Version Changes
Revised. Amendments from Version 4
- We have modified Figure 2 (flowchart) by including standard percentages. - Different datasets mentioned in Table S4 of the Supplementary Material have been named in order to avoid ambiguities. - English wording has been improved and we have restructured the Discussion section.
Abstract
Background: Precision medicine is the Holy Grail of interventions that are tailored to a patient’s individual characteristics. However, conventional clinical trials are designed to find differences in averages, and interpreting these differences depends on untestable assumptions. Although only an ideal, a constant effect of treatment would facilitate individual management. A direct consequence of a constant effect is that the variance of the outcome measure would be the same in the treated and control arms. We reviewed the literature to explore the similarity of these variances as a foundation for examining whether and how often precision medicine is definitively required.
Methods: We reviewed parallel clinical trials with numerical primary endpoints published in 2004, 2007, 2010 and 2013. We collected the baseline and final standard deviations of the main outcome measure. We assessed homoscedasticity by comparing the variance of the primary endpoint between arms through the outcome variance ratio (treated to control group).
Results: The review provided 208 articles with enough information to conduct the analysis. One out of five studies (n = 40, 19.2%) had statistically different variances between groups, implying a non-constant-effect. The adjusted point estimate of the mean outcome variance ratio (treated to control group) is 0.89 (95% CI 0.81 to 0.97).
Conclusions: The mean variance ratio is significantly lower than 1 and the lower variance was found more often in the intervention group than in the control group, suggesting it is more usual for treated patients to be stable. This observed reduction in variance might also imply that there could be a subgroup of less ill patients who derive no benefit from treatment. This would require further study as to whether the treatment effect outweighs the side effects as well as the economic costs. We have shown that there are ways to analyze the apparently unobservable constant effect.
Keywords: Constant Effect, Precision medicine, Homoscedasticity, Clinical Trial, Variability, Standard deviation, Review
Introduction
The goal of precision medicine is to develop prevention and treatment strategies that take into account individual characteristics. As Collins and Varmus stated, “The prospect of applying this concept broadly has been dramatically improved by recent developments in large-scale biologic databases (such as the human genome sequence), powerful methods for characterizing patients (such as proteomics, metabolomics, genomics, diverse cellular assays, and mobile health technology), and computational tools for analyzing large sets of data.” With this words in mind, US President Obama gave his strong endorsement in launching the 2015 Precision Medicine initiative to capitalize on these developments 1, 2. Here, we aim to quantify the proportion of interventions that may benefit from this idea.
The fundamental problem of causal inference is that for each patient in a parallel group trial, we can know the outcome for only one of the interventions. That is, we observe their responses either to the new treatment or to the control, but not both. By experimentally controlling unknown confounders through randomization, a clinical trial may estimate the averaged causal effect. In order to translate this population estimate into effects for individual patients, additional assumptions are needed. The simplest and strongest one is that the effect is constant. Panels A and B in Figure 1 3– 12 represent two scenarios with a common effect in all patients, although it is null in the first case. Following Holland 13, this assumption has the advantage of making the average causal effect relevant to each patient. All other scenarios ( Figure 1, Panels C to F) require additional parameters to fully specify the treatment effect.
As an example, the 10 clinical trials published by the journal Trials in October 2017 ( Supplementary File 1: Table S1) were designed without explicitly allowing for an effect that was not constant within the study population. Furthermore, all their analyses intended to estimate just an average effect with no indication of any possible interaction with baseline variables ( Figure 1, Panels C and E), nor did they discuss any random variability for the treatment effect ( Figure 1, Panels D and F). Therefore, without further specifications, it seems that they were either hoping for the treatment effect to be the same for all patients or assuming that it was not useful to try and investigate this. As a contrary example, Kim et al. 14 designed their trial to test an intervention for: 1) non-inferiority in the overall population and 2) superiority in the subgroup of patients with high epidermal growth factor receptor expression.
