Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 May 9.
Published in final edited form as: Addict Behav. 2016 Mar 31;60:137–143. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.03.018

Neurocognitive, psychiatric, and substance use characteristics in opioid dependent adults

Franchesca Arias a,*, Julia H Arnsten b, Chinazo O Cunningham b, Kelly Coulehan a, Abigail Batchelder b, Mia Brisbane b, Katie Segal b, Monica Rivera-Mindt a,c
PMCID: PMC6508857  NIHMSID: NIHMS1021739  PMID: 27131800

Abstract

Aims:

To describe neurocognitive function among opioid-dependent adults seeking buprenorphine treatment and to explore the impact of lifetime psychiatric conditions on neurocognitive function. To explore the additive interaction of patient-based characteristics that may help to inform treatment.

Design:

Cross-sectional assessment of neurocognitive function, substance use, and psychiatric characteristics of adults seeking buprenorphine treatment within substance use treatment centers in New York City.

Participants:

Thirty-eight opioid-dependent adults seeking buprenorphine treatment.

Measurements:

A comprehensive battery, which included measures of executive functioning, learning, memory, verbal fluency, attention, processing speed, and motor functioning were administered. The Wide Range Achievement Test-Third Edition, the Composite International Diagnostic Interview, and an audio computer assisted structured interview were also completed. Correlations and independent sample t-tests were used to ascertain group differences.

Findings:

Thirty-nine percent of participants were impaired in global neurocognitive function (n = 15). Over one third were impaired in either: learning (n = 28), memory (n = 26), executive functioning (n = 17), motor functioning (n = 17), attention/working memory (n = 14) or verbal fluency (n = 12). Lifetime history of alcohol dependence was associated with impairment in global neurocognitive, executive functioning, and motor functioning. Lifetime history of cocaine dependence was associated with impairment in executive functioning and motor functioning (all p's < 0.05). Major depressive disorder history was not associated with neurocognitive impairment.

Conclusions:

Among this sample of opioid-dependent adults, there were high rates of global and domain-specific neurocognitive impairment, with severe impairment in learning and memory. Lifetime alcohol and cocaine dependence were associated with greater neurocognitive impairment, particularly in executive functioning. Because executive functioning is critical for decision-making and learning/memory dysfunction may interfere with information encoding, these findings suggest that opioid-dependent adults may require enhanced support for medical decision-making.

Keywords: Opioid dependence, Neurocognitive performance, Buprenorphine treatment, Depression, Substance use disorders

1. Introduction

The emerging opioid epidemic constitutes a significant public health problem in the United States (US) (Beletsky, Rich, & Walley, 2012; Jones, 2013; Jones, Mack, & Paulozzi, 2013; Manchikanti et al., 2012). In addition to the estimated 517,000 people currently living with heroin abuse or dependence, approximately 1.9 million Americans abuse or depend on opioid analgesics (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Administration [SAMHSA], 2014). This surge in opioid-related conditions has had significant social, health, and economic repercussions. In the US, women, young adults, and non-Hispanic whites are increasingly affected by opioid dependence (Heil et al., 2011; McCabe, West, Teter, & Boyd, 2012; McCabe et al., 2005; SAMHSA, 2014).

Adults with opioid use disorders are at elevated risk for infectious diseases such as HIV and hepatitis C (Jones, 2013; Meade et al., 2014; Meade, McDonald, & Weiss, 2009). Increased mortality is also associated to opioid-related disorders. In 2010, opioid analgesic overdoses comprised 60% of all fatalities related to prescription drugs in the US (Miech, Koester, & Holliman, 2013; Jones et al., 2013) and it is a leading cause of premature death among non-Hispanic whites, middle-aged men, and young adults (Calcaterra, Glanz, & Binswanger, 2013; Jones et al., 2013; Paulozzi, 2011; Paulozzi, Weisler, & Patkar, 2011). Finally, costs associated to illicit opioid consumptions are exorbitant. Estimates suggest that in the US lost productivity, medical expenses, and criminality resulting from illicit opioid consumption cost approximately $40 billion dollars each year (Ghate, Haroutiunian, Winslow, & McAdam-Marx, 2010; Hansen, Oster, Edelsberg, Woody, & Sullivan, 2011). In light of the changing demographic and the social and economic consequences characterizing the opioid epidemic in America, research in this area is relevant and necessary.

Methadone maintenance therapy (MMT) has been the gold standard intervention for opioid use disorders in the US (Amato, Davoli, Minozzi, Ali, & Ferri, 2013; Mayet, Farrell, Ferri, Amato, & Davoli, 2005). In 2002, the US Food and Drug Administration (FDA) introduced buprenorphine maintenance therapy (BMT) as an alternative treatment option (Fiellin, Kleber, Trumble-Hejduk, McLellan, & Kosten, 2004; Fudala et al., 2003). BMT confers similar benefits as MMT (Kamien, Branstetter, & Amass, 2008; Mattick, Breen, Kimber, & Davoli, 2014), but has lower potential for lethality (Walsh, Preston, Stitzer, Cone, & Bigelow, 1994), offers a more flexible medication schedule, is available via prescription; (Gryczynski et al., 2013; Mauger, Fraser, & Gill, 2014), and is less stigmatizing (Boatwright, 2002; O'Connor et al., 1996). Treatment of opioid use disorders with BMT increased steadily in the US in the last decade (Turner, Kruszewski, & Alexander, 2014). For example, BMT was the treatment of choice for opioid dependence in less than 100.000 ambulatory visits in 2003. By 2013, over 2 million ambulatory visits for opioid dependence were treated with BMT (Turner et al., 2014).

