Abstract
Systemic inflammation can impair cognition with relevance to dementia, delirium and post-operative cognitive dysfunction. Episodes of delirium also contribute to rates of long-term cognitive decline, implying that these acute events induce injury. Whether systemic inflammation-induced acute dysfunction and acute brain injury occur by overlapping or discrete mechanisms remains unexplored. Here we show that systemic inflammation, induced by bacterial LPS, produces both working-memory deficits and acute brain injury in the degenerating brain and that these occur by dissociable IL-1-dependent processes. In normal C57BL/6 mice, LPS (100 µg/kg) did not affect working memory but impaired long-term memory consolidation. However prior hippocampal synaptic loss left mice selectively vulnerable to LPS-induced working memory deficits. Systemically administered IL-1 receptor antagonist (IL-1RA) was protective against, and systemic IL-1β replicated, these working memory deficits. Dexamethasone abolished systemic cytokine synthesis and was protective against working memory deficits, without blocking brain IL-1β synthesis. Direct application of IL-1β to ex vivo hippocampal slices induced non-synaptic depolarisation and irreversible loss of membrane potential in CA1 neurons from diseased animals and systemic LPS increased apoptosis in the degenerating brain, in an IL-1RI-dependent fashion. The data suggest that LPS induces working memory dysfunction via circulating IL-1β but direct hippocampal action of IL-1β causes neuronal dysfunction and may drive neuronal death. The data suggest that acute systemic inflammation produces both reversible cognitive deficits, resembling delirium, and acute brain injury contributing to long-term cognitive impairment but that these events are mechanistically dissociable. These data have significant implications for management of cognitive dysfunction during acute illness.
Subject terms: Neuroscience, Physiology, Psychology
Introduction
Peripheral infections are known to trigger episodes of acute cognitive impairment, including delirium, in older populations and in those with dementia [1, 2]. Sterile inflammation, resulting from tissue trauma or surgery, can also induce post-operative cognitive dysfunction and delirium [3, 4]. Cytokines are key mediators of septic and aseptic inflammation and, given its important role in coordinating CNS responses to systemic inflammation [5, 6], the pro-inflammatory cytokine IL-1β might be predicted equally to underlie infection- and sterile inflammation-induced cognitive dysfunction. Consistent with this idea, IL-1β levels have been associated with delirium in hip fracture patients and in septic encephalopathy [7, 8]. Delirium is a profound and acute onset brain dysfunction with impairments in attention and other aspects of cognition. Its high prevalence after surgery and infection emphasises the deleterious consequences that systemic inflammation has for cognitive function, particularly in older, cognitively impaired, populations [9, 10]. It is now clear that acute systemic inflammation and delirium also significantly increase the risk of long-term cognitive decline and dementia [11–13], and accelerate the course of existing dementia [14, 15]. Despite the enormous medical and economic implications [16] of these findings, whether systemic inflammation-induced acute dysfunction and acute brain injury occur by overlapping or discrete mechanisms has not been invest
It is established that IL-1 impacts on cognitive function [17, 18]. IL-1 disrupts consolidation of context-associated fear memory [19–21] and IL-1 receptor antagonist (IL-1RA) is protective against post-acquisition memory consolidation deficits induced by both surgery and systemic lipopolysaccharide (LPS) [20, 22, 23]. However, it is likely that systemic inflammation impacts upon multiple cognitive processes. Furthermore, long-term memory consolidation is manifestly different both from the acute fluctuating short-term memory processes that are affected in delirium and in relevant models [9, 24] and from acute neuronal death events described after systemic inflammation [25]. Given the massive public health burden of dementia and delirium, both of which have been associated with IL-1 [26,27], it is important to characterise differential roles of IL-1β in these processes. In the current study we hypothesised that (1) bacterial endotoxin (LPS; lipopolysaccharide) would have multiple effects on cognition and neuronal integrity and that (2) IL-1 would be causative in these changes.
Materials and methods
Animals
Female C57BL/6 mice at 8–12 weeks of age (Harlan Olac Ltd, UK) were housed in cages of 5 at 21 °C with a 12:12 h light–dark cycle with food and water ad libitum. To test the effects of LPS in cognitive function in young healthy mice (2–4 months) C57BL/6 mice were injected intraperitoneally (i.p.) with 100 (or 200) μg/kg of LPS (Salmonella Equine abortus, Sigma (L5886)) in sterile saline. To examine effects of LPS in animals with prior synaptic loss mice were first intrahippocampally inoculated with 1 µl of 10% w/v prion infected- (ME7) or normal brain homogenate (NBH), at mm from bregma: anterior–posterior −2.0, lateral −1.7, depth −1.6) using a Hamilton microsyringe, under anaesthesia with intra-peritoneal 2,2,2-tribromoethanol. Experimental groups (ME7, NBH) were then injected i.p. with 100 μg/kg of LPS at 15–16 weeks post inoculation (demonstrated vulnerability to LPS-induced working-memory deficits [28]). Control animals received non-pyrogenic saline.
Alternative inflammatory stimuli and anti-inflammatory interventions were also examined: IL-1β (R&D Systems, Minneapolis, MN, USA, 401-ML/CF) and TNF-α (Peprotech, Rocky Hill, NJ, USA, 305-01A) were injected at 15 µg/kg at 50 µg/kg respectively in sterile saline (i.p.). IL-1RA (10 mg/kg, i.p.; Kineret, Biovitrum, Sweden) was given immediately before LPS or IL-1β (15 µg/kg); dexamethasone-21-phosphate (Sigma, D1159) was administered i.p. at a dose of 2 mg/kg, 60 min before LPS (sufficient to robustly suppress the systemic secretion of IL-1β, TNF-α and IL-6 [29]. Samples sizes for all groups in all animal experiments were chosen on the basis of our prior published studies using the ME7 + LPS paradigm [25, 28, 30] and are specified in the appropriate figure legends.
IL-1RI−/− mice (B6.129S7-Il1r1<tm1Imx>; kindly provided by Prof. Kingston Mills) have a null mutation in Il1r1 and were backcrossed seven times to C57BL/6 before maintenance as an inbred colony (with C57BL/6 as controls). IL-1RI−/− and C57BL/6 mice were not significant different in working memory or contextual fear conditioning (CFC) tasks [31]. Body temperature was measured using a rectal probe (TH-5 thermoprobe, Physitemp, NJ) 18 h post challenge with LPS (750 µg/kg i.p.) in WT and IL-1RI−/− mice inoculated with ME7 or NBH.
All animal procedures were performed in accordance with Irish Department of Health & Children, Health Products Regulatory Authority and UK Home Office regulations and all efforts were made to minimise suffering to the animals.
Working memory: Food-rewarded and escape from water T-maze alternation tasks
We assessed short-term/working memory using alternation behaviour in both food-rewarded and escape-from-water-motivated T-maze tasks (3–9 h post LPS and 1 to 7 h post IL-1β). These times were based on pilot data and are justified by the recovery by 5 h in IL-1-treated mice. In both cases mice were placed in the start arm of the maze with one arm of the T blocked such that they were forced to make a left (or right) turn, predetermined by a pseudo-random sequence (equal numbers of left and right turns, no more than two consecutive runs to the same arm). Mice could then escape the maze via an escape tube before being held in a holding cage for 25 s (water version) or could remain in the forced arm for 30 s to consume 70 µl of sweetened condensed milk (food-rewarded version). Mice were then replaced in the start arm and had to choose the alternate arm to once again escape the maze or to receive a second food reward. Ten or 15 trials were performed on these tasks under the influence of systemic inflammation. Only in animals who had achieved a criterion baseline performance of ≥70% (water) or ≥80% (food-rewarded) alternation for 2 or more consecutive days. Baseline T-maze performance was ranked and animals were assigned to different treatments groups such that all treatment groups had equivalent, or very similar, baseline performance. Experimenters were blind to treatment during scoring of cognitive function. This water-escape task has previously been published [28] and a full description of these methods is available in supplementary material.
