Abstract
Electrical synapses between neurons, also known as gap junctions, are direct cell membrane channels between adjacent neurons. Gap junctions play a role in the synchronization of neuronal network activity; however, their involvement in cognition has not been well characterized. Three-hour olfactory associative memory in Drosophila has two components: consolidated anesthesia-resistant memory (ARM) and labile anesthesia-sensitive memory (ASM). Here, we show that knockdown of the gap junction gene innexin5 (inx5) in mushroom body (MB) neurons disrupted ARM, while leaving ASM intact. Whole-mount brain immunohistochemistry indicated that INX5 protein was preferentially expressed in the somas, calyxes, and lobes regions of the MB neurons. Adult-stage-specific knockdown of inx5 in αβ neurons disrupted ARM, suggesting a specific requirement of INX5 in αβ neurons for ARM formation. Hyperpolarization of αβ neurons during memory retrieval by expressing an engineered halorhodopsin (eNpHR) also disrupted ARM. Administration of the gap junction blocker carbenoxolone (CBX) reduced the proportion of odor responsive αβ neurons to the training odor 3 hours after training. Finally, the α-branch-specific 3-hour ARM-specific memory trace was also diminished with CBX treatment and in inx5 knockdown flies. Altogether, our results suggest INX5 gap junction channels in αβ neurons for ARM retrieval and also provide a more detailed neuronal mechanism for consolidated memory in Drosophila.
Author summary
One of the most important questions in the neuroscience is how the brain process memory. Memory formation requires neuronal communication in the brain via synaptic transmissions, which include chemical and electrical synapses. Unlike the chemical synapses, the biological functions of electrical synapses in memory formation remain poorly understood. Here, we revealed that electrical synapses between mushroom body (MB) αβ neurons in Drosophila are critical for consolidated memory retrieval. We also showed that the electrical synapses are important for the branch-specific modification of calcium influx into the αβ neurons during memory retrieval. Our results provide novel insights into the molecular mechanisms and synaptic networks underlying memory retrieval.
Introduction
Pavlovian olfactory learning in Drosophila melanogaster, the fruit fly, is a well-characterized behavioral paradigm in which flies are subjected to a training session of sequential exposures to two distinct odors (conditioned stimulus, CS) with or without electric foot shock (unconditioned stimulus, US)[1]. The assay involves CS-US coincidence detection in the mushroom bodies (MBs), the olfactory learning and memory centers of the fly brain. The MBs are a pair of neuropils composed of around 2,000 major intrinsic neurons, called the Kenyon cells (KCs), in each brain hemisphere[2]. The dendrites of the MB neurons form a calyx, and their axons project anteriorly through the peduncle to give rise to the αβ, α'β', and γ lobes in the middle brain. Three hours after a single training session consists of two genetically distinct forms of memory, anesthesia-sensitive memory (ASM) and anesthesia-resistant memory (ARM), with each accounting for about half of the retention level[3–5].
Synaptic transmission in the brain has two different modalities, chemical and electrical synapses. Neurons mainly use neurotransmitters or neuropeptides to communicate and regulate one another’s functions, which is mediated by chemical synapses. In contrast, electrical synaptic transmission depends on clusters of intercellular channels called gap junctions, which form the pores approximately 1.2 nm in diameter between neurons[6]. These pore structures allow diffusion of small molecules and ions, thus enabling bidirectional electronic signal transmission between neurons. The synchronization of neuronal activity in the hippocampus is mediated by gap junctions in the mammalian brain, which is critical for memory consolidation[7, 8]. In the human and mouse genomes, 21 and 20 gap junction genes have been identified, respectively[9]. The connexin and pannexin gap junction gene families are found in vertebrates, whereas the innexin (inx) gene family is found in invertebrates[10, 11]. Drosophila melanogaster has 8 gap junction genes, named inx1-inx8. Our previous study showed that two MB modulatory neurons in the Drosophila brain, the anterior paired lateral (APL) and dorsal paired medial (DPM) neurons, formed heterotypic gap junction channels via INX6 and INX7, and that disrupting communication through these gap junctions impaired 3-hour ASM[12]. Moreover, a recent study indicated that gap junctions in αβ, α'β', and MB output neurons (MBON-β'2mp) were involved in Drosophila visual learning[13].
To determine whether gap junctions in MB neurons are essential for olfactory memory formation, we knocked down each innexin gene in MB neurons and found that only the downregulation of inx5 specifically disrupted 3-hour ARM. Consistent with this result, whole-mount brain immunostaining showed INX5-positive signals in the somas, calyxes, and lobes of the MBs, suggesting the existence of gap junction channels between MB neurons. Knockdown of inx5 in αβ, but not α'β' or γ, neurons disrupted ARM, indicating that INX5 in αβ neurons was involved in ARM formation. Furthermore, adult-stage-specific knockdown of inx5 in αβ neurons disrupted ARM, demonstrating that the ARM deficiency was not caused by defects in MB development. We performed a transient inhibition of the action potential in αβ neurons by expressing an engineered halorhodopsin protein (eNpHR)[14], which acts as a light-driven chloride pump, specifically during memory retrieval, but not during acquisition or consolidation. This also led to the disruption of ARM, suggesting that INX5 was involved in ARM retrieval in αβ neurons. We observed a training-induced increase in the proportion of odor-responsive αβ neurons to the training odor (CS+ odor) 3 hours after conditioning, and this phenomenon was disrupted by treatment with the gap junction blocker carbenoxolone (CBX). Finally, we found increased calcium responses to the training odor in the MB α-lobe branch region 3 hours after conditioning, and this increased calcium response was diminished by both gap junction blocker CBX treatment and in inx5 knockdown flies. These data suggest that INX5 channels coordinate the MB neuronal activity changes to training odor at 3-hour after odor/shock association. Together, our results show that ARM retrieval in Drosophila is mediated by gap junction channels composed of INX5 in αβ neurons.