The variability of a clinical trial outcome measure is relevant because it conveys important information about whether or not precision medicine is achievable. Does variance come only from unpredictable sources of patient variability? Or should it also be attributed to different treatment effects that require more precise prescription rules 15– 17? One observable consequence of a constant effect is that the treatment will not affect variability, and therefore the outcome variances in both arms should be equal (“homoscedasticity”).
Below, we will elucidate whether the comparison of observed variances may shed some light on the non-observable individual treatment effect.
Our objectives are, first, to compare the variability of the main outcome between arms in parallel randomized controlled trials published in medical journals; and, second, to provide a rough estimate of the proportion of studies that could potentially benefit from precision medicine. To assess the consistency of results, we also explore the evolution of the variability of the treated arm over time (from baseline to the end of the study).
Methods
Population
Our target population was parallel, randomized controlled trials with numerical primary endpoint. The trials should provide enough information to assess two homoscedasticity assumptions in the primary endpoint: between arms at trial end; and baseline to outcome over time in the treated arm. Therefore, baseline and final SDs for the main outcome were necessary or, lacking those, we required at least one measure that would allow us to calculate them (variances, standard errors or mean confidence intervals).
Data collection
Using the Medline database, we selected articles on parallel clinical trials from the years 2004, 2007, 2010 and 2013 with the following criteria: “ AB (clinical trial* AND random*) AND AB (change OR evolution OR (difference AND baseline))” [The word “difference” was paired with “baseline” because the initial purpose of the data collection (although it was subsequently modified) was to estimate the correlation between baseline and final measurements]. The rationale behind choosing these years was to have a global view of the behavior of the studies over a whole decade. For the years 2004 and 2007, we selected all papers that met the inclusion criteria. However, we retrieved a greater number of articles from our search for the years 2010 and 2013 (478 and 653, respectively); therefore, we chose a random sample of 300 papers (Section II in Supplementary File 1).
Data were collected by two researchers (NM, MkV) in two phases: 2004/2007 and 2010/2013. Later, two statisticians (JC, MtV) verified the data and made them accessible to readers through a Shiny application and through the Figshare repository 18.
Variables
Collected variables were: baseline and outcome SDs; experimental and control interventions; sample size in each group; medical field according to Web of Science (WOS) classification; main endpoint; indication; type of disease (chronic versus acute); endpoint type (measured versus scored); intervention type (pharmacological versus non-pharmacological); improvement direction (positive versus negative); and whether or not the main effect was statistically significant.
For studies that reported more than one numerical endpoint and failed to clarify which endpoint was the primary endpoint, the latter was determined using the following hierarchical criteria: (1) objective or hypothesis; (2) sample size determination; (3) main statistical method; (4) first numerical variable reported in results.
In the same way, the choice of the "experimental" arm was determined depending on its role in the following sections of the article: (1) objective or hypothesis; (2) sample size determination; (3) rationale in the introduction; (4) first comparison reported in results (in the case of more than two arms).
Statistical analysis
We assessed homoscedasticity between treatments and over time. For the former, our main analysis compared the outcome variability between treated (T) and control (C) arms at the end of the trial. For the latter, we compared the variability between outcome (O) and its baseline (B) value for the treated arm.
Three different methods were used to compare the variances: 1) a random-effects model; 2) a heuristic procedure based on the heterogeneity obtained from the previous random-effects model; and 3) a classical test for equality of variances.
To distinguish between the random sampling variability and heterogeneity, we fitted a random-effects model. The response was the logarithm of the outcome variance ratio at the end of the trial. The covariates were the study as a random effect, while the logarithm of the variance ratio at baseline served as a fixed effect 19.
The main fitted model for between-arm comparison was:
where V ij represents the variances of the outcome in each arm (V iT, V iC) at the end of the study (V OT, V OC) and at baseline (V BT, V BC). The parameter μ is the logarithm of the average variance ratio across all the studies; s i represents the heterogeneity of the between-study effect associated with study i and having variance τ 2; β is the coefficient for the linear association with the baseline variance ratio; and e i represents the intra-study random errors with variance vi2 .
The parameter μ represents a measure of the imbalance between the variances at the end of the study, which we call heteroscedasticity.
