Purpose
The Association of Primate Veterinarians (APV) recognizes that several forms of restraint, including physical and chemical, are necessary for the safe handling of nonhuman primates (NHPs). The following guidelines aim to provide information to researchers, animal caregivers, veterinarians, and institutional animal care and use committees (IACUCs) on safe and humane restraint for primates.
Background
The purpose of physical or chemical restraint is to allow animal-related tasks (e.g. husbandry, veterinary care, experimental procedures, manipulation for data collection, etc.) to be conducted humanely and efficiently with both animal and human safety in mind, and while minimizing animal stress or distress.
Procedural Guidance
General Considerations.
Whenever possible, nonhuman primates should be trained, using positive reinforcement principles, to cooperate with routine husbandry tasks, such as cage changing, as well as to participate voluntarily with routine experimental practices. Nonhuman primates have significant cognitive abilities and training for cooperation in husbandry and research tasks. This can significantly reduce human and animal stress and the likelihood for injury or accidents to occur. When it is not possible to train animals to cooperate because of time or other constraints, physical and chemical methods of restraint may need to be considered. The following are considerations when determining the most appropriate restraint equipment or method to use:
Time required for animal habituation and training to restraint method
Potential impact on the research;
Frequency of handling required;
Type of equipment available;
Duration of restraint;
Species-specific behavioral and postural differences; and
Other considerations (e.g. pregnancy, animals with pre-existing conditions, animals with jackets or implants, etc.).
Positive Reinforcement Training.
Training animals to cooperate without the need for restraint devices or drugs is strongly encouraged and convenience alone must not dictate the method used for restraint. Whenever possible, cooperation by non-stressed, conscious animals yields the most relevant data for studies. Ideally, the veterinarian and researcher should work with a behaviorist and the training plan should have reasonable, sequential steps and attainable goals. The plan also should take into consideration the unique personality traits of each animal and their capacity to learn and remember tasks to condition them to be a willing worker. The plan should be based on receiving rewards (e.g. treats, juice, and other rewards) for the tasks learned and reproduced, and receiving no rewards for the lack of progress (i.e. positive reinforcement). Clicker training is designed to obtain desired behaviors by giving food and treats immediately after the sound of a clicker and has been used with very good success in primates. (Wolf, 2012).
Habituation to Restraint.
It is essential that NHPs be habituated to minimize stress when they must be restrained awake in a device for a prolonged period of time. Examples of situations that might require habituation include placement in a restraining device for >30 min on a regular basis in repeated sessions e.g., blood sampling, head restraint, reach/grasp motor tasks and visual tasks. The duration for which a primate must be restrained is dependent upon the duration of the procedure and individual behavioral traits and cognitive capacity of each animal. For example, aggressive, dominant animals may require different habituation strategies compared to more submissive animals. Appropriate habituation to restraint is achieved when the animal exhibits normal physiologic and behavioral characteristics and when they enter the restraint device voluntarily (NRC, 2002). In lieu of habituation some animals may be conditioned to using the restraint device with use of the positive reinforcement training.
Physical Restraint.
Methods of physical restraint include hand restraint, pole and collar, transfer box, restraint chair, jacket, and portable squeeze unit. Animals hand caught or trained to a collar restraint should have lower body support provided directly by the restrainer or via a flat surface, such as the floor. Alternatively, after capture, the primate should be immediately transferred to a primate chair or other restraint device to minimize potential discomfort and distress. When a restraint device must be used it should be: clean, of an appropriate size, and maintained in proper working order to ensure the comfort and safety of the animal and human handler. The device should be inspected before each use to ensure that all mechanical parts are secure and functioning smoothly to avoid injury to the animal or the handler. Ideally, restraint chairs that allow primates free use of arms and legs should be equipped with a guard to protect handlers from exposures.
Chemical Restraint.