Results
MB INX5 is required for ARM
In our previous study, we found that heterotypic gap junctions in two MB modulatory neurons, the APL and DPM neurons, are required for 3-hour ASM formation[12]. In addition, the existence of gap junctions in MB neurons was reported in a recent study[13]. We therefore sought to examine whether gap junctions in MB neurons were involved in olfactory memory formation. We used a Drosophila RNAi library to express UAS-inxRNAi transgenes under the control of OK107-GAL4 to individually silence the inx genes in the entire MBs[15, 16]. We found that only inx5 knockdown disrupted 3-hour memory (Fig 1A and S1A Fig). In the fly, olfactory 3-hour memory consists of the labile ASM and consolidated ARM. We applied 2-min cold-shock anesthetization at 2-hour after training to abolish ASM, and tested the 3-hour ARM retention[5, 17–20]. Interestingly, the memory defect persisted after the cold-shock treatment in inx5 knockdown flies, suggesting that the memory loss was attributable to the disruption of ARM rather than ASM (Fig 1B–1D; S1B–S1E Fig).
It has been shown that inx5 is expressed 50 hours after pupal formation[21]. To further characterize INX5 protein expression in the fly brain, a rabbit polyclonal antibody recognizing Drosophila INX5 was generated. Whole-mount brain immunohistochemistry with this antibody showed that INX5 was expressed in the MB calyxes, somas, and lobes (Fig 2A–2C and S2 Fig). Western blotting confirmed that the INX5 protein levels were dramatically decreased in head extracts from two independent UAS-inx5RNAi flies (v6950 and JF02877) in which the RNA transgene was under the control of the pan-neuronal GAL4 driver, elav-GAL4 (Fig 2D). Furthermore, quantitative brain immunohistochemistry indicated that two independent UAS-inx5RNAi flies with the transgene under the control of OK107-GAL4 had greatly decreased INX5 levels in the MBs (Fig 2E).
INX5 function in αβ neurons is essential for ARM formation
Brain immunostaining showed INX5-positive signals in most if not all MB calyxes and somas. To identify the subset of MB neurons in which INX5 expression is required for ARM formation, we performed behavioral screening using RNAi-mediated inx5 knockdown with GAL4 drivers specific for different MB neurons. VT30604-GAL4, VT44966-GAL4, and C739-GAL4 drive expression of UAS-inx5RNAi in α′β′, γ, and αβ neurons respectively (Fig 3A). Genetic knockdown of INX5 in αβ neurons, but not in α′β′ or γ neurons, disrupted ARM, suggesting that INX5 in αβ neurons regulates the ARM process (Fig 3B). It is important to consider that C739-GAL4 is not exclusively expressed in αβ neurons (Fig 3C). We therefore combined the MB-GAL80 transgene to reduce GAL4 expression in the MB neurons. The presence of the MB-GAL80 transgene specifically abolished GAL4 activity in MB neurons, but left its expression unchanged in non-MB neurons (Fig 3D). Three-hour ARM of C739-GAL4/MB-GAL80; UAS-inx5RNAi/+ flies were statistically indistinguishable from both wild-type and MB-GAL80/+ flies and were also statistically different from that of C739-GAL4/+; UAS-inx5RNAi/+ flies (Fig 3E). Moreover, this behavioral result was further confirmed in an additional GAL4 line (VT49246-GAL4) with specifically labeled αβ neurons in the fly brain (Fig 3F and 3G). Furthermore, our previous study found that glutamate release from MB αβ output neurons (MBON-β2β′2a) was required for ARM[20]. To test the possibility that gap junctions between αβ neurons and MBON-β2β′2a are involved in ARM formation, we genetically knocked down each inx gene in MBON-β2β′2a and found that all the modified flies displayed normal ARM (S3 Fig). In addition, using a dye-coupling approach, Liu and colleagues found no gap junction connectivity between αβ neurons and their target MBONs[13]. Furthermore, our pervious study found that knockdown of each inx gene in the projection neurons and APL neurons via expressing individual UAS-inxRNAi by GH146-GAL4 did not impair ARM[12]. Based on these results taken together, we conclude that ARM formation requires gap junctions between αβ neurons. In order to rule out the possibility that the chronic RNA-mediated knockdown of inx5 causes developmental defects in the MBs, we examined the gross morphologies of the MBs in the inx5-manipulated flies and found no significant differences as compared to control flies, suggesting that the MB structure was unaffected by the chronic inx5 knockdown (S4 Fig). Furthermore, we used an inducible knockdown strategy to silence INX5 expression specifically in the adult stage utilizing a temperature-sensitive GAL80 repressor (tubP-GAL80ts). Inducible knockdown of INX5 still induced significant impairment of 3-hour ARM, but not initial learning (Fig 4), suggesting that INX5 in αβ neurons is required post-developmentally for ARM formation.