The estimated value of τ 2 provides a measure of heterogeneity, that is, to what extent the value of μ is applicable to all studies. The larger τ 2 is, the lesser the homogeneity.
The percentage of the response variance explained by the differences among studies in respect to the overall variance is measured by the I 2 statistic 20. That is:
v 2 is the mean of the error variances .
An analogous model was employed to assess the homoscedasticity over time. As there is only one available measure for each study, it is not possible to differentiate both sources of variability: (i) within-study or random variability; and (ii) heterogeneity. To isolate the second, the first was theoretically estimated using either the delta method, in the case of comparison between arms, or some approximation, in the case of comparison over time (see details in Sections VI and VII of Supplementary File 1). Thus, the within-study variance was estimated using the following formulas:
Funnel plots centered at zero are reported in order to help investigate asymmetries. They represent the variance ratios as a function of their standard errors. The first and main analysis considers the studies outside the triangle delimited by ± 2 times the standard error to be those that have statistically significant differences between variances.
The second analysis is heuristic. In order to obtain a reference value for τ 2 in the absence of treatment effect, we first modeled the baseline variance ratio as a response that is expected to have heterogeneity equal to 0 due to randomization – provided no methodological impurities are present (e.g., considering the outcomes obtained 1 month after the start of treatment to be the baseline values). This reference model allows us to know the proportion of studies in the previous models that could increase heterogeneity over levels that are incompatible with a constant effect situation. (Section III in Supplementary File 1). Specifically, studies with larger discrepancies in variances were removed one by one until the estimated value of τ was as close as possible to that of the reference model. These deleted studies were considered to be those that had significantly different variances, perhaps because the experimental treatment either increased or decreased the variance. From now on, the complete dataset and the resulting dataset after removing the abovementioned studies will be called CDB (complete dataset) and RDB (reduced dataset) for between-arm comparison and CDO (Complete) and RDO (Reduced) for over-time comparison.
Thirdly, as an additional sensitivity analysis, we also assessed homoscedasticity in each single study by using tests for comparing variances: (a) between outcomes in both arms with an F-test for independent samples; and (b) between baseline and outcome in the treated arm with a test for paired samples 21 when the variance of the paired difference was available. All tests were two-sided (α=5%).
Several subgroup analyses were carried out according to the statistical significance of the main treatment effect and to the different types of outcomes and interventions.
All analyses were performed with the R statistical package version 3.2.5. (The R code for the main analysis is available from https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1239539 22)
Results
Population
A total of 1214 articles were retrieved from the search. Of those papers, 542 (44.6%) belong to the target population and 208 (17.1%) contained enough information to enable us to conduct the analysis ( Figure 2).
The majority of the selected studies were non-pharmacological (122, 58.6%); referred to chronic conditions (101, 57.4%); had a continuous outcome measured with units (132, 63.8%) instead of a constructed scale; had an outcome that was measured (125, 60.1%) rather than assessed; and had lower values of the outcome indicating positive evolution (141, 67.8%). Regarding the primary objective of each trial, the authors found statistically significant differences between arms (all of which favored the treated group) in 83 (39.9%) studies. Following the Web of Science criteria, 203 articles (97.6%) belonged to at least one medical field. The main areas of study were: General & Internal Medicine (n=31, 14.9%), Nutrition & Dietetics (21, 10.1%), Endocrinology & Metabolism (19, 9.1%), and Cardiovascular System & Cardiology (16, 7.7%).
Homoscedasticity
In descriptive terms, the average of the outcome variance ratio is 0.94, reflecting lower variability in the treated arm. At the end of the study, 113/208 (54%, 95% CI, 47 to 61%) papers showed less variability in the treated arms ( Supplementary File 1: Figure S1 and Figure S2). Among the treated arms, 111/208 (53%, 95% CI, 46 to 60%) had less or equal variability at the end of follow-up than at the beginning ( Supplementary File 1 : Figure S3 and Figure S4).