Despite the recent increase in the use of BMT to treat opioid dependence, there is a paucity of research characterizing the neurocognitive and psychiatric profiles of adults seeking BMT in the US (Dreifuss et al., 2013; Weiss et al., 2011). For example, a PubMed search using the term “neuropsychological performance and buprenorphine treatment” produced only 12 results, 6 of which were focused on issues extraneous to the topic at hand (i.e., cognitive functioning in adults receiving BMT). Of the remaining, none were conducted on US-based samples. An additional PubMed search using the term “cognitive functioning and buprenorphine treatment” produced only 17 results, eight of which were opinion articles. Of the remaining 12 studies, over 90% were conducted on non-US based samples. In total, our literature review revealed a single US-based study characterizing the neurocognitive profile of adults seeking BMT. Mintzer, Correia, and Strain (2004) examined executive functioning, processing speed, memory, and working memory in community-dwelling African American and non-Hispanic white adults with an active diagnosis of opioid dependence (N = 8). Participants ranged in age (6 to 41 years old) and in level of education (10 to 14 years). Results revealed mild cognitive impairment in long-term memory (Mintzer et al., 2004). Despite the novelty of their research design and its scientific relevance, the low sample size limited the generalizability of the findings.

The dearth of research examining neurocognitive characteristics in adults receiving or seeking BMT is particularly troublesome. Baseline cognitive abilities may impact engagement in, and adherence to, substance use treatment (Hinkin et al., 2004; Ornstein et al., 2000; Passetti, Clark, Mehta, Joyce, & King, 2008). Furthermore, a growing body of evidence suggests that patient-specific characteristic (e.g., cognitive abilities, psychiatric presentation) can be used to tailor treatment for opioid dependence. This, in turn, may help to maximize treatment outcomes (Bart, 2012; Gruber, Silveri, & Yurgelun-Todd, 2007; Nosyk et al., 2013; Okie, 2010). Explicitly, examining patients' neurocognitive profiles may serve an important role in informing clinical decision-making throughout the treatment process.

Co-occurring psychiatric conditions of adults seeking BMT in the US have not been carefully examined in the context of cognitive characteristics. For example, while Mintzer et al. (2004) evaluated the cognitive profile of adults receiving BMT across several domains, they did not examine associations between neurocognitive functioning and either psychiatric or substance use histories. Conversely, while Weiss et al. (2011) used the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) to assess psychiatric and substance use characteristics of adults receiving BMT (N = 653), they did not examine neurocognitive functioning. Moreover, Weiss et al. (2011) did not report the prevalence of mood- and anxiety-related conditions in their sample. Like Mintzer et al. (2004); Weiss et al. (2011) studied a group comprised of employed (over 60%), non-Hispanic white adults (over 90%), who had completed an average of 13 years of education. Hence, the neurocognitive, psychiatric, and substance characteristics of more ethnically diverse opioid dependent adults seeking BMT have yet to be well characterized.

The aim of the current study was to examine the neurocognitive, psychiatric, and substance use characteristics of an ethnically diverse US sample of adults seeking BMT. We evaluated global and domain-specific neurocognitive function in opioid-dependent adults presenting for BMT. In addition, we examined rates of current and lifetime psychiatric and substance use diagnoses, and explored associations between neurocognitive function and comorbid lifetime diagnoses of depressive disorder, alcohol dependence, and cocaine dependence. We hypothesized that rates of global and domain-specific impairment would be high. Additionally, we hypothesized that those participants with comorbid lifetime diagnoses of depressive disorder, alcohol dependence, and/or cocaine dependence would have significantly higher rates of neurocognitive impairment compared to those without these comorbidities.

2. Material and methods

We examined baseline data from an ongoing study (ClincalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT01108679). The goal of the neurocognitive effects of buprenorphine among HIV + and HIV − Opioid Users (NEB) study was to examine the effect of buprenorphine treatment on neurocognitive (NC) functioning over a 6-month period. A subset of the data collected as part of the NEB study is presented in this manuscript.

2.1. Setting

Participants were recruited from clinics affiliated with Montefiore Medical Center and Albert Einstein College of Medicine. Recruitment was targeted to a Montefiore community health center with a large buprenorphine treatment program and to a network of drug treatment clinics comprising Einstein's Division of Substance Abuse. Recruitment occurred through word of mouth, flyers, and health care providers. Research visits were conducted at the Clinical Research Center of the Einstein-Montefiore Institute for Clinical and Translational Research.

2.2. Participants

Eligibility criteria for study participation included: 1) current diagnosis of opioid dependence; 2) no buprenorphine use for 15 consecutive days prior to enrollment; 3) anticipated initiation of buprenorphine treatment within 30 days; 4) 18–68 years of age; 5) proficiency in English or Spanish; 6) at least 6 years of formal education; 7) documented HIV status; 8) capacity to consent to study participation. Participants were excluded if they had: 1) serious medical illness (e.g. severe liver or end stage renal disease); 2) seizure disorder; 3) psychotic or bipolar disorders; 4) current comorbid alcohol or benzodiazepine dependence; or 5) history of head trauma with loss of consciousness longer than 12 h. Of the 141 participants who were screened for the study, 41 met our inclusion/exclusion criteria and were enrolled, and 38 completed the baseline assessment. This study was approved by Institutional Review Boards at the Albert Einstein College of Medicine and Fordham University. All study participants provided written informed consent.

2.3. Procedures

After a 15-minute screening interview over the phone, eligible participants were invited for an in-person baseline interview. The baseline interview included a comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation, a structured clinical interview, and several computerized assessments. Overall, interviews lasted 4 to 4.5 h and all measures were completed in a single visit. Participants were alerted about the nature of the visit during the telephone screener and reminded via telephone 24 h prior to the visit. Participants were scheduled for their baseline within the first 10 days prior to starting buprenorphine treatment. Verbal consent was obtained prior to completing the telephone screener. Additionally, informed consent was obtained in person, during the initial phase of the visit, and participants were given ample time to make an informed decision about participating. Participants who appeared intoxicated or who reported recent substance use were re-scheduled for another time.

Unobserved urine was collected to determine active use of opioids (heroin, oxycodone, methadone, and buprenorphine) at a 300 ng/ml cutoff, benzodiazepines, amphetamines, and/or cocaine. Results were taken into consideration when interpreting the neuropsychological data. Information was collected using the methods described below.

2.4. Measures

Sociodemographic and clinical information (e.g., income, years of education, occupation, and developmental history) was ascertained using both staff-administered questionnaires and audio computerized-assisted self-interviews (Jones, 2003).

