Abstract
Flight initiation distance (FID), the distance at which individuals take flight when approached by a potential (human) predator, is a tool for understanding predator–prey interactions. Among the factors affecting FID, tests of effects of group size (i.e., number of potential prey) on FID have yielded contrasting results. Group size or flock size could either affect FID negatively (i.e., the dilution effect caused by the presence of many individuals) or positively (i.e., increased vigilance due to more eyes scanning for predators). These effects may be associated with gregarious species, because such species should be better adapted to exploiting information from other individuals in the group than nongregarious species. Sociality may explain why earlier findings on group size versus FID have yielded different conclusions. Here, we analyzed how flock size affected bird FID in eight European countries. A phylogenetic generalized least square regression model was used to investigate changes in escape behavior of bird species in relation to number of individuals in the flock, starting distance, diet, latitude, and type of habitat. Flock size of different bird species influenced how species responded to perceived threats. We found that gregarious birds reacted to a potential predator earlier (longer FID) when aggregated in large flocks. These results support a higher vigilance arising from many eyes scanning in birds, suggesting that sociality may be a key factor in the evolution of antipredator behavior both in urban and rural areas. Finally, future studies comparing FID must pay explicit attention to the number of individuals in flocks of gregarious species.
Keywords: birds, dilution effect, fear response, FID, gregariousness, human disturbance, social interactions, vigilance
1. INTRODUCTION
Flocking is an important behavior in birds, constituting also antipredator behavior by prey. In general, animals in larger groups will detect predators earlier (many/multiple eyes/ears or early‐warning hypothesis) (Lazarus, 1979). Individual group members have a lower probability of being caught by a predator (“dilution hypothesis”) in larger groups (Lima, 1995; Lima & Dill, 1990; Ydenberg & Dill, 1986). Other advantages of being a member of a large group are that individuals spend more time feeding and less time vigilant as group size increases (Lima & Dill, 1990) and that large groups could quickly respond to new situations (Liker & Bókony, 2009). Foraging prey that have noticed a predator should make a decision, either stay or escape, thereby balancing possible benefits (e.g., decreased capture probability) and costs (e.g., abandoning a food patch, reduced time spent foraging, and increased energy use for locomotion) (Frid & Dill, 2002). Many studies have indicated that prey are more vigilant when predation risk is high (Caro, 2005; Frid & Dill, 2002).
Flight initiation distance (FID) is defined as the distance at which animals take flight from approaching threats (Blumstein, 2013; Hediger, 1934). This behavioral trait has been used as a surrogate for antipredator or fear behavior in many ecological studies (Blumstein, 2006; Glover, Weston, Maguire, Miller, & Christie, 2011; Legagneux & Ducatez, 2013; Møller, 2008a; Møller, Grim, Ibáñez‐Álamo, Markó, & Tryjanowski, 2013; Weston, Mcleod, Blumstein, & Guay, 2012). Briefly, this measure indicates when individuals take more risk (i.e., delayed escape) or take less risk (i.e., escape earlier) (Sol et al., 2018). Many studies of escape behavior in birds focused on the influence of external factors affecting behavioral responses: habitat quality (Burger, Gochfeld, Jenkins, & Lesser, 2010), the direction of approach by predators (Møller & Tryjanowski, 2014), intruder starting distance (Blumstein, 2013; Glover et al., 2011), number or density of intruders (Geist, Liao, Libby, & Blumstein, 2001), population density (Mikula, 2014), urbanization (Samia et al., 2017), road speed limits (Legagneux & Ducatez, 2013), insular distribution (Cooper, Pyron, & Garland, 2014), predator–prey interactions (Møller, 2008b), spatial gradients of predator abundance (Díaz et al., 2013), or daytime and season when FID was measured (Burger & Gochfeld, 1991; Piratelli, Favoretto, & de Almeida Maximiano, 2015). Blumstein (2006) has made links between escape behavior and life history and natural history traits (e.g., diet) in birds. Furthermore, earlier studies assumed that animals will respond to human approach in a similar way as they do when responding to predation (Bötsch, Gugelmann, Tablado, & Jenni, 2018; Frid & Dill, 2002; Møller & Tryjanowski, 2014; Morelli et al., 2018; Weston et al., 2012).
According to Ydenberg and Dill (1986), FID increases with the risk of capture and the increasing cost of flight. Large species of birds have long FIDs, because larger species need more time to get airborne and hence avoid capture (Fernández‐Juricic et al., 2006; Hemmingsen, 1951; Møller, 2008c; Weston et al., 2012). We know that birds from rural areas tend to escape earlier than birds from urban areas, being less tolerant of humans, probably because urban birds live under lower predation risk than their rural counterparts (Møller, 2015; Samia et al., 2017), because urban birds have become adapted or habituated to the presence of humans (Carrete & Tella, 2013; Holtmann, Santos, Lara, & Nakagawa, 2017), or because local selection for bolder individuals has occurred (van Dongen, Robinson, Weston, Mulder, & Guay, 2015). Additionally, we know that behavioral responses of animals to human approach such as FID can be useful for conservation purposes, namely management of disturbance, especially in human‐dominated environments (Guay, Dongen, Robinson, Blumstein, & Weston, 2016; Weston et al., 2012). However, we know very little about the intraspecific factors that can be involved in variation in FID. Group size has been suggested to be another important component that influences escape decisions by prey (Burger & Gochfeld, 1991; Fernández‐Juricic, Jimenez, & Lucas, 2002; Glover et al., 2011; Samia et al., 2017; Yasué, 2005). However, the relationships between group size and FID have been diverse (Deboelpaep, Keleman, Vanschoenwinkel, & Koedam, 2018; Lima & Dill, 1990; Ydenberg & Dill, 1986). According to the early‐warning hypothesis, a larger flock will flee earlier, that is, having a longer FID, because it will detect a predator earlier despite per capita decreases in vigilance rates. However, according to the dilution hypotheses, the cost of remaining may be smaller in larger flocks, that is, FID will be reduced. It is also possible that if foraging efficiency is superior in larger than in smaller groups, then any response of the predator may be delayed, causing a shorter FID.