Contextual fear conditioning
CFC was recorded using a clear perspex box (40 cm × 10 cm × 16 cm) with a floor containing metal rods connected to a shock generator (UGO Basile, Italy). The mice were placed into the box and allowed to explore for 2 min. A tone at 2.9 kHz for 20 s was presented, followed by a shock of 0.4 mA for 2 s. This tone/shock pairing was repeated after 2 min. After a further 30 s of exploration mice were removed to a holding cage. After 30 s in the holding cage saline or LPS (±saline or IL-1RA, at discrete i.p. sites) were administered before returning the animals to the home cage. IL-1RA was administered at this time (rather than preceding the test of retention) because it is consolidation, rather than retention, of memory that has been shown to be impaired by LPS [32, 33]. Fear conditioning was assessed for 5 min in the same location 48 h later. Freezing was defined as the complete absence of movement, except those related to respiration [34]. Auditory fear conditioning was also assessed 48 h post-fear conditioning. Animals were placed in a different context (a novel empty cage) for 6 min and were allowed to explore for 3 min (baseline) before presentation of the tone for 20 s. The time spent freezing during the final 3 min was recorded (i.e. during and post tone).
ELISA for cytokines
Under terminal anaesthesia, blood was collected directly from the right atrium into heparinised tubes, was centrifuged at 3000 rpm for 15 min at 4 °C and the remaining plasma aliquoted and stored at −20 °C. Samples were then analysed for CCL2 and CXCL1 using R&D systems sandwich-type duo set ELISA kits (DY479, DY453) while IL-1β was analysed using a Quantikine kit (R&D systems, Minneapolis, MN, USA, MLB00C). A standard protocol was followed as previously described [30] except for IL-1β, which was as per manufacturer’s instructions with minor modifications. To ensure that all cytokines were reliably quantifiable using the appropriate standard curves, samples were serially diluted for CCL2 and CXCL1 (1/9, 1/81 and 1/243). Blood and brain samples were also assayed for the presence of human IL-1RA using an R&D Systems quantikine assay (DRA00B), performed according to manufacturers’ instructions (standards 0–2000 pg/ml). Hippocampal/thalamic tissue punches were homogenised in 150 mM NaCl, 25 mM Tris-HCl and 1% Triton X100 at pH 7.4 before centrifugation at 14,000 rpm for 10 min. Supernatants were diluted 1 in 2 in assay diluent in wells pre-coated with anti-human IL-1RA polyclonal antibody.
Quantitative PCR
The isolation of total RNA, synthesis of cDNA and analysis of transcription by quantitative PCR were performed as previously described [35]. Briefly, after transcardial perfusion, the hippocampus and dorsal thalamus were punched out of 2 mm thick coronal brain sections, snap frozen in liquid nitrogen and stored at −80 °C. Total RNA was extracted using Qiagen RNeasy Plus™ mini kits, with Qiashredders (Qiagen, Crawley, UK, #74134, #79654) according to manufacturer’s instructions. Contaminating gDNA was removed using the Qiagen RNase-free DNase I enzyme (Qiagen #79254). RNA yields were determined by spectrophotometry at 260 and 280 nm and stored at −80 °C. Using a High Capacity cDNA Reverse Transcriptase Kit (Applied Biosystems, Warrington, UK), cDNA was synthesised using 200 ng of total RNA in a 10 μl reaction volume. 1 μl of the reverse transcription reaction was used for quantitative PCR. Reagents were supplied by Applied Biosystems (SYBR® Green PCR Master Mix; 4364344) and Roche (FastStart Universal Probe Master [Rox], Lewes, UK; 04914058001). Assays were designed using Primer Express software and published sequences for the genes of interest. All primer sequences were as previously published [25]. Assays were quantified using a relative standard curve, as previously described [35] constructed from cDNA, synthesised from 1 μg total RNA isolated from mice showing up-regulation of all target transcripts of interest.
Electrophysiology
Transverse hippocampal slices (300 mm) were prepared from brains of NBH or ME7 mice at 18–19 weeks post inoculation and from IL-1RI−/− mice at 8 months of age. Slices were cut in ice-cold artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF) solution containing (in mM) 75 sucrose, 87 NaCl, 25 NaHCO3, 2.5 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 0.5 CaCl2, 7 MgCl2, 10 d-glucose, 1 ascorbic acid and 3 pyruvic acid. During incubation and experiments slices were perfused with aCSF containing (in mM) 125 NaCl, 25 NaHCO3, 2.5 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 2 CaCl2, 1 MgSO4 and 25 d-glucose. The slices were maintained at 33 °C for 1 h following dissection and all recordings were performed at physiological temperature (32–34 °C). Whole-cell patch-clamp recordings were made from CA1 pyramidal neurons, visualised using an upright microscope (Olympus BX51 WI, Middlesex, UK) with infra-red differential interference contrast optics (IR-DIC). Patch pipettes were filled with intracellular solution containing (in mM) 130 KMeSO4, 10 KCl, 0.2 EGTA, 10 HEPES, 20 phosphocreatine, 2 Mg2ATP, 0.3 NaGTP, 5 QX-314, 1 TEA (pH 7.3, 290–300 mOsm). Cells were voltage-clamped at −60 mV and input resistance and membrane capacitance were measured in response to a 10 mV depolarising voltage pulse. During current clamp recordings, current injection was initially set to maintain membrane potential at −60 mV and was not altered for the duration of experiments. Recordings were made using a Multiclamp 700B (Molecular Devices, Foster City, CA). Signals were filtered at 5 kHz using a 4-pole Bessel filter and were digitised at 10 kHz using a Digidata 1440 analogue-digital interface (Molecular Devices). Data were acquired and analysed using PClamp 10 and Clampfit (Molecular Devices). IL-1β was bath-applied to slices at concentrations of 0.1 ng/ml and 1 ng/ml.
TUNEL immunohistochemistry
Immunohistochemistry for apoptotic cells was performed on formalin-perfused, wax embedded tissue from ME7 animals (wild-type and IL-1R1−/−), 18 h post LPS (750 µg/kg i.p.) using the ‘Dead End’ TUNEL staining method (Promega, Southampton, UK; G3250). Non-specific peroxidase activity was eliminated by incubating sections in 1% H2O2 for 10 min. Sections were washed in 0.85% NaCl for 5 min and PBS washed, before 5 min pre-treatment with proteinase K (10 μg/ml). After PBS washing, sections were preincubated with equilibration buffer (10 min) and then with TUNEL buffer (22 μl equilibration buffer, 2.5 μl nucleotide mix, 0.5 μl TdT enzyme per section) for 2 h at 37 °C, labelling apoptotic cells with fluorescein. After reaction termination with 2× sodium citrate (15 min) and PBS washing sections were blocked with 10% normal goat serum (30 min) and incubated with biotinylated anti-fluorescein antibody (5 μg/ml). Thereafter the Avidin–Biotin-Complex protocol was performed according to manufacturers instructions, using H2O2 as substrate and diaminobenzidine as chromagen. TUNEL-positive cells that also showed evidence of nuclear condensation were counted in 10 µm coronal sections of ME7 animals treated with LPS in wild-type and IL-1R1−/− mice. At least two sections were counted, by an observer blind to the experimental conditions and averaged for each animal.
Statistical analyses
Behavioural data were compared by repeated measures ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc tests performed after significant main effects. Molecular, electrophysiology and TUNEL data were analysed by two-way ANOVA, while temperature data were analysed by three-way ANOVA, followed by Bonferroni post hoc tests for a priori selected pairwise comparisons. Sample sizes and variance were similar for all key comparisons.
Results
Differential effects of LPS on different hippocampal-dependent cognitive tasks
We assessed working memory using T-maze alternation tasks requiring both attention to, and retention in the working memory of (for 25 s), the prior location of an exit in order to determine the new exit location. LPS did not impair working memory in food-rewarded T-maze alternation at 3–7 h post treatment at either 100 or 200 µg/kg i.p. (Fig. 1a). To validate this finding in an aversively motivated working memory task, not reliant on motivation for appetitive rewards, we also used an ‘escape from shallow water’ T-maze working memory task specifically adapted for use in animals experiencing acute sickness behaviour. LPS (100 µg/kg) had no impact on working memory in C57BL6 mice in this T-maze (Fig. 1b).
However, LPS (100 µg/kg i.p.) given immediately post acquisition, was sufficient to significantly decrease freezing in CFC 48 h after exposure to context and foot-shock pairing (Fig. 1a, p < 0.001) but had no impact on auditory fear conditioning (Fig. 1d). In the CFC task LPS is known to impair consolidation (in the early hours after exposure to shock) rather than the retrieval of that memory 48 h later [32, 33]. Therefore, systemic LPS has differential effects on two hippocampal-dependent tasks: disrupting consolidation of contextual memory but not affecting working memory in normal animals.