Current and lifetime history of psychiatric and substance use diagnoses were ascertained using the Composite Diagnostic Interview (Robins et al., 1988), a structured clinical interview. Current diagnoses were assigned to participants who met diagnostic criteria in the 12-month period preceding the interview; lifetime (past) diagnoses were assigned to participants who met diagnostic criteria at any point in their life. Depressive symptoms were assessed using the Beck Depression Inventory-Second Edition (BDI-II) (Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996), which was administered using audio computer assisted structured interviews (ACASI) technology. Substance use patterns were assessed using the Addiction Severity Index (ASI) (McLellan, Luborsky, Woody, & O'Brien, 1980). Hazardous alcohol use was also measured using the Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test (AUDIT) (Saunders, Aasland, Babor, De la Fuente, & Grant, 1993). Urine was collected (unobserved) to assess opioid use (heroin, oxycodone, methadone, and buprenorphine) at a 300 ng/ml cutoff, as well as use of benzodiazepines, amphetamines, or cocaine.

Neurocognitive (NC) data were collected in seven domains: executive functioning, learning, memory, verbal fluency, attention/working memory, processing speed, and motor functioning. The reading subtest from the Wide Range Achievement Test-Third Edition (WRAT-III) (Wilkinson, 1993) was administered to estimate premorbid level of intelligence. Although Spanish-speaking bilingual individuals could opt to complete the evaluation in Spanish, all instruments were administered in English by trained psychometrists using standardized procedures. Table 1 lists measures and references for normative data. Of note, norms that controlled for sociodemographic factors known to impact neurocognitive performance (e.g., age, education, race/ethnicity) were used when available (Heaton, 1993; Heaton, Miller, Taylor, & Grant, 2004). Explicitly, 10 out of 12 of the instruments used to assess neurocognitive abilities in this sample used demographically corrected norms. Table 1 indicates which demographic characteristics each instrument corrected for. Neurocognitive impairment was operationalized as one standard deviation below the mean (i.e., average T-scores <40). Psychometrists in the study were supervised by an experienced clinical neuropsychologist. Quality assurance strategies included double scoring all charts and double data entry.

Table 1.

Neurocognitive (NC) and other measures.

NC domains and tests Normative data sources
Executive function
Wisconsin card sorting task-64 item version Heaton (1993) 1, 2, 3, 4
Trail making test (Part B) Heaton et al. (2004) 1, 2, 3, 4
Attention/working memory
WAIS-III letter number sequencing Heaton, Taylor, and Manly (2003)1, 2, 3, 4
PASAT total correct Heaton et al. (2004) 1, 2, 3, 4
Learning
Hopkins verbal learning test-revised (total recall) Benedict, Schretlen, Groninger, and Brandt (1998) 1
Brief visuospatial memory test-revised (total recall) Benedict, Schretlen, Groninger, Dobraski, and Shpritz (1996) 1
Memory (delayed recall)
Hopkins verbal learning test (delayed recall trial) Benedict, Schretlen, Groninger, and Brandt (1998) 1
Brief visuospatial memory test-revised (delayed recall trial) Benedict, Schrentlen, Groninger, Dobraski, and Shpritz (1996) 1
Motor
Grooved pegboard time (dominant hand) Heaton et al. (2004) 1, 2, 3, 4
Grooved pegboard time (non-dominant hand) Heaton et al. (2004) 1, 2, 3, 4
Processing speed
WAIS-III digit symbol Heaton et al. (2003) 1, 2, 3, 4
WAIS-III symbol search Heaton et al. (2003) 1, 2, 3, 4
Trail making test (part A) Heaton et al. (2004) 1, 2, 3, 4
Verbal functioning
Controlled oral word association test (F-A-S) Heaton et al. (2004) 1, 2, 3, 4
Semantic (animal) fluency Heaton et al. (2004) 1, 2, 3, 4

Note. WAIS-III = Wechsler adult intelligence scale; PASAT = Paced auditory serial arithmetic test (PASAT); Norms corrected for the indicated demographic characteristics: 1 = age; 2 = education; 3 = gender; 4 = ethnicity.

2.5. Analysis

Raw scores for neurocognitive data were converted to demographically-corrected T-scores using the best published normative data (See Table 1). Mean domain T-scores were then calculated by averaging the T-scores of all tests within a domain, and global NC T-scores were calculated by averaging T-scores for all tests. Dichotomous variables were created to calculate proportion of neurocognitive impairment. Domain T-scores that were one standard deviation below the mean considered impaired (Heaton et al., 2004; See Table 4).

Table 4.

Neurocognitive (NC) characteristics based on average T-scores and rates of impairment (N = 38).

M (SD) NC impaired % (n)
WRAT-3 standard score 86.2 (13.6)*
Global NC function 41.2 (6.5) 39% (15)
Learning 33.9 (10.2) 73% (28)
Memory 34.8 (11.1) 68% (26)
Executive function 42.7 (9.4) 44% (17)
Motor 40.2 (9.3) 44% (17)
Attention/working memory 42.8 (8.1) 36% (14)
Verbal fluency 44.9 (9.5) 31% (12)
Processing speed 48.1 (8.5) 15% (6)

Note. Scores for the WRAT-3, used to ascertain premorbid functioning is presented in standard score (M = 100, SD = 15). T-Scores < 40 = NC impairment.

*

p < 0.05.

Independent sample t-tests tests were used to compare differences in neurocognitive performance between pre-defined groups: lifetime alcohol dependence versus no alcohol dependence, lifetime cocaine dependence versus no cocaine dependence, and lifetime Major depressive disorder (MDD) versus no MDD. Current depressive symptoms were categorized as: minimal (BDI-II 0–13), mild (14–19), moderate (20–28), or severe (>29). Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test (AUDIT) total scores greater than 8 indicated harmful or hazardous drinking.