Animals may be trained to voluntarily cooperate for many procedures and this should be considered for primates that will be maintained for extended periods of time or for long-term studies. Chemical restraint, for example, ketamine (alone or in combination, e.g., dexmedetomidine, midazolam) may be considered when immobility is required (e.g., electrocardiography) or when the planned procedure has the potential to cause more than slight or momentary distress or discomfort (e.g., removal of skin sutures, etc.). Another consideration for using chemical restraint in primates is to ensure safety for personnel. When the use of chemical restraint is elected, NHPs should be fasted prior to chemical immobilization to reduce the risk of aspiration. Care should be taken to ensure that procedures involving chemical restraint are scheduled with a maximal interval to avoid repeated fasting, inappetence, and associated weight loss; prolonged episodes of hypothermia, bloat, and other stressors associated with administration of sedative or anesthetic agents. Performing multiple experimental/clinical procedures under a single chemical restraint event is often preferred to multiple anesthetic episodes over a short period of time. The lowest dose of drug should be used to achieve the desired effect to minimize potential side effects associated with the particular immobilizing agent.
The following are important considerations when using chemical restraint in primates:
Pain and/or distress expected with the procedure;
Safety of the animal and human handlers;
Size and maturity of the animal; and
Opportunity for positive reinforcement training or habituation to the procedure.
Selection of the appropriate sedative or anesthesia regimen should be made in consultation with the veterinary personnel. Specific pharmacologic reversal agents should be used whenever possible to reduce duration of immobility and counteract potential side effects (e.g., hypothermia, bradycardia or hypotension). Aspiration is always a risk, even in fasted animals. Primates must be monitored appropriately until they have completely recovered from immobilization. Ideally, animals recovering from anesthesia should be placed in a comfortable, warm, quiet area or space, preferably with restricted access to climbing apparatus, until they regain full physical coordination, to minimize potential associated injuries and trauma. Depending on the operational features of some housing facilities it may be possible to use pet carriers or transport boxes to facilitate separation between animals and offer a secure place and additional time for the reflexes to return. There are situations in which one or more of these criteria cannot be met due to facility limitations such as large compound, outdoor housing areas or emergencies. In these instances, proper precautions should be in place and professional judgment with a focus on animal welfare should be exercised. If NHPs are socially housed they should remain separated where possible until they have regained full physical coordination and mental awareness to minimize potential injuries. Animals living in large social outdoor groups may have to be recovered in indoor housing areas where other recovering animals may be present and removing access to climbing structures is not practical. In such cases, it is imperative to have appropriate supervision.
Duration of Restraint.
Restraint should be minimized to the shortest duration possible. If the animal is to be physically restrained for > 30 min on a regular basis, it must be habituated. Other techniques should be investigated to avoid prolonged restraint, such as use of indwelling catheters and vascular access ports for repeated collection of blood samples, alternative study designs, etc. Prolonged restraint in chairs may lead to decubitus ulcer formation in addition to mental fatigue of the animal. In the U.S., the United States Department of Agriculture (USDA) Animal Welfare Regulations (AWR) state that “Maintenance under [physical restraint] must be for the shortest period possible.” The AWR go on to state, “In instances where long-term (more than 12 hours) restraint is required, the nonhuman primate must be provided the opportunity daily for unrestrained activity for at least one continuous hour.” (AWA & AWR, 2002).
Monitoring.
Animals should be carefully observed during all types of restraint, and they should never be left unattended without some form of direct or indirect monitoring to ensure animal safety. Suitable forms of monitoring may include telemetry, vital sign (e.g. heart rate) monitors, a remote video system or monitoring output on computer tasks. Anesthetized animals should not be left unattended, even in closed cages, as ready access may be required for individuals that are entrapped or in respiratory distress. Some institutions, however, may have specific SOPs allowing deviation from such requirement, backed by historical and statistical data and approved by IACUC. Anesthetized animals should be monitored for respiratory/heart rates and body temperature where practical. Awake animals should be closely observed for evidence of pallor, and also vocalization, constant position shifting, and awkward posturing. Proximity to other restrained animals should be considered to minimize aggression and potential trauma.
Personnel Training.
Personnel working with restrained primates must be familiar with normal behavior in healthy unrestrained animals. Such familiarity is vital to being able to recognize any departures from normal and, in particular, any evidence of discomfort and distress exhibited by animals under physical/manual restraint. Knowledge and experience in monitoring vital signs in animals under chemical restraint/sedation is paramount where medical or experimental intervention may be necessary. If any restraint equipment is used, personnel should be trained to operate the equipment and how to deal with equipment failure to minimize animal injury and preserve human safety.
Endpoint Considerations.
Animals that fail to habituate to the planned restraint should be evaluated and considered for removal from study if alternative methods are not viable. Duration of the habituation trials should be discussed by and consensus reached between the veterinary clinician in charge, the principal investigator and IACUC.
Record Keeping.
IACUCs should be aware of the various types of NHP restraint equipment and modalities and review them at least annually within the program description or through specific protocol review. All relevant information (e.g. the animal’s response to the training/physical restraint, drug doses, recovery and complications etc.,) should be recorded in the medical records and research logs.
Disclaimer.
The position statements and/or guidelines produced by the Association of Primate Veterinarians (APV) are intended to be recommendations and guidance and are not a regulatory requirement. The Scientific Advisory Committee (SAC) within APV is tasked with the generation and revision of guidance documents for use by the membership and primate specialists worldwide. A subcommittee of current APV members and subject matter experts that have expertise in the area of interest are recruited to draft a document that is then sent out for comment and input from the SAC committee, the APV Board of Directors, and the APV membership. The final version is approved by the Board of Directors before being published on the APV website. We would like to extend special thanks to the committee members that worked on and contributed to this document: Michelle Delehanty (Covance), Doty Kempf (Yerkes NPRC, Emory Univ.), Robert Hoyt (NIH), Elysse Orchard (Rice University, Houston), and Marek Niekrasz (U of Chicago).
References
- 1. Adams MR, Kaplan JR, Manuck SB, Uberseder B, Larkin KT. “Persistent sympathetic nervous system arousal associated with tethering in cynomolgus macaques.” Lab Anim Sci 38:3( 1988): 279–81. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Breazile JE. “Physiologic basis and consequences of distress in animals.” JAVMA 191: 10( 1987): 1212– 15. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Bryant JM. “Vest and tethering system to accommodate catheters and a temperature monitor for nonhuman primates.” Lab Anim Sci 30( 1980): 706–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Bush M, Custer R, Smeller J, Bush LM. “Physiologic measures of nonhuman primates during physical restraint and chemical immobilization.” JAVMA 171: 9( 1977): 866– 69. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. “Environmental enhancement to promote psychological well-being.” In Animal Welfare Act and Animal Welfare Regulations, § 3.81(d), 94–95, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 6. Foshay WR, Tinkey PT. “Evaluating the effectiveness of training strategies: Performance goals and testing.” ILARJ 48( 2007): 156–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Gillis TE, Janes AC, Kaufman MJ. “Positive Reinforcement Training in Squirrel Monkeys Using Clicker Training.” Am J Primatol 74: 8 ( 2012): 712–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Greig I, Morris KD, Mathiesen E, Mathiesen R, Buchanan-Smith HM. “An Improved Restraint Device for Injections and Collection of Samples from Marmosets.” Laboratory Primate Newsletter 45: 2( 2006): 1–5. [Google Scholar]
- 9. Hawkins P. “Recognizing and assessing pain, suffering and distress in laboratory animals: A survey of current practice in the UK with recommendations.” Lab Anim 36: 4( 2002): 378– 95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Honess PE, Johnson PJ, Wolfensohn SE. “A study of behavioural responses of non-human primates to air transport and rehousing.” Lab Anim 38( 2004): 119–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Howell LL, Hoffman JM, Votaw JR, Landrum AM, Jordan JF. “An apparatus and behavioral training protocol to conduct positron emission tomography (PET) neuroimaging in conscious rhesus monkeys.” J Neurosci Methods 106: 2( 2001): 161–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Keckler MS, Hodara VL, Parodi LM, Giavedoni LD. “Novel application of nonhuman primate tethering system for evaluation of acute phase SIVmac251 infection in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta).” Viral Immunol 20: 4( 2007): 623–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Klein HJ, Bayne KA. “Establishing a culture of care, conscience, and responsibility: Addressing the improvement of scientific discovery and animal welfare through science-based performance standards.” ILARJ 48( 2007): 3–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Lambeth SP, Hau J, Perlman JE, Martino M, Schapiro SJ. “Positive reinforcement training affects hematologic and serum chemistry values in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).” Am J Primatol 68: 3( 2006): 245–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Laties VG. “Control of animal pain and distress in behavioral studies that use food deprivation or aversive stimulation.” JAVMA 191: 10( 1987): 1290– 91. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Laule GE, Bloomsmith MA, Schapiro SJ. “The use of positive reinforcement training techniques to enhance the care, management, and welfare of primates in the laboratory.” J Appl Anim Welf Sci 6( 2003): 163–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Machado CJ, Nelson EE. “Eye-tracking with nonhuman primates is now more accessible than ever before.” Am J Primatol 73: 6 ( 2011): 562–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. McGuffey LH, McCully CL, Bernacky BJ, Blaney SM. “Incorporation of an enrichment program into a study protocol involving long-term restraint in macaques.” Lab Anim 31( 2002): 37–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Morton DB, Griffiths PH. “Guidelines on the recognition of pain, distress and discomfort in experimental animals and an hypothesis for assessment.” Vet Rec 116( 1985): 431– 36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Morton WR, Knitter GH, Smith PM, Susor TG, Schmitt K. “Alternatives to chronic restraint of nonhuman primates.” J Am Vet Med Assoc 191: 10( 1987): 1282–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Morton WR, Knitter GH, Smith PV, Susor TG, Schmitt K. “Alternatives to chronic restraint of nonhuman primates.” JAVMA 191( 1987): 1282–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Nader MA, Grant KA, Gage HD, Ehrenkaufer RL, Kaplan JR, Mach RH. “PET imaging of dopamine D2 receptors with [18F]fluoroclebopride in monkeys: effects of isoflurane- and ketamine-induced anesthesia.” Neuropsychopharmacology 21: 4( 1999): 589–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. National Research Council. Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 24. National Research Council. Guidelines for the Care and Use of Mammals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 2003. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Norman RL, McGlone J, Smith CJ. “Restraint inhibits luteinizing hormone secretion in the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle in rhesus macaques.” Biol Reprod 50: 1( 1994): 16– 26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Norman RL, Smith CJ. “Restraint inhibits luteinizing hormone and testosterone secretion in intact male rhesus macaques: Effects of concurrent naloxone administration.” Neuroendocrinology 55: 4( 1992): 405– 15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Prescott MJ, Buchanan-Smith HM. “Training nonhuman primates using positive reinforcement techniques.” J Appl Anim Welf Sci 6( 2003): 157–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Reinhardt V. “Training adult male rhesus monkeys to actively cooperate during in-homecage venipuncture.” Anim Technol 42( 1991): 11–17. [Google Scholar]
- 29. Reinhardt V, Cowley D. “Training stumptailed monkeys (Macaca arctoides) to cooperate during in-homecage treatment.” Laboratory Primate Newsletter 29: 4( 1990). [Google Scholar]
- 30. Reinhardt V, Liss C, Stevens C. “Restraint methods of laboratory non-human primates: a critical review.” Animal Welfare 4( 1995): 221–38. [Google Scholar]
- 31. Ruys JD, Mendoza SP, Capitanio JP, Mason WA. “Behavioral and physiological adaptation to repeated chair restraint in rhesus macaques.” Physiol Behav 82( 2004): 205–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Sainsbury AW, Eaton BD, Cooper JE. “Restraint and anaesthesia of primates.” Veterinary Record 125( 1989): 640–4. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Sauceda R, Schmidt MG. “Refining macaque handling and restraint techniques.” Lab Anim 29( 2000): 47–9. [Google Scholar]
- 34. Schmidt EM, Dold GM, McIntosh JS. “A simple transfer and chairing technique for nonhuman primates.” Lab Anim Sci 39:3( 1989): 258–60. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Schultz-Darken NJ, Pape RM, Tannenbaum PL, Saltzman W, Abbott DH. “Novel restraint system for neuroendocrine studies of socially living common marmoset monkeys.” Laboratory Animals 38( 2004): 393– 405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Wade CE, Ortiz RM. “Urinary excretion of cortisol from rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) habituated to restraint.” Contemp Top Lab Anim Sci 36: 5( 1997): 55– 7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Wixon SK. “The role of the IACUC in assessing and managing pain and distress in research animals.” In The Care and Feeding of an IACUC: The Organization and Management of an Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee, edited by Lukas V., Podolsky M. L.. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 38. Wolf FR, White G, “Clinical Techniques used for Nonhuman Primates.” In Nonhuman Primates in Biomedical Research: Biology and Management, 2nd Edition. Vol 1, edited by Abee CR, Mansfield K, Tardiff S, Morris T. 323–337. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 2012. [Google Scholar]