LPS differentially affects working memory in vulnerable animals despite equivalent circulating IL-1β
LPS (100 µg/kg) was sufficient to produce robust, acute and transient working memory deficits in animals with prior neurodegenerative pathology (ME7 model of prion disease). Impairment peaked at 5 h and had resolved by 24 h (Fig. 1e), replicating our prior studies [9]. LPS affected performance differently in NBH and ME7 animals (Repeated measures three-way ANOVA: interaction between disease and treatment, F1,32 = 4.64, p = 0.0388). ME7 + LPS at its nadir (5 h) was significantly different to both ME7 + saline and NBH + LPS (p < 0.01, Bonferroni post hoc). Therefore working memory remains intact when normal animals are LPS-challenged but is impaired when animals with neurodegeneration are similarly challenged. Despite the increased susceptibility of ME7 animals to working memory deficits in response to LPS, performance in the CFC task was equally susceptible to LPS disruption in normal and ME7 animals (Fig. S1).
This selective vulnerability of ME7 animals to LPS could potentially be underpinned by differential circulating IL-1β. Peripherally administered LPS (100 µg/kg) induced robust systemic IL-1β synthesis and IL-1β mRNA transcription in the hippocampus at 2 h post challenge. Plasma IL-1β levels were equivalent in ME7 and NBH animals (Fig. 1f), although brain IL-1β mRNA was exaggerated in ME7 + LPS (Fig. 1g), as previously described [25, 28].
Dissociable effects of IL-1RA treatment on LPS-induced cognitive deficits
Since acute cognitive impairments following systemic LPS were immediately preceded by robust circulating IL-1β, we hypothesised that systemic IL-1β contributed to these deficits and we therefore assessed whether peripheral treatment with the receptor antagonist IL-1RA would protect against the T-maze and CFC deficits described in Fig. 1. IL-1RA at 10 mg/kg (i.p.) significantly protected against LPS-induced T-maze deficits in ME7 animals in a time-dependent manner (Fig. 2a). There was a significant effect of treatment (F2,234 = 4.78, p = 0.139) and an interaction of treatment and time (F12,234 = 3.67, p < 0.0001) and Bonferroni post hoc comparison showed a highly significant difference between ME7 + LPS + IL-1RA and ME7 + LPS + veh at 5 h (p < 0.001). However, both LPS-treated groups were equally impaired at 7 h, so protection is robust at 5 h, when LPS-induced deficits peak, but does not last.
Conversely, systemic IL-1RA had no protective effect against the LPS-induced CFC deficit despite mitigating the effects of LPS on open field activity (Fig. 2b). LPS robustly induced decreased freezing in C57BL6 mice (main effect of LPS by two-way ANOVA, F3,36 = 38.68, p < 0.0001). Freezing, 48 h after co-treatment with LPS + IL-1RA (10 mg/kg i.p.), was not significantly different from that induced by LPS (Fig. 2a, no effect of drug: F = 0.08). Therefore peripheral IL-1RA is protective against LPS-induced working memory deficits and against suppression of open field activity but not against LPS-induced impairment of consolidation of contextual memory. Although these LPS-induced deficits become manifest across different timescales (i.e. 3–7 h for T-maze and at 48 h for CFC), LPS and IL-1β impair consolidation of contextual memory in the first hours after exposure to shock and context pairing [33, 36]. Therefore both T-maze and CFC tasks illustrate inflammation-induced disruption of cognitive processes in the hours directly after LPS administration, so it was necessary to apply IL-1RA during this period.
We considered it essential to confirm that i.p. IL-1RA (10 mg/kg) actually blocked IL-1β action so we assessed its ability to block well-recognised indices of systemic and central (hippocampal) inflammatory effects of LPS (100 μg/kg) and IL-1β (15 μg/kg). A range of pro-inflammatory mediators are induced across the timecourse 1–8 h post LPS or IL-1β [28, 37] and here we chose 2 h to capture the peak of most of these. Plasma was prepared 2 h post treatment with recombinant IL-1RA (10 mg/kg, i.p.) and simultaneous injection of IL-1β, LPS or saline. Both LPS and IL-1β increased plasma CXCL1 and CCL2. IL-1RA had no effect on LPS-induced circulating chemokine but completely blocked IL-1-induced chemokine (p < 0.05, Bonferroni post hoc after significant one way ANOVA, Fig. 2c). Hippocampal expression of pro-inflammatory genes IL-1β and PTX3 was also increased by both systemic LPS and IL-1β treatments (Fig. 2d). Once again IL-1-induced increases in both IL-1β and PTX3 were inhibited by systemic IL-1RA (p < 0.05), but this treatment failed to block, and in fact even enhanced, LPS-induced hippocampal increases in IL-1β and PTX3 (Fig. 2d). These data indicate that circulating LPS still alters plasma and brain inflammatory profiles despite successful inhibition of systemic IL-1β action: LPS signals to the brain despite absence of systemic IL-1β activity.
We also assessed whether effects of IL-1RA were different in ME7 by examining expression of these same indices of LPS and IL-1 actions in NBH and ME7 animals 3 h after treatment with LPS ± IL-1RA. As before, LPS robustly increased the circulating levels chemokines CXCL1 and CCL2 but IL-1RA had limited impact on these levels, with huge circulating concentrations remaining regardless of treatment (Fig. 2e). Two-way ANOVA analysis followed by Bonferroni post hoc analysis showed that IL-1RA did not reduce circulating chemokines in ME7 animals: There were no significant main effects or interactions for CXCL1, and while there was an interaction between disease and treatment for CCL2 (F1,12 = 13.39, p = 0.003), Bonferroni post hoc analysis showed that CCL2 was not different between ME7 + LPS and ME7 + LPS + IL-1RA.
Hippocampal expression of pro-inflammatory genes IL-1β and PTX3 was also examined in ME7 and NBH and disease-associated increases were apparent for both transcripts (as previously reported in ref. [38]): main effect of disease (F1,16 ≥ 22.85, p ≤ 0.0002). LPS produced significant increases in both transcripts and IL-1RA did not decrease these, and indeed IL-1β mRNA was significantly increased in ME7 + LPS animals when treated with IL-1RA (Bonferroni post hoc, p < 0.01). Therefore, systemic IL-1RA did not reduce hippocampal IL-1β mRNA. Collectively these data demonstrate that even though IL-1RA blocks circulating IL-1β actions, LPS still induces brain inflammatory activation, including IL-1β.
Finally we used anti-human IL-1RA ELISA on these in vivo tissues to assess whether peripherally applied human IL-1RA reaches the hippocampus, since it could be hypothesised that IL-1RA could show protective effects because of compromised blood brain barrier (BBB) allowing access of IL-1RA selectively to the ME7 hippocampus. Low levels of human IL-1RA were detectable in the hippocampus of all IL-1RA-injected animals but concentrations were equivalent in NBH and ME7 animals (Fig. 2g). These levels (60–70 pg/ml/mg protein) are almost 7500-fold lower than plasma levels (481,511 ± 95,460 pg/ml). These data indicate that (i) a tiny fraction of IL-1RA penetrates the brain parenchyma and (ii) that this is not significantly higher in neurodegeneratively diseased (ME7) animals. Moreover, although IL-1RA is known to have a short half-life [22, 39], the levels measured in the blood at 3 h post treatment show that IL-1RA remained approximately 2000-fold higher than circulating IL-1β at what is a key stage in inducing working memory dysfunction and contextual memory consolidation.
