Abstract
Health warnings are a promising strategy for reducing consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages (SSBs), but uncertainty remains about how to design warnings to maximize their impact. Warnings already implemented in Latin America use nutrient disclosures, while proposed U.S. warnings would describe the health effects of consuming SSBs. We sought to determine whether warning characteristics influence consumers’ reactions to SSB health warnings. A national convenience sample of U.S. adults (n=1,360) completed an online survey in 2018. In a factorial design, we randomly assigned participants to view SSB health warnings that differed in: 1) inclusion of health effects (“Drinking beverages with added sugar contributes to obesity, diabetes, and tooth decay”); 2) inclusion of a nutrient disclosure (“High in added sugar”); 3) inclusion of the marker word “WARNING;” and 4) shape (octagon vs. rectangle). The primary outcome was perceived message effectiveness (PME, range 1–5). PME was higher for warnings that included health effects (average differential effect [ADE]=0.63, p<0.001) or nutrient disclosures (ADE=0.32, p<0.001) compared to warnings without this information. However, adding a nutrient disclosure to a warning that already included health effects did not lead to higher PME compared to warnings with health effects alone. The marker “WARNING” (ADE=0.21) and the octagon shape (ADE=0.08) also led to higher PME compared to warnings without these characteristics (ps<0.001). The same pattern of results held for the secondary outcomes, fear and thinking about harms. SSB health warnings may have more impact if they describe health effects, use the marker “WARNING,” and are octagon-shaped.
Keywords: Health warnings, warnings labels, health communication, obesity prevention, nutrition, front of package labels, sugar-sweetened beverages
Introduction
Excess consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages (SSBs) remains a pressing public health issue in the United States. Half of adults consume SSBs on any given day,1 and average caloric intake from SSBs remains well above national dietary guidelines.2,3 Evidence indicates that SSB consumption increases risk of developing obesity,4,5 diabetes,6,7 and heart disease.8 To reduce consumption of SSBs, five states have proposed requiring front-of-package (FOP) health warnings on SSB containers.9–13
Even as interest in SSB health warning policies has grown, questions remain about how to design warnings to maximize their effectiveness. For example, warnings proposed in the U.S. describe the health effects of consuming SSBs.9–13 In contrast, nutrition warning systems adopted in countries such as Chile do not describe health effects, but instead display a nutrient disclosure that signals when a product exceeds recommended levels of sugar, sodium, saturated fat, or calories. For example, SSBs in Chile display FOP warnings that read “Alto en azúcares” (“high in sugars”).14,15 Another difference is warning label shape: in Chile, warnings are displayed on octagonal labels, while SSB warnings in the U.S. would likely be displayed on rectangular labels. Additionally, the proposed SSB health warnings in the U.S.9–13 begin with a marker word (“WARNING” or “HEALTH WARNING”) that signals that the subsequent text is a warning message, while labels in other countries often do not use marker words.14,16,17
These four warning characteristics – health effects, nutrient disclosures, label shape, and marker words – could influence how effectively SSB health warnings discourage SSB consumption. For example, cigarette warnings that describe health effects elicit higher perceived effectiveness,18 and warnings with health effects statements or nutrient disclosures have been found to reduce consumers’ intentions to purchase SSBs.19–21 Others have found that consumers associate the octagon shape with unhealthfulness.22 Including marker words such as “CAUTION,” or “WARNING” (or similar marker symbols23) may draw attention to warnings,24–26 but makes messages longer, potentially reducing readability.
Limited research has examined these warning characteristics side-by-side or in combination with one another. The objective of this study was to examine the influence of health effects, nutrient disclosures, marker words, and label shape on perceptions of messages’ effectiveness at discouraging SSB consumption. Based on previous research, we predicted that warnings that included health effects18,19 or nutrient disclosures21,27 would elicit higher perceived message effectiveness than warnings without these characteristics, and that octagon-shaped labels would elicit higher perceived message effectiveness than rectangular labels.22,28 We did not make an a priori prediction regarding marker words because they might increase attention but reduce readability. We also examined whether these four warning characteristics elicit more fear or thinking about the harms of SSB consumption. We focused on perceived message effectiveness,29–33 fear,33,34 and thinking about harms33,35,36 because these outcomes have been found to predict warnings’ actual effectiveness. We also assessed whether warning characteristics affect consumers’ knowledge of the health harms of SSB consumption and identified the warning color combinations perceived to be most effective.22
Methods
Participants
In April 2018, we recruited a convenience sample of 1,413 U.S. adults ≥18 years using Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk), an online platform commonly used by social and behavioral science researchers.37–39 Research indicates that experiments conducted on MTurk replicate findings from studies conducted both in the lab40 and via random-digit dial phone surveys.41 Participants earned $2.20 for completing the 10–15 minute survey.
