any given individual synaptic contact, following the appropriate conditioning protocol, will change its state in an all-or-none manner. This was an important point to consider in comparing model and experimental findings since experimental recordings are customarily carried out from the soma, whereas the stimulation most likely involved an unknown number of synapses located in a relatively wide range of distances from the soma. The progressive increase in synaptic potentiation over time may thus be the result of an ensemble dynamics where different synapses undergo potentiation at different times. Unless explicitly stated otherwise, in discussing the model implementation we will always refer to individual synapses.

The biochemical pathways that we considered for this work are schematically represented in Fig 2A, and it is based on the hypothesis that distinct biochemical pathways are activated by different levels of intracellular [Ca2+] in the postsynaptic compartment [27,28]. In our model, there were three different [Ca2+]i thresholds, θ1, θ2, and θ3. Ca2+ entry can independently occur through NMDA receptor or voltage-gated Ca2+ channels, both explicitly included in our model and known to drive postsynaptic BDNF secretion [29].

Fig 2. Schematic representation of our model for the BDNF-dependent t-LTP signaling cascade.

Fig 2

A) For clarity, the biomolecular pathways involved with the 1:1 t-LTP or 1:4 t-LTP induction protocols are represented with blue and pink blocks, respectively. The model inputs are a synaptic activation ("presyn input", on the left) and a postsynaptic depolarization (on the right). The 70x 1:1 t-LTP cascade (blue) is activated if, during the train of stimuli, the [Ca2+]i in the spine crosses the θ1 threshold frequently enough to allow RM to activate presynaptic RM-dependent processes (RM proc). This, in turn, will activate a pre-synaptic signaling cascade leading to the increase in glutamate release. The 25x 1:4 t-LTP cascade (pink) is activated by [Ca2+]i transients above θ2, which trigger a delayed (0–300 s) fusion of vesicles containing mBDNF and proBDNF. The mBDNF activates the postsynaptic TrkB receptors that finally activate an intracellular, post-synaptic, signaling cascade leading to an increase of AMPA peak conductance; [Ca2+]i transients above θ3, will result in a block of RM release. B) The CA1 model neuron used for all simulations, shown during the initial phase of an action potential backpropagating from the soma; the dendritic segments are color-coded for membrane potential (yellow represents a membrane potential above -20mV, purple represents -70mV). The insets show the location of 12 spines on a dendritic branch (branch38 of model morphology [30]). Six additional spines were placed on a different dendritic branch (branch8 of model morphology [30] indicated by the blue bracket on the left plot).

A transient [Ca2+]i increase above each threshold activated one or many pathways in the spine head. The [Ca2+]i range below θ1 corresponded to the non-plastic regime, i.e. any combination of pre- and/or post-synaptic input did not alter the current state of the pre- and/or post-synaptic mechanisms. Above θ1, it activated the 1:1 t-LTP signaling cascade (Fig 2A, blue boxes), which released a yet to be identified retrograde messenger (RM). This release activated presynaptic processes (RM proc and presyn proc in Fig 2A) resulting in a persistent increase in stimulus induced presynaptic glutamate release (release), in agreement with the change in the paired-pulse ratio observed experimentally [11]. Experimental findings suggest that neither nitric oxide (NO) [11] nor endocannabinoids were involved as RM. The fusion of postsynaptic BDNF vesicles was activated by a larger and more long-lasting Ca2+ transient ([Ca2+]i2), which may be obtained with the 70x 1:4 t-LTP protocol (Fig 2A, dark pink blocks). The largest Ca2+ transients ([Ca2+]i3) activated biochemical reactions blocking RM production. The rationale for this choice was that experimental recordings clearly show that the 1:4 t-LTP protocol did not induce presynaptic LTP [11]. For this to happen, there must be an activity-dependent (postsynaptic) process blocking the biochemical pathways leading to presynaptic LTP. In the model, we made the simple assumption that this process could be a Ca2+-dependent block of the retrograde messenger release, occurring for a [Ca2+]i threshold (θ3) that is higher than the one for LTP induction. Other model behaviors were not affected by this assumption, and this scheme left open the possibility, for spines in which [Ca2+]i reaches an intermediate concentration (θ2<[Ca2+]i< θ3), to account for a t-LTP with mixed pre- and post-synaptic mechanisms of expression that is obtained with a 1:2 t-LTP protocol [11].

The complete set of kinetic equations implementing the model (introduced in the next paragraph) were included into the membrane equation for each of the 18 explicitly modeled spines (see Methods). To roughly take into account the local dendritic temporal integration process, 12 of the 18 spines were distributed on one oblique dendrite (Fig 2B), whereas the remaining 6 spines were distributed on a different dendrite (see blue bracket in Fig 2B) and had different values for the θi (see Table 2).

Table 2. Model parameters.

Parameter Model Value
αfuse 5.5e-7 ms-1
αRM 0.007 ms-1
σ1 0.01e-3 mM
σ3 0.0001 mM
αCRM 1e-3 mM/ms
αRMp 1e-3 ms-1
θRM 0.02 mM
σRM 0.001 mM
RMinf 0 mM
αpp 5.5e-7–16.5e-7 ms-1
αRMpU 0.54
θU 0.15 mM
σU 0.001 mM
[Ca]i_max 0.16 mM
v_PC 0.002 mM
v_BDNF 0.002 mM
αPC 1e-4 mM/ms
αdiff 0.01 μM/ms
θTrkB 0.0002 mM
σTrkB 0.00001 mM
αpost 5.5e-6 ms-1
αAMPA 1.5
θAMPA 0.01 mM
σAMPA 0.00001 mM
Parameter Branch38 Branch8
θ1 0.046 mM 0.004 mM
θ2 0.1 mM 0.045 mM
θ3 0.12 mM 0.052 mM

The model: Presynaptic mechanisms

The presynaptic mechanisms specific for this work were added to the phenomenological model discussed in [31], and described by the following set of equations:

dxdt=zτrecUSExδ(ttspike) (1)
dydt=yτin+USExδ(ttspike) (2)
z=1xy (3)

where δ(t) is a delta function, tspike was the time of arrival of a spike at the pre-synaptic terminal, the variables x, y, and z are the fraction of resources in the recovered, active, and inactive states, respectively, and USE was proportional to the glutamate released by each synaptic stimulation. They reproduced the stereotypical synaptic response dynamics between pyramidal neurons under physiological conditions. The values for the presynaptic parameters were those used in Ref. [31], with USE0 = 0.1, τrec = 0.8 sec, and τin = 3 ms. This presynaptic mechanism has been previously shown to reproduce experimental findings on the normalization of temporal summation of synaptic inputs targeting distal or proximal dendrites of CA1 pyramidal neurons [32].

USE was additionally modulated by retrograde messenger-dependent pathways described by the following equations:

dRMdt=αRM(RMRMinf)+αCRMS(cai,θ1,σ1)[1S(cai,θ3,σ3)]αRMp(RMRMinf)S(cai,θRM,σRM) (4)
dRMpdt=αRMp(RMRMinf)S(cai,θRM,σRM)αppRMp (5)
dppdt=αppRMp (6)
USE=USE0(1+αRMpUS(pp,θU,σU)), (7)

where RMp and pp were presynaptic processes activated in cascade by RM accumulation in the synaptic cleft (“RM proc” and “presyn proc” in Fig 2A), and S(i,j,k)=11+e(ji)/k is the typical sigmoidal logistic function ubiquitously observed in biological systems [33]. Our hypothesis is that the activation of these mechanisms follows a dose-response curve. These processes are usually implemented with a sigmoid or a Hill function. Although the latter can be more easily related to the biomolecular pathways it represents, it also implies a significantly higher computational cost (for NEURON running on a PC we verified a 35% difference in CPU time). This occurs because of the internal representation of the computational algorithms used to calculate an exp (in a sigmoid function) or a power (in the Hill function) on any given computer. Since we plan to use this model on a large-scale network, we have preferred to implement these curves with a sigmoid function. The cai was the intracellular calcium concentration [Ca2+]i. Note that pp does not have a decay term. This ensures that a potentiated synapse does not spontaneously fall back to its non-potentiated state. It is technically possible to continue to present the induction protocol for infinite time yielding to RM release and consequent infinite growth of pp. However, this exploratory modeling work does not consider this remote possibility.

The model: Post-synaptic mechanisms

Post-synaptic mechanisms are activated by different levels of [Ca2+]i, with the instantaneous Ca2+ dynamics determined by the complex interaction between AMPAR and NMDAR conductances, voltage-gated Ca2+ channels, and all other active and passive membrane properties. All the equations regulating the instantaneous Ca2+ dynamics were taken from a previously published CA1 neuron model [30] (ModelDB a. n. 55035). We reported here only the equations of the new mechanisms introduced in this work, and directly related to the synaptic transmission pathways using [Ca2+]i as an input (see Methods for detail on how to access the full model).