IL-1β, TNF-α and redundancy in inflammation-induced cognitive dysfunction
To further understand the contribution of IL-1 signalling to the acute working memory deficits induced by LPS in ME7 mice, we inoculated wild-type and IL-1R1−/− mice with ME7 and, at 16 weeks into disease progression, challenged these mice with LPS (100 μg/kg i.p.) or saline (Fig. 3a). LPS treatment induced working memory impairments in IL-1R1−/− ME7 animals (main effect of treatment: F12,245 = 16.61, p < 0.0001) but the deficits were not significantly different to those in wild-type ME7 + LPS animals at any time point (All Bonferroni post hoc tests p > 0.05, Fig. 3a). Thus, IL-1RI is not indispensable for LPS-induced acute working memory deficits in ME7-inoculated animals. While this may appear contradictory, IL-1RI−/− mice also showed the normal appearance of LPS-induced sickness and weight loss (equivalent at 24 h post LPS in WT and IL-1R1−/− mice: 3.22 g vs 2.96 g) and this is consistent with previous data showing that IL-1R1−/− mice display the full spectrum of innate immune responses to LPS (http://jaxmice.jax.org/strain/003245.html; ref. [40]). To confirm this we assessed cFOS responses in known IL-1-responsive brain regions after systemic LPS (100 µg/kg i.p.). These data are shown in supplementary table 1. Despite this, systemic IL-1RA was still effective in reducing LPS-induced sickness behaviour (Fig. 2b). Collectively these data support the concept of significant redundancy of specific cytokines in coordination of inflammatory responses to LPS. Therefore, while IL-1 is indisputably important in inducing sickness behaviour responses (ref. [41] and Fig. 2b), and here systemic IL-1RA is protective against LPS-induced cognitive deficits, other cytokine systems can mediate roles of IL-1 when animals have developed in the absence of IL-1RI−/− signalling.
To accommodate this redundancy in pro-inflammatory cytokine functions, we used dexamethasone-21-phosphate (DEX; 2 mg/kg) to block multiple LPS-induced pro-inflammatory cytokines and to assess its impact on LPS-induced T-maze impairments in IL-1R1−/− ME7 mice (Fig. 3b). This dose was sufficient to block systemic secretion of IL-1β, TNF-α and IL-6 (Fig. S2 and ref. [29]). DEX alone had no significant impact on T-maze alternation. LPS induced working memory impairments in ME7 animals and this was significantly reduced in animals pre-treated with DEX (Fig. 3b). Repeated measures two-way ANOVA showed a main effect of treatment (F2,30 = 8.7, p = 0.001) and a treatment × time interaction (F12,180 = 2.28, p < 0.01). ME7 + LPS was significantly different to both DEX groups at 3 h (p < 0.01; Bonferroni), although ME7 + LPS and ME7 + LPS + DEX were no longer significantly different at 7 h. Thus DEX protects against LPS-induced deficits but some cognitive impairment eventually occurs despite DEX inhibition of systemic cytokine synthesis.
These data suggested that systemic cytokines contribute to LPS-induced working memory impairments. Therefore, we interrogated the roles of specific systemic cytokines. We treated animals with IL-1β (15 µg/kg i.p.) and assessed for working memory deficits. Moreover, it has been demonstrated that in IL-1RI−/− mice, TNF-α compensates for the lack of IL-1 signalling and mediates sickness behavioural and weight loss responses to LPS [42]. Therefore, in separate experiments, the impact of systemically administered TNF-α on T-maze performance was assessed in NBH and ME7 animals. Impairments were not produced by either cytokine in NBH animals but both IL-1β and TNF-α challenges rapidly induced acute working memory deficits in ME7 animals that had largely resolved by the 5–7 h timepoint (Fig. 3c). The IL-1β experiment showed an interaction of disease and treatment (F1,53 = 5.42, p = 0.0236) and planned Bonferroni pairwise comparisons showed significant differences between ME7 + saline and ME7 + IL-1β at 1 and 3 h and between ME7 + saline and ME7 + TNF-α at 1 h (p < 0.05). As previously described [43] ME7 + TNF-α animals were significantly different from both NBH + TNF-α and from ME7 + saline at 1 h after a significant interaction of disease, treatment and time by three-way ANOVA (F6,328 = 3.09, p < 0.01). Thus, systemic IL-1β and TNF-α cause acute and transient working memory deficits in animals with prior neurodegenerative pathology but have no effect on working memory in normal animals. These deficits are somewhat milder than those induced by LPS and recovery is apparent by 5 h.
Neuronal sensitivity to IL-1β
Since equivalent systemic IL-1 levels produce differential cognitive outcomes in ME7 and NBH animals, we predicted that neurons of the degenerating brain might also be more sensitive to equivalent concentrations of IL-1. To address this hypothesis we applied IL-1β directly to ex vivo hippocampal slices and performed whole-cell patch clamp recordings from CA1 pyramidal cells from NBH and ME7 animals at 18–19 weeks post inoculation.
CA1 pyramidal neurons from ME7 animals had a significantly depolarised resting membrane potential (Vm) compared to those from NBH animals (−61 ± 2 mV in NBH and −51 ± mV in ME7, n = 13 cells from five NBH animals and 24 cells from eight ME7 animals, unpaired t-test, *p < 0.0001, Fig. S3).
We then tested the effects of bath application of IL-1β while recording from cells in current clamp mode, using current injection adjusted to maintain membrane potential at −60 mV during baseline recordings (Fig. 4a–c). Neurons from ME7 animals were significantly more sensitive to IL-1β, which depolarised resting membrane potential by 21 ± 4 mV at 0.1 ng/ml in ME7 animals (n = 13 cells from five animals) but had little effect in NBH animals (1.5 ± 2 mV; n = 5 cells from three animals; p = 0.013, Bonferroni post hoc, Fig. 4d). At 1 ng/ml, IL-1β depolarised ME7 animals’ resting membrane potential by 44 ± 7 mV (n = 5 cells from three animals) but also had milder effects in NBH animals (6 ± 3 mV; n = 5 from three animals; p < 0.001, Bonferroni post hoc, Fig. 4d). ME7 were thus, significantly more sensitive to IL-1β-induced depolarisation at both IL-1β concentrations tested.
As a consequence, IL-1β induced action potential firing in ME7 CA1 neurons at 0.1 ng/ml, with a maximum firing rate of 30 ± 9 Hz (0.1 ng/ml, n = 13 cells) but did not do so in NBH animals (Fig. 4e). At 1 ng/ml, IL-1β induced spiking at maximum rates of 23 ± 6 Hz (1 ng/ml, n = 5 cells, Fig. 4e) in ME7 animals and at a highly variable maximum rate of 58 ± 29 Hz in NBH neurons (n = 5 cells, Fig. 4e). The lower firing rates in ME7 cells can be attributed to the greater depolarisation induced by IL-1β, acting to increase sodium channel inactivation and therefore limiting spike firing frequency. Depolarisation of ME7 neurons persisted even after a more prolonged washout of IL-1β, with cells becoming sufficiently depolarised to inhibit further action potential firing, suggesting that exposure to IL-1β had an irreversible, detrimental effect on these cells.
The effect of IL-1β on ME7 neurons does not appear to be mediated by modulation of excitatory synaptic transmission since 1 ng/ml IL-1β had no effect on evoked excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSC amplitude 105 ± 32% of control amplitude, n = 4 cells from two ME7 animals, Supplementary Fig. S4). The ability of IL-1β to induce spiking activity in CA1 neurons was dependent on IL-1R1 expression since IL-1β had little effect on resting membrane potential and did not induce action potential firing in slices from IL-1R1−/− animals (n = 4 cells from two animals, Fig. 4c–e). Moreover, TNF-α (20 ng/ml) had no effect on membrane potential in slices from IL-1R1−/− animals (−64 ± 2 mV in control and −64 ± 2 mV in TNFα, n = 5 cells from two animals, Supplementary data, S5).
Consequences of exaggerated IL-1 responsiveness: apoptosis
Since neurons in the diseased brain were more sensitive to the effects of IL-1 and failed to recover their resting membrane potential, we hypothesised that the previously reported neuronal apoptosis induced by systemic LPS [25] would be mediated by IL-1RI. ME7 animals were challenged with LPS (750 µg/kg i.p.) and euthanised 18 h later, the time at which LPS-induced neuronal apoptosis has previously been demonstrated [25]. TUNEL labelling was performed to assess for apoptotic cell death in 10 µm coronal sections. LPS induced robust apoptosis in wild-type ME7 animals, with respect to ME7 + saline animals and the number of apoptotic cells was significantly reduced in IL-1R1−/− ME7 + LPS animals (Fig. 5a, b). Two-way ANOVA showed a main effect of treatment (F = 38.59, df 1,16 p < 0.0001) and an interaction of treatment and genotype (F1,16 = 6.05, p = 0.026) and Bonferroni post hoc comparison showed that ME7 + LPS was significantly more affected than ME7 IL-1R1−/− + LPS (p < 0.05) and ME7 + saline (p < 0.001), although LPS still increased apoptosis somewhat in IL-1RI−/− (p < 0.05). Therefore, LPS-induced apoptosis in ME7 animals is partially mediated by IL-1RI. Our prior studies had established that NBH animals were not similarly susceptible to LPS-induced apoptosis [25]. However, we have also verified that LPS did not produce acute apoptosis in NBH animals under the current conditions (Supplementary Figure S6). Similarly, the exaggerated hypothermic response of ME7 animals to systemic LPS was also mediated by IL-1RI (Fig. S7).