Hyperpolarization of αβ neurons during memory retrieval disrupts ARM
Consistent with previous studies, blocking chemical synaptic transmission by a temperature sensitive shibire (shits) in αβ neurons during memory retrieval[20, 22] but not during acquisition or consolidation (S5A Fig), disrupted ARM. The shibire gene encodes the Drosophila homologue of dynamin, which is required for the fission of endocytic vesicles from the presynaptic membrane. A dominant-negative mutant form of shibire interferes with the recycling of neurotransmitters but cannot block the gap junction-mediated propagation of action potential between adjacent neurons[23–27]. We therefore applied optogenetic tools to transiently block action potential in αβ neurons during memory acquisition, consolidation, or retrieval which subsequently allowed us to examine the specific memory phase of ARM that gap junctions were involved in. We overexpressed eNpHR to transiently hyperpolarize αβ neurons during different phases of the ARM process. The eNpHR-mediated hyperpolarization of αβ neurons during memory retrieval impaired ARM in C739-GAL4 > UAS-eNpHR and VT49246-GAL4 > UAS-eNpHR flies. In contrast, eNpHR-mediated hyperpolarization of αβ neurons during memory acquisition or consolidation did not impair ARM, suggesting that the activity in αβ neurons is only required for ARM retrieval (Fig 5 and S5B Fig). Although the eNpHR-mediated hyperpolarization in αβ neurons also affects the secretion of neuropeptides, blocking these secretions (e.g. amnesiac) in αβ neurons does not affect ARM [28]. Knockdown of inx5 gap junction gene in αβ neurons disrupted ARM and highlights the role of gap junctions between αβ neurons[23–27] (Figs 2, 3, and 4). ARM deficiency was only observed in all-trans-retinal-fed flies indicates that the ARM defect was caused by the transient blockade of αβ neuronal activity through eNpHR-mediated neuronal silencing (Fig 5C and S5B Fig). Taken together, these data suggest that INX5 gap junctions are involved in ARM retrieval from αβ neurons in the fly brain.
INX5 is essential for 3-hour ARM-specific memory trace in αβ neurons
To further test the hypothesis that INX5 is involved in ARM retrieval, the calcium indicator GCaMP6 was applied to monitor αβ neuronal activity during ARM retrieval[29]. The preferential expression of INX5 in the somas of MB neurons led us to examine the αβ neuronal activity changes in this region. We first observed that odor/shock association increases the proportion of odor responsive αβ neurons to the conditioned odor (CS+ odor) at 3-hour after a 2-min cold shock given at 2-hour after training (Fig 6A). The increased CS+ odor responsive αβ neurons were diminished by treatment with the gap junction blocker CBX, 10 min before image recording (Fig 6A). To further analyze whether this increased CS+ odor responsive αβ neurons correlate to the memory trace[30, 31], we recorded the neuronal activity in the MB α- and β- lobes respectively. The MB α-lobe branch has been shown to produce training-induced modifications in odor-evoked cellular calcium responses[30]. We visualized the functional responses of naïve flies to different odors, 3-octanol (OCT) or 4-methyl-cyclohexanol (MCH), in the αβ lobes by expressing UAS-GCaMP6m under the control of C739-GAL4. However, only the MB α-lobe branch exhibited a significantly elevated GCaMP6 intensity 3 hours after shock/odor association as compared to naïve flies (Fig 6B–6C and S6 Fig). These results indicated that the MB α-lobe branch exhibits a training-induced increase in calcium responses 3 hours after training. The increased calcium response was abrogated by treatment with the gap junction blocker CBX 10 min before image recording, and was recovered after CBX washout (Fig 6B–6C). Although a significant 3-hour memory trace was also observed in α'β' neurons, this phenomenon still occurred with CBX treatment suggesting that the memory trace is independent of the gap junction (S7 Fig). Finally, genetic knockdown of inx5 in αβ neurons also abolished the increased calcium responses in the MB α-lobe to the training odor at 3-hour after conditioning, which suggests that INX5 gap junctions are required for the branch-specific modification of neuronal responses to the conditioned odor during memory retrieval (Figs 6 and 7).
Discussion
In fruit flies, two parallel MB circuits, containing αβ and α'β' neurons, are involved in ARM formation. Radish expression in αβ neurons is required for partial ARM, whereas octβ2R expression in MB α'β' neurons is required for the rest part of ARM, suggesting that two distinct cellular mechanisms regulate ARM in different MB neurons[17, 19, 20, 32]. The radish gene encodes a protein with a predicted cAMP-dependent protein kinase phosphorylation site, which can bind Rac1 to regulate the rearrangement of the cytoskeleton and affect synaptic structural morphology[32]. The interaction of RADISH and BRUCHPILOT at the synaptic active zone has been proposed to regulate neurotransmitter release[17], and genetic knockdown of radish or bruchpilot in αβ neurons disrupts ARM[17, 20]. A recent study indicated that Drk–Drok signaling is essential for ARM formation in αβ neurons, and related to dynamic cytoskeletal changes[33]. In addition, the dopamine type 2 (D2R) and serotonin (5HT1A) receptors in αβ neurons are also critical for ARM formation[34, 35].