3. Results

Table 2 summarizes demographic and clinical characteristics. The mean age was 46.4(SD = 9.6) years and the mean number of years of education was 11.6(SD = 2.6), with 55% having completed at least 12 years of education. Approximately 55% of the sample self-identified as Hispanic/Latino (n = 21), 26% as non-Hispanic black (n = 10), 13% as non-Hispanic white (n = 5) and 5% as non-Hispanic other (n = 2). Sixty-nine percent were men (n = 26) and 18% were HIV-seropositive (n = 7). Over 50% of the participants endorsed minimal depressive symptoms (n = 22). Major depressive disorder (MDD) was the most prevalent lifetime psychiatric diagnosis (31%; n = 12), followed bydysthymia (7%; n = 3).

Table 2.

Demographic and clinical characteristics (N = 38).

Male
Demographic characteristics M (SD)/% (n)

Age (yrs) 46.4 (9.6)
Education (yrs) 11.6 (2.6)
Gender
 Male 69% (26)
Race/ethnicity
 Hispanic 55% (21)
 Non-Hispanic black 26% (10)
 Non-Hispanic white 13% (5)
 Non-Hispanic other 5% (2)
Clinical characteristics M (SD)/(n)
BDI-II total score: 13.6 (10.8)
 Minimal total score < 13 57% (22)
 Mild total score between 14 and 19 13% (5)
 Moderate total score between 20 and 28 23% (9)
 Severe total score between 29 and 63 5% (2)
CIDI diagnosis:
 Major depressive disorder 31% (12)
 Dysthymia 7% (3)
HIV Status:
 HIV+ 18% (7)
 CD4 count < 200a 15% (1)

Note. BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory-II; CIDI = Composite International Diagnostic Interview.

a

% based on HIV+ participants.

Table 3 summarizes substance use diagnoses. Forty-seven percent endorsed lifetime cocaine dependence (n = 18); 34% lifetime alcohol dependence (n = 13); 18% lifetime cannabis dependence (n = 7); and 5% lifetime benzodiazepine dependence (n = 2). Lifetime diagnoses of substance abuse followed a similar pattern: cocaine was the most commonly abused substance (63%; n = 24), followed by alcohol (50%; n = 19), cannabis (47%; n = 18), and benzodiazepines (15%; n = 6). In the 30 days prior to the study visit, 94% of the sample reported using opioids (n = 36), 36% cocaine (n = 14), 47% alcohol (n = 18), 31% reported using cannabis (n = 12), and 15% reported using benzodiazepines (n = 6).

Table 3.

Current and lifetime substance use and diagnoses (N = 38).

Substance Percent (n)
Alcohol
 Positive urine toxicology at baseline NA
 Current use per ASI 47% (18)
 Lifetime dependence 34% (13)
 Lifetime abuse 50% (19)
 Hazardous drinking (AUDIT) 18% (7)
Opioids
 Positive urine toxicology at baseline 97% (37)
 Current use per ASI 94% (36)
 Lifetime dependence 100% (38)
 Lifetime abuse 97% (37)
Cocaine
 Positive urine toxicology at baseline 34% (13)
 Current use per ASI 36% (14)
 Lifetime dependence 47% (18)
 Lifetime abuse 63% (24)
Cannabis
 Positive urine toxicology at baseline 31% (12)
 Current use per ASI 31% (12)
 Lifetime dependence 18% (7)
 Lifetime abuse 47% (18)
Benzodiazepines
 Positive urine toxicology at baseline 15% (6)
 Current use as per ASI 15% (6)
 Lifetime dependence 5% (2)
 Lifetime abuse 15% (6)

Note. CIDI = Composite International Diagnostic Interview; ASI = Addiction Severity Index; Urine toxicology results obtained from sample collected on interview day; AUDIT = Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test.

Table 4 summarizes neurocognitive scores and percent impaired. Overall, average global NC and domain T-scores were mostly in the low average-to-borderline range, with learning and memory domain T-scores in the mild-to-moderately impaired range (M = 33.9, SD = 10.2; M = 34.8, SD = 11.1, respectively). More than 60% scored in the impaired range in learning and memory (Learning n = 28; Memory n = 26). Though T-scores for global neurocognitive (global NC) function, executive function (EF), verbal fluency, attention/working memory, processing speed domain, and motor functioning were in the normal range, 39% demonstrated global NC impairment (n = 15), and over 30% scored in the impaired range in EF (n = 17), verbal fluency (n = 12), attention/working memory (n = 14), and motor functioning (n = 17) were also noted.

Table 5 summarizes differences in rates of NC impairment between study participants with history of lifetime alcohol dependence versus no alcohol dependence; lifetime cocaine dependence versus no cocaine dependence; and lifetime MDD versus no MDD. Participants with lifetime alcohol dependence demonstrated worse impairment in global NC functioning (t(36) = 2.4, p < 0.05) and performed worse in EF (t(36) = 2.9, p < 0.05), motor functioning (t(33) = 3.4, p < 0.05), and processing speed (t(36) = 2.8, p < 0.05) compared to those without lifetime alcohol dependence; effect sizes for these comparisons were large (Cohen's d = 0.97 to 1.19). Participants with a history of lifetime cocaine dependence performed worse in tasks assessing EF (t(36) = 2.3, p < 0.05) and motor functioning (t(33) = 2.4, p < 0.05) compared to those without lifetime cocaine dependence; effect sizes for these comparisons were also large (Cohen's d = 0.75 and 0.81). Current or lifetime cannabis or benzodiazepines diagnoses were not significantly associated with NC impairment (p's > 0.05). Participants with a history of lifetime MDD performed worse across all NC domains than those without MDD, but none of the differences reached statistical significance. Small to large effect sizes were observed in global NC function, processing speed, and motor functioning (Cohen's d ≤ 0.70). In analyses comparing HIV seropositive to HIV negative participants, no significant differences were observed in any neurocognitive domain (all p's > 0.10).

Table 5.

Mean comparisons based on alcohol dependence, cocaine dependence, and depression with neurocognitive (NC) functioning (N = 38).