Discussion
We have demonstrated that systemic inflammation impairs cognitive and neuronal function in multiple ways, by dissociable IL-1-dependent mechanisms. While LPS had no impact on working memory in normal animals it robustly impaired working memory in animals with existing neurodegenerative pathology. Systemic LPS-induced working memory deficits were mediated by circulating IL-1β. Systemic LPS also induced IL-1R1-dependent apoptotic cell death in the neurodegenerating brain and CA1 neurons in hippocampal slices showed heightened vulnerability to IL-1β-induced, non-reversible, loss of membrane potential. The data suggest that systemic inflammation induces both transient cognitive dysfunction and lasting brain injury and that IL-1 contributes to both processes, but by dissociable mechanisms. Since systemic inflammation can trigger episodes of delirium in susceptible individuals [9] and episodes of delirium interact synergistically with existing cognitive impairment to accelerate dementia progression [44], understanding the mechanistic basis of both the acute deficits and the lasting injury is of crucial importance. The current data adds to our knowledge on transient and lasting effects of systemic inflammation on the already degenerating brain.
Dissociations in inflammation-induced, hippocampal-dependent cognitive impairment
Although impairment of memory consolidation in the CFC paradigm is the best characterised effect of systemic inflammation on cognitive function in young healthy rodents (whether induced by LPS, infection or surgical trauma) [17, 20, 21], we have argued that this consolidation of long-term memory may not be optimal for studying the dynamic and fluctuating attentional and working memory deficits observed during delirium [45]. Hippocampal IL-1β clearly impairs late-phase LTP [46] and impairs consolidation of contextual memory both in young and aged LPS/E. coli-treated rats [47] and in post-operative mice [20]. However, the consolidation of memory that is central to CFC is associated with impairment in late-phase LTP [48] and deficits in this relatively slow, protein synthesis-dependent, process cannot explain the disruption in dynamic short-term memory and/or attentional processes described here. There is no intuitive role for memory consolidation in the current T-maze task and the neurological substrates required for CFC and T-maze alternation are manifestly different [49]. Here we show that working memory and contextual memory consolidation are differently affected by systemic inflammation: despite the CFC impairment observed with 100 µg/kg LPS in the current study, neither this nor 200 µg/kg LPS was sufficient to impair working memory in the T-maze in normal animals. Therefore systemic inflammation differentially affects different hippocampal-dependent tasks, as has previously been suggested from context discrimination tasks [32, 50]. This dissociation demonstrates that one cannot assume that all IL-1 β effects on cognition are mediated by the same mechanism and research into IL-1β effects on cognitive function must interrogate multiple mechanisms to explain deficits in different cognitive domains.
The same LPS dose that failed to impair working memory in normal animals did induce robust working memory deficits in animals with prior hippocampal synaptic loss. These deficits were ameliorated by peripheral treatment with IL-1RA, but this treatment had no beneficial effect with respect to the CFC deficits already described. Although the LPS-induced deficits become manifest across different timescales (i.e. 3–7 h for T-maze and at 48 h for CFC), it has been clearly demonstrated that LPS and IL-1β impair consolidation of contextual memory and that this occurs in the hours after exposure to shock and context pairing [33]. Treatment after consolidation has been completed, but directly before testing of retention, does not impair memory [32]. Therefore the cognitive process that LPS is impairing in the CFC task occurs in the hours immediately after exposure to the context. Therefore both T-maze and CFC tasks experience inflammation-induced disruption of cognitive processes in the hours directly after LPS administration and it is this rather than the time at which they are tested that is relevant to determine the appropriate time of application of treatments in order to intervene in these processes. These data illustrate that systemic inflammation produces impairments in multiple cognitive domains and that, at least with respect to these two hippocampal-dependent tasks, there are dissociable IL-1-dependent mechanisms. IL-1β is described to act centrally to impair consolidation in LPS-induced CFC deficits [19–23], but appears to act systemically here to impair working memory, as we discuss below.
Systemic versus central effects
That the working memory impairment induced by LPS is mediated by circulating rather than centrally produced IL-1β is supported by multiple strands of evidence. (1) Peripherally applied IL-1RA was protective against the working memory deficits and CNS concentrations were 1/7500 of levels in the plasma at the time of protection against the deficits, consistent with prior data that peripherally administered IL-1RA (17 kDa) shows limited BBB penetration [51]. Moreover, although IL-1RA entry to the brain is thought to occur primarily when and where the BBB has been breached [39] we observed similar levels in NBH and ME7 animals arguing against the idea that existing neurodegenerative disease made the brain more permeable to IL-1RA (2) Cytokine and chemokine analysis shows that IL-1RA blocked IL-1 action in the blood (Fig. 2c) but did not prevent LPS-induced changes in hippocampal inflammatory transcripts, including de novo IL-1 in the brain. This is consistent with data showing direct activation of the brain vasculature by systemic LPS [52] and CNS inflammatory mediator production occurring despite abrogation of systemic cytokines via dexamethasone-21-phosphate inhibition [30] or by depletion of peripheral TLR4-positive macrophages [6, 53–55]. (3) DEX was 90% effective in reducing systemic IL-1β synthesis but not at all effective at blocking CNS transcription and microglial synthesis of IL-1β (Fig. S2 and ref. [30]). (4) Systemic administration of IL-1β is sufficient to induce the same deficits as LPS. Thus IL-1RA appears to act peripherally rather than centrally to block IL-1 action with respect to working memory.
Time-dependent protection and redundancy in inflammation-induced impairments
The protection afforded by IL-1RA is temporary (Fig. 2a). IL-1RA remained at 481.5 ng/ml at 3 h post injection in the current study, compared to 125 pg/ml plasma IL-1β 2 h post LPS at 100 µg/kg [37]. Therefore, with a stoichiometric excess of greater than 2000-fold over IL-1β, we are confident that the known short half-life of IL-1RA was not sufficient to prevent biologically relevant levels to block effects of circulating IL-1 at a key time in memory disruption. However, it is possible that the rapid renal metabolism of IL-1RA [56] could have caused levels to fall below therapeutic efficacy [22] during the later trials (7–9 h post LPS). The protection offered by dexamethasone administration in IL-1R1−/− animals also waned at 7 h post challenge. Given that CNS inflammatory mediator production persisted even in the presence of systemic IL-1RA or dexamethasone (Fig. S2), it is plausible that propagation of central mediators such as IL-1β may have additional effects, or expression of additional inflammatory mediators sufficient to disrupt cognition, may also have occurred independent of systemic IL-1β. One such candidate, which may contribute to systemic IL-1-independent effects, is TNF-α. We show here that systemically administered TNF-α was sufficient, alone, to produce acute impairments and is robustly expressed after systemic challenge with LPS [28] and after inflammatory trauma such as the tibial fracture used in POCD models [20, 23]. The ability of TNF-α to mimic the effects of IL-1 is important in the light of LPS’ propensity to produce equivalent T-maze impairments in wild-type and in IL-1R1−/−. IL-1RI−/− mice develop in the absence of IL-1 signalling and, despite the acknowledged importance of IL-1 in innate immune and sickness behaviour responses to LPS (ref. [41] and Fig. 2), these mice are known to show normal responses to systemic LPS (http://jaxmice.jax.org/strain/003245.html) [40]. Other cytokines, such as TNF-α, demonstrably compensate for the lack of IL-1 signalling to induce sickness behaviour responses in these mice [42]. Here, reducing systemic cytokines by >90% using DEX [29], produced robust, although temporally restricted, protection against LPS-induced working memory deficits and both TNF-α and IL-1 were sufficient to mimic LPS effects.