Impact of Warning Characteristics on Consumer Reactions
Procedures.
The main experiment varied characteristics of SSB health warnings using a mixed between/within factorial design. First, we randomly assigned participants to one of four between-subjects conditions: 1) control (“Always read the Nutrition Facts Panel”), 2) health effects only (“Drinking beverages with added sugar contributes to obesity, diabetes, and tooth decay,” adapted from California’s proposed warnings9), 3) nutrient disclosure only (“High in added sugar,” adapted from Chile’s warnings14), and 4) health effects and nutrient disclosure. These four conditions represented the combination of two between-subjects factors, each with two levels: 1) whether the warning included health effects and 2) whether the warning included a nutrient disclosure.
Participants viewed their randomly assigned warning message four times, on four labels that differed on two within-subjects factors, each with two levels: whether the message began with the marker word “WARNING” and the shape of the warning label (rectangle vs. octagon). Thus, the experiment had four within-subjects conditions, each representing a different warning label design: 1) no marker and rectangle shape, 2) no marker and octagon shape, 3) “WARNING” marker and rectangle shape, and 4) “WARNING” marker and octagon shape. Participants viewed these four labels in a random order.
In total, we created 16 different warnings: one for each of the four between-subjects conditions, displayed on warnings that varied along each of the four within-subjects conditions (Figure 1). Participants viewed warnings presented mocked up on an unbranded bottle of soda (Figure 2). Presenting warnings on an unbranded soda bottle allowed us to focus participants’ attention on the warning characteristics of interest while also presenting a realistic image of what SSB warnings might look like if implemented. To mimic Chilean labels, we displayed warnings in white text on a black background.
Measures.
Participants viewed warnings one at a time. After viewing each warning, participants rated the warning on effectiveness at discouraging SSB consumption (primary outcome) and on thinking about the harms of SSB consumption and fear (secondary outcomes). The survey assessed perceived message effectiveness (PME) with an adapted version of the UNC Perceived Message Effectiveness Scale.42 PME is commonly used in message development studies43 and was found in a recent meta-analysis to predict messages’ actual behavioral efficacy.44 Our three PME items read: “This label makes me concerned about the health effects of drinking beverages with added sugar;” “This label makes drinking beverages with added sugar seem unpleasant to me;” and “This label discourages me from wanting to drink beverages with added sugar.” The 5-point response scale ranged from “strongly disagree” (coded as 1) to “strongly agree” (coded as 5). We averaged responses to these three items to create a composite score (Cronbach’s alpha=0.93, range across conditions: 2.52 to 3.80).
The survey assessed thinking about the harms of SSB consumption using a single item, adapted from studies of cigarette warnings.45–47 “How much does this label make you think about the health problems caused by drinking beverages with added sugar?” Finally, the survey assessed fear using one item also adapted from previous studies of cigarette warnings,45,48 “How much does this label make you feel scared?” Response options for these items ranged from “not at all” (coded as 1) to “very much” (coded as 5).
Knowledge of Consequences of SSB Consumption
As a secondary outcome, we also assessed the effect of the between-subjects factors, health effects and nutrient disclosure, on knowledge of the health harms of SSB consumption. After rating all four warnings and completing the two items about color described below, participants indicated whether SSB consumption contributes to: obesity, diabetes, tooth decay, and heart disease. Because SSB consumption may increase risk of these outcomes,4,8,49,50 we coded responses as correct if participants reported awareness of each health consequence and incorrect otherwise.
Most Discouraging Color Combinations
In a separate task, we also examined the warning label color combination participants perceived as most discouraging. After rating all four warnings, participants viewed a set of six rectangular warnings with the same text (“WARNING: High in added sugar”) but different combinations of background, border, and text color (Supplemental Table 1) displayed in a random arrangement. Participants selected the color combination that “would discourage you most from wanting to drink beverages with added sugar.” Participants then completed an identical item for octagon-shaped warnings.
Attention Check and Demographics
Participants completed an attention check in which they were asked to intentionally not answer an item. Participants also provided information on their demographic characteristics and health behaviors.
The University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill Institutional Review Board approved this study. Prior to data collection, we pre-registered the study’s sample size, primary hypotheses, design, and analysis plan on AsPredicted.org (https://aspredicted.org/7iz2y.pdf).