Neurocognitive domain Diagnosis of alcohol dependence
Diagnosis of cocaine dependence
Lifetime diagnosis of depression
Dependent
(n = 13)
Non-dependent
(n = 25)
Dependent
(n = 17)
Non-dependent
(n = 21)
Depressed
(n = 13)
Non-depressed
(n = 25)
M (SD) M (SD) t-Test Cohen's
d
M (SD) M (SD) t-Test Cohen's
d
M (SD) M (SD) t-Test Cohen's
d
Global NC functioning 37.9 (6.5) 42.9 (5.8) 2.4* 0.68 39.6 (6.8) 42.5 (6.0) 1.4 0.46 39.8 (8.8) 41.9 (4.9) 0.82 0.32
Learning 33.1 (12.2) 34.4 (9.2) 0.4 0.06 34.6 (10.8) 33.4 (9.8) −0.4 0.05 33.9 (13.5) 34.0 (8.3) 0.03 0.00
Memory 32.3 (12.0) 36.1 (10.6) 1.0 0.17 34.3 (11.8) 35.2 (10.8) 0.3 0.04 34.4 (13.7) 35.0 (9.8) 0.16 0.03
Executive functioning 37.2 (5.9) 44.5 (6.7) 2.9* 1.13 38.6 (6.7) 43.6 (6.6)  2.3* 0.75 40.6 (7.4) 41.8 (6.9) 0.44 0.17
Motor 34.2 (9.0) 43.8 (7.6) 3.4* 1.19 36.4 (9.6) 43.5 (8.0)  2.4* 0.81 36.3 (8.6) 42.6 (9.2) 1.9 0.70
Attention/working Memory 40.4 (7.3) 44.0 (8.3) 1.3 0.45 40.8 (8.3) 44.3 (7.7) 1.3 0.44 43.0 (8.8) 42.9 (7.8) 0.12 0.01
Verbal fluency 44.5 (8.7) 45.0 (9.9) 0.18** 0.05 45.3 (8.7) 45.3 (10.2) 0.3 0.00 43.7 (12.8) 45.4 (7.6) 0.53 0.18
Processing speed 43.2 (5.8) 50.7 (8.5) 2.8* 0.97 45.7 (9.3) 50.1 (7.3) 1.6 0.53 46.8 (11.6) 48.8 (6.4) 0.72 0.24

Note. Mean and Standard Deviations (M, SD) based on average T Scores; T-Scores < 40 = NC impairment; Diagnosis assessed using the Composite Diagnostic Interview (CIDI).

*

p < 0.05.

**

p < 0.001.

4. Discussion

We observed significant global and domain-specific neurocognitive impairment in this sample of opioid-dependent adults seeking treatment with buprenorphine. Forty-two percent demonstrated global NC impairment and one-third demonstrated impaired EF, attention/working memory, or motor functioning. Most notably, approximately three-quarters of the sample had impaired learning and memory with domain T-scores in the mild-to-moderately impaired range. Lifetime (i.e., past) alcohol and cocaine dependence were common, and participants with these comorbidities had greater neurocognitive impairment than those without. Major depressive disorder (MDD) was also common, but lifetime MDD diagnosis was not significantly associated with worse neurocognitive function.

Findings on animal models document the role of opioids in the modulation of neuronal excitability in the hippocampus (Derrick & Martinez, 1994) and link spatial learning and memory deficits to decreased hippocampal functioning (Meilandt, Barea-Rodriguez, Harvey, & Martinez, 2004; Simmons & Chavkin, 1996). The human literature, though equivocal, reports neurocognitive impairment in cognitive flexibility, inhibitory control, and working memory (Baldacchino, Balfour, Passetti, Humphris, & Matthews, 2012; Mitrovic, Dickov, Vuckovic, Mitrovic, & Budisa, 2011), as well as impaired processing and psychomotor speed, verbal learning, and verbal fluency (Loeber et al., 2012; Soyka et al., 2008) in former and current opioid users. The current study provides support for these results, as we found that approximately 70% of treatment-seeking opioid-dependent adults demonstrated impaired learning and memory. This risk is critically important for treatment providers to consider, as impaired learning and memory may translate into difficulties retaining treatment-related information and subsequently to suboptimal treatment adherence. To avoid this and enhance treatment engagement and adherence, clinical providers should consider tailoring treatment-related information for individuals at high risk for learning and memory impairment.

The complex clinical presentations that characterize opioid-dependent adults who seek treatment often include psychiatric and other substance use comorbidity (Brooner, King, Kidorf, Schmidt, & Bigelow, 1997; Highfield, Schwartz, Jaffe, & O'Grady, 2007; Schäfer, Fischer, Reimer, Karow, & Haasen, 2011). In the current study, comorbid lifetime alcohol and cocaine dependence diagnoses were common, and were significantly associated with deficits in EF. These findings extend prior research on the prevalence of executive dysfunction among opioid users with current substance-related diagnoses (Fishbein et al., 2007; Pau, Lee, & Shui-fun, 2002) by documenting that high risk of impaired EF is also present among opioid-dependent adults with lifetime (past) substance-related diagnoses. Impaired EF functioning can result in poor goal-oriented behavior and difficulty managing complex treatment regimens (Fishbein et al., 2007), and is crucial to recognize among persons with complex medical and psychiatric comorbidity. Our study is also unique in that it addresses gaps in the literature examining the synergistic effect of multiple substance-related comorbidities on neurocognitive functioning. Specifically, a lifetime diagnosis of cocaine or alcohol dependence impacts cognitive functioning in adults with opioid-dependence, which may results in functional implications.

Lifetime diagnosis of MDD was also relatively common (34%) in this sample. Previous studies on opioid-dependent adults report that current MDD diagnosis negatively affects neurocognitive performance and reduces treatment gains (Loeber et al., 2012; Schäfer et al., 2011). Although our analysis did not reveal a significant association between lifetime MDD diagnosis and neurocognitive functioning, those with lifetime MDD comorbidity performed worse in all domains than those without MDD. The impact of comorbid lifetime MDD was most pronounced in processing speed and motor functioning, with small to medium effect sizes respectively. In contrast to prior studies, we examined lifetime MDD rather than current MDD. Hence, our analysis was limited and did not capture the impact of current MDD on NC functioning.