It remains unclear exactly how systemic IL-1 (or TNF-α) alters cognitive function in the current paradigm. Based on its rich expression of IL-1RI [57–59], the brain endothelium is an obvious first target and indeed we investigated and refuted the hypothesis that endothelial COX2 mediates this LPS-induced working memory deficit: we found that non-selective COX inhibitors protected against both LPS and IL-1-induced T-maze deficits, but only COX-1 inhibitors protected against LPS-induced T-maze deficits [60]. The rich expression of IL-1 receptors on hippocampal neurons [61] offers compelling support for the direct effect of locally produced IL-1 on memory function and LTP, but this is most relevant for the IL-1-dependence of CFC deficits shown by several previous authors [19, 20, 36]. The demonstration here that the CFC and T-maze tasks are dissociable on a number of levels emphasises that IL-1 exerts effects on different types of memory function through different mechanisms and here, the data support the idea of IL-1 acting first on targets outside the brain. IL-1 can also act directly on vagal afferents or on neurons proximal to the circumventricular organs, which lack a patent blood brain barrier. It is known that peripheral IL-1β can act directly on these neurons, which are not directly responsive to LPS [53], to induce expression of the immediate early gene cFOS here and in the amygdaloid complex [62, 63]. Similarly IL-1 has robust effects on peripheral energy metabolism [64] which may have multiple effects on brain function. Ultimately it will be important to dissect the precise role of IL-1 in this paradigm using cell-specific knockouts of IL-1RI and this is an important target for future experiments.
Direct effects of IL-1β on hippocampal neurons
Despite equivalent systemic inflammation in NBH and ME7 animals, differential CNS outcomes were observed in ME7 animals and this may occur in several different ways. Firstly, the degenerating brain is ‘primed’ to show exaggerated CNS IL-1β responses to systemic LPS [25, 65–67]. This exaggerated CNS IL-1 has been assumed to be responsible for the exaggerated sickness behaviour responses to systemic LPS observed in aged and ME7 prion-diseased animals [65, 68] and here we demonstrated that exaggerated hypothermia induced by LPS in ME7 animals is very much mitigated in IL-1R1−/− mice, confirming that IL-1 is a major driver of this heightened sickness response (Fig. S7).
Secondly, neurons in the diseased brain may be more susceptible to the effects of inflammatory mediators, and concentrations not deleterious to neuronal function in healthy individuals might disrupt function in diseased neurons. Here we ‘by-passed’ microglial priming by applying equal IL-1β concentrations directly to ex vivo hippocampal slices. IL-1β at 0.1 ng/ml (5.9 pM) had no effect on CA1 neurons from NBH animals but was sufficient to depolarise and induce maximal action potential firing in ME7 CA1 neurons. These diseased CA1 neurons were significantly more sensitive (low pM) than prior studies of IL-1-induced depolarisation (1 nM: [69, 70]) and the depolarisation observed appeared to be non-synaptic, in that IL-1 had no effect on evoked EPSCs or IPSCs (Fig. S4). These effects of IL-1β on CA1 neurons in ME7 animals likely disrupt the precise firing patterns of CA1 pyramidal cells that underlie the rate and temporal codes mediating hippocampal information processing and could contribute to acutely compromised function in multiple hippocampal-dependent tasks. The ex vivo CA1 spiking activity was dependent on IL-1RI−/− and the observation that IL-1β had an irreversible, detrimental effect on CA1 cells from ME7 animals also suggests a role for hippocampal IL-1β in systemic inflammation-induced neuronal death. Increased neuronal death after systemic LPS in ME7 animals was independent of circulating IL-1β [30] and here we show that this LPS-induced apoptotic cell death was significantly reduced in IL-1R1−/− mice. Although the specific target of LPS-induced IL-1 in producing cell death was not identified here, we propose that the microglial IL-1β shown here and previously, directly targets proximal neurons and contributes to loss of viability. Consistent with this, IL-1β applied directly to ME7 neurons, was sufficient to produce irreversible depolarisation and this effect was IL-1RI−/− dependent. Thus we present evidence for mechanistic dissociation whereby systemic IL-1β has significant effects on working memory while central IL-1β has significant effects on neuronal integrity in the vulnerable brain.
The mechanisms by which IL-1 leads to non-reversible membrane depolarisation require further study. IL-1 is widely reported to have pro-convulsant activity and there are data supporting tyrosine kinase phosphorylation of NMDA receptor subunits [71, 72], contributing to IL-1-dependent neuronal death. Recently, altered IL-1R1 accessory protein (IL1RAcP) isoform expression in the aged brain was reported to underpin exaggerated effects of IL-1 in the hippocampus [73] it is important to investigate whether this mechanism might contribute to decreased viability of neurons in the degenerating brain upon acute elevations of IL-1.
It is a limitation of this study that higher doses of LPS were used to produce new apoptosis in the brain, than those used to produce working memory dysfunction. Our experience with this model over several studies has been that 100 µg/kg LPS is insufficient to produce measurable new pathology (no significant apoptosis or synaptic loss) or to alter the trajectory of disease (all animals return to baseline performance after acute deficits) [9, 74]. Conversely higher doses of LPS, which do alter pathology and change trajectory of disease [25, 30, 74], produce acute sickness that is ethically and scientifically incompatible with cognitive testing during the inflammatory episode.
Conclusion
Systemic IL-1β drives acute working memory dysfunction but IL-1β also has robust direct effects on hippocampal neurons, leading to hyperexcitation and irreversible loss of membrane potential. This may provide a mechanistic explanation of LPS-induced IL-1RI-dependent cell death in the brain. The data support the idea that the acute and reversible cognitive deficits (including delirium) caused by systemic inflammation may operate via different mechanisms to the concurrent acute brain injury that presumably contributes to the negative long-term cognitive outcomes for patients after recovery from acute complications. Given that delirium contributes to progression of dementia, without necessarily exacerbating classical CERAD features like amyloid-β and Tau [11, 44] it is crucial to understand the extent to which delirium and acute brain injury occur by overlapping or dissociable mechanisms. The relevance of the current model for such investigations has been recently validated with respect to DSM-IV criteria and to alternative models [9, 75].
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the Wellcome Trust (SRF 090907). DS was supported by a HRB PhD studentship and Edel Hennessy was supported by a Trinity Foundation Studentship. AN, LT were supported by NIH R01AG050626. We would like to thank Prof. Kingston Mills for the gift of IL-1R1−/− mice, Stuart Allan for the gift of human IL-1ra and Prof. Roger Anwyl for facilitating electrophysiological studies.
Compliance with ethical standards
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Electronic supplementary material
The online version of this article (10.1038/s41380-018-0075-8) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorised users.