Based on the random-effects model ( Supplementary File 1: Table S4, model 3 with CDB) the adjusted point estimate of the mean outcome variance ratio for comparison between arms (Treated to Control group) is 0.89 (95% CI 0.81 to 0.97). This indicates that treatments tend to reduce the variability of the patient's response by about 11% on average. As for the comparison over time ( Supplementary File 1 : Table S4, Model 6 with CDO), the average variability at the end of the studies is 14% lower than that at the beginning. Figure 3 shows the funnel plots derived from the random-effects models. The triangles delimit the 95% confidence regions of random variability. In the between-arm comparison, the studies (represented by the circles) to the right of the triangle have variances that are significantly larger in the treatment arm than in the control arm, while those on the left are significantly larger in the control arm. As for the over-time comparison, the studies to the right have a significantly higher variance at the end of the study in the treated group, while those on the left are significantly larger at the beginning of the study. Table 1 ( random-effects method) shows the frequencies and percentages of the studies according to the classification illustrated in these funnel plots.
Table 1. Variance comparison.
Comparing
variances |
N | Method | After treatment, variability is… | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Increased
n (%) |
Decreased
n (%) |
Not changed
n (%) |
|||
Outcome between
treatment arms |
208 | Random-effects
model |
14(6.7%) | 26 (12.5%) | 168(80.8%) |
Heuristic | 11 (5.3%) | 19 (9.1%) | 178 (85.6%) | ||
F-test | 15 (7.2%) | 26 (12.5%) | 167 (80.3%) | ||
Outcome versus
baseline in treated arm |
95 ¥ | Random-effects
model |
16 (16.8%) | 22(23.2%) | 57(60.0%) |
Heuristic | 13 (13.7%) | 19 (20.0%) | 63 (66.3%) | ||
Paired test | 16 (16.8%) | 22 (23.2%) | 57 (60.0%) |
The second heuristic analysis was motivated by the fact that the estimated baseline heterogeneity (τ 2) was 0.31 ( Supplementary File 1 : Table S4, Model 1 with CDB), which is a very high value that could be explained by methodological flaws similar to those presented by Carlisle 23. Fortunately, the exclusion of the four most extreme papers reduced it to 0.07 ( Supplementary File 1 : Table S4, Model 1 with RDB); one of these was the study by Hsieh et al. 24, whose “baseline” values were obtained 1 month after the treatment started. When we modeled the outcome instead of the baseline variances as the response, estimated heterogeneity (τ^=0.55) was almost doubled ( Supplementary File 1 : Table S4, Model 6 with CDB). We found 30 studies that compromised homoscedasticity: 11 (5.3%) with higher variance in the treated arm and 19 (9.1%), with lower variance (see heuristic method in Table 1). Based on the classical variance comparison tests (sensitivity analysis), these figures were slightly higher: 41 studies (19.7%) had statistically significant differences between outcome variances; 15 (7.2%) favored greater variance in the treated arm; and 26 (12.5%) were in the opposite direction. Larger proportions were obtained from the comparisons over time of 95 treated arms: 16.8% had significantly greater variability at the end of the study and 23.2% at the beginning. Table 1 also summarizes those numbers for the F-test and paired Test.
Subgroup analyses suggest that significant interventions had an effect on reducing variability ( Supplementary File 1 : Figures S5–S7), a fact which has already been observed in other studies 25, 26. Even more importantly, lower variances in the treated arm occur only in outcomes for which a positive response is defined as a decrease from baseline. This is in line with other works that have found a positive correlation between the effect size and its heteroscedasticity 27, 28. The fact is that it is difficult to find heteroscedasticity when there is no overall treatment effect. The remaining subgroup analyses did not raise concerns (Section V in Supplementary File 1).
Discussion
Main findings
We aimed to show that comparing variances provides evidence about whether or not precision medicine is a sensible choice. When both arms have equal variances, then a simple and believable interpretation is that the treatment effect is constant, which, if correct, would render futile any search for predictors of differential response. This means that the average treatment effect can be seen as an individual treatment effect (not directly observable), which supports the use of a unique clinical guideline for all patients within the eligibility criteria, thus in turn also supporting the use of parallel controlled trials to guide decision-making in these circumstances. Otherwise, heteroscedasticity may suggest a need to specify further the eligibility criteria or search for an additive scale 25, 29. Because interaction analyses cannot include unknown variables, there might be value in repeating trials once any new potential interaction variable emerges (e.g., a new biomarker) as a candidate for a new subgroup analysis. We have described how homoscedasticity can be assessed when reporting trials with numerical outcomes, regardless of whether every potential effect modifier is known.