Predation has been shown to be an important selective force affecting patterns of sociality, such as grouping (Lima & Dill, 1990). Vigilance in response to predators as a social phenomenon has been studied intensively as a component of antipredator behavior (Caro, 2005). In general, members in large groups spend less time vigilant (Caro, 2005; Lima & Dill, 1990). However, a large amount of variation in the relationships between group size and vigilance remains unexplained, for example, due to the spatial position of group members in the flock, dominance status, sex, and probably other factors (Beauchamp, 2008; Ydenberg & Dill, 1986). It is also possible that vigilance could firstly decline and thereafter increase when group size increases (Wang, Li, Beauchamp, & Jiang, 2011). Møller (2015) reviewed the literature on FID in birds and showed that sociality is an important factor influencing FID. There is less information available for other classes of animals (Cooper & Blumstein, 2015). Under classical ecological models of predation risk, which predict a decrease in individual risk when group size increases (Alexander, 1974; Pulliam, 1973), we would expect a reduction in FID among individuals in large groups through the dilution effect (Fernández‐Juricic et al., 2002; Pulliam, 1973; Roberts, 1996). However, the opposite response could also be expected (increasing FID with increasing flock size) because fear responses may be socially transmitted (Griffin, 2004), as is early detection of predators by large groups (Hingee & Magrath, 2009; Stankowich & Blumstein, 2005). Awareness or nervous reactions can be positively related to flock size because of higher vigilance (effect of many eyes scanning for predators) (Pulliam, 1973). Thus, under threat, individuals in large flocks should react more rapidly than solitary individuals or individuals in small groups, as a consequence of cascade effects or contagious alertness.
Although the literature on fear responses and sociality is limited, cooperative breeders are known to be more alarmed than species with other breeding systems (Blumstein, 2006), which is consistent with the second hypothesis. The study by Laursen, Kahlert, and Frikke (2005) showed that, in different species of waterbirds, FID increased with flock size. These findings are inconsistent with dilution effects, because if each individual in a flock experienced a smaller risk, we should expect a shorter FID in larger flocks. In contrast, the results are consistent with effects of many eyes scanning for the presence of a predator, although differences in phenotypic composition of differently sized flocks may be an alternative explanation for these findings. Finally, Tätte, Møller, and Mänd (2018) showed that flock size increased FID, but not the distance fled.
Gregariousness is common in nature and can be defined as the tendency to live in flocks (Miller, 1922). A flock is a term used to define any aggregation of homogeneous individuals, that live, travel, or feed together, regardless of size or density (Emlen, 1952). As pointed out by Miller (1922), probably the most obvious advantage of gregarious behavior in birds is that it affords a multiplicity of eyes, increasing the probability of sighting a potential predator or prey. Thus, any defensive measures can be taken early, increasing the probability of successful escape when encountering a risk (Miller, 1922).
We hypothesized that FID in response to human approach would be longer in individuals aggregated in large flocks. The ecological rationale for this expectation is that vigilance and FID increase with group size because of the many eyes effect, and vigilance can be transmitted more easily (or quickly) when there are many individuals in a flock. As a consequence, the aim of this study was to test whether FID in birds increases with the number of conspecifics (flock size), focusing on differences between environments, latitudes, and species’ traits such as diet. We explored differences in FID between environments because previous studies suggested significant differences between urban and rural birds (Díaz et al., 2013; Piratelli et al., 2015; Samia et al., 2017). Additionally, we focused on potential differences associated with diet in an effort to test whether foraging strategies can affect the escape behavior of species. Flock size was measured as the number of individuals of the same species aggregated in a group. We focused on gregarious bird species because such species tend to stay in groups. In this study, we used body mass‐corrected FID throughout, because large‐bodied species require more effort to get airborne.
2. METHODS
2.1. Study area and flight initiation distance
Data were collected during the breeding period in each study area (April–September 2015) using a standard protocol (Blumstein, 2006; Samia et al., 2017) in urban and adjacent rural areas of eight cities in eight European countries: Czech Republic, Denmark, Estonia, Finland, France, Hungary, Poland, and Spain (Figure 1; Table S1). Because the wide latitudinal gradient in our study, FID data were collected in all localities during a comparable period using a narrow temporal phenological window according to latitude, in order to control any effect of seasonality (Weston, Ju, Guay, & Naismith, 2018). For the same reason, we focused our study almost exclusively on adult individuals during the peak of the breeding season, in each country.
We used a study design collecting data in urban and rural sites (habitat type) in each study location, because a large amount of literature highlights the main differences between urban and rural environments, in terms of responses of birds to risk of predation (Møller, 2012; Møller et al., 2013; Samia et al., 2017; Sol et al., 2018). The distance between each pair of urban and rural site was always shorter than 20 km (with a minimum of 3.5 km). The sites classified as “urban” were characterized by areas with multistory buildings or by areas with single‐family houses (suburban areas). The sites classified as “rural” were dominated by open farmland with scattered houses (Samia et al., 2017). For the classification of urban and rural habitats, we followed the definitions provided in Marzluff, Bowman, and Donnelly (2001). Urban habitats were defined as areas with at least 50% built‐up area, building density >10 buildings/ha, and a residential human density >10 humans/ha. Rural habitats were defined as the areas with 5%–20% built‐up areas, a building density <2.5 buildings/ha, and residential human density between 1 and 10 humans/ha (Marzluff et al., 2001).
Observers used binoculars to identify birds that were foraging or engaged in “relaxed behavior” (i.e., roosting or preening). Flight initiation distance observation is considered reliable even when collected by different observers (Guay, McLeod, et al., 2013b). Highly vigilant or obviously alarmed individuals were not approached. Furthermore, data from breeding sites (e.g., from gull colonies) or anthropogenic feeding sites (e.g., rubbish dumps) were not collected, in order to reduce disturbance (breeding sites) or avoid an excessive effect of artificial food source on the behavior of birds. Each individual bird was approached in a straight line by the observer walking at a constant speed (0.5 m/s). Starting distance was measured as the distance at which an observer started the approach to the bird, in meters (Blumstein, 2013). Flight initiation distance was measured as the distance between the observer and the point where the individual bird began to flee (see more details in Samia et al., 2017). Only bird species detected on the ground were considered. Sol et al. (2018) showed that species with more than 10 recorded cases per study site provided reliable estimates of FID. Here, we only selected bird species with more than 10 observations of FID. To avoid collecting the same flock several times, we visited every site just once.
Flock size was defined as the number of aggregated individuals of the same species, implying that individuals in flocks are closer to each other than individuals that are not in flocks. We collected data on FID for single individuals or well‐recognizable flocks, at a distance from other individuals or flocks longer than 10 m. Only single‐species flocks were targeted in this study, because mixed flocks could be problematic if the species present differ in their tolerance to humans. When birds were in a flock, we always selected the closest individual to the observer, because that individual generally would have the shortest FID.
2.2. Ecological variables: gregariousness, trophic guild, and body mass
In this study, we focused only on “gregarious” species because, by definition, such species can be found in aggregated groups. The gregariousness was classified using information from the Handbook of the Birds of the Western Palearctic (Cramp & Perrins, 1994) (Table S2). Birds were classified as “gregarious" when species have shown gregarious activities either during breeding or nonbreeding, following the classification made in the same book (Cramp & Perrins, 1994). There was a positive correlation between gregariousness during these two periods (Kendall rank order correlation τ = 0.40, p = 0.020), implying that species that were gregarious during breeding also tend to be gregarious during the nonbreeding season. We decided to group breeding and nonbreeding gregarious species, because we assumed that social cognition, that is, the capacity to communicate with other individuals belonging to the same species, is a species‐specific trait that may be manifest all the time (Yu et al., 2016, 2017).
For each gregarious species recorded in this study, we included the following information: trophic guild or diet (main type of food consumed, following the bird traits of feeding ecology provided in Pearman et al. (2014)). All species were classified into five main categories as granivorous, granivorous–insectivorous, insectivorous, carnivorous, and carrion‐eater (Table S2). Body mass for each species was obtained from the same source (Pearman et al., 2014), and this variable was log‐transformed to fit normality.
2.3. Statistical analyses
The average values of FID and flock size between birds classified on the basis of their species‐specific gregariousness were compared using the standard nonparametric Wilcoxon test (Triola, 2012).
To test the presence of a phylogenetic signal (Blomberg & Garland, 2003) in FID data for gregarious bird species, we used Blomberg's K statistic (Blomberg, Garland, & Ives, 2003). When K approaches 1, trait evolution follows a mode of evolution that is consistent with Brownian motion, and if K > 1, close relatives are more similar than expected under Brownian motion and indicate a strong phylogenetic signal, while K values closer to zero correspond to a random or convergent pattern of evolution, and that closely related species are less similar than expected (Blomberg et al., 2003). Blomberg's K statistic was estimated using the R package “phylosignal” (Keck, Rimet, Bouchez, & Franc, 2016). To control for the phylogenetic relationship among species, we used phylogenetic generalized least square regression models to analyze the changes in FID behavior of bird species in relation to flock size and characteristics of species. Models were fitted using the package “ape” (Paradis, Claude, & Strimmer, 2004), “nlme” (Pinheiro, Bates, DebRoy, & Sarkar, 2017), and the function “gls” with correlation equals consensus tree. We extracted the phylogenetic relationship for all 23 species from the phylogeny available online (Jetz, Thomas, Joy, Hartmann, & Mooers, 2012; Jetz et al., 2014) and obtained consensus phylogenies from 100 random trees with Mesquite (Maddison & Maddison, 2018). Different populations of the same species from different countries and habitats were defined with a relatively different branch length of 1E−18 just to fit the models. We weighted models by sample size (see more details in Garamszegi (2014)). In order to reduce any effect associated with a strong correlation between FID and body mass (Møller, Samia, Weston, Guay, & Blumstein, 2016), we first did a log–log‐linear regression for FID and body mass and use the residuals of this model to represent relative FID. This allowed us to focus on the main effects of selected predictors. The full model considered relative FID as response variable, while flock size, starting distance, habitat type (urban or rural), latitude, and diet were introduced as predictors.
All statistical tests were performed with R software version 3.2.4 (R Development Core & Team, 2017).
3. RESULTS
From a total of 5,783 observations from eight different European countries (Figure 1; Table S1), all observations of FID for the 23 gregarious birds were collected with sample size large than 10 observations per species (Table S2).
In the initial exploration of data, FID was positively correlated with body mass (Figure 2). The FID for gregarious bird species ranged from a minimum of close to 0 m to a maximum of 152 m, with a mean value = 15.2 with SD = 13 m in rural habitat and 8.7 + SD = 7 m in urban habitats. A preliminary graphical exploration showed that FID was shorter in urban than in rural habitats for the majority of gregarious birds that were the focus of this study, with the only exceptions being Corvus monedula and Parus caeruleus (Figure S1).
The FID for gregarious bird species showed a strong phylogenetic signal with a K statistic approaching 1 and with p < 0.01, suggesting a model similar to Brownian motion. The result of a phylogenetic generalized linear regression model (PGLS) showed that relative FID of individuals from rural and urban habitats was positively associated with flock size (Figure 3) and starting distance, while relative FID was shorter in urban habitats and for granivorous–insectivorous and insectivorous species (Table 1). Latitude and granivorous diet were both unrelated to the values of relative FID (Table 1).
Table 1.
Variables | Estimate | SE | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 0.109 | 0.047 | 2.335 | 0.019 |
Flock | 0.007 | 0.001 | 6.220 | <0.0001 |
Starting distance | 0.005 | 0.001 | 10.163 | <0.0001 |
Latitude | −0.001 | 0.001 | −1.218 | 0.223 |
Habitat (urban) | −0.175 | 0.012 | −14.084 | <0.0001 |
Diet (granivorous) | −0.064 | 0.038 | −1.652 | 0.097 |
Diet (granivorous–insectivorous) | −0.119 | 0.031 | −3.877 | 0.0001 |
Diet (insectivorous) | −0.149 | 0.037 | −3.992 | 0.0001 |
Models were based on data from eight European countries, weighted by sample size for species of birds. Significant variables are highlighted in bold. Model statistics: residual standard error: 0.584, degrees of freedom: 1,146 total; 1,137 residual, R 2 = 0.202. SE: standard error.
4. DISCUSSION
The main finding of this study of FID in birds was that FID increased with flock size in European gregarious bird species, in rural and urban habitat. Gregarious species may be more susceptible to human disturbance than nongregarious species, both at the individual and probably at the population levels (Weston et al., 2012). Our statistical model also confirmed the positive association between FID and starting distance of observer, already shown in other studies (Blumstein, 2013). In addition, relative FID tended to decrease in urban habitats when compared with rural areas, confirming previous studies (Díaz et al., 2013; Møller et al., 2016; Samia et al., 2017; Weston et al., 2012). In this study on European gregarious birds, diet was significantly associated with relative FID, with insectivorous and granivorous–insectivorous species having the shortest relative FID (Figure S2). We believe that this association between foraging strategy or diet and escape behavior of birds deserves further study, as suggested also in a previous study (Blumstein, 2006).
We tried to test indirectly the two hypotheses presented in the Introduction: the dilution effect (Stankowich & Blumstein, 2005) and the many eyes effect (Hingee & Magrath, 2009; Stankowich & Blumstein, 2005). According to these hypotheses, larger flocks have more individuals scanning for predators, but larger flocks also result in greater dilution effects because the risk of mortality is smaller for each individual in a larger flock. Here, mainly using Passeriformes, we have shown a positive relationship between FID and flock size, which supports the many eyes effect hypothesis. A similar general pattern was previously reported by Laursen et al. (2005) for waterbirds and by Glover et al. (2011) for red‐necked stint Calidris ruficollis, while other studies suggested no influence of flock size on FID for the particular case of black swans Cygnus atratus (Guay, Lorenz, Robinson, Symonds, & Weston, 2013) or for other bird species (Fernández‐Juricic et al., 2006; Guay, McLeod, et al., 2013b) or negative association between flock size and FID for waders (Charadriiformes) (Mikula et al., 2018). The present study of mainly passerine birds compares well with that by Laursen et al. (2005) for waterbirds comprised of geese, ducks, waders, and gulls. In fact, both studies have very similar effect sizes despite the taxa being completely different, but the association with urbanization was focused only in our study. However, hunting activities affected FID in the study by Laursen et al. (2005), but not in our study in which only three of 23 focal species were hunted: mallard Anas platyrhynchos, wood pigeon Columba palumbus, and rook Corvus frugilegus. Indeed Laursen et al. (2005) showed an association between flock size and FID for nine waterbird species in fall, when hunting is common, but only for two species in spring when hunting ceased.
Vigilance in large groups can provide increased capacity to detect predators, thereby allowing individuals to spend additional time on foraging activities (Olson, Haley, Dyer, & Adami, 2015). Longer FID in larger flocks of a given species implies that individuals on average run higher risk in small flocks. This could either be due to such larger flocks being composed of individuals of lower phenotypic quality, or that individuals in small flocks with short FID run higher risks of mortality. Indeed, Møller (2014) has shown that barn swallow Hirundo rustica individuals with short FID are more likely to be caught by sparrowhawks Accipiter nisus. In addition, bird species with shorter FID are more vulnerable to predation by raptors (Møller, Nielsen, & Garamszegi, 2008) and cats Felis catus (Møller, Berthold, & Fiedler, 2010), but also to be killed by cars (Møller, Erritzøe, & Erritzøe, 2011). An alternative interpretation when measuring FID in flocks of many individuals could be that the first individual responder may set off a social escape response. In that case, FID would not reflect average tolerance of the group to predators, but rather the least tolerant individual in the flock (e.g., large flocks may flush earlier because there is a probability that they contain especially sensitive individuals). However, our study does not allow discrimination between these different hypotheses.
We assumed that gregarious species would encounter conspecifics more often than nongregarious species (Emlen, 1952). For this reason, we hypothesized that individuals of gregarious species are better adapted at extracting information from other individuals than nongregarious species, and then, their escape behavior could be affected by the number of surrounding individuals. The increased skill to exploit information from other individuals and signal effectively should improve the efficiency of the group at detection and defense against predators (Krebs, MacRoberts, & Cullen, 1972; Treisman, 1975). Thus, individuals belonging to gregarious species would experience a trade‐off between foraging (or resting) under the safety of the presence of many conspecifics and hence long FIDs, or such individuals may have short FIDs in the presence of few conspecifics (Laursen et al., 2005). Accordingly, our results highlighted that relative FID was positively associated with flock size in European gregarious birds.
We explicitly recorded FID observations from urban and rural habitats. While previous studies have shown consistently longer FIDs in rural than in urban habitats (Samia et al., 2017), we are only aware of a single other study investigating the independent effects of rural versus urban habitats and flock size on FID (Tätte et al., 2018). The latter study also found a similar effect of flock size on FID in rural and urban habitats. Our results suggested also that FID in urban areas tends to be shorter than in rural areas. However, the positive association between flock size and FID found for gregarious species was similar across the two types of habitats.
Our findings suggest that future studies on escape behavior of birds should explicitly consider flock size, at least in gregarious bird species. The influence of the many eyes effect in the presence of numerous conspecifics can significantly alter the escape behavior of social birds. We hypothesize that experimental change in sociality will affect FID. Indeed, Laursen, Møller, and Holm (2016) have shown that flock size changes adaptively in response to intense hunting. We predicted that such changes in immediate risk will be accompanied by similarly directed changes in FID. In conclusion, relative FID increased with flock size in gregarious species, independently of the rural versus urban areas. Our results support the role of sociality for risk‐taking behavior and hence for social organization. These conclusions have broad biological implications, especially considering the role of sociality (gregariousness) as possible factor facilitating colonization of urban environments and adaptation to such human‐impacted environments.
ETHICS STATEMENTS
In this study, we did not capture or band birds, but only estimated of flight initiation distance (FID) based on observations of long‐distance behavior. The jurisdictions and institutions involved in this study required neither ethics clearance nor research permits for the noninvasive methodology employed.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The author(s) declare not to have any conflict of interest or competing interests.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
F.M., Y.B., and A.P.M. planned the research. A.P.M., M.D., T.G., J.D.I.A., J.J., M.L.K.J, K.T., G.M., and P.T. collected data and curated the dataset. F.M., Y.B., and Y.J performed the data analyses and prepared the figures. F.M., Y.B, and A.P.M. wrote the first draft of the paper. All the authors contributed to the writing, and read and approved the final version of the manuscript.
Supporting information
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
F.M. and Y.B. were financially supported by the Czech Science Foundation GAČR (project number 18‐16738S) during the data analyses and writing of this paper. K.T. was supported by institutional research funding IUT (34‐8) of the Estonian Ministry of Education and Research. During the study, G.M. was supported by the Higher Education Institutional Excellence Program (1783‐3/2018/FEKUTSTRAT).
Morelli F, Benedetti Y, Díaz M, et al. Contagious fear: Escape behavior increases with flock size in European gregarious birds. Ecol Evol. 2019;9:6096–6104. 10.1002/ece3.5193
DATA ACCESSIBILITY
After manuscript acceptance, data will be archived in an appropriate public repository or journal website. Data available from the Dryad Digital Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.3q25r77.
REFERENCES
- Alexander, R. D. (1974). The evolution of social behavior. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 5, 325–383. 10.1146/annurev.es.05.110174.001545 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Beauchamp, G. (2008). What is the magnitude of the group‐size effect on vigilance? Behavioral Ecology, 19, 1361–1368. 10.1371/journal.pone.0018631 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Blomberg, S. P. , & Garland, T. (2003). Tempo and mode in evolution: Phylogenetic inertia, adaptation and comparative methods. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 15, 899–910. 10.1046/j.1420-9101.2002.00472.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Blomberg, S. P. , Garland, T. , & Ives, A. R. A. R. (2003). Testing for phylogenetic signal in comparative data: Behavioral traits are more labile. Evolution, 57, 717–745. 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2003.tb00285.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumstein, D. T. (2006). Developing an evolutionary ecology of fear: How life history and natural history traits affect disturbance tolerance in birds. Animal Behaviour, 71, 389–399. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.05.010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Blumstein, D. T. (2013). Flight‐initiation distance in birds is dependent on intruder starting distance. The Journal of Wildlife Management, 67, 852–857. 10.2307/3802692 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bötsch, Y. , Gugelmann, S. , Tablado, Z. , & Jenni, L. (2018). Effect of human recreation on bird anti‐predatory response. PeerJ, 6, e5093 10.7717/peerj.5093 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burger, J. , & Gochfeld, M. (1991). Human activity influence and diurnal and nocturnal foraging of Sanderlings (Calidris alba). Condor, 93, 259–265. 10.2307/1368941 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Burger, J. , Gochfeld, M. , Jenkins, C. D. , & Lesser, F. (2010). Effect of approaching boats on nesting black skimmers: Using response distances to establish protective buffer zones. Journal of Wildlife Management, 74, 102–108. 10.2193/2008-576 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Caro, T. M. (2005). Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals. Chicago, IL and London, UK: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Carrete, M. , & Tella, J. L. (2013). High individual consistency in fear of humans throughout the adult lifespan of rural and urban burrowing owls. Scientific Reports, 3, 3524 10.1038/srep03524 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper, J. W. E. , Pyron, R. A. , & Garland, T. (2014) Island tameness: living on islands reduces flight initiation distance. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281, 20133019 10.1098/rspb.2013.3019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper, W. E. J. , & Blumstein, D. T. (2015). Escaping from predators: An integrative view of escape decisions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Cramp, S. , & Perrins, C. (1994). The birds of the western Palearctic. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Deboelpaep, E. , Keleman, P. J. , Vanschoenwinkel, B. , & Koedam, N. (2018). Gallant geese, fearful flocks? Flock size and heterospecifics alter the escape behaviour of an invasive goose. Belgian Journal of Zoology, 148, 135–147. 10.26496/bjz.2018.23 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Díaz, M. , Møller, A. P. , Flensted‐Jensen, E. , Grim, T. , Ibáñez‐Álamo, J. D. , Jokimäki, J. , … Tryjanowski, P. (2013). The geography of fear: A latitudinal gradient in anti‐predator escape distances of birds across Europe. PLoS ONE, 8, e64634 10.1371/journal.pone.0064634 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Emlen, J. T. (1952). Flocking behavior in birds. The Auk, 69, 160–170. 10.2307/4081266 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fernández‐Juricic, E. , Blumstein, D. T. , Abrica, G. , Manriquez, L. , Adams, L. B. , Adams, R. , … Rodriguez‐Prieto, I. (2006). Relationships of anti‐predator escape and post‐escape responses with body mass and morphology: A comparative avian study. Evolutionary Ecology Research, 8, 731–752. [Google Scholar]
- Fernández‐Juricic, E. , Jimenez, M. D. , & Lucas, E. (2002). Factors affecting intra‐ and inter‐specific variations in the difference between alert distances and flight distances for birds in forested habitats. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 80, 1212–1220. 10.1139/z02-104 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Frid, A. , & Dill, L. M. (2002). Human‐caused disturbance stimuli as a form of predation risk. Conservation Ecology, 6, 11 10.5751/ES-00404-060111 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Garamszegi, L. Z. (2014). Uncertainties Due to Within-Species Variation in Comparative Studies: Measurement Errors and Statistical Weights In Garamszegi L. Z., (Ed.). Modern phylogenetic comparative methods and their application in evolutionary biology. New York, NY: Springer Verlag. [Google Scholar]
- Geist, C. , Liao, J. , Libby, S. , & Blumstein, D. T. (2001). Does intruder group size and orientation affect flight initiation distance in birds? Animal Biodiversity and Conservation, 28, 69–73. [Google Scholar]
- Glover, H. K. , Weston, M. A. , Maguire, G. S. , Miller, K. K. , & Christie, B. A. (2011). Towards ecologically meaningful and socially acceptable buffers: Response distances of shorebirds in Victoria, Australia, to human disturbance. Landscape and Urban Planning, 103, 326–334. 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2011.08.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Griffin, A. S. (2004). Social learning about predators: A review and prospectus. Animal Learning & Behavior, 32, 131–140. 10.3758/BF03196014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guay, P.‐J. , Lorenz, R. D. A. , Robinson, R. W. , Symonds, M. R. E. , & Weston, M. A. (2013). Distance from water, sex and approach direction influence flight distances among habituated black swans. Ethology, 119, 552–558. 10.1111/eth.12094 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Guay, P.‐J. , McLeod, E. M. , Cross, R. , Formby, A. J. , Maldonado, S. P. , Stafford‐Bell, R. E. , … Weston, M. A. (2013b). Observer effects occur when estimating alert but not flight‐initiation distances. Wildlife Research, 40, 289–293. [Google Scholar]
- Guay, P.‐J. , van Dongen, W. F. D. , Robinson, R. W. , Blumstein, D. T. , & Weston, M. A. (2016). AvianBuffer: An interactive tool for characterising and managing wildlife fear responses. Ambio, 45, 841–851. 10.1007/s13280-016-0779-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hediger, H. (1934). Zur Biologie und Psychologie der Flucht bei Tieren. Biologisches Zentralblatt, 54, 21–40. [Google Scholar]
- Hemmingsen, A. (1951). The relation of shyness (flushing distance) to body size. Spolia Zool Musei Hauniensis, 11, 74–76. [Google Scholar]
- Hingee, M. , & Magrath, R. D. (2009). Flights of fear: A mechanical wing whistle sounds the alarm in a flocking bird. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276, 4173–4179. 10.1098/rspb.2009.1110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holtmann, B. , Santos, E. S. A. , Lara, C. E. , & Nakagawa, S. (2017). Personality‐matching habitat choice, rather than behavioural plasticity, is a likely driver of a phenotype‐environment covariance. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 284, 20170943 10.1098/rspb.2017.0943 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jetz, W. , Thomas, G. H. , Joy, J. B. , Hartmann, K. , & Mooers, A. O. (2012). The global diversity of birds in space and time. Nature, 491, 444–448. 10.1038/nature11631 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jetz, W. , Thomas, G. H. , Joy, J. B. , Redding, D. W. , Hartmann, K. , & Mooers, A. Ø. (2014). Global distribution and conservation of evolutionary distinctness in birds. Current Biology, 24, 919–930. 10.1016/j.cub.2014.03.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keck, F. , Rimet, F. , Bouchez, A. , & Franc, A. (2016). Phylosignal: An R package to measure, test, and explore the phylogenetic signal. Ecology and Evolution, 6, 2774–2780. 10.1002/ece3.2051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krebs, J. R. , MacRoberts, M. H. , & Cullen, J. M. (1972). Flocking and feeding in the Great tit Parus major ‐ An experimental study. Ibis, 114, 507–530. 10.1111/j.1474-919X.1972.tb00852.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Laursen, K. , Kahlert, J. , & Frikke, J. (2005). Factors affecting escape distances of staging waterbirds. Wildlife Biology, 11, 13–19. 10.2981/0909-6396(2005)11[13:FAEDOS]2.0.CO;2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Laursen, K. , Møller, A. P. , & Holm, T. E. (2016). Dynamic group size and displacement as avoidance strategies by eiders in response to hunting. Wildlife Biology, 22, 174–181. 10.2981/wlb.00197 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lazarus, J. (1979). The early warning function of flocking in birds: An experimental study with captive quelea. Animal Behaviour, 27, 855–865. 10.1016/0003-3472(79)90023-X [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Legagneux, P. , & Ducatez, S. (2013). European birds adjust their flight initiation distance to road speed limits. Biology Letters, 9, 20130417 10.1098/rsbl.2013.0417 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liker, A. , & Bókony, V. (2009). Larger groups are more successful in innovative problem solving in house sparrows. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 7893–7898. 10.1073/pnas.0900042106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lima, S. L. (1995). Back to the basics of anti‐predatory vigilance: The group‐size effect. Animal Behaviour, 49, 11–20. 10.1016/0003-3472(95)80149-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lima, S. L. , & Dill, L. M. (1990). Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: A review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 68, 619–640. 10.1139/z90-092 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Maddison, W. P. , & Maddison, D. R. (2018). Mesquite: A modular system for evolutionary analysis. Version 3.51.
- Marzluff, J. , Bowman, R. , & Donnelly, R. (2001). Avian ecology and conservation in an urbanizing world. New York, NY: Springer Science. [Google Scholar]
- Mikula, P. (2014). Pedestrian density influences flight distances of urban birds. Ardea, 102, 53–60. 10.5253/078.102.0105 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mikula, P. , Díaz, M. , Albrecht, T. , Jokimäki, J. , Kaisanlahti‐Jokimäki, M. L. , Kroitero, G. , … Hromada, M. (2018). Adjusting risk‐taking to the annual cycle of long‐distance migratory birds. Scientific Reports, 8, 13989 10.1038/s41598-018-32252-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller, R. C. (1922). The significance of the gregarious habit. Ecology, 3, 122–126. 10.2307/1929145 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Møller, A. P. (2008a). Flight distance and population trends in European breeding birds. Behavioral Ecology, 19, 1095–1102. 10.1093/beheco/arn103 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Møller, A. P. (2008b). Interactions between interactions: Predator‐prey, parasite‐host, and mutualistic interactions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1133, 180–186. 10.1196/annals.1438.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Møller, A. P. (2008c). Flight distance of urban birds, predation and selection for urban life. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 63, 63–75. 10.1038/srep13723 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Møller, A. P. (2012). Urban areas as refuges from predators and flight distance of prey. Behavioral Ecology, 23, 1030–1035. 10.1093/beheco/ars067 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Møller, A. P. (2014). Life history, predation and flight initiation distance in a migratory bird. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 27, 1105–1113. 10.1111/jeb.12399 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Møller, A. P. (2015). Birds In Cooper W. E. J., & Blumstein D. T. (Eds.), Escaping from predators: An integrative view of escape decisions and refuge use (pp. 88–112). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Møller, A. P. , Berthold, P. , & Fiedler, W. (2010). Effects of climate change on birds. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Møller, A. P. , Erritzøe, H. , & Erritzøe, J. (2011). A behavioral ecology approach to traffic accidents: Interspecific variation in causes of traffic casualties among birds. Zoological Research, 32, 115–127. 10.3724/SP.J.1141.2011.02115 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Møller, A. P. , Grim, T. , Ibáñez‐Álamo, J. D. , Markó, G. , & Tryjanowski, P. (2013). Change in flight initiation distance between urban and rural habitats following a cold winter. Behavioral Ecology, 24, 1211–1217. 10.1093/beheco/art054 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Møller, A. P. , Nielsen, J. T. , & Garamszegi, L. Z. (2008). Risk taking by singing males. Behavioral Ecology, 19, 41–53. 10.1093/beheco/arm098 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Møller, A. P. , Samia, D. S. M. , Weston, M. A. , Guay, P. J. , & Blumstein, D. T. (2016). Flight initiation distances in relation to sexual dichromatism and body size in birds from three continents. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 117, 823–831. 10.1111/bij.12706 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Møller, A. P. , & Tryjanowski, P. (2014). Direction of approach by predators and flight initiation distance of urban and rural populations of birds. Behavioral Ecology, 25, 960–966. 10.1093/beheco/aru073 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Morelli, F. , Mikula, P. , Benedetti, Y. , Bussière, R. , Jerzak, L. , & Tryjanowski, P. (2018). Escape behaviour of birds in urban parks and cemeteries across Europe: Evidence of behavioural adaptation to human activity. Science of the Total Environment, 631–632, 803–810. 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2018.03.118 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olson, R. S. , Haley, P. B. , Dyer, F. C. , & Adami, C. (2015). Exploring the evolution of a trade‐off between vigilance and foraging in group‐living organisms. Royal Society Open Science, 2, 150135 10.1098/rsos.150135 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paradis, E. , Claude, J. , & Strimmer, K. (2004). APE: Analyses of phylogenetics and evolution in R language. Bioinformatics, 20, 289–290. 10.1093/bioinformatics/btg412 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pearman, P. B. , Lavergne, S. , Roquet, C. , Wüest, R. , Zimmermann, N. E. , & Thuiller, W. (2014). Phylogenetic patterns of climatic, habitat and trophic niches in a European avian assemblage. Global Ecology and Biogeography, 23, 414–424. 10.1111/geb.12127 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinheiro, J. , Bates, D. , DebRoy, S. , Sarkar, D. , &R Core Team .(2017). nlme: Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. R package version 3.1‐131.
- Piratelli, A. J. , Favoretto, G. R. , & de Almeida Maximiano, M. F. (2015). Factors affecting escape distance in birds. Zoologia (Curitiba), 32, 438–444. 10.1590/s1984-46702015000600002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pulliam, H. (1973). On the advantages of flocking. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 38, 419–422. 10.1016/0022-5193(73)90184-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Development Core Team . (2017). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing.
- Roberts, G. (1996). Why individual vigilance declines as group size increases. Animal Behaviour, 51, 1077–1086. 10.1006/anbe.1996.0109 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Samia, D. S. M. , Blumstein, D. T. , Diaz, M. , Grim, T. , Ibáñez‐Álamo, J. D. , Jokimäki, J. , … Møller, A. P. (2017). Rural‐urban differences in escape behavior of European birds across a latitudinal gradient. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 5, 66 10.3389/fevo.2017.00066 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sol, D. , Maspons, J. , Gonzalez‐Voyer, A. , Morales‐Castilla, I. , Garamszegi, L. Z. , & Møller, A. P. (2018). Risk‐taking behavior, urbanization and the pace of life in birds. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 72, 59 10.1007/s00265-018-2463-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stankowich, T. , & Blumstein, D. T. (2005). Fear in animals: a meta‐analysis and review of risk assessment. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 272, 2627–2634. 10.1098/rspb.2005.3251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tätte, K. , Møller, A. P. , & Mänd, R. (2018). Towards an integrated view of escape decisions in birds: Relationship between flight initiation distance and distance fled. Animal Behaviour, 136, 75–86. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.12.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Treisman, M. (1975). Predation and the evolution of gregariousness. I. Models for concealment and evasion. Animal Behaviour, 23, 779–800. 10.1016/0003-3472(75)90106-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Triola, M. F. (2012). Elementary statistics, 12th ed. London, UK: Pearson International. [Google Scholar]
- van Dongen, W. F. D. , Robinson, R. W. , Weston, M. A. , Mulder, R. A. , & Guay, P.‐J. (2015). Variation at the DRD4 locus is associated with wariness and local site selection in urban black swans. BMC Evolutionary Biology, 15, 253 10.1186/s12862-015-0533-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang, Z. , Li, Z. , Beauchamp, G. , & Jiang, Z. (2011). Flock size and human disturbance affect vigilance of endangered red‐crowned cranes (Grus japonensis). Biological Conservation, 144, 101–105. 10.1016/j.biocon.2010.06.025 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Weston, M. A. , Ju, Y.‐K. , Guay, P.‐J. , & Naismith, C. (2018). A test of the “Leave Early and Avoid Detection” (LEAD) hypothesis for passive nest defenders. The Wilson Bulletin, 130, 1011–1013. 10.1676/1559-4491.130.4.1011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Weston, M. A. , Mcleod, E. M. , Blumstein, D. T. , & Guay, P. J. (2012). A review of flight‐initiation distances and their application to managing disturbance to Australian birds. Emu ‐ Austral Ornithology, 112, 269–286. 10.1071/MU12026 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Yasué, M. (2005). The effects of human presence, flock size and prey density on shorebird foraging rates. Journal of Ethology, 23, 199–204. 10.1007/s10164-005-0152-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ydenberg, R. C. , & Dill, L. M. (1986). The economics of fleeing from predators. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 16, 229–249. 10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60192-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Yu, J. , Wang, L. , Xing, X. , Yang, C. , Ma, J. , Møller, A. P. , … Liang, W. (2016). Barn swallows (Hirundo rustica) differentiate between common cuckoo and sparrowhawk in China: Alarm calls convey information on threat. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 70, 171–178. 10.1007/s00265-015-2036-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Yu, J. , Xing, X. , Jiang, Y. , Liang, W. , Wang, H. , & Møller, A. P. (2017). Alarm call‐based discrimination between common cuckoo and Eurasian sparrowhawk in a Chinese population of great tits. Ethology, 123, 542–550. 10.1111/eth.12624 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
After manuscript acceptance, data will be archived in an appropriate public repository or journal website. Data available from the Dryad Digital Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.3q25r77.