The fusion of BDNF-containing vesicle with the spine head membrane is a complex process, possibly involving several biochemical pathways for which there are not enough experimental constraints to build a detailed kinetic scheme. For this reason, we implemented the effective action of these pathways using two mechanisms, accounting for the dependency of BDNF vesicle fusion probability and delay with respect to the STDP induction protocol.

The first mechanism is implemented with an empirical variable, which we called intracellular signaling (is). It is based on the experimental findings [11] suggesting that the fusion of BDNF-containing vesicles occurs only for conditioning protocols consisting of at least 25 induction stimuli repeated at a frequency close to 0.5 Hz, while no fusion was achieved in response to test stimulations at 0.05 Hz. In the model, this was obtained by increasing is by a fixed amount every time [Ca2+]i crossed the θ2 threshold, and decreasing it with a time constant of 8 s. The fusion was allowed to occur only for is>0.15.

With the second mechanism, we took into account the experimental findings ([20], supplementary Fig. 5 in [11]) showing that the fusion of an individual vesicle containing BDNF, when activated, is a stochastic process occurring over a relatively long time window. We modeled all the involved processes by assuming that the fusion process happened with probability pf (defined for [Ca2+]i2), and delay df, calculated as:

pf={[Ca]iθ2[Ca]i_maxθ2(is>0.15)0(is0.15) (8)
df=300(1[Ca]iθ2[Ca]i_maxθ2)rand[0,1], (9)

and assuming that

Fused_vescicles=f(pf,df) (10)

The function f(pf,df) keeps track of how many vesicles, in each synapse, have fused with the plasmatic membrane and were in the process of releasing BDNF. For each synapse, this function is increased by 1 with probability pf after a time interval df from the instant at which [Ca2+]i crosses the θ2 threshold. The function is updated, asynchronously for each vesicle, every 1 ms of simulated time, theoretically leading to a minimum interval of 1 ms between the start of a new vesicular release, with a maximum number of available vesicles in each synapse set at 200, consistent with experimental data [34]. The function decreases by 1 (with a minimum value of zero) every time a vesicle has been fused for 30min. Random numbers from a uniform distribution in the interval [0–1] were used to choose the values for df, and pf; it should be stressed that this choice should not be considered as parametric randomization but, rather, as a way to introduce into the model an intrinsic stochastic behaviour.

During the time the [Ca2+]i remains above the θ2 threshold the process leading to the release of a quantal amount of BDNF is active. In this time window, the fusion process of individual vesicles is initiated with probability pf and results in an actual fusion starting at a random time df (up to 300 sec) from activation, in agreement with experimental observations ([20], supplementary Fig. 5 in [11]). This also means that for [Ca2+]i remaining for a prolonged time above threshold, more fusion processes are started. Once a vesicle has fused with the membrane, it continuously releases a fraction of the stored mBDNF and proBDNF for some time. The experimental evidence for this process is indirect, and it suggests a lower and an upper bound for the overall process: the release lasts for at least 5 min [29], but the overall LTP induction proceeds for approximately 30 min [11]. We made the somewhat simplifying and minimal assumption that the BDNF release lasts for 30 min. However, if this assumption would be invalidated by new experimental data, for example with longer experimental recordings of the BDNF release from single vesicles, the model could be straightforwardly revised by including an additional variable activated by a short BDNF release and slowly decaying over a period of 30 min. In any case, it is important to stress that in order to be consistent with the available experimental findings, the process modulating the magnitude of induced LTP must have a time course of approximately 30 min.

The ratio between mBDNF and proBDNF inside these vesicles is unknown. Indirect experimental evidence [35,36] indicate for the mBDNF:proBDNF proportion a value in the range of 10% to 90%. This ratio also depends on the pH inside the vesicle [20,37]. Since in our mouse brain slices we detected ~66% mBDNF vs. ~33% proBDNF in cell lysates, we used a 70%:30% proportion of mBDNF and proBDNF, respectively. In the Golgi apparatus and in BDNF-containing vesicles proBDNF can be cleaved by protein convertases (PC) into mBDNF and BDNF pro-peptide. Following the release, remaining proBDNF can be cleaved by plasmin or matrixmetallo proteinases [20]. To empirically model extrasynaptic diffusion and reuptake [38,39], mBDNF, proBDNF, and PC were all assumed to decay at a constant rate αdiff. The overall level of mBDNF present in the synaptic cleft determined the extent of TrkB receptor activation, Through a chain of postsynaptic processes represented by postsyn in Fig 2A, TrkB induces t-LTP by increasing the AMPA receptor conductance [11]. We implemented these processes as:

dproBDNFdt=αfuse0.3Fused_vesiclesv_BDNFαPCPCproBDNFαdiffproBDNF (11)
dmBDNFdt=αfuse0.7Fused_vesiclesv_BDNF+αPCPCproBDNFαdiffmBDNF (12)
dPCdt=αfuseFused_vesiclesv_PCαdiffPC (13)
TrkB=mBDNFS(mBDNF,θTrkB,σTrkB) (14)
dpostdt=αpostTrkB (15)
gAMPA=gmax[1+αAMPAS(post,θAMPA,σAMPA)], (16)

where gAMPA is the peak AMPA conductance, gmax its maximum value before LTP, and post represents the long-term effects of TrkB-dependent processes on the overall AMPA conductance. Note that post does not have a decay term. This ensures that a potentiated synapse does not spontaneously fall back to its non-potentiated state. It is technically possible to continue to present the induction protocol for infinite time yielding to BDNF release and consequent infinite growth of post. However, this exploratory modeling work does not consider this remote possibility.

The overall model was too complex to attempt an automatic fitting procedure, especially considering that there were not enough clear experimental constraints to reduce the number of free parameters. For this reason, the parameters were set in two steps: 1) for each block shown in Fig 2A, an initial estimate for the involved parameters was obtained by presenting inputs that mimic the signals that could be generated in the full model, and manually adjusting the values to obtain what we considered a reasonable output signal; 2) test simulations of the full model were carried out with all spines placed on the dendrites. In this latter step, which can take into account the non-linear interaction between a spine and a backpropagating action potential, the parameters were further adjusted in such a way to result in an overall LTP level consistent with the experimental findings shown in Fig 1A. It is important to stress that the key point in this paper was not to explore the parameter space or to find their best values but to study if, how, and to what extent, the proposed scheme was able to take into account the basic experimental findings on BDNF-dependent spike-time-dependent LTP.

As mentioned when discussing Fig 2, we explicitly modeled eighteen independent spines, each containing the mechanisms described above with the parameters reported in Table 2. To introduce the physiological variability of the biochemical pathway dynamics in the model, the αpp value in each synapse was drawn from a random uniform distribution. The number of synapses was not important for the scope of the paper. We found it a convenient number to illustrate and demonstrate that the overall effect measured at the soma was the result of a number of independent synapses. The key point here is that, as we will discuss later, the experimental findings cannot be reproduced by modeling a single synapse or a group of identical synapses. It should also be noted that there are many sources of noise that could affect the model behaviour. For example, random background synaptic activity could jitter the interaction between the elicited EPSPs and the bAPs. However, due to the large number of stimuli repetitions and the slow processes that they activate, this contributed to the overall behaviour in a way similar to the random localisation of the spines. The same would be with variability in the morphological and/or electrophysiological spine parameters.

The table shows only the model parameters introduced in this work. All model files and the Python scripts used to run the simulations described in the paper are available for public download under the ModelDB section of the Senselab database (http://senselab.med.yale.edu, a.n. HYPERLINK "http://modeldb.yale.edu/244412" 244412).

In summary, we have introduced a biophysical model of spike timing-dependent LTP at the Schaffer collateral synapse s of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. The model took explicitly into account, for the first time, several experimental findings on the BDNF-dependent biochemical pathways.

LTP elicited by a 70x 1:1 t-LTP induction protocol

In Fig 3A, we plotted the membrane potential at a spine head during a conditioning stimulus in which a synaptic activation (arrow) was paired with a bAP elicited with a Δt = +5 or +50 ms. The same time course was typically observed at all synapses. Note that for Δt = +50 ms (Fig 3A, thin grey trace) the synaptic activation and the bAP could be considered as completely separate events. In this case, the maximum voltage deflection observed in the spine head was approximately 22 mV during the EPSP alone and 17 mV for the bAP. With a Δt = +5 ms, the two events overlapped and summed nonlinearly, with a maximum deflection of 66 mV. The nonlinear summation of an EPSP paired with a properly timed bAP has been experimentally observed [40], and in our model was a key factor in inducing LTP. It can be explained by considering that the depolarization caused by the synaptic activation has the effect of inactivating the KA channels, allowing a bAP arriving within a relatively narrow time window to better propagate in the dendrite and the spine. The resulting depolarization released the NMDA receptor Mg-block and allowed a supralinear Ca2+ influx.

Fig 3. Signals triggered by the 70x 1:1 t-LTP protocol.