References
- 1.Elie M, Cole MG, Primeau FJ, Bellavance F. Delirium risk factors in elderly hospitalized patients. J Gen Intern Med. 1998;13:204–12. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.1998.00047.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.George J, Bleasdale S, Singleton SJ. Causes and prognosis of delirium in elderly patients admitted to a district general hospital. Age Ageing. 1997;26:423–7. doi: 10.1093/ageing/26.6.423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kat MG, Vreeswijk R, de Jonghe JF, van der Ploeg T, van Gool WA, Eikelenboom P, et al. Long-term cognitive outcome of delirium in elderly hip surgery patients. A prospective matched controlled study over two and a half years. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2008;26:1–8. doi: 10.1159/000140611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.van Munster BC, Korevaar JC, Zwinderman AH, Levi M, Wiersinga WJ, De Rooij SE. Time-course of cytokines during delirium in elderly patients with hip fractures. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008;56:1704–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01851.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Dantzer R, O’Connor JC, Freund GG, Johnson RW, Kelley KW. From inflammation to sickness and depression: when the immune system subjugates the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:46–56. doi: 10.1038/nrn2297. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gosselin D, Rivest S. MyD88 signaling in brain endothelial cells is essential for the neuronal activity and glucocorticoid release during systemic inflammation. Mol Psychiatry. 2008;13:480–97. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Cape E, Hall RJ, van Munster BC, de Vries A, Howie SE, Pearson A, et al. IL-1 beta in delirium after hip fracture. J Psychosom Res. 2014;77:219–25. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2014.06.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Serantes R, Arnalich F, Figueroa M, Salinas M, Andres-Mateos E, Codoceo R, et al. Interleukin-1 beta enhances GABAA receptor cell-surface expression by a phosphatidylinositol 3-kinase/Akt pathway: relevance to sepsis-associated encephalopathy. J Biol Chem. 2006;281:14632–43. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M512489200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Davis DH, Skelly DT, Murray C, Hennessy E, Bowen J, Norton S, et al. Worsening cognitive impairment and neurodegenerative pathology progressively increase risk for delirium. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2015;23:403–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2014.08.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Fong TG, Davis D, Growdon ME, Albuquerque A, Inouye SK. The interface between delirium and dementia in elderly adults. Lancet Neurol. 2015;14:823–32. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00101-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Davis DH, Muniz Terrera G, Keage H, Rahkonen T, Oinas M, Matthews FE, et al. Delirium is a strong risk factor for dementia in the oldest-old: a population-based cohort study. Brain. 2012;135:2809–16. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.MacLullich AM, Beaglehole A, Hall RJ, Meagher DJ. Delirium and long-term cognitive impairment. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2009;21:30–42. doi: 10.1080/09540260802675031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Pandharipande PP, Girard TD, Jackson JC, Morandi A, Thompson JL, Pun BT, et al. Long-term cognitive impairment after critical illness. N Engl J Med. 2013;369:1306–16. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1301372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Fong TG, Jones RN, Shi P, Marcantonio ER, Yap L, Rudolph JL, et al. Delirium accelerates cognitive decline in Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2009;72:1570–5. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181a4129a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Holmes C, Cunningham C, Zotova E, Woolford J, Dean C, Kerr S, et al. Systemic inflammation and disease progression in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 2009;73:768–74. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181b6bb95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Leslie DL, Marcantonio ER, Zhang Y, Leo-Summers L, Inouye SK. One-year health care costs associated with delirium in the elderly population. Arch Intern Med. 2008;168:27–32. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2007.4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Cunningham C, Sanderson DJ. Malaise in the water maze: untangling the effects of LPS and IL-1 beta on learning and memory. Brain Behav Immun. 2008;22:1117–27. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2008.05.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Yirmiya R, Goshen I. Immune modulation of learning, memory, neural plasticity and neurogenesis. Brain Behav Immun. 2011;25:181–213. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2010.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Barrientos RM, Higgins EA, Sprunger DB, Watkins LR, Rudy JW, Maier SF. Memory for context is impaired by a post context exposure injection of interleukin-1 beta into dorsal hippocampus. Behav Brain Res. 2002;134:291–8. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00043-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Cibelli M, Fidalgo AR, Terrando N, Ma D, Monaco C, Feldmann M, et al. Role of interleukin-1 beta in postoperative cognitive dysfunction. Ann Neurol. 2010;68:360–8. doi: 10.1002/ana.22082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Goshen I, Kreisel T, Ounallah-Saad H, Renbaum P, Zalzstein Y, Ben-Hur T, et al. A dual role for interleukin-1 in hippocampal-dependent memory processes. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007;32:1106–15. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Barrientos RM, Hein AM, Frank MG, Watkins LR, Maier SF. Intracisternal interleukin-1 receptor antagonist prevents postoperative cognitive decline and neuroinflammatory response in aged rats. J Neurosci. 2012;32:14641–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2173-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Terrando N, Rei Fidalgo A, Vizcaychipi M, Cibelli M, Ma D, Monaco C, et al. The impact of IL-1 modulation on the development of lipopolysaccharide-induced cognitive dysfunction. Crit Care. 2010;14:R88. doi: 10.1186/cc9019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Brown LJ, Ferner HS, Robertson J, Mills NL, Pessotto R, Deary IJ, et al. Differential effects of delirium on fluid and crystallized cognitive abilities. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2011;52:153–8. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2010.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Cunningham C, Wilcockson DC, Campion S, Lunnon K, Perry VH. Central and systemic endotoxin challenges exacerbate the local inflammatory response and increase neuronal death during chronic neurodegeneration. J Neurosci. 2005;25:9275–84. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2614-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Heneka MT, Kummer MP, Stutz A, Delekate A, Schwartz S, Vieira-Saecker A, et al. NLRP3 is activated in Alzheimer’s disease and contributes to pathology in APP/PS1 mice. Nature. 2013;493:674–8. doi: 10.1038/nature11729. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Holmes C, El-Okl M, Williams AL, Cunningham C, Wilcockson D, Perry VH. Systemic infection, interleukin 1 beta, and cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2003;74:788–9. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.74.6.788. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Murray C, Sanderson DJ, Barkus C, Deacon RM, Rawlins JN, Bannerman DM, et al. Systemic inflammation induces acute working memory deficits in the primed brain: relevance for delirium. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33:603–16. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Teeling JL, Cunningham C, Newman TA, Perry VH. The effect of non-steroidal anti-inflammatory agents on behavioural changes and cytokine production following systemic inflammation: implications for a role of COX-1. Brain Behav Immun. 2010;24:409–19. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2009.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Murray CL, Skelly DT, Cunningham C. Exacerbation of CNS inflammation and neurodegeneration by systemic LPS treatment is independent of circulating IL-1 beta and IL-6. J Neuroinflamm. 2011;8:50. doi: 10.1186/1742-2094-8-50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Murray CL, Obiang P, Bannerman D, Cunningham C. Endogenous IL-1 in cognitive function and anxiety: a study in IL-1RI-/- mice. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e78385. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0078385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Czerniawski J, Guzowski JF. Acute neuroinflammation impairs context discrimination memory and disrupts pattern separation processes in hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2014;34:12470–80. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0542-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Pugh CR, Kumagawa K, Fleshner M, Watkins LR, Maier SF, Rudy JW. Selective effects of peripheral lipopolysaccharide administration on contextual and auditory-cue fear conditioning. Brain Behav Immun. 1998;12:212–29. doi: 10.1006/brbi.1998.0524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Fanselow MS. Contextual fear, gestalt memories, and the hippocampus. Behav Brain Res. 2000;110:73–81. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00186-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Cunningham C, Campion S, Teeling J, Felton L, Perry VH. The sickness behaviour and CNS inflammatory mediator profile induced by systemic challenge of mice with synthetic double-stranded RNA (poly I:C) Brain Behav Immun. 2007;21:490–502. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2006.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Pugh CR, Nguyen KT, Gonyea JL, Fleshner M, Wakins LR, Maier SF, et al. Role of interleukin-1 beta in impairment of contextual fear conditioning caused by social isolation. Behav Brain Res. 1999;106:109–18. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00098-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Skelly DT, Hennessy E, Dansereau MA, Cunningham C. A systematic analysis of the peripheral and CNS effects of systemic LPS, IL-1 beta, [corrected] TNF-alpha and IL-6 challenges in C57BL/6 mice. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e69123. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Cunningham C, Wilcockson DC, Boche D, Perry VH. Comparison of inflammatory and acute-phase responses in the brain and peripheral organs of the ME7 model of prion disease. J Virol. 2005;79:5174–84. doi: 10.1128/JVI.79.8.5174-5184.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Greenhalgh AD, Galea J, Denes A, Tyrrell PJ, Rothwell NJ. Rapid brain penetration of interleukin-1 receptor antagonist in rat cerebral ischaemia: pharmacokinetics, distribution, protection. Br J Pharmacol. 