As buprenorphine treatment gains popularity in the US, research on the neurocognitive characteristics of opioid-dependent adults seeking BMT may help treatment providers make more informed decisions in regards to treatment delivery. Additional research in this area may help elucidate the role of NC functioning in treatment success. Our findings have the potential to foster interdisciplinary collaboration among clinicians treating adults for opioid dependence. Specifically, neuropsychological evaluation may help optimize treatment outcome by offering medical providers information that may help tailor treatment interventions. Similarly, providers may opt to monitor selected patients more closely based on their neurocognitive characteristics.

Despite its strengths, this study has limitations. Of the 141 adults screened for this study, only 41 met our inclusion/exclusion criteria and were enrolled. Of note, 93% of those who met inclusion/exclusion criteria (N = 38) successfully completed our baseline evaluation. Our small sample size likely reflects two issues. First, successfully recruiting opioid dependent adults into research is challenging, (Galea & Tracy, 2007; Verdejo-García, López-Torrecillas, Giménez, & Pérez-García, 2004) particularly in ethnically diverse, low SES area such as the Bronx, NY. However, it is notable that our study was able to retain 93% of enrolled participants to complete an exhaustive baseline evaluation. The second issue is that our relatively stringent exclusion of individuals with non-substance use related neurologic and medical confounding conditions narrowed our pool of eligible participants, but were necessary to strengthen the validity of our neurocognitive findings.

Due to our relatively small sample size, we had limited power to conduct analyses to further examine our data. For instance, we did not have sufficient power to examine the multiplicative influence of co-occurring substance use on neurocognitive functioning. Future studies would benefit from larger sample sizes to investigate potential mechanisms underlying why individuals with comorbid alcohol and/or substance dependence have poorer neurocognitive function in comparison to those without such co-occurring conditions. In addition, within the current study, we may not have been able to observe the true impact of MDD, because we focused on lifetime rather than current MDD. Moreover, we did not assess anxiety disorders, including post-traumatic stress disorder, which is highly prevalent among US adults receiving opioid agonist therapy (OATs) (Brooner et al., 1997; Schäfer et al., 2011). Finally, the nature of our research design (i.e., observational, cross-sectional, single sample design) limits our ability to establish a causal association between addiction to multiple substances and NC impairment. Hence, these findings do not establish the etiology of the neurocognitive deficits observed. Additional research in this area, particularly prospective cohort studies, will help to elucidate the direction of the association between substance use disorder and neurocognitive deficits.

Additionally, given that our study did not have a comparison group, it is remains to be seen if the current findings are exclusive to a buprenorphine-seeking group or more generally applicable to those seeking other types of treatment for opioid dependence. In general, this study aimed to characterize the NC, substance use, and psychiatric profiles of community dwelling adults seeking buprenorphine treatment. Future research would benefit from more fine-grained analysis of current and past psychiatric comorbid diagnoses, as well as acute symptomology, larger samples, and more rigorous methodologies (including treatment comparison groups) to clarify the current findings. Our findings are specific to an ethnically diverse, urban population seeking buprenorphine treatment; thus caution should be exercised when generalizing these findings to other groups.

In sum, we observed high rates of neurocognitive impairment among treatment-seeking opioid-dependent adults, with three-quarters of study subjects demonstrating impaired learning and memory, and notable impairments in EF, attention/working memory, and motor functioning. Lifetime comorbid alcohol and cocaine dependence were both associated with greater neurocognitive impairment. These findings are important for treatment providers to be aware of, and should inform treatment decisions with opioid-dependent adults.

HIGHLIGHTS.

  • Community-dwelling adults, of diverse socio-economic status, seeking buprenorphine treatment.

  • A comprehensive neurocognitive battery that evaluated baseline cognitive functioning.

  • Psychiatric, functional, and psychological factors were examined.

  • Interpreted overall performance emphasizing the effect of multiple factors on neurocognitive performance.

  • Adults who present for opioid agonist treatment with buprenorphine, may exhibit diminished neurocognitive functioning. Also, preexisting alcohol and cocaine use disorders are associated with poorer scores.

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank our participants for their contributions to this research. This research was supported by the National Institute of Health P20 DA026149 and R25 DA023021 (Julia Arnsten), K23 MH07971801 (Monica Rivera Mindt), and the Einstein-Montefiore Center for AIDS Research (NIH AI-51519).

Role of funding source

This research was supported by P20 DA026149 (Julia Arnsten), K23 MH07971801 (Monica Rivera Mindt), R25 DA023021 (Julia Arnsten), and the Einstein-Montefiore Center for AIDS Research (NIH AI-51519). Funding sources had no role in the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of the data. Moreover, these sources had no role in the writing of the manuscript or the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest: none

Conflict of interest

Through this letter we confirm that there are no known conflicts of interest associated with this publication. Explicitly, there has been no significant financial support for this work, or any of its authors, that could comprise the objectivity its conclusions. Should you have any questions regarding this manuscript, do not hesitate to call me at 917-771-3161.