Fig 3

A) Membrane potential in a spine during a single induction stimulus (black trace); an EPSP was triggered (arrowhead) Δt = 5 ms before the arrival of a bAP (t = 0 was the time of the bAP peak); the grey trace shows the result for Δt = 50 ms. B) The [Ca2+]i transient generated in the individual spines by a single conditioning stimulus; the thin grey trace shows the transient in one of the spines for Δt = 50 ms. C) and D) show the time course for the normalized glutamate release and the peak AMPA conductance, respectively, in individual synapses, after the train of 70 conditioning stimuli. Note that the induction protocol triggered pre-synaptic potentiation in almost all synapses; post-synaptic potentiation was induced in the only synapse (green trace) in which the [Ca2+]i crossed the threshold θ2 during each conditioning stimulus. Note that the vertical axes of panels C and D do not have the same scales.

The [Ca2+]i time course, recorded in the 12 spines distributed along one of the oblique dendritic branches during a synchronous activation of all synapses, is shown in Fig 3B (colored lines correspond to different spines). For comparison, we also plotted [Ca2+]i in a single spine for Δt = +50 ms (grey trace). In all 12 spines of the dendritic segment shown at higher magnification in Fig 2B, the [Ca2+]i transiently raised above the θ1 threshold for a Δt = +5 ms, whereas none of the spines in the other branch (compare blue bracket in Fig 2B) reached the θ1 threshold (remaining coloured transients in Fig 3B). The [Ca2+]i transient was significantly different among spines. This occurred because the back-propagation of an AP depends on the local dendritic properties. Since the RM release is proportional to the amount of [Ca2+]i above the θ1 threshold, the spines with larger [Ca2+]i transients (e.g. bright green and red traces in Fig 3B) were able to accumulate in a shorter time the amount of RM required to activate the pre-synaptic mechanisms of plasticity. This resulted in t-LTP induction (in terms of an increase in the glutamate release) earlier than in other spines (Fig 3C dark green and brown traces). Spines for which there was a higher release of RM were potentiated earlier and with a faster transition (bright green trace); spines with lower RM release switched to a potentiated state later and with a slower transition (e.g. dark green trace). Two spines did not release a sufficient amount of RM to trigger potentiation (Fig 3B cyan and thick grey). In only one spine the [Ca2+]i transients crossed also the θ2 threshold triggering the postsynaptic potentiation mechanisms with a time course depending on TrkB activation (Fig 3D bright green trace).

In agreement with the experiments (Fig 1A), it took around 25 min after the induction protocol to switch all synapses to a potentiated state. In the model, we assumed that this could be caused by the slow time constants of the biochemical pathways involved with the retrograde messenger (see αpp in Table 2). As expected, since the 1:1 t-LTP protocol was in general not able to generate enough Ca2+ entry to cross the θ2 threshold, the AMPA conductance, which was modulated by the post-synaptic plasticity mechanisms, did not increase for all but one of the synapses (Fig 3D).

Taken together these results suggest that, in order to be consistent with the experimental findings, it was necessary to make the physiologically reasonable assumption that the RM-dependent mechanisms needed to generate a different response at each synaptic location, which is a physiologically plausible condition. This was an important issue that is usually not considered in implementing subcellular models for synaptic transmission. Alternatively, it is possible that long time constants in downstream processes (not explicitly modeled here), such as the incorporation of new glutamate-containing vesicles into the readily releasable pool, are responsible for the approximately 25 min delay in completing the induction of synaptic potentiation [41,42].

LTP elicited by a 25x 1:4 t-LTP induction protocol

Pairing one synaptic stimulation with four bAPs (Fig 4A) resulted in [Ca2+]i transients spanning a range covering all thresholds (Fig 4B both panels). For 2 of the 12 spines in one branch, the [Ca2+]i transient crossed only the θ1 threshold, resulting in a pre-synaptic t-LTP induction (Fig 4C, cyan and thick grey traces). In other 2 synapses (Fig 4B left panel, light brown and dark green traces) it was θ2<[Ca2+]i3. This indicated the activation of both pre- and post-synaptic mechanisms. For the other 10 synapses, the [Ca2+]i crossed also the θ3 threshold, eliciting the activation of all the post-synaptic (but not presynaptic) pathways, with a consequent long-term potentiation of the AMPA peak conductance (Fig 4D). For the 6 spines in the other dendritic branch, [Ca2+]i crossed the θ3 threshold in all spines, but only two spines were potentiated (Fig 4D, yellow and magenta traces), while the other 4 spines were not because they did not release and accumulate enough BDNF in the cleft to activate the downstream signaling cascade. This behaviour allowed us to point out a suggestion of our model that will turn out to be extremely important later: BDNF release was necessary but not sufficient to trigger postsynaptic t-LTP. The model suggested that BDNF must accumulate in the synaptic cleft up to an amount sufficient to activate TrkB receptors, i.e. the release must be sufficiently frequent and strong. During the 25 stimulus repetitions, this condition was achieved for only two of the 6 spines (Fig 4D, yellow and magenta traces).

Fig 4. Signals triggered by the 25x 1:4 t-LTP protocol.

Fig 4

A) Membrane potential in a spine during a single induction stimulus (black trace); an EPSPs was triggered (arrowhead) before, Δt = 5 ms, the arrival of four bAPs (t = 0 is the time of the first bAP peak); the grey trace shows the result for Δt = 50 ms. B) The [Ca2+]i transient generated in all the 18 spines by a single conditioning stimulus; the grey trace shows the transient in one of the spines for Δt = 50 ms; for clarity, the two sets of spines (compare Fig 2B) are shown in two separate panels. C) and D) show the time course for the normalized glutamate release and the peak AMPA conductance, respectively, in individual spines, after the train of conditioning stimuli. Note that the induction protocol triggered pre-synaptic potentiation only in a few synapses (panel C), whereas the peak AMPA conductance was increased (resulting in post-synaptic potentiation) in most synapses. Note that the vertical axes of panels C and D do not have the same scales.

These findings show how the effect of a 25x 1:4 t-LTP conditioning, as observed from the soma, could result from a complex dendritic signal integration process independently occurring in each synapse, and involving diverse biochemical pathways that may interplay in different ways.

Average 1:1 and 1:4 t-LTP observed from the soma

The results described above were discussed in terms of the processes occurring at the single spine level. We now turn our attention to the average results obtained from several cells. In preliminary simulations, we found that synaptic location and biochemical dynamics in individual spines may result in quite different temporal profiles for the observed t-LTP. This suggests that the overall time course was the result of t-LTP induction and expression mechanisms at each synapse, which respond to the same conditioning protocol by switching to a potentiated state at different times. During a manual trial and error procedure, we thus found a possible combination of synaptic potentiation times that best represented the average experimental results (Fig 5A, compare circles with open squares). The model results for the average EPSP slope measured at the soma were in the range of the experimental data for different values of Δt (see Fig 5B, red and blue squares). The model was also able to reproduce several experimental recordings obtained from individual cells, as shown in Fig 5C for three examples using the 1:1 or 1:4 protocol. For these cases, we found that in order to match the recordings from an individual cell, it was sufficient to distribute the spines along the branch and assume different values for αUSE and αgampa. This is physiologically plausible since these parameters represent the amount of presynaptic glutamate released in the cleft and the density of postsynaptic AMPA receptors, respectively. These factors and the location of activated spines along the dendrite can be expected to be quite different among cells.

Fig 5. Comparison between model and experiments.

Fig 5

A) Average EPSP slope, calculated from the 18 model synapses (red squares, using a Δt = 5 ms) and experimental results after 70x 1:1 t-LTP (blue diamonds with error bars) or 25x 1:4 T-LTP (red circles with error bars) conditioning protocol. B) The final slope of simulated EPSPs (squares) for Δt = 5 ms, 10 ms, 20 ms, and 30 ms are compared with experimental findings (circles). The inset shows typical model traces of EPSPs measured at the soma before (green) and after 1:1 t-LTP (blue) or 1:4 t-LTP (red). C) Experimental findings from individual neurons during 1:1 t-LTP (blue) or 1:4 t-LTP (red) compared with modeling results (yellow traces). In each panel, the inset shows the individual spines along the dendritic branch. The model parameters αAMPA and αRMpU were set to the following values to fit the experimental data. In the upper panels αRMpU = 0.9, 2.2, 7 (from left to right), in the lower panels αAMPA = 0.1, 0.5, 1.5 (from left to right).

These results show that the set of pathways included in the model is able to take into account the main mechanisms underlying BDNF-dependent t-LTP in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. The model can, therefore, be conveniently used to investigate additional BDNF-dependent effects at these synapses.