We have provided a rough estimate of the proportion of interventions with different variability that might benefit from more precise medicine: Considering the most extreme result from Table 1 for comparison between arms, 1 out of 14 interventions (7.2%) had greater variance in the treated arm while 1 out of 8 interventions (12.5%) had lower variance. That is, we have found evidence of effect variation in only 1 out of 5 trials (40/208), suggesting a limited role for tailored interventions. These might be pursued by either a finer selection criteria (common effect within specific subgroups), or with n-of-1 trials (no subgroups of patients with a common effect).
The sensitivity analysis of the change over time in the treated arm agreed with the findings in the comparison between arms, although this comparison is not protected by randomization. For example, the existence of eligibility criteria at baseline may have limited the initial variance (a hypertension trial might recruit patients with baseline SBP between 140 and 159 mm Hg), leading to the variance increasing naturally over time.
Regarding the subgroup analyses, we found that variability seems to decrease for treatments that perform significantly better than the reference; otherwise, it remains similar. Therefore, the treatment seems to be doing what medicine should do: having larger effects in the most ill patients. Two considerations may be highlighted here: (1) as the outcome range becomes reduced, we may interpret that, following the intervention, this population is under additional control; but also, (2) as subjects are responding differently to treatment, this opens the way for not treating some (e.g., those subjects who are not very ill and thus lack the scope to respond very much), which subsequently incurs savings in side effects and costs.
This reduced variability could also be due to methodological reasons. One is that some measurements may have a “ceiling” or “floor” effect (e.g., in the extreme case, if a treatment heals someone, no further improvement is possible). In fact, according to the subgroup analysis of the studies with outcomes that indicate the degree of disease (high values imply greater severity; e.g., pain), a greater variance (25%) is obtained in the treated arm (see Figure S5). However, in the studies with outcomes that measure the degree of healthiness (high values imply better condition; e.g., mobility), the average variances match between arms, and this does not suggest a ceiling effect. As mentioned above, another reason might be that the treatment effect is not additive on the scale used for analysis, suggesting that it would be suitable to explore other metrics and transformations. For example, if the treatment acts proportionally rather than linearly, the logarithm of the outcome would be a better scale.
Limitations
There are three reasons why these findings do not invalidate precision medicine in all settings. First, there are studies where the variability in the response is glaringly different, indicating the presence of a non-constant effect. Second, the outcomes of some type of interventions such as surgeries, for example, are greatly influenced by the skills and training of those administering the intervention; and these situations could have some effect on increasing variability. And, third, this study focuses on numerical endpoints; thus, time-to-event or categorical outcomes are out of scope.
The results rely on published articles, which raises some relevant issues. First, some of our analyses are based on Normality assumptions that are unverifiable without access to raw data. Second, a high number of manuscripts (61.6%, Figure 2) act contrary to CONSORT 30 advice in that they do not report variability. Thus, the included studies may not be representative. Third, trials are usually powered to test constant effects and thus the presence of greater variability would lead to an underpowered design; that is, if the control group variance is used to plan the trial, increased treatment group variance would reduce power (perhaps leading to non-publication). Fourth, the heterogeneity observed in the random-effects model may be the result of methodological inaccuracies 23 arising from typographical errors in data translation, inadequate follow-up, insufficient reporting, or even data fabrication. On the other hand, this heterogeneity could also be the result of relevant undetected factors interacting with the treatment, which would indeed justify the suitability of precision medicine. A fifth limitation is that many clinical trials are not completely randomized. For example, multicenter trials often use a permuted blocks method. This means that if variances are calculated as if the trial were completely randomized (which is standard practice), the standard simple theory covering the random variation of variances from arm to arm is at best approximately true 25
The main limitation of our study arises from the fact that, although a constant effect always implies homoscedasticity on the chosen scale, the reverse is not true; i.e., homoscedasticity does not necessarily imply a constant effect. For example, the highly specific and non-parsimonious situation reflected in Figure 4 indicates homoscedasticity but without a constant effect. Nevertheless, a constant effect is the simplest explanation for homoscedasticity (Section VIII of Supplementary File 1: Conditions for homoscedasticity to hold without a constant effect under an additive model).