2010;160:153–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.2010.00684.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Glaccum MB, Stocking KL, Charrier K, Smith JL, Willis CR, Maliszewski C, et al. Phenotypic and functional characterization of mice that lack the type I receptor for IL-1. J Immunol. 1997;159:3364–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Konsman JP, Veeneman J, Combe C, Poole S, Luheshi GN, Dantzer R. Central nervous action of interleukin-1 mediates activation of limbic structures and behavioural depression in response to peripheral administration of bacterial lipopolysaccharide. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28:2499–510. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06549.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Bluthe RM, Laye S, Michaud B, Combe C, Dantzer R, Parnet P. Role of interleukin-1beta and tumour necrosis factor-alpha in lipopolysaccharide-induced sickness behaviour: a study with interleukin-1 type I receptor-deficient mice. Eur J Neurosci. 2000;12:4447–56. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Hennessy E, Gormley S, Lopez-Rodriguez AB, Murray C, Cunningham C. Systemic TNF-alpha produces acute cognitive dysfunction and exaggerated sickness behavior when superimposed upon progressive neurodegeneration. Brain Behav Immun. 2017;59:233–44. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2016.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Davis DH, Muniz Terrera G, Keage H, Stephan BC, Fleming J, Ince PG, et al. Association of delirium with cognitive decline in late life: a neuropathologic study of 3 population-based cohort studies. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017;74:244–51. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Cunningham C, Maclullich AM. At the extreme end of the psychoneuroimmunological spectrum: delirium as a maladaptive sickness behaviour response. Brain Behav Immun. 2013;28:1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2012.07.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Tong L, Prieto GA, Kramar EA, Smith ED, Cribbs DH, Lynch G, et al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor-dependent synaptic plasticity is suppressed by interleukin-1 via p38 mitogen-activated protein kinase. J Neurosci. 2012;32:17714–24. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1253-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Frank MG, Barrientos RM, Hein AM, Biedenkapp JC, Watkins LR, Maier SF. IL-1RA blocks E. coli-induced suppression of Arc and long-term memory in aged F344xBN F1 rats. Brain Behav Immun. 2010;24:254–62. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2009.10.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Chapman TR, Barrientos RM, Ahrendsen JT, Maier SF, Patterson SL. Synaptic correlates of increased cognitive vulnerability with aging: peripheral immune challenge and aging interact to disrupt theta-burst late-phase long-term potentiation in hippocampal area CA1. J Neurosci. 2010;30:7598–603. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5172-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bannerman DM, Sprengel R, Sanderson DJ, McHugh SB, Rawlins JN, Monyer H, et al. Hippocampal synaptic plasticity, spatial memory and anxiety. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2014;15:181–92. doi: 10.1038/nrn3677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Czerniawski J, Miyashita T, Lewandowski G, Guzowski JF. Systemic lipopolysaccharide administration impairs retrieval of context-object discrimination, but not spatial, memory: evidence for selective disruption of specific hippocampus-dependent memory functions during acute neuroinflammation. Brain Behav Immun. 2015;44:159–66. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2014.09.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Gutierrez EG, Banks WA, Kastin AJ. Blood-borne interleukin-1 receptor antagonist crosses the blood-brain barrier. J Neuroimmunol. 1994;55:153–60. doi: 10.1016/0165-5728(94)90005-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Singh AK, Jiang Y. How does peripheral lipopolysaccharide induce gene expression in the brain of rats? Toxicology. 2004;201:197–207. doi: 10.1016/j.tox.2004.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Chakravarty S, Herkenham M. Toll-like receptor 4 on nonhematopoietic cells sustains CNS inflammation during endotoxemia, independent of systemic cytokines. J Neurosci. 2005;25:1788–96. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4268-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Chen Z, Jalabi W, Shpargel KB, Farabaugh KT, Dutta R, Yin X, et al. Lipopolysaccharide-induced microglial activation and neuroprotection against experimental brain injury is independent of hematogenous TLR4. J Neurosci. 2012;32:11706–15. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0730-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Serrats J, Schiltz JC, Garcia-Bueno B, van Rooijen N, Reyes TM, Sawchenko PE. Dual roles for perivascular macrophages in immune-to-brain signaling. Neuron. 2010;65:94–106. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Cawthorne C, Prenant C, Smigova A, Julyan P, Maroy R, Herholz K, et al. Biodistribution, pharmacokinetics and metabolism of interleukin-1 receptor antagonist (IL-1RA) using [18F]-IL1RA and PET imaging in rats. Br J Pharmacol. 2011;162:659–72. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.2010.01068.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Ericsson A, Liu C, Hart RP, Sawchenko PE. Type 1 interleukin-1 receptor in the rat brain: distribution, regulation, and relationship to sites of IL-1-induced cellular activation. J Comp Neurol. 1995;361:681–98. doi: 10.1002/cne.903610410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Liu X, Yamashita T, Chen Q, Belevych N, McKim DB, Tarr AJ, et al. Interleukin 1 type 1 receptor restore: a genetic mouse model for studying interleukin 1 receptor-mediated effects in specific cell types. J Neurosci: Off J Soc Neurosci. 2015;35:2860–70. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3199-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Wong ML, Licinio J. Localization of interleukin 1 type I receptor mRNA in rat brain. Neuroimmunomodulation. 1994;1:110–5. doi: 10.1159/000097143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Griffin EW, Skelly DT, Murray CL, Cunningham C. Cyclooxygenase-1-dependent prostaglandins mediate susceptibility to systemic inflammation-induced acute cognitive dysfunction. J Neurosci. 2013;33:15248–58. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6361-11.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Chen Q, Zhang H, Li Q, An Y, Herkenham M, Lai W, et al. Three promoters regulate tissue- and cell type-specific expression of murine interleukin-1 receptor type I. J Biol Chem. 2009;284:8703–13. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M808261200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Engler H, Doenlen R, Engler A, Riether C, Prager G, Niemi MB, et al. Acute amygdaloid response to systemic inflammation. Brain Behav Immun. 2011;25:1384–92. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2011.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Nadjar A, Combe C, Laye S, Tridon V, Dantzer R, Amedee T, et al. Nuclear factor kappaB nuclear translocation as a crucial marker of brain response to interleukin-1. A study in rat and interleukin-1 type I deficient mouse. J Neurochem. 2003;87:1024–36. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2003.02097.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Ota K, Wildmann J, Ota T, Besedovsky HO, Del Rey A. Interleukin-1beta and insulin elicit different neuroendocrine responses to hypoglycemia. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1153:82–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2008.03981.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Godbout JP, Chen J, Abraham J, Richwine AF, Berg BM, Kelley KW, et al. Exaggerated neuroinflammation and sickness behavior in aged mice following activation of the peripheral innate immune system. FASEB J. 2005;19:1329–31. doi: 10.1096/fj.05-3776fje. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Palin K, Cunningham C, Forse P, Perry VH, Platt N. Systemic inflammation switches the inflammatory cytokine profile in CNS Wallerian degeneration. Neurobiol Dis. 2008;30:19–29. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2007.11.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Pott-Godoy MC, Tarelli R, Ferrari CC, Sarchi MI, Pitossi FJ. Central and systemic IL-1 exacerbates neurodegeneration and motor symptoms in a model of Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2008;131:1880–94. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Combrinck MI, Perry VH, Cunningham C. Peripheral infection evokes exaggerated sickness behaviour in pre-clinical murine prion disease. Neuroscience. 2002;112:7–11. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(02)00030-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Coogan A, O’Connor JJ. Inhibition of NMDA receptor-mediated synaptic transmission in the rat dentate gyrus in vitro by IL-1 beta. Neuroreport. 1997;8:2107–10. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199707070-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Ferri CC, Ferguson AV. Interleukin-1 beta depolarizes paraventricular nucleus parvocellular neurones. J Neuroendocrinol. 2003;15:126–33. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.2003.00870.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Balosso S, Maroso M, Sanchez-Alavez M, Ravizza T, Frasca A, Bartfai T, et al. A novel non-transcriptional pathway mediates the proconvulsive effects of interleukin-1 beta. Brain. 2008;131:3256–65. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Viviani B, Bartesaghi S, Gardoni F, Vezzani A, Behrens MM, Bartfai T, et al. Interleukin-1beta enhances NMDA receptor-mediated intracellular calcium increase through activation of the Src family of kinases. J Neurosci. 2003;23:8692–700. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-25-08692.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Prieto GA, Snigdha S, Baglietto-Vargas D, Smith ED, Berchtold NC, Tong L, et al. Synapse-specific IL-1 receptor subunit reconfiguration augments vulnerability to IL-1 beta in the aged hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2015;112:E5078–87. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1514486112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Cunningham C, Campion S, Lunnon K, Murray CL, Woods JF, Deacon RM, et al. Systemic inflammation induces acute behavioral and cognitive changes and accelerates neurodegenerative disease. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65:304–12. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.07.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Schreuder L, Eggen J, Biber K, Schoemaker R, Laman J, de Rooij S. Pathophysiological and behavioral effects of systemic inflammation in aged and diseased rodents with relevance to delirium: a systematic review. Brain Behav Immun. 2017;62:362–81. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2017.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.