Changes in BDNF release

To test the behavior of the model under different conditions of BDNF release, we selected two physiologically plausible cases related to specific biochemical processes that may be of particular interest. As mentioned before, the proportion of mBDNF and proBDNF inside the BDNF-containing vesicles is currently unknown, and it may be an important factor in regulating the series of biochemical cascades that they activate. Based on indirect measurements [38,43], in our control model we had set this proportion to 70:30 (see the postsynaptic mechanisms section). To test what would happen if this ratio was changed for physiological or pathological reasons, we carried out a set of simulations using the 25x 1:4 t-LTP protocol using a 30:70 proportion for mBDNF:proBDNF. The simulation results are summarized in Fig 6A (green squares). As expected, lowering the initial amount of mBDNF (Fig 6B from control, solid red line, to mBDNF30, solid green line) resulted in a weaker t-LTP in the first minutes after induction (see open green squares in Fig 6A up to 6 min). In Fig 6B we show that in this case the mBDNF level was partially restored to the level observed with a 70:30 ratio (compare the red dashed line with the solid green line) by the cleaving action of PC on the higher level of proBDNF. The lower concentration of mBDNF yielded a higher latency in the expression of potentiation (see green squares in Fig 6A in the range of 8–16 min after induction of t-LTP) and an overall t-LTP after 30 min that was approximately 50% less pronounced than in control. This may appear surprising since it could be argued that the overall amount of BDNF released was the same as in control. This effect can be explained by the lower overall mBDNF accumulation during the 25 min period after conditioning, caused by extrasynaptic diffusion and reuptake processes. The delay in the accumulation of mBDNF resulted in a lower total concentration in the cleft at 25 min from induction failing to potentiate the spines with least [Ca2+]i influx. The final result was a failure to potentiate those synapses in which the BDNF accumulation was not enough to activate TrkB receptors and trigger the potentiation of the relative AMPA conductance. It should be stressed that here we were interested in illustrating the possible consequences of changing the mBDNF:proBDNF ratio before they were released from the post-synaptic vesicles. An additional effect, not included in this model, would be caused by the two factors preferentially binding, after their release, to different receptors (proBDNF to p75 receptors versus mBDNF binding TrkB receptors) followed by receptor mediated endocytosis (discussed e.g. in [3]).

Fig 6. Model robustness and predictions.

Fig 6

EPSP slope after STDP induction under different conditions. A) open red squares: model control (same model data as in Fig 5 open red squares); open green squares: model, with the mBDNF:proBDNF ratio set to 30:70 (instead of 70:30 in control); open black squares: BDNF release reduced to 15 min; closed blue circles: BDNF release compressed to 15 min. B) Solid lines show the concentration in mM of mBDNF over time, dashed lines show the proBDNF. Color coding as in panel A. C) Removal of the RM block (RMBLK in the legend; closed red squares) resulted in 1:4 t-LTP protocol inducing both pre- and post-synaptic t-LTP, with a 50% increase in the overall potentiation. Increasing by 20% the concentration of BDNF inside the vesicles (closed cyan squares) resulted in a 25% increase of LTP.

Another process that we investigated was the duration for the vesicular release of BDNF. In contrast with the extremely fast glutamatergic mechanisms of release, the processes underlying BDNF release are known to last for minutes ([11,26,29]; reviewed e.g. in [20]). To be consistent with experimental findings, under control conditions we used a 30 min time window, but what would be the consequence of having the release reduced or compressed into a shorter time window? For example, reducing the release to a 15 min window corresponds to a proportionally reduced BDNF level; the model predicted that this would result in a 40% reduction in t-LTP expression, with respect to control (Fig 6A, open black squares). Instead, compressing (i.e. releasing the same amount of BDNF during a shorter time interval) the entire release process leads to an overall faster dynamics, through which the same amount of BDNF was released within a shorter time; under this condition the model predicted that, although the final amount of t-LTP would be the same as control (Fig 6A, blue circles), the maximum induction would be reached much earlier. The results obtained with the model were rather robust against changes in starting parameters (see Fig A in S1 Text), and suggesting that the overall time course of t-LTP recorded at the soma could reflect the compound effect of the dynamics of the release process at the individual synaptic contacts.

Experimentally testable predictions

The kinetic scheme of the model was constructed to include a small set of building blocks needed to take into account the constraints that can be derived from experimental findings on the biochemical pathways that may be involved in BDNF-dependent t-LTP. We considered it a useful template to explore the consequences of selective changes in one or more of the pathways, in the attempt to elaborate a few model predictions fostering future experimental work. We were particularly interested in figuring out the possible mechanisms that could be exploited to rescue normal cognitive functions that under pathological conditions reduce synaptic transmission, such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD). For example, we could assume that under AD conditions AMPA/NMDA receptors were less efficient in providing the depolarization needed to activate enough Ca2+ entry and to activate LTP induction [4447]. This could occur, for example, because of AD-dependent alterations in the process of binding glutamate to receptors or in a conformational change affecting their peak conductance [44]. A possible rescue mechanism, in this case, could be a minimal perturbation of the pathways, in such a way to increase the magnitude of LTP that could be induced by the STDP protocol.

Experimental findings suggest two possible ways in which this could be obtained. The first way is related to the clear separation in the induction processes between the pre- and post-synaptic t-LTP pathways. It has been shown that both processes did not occlude each other [11]. In the model, we have obtained this condition by including in the kinetic scheme a block of retrograde messenger production for high levels of [Ca2+]i. By removing this block, one could obtain a further increase in potentiation (with a 1:4 protocol), since the two pathways would both be activated. In Fig 6C, we showed the results after removing the “RM block” from the kinetic scheme (see Fig 2A). Under this condition, the combined pre- and post-synaptic t-LTP expression during a 1:4 protocol would be substantially higher than in control, with an overall 50% increase (Fig 6C closed red squares). It should be stressed that, to the best of our knowledge, this was the only explanation for the surprising experimental finding that a 1:4 t-LTP protocol does not activate a presynaptic component that is instead induced by a 1:1 t-LTP protocol.

A second possibility to increase the LTP level during pathological conditions may be to increase BDNF release. There are clear experimental indications suggesting that BDNF expression, and therefore most likely also BDNF release, is increased in response to physical exercise ([4853]; for a recent review see [54]). A direct connection between exercise, elevated brain BDNF levels, and rescue of synaptic function in Alzheimer’s disease has been described recently [55]. Also, a compensatory elevation of BDNF in AD affected brain areas has been reported (reviewed e.g. in [56,57]), indicating that BDNF-dependent compensation of AD related synaptic deficits might be a spontaneously occurring endogenous protective mechanism that exists even in the absence of physical exercise—but that is further exploited—by physical exercise.

Next, a set of simulations was then carried out with the BDNF release being increased in all spines by 20% (Fig 6C, closed cyan squares). Also under these conditions, the model predicted an increase of the overall potentiation, which in this case was 25% higher than control. However, it should be noted that this increase would strongly depend on the number of spines that were not potentiated by the BDNF released under control conditions. An increase in BDNF release would thus recruit also those synapses and could result in an increase of the overall potentiation observed at the soma. A specific increase of BDNF release, without affecting other processes, has been previously demonstrated experimentally [11].

Taken together, the model predictions might contribute to design experimental investigations aiming to enhance LTP induction in order to rescue neuronal pathologies underlying learning deficits under pathological conditions.

Discussion

BDNF signaling drives a widespread arsenal of synaptic functional and structural plasticity processes that shape synaptic circuits in many regions of the mammalian brain. Failure of this signaling pathway is known to take part in the progressive development of pathological neurodegenerative diseases such as AD [58] and Huntington's disease [59,60] with loss of memory formation and recall. To begin to sort out the role and intricacies of the many biochemical pathways involved in these processes, a detailed kinetic model of the BDNF signaling driving LTP would be very useful. The main aim of this study was thus the construction of such a model, making available to the scientific community a starting modular representation, able to capture most of the current knowledge on BDNF-driven t-LTP. The minimal set of signaling pathways implemented here, constrained by specific experimental findings [11], include a cascade of reactions that can be individually subjected to further additions/extensions to take into account more specific molecular interactions. It can be argued that the process of releasing BDNF that will act on the same compartment where the release occurred is rather peculiar. However, it should be noted that this type of autocrine signaling is often observed for hormones and other growth factors. For BDNF, this is discussed in [3], and most recent experimental evidence is demonstrated in [11,13,61,62].

The proposed kinetic scheme is a useful framework that can be extended in future work to explore in more details additional mechanisms that are known to be involved in synaptic plasticity, such as Arc protein-dependent processes [63], the role of proBDNF/p75 signaling in t-LTD, or CAMKII auto-phosphorylation to account for synaptic bi-stability [19] and its role in both learning and forgetting [6465].

The model provides an explicit biophysically and physiologically plausible representation, at the subcellular level, of the interplay among the timing of evoked pre- and post-synaptic activity, the active properties of the membrane, and the intracellular Ca2+ dynamics. The study of the conditions under which a local Ca2+ influx can trigger the induction of t-LTP in Schaffer collateral to CA1 neurons, allowed us to suggest the requirements for BDNF-dependent signaling at individual synaptic contacts. The results obtained in this work go beyond a simple reproduction of the main experimental results on the BDNF-dependent processes underlying t-LTP. We were able to make experimentally testable predictions on how and to what extent it would be possible to affect the overall magnitude of t-LTP, by manipulating specific synaptic transmission pathways. In this respect, we explored three possible alterations:

  1. remove the block for the retrograde messenger released under high [Ca2+]i level condition; this would add a presynaptic contribution to t-LTP during the 1:4 t-LTP protocol. In order to act on this mechanism, the retrograde messenger responsible for carrying the induction signal to the presynaptic terminal, and the associated biochemical pathways, need to be identified first. The currently available experimental data seem to rule out NO or endocannabinoids as RM in this respect.