Conclusion
In summary, for most trials, the variability of the response to treatment scarcely changes or even decreases. Thus, if we take into account the limitation previously explained in Figure 4, this suggests that the scope of precision medicine may be less than what is commonly assumed. Evidence-Based Medicine (EBM) operates under the paradigm of a constant effect assumption, by which we learn from previous patients in order to develop practical clinical guidelines for future treatments. Here, we have provided empirical insights to postulate that such a premise is reasonable in most published parallel randomized controlled trials. However, even where one common effect applies to all patients fulfilling the eligibility criteria, this does not imply that the same decision is optimal for all patients. More specifically, this is because different patients and stakeholders may vary in their weighting not only of efficacy outcomes, but also of the harm and cost of the interventions – thus bridging the gap between common evidence and personalized decisions.
Our results uphold the assertion by Horwitz et al. that there is a “need to measure a greater range of features to determine [...] the response to treatment” 31. One of these features is an old friend of statisticians, the variance. Looking only at averages can cause us to miss out on important information.
Data availability
Data is available through two sources:
A shiny app that allows the user to interact with the data without downloading it: http://shiny-eio.upc.edu/pubs/F1000_precision_medicine/
The Figshare repository: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.5552656 18
In both sources, the data can be downloaded under a Creative Commons License v. 4.0.
The code for the main analysis is available at the following link: https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1239539 22
Acknowledgments
We thank to Dr. Joan Bigorra ( Barcelona Institute for Global Health, ISGlobal) for his contribution to improving the readability of the manuscript.
Funding Statement
Partially supported by Methods in Research on Research (MiRoR, Marie Skłodowska-Curie No. 676207); MTM2015-64465-C2-1-R (MINECO/FEDER); and 2014 SGR 464.
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
[version 5; peer review: 2 approved
Supplementary material
Supplementary File 1: The supplementary material contains the following sections- Section I: Constant effect assumption in sample size rationale
- Section II: Bibliographic review
- Section III: Descriptive measures
- Section IV: Random-effects models
- Section V: Subgroup analyses
- Section VI: Standard error of log(V OT/V OC) in independent samples
- Section VII: Standard error of log(V OT/V BT) in paired samples
- Section VIII: Conditions for homoscedasticity to hold without a constant effect under an additive model
References
- 1. Collins FS, Varmus H: A new initiative on precision medicine. N Engl J Med. 2015;372(9):793–5. 10.1056/NEJMp1500523 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Kohane IS: HEALTH CARE POLICY. Ten things we have to do to achieve precision medicine. Science. 2015;349(6243):37–8. 10.1126/science.aab1328 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Durán-Cantolla J, Aizpuru F, Montserrat JM, et al. : Continuous positive airway pressure as treatment for systemic hypertension in people with obstructive sleep apnoea: randomised controlled trial. BMJ. 2010;341:c5991. 10.1136/bmj.c5991 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Kojima Y, Kaga H, Hayashi S, et al. : Comparison between sitagliptin and nateglinide on postprandial lipid levels: The STANDARD study. World J Diabetes. 2013;4(1):8–13. 10.4239/wjd.v4.i1.8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. International conference on harmonisation: Statistical principles for clinical trials ICH-E9.1998. Accessed September 14 2017. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- 6. Shamseer L, Sampson M, Bukutu C, et al. : CONSORT extension for reporting N-of-1 trials (CENT) 2015: Explanation and elaboration. BMJ. 2015;350:h1793. 10.1136/bmj.h1793 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Araujo A, Julious S, Senn S: Understanding Variation in Sets of N-of-1 Trials. PLoS One. 2016;11(12):e0167167. 10.1371/journal.pone.0167167 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Senn S: Individual response to treatment: is it a valid assumption? BMJ. 2004;329(7472):966–68. 10.1136/bmj.329.7472.966 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Senn S: Mastering variation: variance components and personalised medicine. Stat Med. 2016;35(7):966–77. 10.1002/sim.6739 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Wang R, Lagakos SW, Ware JH, et al. : Statistics in medicine--reporting of subgroup analyses in clinical trials. N Engl J Med. 2007;357(21):2189–94. 10.1056/NEJMsr077003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Senn S, Richardson W: The first t-test. Stat Med. 1994;13(8):785–803. 10.1002/sim.4780130802 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Kim SH, Schneider SM, Bevans M, et al. : PTSD symptom reduction with mindfulness-based stretching and deep breathing exercise: randomized controlled clinical trial of efficacy. J Clin Endocr Metab. 2013;98(7):2984–92. 10.1210/jc.2012-3742 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Holland P: Statistics and Causal Inference. J Am Stat Assoc. 1986;81(396):945–60. 10.2307/2289064 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Schork NJ: Personalized medicine: Time for one-person trials. Nature. 2015;520(7549):609–11. 10.1038/520609a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Willis JC, Lord GM: Immune biomarkers: the promises and pitfalls of personalized medicine. Nat Rev Immunol. 2015;15(5):323–29. 10.1038/nri3820 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Wallach JD, Sullivan PG, Trepanowski JF, et al. : Evaluation of Evidence of Statistical Support and Corroboration of Subgroup Claims in Randomized Clinical Trials. JAMA Intern Med. 2017;177(4):554–60. 10.1001/jamainternmed.2016.9125 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Kim ES, Hirsch V, Mok T, et al. : Gefitinib versus docetaxel in previously treated non-small-cell lung cancer (INTEREST): a randomised phase III trial. Lancet. 2008;372(9652):1809–1818. 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61758-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Cortés J: Variability measures for clinical trials at baseline and at the end of study.[Data set].2018. 10.6084/m9.figshare.5552656.v3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Bartlett MS, Kendall DG: The statistical analysis of variance-heterogeneity and the logarithmic transformation. J R Stat Soc. 1946;8(1):128–38. 10.2307/2983618 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, et al. : Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ. 2003;327(7414):557–560. 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Sachs L: Applied Statistics: A Handbook of Techniques.2nd ed. New York: Springer-Verlag,1984. 10.1007/978-1-4612-5246-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Cortés J: R code for analysis of homoscedasticity in clinical trials. Zenodo. 2017. 10.5281/zenodo.1239539 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Carlisle JB: Data fabrication and other reasons for non-random sampling in 5087 randomised, controlled trials in anaesthetic and general medical journals. Anaesthesia. 2017;72(8):944–952. 10.1111/anae.13938 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Hsieh LL, Kuo CH, Yen MF, et al. : A randomized controlled clinical trial for low back pain treated by acupressure and physical therapy. Prev Med. 2004;39(1):168–76. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.01.036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Senn S: Controversies concerning randomization and additivity in clinical trials. Stat Med. 2004;23(24):3729–53. 10.1002/sim.2074 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Jamieson J: Measurement of change and the law of initial values: A computer simulation study. Educ Psychol Meas. 1995;55(1):38–46. 10.1177/0013164495055001004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Senn S: Trying to be precise about vagueness. Stat Med. 2007;26(7):1417–30. 10.1002/sim.2639 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Greenlaw N: Constructing appropriate models for meta-analyses. University of Glasgow,2010. Accessed September 14, 2017. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- 29. Rothman KJ, Greenland S, Walker AM: Concepts of interaction. Am J Epidemiol. 1980;112(4):467–70. 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a113015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Schulz KF, Altman DG, Moher D, et al. : CONSORT 2010 statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. BMJ. 2010;340:c332. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.02.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Horwitz RI, Cullen MR, Abell J, et al. : Medicine. (De)personalized medicine. Science. 2013;339(6124):1155–6. 10.1126/science.1234106 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]