The key finding of our study is that the gap junction protein INX5 in αβ neurons is critical for 3-hour ARM retrieval. This conclusion is supported by four independent lines of evidence. First, immunohistochemistry data indicated that INX5 was preferentially expressed in the MB calyxes and somas, and these INX5-positive signals were reduced in OK107-GAL4 > UAS-inx5RNAi flies (Fig 2). Second, adult-stage-specific knockdown of inx5 in αβ neurons impaired ARM (Fig 4). Third, eNpHR-mediated inhibition of action potential in αβ neurons during retrieval also impaired ARM (Fig 5). Forth, knockdown of inx5 in αβ neurons inhibited the training-induced cellular calcium responses in the MB α-lobe region 3 hours after odor/shock association (Fig 6).
Previous studies have concluded that αβ neuronal activity is involved in 3-hour memory retrieval using shibirets to transiently block chemical synaptic transmissions via inhibiting neurotransmitter recycling[22, 36]. Three-hour memory is composed of ASM and ARM, each accounting for about half of the memory retention level[3–5]. In our recent study, we further showed that the inhibition of neurotransmitter recycling in αβ neurons during memory retrieval disrupted 3-hour ARM[20]. However, blocking neurotransmitter recycling in αβ neurons during memory acquisition and consolidation did not affect 3-hour ARM (S5A Fig). The function of gap junctions in the electrical synapses is to coordinate the propagation of action potential in neuronal networks[23–27], and shibirets cannot block gap junction-mediated electrical synapses. We therefore used eNpHR to transiently silence action potential in αβ neurons to confirm the requirement of αβ neuronal activity during the ARM formation process. Our data showed an eNpHR-mediated hyperpolarization of αβ neurons during memory retrieval but not acquisition or consolidation, impaired ARM, suggesting that action potential in αβ neurons is required only for ARM retrieval (Fig 5 and S5B Fig). Brain immunostaining data showed that INX5 gap junction proteins are strongly expressed in the calyxes and somas of αβ neurons (Fig 2 and S2 Fig), and knockdown of inx5 gap junction gene in αβ neurons disrupted ARM (Figs 3 and 4). The expression of gap junction is critical for neuronal functions since it plays a role in the propagation of action potential between adjacent neurons[23–27]. We therefore conclude that the gap junction channels composed of INX5 in αβ neurons are critical for ARM retrieval (Fig 7). A recent study showed that the gap junction protein INX2 regulates calcium transmission across the follicle cells during Drosophila oogenesis[37]. In addition, INX1/INX2 induces calcium oscillations in the glial cells of the blood-brain barrier (BBB), enabling signal amplification and synchronization across the BBB in fruit flies[26]. Furthermore, the gap junction protein INX6 is important for promoting synchronous neuronal activity in the dorsal fan-shaped body (dFB) in the fly brain that is critical for the sleep switch[38]. In mammals, most neuronal gap junctions in the brain are composed of Connexin-36 (Cx36) and are involved in synchronizing the hippocampal neuronal oscillatory patterns[39], which is required for emotional memories[8]. Therefore, it is possible that gap junction channels composed of INX5 mediate neuronal activity amplification and synchronization across αβ neurons, boosting the synaptic output strength during ARM retrieval (Fig 7).
By using the newly developed calcium indicator GCaMP6[29], we observed the increased proportion of training odor-responsive αβ neurons 3 hours after odor/shock association, and this phenomenon was abolished after treatment with gap junction blocker CBX (Fig 6A). Furthermore, we observed significant enhancement of the training-induced cellular calcium response to the training odor in the MB α-lobe branch 3 hours after odor/shock association (Fig 6B–6C). According to the broad consensus of the field, the memory trace is supposed to be formed in the vertical lobe of the MBs by the activity contingency of MBs and dopaminergic Protocerebral Posterior Lateral 1 (PPL1) neurons, which represent odor and punitive shock, respectively[40–42]. Therefore, it is possible that 3-hour memory trace back propagation of somas’ activity occurs from the MB lobes during memory retrieval. In addition, the branch specific modifications via MB input neurons (e.g., Protocerebral Anterior Medial, PAM) may occur during memory retrieval[43, 44], hence the memory trace was only observed in α-lobe branch but not the β-lobe of MBs (Fig 6B–6C & S6 Fig). This training-induced 3-hour ARM-specific memory trace was eliminated by treatment with the gap junction blocker, CBX, during memory retrieval or by genetic knockdown of inx5 in αβ neurons. Although a significant 3-hour ARM-specific memory trace was also observed in α'β' neurons, this phenomenon was independent of the gap junction (S7 Fig). From this, we propose that an unknown dynamic mechanism regulates the permeability of gap junction channels composed of INX5 in αβ neurons after training. Recently, cryoelectron microscopy revealed that the structure of C.elegans INX6 was highly similar to that of the vertebrate gap junction protein Connexin-26 (Cx26)[45]. Connexin properties, such as gating and assembly, can be regulated by phosphorylation[46, 47]. Additionally, the functions of Innexins or Connexins can also be regulated by changes in the intracellular pH and calcium levels[48–50]. Establishing whether the properties of INX5 in the MBs are modified following conditioned training will provide insights into the neuronal mechanisms of ARM.
Materials and methods
Fly stocks
Flies were raised on standard cornmeal food media at 25°C and 70% relative humidity under a 12:12-hour light: dark cycle. The “Cantonized” w1118 w (CS10) strain was used as a wild-type control. The MB-GAL80, UAS-GCaMP6m, and OK107-GAL4 flies were obtained from Bloomington Drosophila stock center. The UAS-eNpHR-YFP; UAS-eNpHR-YFP fly line was obtained from Ann-Shyn Chiang. The RNAi lines were obtained from the Vienna Drosophila RNAi Center or TRiP RNAi fly stocks. All RNAi lines from the Vienna Drosophila RNAi Center have been described previously[12]. The VT30604-GAL4, VT44966-GAL4, C739-GAL4, VT49246-GAL4, VT0765-GAL4, UAS-shits, and tubP-GAL80ts flies have been described[20].
Whole-mount immunostaining
Brain samples were stained with the mouse 4F3 anti-discs large (DLG) monoclonal antibody (Hybridoma Bank) to label all neuronal synapses, or with a rabbit polyclonal anti-INX5 antibody. The rabbit INX5 antibody was generated by Antibody International, Inc., with an HPLC-purified synthetic peptide, NH2– PHFRSSLRRIGEYNEAYAR–COOH, selected from the INX5 sequence. Fixed brain samples were incubated in PBS containing 1% Triton X-100 and 0.25% normal goat serum (PBS-T) with mouse 4F3 anti-DLG antiserum (1:10) or rabbit anti-INX5 (1:1,000) as primary antibodies at 25°C for 1 day. After three washes in PBS-T, the samples were incubated in biotinylated goat anti-mouse or rabbit IgG (1:200; Invitrogen) at 25°C for 1 day. Next, the brain samples were washed and incubated in Alexa Fluor 633 streptavidin (1:500; Invitrogen) at 25°C overnight. After extensive washing, the brain samples were cleared and mounted in FocusClearT (CelExplorer) for confocal imaging.
Confocal microscopy
Sample brains were imaged under a Zeiss LSM 700 confocal microscope with either a 40× C-Apochromat water-immersion objective lens for whole-brain images (N.A. value, 1.2; working distance, 220 μm) or a 63× glycerol-immersion objective lens for horizontal, sagittal, and frontal cross sections (N.A. value, 1.4; working distance, 170 μm). To overcome the limited field of view, some samples were imaged twice, one for each hemisphere, with overlap in between. We then stitched the two parallel image stacks into a single dataset online with the ZEN software, using the overlapping region to align the two stacks.
Behavioral assay
Groups of approximately 100 flies were exposed first to one odor (CS+; OCT or MCH) paired with 12 1.5-s pulses of 75-V DC electric shock presented at 5-s interpulse intervals. This was followed by the presentation of a second odor (CS–; MCH or OCT) without electric shock. In the testing phase, the flies were presented with a choice between the CS+ and CS–odors in a T-maze for 2-min. At the end of the 2-min period, the flies in each T-maze arm were trapped, anesthetized, and counted. From the distribution of flies between the 2 arms, the performance index (PI) was calculated as the number of flies avoiding the shock-associated odor (CS+) minus the number avoiding the non-shock-associated odor (CS–), divided by the total number of flies and multiplied by 100. If the flies did not learn, they were distributed equally between the 2 arms; hence, the calculated PI was 0. If all flies avoided the shock-paired odor and were distributed 0:100 between the CS+ and CS–arms in the T-maze, the PI was 100. To assess learning, performance was measured immediately after training. To evaluate intermediate-term memory, testing was performed 3 hours after training. ARM was defined as 3-hour memory after a 2-min cold shock presented at 2-hour post-training (i.e., 1 hour before testing) by placing a plastic vial containing the trained flies in ice water. A brief cold shock completely erases short-term memory and the labile ASM, preserving only ARM. For the adult-stage-specific RNAi-mediated knockdown of inx5 with tubP-GAL80ts, flies were kept at 18°C until eclosion and then shifted to 30°C for 7 days before training. The 3-hour ARM assay was also performed at 30°C. Control flies were kept at 18°C throughout the experiment. For eNpHR-mediated light-inactivation during memory acquisition, a group of approximately 100 flies were put into a custom-made light-delivering electrical shock tube and received electrical shock alternately paired with either OCT or MCH. For eNpHR-mediated light-inactivation during memory consolidation, the conditioned flies were put into the LED-embedded tube for 1.5 hours immediately after training. For eNpHR-mediated light-inactivation during memory retrieval, the conditioned flies were tested for approach to OCT or MCH in LED-embedded tubes. The light intensity was approximately 9.35 mW/cm2, and the wavelength was 590 nm. Flies were fed a standard food medium with or without 100 μM all-trans-retinal (Sigma-Aldrich) for at least 5 days before the experiments.
GCaMP imaging
Sample preparation for in vivo calcium imaging was modified from a previous study[31]. The fly was fixed in a 250-μl pipette tip, a small window was opened on the head capsule using fine tweezers and fixed in place with dental glue. Next, a drop of adult hemolymph-like (AHL) saline (108 mM NaCl, 5 mM KCl, 2 mM CaCl2, 8.2 mM MgCl2, 4 mM NaHCO3, 1 mM NaH2PO4, 5 mM trehalose, 10 mM sucrose and 5 mM HEPES [pH 7.5, 265 mOsm]) was added to the window to prevent dehydration. The fly and pipette tip were fixed to a coverslip by tape and time-lapse recording of changes in GCaMP6m intensity before and after odor delivery was performed on a Zeiss LSM 700 confocal microscope with a 40X water-immersion objective (W Plan-Apochromat 40× /1.0 DIC M27), a 488-nm excitation laser, and a detector for emissions passing through a 555 nm short-pass filter. An optical slice with a resolution of 512 × 512 pixels was continuously monitored for 60 s at 2 frames per second. Odorants were delivered at 11 s and 29 s in each 60 s trial. To correct the motion artifacts, frames were aligned using a lightweight SIFT-implementation[51]. Response amplitudes were calculated as the mean change in fluorescence (dF/F) in the 0.1–5 s window after stimulus onset. To quantify the numbers of the MB neurons in odor stimulus, the response was judged to be significant if the peak was > 0.2 (dF/F). For the lobe specific memory trace assay, regions of interest (ROI) were manually assigned to anatomically different regions of the MB lobe. To evaluate responses to different odors in flies, we calculated the change in GCaMP6 fluorescence as ΔF (Ft–F0)/F0. Changes in GCaMP6 fluorescent intensity for the CS+ vs. CS− odors were calculated as log10 (ΔFCS+/ΔFCS-). For the CBX treatment experiments, flies were dissected and immediately placed in a drop of adult AHL saline containing 1 mM CBX for 10 min before image recording. The CBX solution was washed out using standard AHL solution. ΔF/F0 intensity maps were generated using ImageJ.
Quantification of INX5 immunostaining
For the quantification of INX5 protein, fly brains were immunostained with rabbit INX5 antibody (1:1,000) at 25°C for 1 day. After three washes in PBS-T, the samples were incubated in biotinylated goat anti-mouse or rabbit IgG (1:200; Invitrogen) at 25°C for 1 day. The brain samples were then washed and incubated in Alexa Fluor 635 streptavidin (1:500; Invitrogen) at 25°C overnight. After extensive washing, the brain samples were cleared and mounted in FocusClear (CelExplorer). Brain images were obtained using a Zeiss LSM 700 confocal microscope under the same confocal settings for each sample, and the images were further analyzed using ImageJ. Single optical sections were used to calculate the average intensity values per voxel of the INX5 immunopositive signals in the MB calyx, α lobe, α' lobe, β lobe, β' lobe, γ lobe, and protocerebrum bridge (PB). The fluorescent intensity in the PB was used as an adjacent-region control.
Quantitative PCR (qPCR)
The efficiency of gene inactivation in each inxRNAi line from TRiP collections was verified with qPCR. Flies for qPCR were generated by crossing elav-GAL4 virgin flies to either wild-type males or the various UAS-inxRNAi males. RNA from the isolated heads of adult flies was extracted with TRIzol Reagent (Invitrogen). The extracted RNA was used to synthesize first-strand cDNA with RevertAid First Strand cDNA Synthesis Kit (Thermo Fisher Scientific). RNA expression levels were quantified with SYBR Green PCR Master Mix on a StepOnePlus System (Thermo Fisher Scientific).
Western blotting
For western blotting, the heads of adult flies were homogenized in lysis buffer (25 mM HEPES [pH 7.5], 100 mM NaCl, 1 mM MgCl2, 1 mM CaCl2, 0.1% SDS, 0.2% TritonX-100, 0.2% NP-40, 1 mM EDTA, 1 mM EGTA, and protease inhibitor cocktail [Roche, CH]), the lysates were centrifuged at 14,000 rpm for 30 min at 4°C, and the supernatants were collected. Lysate proteins were electrophoresed on an SDS-PAGE and electroblotted onto PVDF membranes. The immobilized proteins were probed with rabbit anti-INX5 (1:20,000) and anti-β-actin (1:10,000) antibodies. The membrane was then incubated with horse radish peroxidase (HRP)-conjugated goat-anti-rabbit IgG secondary antibody (1:10,000). The positive signal was detected with SuperSignal West Pico PLUS Chemiluminescent Substrate (Thermo Fisher Scientific).
Statistical analysis
Raw data were analyzed parametrically using the Prism 7.0 software (GraphPad). Because of the nature of their mathematical derivation, PIs were distributed normally. Hence, data from more than two groups were evaluated by one-way analysis of variance and Tukey’s multiple-comparisons tests. Data from only two groups were evaluated by the paired t-test. A statistically significant difference was defined as P < 0.05. The data in the bar graphs are presented as means ± SEM.
Supporting information
Acknowledgments
We thank Meng-Fu Maxwell Shih and Yi-Wen Chen for the assistance with the initial behavioral experiments, and Yen-Yin Lin for help with the functional image assays. We thank Suewei Lin and Tzu-Yang Lin for their critical comments. We also thank Ann-Shyn Chiang, Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center, Vienna Drosophila RNAi Center, Vienna Tile (VT) Library and Fly Core in Taiwan for providing fly stocks.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by grants from the Ministry of Science and Technology (106-2311-B-182-004-MY3) to CLW & Chang Gung Memorial Hospital (CMRPD1G0341-3, and BMRPC75) to CLW. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Tully T, Quinn WG. Classical conditioning and retention in normal and mutant Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol A. 1985;157:263–77. 10.1007/BF01350033 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Aso Y, Grubel K, Busch S, Friedrich AB, Siwanowicz I, Tanimoto H. The mushroom body of adult Drosophila characterized by GAL4 drivers. J Neurogenet. 2009;23:156–72. 10.1080/01677060802471718 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Quinn WG, Dudai Y. Memory phases in Drosophila. Nature. 1976;262:576–7. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Margulies C, Tully T, Dubnau J. Deconstructing memory in Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2005;15:R700–13. 10.1016/j.cub.2005.08.024 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Horiuchi J, Yamazaki D, Naganos S, Aigaki T, Saitoe M. Protein kinase A inhibits a consolidated form of memory in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:20976–81 10.1073/pnas.0810119105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Hervé J-C, Derangeon M. Gap-junction-mediated cell-to-cell communication. Cell Tissue Res. 2013;352:21–31. 10.1007/s00441-012-1485-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Beheshti S, Eivani M, Moshtaghian J. Gap junctions of the hippocampal CA1 area are crucial for memory consolidation. Physiol Pharmacol. 2015;19(3):177–84. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bissiere S, Zelikowsky M, Ponnusamy R, Jacobs NS, Blair HT, Fanselow MS. Electrical synapses control hippocampal contributions to fear learning and memory. Science. 2011;331:87–91. 10.1126/science.1193785 PMC4276370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Sohl G, Willecke K. An update on connexin genes and their nomenclature in mouse and man. Cell Commun Adhes. 2003;10:173–80. 10.1080/cac.10.4-6.173.180 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Paul DL. Molecular cloning of cDNA for rat liver gap junction protein. J Cell Biol. 1986;103:123–34. 10.1083/jcb.103.1.123 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Phelan P, Starich TA. Innexins get into the gap. Bioessays. 2001;23:388–96. 10.1002/bies.1057 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Wu CL, Shih MF, Lai JS, Yang HT, Turner GC, Chen L, et al. Heterotypic gap junctions between two neurons in the Drosophila brain are critical for memory. Curr Biol. 2011;21:848–54. 10.1016/j.cub.2011.02.041 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Liu Q, Yang X, Tian J, Gao Z, Wang M, Li Y, et al. Gap junction networks in mushroom bodies participate in visual learning and memory in. Elife. 2016;5 10.7554/eLife.13238 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Gradinaru V, Thompson KR, Deisseroth K. eNpHR: a Natronomonas halorhodopsin enhanced for optogenetic applications. Brain Cell Biol. 2008;36:129–39. 10.1007/s11068-008-9027-6 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Dietzl G, Chen D, Schnorrer F, Su K-C, Barinova Y, Fellner M, et al. A genome-wide transgenic RNAi library for conditional gene inactivation in Drosophila. Nature. 2007;448:151–6. 10.1038/nature05954 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Ni J-Q, Liu L-P, Binari R, Hardy R, Shim H-S, Cavallaro A, et al. A Drosophila Resource of Transgenic RNAi Lines for Neurogenetics. Genetics. 2009;182:1089–100. 10.1534/genetics.109.103630 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Knapek S, Sigrist S, Tanimoto H. Bruchpilot, a synaptic active zone protein for anesthesia-resistant memory. J Neurosci. 2011;31:3453–8. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2585-10.2011 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Scheunemann L, Jost E, Richlitzki A, Day JP, Sebastian S, Thum AS, et al. Consolidated and labile odor memory are separately encoded within the Drosophila brain. J Neurosci. 2012;32:17163–71. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3286-12.2012 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wu CL, Shih MF, Lee PT, Chiang AS. An octopamine-mushroom body circuit modulates the formation of anesthesia-resistant memory in Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2013;23:2346–54 10.1016/j.cub.2013.09.056 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Yang CH, Shih MF, Chang CC, Chiang MH, Shih HW, Tsai YL, et al. Additive expression of consolidated memory through Drosophila mushroom body subsets. PLoS Genet. 2016;12:e1006061 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006061 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Stebbings LA, Todman MG, Phillips R, Greer CE, Tam J, Phelan P, et al. Gap junctions in Drosophila: developmental expression of the entire innexin gene family. Mech Dev. 2002;113:197–205. 10.1016/S0925-4773(02)00025-4 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Krashes MJ, Keene AC, Leung B, Armstrong JD, Waddell S. Sequential use of mushroom body neuron subsets during Drosophila odor memory processing. Neuron. 2007;53:103–15. 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.021 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Coulon P, Landisman CE. The Potential Role of Gap Junctional Plasticity in the Regulation of State. Neuron. 2017;93(6):1275–95. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.02.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Pereda AE, Curti S, Hoge G, Cachope R, Flores CE, Rash JE. Gap junction-mediated electrical transmission: Regulatory mechanisms and plasticity. Biochim Biophys Acta. 2013;1828:134–46. 10.1016/j.bbamem.2012.05.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Phelan P, Goulding LA, Tam JLY, Allen MJ, Dawber RJ, Davies JA, et al. Molecular Mechanism of Rectification at Identified Electrical Synapses in the Drosophila Giant Fiber System. Curr Biol. 2008;18:1955–60. 10.1016/j.cub.2008.10.067 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Spéder P, Brand AH. Gap junction proteins in the blood-brain barrier control nutrient-dependent reactivation of Drosophila neural stem cells. Dev Cell. 2014;30:309–21. 10.1016/j.devcel.2014.05.021 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Thomas JB, Wyman RJ. Mutations altering synaptic connectivity between identified neurons in Drosophila. J Neurosci. 1984;4(2):530–8. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-02-00530.1984 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Turrel O, Goguel V, Preat T. Amnesiac is required in the adult mushroom body for memory formation. J Neurosci. 2018;38(43):9202 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0876-18.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Chen T-W, Wardill TJ, Sun Y, Pulver SR, Renninger SL, Baohan A, et al. Ultra-sensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature. 2013;499:295–300. 10.1038/nature12354 PMC3777791. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Yu D, Akalal DB, Davis RL. Drosophila alpha/beta mushroom body neurons form a branch-specific, long-term cellular memory trace after spaced olfactory conditioning. Neuron. 2006;52:845–55. 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Wang Y, Mamiya A, Chiang AS, Zhong Y. Imaging of an early memory trace in the Drosophila mushroom body. J Neurosci. 2008;28:4368–76. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2958-07.2008 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Folkers E, Waddell S, Quinn WG. The Drosophila radish gene encodes a protein required for anesthesia-resistant memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103:17496–500. 10.1073/pnas.0608377103 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Kotoula V, Moressis A, Semelidou O, Skoulakis EMC. Drk-mediated signaling to Rho kinase is required for anesthesia-resistant memory in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2017;114:10984–9. 10.1073/pnas.1704835114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Lee P-T, Lin H-W, Chang Y-H, Fu T-F, Dubnau J, Hirsh J, et al. Serotonin-mushroom body circuit modulating the formation of anesthesia-resistant memory in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2011;108:13794 10.1073/pnas.1019483108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Scholz-Kornehl S, Schwärzel M. Circuit analysis of a Drosophila dopamine type 2 receptor that supports anesthesia-resistant memory. J Neurosci. 2016;36:7936–45. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4475-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.McGuire SE, Le PT, Davis RL. The role of Drosophila mushroom body signaling in olfactory memory. Science. 2001;293:1330–3. 10.1126/science.1062622 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Sahu A, Ghosh R, Deshpande G, Prasad M. A gap junction protein, inx2, modulates calcium flux to specify border cell fate during Drosophila oogenesis. PLoS Genet. 2017;13:e1006542 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006542 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Troup M, Yap MHW, Rohrscheib C, Grabowska MJ, Ertekin D, Randeniya R, et al. Acute control of the sleep switch in Drosophila reveals a role for gap junctions in regulating behavioral responsiveness. Elife 2018;7:e37105 10.7554/eLife.37105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Posłuszny A. The contribution of electrical synapses to field potential oscillations in the hippocampal formation. Front Neural Circuits. 2014;8:32 10.3389/fncir.2014.00032 PMC3982077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Aso Y, Herb A, Ogueta M, Siwanowicz I, Templier T, Friedrich AB, et al. Three dopamine pathways induce aversive odor memories with different stability. PLoS Genet. 2012;8(7):e1002768 10.1371/journal.pgen.1002768 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Waddell S. Dopamine reveals neural circuit mechanisms of fly memory. Trends Neurosci. 2010;33(10):457–64. 10.1016/j.tins.2010.07.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Claridge-Chang A, Roorda RD, Vrontou E, Sjulson L, Li H, Hirsh J, et al. Writing memories with light-addressable reinforcement circuitry. Cell. 2009;139(2):405–15. 10.1016/j.cell.2009.08.034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Cohn R, Morantte I, Ruta V. Coordinated and compartmentalized neuromodulation shapes sensory processing in Drosophila. Cell. 2015;163(7):1742–55. 10.1016/j.cell.2015.11.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Aso Y, Hattori D, Yu Y, Johnston RM, Iyer NA, Ngo T-TB, et al. The neuronal architecture of the mushroom body provides a logic for associative learning. Elife. 2014;3:e04577 10.7554/eLife.04577 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Oshima A, Matsuzawa T, Murata K, Tani K, Fujiyoshi Y. Hexadecameric structure of an invertebrate gap junction channel. J Mol Biol. 2016;428:1227–36. 10.1016/j.jmb.2016.02.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Laird DW. Connexin phosphorylation as a regulatory event linked to gap junction internalization and degradation. Biochim Biophys Acta. 2005;1711:172–82. 10.1016/j.bbamem.2004.09.009 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Moreno AP. Connexin phosphorylation as a regulatory event linked to channel gating. Biochim Biophys Acta. 2005;1711:164–71. 10.1016/j.bbamem.2005.02.016 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.LW R., Birgit R. Calcium in (junctional) intercellular communication and a thought on its behavior in intracellular communication. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1978;307:285–307. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1978.tb41958.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Spray DC, Bennett MVL. Physiology and pharmacology of gap junctions. Annu Rev Physiol. 1985;47:281–303. 10.1146/annurev.ph.47.030185.001433 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Lampe PD, Lau AF. The effects of connexin phosphorylation on gap junctional communication. Int J Biochem Cell Biol. 2004;36:1171–86. 10.1016/S1357-2725(03)00264-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Lowe DG. Distinctive Image Features from Scale-Invariant Keypoints. Int J Comput Vision. 2004;60:91–110. 10.1023/B:VISI.0000029664.99615.94 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.