Analysis
We identified duplicate IP addresses and MTurk usernames and retained the record with the most complete data, or, when the amount of missing data was equivalent, the first record. This resulted in dropping 40 records. We also excluded 13 records for people who previously participated in pilot testing of the experiment, yielding a final analytic sample of n=1,360. These 1,360 participants each rated at least one warning and were included in analyses of the primary outcome see CONSORT flow diagram in (Supplemental Figure 1). We used intent-to-treat analyses, analyzing all participants in their assigned conditions including those who did not pass the attention check.51 We conducted analyses in Stata/SE version 15.1 (StataCorp LLC, College Station, TX).
We used mixed effects (i.e., multi-level) linear models to assess how the four manipulated warning characteristics (health effects, nutrient disclosure, marker word, and label shape) affected the primary outcome of perceived message effectiveness while accounting for the repeated measures design. We entered the within-subjects factors (marker word, label shape) as Level 1 variables and the between-subjects factors (health effects, nutrient disclosure) as Level 2 variables, treating the intercept as a random effect. Sample characteristics did not differ by experimental arm, so we conducted unadjusted analyses. The initial model included indicators for the four manipulated warning characteristics and all interactions between these four factors. The final model retained only significant interactions from the initial model. We used the same approach to examine the effects of warning characteristics on our secondary outcomes, thinking about harms and fear. We report raw means and average differential effects of each experimental factor on the outcomes as generated by the mixed models. We probed interactions by calculating means and average differential effects at different levels of the moderating factors.
In pre-specified analyses, we examined whether participant characteristics moderated the relationship between warning characteristics and PME. We examined six moderators: overweight/obese status (BMI ≥25 vs. <25 kg/m2), obese status (BMI ≥30 vs. <30 kg/m2), SSB consumption (≥ 4.5 vs. < 4.5 servings/week [sample median]), educational attainment (college degree or more vs. some college or less), income (>150% of the Federal Poverty Level [FPL] vs. ≤ 150% FPL), and race (white vs. non-white).
We assessed the impact of the two between-subjects factors (health effects and nutrient disclosure) on knowledge of SSB health consequences using general (i.e., not mixed) logistic regression, reflecting that participants responded to knowledge items only once, after seeing all of their assigned warnings. The initial models included both factors and their interaction; the interactions were not significant in any model so were removed from final models. To identify the color combinations perceived as most effective, we calculated the proportion of participants who selected each color combination as the “most discouraging” for each label shape (rectangular and octagonal).
Results
Sample
Participants’ average age was 37.4 years (Table 1). About 17% of participants had a household income of 150% FPL or less. The sample was younger, more likely to identify as gay, lesbian, or bisexual, less likely to identify as Hispanic, more likely to smoke, and less likely to have a BMI in the obese category compared to nationally representative samples (Supplemental Table 2). Nearly all participants (98%) passed the attention check. Sample characteristics did not differ by experimental condition.
Table 1.
Characteristic | n | % |
---|---|---|
Age | ||
18–29 years | 361 | 27% |
30–39 years | 547 | 40% |
40–54 years | 295 | 22% |
55+ years | 149 | 11% |
Mean (SD) | 37.4 | 11.5 |
Gender | ||
Male | 704 | 52% |
Female | 639 | 47% |
Transgender or other | 9 | 1% |
Gay, lesbian, or bisexual | 141 | 10% |
Hispanic | 122 | 9% |
Race | ||
White | 1,106 | 82% |
Black or African American | 127 | 9% |
Asian | 63 | 5% |
Other/multiracial | 47 | 3% |
American Indian or Alaskan Native | 8 | 1% |
Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander | 1 | 0.1% |
Education | ||
High school diploma or less | 170 | 13% |
Some college | 313 | 23% |
College graduate or associates degree | 699 | 52% |
Graduate degree | 170 | 13% |
Household income, annual | ||
$0-$24,999 | 234 | 17% |
$25,000-$49,999 | 425 | 31% |
$50,000-$74,999 | 322 | 24% |
$75,000+ | 370 | 27% |
Low income (≤ 150% of the Federal Poverty Level) | 224 | 17% |
Current smoker | 298 | 22% |
Sugar-sweetened beverage consumption | ||
<1 time per day | 866 | 64% |
1 to <3 times per day | 312 | 23% |
3 or more times per day | 175 | 13% |
Body mass index (BMI, kg/m2) | ||
Underweight | 49 | 4% |
Healthy weight | 519 | 38% |
Overweight | 409 | 30% |
Obese | 301 | 22% |
Not reported | 82 | 6% |
Mean (SD) | 26.6 | 6.8 |
Passed attention check | 1,338 | 98% |
Note. Characteristics and outcomes did not differ by experimental arms. Missing demographic data ranged from 0.5% to 0.9%, except for BMI (6.0% missing) (see Supplemental Table 3).
Perceived Message Effectiveness
Main effects of experimental factors.
Warnings that included health effects were perceived as more effective than warnings without health effects (average differential effect [ADE]=0.63, p<0.001) (Figure 3). Warnings with nutrient disclosures also led to higher PME compared to warnings without nutrient disclosures (ADE=0.32, p<0.001). Likewise, PME was higher for warnings that included the marker word “WARNING” (ADE=0.21, p<0.001) than warnings without a marker word and for warnings displayed on octagon-shaped labels compared to rectangular labels (ADE=0.08, p<0.001).
Interactions between experimental factors.
Nutrient disclosure interacted with health effects (p for interaction <0.001, Supplemental Table 4). Adding a nutrient disclosure led to higher PME when the warning did not include health effects (Mean [M]=2.75 vs. M=3.41; ADE=0.66, p<0.001) (Figure 4). However, the addition of a nutrient disclosure had no benefit when a health effects statement was also included (M=3.71 vs. M=3.70; ADE=−0.01, p=0.90).
Marker word interacted with health effects (p for interaction<0.001, Supplemental Table 4). For warnings that did not include health effects, adding a marker word led to higher PME compared to not having a marker word (M=2.91 vs. M=3.24; ADE = 0.32, p < 0.001, Supplemental Fig. 2). For warnings that included health effects, adding a marker word still increased PME, but the impact was smaller (M=3.66 vs. M=3.75; ADE=0.09, p<0.001).
Marker word also interacted with nutrient disclosure (p for interaction<0.001, Supplemental Table 2). For warnings that did not include a nutrient disclosure, adding the marker word led to higher PME compared to warnings without a marker word (M=3.10 vs. M=3.35; ADE=0.25, p<0.001) (Supplemental Figure 3). For warnings with a nutrient disclosure, adding the marker word again led to higher PME (M=3.47 vs. M=3.64; ADE=0.16, p<0.001), though the effect was smaller.
Interactions between experimental factors and participant characteristics.
Only two of the twenty-four interactions between participant characteristics (income, education, race, overweight, obesity, or SSB consumption) and the experimental factors on PME were statistically significant, potentially indicating type I error. Nutrient disclosure had a smaller impact on PME for high SSB consumers compared to low-consumers (p for interaction=0.012). Octagon-shaped labels had a larger impact on PME for participants with an overweight/obese BMI than those with BMI in the normal range (p for interaction=0.038).
Fear and Thinking about Harms
Main effects of experimental factors.
A similar pattern of results emerged for fear and thinking about harms, the secondary study outcomes. Of the warning characteristics, health effects had the largest impact on both thinking about harms (ADE=0.66, p<0.001) and fear (ADE=0.42, p<0.001) (Figure 3). Including a nutrient disclosure also increased thinking about harms (ADE=0.23, p<0.001) and fear (ADE=0.15, p=0.013). The marker word “WARNING” increased thinking about harms and fear (ADE=0.22 and 0.23, respectively, both p’s<0.001). Finally, compared to rectangular labels, octagon-shaped labels elicited more thinking about harms (ADE=0.08, p<0.001) and fear (ADE=0.09, p<0.001).
Interactions between experimental factors.
Nutrient disclosure again interacted with health effects, a finding replicated for both thinking about harms (p for interaction <0.001) and fear (p for interactions <0.05). Including both health effects and a nutrient disclosure again did not perform better than including health effects alone (Figure 4). Marker word again interacted with health effects, showing a similar pattern as for PME (ps for interactions <0.001) (Supplemental Table 4, Supplemental Figure 2). However, unlike for PME, marker word did not interact with nutrient disclosure for either secondary outcome (ps for interactions >0.30).
Knowledge of Consequences of SSB Consumption
Knowledge that SSB consumption contributes to tooth decay was 2.1 percentage points higher among participants exposed to warnings that included health effects (p=0.048) (Supplemental Table 5). Exposure to health effects messages did not affect knowledge that SSBs contribute to obesity or diabetes (ps > 0.25), but led to lower knowledge that SSBs contribute to heart disease, information not included in the warnings, by 9.4 percentage points (60.8% vs. 51.4% answered correctly, p < 0.001).
Color Combinations Selected as Most Discouraging
For octagon-shaped labels, the majority of participants (75%) said that a warning with red background and white text would most discourage them from consuming beverages with added sugar (Supplemental Table 1). Likewise, for rectangle-shaped labels, most (66%) participants indicated this color combination would most discourage them. The between-subjects factors (health effects and nutrient disclosure) did not impact color combination selections (ps>0.19).
Discussion
SSB health warnings are a promising policy strategy for reducing SSB consumption. Yet little is known about how to best design such warnings to maximize their impact. In this experimental study of U.S. adults, we found that warning characteristics influence reactions to SSB health warnings. Specifically, warnings that described health effects, included a nutrient disclosure, began with the marker word “WARNING,” and were displayed on octagon-shaped labels were perceived to be more effective than warnings without these characteristics. These characteristics also increased thinking about the harms of SSB consumption and feelings of fear. Participants selected the red background with white text as the most discouraging color combination for both octagonal and rectangular warnings. Because past research has shown that these reactions (perceived message effectiveness,29–33,44 thinking about harms,33,35,36 and fear33,34) predict warnings’ actual effectiveness, our findings suggest design choices that could increase the impact of SSB health warnings.
SSB health warnings proposed in the U.S. have all included health effects.9–13 This is a wise choice, given that health effects had the largest impact of the warning characteristics we studied. This finding is consistent with cigarette warning research, which has found that health effects messages are generally more potent than “found in” statements identifying toxic products that contain cigarette smoke chemicals.52 Others have suggested health effects increase perceived message effectiveness by providing contextualizing information that increases motivation to think about the warning message and helps consumers understand the harms of a particular product.18,52 In contrast to the U.S., warning systems implemented in Latin American countries do not describe health effects, instead using nutrient disclosures.14–16,53 These nutrient disclosures accompany all foods and beverages that exceed thresholds for certain nutrients, not just SSBs. Future research should compare health effects warnings to nutrient disclosures on a larger variety of products in U.S. and non-U.S. samples.
Adding more text to warnings in our experiment had diminishing returns. Across outcomes, the textual warning characteristics we manipulated (health effects, nutrient disclosure, and marker word) interacted with one another, such that the additional impact of a textual characteristic (e.g., a marker word) was generally lower when a message already included another textual warning characteristic (e.g., health effect) than when it did not. The interaction between health effects and nutrient disclosures was particularly large: adding a nutrient disclosure to a warning that did not include health effects increased perceived message effectiveness, thinking about harms, and fear, but adding a nutrient disclosure to a warning that already included a health effects statement had no additional influence on these outcomes. These results suggest that SSB health warnings may perform best when they include only a nutrient disclosure or only health effects, but not both. These findings are consistent with other studies suggesting that “less is more” when showing consumers comparative quality information.54 Our findings also replicate studies from the tobacco warnings literature.18,52 For example, cigarette warnings studies have shown the same pattern of “less is more” interaction such that combining the two forms of risk information (health effects and “found in” statements) did little or no better than presenting either one alone.52
Consistent with previous research on SSB and tobacco warnings,19,20,55 warning characteristics had similar impact regardless of participants’ income, education level, and race/ethnicity. One exception was that nutrient disclosures had a slightly smaller influence on perceived message effectiveness for high SSB consumers compared to low consumers. This finding could be explained by the defensive processing literature, which suggests that resistance to messages is strongest among people engaging in the behavior targeted by the message.56,57 The other exception was that the octagon shape had a larger influence on perceived message effectiveness for participants with an overweight/obese BMI.
Strengths of our experiment include the large sample from across the U.S. and that we randomly assigned participants to conditions using a fully factorial design. Limitations include using a convenience sample, which may limit the generalizability of the findings. However, recent research has found that experiments conducted on MTurk generally replicate findings of experiments conducted using probability-based samples.39,58,59 Previous research has found that the impact of SSB health warnings on consumer perceptions varies by SSB type (e.g., fruit drinks vs. sodas).60 Because we only displayed warnings on sodas, we were unable to examine whether SSB type moderated the impact of the manipulated warning characteristics on our study outcomes. We also displayed warnings on non-branded SSBs on a computer screen, and warnings were likely more noticeable than they would be if implemented on actual SSBs in retail settings. Finally, study outcomes were all based on self-report after brief exposure to the warnings. A recent meta-analysis indicates that self-reported perceived message effectiveness (our primary outcome) predicts actual behavior change for tobacco messages,44 but future studies should examine whether warnings with these characteristics affect consumer behavior.
Conclusions
To maximize the impact of SSB health warnings, policymakers should consider adopting warnings that describe health effects, begin with the marker word “WARNING,” and are displayed on an octagon-shaped label, as warnings with these characteristics are perceived to be more effective, and elicit more thinking about harms and fear, than warnings without these characteristics. Warnings that include a nutrient disclosure also increase perceived effectiveness over warnings that do not, but to a lesser extent than warnings with health effects. Further, including both a nutrient disclosure and health effects is unlikely to improve effectiveness over health effects alone. Future work should assess whether these principles apply to other types of warnings (e.g., on alcohol or junk food) and in other countries, and should examine whether warnings with these characteristics influence behavioral outcomes.
Supplementary Material
Highlights.
Uncertainty remains about how to design impactful health warnings for sugary drinks
Describing health effects increases perceived effectiveness of sugary drink warnings
Including a nutrient disclosure also increases perceived effectiveness
But, including both of these elements reduces each’s impact on perceived effectiveness
“Marker” words and octagon-shaped labels also increase perceived effectiveness
Acknowledgments
We thank Emily Busey for help creating the experimental stimuli and Cathy Zimmer for statistical consulting.
Funding
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (T32 CA057726 and P50 CA180907). Training and general support were provided by the Carolina Population Center (P2C HD050924 and T32 HD007168).
Abbreviations:
- BMI
body mass index
- FOP
front-of-package
- MTurk
Mechanical Turk
- SSBs
sugar-sweetened beverages
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors have no conflicts to declare.
References
- 1.Bleich SN, Vercammen KA, Koma JW, Li Z. Trends in beverage consumption among children and adults, 2003–2014. Obesity. 2017. doi: 10.1002/oby.22056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.United States Department of Health and Human Services, United States Department of Agriculture. Dietary Guidelines for Americans 2015–2020. 8th Edition. 8th ed.; 2015. http://health.gov/dietaryguidelines/2015/guidelines/. Accessed November 10, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Johnson RK, Appel LJ, Brands M, et al. Dietary sugars intake and cardiovascular health: A scientific statement from the American Heart Association. Circulation. 2009;120(11):1011–1020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Malik V, Pan A, Willett WC, Hu FB. Sugar-sweetened beverages and weight gain in children and adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;98(4):1084–1102. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.113.058362 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hu F Resolved: There is sufficient scientific evidence that decreasing sugar-sweetened beverage consumption will reduce the prevalence of obesity and obesity-related diseases. Obes Rev. 2013;14(8):606–619. doi: 10.1111/obr.12040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Malik VS, Popkin BM, Bray GA, Després J-P, Willett WC, Hu FB. Sugar-sweetened beverages and risk of metabolic syndrome and type 2 diabetes: A meta-analysis. Diabetes Care. 2010;33(11):2477–2483. doi: 10.2337/dc10-1079 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Greenwood D, Threapleton D, Evans C, et al. Association between sugar-sweetened and artificially sweetened soft drinks and type 2 diabetes: systematic review and dose–response meta-analysis of prospective studies. Br J Nutr. 2014;112(5):725–734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Malik VS, Popkin BM, Bray GA, Després J-P, Hu FB. Sugar-sweetened beverages, obesity, type 2 diabetes mellitus, and cardiovascular disease risk. Circulation. 2010;121(11):1356–1364. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.109.876185 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Monning B Sugar-Sweetened Beverages: Health Warnings.; 2017. https://leginfo.legislature.ca.gov/faces/billTextClient.xhtml?bill_id=201720180SB300&search_keywords=soda+label.
- 10.Stevens T, Carr S. An Act Related to Health and Safety Warnings on Sugar-Sweetened Beverages.; 2017. http://legislature.vermont.gov/assets/Documents/2018/Docs/BILLS/H-0433/H-0433%20As%20Introduced.pdf.
- 11.Kobayashi B, Lopresti M, Morikawa D. Relating to Health.; 2017. http://www.capitol.hawaii.gov/measure_indiv.aspx?billtype=HB&billnumber=1209&year=2017.
- 12.Robinson J Concerning Mitigation of the Adverse Impacts of Sugar-Sweetened Beverages.; 2016. http://app.leg.wa.gov/billsummary?BillNumber=2798&Year=2016.
- 13.Rivera G Requires Sugar-Sweetened Beverages to Be Labeled with a Safety Warning.; 2017. https://www.nysenate.gov/legislation/bills/2017/S162.
- 14.Corvalán C, Reyes M, Garmendia ML, Uauy R. Structural responses to the obesity and non-communicable diseases epidemic: The Chilean Law of Food Labeling and Advertising. Obes Rev. 2013;14:79–87. doi: 10.1111/obr.12099 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Corvalán C, Reyes M, Garmendia ML, Uauy R. Structural responses to the obesity and non-communicable diseases epidemic: Update on the Chilean law of food labelling and advertising. Obes Rev. 2018. doi: 10.1111/obr.12802 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.United States Department of Agriculture Foreign Agriculture Service. Chile: Chile’s New Nutritional Labeling Law. https://www.fas.usda.gov/data/chile-chiles-new-nutritional-labeling-law. Published June 29, 2015. Accessed December 3, 2016.
- 17.State of Israel Ministry of Health. Food Label and Nutritional Labeling. https://www.health.gov.il/English/News_and_Events/Spokespersons_Messages/Pages/20122017_1.aspx. Published 2018. Accessed June 23, 2018.
- 18.Noar SM, Kelley DE, Boynton MH, et al. Identifying principles for effective messages about chemicals in cigarette smoke. Prev Med. 2018;106:31–37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Roberto CA, Wong D, Musicus A, Hammond D. The influence of sugar-sweetened beverage health warning labels on parents’ choices. Pediatrics. 2016;137(2):e20153185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.VanEpps EM, Roberto CA. The influence of sugar-sweetened beverage warnings: A randomized trial of adolescents’ choices and beliefs. Am J Prev Med. 2016;51(5):664–672. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2016.07.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bollard T, Maubach N, Walker N, Mhurchu CN. Effects of plain packaging, warning labels, and taxes on young people’s predicted sugar-sweetened beverage preferences: An experimental study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2016;13(1):95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Cabrera M, Machín L, Arrúa A, et al. Nutrition warnings as front-of-pack labels: Influence of design features on healthfulness perception and attentional capture. Public Health Nutr. 2017:1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Acton RB, Vanderlee L, Roberto CA, Hammond D. Consumer perceptions of specific design characteristics for front-of-package nutrition labels. Health Educ Res. 2018;33(2):167–174. doi: 10.1093/her/cyy006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Mahood G Canadian tobacco package warning system. Tob Control. 1995;4(1):10. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Mahood G Canada’s Tobacco Package Label or Warning System: “Telling the Truth” about Tobacco Product Risks. World Health Organization; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Sebrié EM, Blanco A, Glantz SA. Cigarette labeling policies in Latin America and the Caribbean: Progress and obstacles. Salud Pública México. 2010;52:S233–S243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Khandpur N, Sato P, Mais L, et al. Are front-of-package warning labels more effective at communicating nutrition information than traffic-light labels? A randomized controlled experiment in a Brazilian sample. Nutrients. 2018;10(6). doi: 10.3390/nu10060688 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Williams DJ, Noyes JM. How does our perception of risk influence decision-making? Implications for the design of risk information. Theor Issues Ergon Sci. 2007;8(1):1–35. doi: 10.1080/14639220500484419 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Davis KC, Duke J, Shafer P, Patel D, Rodes R, Beistle D. Perceived effectiveness of antismoking ads and association with quit attempts among smokers: evidence from the tips from former smokers campaign. Health Commun. 2017;32(8):931–938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Brennan E, Durkin SJ, Wakefield MA, Kashima Y. Assessing the effectiveness of antismoking television advertisements: Do audience ratings of perceived effectiveness predict changes in quitting intentions and smoking behaviours? Tob Control. 2013:tobaccocontrol-2012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Davis KC, Nonnemaker J, Duke J, Farrelly MC. Perceived effectiveness of cessation advertisements: The importance of audience reactions and practical implications for media campaign planning. Health Commun. 2013;28(5):461–472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Bigsby E, Cappella JN, Seitz HH. Efficiently and effectively evaluating public service announcements: Additional evidence for the utility of perceived effectiveness. Commun Monogr. 2013;80(1):1–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Brewer NT, Parada Humberto Jr, Hall MG, Boynton MH, Noar SM, Ribisl KM. Understanding why pictorial cigarette pack warnings increase quit attempts. Ann Behav Med. May 2018:kay032–kay032. doi: 10.1093/abm/kay032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Hammond D, Fong GT, McDonald PW, Brown KS, Cameron R. Graphic Canadian cigarette warning labels and adverse outcomes: Evidence from Canadian smokers. Am J Public Health. 2004;94(8):1442–1445. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Fathelrahman AI, Li L, Borland R, et al. Stronger pack warnings predict quitting more than weaker ones: Finding from the ITC Malaysia and Thailand surveys. Tob Induc Dis. 2013;11(1):20–20. doi: 10.1186/1617-9625-11-20 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Borland R, Wilson N, Fong GT, et al. Impact of graphic and text warnings on cigarette packs: Findings from four countries over five years. Tob Control. 2009;18(5):358. doi: 10.1136/tc.2008.028043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling S. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspect Psychol Sci. 2011;6(1):3–5. doi: 10.1177/1745691610393980 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Paolacci G, Chandler J. Inside the Turk: Understanding Mechanical Turk as a participant pool. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2014;23(3):184–188. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Berinsky AJ, Huber GA, Lenz GS. Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon.com’s Mechanical Turk. Polit Anal. 2012;20(3):351–368. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Crump MJ, McDonnell JV, Gureckis TM. Evaluating Amazon’s Mechanical Turk as a tool for experimental behavioral research. PloS One. 2013;8(3):e57410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Jeong M, Zhang D, Morgan JC, et al. Similarities and differences in tobacco control research findings from convenience and probability samples. Ann Behav Med. July 2018:kay059–kay059. doi: 10.1093/abm/kay059 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Baig SA, Noar SM, Gottfredson NC, Boynton MH, Ribisl KM, Brewer NT. UNC Perceived Message Effectiveness: Validation of a brief scale. Ann Behav Med. October 2018:kay080–kay080. doi: 10.1093/abm/kay080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Noar SM, Bell T, Kelley D, Barker J, Yzer M. Perceived message effectiveness measures in tobacco education campaigns: A systematic review. Commun Methods Meas. 2018:1–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Noar SM, Barker J, Bell T, Yzer M. Does perceived message effectiveness predict the actual effectiveness of tobacco education messages? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Commun. 2018:1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Brewer NT, Jeong M, Mendel JR, et al. Cigarette pack messages about toxic chemicals: A randomised clinical trial. Tob Control. April 2018. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2017-054112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Fathelrahman AI, Omar M, Awang R, Cummings KM, Borland R, Samin ASBM. Impact of the new Malaysian cigarette pack warnings on smokers’ awareness of health risks and interest in quitting smoking. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2010;7(11). doi: 10.3390/ijerph7114089 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Moodie C, MacKintosh AM, Hammond D. Adolescents’ response to text-only tobacco health warnings: Results from the 2008 UK Youth Tobacco Policy Survey. Eur J Public Health. 2010;20(4):463–469. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckp199 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Nonnemaker J, Farrelly M, Kamyab K, Busey A, Mann N. Experimental study of graphic cigarette warning labels. Final Results Rep RTI Proj. 2010;7. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bernabé E, Vehkalahti MM, Sheiham A, Aromaa A, Suominen AL. Sugar-sweetened beverages and dental caries in adults: A 4-year prospective study. J Dent. 2014;42(8):952–958. doi: 10.1016/j.jdent.2014.04.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Marshall TA, Levy SM, Broffitt B, et al. Dental caries and beverage consumption in young children. Pediatrics. 2003;112(3 Pt 1):e184–91. doi: 10.1542/peds.112.3.e184 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Berinsky AJ, Margolis MF, Sances MW. Separating the shirkers from the workers? Making sure respondents pay attention on self-administered surveys. Am J Polit Sci. 2014;58(3):739–753. doi: 10.1111/ajps.12081 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Baig SA, Byron MJ, Boynton MH, Brewer NT, Ribisl KM. Communicating about cigarette smoke constituents: An experimental comparison of two messaging strategies. J Behav Med. 2017;40(2):352–359. doi: 10.1007/s10865-016-9795-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.United States Department of Agriculture Foreign Agriculture Service. Peru Publishes Warning Manual for Processed Product Food Labels. https://www.fas.usda.gov/data/peru-peru-publishes-warning-manual-processed-product-food-labels. Published October 25, 2017. Accessed September 3, 2018.
- 54.Peters E, Dieckmann N, Dixon A, Hibbard JH, Mertz CK. Less is more in presenting quality information to consumers. Med Care Res Rev MCRR. 2007;64(2):169–190. doi: 10.1177/10775587070640020301 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Brewer NT, Hall MG, Noar SM, et al. Effect of pictorial cigarette pack warnings on changes in smoking behavior: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Intern Med. 2016;176(7):905–912. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2016.2621 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Brehm SS, Brehm JW. Psychological Reactance: A Theory of Freedom and Control. Academic Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Hall MG, Sheeran P, Noar SM, Ribisl KM, Bach LE, Brewer NT. Reactance to Health Warnings Scale: Development and validation. Ann Behav Med. 2016;50(5):736–750. doi: 10.1007/s12160-016-9799-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Jeong M, Zhang D, Morgan J, et al. Similarities and differences in health behavior research findings from convenience and probability samples. Under review. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 59.Weinberg JD, Freese J, McElhattan D. Comparing Data Characteristics and Results of an Online Factorial Survey between a Population-Based and a Crowdsource-Recruited Sample. Sociol Sci. 2014;1. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Moran AJ, Roberto CA. Health warning labels correct parents’ misperceptions about sugary drink options. Am J Prev Med. 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.