References

  1. Amato L, Davoli M, Minozzi S, Ali R, & Ferri M (2013). Methadone at tapered doses for the management of opioid withdrawal. Chochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 3, CD003409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baldacchino A, Balfour DJ, Passetti F, Humphris G, & Matthews K (2012). Neuropsychological consequences of chronic opioid use: A quantitative review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 2056–2068. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bart G (2012). Maintenance medication for opiate addiction: The foundation of recovery. Journal of Addictive Diseases, 31, 207–225. 10.1080/10550887.2012.694598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Beck AT, Steer RA, & Brown GK (1996). Beck depression inventory (2nd ed.). San Antonio: The psychological corporation. [Google Scholar]
  5. Beletsky L, Rich JD, & Walley AY (2012). Prevention of fatal opioid overdose. Journal of the American Medical Association, 308,1863–1864. 10.1001/jama.2012.14205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Benedict RHB, Schretlen D, Groninger L, & Brandt J (1998). Hopkins verbal learning test-Revised: Normative data and analysis of inter-form and test-retest reliability. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12, 43–55. [Google Scholar]
  7. Benedict RH, Schretlen D, Groninger L, Dobraski M, & Shpritz B (1996). Revision of the brief Visuospatial memory test: Studies of normal performance, reliability, and validity. Psychological Assessment, 8, 145. [Google Scholar]
  8. Boatwright DE (2002). Buprenorphine and addiction: Challenges for the pharmacist. Journal of the American Pharmaceutical Association, 42, 432–438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brooner RK, King VL, Kidorf M, Schmidt CW Jr., & Bigelow GE (1997). Psychiatric and substance use comorbidity among treatment-seeking opioid abusers. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 71–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Calcaterra S, Glanz J, & Binswanger IA (2013). National trends in pharmaceutical opioid related overdose deaths compared to other substance related overdose deaths: 1999-2009. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 131, 263–270. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.11.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Derrick BE, & Martinez JL Jr. (1994). Opioid receptor activation is one factor underlying the frequency dependence of mossy fiber LTP induction. The Journal of Neuroscience, 14, 4359–4367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dreifuss JA, Griffin ML, Frost K, Fitzmaurice GM, Potter JS, Fiellin DA, … Weiss RD (2013). Patient characteristics associated with buprenorphine/naloxone treatment outcome for prescription opioid dependence: Results from a multisite study. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 131, 112–118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fiellin DA, Kleber H, Trumble-Hejduk JG, McLellan AT, & Kosten TR (2004). Consensus statement on office-based treatment of opioid dependence using buprenorphine. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 27, 153–159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Fishbein DH, Flannery B, Krupitsky E, Langevin D, Bobashev GV, Verbitskaya E, … Tsoy M (2007). Neurocognitive characterizations of Russian heroin addicts without a significant history of other drug use. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 90, 25–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fudala PJ, Bridge TP, Herbert S, Williford WO, Chiang CN, & Jones K the Buprenorphine/Naloxone Collaborative Study Group. (2003). Office-based treatment of opiate addiction with a sublingual-tablet formulation of buprenorphine and naloxone. New England Journal of Medicine, 349, 949–958. 10.1056/NEJMoa022164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Galea S, & Tracy M (2007). Participation rates in epidemiologic studies. Annals of Epidemiology, 17, 643–653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Ghate SR, Haroutiunian S, Winslow R, & McAdam-Marx C (2010). Cost and comorbidities associated with opioid abuse in managed care and Medicaid patients in the United States: A comparison of two recently published studies. Journal of Pain & Palliative Care Pharmacotherapy, 24, 251–258. 10.3109/15360288.2010.501851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gruber SA, Silveri MM, & Yurgelun-Todd DA (2007). Neuropsychological consequences of opiate use. Neuropsychology Review, 17, 299–315. 10.1007/s11065-007-9041-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gryczynski J, Jaffe JH, Schwartz RP, Dušek KA, Gugsa N, Monroe CL, … Mitchell SG (2013). Patient perspectives on choosing buprenorphine over methadone in an urban, equal-access system. The American Journal on Addictions, 22, 285–291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hansen RN, Oster G, Edelsberg J, Woody GE, & Sullivan SD (2011). Economic costs of nonmedical use of prescription opioids. The Clinical Journal of Pain, 27, 194–202. 10.1097/AJP.0b013e3181ff04ca. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Heaton RK (1993). Wisconsin card sorting test: Computer version 2. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources. [Google Scholar]
  22. Heaton RK, Miller SW, Taylor MJ, & Grant I (2004). Revised comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead-Reitan battery: Demographically adjusted norms for African American and Caucasian adults. Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resources. [Google Scholar]
  23. Heaton RK, Taylor MJ, & Manly J (2003). Demographic effects and use of demographically corrected norms for withthe WAIS-III and WMS-III In Tulsky D (Ed.), Clinical interpretation of the WAIS-III and WMS-III (pp. 181–209). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  24. Heil SH, Jones H, Arria A, Kaltenbach K, Coyle M, Fischer G, … Martin P (2011). Unintended pregnancy in opioid-abusing women. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 40, 199–202. 10.1016/j.jsat.2010.08.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Highfield DA, Schwartz RP, Jaffe JH, & O'Grady KE (2007). Intravenous and intranasal heroin-dependent treatment-seekers: Characteristics and treatment outcome. Journal of Addiction Medicine, 102, 1816–1823. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2007.01998.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hinkin CH, Hardy DJ, Mason KI, Castellon SA, Durvasula RS, Lam MN, & Stefaniak M (2004). Medication adherence in HIV-infected adults: Effect of patient age, cognitive status, and substance abuse. AIDS, 18, S19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Jones R (2003). Survey data collection using audio computer assisted self-interview. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 25, 349–358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Jones CM (2013). Heroin use and heroin use risk behaviors among nonmedical users of prescription opioid pain relievers - United States, 2002–2004 and 2008–2010. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 132, 95–100. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Jones CM, Mack KA, & Paulozzi LJ (2013). Pharmaceutical overdose deaths, United States, 2010. Journal of the American Medical Association, 309, 657–659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kamien JB, Branstetter SA, & Amass L (2008). Buprenorphine-naloxone versus methadone maintenance therapy: A randomised double-blind trial with opioid-dependent patients. Heroin Addiction & Related Clinical, 10, 5–18. [Google Scholar]