  2. increase the mBDNF/proBDNF ratio in single vesicles; the rationale for testing this hypothesis is that there is indirect experimental evidence suggesting a large range for this ratio inside the vesicles. In principle, a larger proportion of mBDNF could generate more t-LTP. In practice, however, a ratio too high would interfere with the magnitude of t-LTD induction, which depends on proBDNF/p75 signaling [1,8,9], with potentially important but unpredictable effects on cognitive functions. One possible manipulation that might reduce such unpredictable effects could be to increase the mBDNF content selectively in vesicles with initially low mBDNF/proBDNF ratio (e.g. by changing PC activity), also taking into account that the mBDNF/proBDNF ratio is known to depend on the stimulus repetition frequency [36]. However, although proBDNF/p75 signaling in conventional LTD is clearly established [6668], its role in t-LTD is currently unknown and should be addressed in future studies.

  3. increase the overall amount of BDNF released upon stimulation. This is by far the most interesting prediction, as it could have direct implications for anti-depressive therapies, known to restore cortical BDNF/TrkB signaling [69] and can possibly also counteract dementia [70]. In the model, the effect of increased BDNF release is based on the physiologically plausible hypothesis that not all of the stimulated synapses are actually potentiated by a conditioning stimulation. This can occur for a variety of reasons, such as dendritic location, and physiological variability of the local biochemical or electrophysiological conditions. The effect may be even more important during any given normal in vivo activity, rather than under the more tightly controlled in vitro conditions. Our model suggests that the non-potentiated synapses could be recruited by increasing the BDNF release, leading to an increased magnitude of t-LTP. This can be obtained by either pharmacological or external factors. For example, it has been shown that the overall expression of BDNF can be significantly increased by physical exercise [54] suggesting that more BDNF containing vesicles are available for release under these conditions. Apart from optogenetic and transcranial magnetic stimulation techniques to be designed for selective increase of BDNF release, the effect of increasing BDNF release could be experimentally tested in vitro by applying ultrasound stimulation to hippocampal brain slices, which has been shown to increase the amount of endogenously secreted BDNF [71] by more than 20%.

Another important result of the model is the clear evidence that, what is observed with electrophysiological recording at the soma, cannot be reproduced from scratch by modeling a single synaptic contact and/or a single point neuron. We consider this a key point for both modelers and experimentalists: modelers, interested in implementing biologically accurate rules for synaptic transmission, plasticity, and dendritic signal integration, must take into account the physiological variability of the biochemical processes, independently occurring at individual synapses according to the local electrophysiological and biochemical environment [72]; experimentalists, interested in extracting as much as possible information from somatic recordings, will better understand the large scatter in the time course of STDP induced synaptic changes at individual synaptic contacts, and could eventually use the model to deduce location and biochemical properties of the potentiated synapses from the STDP time course.

The mechanisms we combined to reproduce the cascade of gating activations could configure a highly unstable dynamical system. In order to demonstrate that our model is robust against small changes of the parameters, we performed 5 new simulation sets, each of which including two simulations where a specific model parameter was increased or decreased by a small fraction. These parameters were chosen because they control the most critical points of the dynamical system. For both the BDNF- and the RM-mediated plasticity chain, we have chosen to vary the [Ca2+] threshold that controls the activation of the mechanism and the gain factor that controls the amount of induced potentiation. In our model, these parameters control the beginning and the end of the chain of reactions leading to synaptic plasticity. Therefore, if the model would introduce instability at any point of its cascade of reactions, this would become evident after changing the [Ca2+] threshold and not after changing the final gain factor. In contrast, if instability was present in the system, but it was due to non-linear interaction of the potentiated synapses, the unstable behavior would be triggered by changes in either of the two parameters. For the RM-mediated plasticity chain, we selected an additional parameter in the middle of the chain. This controls the RM production and makes it relevant to distinguish between pre- or post-synaptic location of the potentially instable mechanism. The modeling results are shown in the Fig A in S1 Text. The panels A and B show the effects of parameter perturbations in the 25x 1:4 t-LTP-driven, BDNF-mediated plasticity chain. It is evident that a 20% change of the gain factor αAMPA (Eq 16) effective at the end of the activation chain yields an effect similar to a 10% change in the [Ca2+] threshold θ2 (Eqs 8 and 9), which affects the entire cascade of gating activations. The panels C and D show results for the same kind of test for the RM mediated 70x 1:1 T-plascticity chain RM-mediated. Also in this case, a parametric perturbation at the beginning (θ1 Eq 4), in the middle (θRM Eq 5), and at the end (αRMpU Eq 7) of the cascade of mechanisms yielded similar effects. These results indicate that the model is loosely sensitive to small perturbations of its key parameter, the [Ca2+] threshold. In the model construction, we used a sharp transition for the [Ca2+] threshold θ1 having set σ1 = 0.01e-3 mM to simplify the model tuning. In order to show that this very specific choice is irrelevant with respect to our results, we performed one supplemental simulation using a 100 times smoother transition (σ1 = 0.001 mM = θ1/46, Eq 4). However, this simulation did not disclose any changes in the model behavior.

One limitation of the model is the lack of LTD pathways in the kinetic scheme. The reason for this choice is the current lack of sufficiently detailed experimental constraints on the BDNF-dependent pathways that could be involved in the induction of LTD. The model can be considered a relatively simple template that can be extended to include LTD mechanisms, as soon as more experimental data become available. It should be noted that a synaptic stimulation after postsynaptic spikes (a classic induction protocol for spike-time-dependent LTD) will not generate enough Ca2+ entry to activate the cascade of processes leading to t-LTP (Fig B in S1 Text).

Finally, this model provides a foundation to investigate the multi-scale link between the short t-LTP induction period (in the seconds range) and those cases in which plastic changes occur at a much longer time-scale (several minutes), such as BDNF-dependent STDP. The delay in BDNF release may encode for crossing a threshold of relevance caused by a stimulation that shall be remembered for many seconds. For example, this delay can take into account the time of arrival of rewards expected in procedural learning and mediated by dopamine signals. So far there is experimental evidence for BDNF vesicular release lasting at least 5 min [11,29]. The release of BDNF beyond this limit, if confirmed by experimental recordings, opens up the possibility to further fine-tune, on a longer time scale, the extent of how much of the initial learning stimulus is finally converted into a long-lasting memory. These processes might involve additional biochemical cascades regulating neuromodulatory transmitter signaling such as dopamine, noradrenaline or acetylcholine pathways [72].

The overall organization of these mechanisms can thus provide, for example, the neural correlate for the synaptic eligibility traces expected in reinforcement learning to solve the distal reward problem [73]. The model presented here can be used to investigate this type of problem, first at the subcellular level in a single cell and then to extract an effective and computationally more efficient algorithm to be used in large-scale network simulations, where the use of the full implementation would require prohibitively long computing times.

Methods

Experimental methods and data

STDP experiments were performed on transversal hippocampal slices (350- to 400-μm thickness) from either P15-P23 Wistar rats (Charles River, Sulzfeld, both sexes), or P25-P35-day-old male BDNF+/- mice bred on a C57Bl/6J genetic background [74] or their WT littermates, respectively, as described previously (compare [11] and references therein). All electrophysiological experimental data were taken from [11]. In short, timing-dependent (t-)LTP was induced with repeated pairings of one presynaptically induced EPSP, evoked by stimulation of Schaffer collaterals and one, two, or four postsynaptic APs induced by somatic current injection (2–3 ms, 1 nA) via the recording electrode in current clamp configuration. T-LTP was induced after a 10 min baseline recording. Cells were held in the current clamp mode at -70 mV. Pairings were repeated 20–150 times depending on the specific protocol. T-LTP was induced by pre-post pairings (indicated by positive spike timings) consisting of either 1 EPSP/1 AP stimulation (70–100 repeats at 0.5 Hz), 1 EPSP/2 AP stimulation (50 repeats at 0.5 Hz), or 1 EPSP/4 AP stimulation (20–35 repeats at 0.5 Hz). Whole-cell recordings were performed at 30.5 °C ± 0.2 °C, with pipettes (pipette resistance 6–10 MΩ) filled with internal solution containing (in mM): 115 potassium gluconate, 10 HEPES, 20 KCl, 4 Mg-ATP, 0.3 Na-GTP, 10 Na-phosphocreatine, 0.00075 CaCl2; pH was adjusted to 7.4 using KOH (280–290 mosmol/kg). The bath solution contained (in mM): 125 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 25 NaHCO3, 0.8 mM NaH2PO4, 20 glucose, 2 CaCl2, 1 MgCl2, saturated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2 (pH 7.4; 304–306 mosmol/kg). Whole-cell recordings were obtained using either an EPC8 patch clamp amplifier connected to a LiH8+8 interface or an EPC10 patch clamp amplifier (HEKA, Germany) operated with PATCHMASTER software (HEKA, Germany). Data were filtered at 3 kHz and digitized at 10 kHz. Data analysis was performed using FITMASTER (HEKA, Germany) and Mini Analysis software (Synaptosoft, USA). Data analysis was performed as described previously [11]. Matched unpaired controls (negative controls) were performed for quality control with ongoing stimulation of 45min but in absence of any t-LTP induction paradigm [11]. Every recording of t-LTP started with 10 min baseline recording of EPSP at 0.05 Hz. EPSP slopes were calculated from the initial 2 ms after EPSP onset. The mean slope of this baseline was set to 100%. After the 10 min of baseline recording (time interval from -10 to 0 min in all graphs) the STDP protocol was executed, and all subsequent EPSP slopes were normalized to the 100% value during baseline recording.

To describe the experimental results, we extensively use the terms induction and expression to refer to two distinct phases of t-LTP; LTP induction is the pattern of electrical activity in pre- and postsynaptic neurons that triggers the second messenger processes (e.g. intracellular Ca2+ or cAMP elevation) that set in motion the biochemical processes underlying enhanced synaptic transmission; LTP expression is the biochemical mechanism that accounts for the altered synaptic strength at the potentiated synapse, e.g. incorporation of new postsynaptic AMPA receptors, increased probability of presynaptic transmitter release, spine growth.

Computational methods

All simulations were implemented using the NEURON simulation environment (v7.4, [75]) using the variable time step feature. Additional custom code was written in Python. Model files will be available for public download under the ModelDB section of the Senselab database (http://senselab.med.yale.edu, accession number 244412). For all simulations, we started from a pre-existing CA1 pyramidal cell model [30], downloaded from Senselab (accession number 55035). In this model, already validated against a number of different experimental findings achieved in rat CA1 neurons [30], voltage-gated sodium (INa) and delayed rectifier potassium (KDR) conductances were uniformly distributed throughout the dendrites, while the KA [30] and Ih (hyperpolarization induced inward current) conductance linearly increased up to 500 μm from the soma. For the purposes of this paper, the peak KA conductance was increased by 30%, with respect to its original value to account for the shorter dendrite of CA1 neurons in mice compared to rats.

To take into account local dendritic integration processes and allow for some physiological variability in the subcellular mechanisms underlying plasticity at individual synapses, eighteen explicit dendritic spines were modeled. The differences among spines are described in Results. Each spine was implemented with two compartments, one for the spine neck and one for the spine head. Following experimental indications [76], the spine neck compartment was 2 μm long with a diameter of 0.5 μm, and the spine head was 0.264 μm long and 1 μm thick.

Active ion channels were inserted in the spine head and included L-, N-, and T-type Ca2+ ion channels and Ca2+-dependent K channels downloaded from a previously published model ([77], ModelDB acc.n. 151126). The resting Ca2+ concentration was set at 50 nM and, consistently with experimental evidences for the spine specific Ca2+ dynamics [76], the spine intracellular [Ca2+] extrusion pump and buffering mechanisms were approximated with a single exponential decay (τCa = 12 ms [76]).

AMPA and NMDA receptor models

AMPA and NMDA receptor channels were placed on the spine head. Their kinetic models were adapted from [78] to include the vesicular cycling dynamics of glutamate release [31]. The effects of glutamate reuptake and diffusion away from the synaptic cleft were modeled using a fast exponential decay (τglu = 0.1 ms) for the extracellular glutamate concentration [79,80]. With respect to the original model [78], we did not use the slow adapting and [Ca2+]i-dependent component of the AMPA kinetic, which in the original paper take into account a special form of BCM like synaptic meta-plasticity rule. Also, in our simulations the intracellular and extracellular concentration of Na+ and K+ ions was fixed.

Model stimulation protocols

For the purposes of this work, we considered the following stimulation protocols delivered at 0.5Hz:

  • 1:1 t-LTP: 70–100 repetitions of a single pre and post-synaptic stimuli, for a total stimulation time of 140–200 sec.

  • 1:2 t-LTP: 50 repetitions of one pre- and two post-synaptic stimuli (at 200Hz), for a total stimulation time of 100 sec.

  • 1:4 t-LTP: 25–30 repetitions of one pre- and four post-synaptic stimuli (at 200Hz), for a total stimulation time of 50–60 sec.

In all cases, simulations were repeated using different delays, Δt from +5 ms to +30 ms, between the pre- and post-synaptic activation. Post-synaptic action potential activation was obtained with a suprathreshold somatic current pulse (1 nA for 2.5 ms). Test stimuli were delivered at 0.05 Hz before and after the induction protocols described above. The EPSP elicited by the last test stimulus delivered before initiation of the t-LTP induction protocol was used to compute the reference (100%) EPSP slope (maximum value of the time derivative in the 2 ms after EPSP onset). All figures show the EPSP slopes computed from the EPSP elicited after t-LTP induction and normalized by the reference slope described above.

Supporting information

S1 Text. Model behaviour sensitivity to parameters and effect of anti-causal stimuli.

To demonstrate that our model is robust against a change in parameter values, we ran new simulations, in which a specific model parameter was changed, in most cases, by a ±10% or ±20% fraction. This range was chosen to test if the overall parameter configuration was stable or close to instability. If the model in a stable condition, we do not expect significant changes for any parameter’s change within this relatively small range. We choose to test several parameters controlling the dynamics and the overall amount of LTP, such as the [Ca2+] and RM thresholds (θ1, θ2, and θRM Eqs 4, 5, 8 and 9), the overall gain factors, αRMpU (Eq 7) and αAMPA (Eq 16), and the steepness of the sigmoid function activating the 1:1 t-LTP pathway, σ1 (Eq 4). As can be inferred by looking at panel B in Figs 3 and 4, variations in θ1, and θ2 may result in a different number of synapses crossing the respective thresholds for LTP induction. In practice, this will result in a roughly proportional change in the overall amount of LTP observed at the soma. The ±10% change investigated here (Figure A, panels B and D) resulted in negligible difference in the overall amount of LTP. A proportional change was also observed by modifying αRMpU and αAMPA (Figure A, panels A and C). A change in the pathway producing RM was also rather robust after a ±10% change (Figure A, panel E), and the same occurred for a quite drastic 100-fold change in the parameter regulating the steepness of the function activating the 1:1 t-LTP pathway, σ1 (Figure A, panel F).

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank Michael Hines for assistance in the coding of new NEURON mechanisms and Kurt Gottmann for stimulating discussions regarding the requirement for single spine modeling of synaptic plasticity. All authors are part of the CIRCPROT Consortium (https://www.circprot.eu/).

Data Availability

All model files are available from the ModelDB section of the Senselab database (accession number 244412).

Funding Statement

This work has been supported by the EU Joint Program–Neurodegenerative Disease Research (JPND) project CIRCPROT (jointly funded by BMBF, MIUR, and EU Horizon 2020 grant agreement no. 643417) and the Horizon 2020 Framework Programme for Research and Innovation under the Specific Grant Agreement 785907 (Human Brain Project SGA2). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Park H, Poo M. Neurotrophin regulation of neural circuit development and function. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013;14: 7–23. 10.1038/nrn3379 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Reichardt LF. Neurotrophin-regulated signalling pathways. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2006;361: 1545–1564. 10.1098/rstb.2006.1894 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Lessmann V, Gottmann K, Malcangio M. Neurotrophin secretion: current facts and future prospects. Prog Neurobiol. 2003;69: 341–374. 10.1016/S0301-0082(03)00019-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Bramham CR, Messaoudi E. BDNF function in adult synaptic plasticity: The synaptic consolidation hypothesis. Prog Neurobiol. 2005;76: 99–125. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Gottmann K, Mittmann T, Lessmann V. BDNF signaling in the formation, maturation and plasticity of glutamatergic and GABAergic synapses. Exp Brain Res. 2009;199: 203–234. 10.1007/s00221-009-1994-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Cheng Q, Yeh HH. PLCgamma signaling underlies BDNF potentiation of Purkinje cell responses to GABA. J Neurosci Res. 2005;79: 616–627. 10.1002/jnr.20397 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Huang Y, Wang J-J, Yung W-H. Coupling between GABA-A receptor and chloride transporter underlies ionic plasticity in cerebellar Purkinje neurons. Cerebellum Lond Engl. 2013;12: 328–330. 10.1007/s12311-013-0453-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Edelmann E, Leßmann V, Brigadski T. Pre- and postsynaptic twists in BDNF secretion and action in synaptic plasticity. Neuropharmacology. 2014;76: 610–627. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.05.043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Leal G, Bramham CR, Duarte CB. Chapter Eight—BDNF and Hippocampal Synaptic Plasticity In: Litwack G, editor. Vitamins and Hormones. Academic Press; 2017. pp. 153–195. 10.1016/bs.vh.2016.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Cowansage KK, LeDoux JE, Monfils MH. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor: a dynamic gatekeeper of neural plasticity. Curr Mol Pharmacol. 2010;3: 12–29. 10.2174/1874467211003010012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Edelmann E, Cepeda-Prado E, Franck M, Lichtenecker P, Brigadski T, Leßmann V. Theta Burst Firing Recruits BDNF Release and Signaling in Postsynaptic CA1 Neurons in Spike-Timing-Dependent LTP. Neuron. 2015;86: 1041–1054. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.04.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Tanaka J, Horiike Y, Matsuzaki M, Miyazaki T, Ellis-Davies GCR, Kasai H. Protein Synthesis and Neurotrophin-Dependent Structural Plasticity of Single Dendritic Spines. Science. 2008;319: 1683–1687. 10.1126/science.1152864 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Harward SC, Hedrick NG, Hall CE, Parra-Bueno P, Milner TA, Pan E, et al. Autocrine BDNF–TrkB signalling within a single dendritic spine. Nature. 2016;538: 99–103. 10.1038/nature19766 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Engelhardt M, Cristo GD, Berardi N, Maffei L, Wahle P. Differential effects of NT-4, NGF and BDNF on development of neurochemical architecture and cell size regulation in rat visual cortex during the critical period. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25: 529–540. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05301.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bonhoeffer T. Neurotrophins and activity-dependent development of the neocortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1996;6: 119–126. 10.1016/S0959-4388(96)80017-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Wardle RA, Poo M. Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor Modulation of GABAergic Synapses by Postsynaptic Regulation of Chloride Transport. J Neurosci. 2003;23: 8722–8732. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-25-08722.2003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Brigadski T, Lessmann V. BDNF: a regulator of learning and memory processes with clinical potential. E-Neuroforum. 2014;5: 1–11. 10.1007/s13295-014-0053-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Guo W, Nagappan G, Lu B. Differential effects of transient and sustained activation of BDNF-TrkB signaling. Dev Neurobiol. 2018;78: 647–659. 10.1002/dneu.22592 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Kowiański P, Lietzau G, Czuba E, Waśkow M, Steliga A, Moryś J. BDNF: A Key Factor with Multipotent Impact on Brain Signaling and Synaptic Plasticity. Cell Mol Neurobiol. 2018;38: 579–593. 10.1007/s10571-017-0510-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Leßmann V, Brigadski T. Mechanisms, locations, and kinetics of synaptic BDNF secretion: An update. Neurosci Res. 2009;65: 11–22. 10.1016/j.neures.2009.06.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Zhang Y, Smolen P, Alberini CM, Baxter DA, Byrne JH. Computational model of a positive BDNF feedback loop in hippocampal neurons following inhibitory avoidance training. Learn Mem. 2016;23: 714–722. 10.1101/lm.042044.116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Petersen CCH, Malenka RC, Nicoll RA, Hopfield JJ. All-or-none potentiation at CA3-CA1 synapses. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1998;95: 4732–4737. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.O’Connor DH, Wittenberg GM, Wang SS-H. Graded bidirectional synaptic plasticity is composed of switch-like unitary events. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102: 9679–9684. 10.1073/pnas.0502332102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Markram H, Lübke J, Frotscher M, Sakmann B. Regulation of Synaptic Efficacy by Coincidence of Postsynaptic APs and EPSPs. Science. 1997;275: 213–215. 10.1126/science.275.5297.213 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Dan Y, Poo M. Spike Timing-Dependent Plasticity of Neural Circuits. Neuron. 2004;44: 23–30. 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Brigadski T, Hartmann M, Lessmann V. Differential Vesicular Targeting and Time Course of Synaptic Secretion of the Mammalian Neurotrophins. J Neurosci. 2005;25: 7601–7614. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1776-05.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Shouval HZ, Bear MF, Cooper LN. A unified model of NMDA receptor-dependent bidirectional synaptic plasticity. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2002;99: 10831–10836. 10.1073/pnas.152343099 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Graupner M, Brunel N. Calcium-based plasticity model explains sensitivity of synaptic changes to spike pattern, rate, and dendritic location. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2012;109: 3991–3996. 10.1073/pnas.1109359109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Kolarow R, Brigadski T, Lessmann V. Postsynaptic Secretion of BDNF and NT-3 from Hippocampal Neurons Depends on Calcium–Calmodulin Kinase II Signaling and Proceeds via Delayed Fusion Pore Opening. J Neurosci. 2007;27: 10350–10364. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0692-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Migliore M, Ferrante M, Ascoli GA. Signal Propagation in Oblique Dendrites of CA1 Pyramidal Cells. J Neurophysiol. 2005;94: 4145–4155. 10.1152/jn.00521.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Migliore M, Lansky P. Long-Term Potentiation and Depression Induced by a Stochastic Conditioning of a Model Synapse. Biophys J. 1999;77: 1234–1243. 10.1016/S0006-3495(99)76975-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Magee JC. Dendritic Ih normalizes temporal summation in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2: 508–514. 10.1038/9158 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Edelstein-Keshet L. Mathematical Models in Biology [Internet]. Society for Industrial and Applied Mathematics; 2005. 10.1137/1.9780898719147 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Persoon CM, Moro A, Nassal JP, Farina M, Broeke JH, Arora S, et al. Pool size estimations for dense‐core vesicles in mammalian CNS neurons. EMBO J. 2018;37: e99672 10.15252/embj.201899672 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Matsumoto T, Rauskolb S, Polack M, Klose J, Kolbeck R, Korte M, et al. Biosynthesis and processing of endogenous BDNF: CNS neurons store and secrete BDNF, not pro-BDNF. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11: 131–133. 10.1038/nn2038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Nagappan G, Zaitsev E, Senatorov VV, Yang J, Hempstead BL, Lu B. Control of extracellular cleavage of ProBDNF by high frequency neuronal activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106: 1267–1272. 10.1073/pnas.0807322106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Eckenstaler R, Lessmann V, Brigadski T. CAPS1 effects on intragranular pH and regulation of BDNF release from secretory granules in hippocampal neurons. J Cell Sci. 2016;129: 1378–1390. 10.1242/jcs.178251 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Bergami M, Santi S, Formaggio E, Cagnoli C, Verderio C, Blum R, et al. Uptake and recycling of pro-BDNF for transmitter-induced secretion by cortical astrocytes. J Cell Biol. 2008;183: 213–221. 10.1083/jcb.200806137 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Wong Y-H, Lee C-M, Xie W, Cui B, Poo M. Activity-dependent BDNF release via endocytic pathways is regulated by synaptotagmin-6 and complexin. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2015;112: E4475–E4484. 10.1073/pnas.1511830112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Watanabe S, Hoffman DA, Migliore M, Johnston D. Dendritic K+ channels contribute to spike-timing dependent long-term potentiation in hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99: 8366–8371. 10.1073/pnas.122210599 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Schinder A. The neurotrophin hypothesis for synaptic plasticity. Trends Neurosci. 2000;23: 639–645. 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01672-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Pozzo-Miller LD, Gottschalk W, Zhang L, McDermott K, Du J, Gopalakrishnan R, et al. Impairments in High-Frequency Transmission, Synaptic Vesicle Docking, and Synaptic Protein Distribution in the Hippocampus of BDNF Knockout Mice. J Neurosci. 1999;19: 4972–4983. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-12-04972.1999 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Yang J, Siao C-J, Nagappan G, Marinic T, Jing D, McGrath K, et al. Neuronal release of proBDNF. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12: 113–115. 10.1038/nn.2244 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Zhang Y, Guo O, Huo Y, Wang G, Man H-Y. Amyloid-β Induces AMPA Receptor Ubiquitination and Degradation in Primary Neurons and Human Brains of Alzheimer’s Disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2018;62: 1789–1801. 10.3233/JAD-170879 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Zhang M-Q, Li R, Wang Y-Q, Huang Z-L. Neural Plasticity Is Involved in Physiological Sleep, Depressive Sleep Disturbances, and Antidepressant Treatments. In: Neural Plasticity [Internet]. 2017. [cited 10 Jan 2019]. 10.1155/2017/5870735 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Reinders NR, Pao Y, Renner MC, da Silva-Matos CM, Lodder TR, Malinow R, et al. Amyloid-β effects on synapses and memory require AMPA receptor subunit GluA3. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2016;113: E6526–E6534. 10.1073/pnas.1614249113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Whitehead G, Regan P, Whitcomb DJ, Cho K. Ca2+-permeable AMPA receptor: A new perspective on amyloid-beta mediated pathophysiology of Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropharmacology. 2017;112: 221–227. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2016.08.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Aguiar AS, Castro AA, Moreira EL, Glaser V, Santos ARS, Tasca CI, et al. Short bouts of mild-intensity physical exercise improve spatial learning and memory in aging rats: Involvement of hippocampal plasticity via AKT, CREB and BDNF signaling. Mech Ageing Dev. 2011;132: 560–567. 10.1016/j.mad.2011.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Berchtold NC, Chinn G, Chou M, Kesslak JP, Cotman CW. Exercise primes a molecular memory for brain-derived neurotrophic factor protein induction in the rat hippocampus. Neuroscience. 2005;133: 853–861. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.03.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Gómez-Pinilla F, Ying Z, Roy RR, Molteni R, Edgerton VR. Voluntary Exercise Induces a BDNF-Mediated Mechanism That Promotes Neuroplasticity. J Neurophysiol. 2002;88: 2187–2195. 10.1152/jn.00152.2002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Neeper SA, Góauctemez-Pinilla F, Choi J, Cotman C. Exercise and brain neurotrophins. Nature. 1995;373: 109 10.1038/373109a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Soya H, Nakamura T, Deocaris CC, Kimpara A, Iimura M, Fujikawa T, et al. BDNF induction with mild exercise in the rat hippocampus. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 2007;358: 961–967. 10.1016/j.bbrc.2007.04.173 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Vaynman S, Ying Z, Gomez‐Pinilla F. Hippocampal BDNF mediates the efficacy of exercise on synaptic plasticity and cognition. Eur J Neurosci. 20: 2580–2590. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03720.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Stimpson NJ, Davison G, Javadi A-H. Joggin’ the Noggin: Towards a Physiological Understanding of Exercise-Induced Cognitive Benefits. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;88: 177–186. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.03.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Choi SH, Bylykbashi E, Chatila ZK, Lee SW, Pulli B, Clemenson GD, et al. Combined adult neurogenesis and BDNF mimic exercise effects on cognition in an Alzheimer’s mouse model. Science. 2018;361: eaan8821. 10.1126/science.aan8821 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Balietti M, Giuli C, Conti F. Peripheral Blood Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor as a Biomarker of Alzheimer’s Disease: Are There Methodological Biases? Mol Neurobiol. 2018; 1–12. 10.1007/s12035-017-0866-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Kim W, Noh H, Lee Y, Jeon J, Shanmugavadivu A, McPhie DL, et al. MiR-126 Regulates Growth Factor Activities and Vulnerability to Toxic Insult in Neurons. Mol Neurobiol. 2016;53: 95–108. 10.1007/s12035-014-8989-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Lim YY, Villemagne VL, Laws SM, Pietrzak RH, Snyder PJ, Ames D, et al. APOE and BDNF polymorphisms moderate amyloid β-related cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Mol Psychiatry. 2015;20: 1322–1328. 10.1038/mp.2014.123 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Zuccato C, Marullo M, Vitali B, Tarditi A, Mariotti C, Valenza M, et al. Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor in Patients with Huntington’s Disease. PLoS ONE. 2011;6 10.1371/journal.pone.0022966 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Zuccato C, Ciammola A, Rigamonti D, Leavitt BR, Goffredo D, Conti L, et al. Loss of Huntingtin-Mediated BDNF Gene Transcription in Huntington’s Disease. Science. 2001;293: 493–498. 10.1126/science.1059581 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Colgan LA, Hu M, Misler JA, Parra-Bueno P, Moran CM, Leitges M, et al. PKCα integrates spatiotemporally distinct Ca 2+ and autocrine BDNF signaling to facilitate synaptic plasticity. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21: 1027 10.1038/s41593-018-0184-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Jin I, Udo H, Nicholls R, Zhu H, Kandel ER, Hawkins RD. Autocrine signaling by an Aplysia neurotrophin forms a presynaptic positive feedback loop. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2018;115: E11168–E11177. 10.1073/pnas.1810649115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Nikolaienko O, Patil S, Eriksen MS, Bramham CR. Arc protein: a flexible hub for synaptic plasticity and cognition. Semin Cell Dev Biol. 2018;77: 33–42. 10.1016/j.semcdb.2017.09.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Graupner M, Brunel N. STDP in a bistable synapse model based on CaMKII and associated signaling pathways. PLoS Comput Biol. 2007;3: e221 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0030221 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Fraize N, Hamieh AM, Joseph MA, Touret M, Parmentier R, Salin PA, et al. Differential changes in hippocampal CaMKII and GluA1 activity after memory training involving different levels of adaptive forgetting. Learn Mem. 2017;24: 86–94. 10.1101/lm.043505.116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Woo NH, Teng HK, Siao C-J, Chiaruttini C, Pang PT, Milner TA, et al. Activation of p75NTR by proBDNF facilitates hippocampal long-term depression. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8: 1069–1077. 10.1038/nn1510 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Sakuragi S, Tominaga-Yoshino K, Ogura A. Involvement of TrkB- and p75NTR-signaling pathways in two contrasting forms of long-lasting synaptic plasticity. Sci Rep. 2013;3: 3185 10.1038/srep03185 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Yang J, Harte-Hargrove LC, Siao C-J, Marinic T, Clarke R, Ma Q, et al. proBDNF Negatively Regulates Neuronal Remodeling, Synaptic Transmission, and Synaptic Plasticity in Hippocampus. Cell Rep. 2014;7: 796–806. 10.1016/j.celrep.2014.03.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Castrén E. Neurotrophins and Psychiatric Disorders Neurotrophic Factors. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg; 2014. pp. 461–479. 10.1007/978-3-642-45106-5_17 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Nagahara AH, Merrill DA, Coppola G, Tsukada S, Schroeder BE, Shaked GM, et al. Neuroprotective effects of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in rodent and primate models of Alzheimer’s disease. Nat Med. 2009;15: 331–337. 10.1038/nm.1912 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Tufail Y, Matyushov A, Baldwin N, Tauchmann ML, Georges J, Yoshihiro A, et al. Transcranial Pulsed Ultrasound Stimulates Intact Brain Circuits. Neuron. 2010;66: 681–694. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Edelmann E, Cepeda-Prado E, Leßmann V. Coexistence of Multiple Types of Synaptic Plasticity in Individual Hippocampal CA1 Pyramidal Neurons. Front Synaptic Neurosci. 2017;9 10.3389/fnsyn.2017.00007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Izhikevich EM. Solving the Distal Reward Problem through Linkage of STDP and Dopamine Signaling. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17: 2443–2452. 10.1093/cercor/bhl152 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Korte M, Carroll P, Wolf E, Brem G, Thoenen H, Bonhoeffer T. Hippocampal long-term potentiation is impaired in mice lacking brain-derived neurotrophic factor. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1995;92: 8856–8860. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Carnevale NT, Hines ML. The NEURON Book. Cambridge University Press; 2006. 10.1017/CBO9780511541612 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Sabatini BL, Oertner TG, Svoboda K. The life cycle of Ca(2+) ions in dendritic spines. Neuron. 2002;33: 439–452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Bianchi D, De Michele P, Marchetti C, Tirozzi B, Cuomo S, Marie H, et al. Effects of increasing CREB-dependent transcription on the storage and recall processes in a hippocampal CA1 microcircuit. Hippocampus. 2014;24: 165–177. 10.1002/hipo.22212 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Narayanan R, Johnston D. The h current is a candidate mechanism for regulating the sliding modification threshold in a BCM-like synaptic learning rule. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104: 1020–1033. 10.1152/jn.01129.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Bergles DE, Diamond JS, Jahr CE. Clearance of glutamate inside the synapse and beyond. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1999;9: 293–298. 10.1016/S0959-4388(99)80043-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Clements JD, Lester RAJ, Tong G, Jahr CE, Westbrook GL. The Time Course of Glutamate in the Synaptic Cleft. Sci New Ser. 1992;258: 1498–1501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 Text. Model behaviour sensitivity to parameters and effect of anti-causal stimuli.

To demonstrate that our model is robust against a change in parameter values, we ran new simulations, in which a specific model parameter was changed, in most cases, by a ±10% or ±20% fraction. This range was chosen to test if the overall parameter configuration was stable or close to instability. If the model in a stable condition, we do not expect significant changes for any parameter’s change within this relatively small range. We choose to test several parameters controlling the dynamics and the overall amount of LTP, such as the [Ca2+] and RM thresholds (θ1, θ2, and θRM Eqs 4, 5, 8 and 9), the overall gain factors, αRMpU (Eq 7) and αAMPA (Eq 16), and the steepness of the sigmoid function activating the 1:1 t-LTP pathway, σ1 (Eq 4). As can be inferred by looking at panel B in Figs 3 and 4, variations in θ1, and θ2 may result in a different number of synapses crossing the respective thresholds for LTP induction. In practice, this will result in a roughly proportional change in the overall amount of LTP observed at the soma. The ±10% change investigated here (Figure A, panels B and D) resulted in negligible difference in the overall amount of LTP. A proportional change was also observed by modifying αRMpU and αAMPA (Figure A, panels A and C). A change in the pathway producing RM was also rather robust after a ±10% change (Figure A, panel E), and the same occurred for a quite drastic 100-fold change in the parameter regulating the steepness of the function activating the 1:1 t-LTP pathway, σ1 (Figure A, panel F).

(DOCX)

Data Availability Statement

All model files are available from the ModelDB section of the Senselab database (accession number 244412).


Articles from PLoS Computational Biology are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES