Skip to main content
Brain Sciences logoLink to Brain Sciences
. 2019 Apr 18;9(4):87. doi: 10.3390/brainsci9040087

The Temporal Effects of Acute Exercise on Episodic Memory Function: Systematic Review with Meta-Analysis

Paul D Loprinzi 1,*, Jeremiah Blough 1, Lindsay Crawford 1, Seungho Ryu 1, Liye Zou 2, Hong Li 3,4,*
PMCID: PMC6523402  PMID: 31003491

Abstract

Background: Accumulating research demonstrates that the timing of exercise plays an important role in influencing episodic memory. However, we have a limited understanding as to the factors that moderate this temporal effect. Thus, the purpose of this systematic review with meta-analysis was to evaluate the effects of study characteristics (e.g., exercise modality, intensity and duration of acute exercise) and participant attributes (e.g., age, sex) across each of the temporal periods of acute exercise on episodic memory (i.e., acute exercise occurring before memory encoding, and during memory encoding, early consolidation, and late consolidation). Methods: The following databases were used for our computerized searches: Embase/PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar, Sports Discus and PsychInfo. Studies were included if they: (1) Employed an experimental design with a comparison to a control group/visit, (2) included human participants, (3) evaluated exercise as the independent variable, (4) employed an acute bout of exercise (defined as a single bout of exercise), (5) evaluated episodic memory as the outcome variable (defined as the retrospective recall of information either in a spatial or temporal manner), and (6) provided sufficient data (e.g., mean, SD, and sample size) for a pooled effect size estimate. Results: In total, 25 articles met our inclusionary criteria and were meta-analyzed. Acute exercise occurring before memory encoding (d = 0.11, 95% CI: −0.01, 0.23, p = 0.08), during early memory consolidation (d = 0.47, 95% CI: 0.28, 0.67; p < 0.001) and during late memory consolidation (d = 1.05, 95% CI: 0.32, 1.78; p = 0.005) enhanced episodic memory function. Conversely, acute exercise occurring during memory encoding had a negative effect on episodic memory (d = −0.12, 95% CI: −0.22, −0.02; p = 0.02). Various study designs and participant characteristics moderated the temporal effects of acute exercise on episodic memory function. For example, vigorous-intensity acute exercise, and acute exercise among young adults, had greater effects when the acute bout of exercise occurred before memory encoding or during the early memory consolidation period. Conclusions: The timing of acute exercise plays an important role in the exercise-memory interaction. Various exercise- and participant-related characteristics moderate this temporal relationship.

Keywords: cognition, memory function, physical activity

1. Introduction

Episodic memory refers to the retrospective recall of information from a spatial-temporal context [1]. That is, retrieving previously encoded information based on its place and location in time. Episodic memory function is critical for communicative behavior [2], is predictive of longevity [3], and the deterioration of episodic memory is associated with several neurocognitive and neuropsychiatric disorders [4]. Thus, identification of behaviors that enhance episodic memory is a worthwhile endeavor.

Accumulating research demonstrates that both acute and chronic exercise behavior can subserve cognitive function, including episodic memory [5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27]. Of interest to this review are the effects of acute exercise (vs. chronic exercise) on episodic memory. This tailored focus on acute exercise is justified, as the mechanisms through which acute and chronic exercise influence episodic memory is distinct (discussed elsewhere [28,29]). Recent excellent reviews (e.g., see Roig et al. [14,15]) have qualitatively and quantitatively summarized the effects of acute exercise on memory function. These reviews, as well as other reviews [16,30], have demonstrated that acute exercise has small-to-moderate effects on enhancing memory, and that these effects may, in part, be moderated by key study characteristics (e.g., timing of exercise and the memory assessment, exercise modality, intensity and duration of exercise) and participant attributes (e.g., age, sex).

For example, the timing of the exercise and memory stimulus plays a critical role in the exercise-memory interaction [14,15]. Various temporal periods have been evaluated in recent work on this topic, including acute exercise occurring before memory encoding, during memory encoding, and during the early and late phases of memory consolidation. Accumulating research suggests that when the acute bout of exercise occurs shortly before the memory task, episodic memory may be enhanced [31,32,33,34]. As we have discussed elsewhere [29], this may be a result of acute exercise-induced alterations in long-term potentiation (LTP), or the functional connectivity of communicating neurons.

Although there is accumulated research demonstrating that the timing of exercise plays an important role in influencing episodic memory, we have a limited understanding as to whether this temporal effect of acute exercise on episodic memory is influenced by study characteristics (e.g., exercise modality, intensity and duration of acute exercise) and participant attributes (e.g., age, sex). Such an effect is plausible for several reasons [35]. These characteristics appear to play a moderating role on their own (while not considering the temporal effects of acute exercise on memory). For example, we recently demonstrated that the vigorous-intensity, acute exercise has greater effects in enhancing episodic memory when compared to the lower-intensity, acute exercise [34]. However, it is plausible that vigorous-intensity, acute exercise would have a differential effect on episodic memory based on the timing of the acute bout of exercise and the memory stimulus. For example, acute, vigorous-intensity exercise may enhance memory when it occurs prior to memory encoding (via LTP) and may also enhance long-term memory if it occurs during the consolidation period (e.g., vigorous-intensity, acute exercise-induced increases in neurotrophins may help stabilize the memory trace) [36]. However, vigorous-intensity, acute exercise may have a negative effect on episodic memory if occurring during memory encoding, as this higher-intensity bout of exercise may induce a transient hypofrontality effect and mental fatigue [37,38], which in turn, may reduce memory encoding.

Regarding participant attributes, other reviews demonstrate that, for example, young adults (vs. older adults) appear to have greater memory-enhancing effects from acute exercise [15]. However, it is uncertain as to whether these (exercise intensity, age) and other characteristics (e.g., sex) consistently influence the acute exercise-memory interaction across all exercise-memory temporal periods. Thus, the purpose of this systematic review with meta-analysis was to evaluate the effects of study characteristics (e.g., exercise modality, intensity and duration of acute exercise) and participant attributes (e.g., age, sex) across each of the temporal periods of acute exercise on episodic memory (i.e., acute exercise occurring before memory encoding, and during memory encoding, early consolidation, and late consolidation). We hypothesize that the acute exercise occurring before memory encoding and during early and late consolidation will enhance episodic memory, whereas acute exercise occurring during memory encoding will impair episodic memory. However, of central focus of this review, we hypothesize that key moderators (e.g., vigorous-intensity exercise, age) will influence this temporal relationship between acute exercise and episodic memory function. The identification of potential moderators of this temporal relationship should aid in the design of future projects on this topic as well as, even within one temporal period (e.g., acute exercise before memory encoding), demonstrate the complexity of the acute exercise-memory interaction.

2. Methods

2.1. Data Sources and Search Strategy

The following databases were used for our computerized searches: Embase/PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar, Sports Discus, and PsychInfo [39]. Articles were retrieved from inception to 14 February 2019. The search terms, including their combinations, were: Physical activity, exercise, physical exercise, cognition, memory, episodic memory, and temporality.

2.2. Study Selection

The literature searches were performed independently by two separate authors and comparisons were made to determine the number of eligible studies. Consensus was reached from these two independent reviews. Upon performing the computerized searches, the article titles and abstracts were reviewed to identify potentially relevant articles. Articles appearing to meet the inclusionary criteria were retrieved and reviewed at the full text level.

2.3. Inclusionary Criteria

Studies were included if they: (1) Employed an experimental design with a comparison to a control group/visit, (2) included human participants, (3) evaluated exercise as the independent variable, (4) employed an acute bout of exercise (defined as a single bout of exercise), (5) evaluated episodic memory as the outcome variable (defined as the retrospective recall of information either in a spatial or temporal manner), and (6) provided sufficient data (e.g., mean, SD, and sample size) for an effect size estimate.

2.4. Methodological Quality of Evaluated Studies

Two authors independently evaluated the methodological quality of the included studies. When disagreements between the two reviewers occurred, they discussed the discrepant results together and reached a consensus. The methodological quality of each included study was evaluated using the Physiotherapy Evidence Database (PEDro) scale. This scale is based on 11 items to assess study rigor, including: Eligibility criteria, random allocation, concealed allocation, baseline comparability, blinded subjects, blinded therapists, blinded assessors, follow-up, intention-to-treat, between group analysis, and outcome point estimates. We adapted the PEDro scale to fit our study topic. Of the 11 items, we removed the eligibility criteria item because this focuses more on external validity as opposed to internal validity. Further, blinded subjects and blinded therapists were removed because it is not feasible to blind the subject/researcher for an exercise protocol. Further, follow-up and intention-to-treat items were removed because of the acute exercise paradigm employed in our evaluated experiments. Thus, we retained and evaluated the following items: Random allocation, concealed allocation, baseline comparability, blinded assessors, between group analysis, and outcome point estimates. In addition to these six items, we added an additional internal validity item, including whether studies reported an objective measure of exercise intensity (e.g., heart rate). In total, seven items were evaluated. Studies that met the evaluated criteria were given a point (seven points maximum), with a greater score indicative of higher methodological quality. Points were only awarded if the criterion was clearly satisfied in the paper. Given that some of the studies employed a within-subject design, these studies were automatically awarded a point for the baseline comparability item.

2.5. Data Extraction of Included Studies

Detailed information from each of the included studies was extracted, including the following information: Author, sample characteristics, study design, exercise temporality (i.e., when the exercise took place in reference to the memory assessment), exercise protocol, memory assessment, and results.

2.6. Categorization of Temporal Period

We evaluated four temporal periods regarding the timing of exercise and the memory assessment. These included: (1) Exercise before memory encoding vs. control, (2) exercise during memory encoding vs. control, (3) exercise during early consolidation vs. control, and (4) exercise during late consolidation vs. control.

The temporal period of ‘exercise before memory encoding’ was defined as the acute bout of exercise occurring prior to encoding the memory stimuli. We defined ‘exercise during memory encoding’ if the acute bout of exercise occurred while encoding the memory stimuli. Early consolidation was considered within the first four h post memory encoding, whereas late consolidation was defined as four or more h after memory encoding. This specific threshold of four h was utilized as the two studies evaluating the effects of exercise during the late consolidation period employed this time frame [19,40].

2.7. Categorization of Moderators

The evaluated moderators included age, sex, race-ethnicity, memory type, exercise intensity, exercise duration, and exercise modality. We were not able to evaluate the moderation effects of cardiorespiratory fitness, as too few studies evaluated this parameter. The evaluated moderators were chosen as they have been shown to influence episodic memory [15,30,41]. Notably, however, not all of these moderators could be evaluated for each of the temporal periods. Moderation analyses were computed when at least two studies provided data for the moderation analysis.

Age was categorized as young-adult (18–24 years), adult (25–44 years), middle-age (45–60 years), and older adults (>60 years) [15]. The sex moderation analyses were evaluated for males, females, mixed samples, and predominately male/female. Predominately male/female was defined as a study including >71% of a particular sex [42]. Race-ethnicity was defined as non-Hispanic white and other. For the episodic memory type, we evaluated both short-term and long-term episodic memory, with the latter defined as a delayed period >2-min [15]. Exercise-intensity was based on thresholds suggested by the American College of Sports Medicine [43]. For example, based on maximum heart rate estimates, light, moderate and vigorous-intensity exercise, respectively, were defined as <64%, 64%–76%, and >76%. Exercise duration was defined as short duration (<20 min), medium (20–40 min), and long duration (>40 min) [15]. Lastly, exercise modality was defined as treadmill-based walking/running or cycling.

2.8. Data Synthesis

The Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software (Version 3, Biostat, NJ, USA) was used to calculate effect sizes (Cohen’s d) and 95% CI, employing a random-effects model. The weighted mean effect size (Cohen’s d) and 95% CI were calculated using the inverse variance weighting method. Effect size estimates were evaluated for each of the above-mentioned moderators for each temporal period. The degree of heterogeneity of the effect sizes was evaluated with the Cochran’s Q-statistic. Egger’s regression test was used to evaluate the potential publication bias.

3. Results

3.1. Retrieved Articles

Figure 1 displays the flow chart of the article retrieval process. The computerized searches identified 6621 articles. Among the 6621 articles, 6569 were excluded and 52 full text articles were reviewed. Among these 52 articles, five were duplicates and 22 were ineligible [5,24,25,26,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61] as they did not provide enough data for an effect size calculation. Thus, in total, 25 articles met our inclusionary criteria and were eligible for the quantitative meta-analysis.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Flow chart of article retrieval.

3.2. Study Quality

The methodological quality of the studies is shown in Table 1. On a scale from 0–7, the mean score was 4.72 (SD = 0.79). For the random allocation (between-subject design) or counterbalancing (within-subject design) item, 24 of the 25 studies (96.0%) satisfied this criterion. Similarly, the majority of studies (N = 21; 84.0%) employed a baseline comparison assessment, objective measure of exercise intensity (N = 22; 88.0%), between/within-group analysis (N = 25; 100%), and outcome point estimate (N = 25; 100%). However, only one study (4.0%) provided sufficient details on whether allocation concealment was employed, and zero studies (0.0%) blinded the assessors to the outcome.

Table 1.

Methodological quality of the included studies.

Author Random Allocation/Counterbalance Concealed Allocation Baseline Comparability Blinding of Assessors to Outcome Between/Within Group Analysis Reported Objective Measure of Exercise Intensity Outcome Point Estimate Total
Stones et al. (1993) [62] 5
Schramke et al. (1997) [63] 5
Labban et al. (2011) [6] 5
Salas et al. (2011) [64] 3
Nanda et al. (2013) [65] 2
Schmidt-Kassow et al. (2014) [10] 5
Weinberg et al. (2014) [66] 4
Basso et al. (2015) [67] 5
Loprinzi et al. (2015) [68] 5
Bantoft et al. (2016) [69] 4
van Dongen et al. (2016) [19] 5
Crush et al. (2017) [70] 5
Frith et al. (2017) [31] 5
Keyan et al. (2017) [71] 5
Keyan et al. (2017) [72] 5
McNerney et al. (2017) [73] 5
Most et al. (2017) [74] 4
Sng et al. (2017) [32] 5
Delancey et al. (2018) [40] 6
Haynes et al. (2018) [33] 5
Labban et al. (2018) [75] 5
Siddiqui et al. (2018) [76] 5
Wade et al. (2018) [77] 5
Yanes et al. (2018) [78] 5
Zuniga et al. (2018) [79] 5

3.3. Article Synthesis

Details on the study characteristics are displayed in Table 2 (extraction table). Studies ranged from young adults (18 years) to older adults (84 years). Sample sizes ranged from 10 to 352 participants. Among the 25 studies, 17 (68%) employed a between-subject design. The acute exercise protocols varied, ranging from an isokinetic resistance exercise, to a 2-min run, to a 60-min brisk walk. Common memory assessments included word list formats (e.g., CVLT, California Verbal Learning Test; RAVLT, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test), but other episodic memory assessments included, for example, paragraph recalls, image recognition, and recall of visually observed film stimuli.

Table 2.

Extraction table of the evaluated studies.

Author Sample Study Design Exercise Temporality Exercise Protocol Memory Assessment Results
Stones et al. (1993) [62] 20 older adults,
Mage = 84.5
Experimental; between-subject Memory battered occurred before exercise, immediately after exercise and then 30-min post-exercise 15-min exercises, which occurred while sitting in a chair (e.g., stretching, low-intensity aerobic activity, slow rhythmical movement) Word fluency Exercise was associated with greater semantically cued memory (p < 0.01).
Schramke et al. (1997) [63] Two age groups, each including 48 adults. Younger group, 18–38 year. Older group, 60–80 year. Experimental; within-subject Exercise occurred either at rest or during encoding, and similarly, either during retrieval or not. 5–7 minutes of walking in a long internal corridor. CVLT; California verbal learning test There was no difference in learning that was due to initial exercise condition, but both age groups showed greater recall when state was congruent before learning and delayed recall.
Labban et al. (2011) [6] 48 young adults (Mage = 22.0) Experimental; between-subject Exercise occurred before and after encoding 30-min of cycle ergometer exercise, with 20-min at moderate-intensity Paragraph recall, with participants listening to two paragraphs and then recalling as much information as possible from the paragraphs Exercise occurring prior to the memory task was effective in enhancing memory (p < 0.05).
Salas et al. (2011) [64] 80 college undergraduate students (46 women). Mage = 19.3, SD = 2.3 Experimental; between-subject factorial design. A 2 (encoding condition: walking vs. sitting) × 2 (retrieval condition: walking vs. sitting). Exercise occurred either at rest or during encoding, and similarly, either during retrieval or not. 10 minutes of walking outside at a brisk pace Word-list memory task (10 nouns presented sequentially for 6 s each) Students who walked before encoding had significantly higher recall (M = 0.45, SD = 0.17) compared to students who sat before encoding (M = 0.36, SD = 0.15), F(1,76) = 6.34, ƞ2p = 0.08.
Nanda et al. (2013) [65] 10 healthy adult male medical students. Mage = 19.5, SD = 0.9 Quasi-experimental; within-subject Exercise occurred between pre- and post- memory assessments. Cycle ergometer exercise for 30-min at moderate-intensity of 70% of heart rate reserve Spatial span and paired associates memory task Spatial span did not increase from pre- to post, but paired associates was significantly higher after the exercise bout.
Schmidt-Kassow et al. (2014) [10] 49 right-handed German young adults (18–30 year) Experimental; within-subject Exercised during encoding Self-selected walking pace during memory encoding 40-item (Polish) word list. Experiment 1: words recalled during walking was higher than non-walking (5.5, SD = 3.3; vs. 4.8, SD = 4.2), F = 6.98, p = 0.02, ƞ2p = 0.31.
Experiment 2: words recalled during walking was higher than non-walking (5.3, SD = 4.6; vs. 4.1, SD = 3.5), F = 6.44, p = 0.02, ƞ2p = 0.19.
Weinberg et al. (2014) [66] 23 participants (Mage = 20.6 year) in the exercise group and 23 (Mage = 20.2 year) in the control group. Experimental; between-subject Exercised during early consolidation Isokinetic dynamometer knee extension exercise. Session consisted of submaximal voluntary dynamic contractions for a warm-up, maximal voluntary isometric contractions, and 6 sets of 10 repetitions of maximal voluntary knee extension contractions. Both legs were exercised. In the control (passive) group, the experimenter passively moved the participant leg between extension and flexion. 180 images from the IAPS. Follow-up memory recall assessment took place 48-h later. The retrieval task included 90 studied images and 90 new images. Participants were instructed to indicate “remember”, “familiar”, or “new” after seeing each image. There was no valence × group interaction effect. There was a main effect for valence in that participants remembered more positive and negative images than neutral images.
Basso et al. (2015) [67] 85 young adults, Mage = 22.1 Experimental; between-subject Memory tasks occurred before exercise and at various time-points after exercise (30–120 min) 50-min of vigorous-intensity exercise on cycle ergometer Hopkins verbal learning test revised, modified Benton visual retention test, Digit span Acute exercise improved prefrontal-cortex, but not hippocampal-dependent memory function.
Loprinzi et al. (2015) [68] 87 young adults, Mage = 21.4 year Experimental; between-subject Exercise before memory task Light, moderate, and vigorous exercise Spatial span and paired associates Acute exercise was not associated with either memory outcome.
Bantoft et al. (2016) [69] 45 undergraduate students, Mage = 22.6 year (6.2) Experimental; within-subject Sitting, standing or walking during memory task Low-intensity walking Digit span There were no differences in memory performance across the three conditions.
van Dongen et al. (2016) [19] 72 young adults, approximately 22 years Experimental; between-subject Exercise immediately after encoding and 4 hours after encoding 35 min of intermittent high-intensity exercise on cycle ergometer Paired associates learning task Exercising 4 hours after memory encoding was advantageous in improving memory function.
Crush et al. (2017) [70] 352 participants, mean age approximately 21 years Experimental; between-subject Exercise occurring before memory assessment 16 total groups, with groups ranging from 10 min of exercise to 60 min of exercise, including resting periods of either 5, 15, or 30 min Spatial span Shorter exercise recovery periods had a greater effect on memory performance.
Frith et al. (2017) [31] 88 participants (22 per group), approximate age = 21 years. Experimental; between-subject Exercise occurring before, during, and after memory encoding 15-min treadmill bout of progressive high-intensity aerobic exercise RAVLT High-intensity exercise prior to memory encoding was effective in enhancing long-term memory, for both 20-min delay (F = 3.36, p = 0.02, ƞ2p = 0.11) and 24-h delay (F =2.80, p = 0.04, ƞ2p = 0.09).
Keyan et al. (2017) [71] 49 undergraduates between 18–29 years Experimental; between-subject Exercise occurred during the early memory consolidation period Stepping exercise for 10-min on a 15 cm stepper, with a goal of exercising at 50%–85% of max. Viewed a film depicting a car accident. Involves 10 min of live footage depicting emergency workers attending the scene of a motor vehicle accident. Exercise (vs. control) did not induce more recall of central (t = 0.11, p > 0.05) or peripheral (t = 0.42, p > 0.05) details of the accident film. However, those that exercise recalled more intrusive memories of the car accident (t = 2.36, p = 0.02, d = 0.68).
Keyan et al. (2017) [72] 54 healthy undergraduate students, Mage = 19.5 (3.0) Experimental; between-subject During a memory reconsolidation paradigm, participants either exercised or did not exercise after memory reactivation 20–25 min of incremental cycling Trauma film depicting the aftermath of a highway car crash The exercise with reactivation condition recalled more central details of the trauma film.
McNerney et al. (2017) [73] Experiment 1: 136 young adults, Mage = 19.2 (1.2)
Experiment 2: 132 young adults, Mage = 19.1 (1.2)
Experimental; between-subject Exercise occurring before and after memory encoding 2-min of sprints Paired associate learning, procedural learning, and text memory Improvements in procedural and situation model memory occurred, regardless of whether exercise occurred before or after memory encoding.
Most et al. (2017) [74] Experiment 1: 82 undergraduate psychology students (Mage = 19.9). Experiment 2: 83 undergraduate psychology students (Mage = 19.9). Experiment 3: 48 undergraduate psychology students (Mage = 19.2). Experiment 4: 75 undergraduate psychology students (Mage = 21.1). Experimental; between-subject Exercise occurring after memory encoding 5-min of step exercise Paired faces and names. Acute exercise in the early consolidation period enhanced memory.
Sng et al. (2017) [32] 88 participants, approximately 21–25 years (mean for each group) Experimental; between-subject Exercise occurred before, during and immediately after memory encoding 15-min moderate intensity brisk walking (self-selected) RAVLT Exercising before memory encoding was superior for enhancing learning (p = 0.05), 24-h memory recognition (p = 0.05) and 24-h memory attribution (p = 0.006).
Delancey et al. (2018) [40] 40 participants, approximately 20 years of age Experimental; between-subject Exercise occurring 4 hours after memory encoding High-intensity bout of exercise for 15 minutes RAVLT Those who exercise during the consolidation period have a greater 24-h follow-up memory attribution (p = 0.04).
Haynes et al. (2018) [33] 24 participants (Mage = 20.9; SD = 1.9), with 66.7% being female. Experimental; within-subject Exercise occurring before, during, and after memory encoding Self-selected brisk walking pace for 15-min RAVLT Short-term memory was greater in the visit that involved exercise prior to the memory task (F= 3.76, p = 0.01, ƞ2p = 0.79). Similar results occurred for long-term memory, but there were no exercise effects on prospective memory.
Labban et al. (2018) [75] 15 Participants; Mage = 22.7, SD = 3.1 Experimental; within-subject Exercise occurring both before and after memory encoding. 30-min of moderate intensity cycling RAVLT Exercise that occurred before encoding (vs. control) was advantageous in enhancing long-term memory, including both 60-min delayed memory (p = 0.03) and 24-h delayed recall (p = 0.03).
Siddiqui et al. (2018) [76] 20 participants (60% male). Mage = 21.1; SD = 1.0 Experimental; within-subject Exercise occurring both before and during memory encoding. 20-min treadmill walk at a self-selected brisk walking pace The Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm. Included a 15-item word list. For both short-term and long-term memory, the visit the involved exercise before the memory task resulted in the greatest memory performance (F = 11.56, p < 0.001, ƞ2p = 0.38)
Wade et al. (2018) [77] 34 female participants; Mage = 20.5 (1.2) in the exercise group and 20.8 (1.8) in the control group. Experimental; between-subject Exercise occurred before memory encoding 15-min treadmill walk at a self-selected brisk walking pace Emotional memory assessment using images from the IAPS (International Affective Picture System). There were no statistically significant group differences across any of the assessment periods (i.e., 1-day, 7-day, and 14-day follow-up assessments).
Yanes et al. (2018) [78] 40 participants, Mage = 21.0 Experimental; between-subject Exercise occurred before memory encoding 15-min treadmill walk at a self-selected brisk walking pace 6-paragraph passage for memory recall Exercise before encoding had greater scores on the short-term and long-term memory assessments, but this did not reach statistical significance (F = 1.0, p = 0.32, ƞ2p = 0.03).
Zuniga et al. (2018) [79] Experiment 1 (N = 30), Mage = 20.4 (1.8); Experiment 2 (N = 57), Mage = 20.6 (4.1) in low-fit group and Mage = 19.4 (1.6) in high-fit group. Experimental; within-subject Exercise occurred before memory encoding 3-min warm-up period on the treadmill, followed by 10-min of walking at either light or moderate-intensity. Three lists of 30 concrete English nouns from the MRC Psycholinguistic database. Both light-intensity (t = 2.79, p = 0.01) and moderate-intensity (t = 3.02, p = 0.006) recalled more words than the sedentary condition. Results were similar when comparing high-fit to low-fit individuals.

CVLT; California verbal learning test; IAPS, International Affective Picture System; RAVLT, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Task.

3.4. Article Reference

There were too many effect sizes to report in a standard forest plot (as noted in Table 3, Table 4, Table 5 and Table 6). Thus, these quantitative results are not displayed in a forest plot, but rather, are shown in Tabular format (Table 3, Table 4, Table 5 and Table 6). However, to indicate which studies contributed to the moderation analyses, Table 3, Table 4, Table 5 and Table 6 notes the reference for each study stratified by the moderator, as well as the exercise and memory temporal period.

Table 3.

Moderation results for exercise before memory encoding vs. control.

Moderator Exercise Before Memory Encoding vs. Control
Reference Number of Effect Size Contributions Effect Size (Cohen’s d) Lower CI Upper CI
Age
 Young Adult [6,31,32,33,63,64,65,68,70,71,72,73,74,75,76,77,78,79] 66 0.18 * 0.06 * 0.29 *
 Older Adults [62,63] 9 −0.53 * −0.88 * −0.18 *
Sex
 Male [65] 2 0.32 −0.41 1.05
 Female [77] 3 −0.14 −0.75 0.46
 Mixed [6,31,32,33,64,68,71,72,73,74,75,76,78,79] 42 0.28 * 0.14 * 0.43 *
 Predominately Female [62,70,79] 22 −0.06 −0.27 0.15
Race-Ethnicity
 Predominately white [33,68,76,78,79] 17 0.26 * 0.02 * 0.50 *
 Mixed [6,31,32,70,75,77,79] 31 0.10 -0.08 0.29
Memory Type
 Short-term [31,32,33,65,70,73,74,76] 23 −0.01 −0.22 0.21
 Long-term [6,31,32,33,62,63,64,71,72,73,75,76,77,78,79] 46 0.19 * 0.03 * 0.34 *
Exercise Intensity
 Light [62,63,68,79] 17 −0.20 −0.45 0.04
 Moderate [6,32,33,64,68,70,73,74,75,76,77,78,79] 45 0.14 −0.01 0.28
 Vigorous [31,65,68,71] 9 0.54 * 0.19 * 0.89 *
Exercise Duration
 Short [31,32,33,62,63,64,70,71,73,74,77,78,79] 45 0.07 −0.09 0.22
 Medium [6,65,68,70,72,75,76] 24 0.20 −0.02 0.41
 Long [70] 6 0.04 −0.37 0.45
Exercise Modality
 Walking/Running [31,32,33,63,64,68,70,73,76,77,78,79] 57 0.06 −0.07 0.19
 Cycling [6,65,72,75] 10 0.46 * 0.12 * 0.81 *

* indicates statistically significant effect size (p < 0.05).

Table 4.

Moderation results for exercise during memory encoding vs. control.

Moderator Exercise During Memory Encoding vs. Control
Reference Number of Effect Size Contributions Effect Size (Cohen’s d) Lower CI Upper CI
Sex
 Mixed [10,31,32,33,76] 16 −0.13 * −0.26 * 0.00 *
 Predominately Female [69] 2 −0.09 −0.33 0.15
Race-Ethnicity
 Predominately white [33,76] 6 0.00 −0.17 0.17
 Mixed [31,32] 8 −0.27 * −0.48 * −0.06 *
Memory Type
 Short-term [31,32,33,69,76] 6 0.02 -0.14 0.18
 Long-term [10,31,32,33,76] 12 −0.23 * −0.36 * −0.09 *
Exercise Intensity
 Light [10,69] 4 −0.15 −0.35 0.05
 Moderate [32,33,76] 10 −0.09 −0.26 0.07
 Vigorous [31] 4 −0.18 −0.49 0.14
Exercise Duration
 Short [31,32,33] 12 −0.20 * −0.35 * −0.04 *
 Medium [10,76] 4 0.00 −0.22 0.22

* indicates statistically significant effect size (p < 0.05).

Table 5.

Moderation results for exercise during early consolidation vs. control.

Moderator Exercise During Early Consolidation vs. Control
Reference Number of Effect Size Contributions Effect Size (Cohen’s d) Lower CI Upper CI
Age
 Young Adult [6,19,31,32,33,63,64,67,73,74,75] 59 0.54 * 0.35 * 0.73 *
 Older Adults [63] 3 −0.95 −1.76 −0.15
Sex
 Mixed [6,19,31,32,33,64,67,73,74,75] 55 0.60 * 0.40 * 0.80 *
Race-Ethnicity
 Predominately white [33] 4 −0.07 −0.76 0.63
 Mixed [6,31,32,75] 12 −0.14 −0.56 0.28
Memory Type
 Short-term [31,32,33,67,73,74] 35 1.05 * 0.79 * 1.30 *
 Long-term [6,19,31,32,33,63,64,73,74,75] 26 −0.14 −0.40 0.11
Exercise Intensity
 Light [63] 6 −0.59 * −1.12 * −0.06*
 Moderate [6,32,33,64,73,75] 19 −0.02 −0.31 0.27
 Vigorous [19,31,67] 33 1.09 * 0.83 * 1.35 *
Exercise Duration
 Short [31,32,33,63,64,73,74] 29 −0.13 −0.34 0.09
 Medium [6,19,75] 5 0.21 −0.32 0.74
 Long [67] 28 1.36 * 1.09 * 1.64 *
Exercise Modality
 Walking/Running [31,32,33,63,64,73,75] 26 −0.19 −0.43 0.05
 Cycling [6,19,67] 32 1.17 * 0.91 * 1.43 *

* indicates statistically significant effect size (p < 0.05).

Table 6.

Moderation results for exercise during late consolidation vs. control.

Moderator Exercise During Late Consolidation vs. Control
Reference Number of Effect Size Contributions Effect Size (Cohen’s d) Lower CI Upper CI
Memory Type
 Long-term [19,40] 3 1.20 * 0.13 * 2.27 *
Exercise Duration
 Short [40] 3 1.31 * 0.20 * 2.43 *
Exercise Modality
 Walking/Running [40] 3 1.31 * 0.20 * 2.43 *

* indicates statistically significant effect size (p < 0.05).

3.5. Quantitative Analysis

Table 3 displays the moderation results for the studies comparing exercise before memory encoding vs. control scenarios. The overall effect size for this temporal period was, d = 0.11 (95% CI: −0.01, 0.23, p = 0.08). This overall effect for this temporal period is also shown in Figure 2. There was evidence of a significant moderation effect for this temporal period (Q = 356.0, df (74), p < 0.001, I2 = 79.2). For young adults, acute exercise occurring before memory encoding enhanced episodic memory (d = 0.18, 95% CI: 0.06, 0.29). However, for older adults, acute exercise occurring before memory encoding impaired memory function (d = −0.53, 95% CI: −0.88, −0.18). Other significant moderators that demonstrated an enhancement effect of acute exercise prior to memory encoding included samples that utilized a mixed sex sample (d = 0.28, 95% CI: 0.14, 0.43), samples that were predominately white (d = 0.26, 95% CI: 0.02, 0.50), long-term memory outcomes (d = 0.19, 95% CI: 0.03, 0.34), vigorous-intensity exercise (d = 0.54, 95% CI: 0.19, 0.89), and cycling exercise (d = 0.46, 95% CI: 0.12, 0.81). The regression intercept for the Egger’s test (intercept = 1.34, p = 0.11) was not statistically significant, indicating that there was no evidence of publication bias.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Overall pooled effect size estimates (Cohen’s d with 95% CI) across the four exercise temporal periods.

Table 4 displays the moderation results for the studies comparing exercise during memory encoding vs. control. The overall effect size for this temporal period was, d = −0.12 (95% CI: −0.23, −0.01, p = 0.03). This overall effect for this temporal period is also shown in Figure 2. There was no evidence of a significant moderation effect for this temporal period (Q = 18.4, df (17), p = 0.36, I2 = 7.6). In this temporal paradigm, studies that included a mixed sex sample (d = −0.13, 95% CI: −0.26, 0.00) and a racially mixed sample (d = −0.27, 95% CI: −0.48, −0.06) demonstrated that acute exercise during memory encoding (vs. control) had a worse episodic memory function. Similar results occurred for long-term memory (d = −0.23, 95% CI: −0.36, −0.09) and short duration acute exercise (d = −0.20, 95% CI: −0.35, −0.04). The regression intercept for the Egger’s test (intercept = −0.90, p = 0.37) was not statistically significant, indicating that there was no evidence of publication bias.

Table 5 displays the moderation results for the studies comparing exercise during early consolidation vs. control scenarios. The overall effect size for this temporal period was, d = 0.47 (95% CI: 0.28, 0.67, p < 0.001). This overall effect for this temporal period is also shown in Figure 2. There was evidence of a significant moderation effect for this temporal period (Q = 387.2, df (61), p < 0.001, I2 = 84.2). When acute exercise occurred during the early consolidation period (vs. control), acute exercise enhanced episodic memory for young adults (d = 0.54, 95% CI: 0.35, 0.73), mixed sex samples (d = 0.60, 95% CI: 0.40, 0.80), vigorous-intensity exercise (d = 1.09, 95% CI: 0.83, 1.35), long duration acute exercise (d = 1.36, 95% CI: 1.09, 1.64), and cycling-based acute exercise (d = 1.17, 95% CI: 0.91, 1.43). Notably, the light-intensity, acute exercise during the early consolidation period (vs. control) impaired the episodic memory function (d = −0.59, 95% CI: −1.12, −0.06). The regression intercept for the Egger’s test (intercept = 3.54, p < 0.001) was statistically significant, indicating that there was evidence of publication bias.

Table 6 displays the moderation results for the studies comparing exercise during late consolidation vs. control scenarios. The overall effect size for this temporal period was, d = 1.05 (95% CI: 0.32, 1.78, p = 0.005). This overall effect for this temporal period is also shown in Figure 2. There was evidence of a significant moderation effect for this temporal period (Q = 21.2, df (3), p < 0.001, I2 = 85.9). When acute exercise occurred during the late consolidation period (vs. control), acute exercise enhanced episodic memory for long-term memory (d = 1.20, 95% CI: 0.13, 2.27), short-duration acute exercise (d = 1.31, 95% CI: 0.20, 2.43) and walking/running (d = 1.31, 95% CI: 0.20, 2.43). The regression intercept for the Egger’s test (intercept = 8.30, p < 0.001) was statistically significant, indicating that there was evidence of publication bias.

The summative findings across the four temporal periods are displayed in Table 7. These findings (which summarize the results from the previous tables) indicate the statistically significant positive and negative effects of acute exercise on memory across the evaluated moderators (demographic, exercise, and memory characteristics).

Table 7.

Summative findings of the moderation results across the four acute exercise and memory temporal periods.

Exercise and Memory Temporal Periods
Moderator Before vs. Control During vs. Control Early vs. Control Late vs. Control
Demographic Characteristic
Young adults + +
Older adults
Mixed-sex sample + +
 Predominately white +
Racially-Ethnically mixed sample
Exercise Characteristic
Light-intensity
Vigorous-intensity + +
Short-duration +
Long-duration +
Cycling + +
Memory
Short-term memory +
Long-term memory + +

The four temporal periods included: (1) exercise before memory encoding vs. control, (2) exercise during memory encoding vs. control, (3) exercise during early consolidation vs. control, and (4) exercise during late consolidation vs. control. +, statistically significant positive effect, −, statistically significant negative effect.

4. Discussion

Previous experimental work demonstrates that acute exercise (i.e., a structured bout of treadmill or cycling exercise) can enhance episodic memory function [15,30]. Further, as demonstrated in recent experiments [31,32,33], narrative reviews [14,35], qualitative reviews [34], and meta-analytic reviews [15,16], the timing of acute exercise appears to play a key role in subserving episodic memory function. In alignment with these previous publications, our meta-analysis demonstrates that acute exercise occurring during early memory consolidation (d = 0.47, 95% CI: 0.28, 0.67; p < 0.001) and during late memory consolidation (d = 1.05, 95% CI: 0.32, 1.78; p = 0.005) enhanced episodic memory function. These respective effect sizes represent medium and large effects. Conversely, our meta-analytic results demonstrate that acute exercise occurring during memory encoding had a negative effect on episodic memory (d = −0.12, 95% CI: −0.22, −0.02; p = 0.02). The temporal period involving acute exercise before memory encoding was not statistically significant at the group level (d = 0.11, 95% CI: −0.01, 0.23, p = 0.08), but this was driven by the statistically significant (Q = 356.0, df (74), p < 0.001, I2 = 79.2) moderation effects for various characteristics, as described below.

Of central interest of the present meta-analysis was whether study characteristics and participant attributes moderated the temporal effects of acute exercise on episodic memory function. These summative findings across the four temporal periods are displayed in Table 7. Regarding the temporal period of acute exercise occurring before memory encoding, notably, we observed an interesting age-specific effect. That is, acute exercise before memory encoding was advantageous in enhancing episodic memory function for young adults, but impaired memory function for older adults. This enhancement effect for younger adults aligns with the moderation results from the meta-analysis of Roig et al. [15]. That is, the meta-analysis of Roig et al. evaluated the effects of exercise on memory function and also evaluated various moderators (e.g., age) of this effect. We extend the meta-analysis of Roig et al. [15] by demonstrating that this age-specific effect occurs for both exercise prior to memory encoding and acute exercise during the early memory consolidation period. It is uncertain as to why older adults may have impaired episodic memory function after an acute bout of exercise. Speculatively, for older adults (vs. younger adults), acute exercise may impose greater physiological and cognitive stress, and ultimately, may impair memory function from an enhanced cognitive load effect. Similarly, it may take older adults longer to recover from an acute bout of exercise, which could have influenced these age-specific effects. Future work should conduct a side-to-side comparison of acute exercise on episodic memory among both young- and older-adults. Such work should also vary the acute exercise recovery period to determine whether this attenuates the negative memory effects observed among older adults.

Other notable findings for acute exercise before the memory-encoding period was the moderation effects for long-term memory, vigorous-intensity exercise, and cycling-based exercise. It is likely that acute exercise has a greater effect on long-term memory (vs. short-term) due to the acute exercise-induced molecular pathways that are activated to subserve long-term memory (e.g., neurotrophin production, long-term potentiation) [28,29]. We also demonstrated that vigorous-intensity, acute exercise was optimal in enhancing episodic memory, which aligns with a recent systematic review [34]. Similar to the mechanisms noted above for long-term memory, higher intensity acute exercise may more robustly modulate mechanisms (e.g., long-term potentiation) related to episodic memory function. Lastly, an interesting finding for this temporal period was that cycling-based exercise had a greater effect on enhancing episodic memory function. This was a surprising finding to us. It is expected that ambulatory exercise (vs. cycling) is a more complex movement pattern, and more complex movement patterns have greater effects on regional cerebral blood flow and cortical excitability [80,81], factors likely to subserve episodic memory [82]. However, perhaps these ambulatory activities are not more complex movement patterns than cycling, given that walking (and perhaps jogging) is part of the daily routine of the participants evaluated in these experiments. Notably, cycling may be considered a complex movement pattern, especially among novice cyclists. Future work should conduct a side-to-side comparison of ambulatory vs. cycling exercise on episodic memory function, while considering the participant’s experience with these exercise modalities. Further, particularly among novice cyclists, even a relatively light workload may be perceived as vigorous-intensity exercise. Thus, it is possible that this exercise modality-specific effect may actually be driven by an exercise intensity-specific effect. Future work is needed to help disentangle these interrelationships.

Regarding the early memory consolidation period, notable observations were that vigorous-intensity, acute exercise and long-duration acute exercise were positively associated with episodic memory function. These observations align with the mechanisms thought to enhance memory stabilization, specifically, the late-phase of long-term potentiation (discussed in detail elsewhere [28]), which is a protein synthesis-dependent process [28]. Higher-intensity and longer duration acute exercise are likely to have greater effects on the production of key proteins (e.g., brain-derived neurotrophic factor) that would subserve the late-phase of long-term potentiation [36]. In our late-phase memory consolidation temporal period analyses, our findings demonstrated that the short-duration acute exercise was advantageous in enhancing episodic memory when it occurred in this temporal period. At this point in the memory stabilization process (4-h post memory encoding), it is likely that only a short duration bout of exercise (vs. longer duration) is needed to further stabilize the memory trace.

Regarding the temporal period involving acute exercise during memory encoding, our findings demonstrated that, across various factors (e.g., mixed sex, short-duration acute exercise), acute exercise impaired episodic memory function. This finding is likely a result of reduced cognitive resources toward encoding the memory stimuli during an acute bout of exercise. Cognitive resources may be redistributed away from encoding the stimuli in order to sustain the movement itself [37,38].

When considering all four temporal periods, there was not a consistent effect of a particular moderator (e.g., young adults) across the different temporal periods. However, there were relatively few studies conducted in the early- and late-memory consolidation temporal periods; thus, we should interpret our findings accordingly. Further, we observed evidence of publication bias for both of these temporal periods, and as such, future work in these temporal periods is needed before we can definitely conclude whether or not the study and participant characteristics differentially influences episodic memory across these four temporal periods.

In addition to future work evaluating the effects of these temporal periods, as well as potential moderators, it would be a worthwhile endeavor for future work to continue to explore the mechanisms of acute exercise on episodic memory function. As discussed thoroughly elsewhere [83], exercise may help to rewire the neuronal networks involved in memory function. For example, exercise may help to coordinate neuronal firing in hippocampal circuits and enhance integration of adult-born neurons into existing hippocampal-entorhinal circuity [83]. As we recently demonstrated, exercise may enhance the functional connectivity of key memory-related brain structures (e.g., connectivity between parahippocampi and connectivity of hippocampal-orbitofrontal pathway) [84,85]. Relatedly, structural brain changes have been observed from chronic exercise [86]. From cellular and molecular perspectives, acute exercise may upregulate key neurotrophins (e.g., brain-derived neurotrophic factor) that may help facilitate neuronal communication via increasing neural activity and receptor activity [87]. Acute exercise may also be beneficial via exercise-induced increases in glucose and oxygen metabolism, as well as increases in neurotransmitter concentrations [87]. Importantly, future work should evaluate whether there are distinct mechanisms across the different exercise-memory temporal periods.

In conclusion, our meta-analytic findings demonstrate two key observations. First, the temporal period of acute exercise on episodic memory plays an important role in the exercise-memory interaction. When acute exercise occurs before memory encoding or during early and late memory consolidation, then memory enhancement effects are likely to be observed. However, when acute exercise occurs during memory encoding, memory function is likely to be impaired. Secondly, various study design and participant characteristics are likely to moderate the temporal effects of acute exercise on episodic memory function. For example, vigorous-intensity, acute exercise, and acute exercise among young adults, are likely to have greater effects when the acute bout of exercise occurs before memory encoding or during the early memory consolidation period.

Funding

This research project is supported by both Guangdong-Government Funding for Scientific Research (2016KZDXM009) and Shenzhen-Government Research Grants Programme in Basci Sciences (JCYJ20150729104249783).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  • 1.Tulving E. Elements of Episodic Memory. Oxford University Press; Oxford, UK: 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Mahr J., Csibra G. Why do we remember? The communicative function of episodic memory. Behav. Brain Sci. 2017;41:1–93. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X17000012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Frith E., Addoh O., Mann J.R., Windham B.G., Loprinzi P.D. Individual and combined associations of cognitive and mobility limitations on mortality risk in older adults. Mayo Clin. Proc. 2017;92:1494–1501. doi: 10.1016/j.mayocp.2017.06.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Dickerson B.C., Eichenbaum H. The episodic memory system: Neurocircuitry and disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:86–104. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Etnier J.L., Wideman L., Labban J.D., Piepmeier A.T., Pendleton D.M., Dvorak K.K., Becofsky K. The Effects of acute exercise on memory and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2016;38:331–340. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2015-0335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Labban J.D., Etnier J.L. Effects of acute exercise on long-term memory. Res. Q Exerc. Sport. 2011;82:712–721. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2011.10599808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Erickson K.I., Voss M.W., Prakash R.S., Basak C., Szabo A., Chaddock L., Kim J.S., Heo S., Alves H., White S.M., et al. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2011;108:3017–3022. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015950108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hillman C.H., Erickson K.I., Kramer A.F. Be smart, exercise your heart: Exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008;9:58–65. doi: 10.1038/nrn2298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Pontifex M.B., Gwizdala K.L., Parks A.C., Pfeiffer K.A., Fenn K.M. The Association between physical activity during the day and long-term memory stability. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:38148. doi: 10.1038/srep38148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Schmidt-Kassow M., Zink N., Mock J., Thiel C., Vogt L., Abel C., Kaiser J. Treadmill walking during vocabulary encoding improves verbal long-term memory. Behav. Brain Funct. 2014;10:24. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-10-24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Schmidt-Kassow M., Deusser M., Thiel C., Otterbein S., Montag C., Revter M., Banzer W., Kaiser J. Physical exercise during encoding improves vocabulary learning in young female adults: A neuroendocrinological study. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e64172. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Rendeiro C., Rhodes J.S. A new perspective of the hippocampus in the origin of exercise-brain interactions. Brain Struct. Funct. 2018;223:2527–2545. doi: 10.1007/s00429-018-1665-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.McMorris T., Sproule J., Turner A., Hale B.J. Acute, intermediate intensity exercise, and speed and accuracy in working memory tasks: A meta-analytical comparison of effects. Physiol. Behav. 2011;102:421–428. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Roig M., Thomas R., Mang C.S., Snow N.J., Ostadan F., Boyd L.A., Lundbye J.J. Time-dependent effects of cardiovascular exercise on memory. Exerc. Sport Sci Rev. 2016;44:81–88. doi: 10.1249/JES.0000000000000078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Roig M., Nordbrandt S., Geertsen S.S., Nielsen J.B. The effects of cardiovascular exercise on human memory: A review with meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013;37:1645–1666. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Chang Y.K., Labban J.D., Gapin J.I., Etnier J.L. The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Brain Res. 2012;1453:87–101. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.02.068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Heisz J.J., Clark I.B., Bonin K., Paolucci M.E. The effects of physical exercise and cognitive training on memory and neurotrophic factors. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017;29:1895–1907. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Heisz J.J., Vandermorris S., Wu J., McIntosh A.R., Ryan J.D. Age differences in the association of physical activity, sociocognitive engagement, and TV viewing on face memory. Health Psychol. 2015;34:83–88. doi: 10.1037/hea0000046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Van Dongen E.V., Kersten I.H., Wagner I.C., Morris R.G., Fernandez G. Physical exercise performed four hours after learning improves memory retention and increases hippocampal pattern similarity during retrieval. Curr. Biol. 2016;26:1722–1777. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.04.071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Wenderoth N. Motor learning triggers neuroplastic processes while awake and during sleep. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 2018;46:152–159. doi: 10.1249/JES.0000000000000154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Pontifex M.B., McGowan A.L., Chandler M.C., Gwizdala M.C., Gwizdala K.L., Parks A.C., Fenn K., Kamijo K. A primer on investigating the after effects of acute bouts of physical activity on cognition. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2019;40:1–22. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.08.015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Moore R.D., Romine M.W., O’Connor P.J., Tomporowski P.D. The influence of exercise-induced fatigue on cognitive function. J. Sports Sci. 2012;30:841–850. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2012.675083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Tomporowski P.D., Pendleton D.M. Effects of the timing of acute exercise and movement complexity on young adults’ psychomotor learning. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2018;40:240–248. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2017-0289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Coles K., Tomporowski P.D. Effects of acute exercise on executive processing, short-term and long-term memory. J. Sports Sci. 2008;26:333–344. doi: 10.1080/02640410701591417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Tomporowski P.D., Ganio M.S. Short-term effects of aerobic exercise on executive processing, memory, and emotional reactivity. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2006;4:57–72. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2006.9671784. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Tomporowski P.D., Ellis N.R., Stephens R. The immediate effects of strenuous exercise on free-recall memory. Ergonomics. 1987;30:121–129. doi: 10.1080/00140138708969682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Tomporowski P.D. Effects of acute bouts of exercise on cognition. Acta Psychol. (Amst). 2003;112:297–324. doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(02)00134-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Loprinzi P.D., Edwards M.K., Frith E. Potential avenues for exercise to activate episodic memory-related pathways: A narrative review. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2017;46:2067–2077. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Loprinzi P.D., Ponce P., Frith E. Hypothesized mechanisms through which acute exercise influences episodic memory. Physiol. Int. 2018;105:285–297. doi: 10.1556/2060.105.2018.4.28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Loprinzi P.D., Frith E., Edwards M.K., Sng E., Ashpole N. The effects of exercise on memory function among young to middle-aged adults: Systematic review and recommendations for future research. Am. J. Health Promot. 2018;32:691–704. doi: 10.1177/0890117117737409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Frith E., Sng E., Loprinzi P.D. Randomized controlled trial evaluating the temporal effects of high-intensity exercise on learning, short-term and long-term memory, and prospective memory. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2017;46:2557–2564. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Sng E., Frith E., Loprinzi P.D. Temporal effects of acute walking exercise on learning and memory function. Am. J. Health Promot. 2018;32:1518–1525. doi: 10.1177/0890117117749476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Haynes Iv J.T., Frith E., Sng E., Loprinzi P.D. Experimental effects of acute exercise on episodic memory function: Considerations for the timing of exercise. Psychol. Rep. 2018 doi: 10.1177/0033294118786688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Loprinzi P.D. Intensity-specific effects of acute exercise on human memory function: Considerations for the timing of exercise and the type of memory. Health Promot. Perspect. 2018;8:255–262. doi: 10.15171/hpp.2018.36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Loprinzi P.D. An integrated model of acute exercise on memory function. Med. Hypotheses. 2019;126:51–59. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2019.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Loprinzi P.D., Frith E. A brief primer on the mediational role of BDNF in the exercise-memory link. Clin. Physiol. Funct. Imaging. 2019;39:9–14. doi: 10.1111/cpf.12522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Dietrich A. Transient hypofrontality as a mechanism for the psychological effects of exercise. Psychiatry Res. 2006;145:79–83. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.07.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Dietrich A., Audiffren M. The reticular-activating hypofrontality (RAH) model of acute exercise. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2011;35:1305–1325. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Bramer W.M., Rethlefsen M.L., Kleijnen J., Franco O.H. Optimal database combinations for literature searches in systematic reviews: A prospective exploratory study. Syst. Rev. 2017;6:245. doi: 10.1186/s13643-017-0644-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Delancey D., Frith E., Sng E., Loprinzi P.D. Randomized controlled trial examining the long-term memory effects of acute exercise during the memory consolidation stage of memory. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 2018:1–6. doi: 10.1007/s41465-018-0106-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Loprinzi P.D., Frith E. The role of sex in memory function: Considerations and recommendations in the context of exercise. J. Clin. Med. 2018;7:132. doi: 10.3390/jcm7060132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Barha C.K., Davis J.C., Falck R.S., Nagamatsu L.S., Liu-Ambrose T. Sex differences in exercise efficacy to improve cognition: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials in older humans. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2017;46:71–85. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2017.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Garber C.E., Blissmer B., Deschenes M.R., Franklin B.A., Lamonte M.J., Lee I.M., Neiman D.C., Swain D.P. American College of Sports Medicine position stand. Quantity and quality of exercise for developing and maintaining cardiorespiratory, musculoskeletal, and neuromotor fitness in apparently healthy adults: Guidance for prescribing exercise. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2011;43:1334–1359. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e318213fefb. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Potter D., Keeling D. Effects of moderate exercise and circadian rhythms on human memory. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2005;27:117–125. doi: 10.1123/jsep.27.1.117. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Coleman M., Offen K., Markant J. Exercise similarly facilitates men and women’s selective attention task response times but differentially affects memory task performance. Front. Psychol. 2018;9:1405. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Covassin T., Weiss L., Powell J., Womack C. Effects of a maximal exercise test on neurocognitive function. Br. J. Sports Med. 2007;41:370–374. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2006.032334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Davey C.P. Mental performance after physical activity. Aust. J. Sports Mede. 1972;4:25–33. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Davey C.P. Physical exertion and mental performance. Ergonomics. 1973;16:595–599. doi: 10.1080/00140137308924550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Dietrich A., Sparling P.B. Endurance exercise selectively impairs prefrontal-dependent cognition. Brain Cogn. 2004;55:516–524. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Green D., Loprinzi P.D. Experimental effects of acute exercise on prospective memory and false memory. Psychol. Rep. 2018 doi: 10.1177/0033294118782466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Griffin E.W., Mullally S., Foley C., Warmington S.A., O’Mara S.M., Kelly A.M. Aerobic exercise improves hippocampal function and increases BDNF in the serum of young adult males. Physiol. Behav. 2011;104:934–941. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Hancock S., McNaughton L. Effects of fatigue on ability to process visual information by experienced orienteers. Percept. Mot. Skills. 1986;62:491–498. doi: 10.2466/pms.1986.62.2.491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Hopkins M.E., Davis F.C., Vantieghem M.R., Whalen P.J., Bucci D.J. Differential effects of acute and regular physical exercise on cognition and affect. Neuroscience. 2012;215:59–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.04.056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Hotting K., Schickert N., Kaiser J., Roder B., Schmidt-Kassow M. The effects of acute physical exercise on memory, peripheral BDNF, and cortisol in young adults. Neural. Plast. 2016;2016:6860573. doi: 10.1155/2016/6860573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Nielson K.A., Wulff L.L., Arentsen T.J. Muscle tension induced after learning enhances long-term narrative and visual memory in healthy older adults. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2014;109:144–150. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Segal S.K., Cotman C.W., Cahill L.F. Exercise-induced noradrenergic activation enhances memory consolidation in both normal aging and patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2012;32:1011–1018. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2012-121078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Sjoberg H. Physical fitness and mental performance during and after work. Ergonomics. 1980;23:977–985. doi: 10.1080/00140138008924807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Libkuman T.M., Nichols-Whitehead P., Griffith J., Thomas R. Source of arousal and memory for detail. Mem. Cognit. 1999;27:166–190. doi: 10.3758/BF03201222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Tomporowski P.D., Albrecht C., Pendleton D.M. Effects of isometric hand-grip muscle contraction on young adults’ free recall and recognition memory. Res. Q Exerc. Sport. 2017;88:95–100. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2016.1264567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Suwabe K., Hyodo K., Byun K., Ochi G., Yassa M.A., Soya H. Acute moderate exercise improves mnemonic discrimination in young adults. Hippocampus. 2017;27:229–234. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22695. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Winter B., Breitenstein C., Mooren F.C., Voelker K., Fobker M., Lechtermann A., Krueger K., Fromme A., Korsukewitz C., Floel A., et al. High impact running improves learning. Neurobiol. Learn Mem. 2007;87:597–609. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Stones M.J., Dawe D. Acute exercise facilitates semantically cued memory in nursing home residents. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 1993;41:531–534. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1993.tb01890.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Schramke C.J., Bauer R.M. State-dependent learning in older and younger adults. Psychol. Aging. 1997;12:255–262. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.12.2.255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Salas C.R., Minakata K., Kelemen W.L. Walking before study enhances free recall but not judgement-of-learning magnitude. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2011;23:507–513. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2011.532207. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Nanda B., Balde J., Manjunatha S. The acute effects of a single bout of moderate-intensity aerobic exercise on cognitive functions in healthy adult males. J. Clin. Diagn. Res. 2013;7:1883–1885. doi: 10.7860/JCDR/2013/5855.3341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Weinberg L., Hasni A., Shinohara M., Duarte A. A single bout of resistance exercise can enhance episodic memory performance. Acta Psychol. (Amst). 2014;153:13–19. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.06.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Basso J.C., Shang A., Elman M., Karmouta R., Suzuki W.A. Acute exercise improves prefrontal cortex but not hippocampal function in healthy adults. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2015;21:791–801. doi: 10.1017/S135561771500106X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Loprinzi P.D., Kane C.J. Exercise and cognitive function: A randomized controlled trial examining acute exercise and free-living physical activity and sedentary effects. Mayo Clin. Proc. 2015;90:450–460. doi: 10.1016/j.mayocp.2014.12.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Bantoft C., Summers M.J., Tranent P.J., Palmer M.A., Cooley P.D., Pedersen S.J. Effect of standing or walking at a workstation on cognitive function: A randomized counterbalanced trial. Hum. Factors. 2016;58:140–149. doi: 10.1177/0018720815605446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Crush E.A., Loprinzi P.D. Dose-response effects of exercise duration and recovery on cognitive functioning. Percept. Mot. Skills. 2017;124:1164–1193. doi: 10.1177/0031512517726920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Keyan D., Bryant R.A. Brief exercise enhances intrusive memories of traumatic stimuli. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2017;141:9–13. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2017.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Keyan D., Bryant R.A. Acute physical exercise in humans enhances reconsolidation of emotional memories. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2017;86:144–151. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.09.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.McNerney M.W., Radvansky G.A. Mind racing: The influence of exercise on long-term memory consolidation. Memory. 2015;23:1140–1151. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2014.962545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Most S.B., Kennedy B.L., Petras E.A. Evidence for improved memory from 5 minutes of immediate, post-encoding exercise among women. Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic. 2017;2:33. doi: 10.1186/s41235-017-0068-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Labban J.D., Etnier J.L. The effect of acute exercise on encoding and consolidation of long-term memory. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2018;40:336–342. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2018-0072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Siddiqui A., Loprinzi P.D. Experimental investigation of the time course effects of acute exercise on false episodic memory. J. Clin. Med. 2018;7:157. doi: 10.3390/jcm7070157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Wade B., Loprinzi P.D. The experimental effects of acute exercise on long-term emotional memory. J. Clin. Med. 2018;7:486. doi: 10.3390/jcm7120486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Yanes D., Loprinzi P.D. Experimental effects of acute exercise on iconic memory, short-term episodic, and long-term episodic memory. J. Clin. Med. 2018;7:146. doi: 10.3390/jcm7060146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Zuniga K.E., Mueller M., Santana A.R., Kelemen W.L. Acute aerobic exercise improves memory across intensity and fitness levels. Memory. 2018;27:1–9. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2018.1546875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Winstein C.J., Grafton S.T., Pohl P.S. Motor task difficulty and brain activity: Investigation of goal-directed reciprocal aiming using positron emission tomography. J. Neurophysiol. 1997;77:1581–1594. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.3.1581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Carey J.R., Bhatt E., Nagpal A. Neuroplasticity promoted by task complexity. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 2005;33:24–31. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Gur R.C., Jaggi J.L., Ragland J.D., Resnick S.M., Shtasel D., Muenz L., Gur E.R. Effects of memory processing on regional brain activation: Cerebral blood flow in normal subjects. Int. J. Neurosci. 1993;72:31–44. doi: 10.3109/00207459308991621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Voss M.W., Soto C., Yoo S., Sodoma M., Vivar C., van Praag H. Exercise and hippocampal memory systems. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2019;23:318–333. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.01.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Ikuta T., Loprinzi P.D. Association of cardiorespiratory fitness on interhemispheric hippocampal and parahippocampal functional connectivity. Eur. J. Neur. 2019 doi: 10.1111/ejn.14366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Ikuta T., Frith E., Ponce P., Loprinzi P.D. Association of physical activity on the functional connectivity of the hippocampal-orbitofrontal pathway. Phys. Sportsmed. 2018 doi: 10.1080/00913847.2018.1549461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Zheng G., Ye B., Zheng Y., Xiong Z., Xia R., Qiu P., Tao J., Chen L. The effects of exercise on the structure of cognitive related brain regions: A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging data. Int. J. Neurosci. 2018;129:406–415. doi: 10.1080/00207454.2018.1508135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.El-Sayes J., Harasym D., Turco C.V., Locke M.B., Nelson A.J. Exercise-induced neuroplasticity: A mechanistic model and prospects for promoting plasticity. Neuroscientist. 2018;25:1–21. doi: 10.1177/1073858418771538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Brain Sciences are provided here courtesy of Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute (MDPI)

RESOURCES