  31. Loeber S, Nakovics H, Kniest A, Kiefer F, Mann K, & Croissant B (2012). Factors affecting cognitive function of opiate-dependent patients. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 120, 81–87. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Manchikanti L, Helm S 2nd, Fellows B, Janata JW, Pampati V, Grider JS, & Boswell MV (2012). Opioid epidemic in the United States. Pain Physician, 15, ES9–E38. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Mattick RP, Breen C, Kimber J, & Davoli M (2014). Buprenorphine maintenance versus placebo or methadone maintenance for opioid dependence. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 2, CD002207 10.1002/14651858.CD002207.pub4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Mauger S, Fraser R, & Gill K (2014). Utilizing buprenorphine–naloxone to treat illicit and prescription-opioid dependence. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 10, 587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Mayet S, Farrell M, Ferri M, Amato L, & Davoli M (2005). Psychosocial treatment for opiate abuse and dependence. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 1, CD004330 10.1002/14651858.CD004330.pub2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. McCabe SE, Teter CJ, Boyd CJ, Knight JR, & Wechsler H (2005). Nonmedical use of prescription opioids among U.S. college students: Prevalence and correlates from a national survey. Journal of Addictive Behaviors, Therapy & Rehabilitation, 30, 789–805. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.08.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. McCabe SE, West B, Teter CJ, & Boyd CJ (2012). Medical and nonmedical use of prescription opioids among high school seniors in the United States. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 166, 797–802. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. McLellan AT, Luborsky L, Woody GE, & O'Brien CP (1980). An improved diagnostic evaluation instrument for substance abuse patients: The addiction severity index. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 168, 26–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Meade CS, Bevilacqua LA, Moore ED, Griffin ML, Gardin JG 2nd, Potter JS, … Weiss RD (2014). Concurrent substance abuse is associated with sexual risk behavior among adults seeking treatment for prescription opioid dependence. The American Journal on Addictions, 23, 27–33. 10.1111/j.1521-0391.2013.12057.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Meade CS, McDonald LJ, & Weiss RD (2009). HIV risk behavior in opioid dependent adults seeking detoxification treatment: An exploratory comparison of heroin and oxycodone users. The American Journal on Addictions, 18, 289–293. 10.1080/10550490902925821. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Meilandt WJ, Barea-Rodriguez E, Harvey SA, & Martinez JL Jr. (2004). Role of hippocampal CA3 mu-opioid receptors in spatial learning and memory. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 2953–2962. 10.1523/jneurosci.5569-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Miech R, Koester S, & Holliman BD (2013). Towards an explanation of the recent increase in drug-related mortality Crime, HIV and health: Intersections of criminal justice and public health concerns (pp. 129–149). Netherlands: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  43. Mintzer MZ, Correia CJ, & Strain EC (2004). A dose-effect study of repeated administration of bupe/naloxone or performance in opioid-dependent volunteers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 74, 205–209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Mitrovic SM, Dickov A, Vuckovic N, Mitrovic D, & Budisa D (2011). The effect of heroin on verbal memory. Psychiatria Danubina, 23, 53–59. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Nosyk B, Anglin MD, Brissette S, Kerr T, Marsh DC, Schackman BR, … Montaner JS (2013). A call for evidence-based medical treatment of opioid dependence in the United States and Canada. Health Affairs, 32, 1462–1469. 10.1377/hlthaff.2012.0846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. O'Connor PG, Oliveto AH, Shi JM, Triffleman E, Carroll K, Kosten T, & Rounsaville B (1996). A pilot study of primary-care-based buprenorphine maintenance for heroin dependence. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 22, 523–531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Okie S (2010). A flood of opioids, a rising tide of deaths. New England Journal of Medicine, 363, 1981–1985. 10.1056/NEJMp1011512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Ornstein TJ, Iddon JL, Baldacchino AM, Sahakian BJ, London M, Everitt BJ, & Robbins T (2000). Profiles of cognitive dysfunction in chronic amphetamine and heroin abusers. Neuropharmacology, 23, 113–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Passetti F, Clark L, Mehta MA, Joyce E, & King M (2008). Neuropsychological predictors of clinical outcome in opiate addiction. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 94, 82–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Pau CW, Lee TM, & Shui-fun FC (2002). The impact of heroin on frontal executive functions. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17, 663–670. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Paulozzi LJ (2011). Drug-induced deaths - United States, 2003–2007. (1545–8636 (electronic) 0892–3787 (linking)). C Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21430623. [PubMed]
  52. Paulozzi LJ, Weisler RH, & Patkar AA (2011). A national epidemic of unintentional prescription opioid overdose deaths: How physicians can help control it. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 72, 589–592. 10.4088/JCP.10com06560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Robins LN, Wing J, Wittchen HU, Helzer JE, Babor TF, Burke J, … Towle LH (1988). The composite international diagnostic interview: An epidemiologic instrument suitable for use in conjunction with different diagnostic systems and in different cultures. Archives of General Psychiatry, 45, 1069–1077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, De la Fuente JR, & Grant M (1993). Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption–II. Journal of Addiction Medicine, 88, 791–804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Schäfer I, Fischer M, Reimer J, Karow A, & Haasen C (2011). Significance of psychiatric comorbidity for the outcome of maintenance treatment–A review of the literature. Mental Health and Substance Use, 4, 62–71. [Google Scholar]
  56. Simmons ML, & Chavkin C (1996). Endogenous opioid regulation of hippocampal function. International Review of Neurobiology, 39, 145–196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Soyka M, Lieb M, Kagerer S, Zingg C, Koller G, Lehnert P, … Hennig-Fast K (2008). Cognitive functioning during methadone and buprenorphine treatment: Results of a randomized clinical trial. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 28, 699–703. 10.1097/JCP.0b013e31818a6d38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Administration, SAMHSA (2014). Results from the 2013 national survey on drug use and health: Summary of national findings NSDUH Series H-48, HHS publication no. (SMA) 14–4863 MD: Rockville: Retrieved from http://www.samhsa.gov/data/sites/default/files/NSDUHresultsPDFWHTML2013/Web/NSDUHresults2013.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  59. Turner L, Kruszewski SP, & Alexander GC (2014). Trends in the use of buprenorphine by office-based physicians in the United States, 2003–2013. The American Journal on Addictions. 10.1111/j.1521-0391.2014.12174.x 24–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Verdejo-García A, López-Torrecillas F, Giménez CO, & Pérez-García M (2004). Clinical implications and methodological challenges in the study of the neuropsychological correlates of cannabis, stimulant, and opioid abuse. Neuropsychology Review, 14, 1–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Walsh SL, Preston KL, Stitzer ML, Cone EJ, & Bigelow GE (1994). Clinical pharmacology of buprenorphine: Ceiling effects at high doses. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 55, 569–580. 10.1038/clpt.1994.71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Weiss RD, Potter JS, Fiellin DA, Byrne M, Connery HS, Dickinson W, … Ling W (2011). Adjunctive counseling during brief and extended buprenorphine-naloxone treatment for prescription opioid dependence: A 2-phase randomized controlled trial. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 1238–1246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Wilkinson GS (1993). WRAT-3: Wide range achievement test administration manual (3rd ed.). Wilmington: Wide Range, Inc. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES