Abstract
Background
Substance use at an early age conveys substantial risk for later substance-related problems. A better understanding of early risk factors could result in more timely and effective intervention. This study investigated the predictive utility of the brain’s response to reward anticipation as a risk factor for early substance use initiation.
Methods
Participants were 34 children (25 male) at high risk for alcohol and other substance use disorders from a longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging study, scanned at a mean age of 10.5 years (SD = 1.2) when participants were substance-naïve. We used a monetary incentive delay task to examine the hemodynamic response of the nucleus accumbens to gain and loss anticipation. Logistic regression was used to test the hypothesis that these brain response patterns would have predictive utility over and above early externalizing behaviors and family history of substance use disorder, two key risk factors for substance use problems, in differentiating those who initiated substance use before age 16 (n = 18) and those who did not (n = 16).
Results
Greater nucleus accumbens activation during monetary gain anticipation in childhood increased the likelihood of initiating substance use during early adolescence (p = .023). The model that comprised neural data in addition to early externalizing behaviors and family history showed significantly better fit than the model without neural data (χ22 = 7.38, p = .025).
Conclusions
Heightened gain anticipation activation in the nucleus accumbens may predispose individuals to early substance use, beyond the risk conveyed by other known factors.
Keywords: Monetary Incentive Delay (MID), Nucleus Accumbens (NAcc), functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI), Externalizing, Family History, Logistic Regression
1. Introduction
Adolescence is marked by heightened risk for maladaptive behaviors including experimentation with drugs and alcohol. Despite its prevalence (Miech et al., 2018), adolescent substance use is associated with negative outcomes, including problematic use and substance use disorder (SUD) (Dawson et al., 2008; Grant and Dawson, 1997, 1998; King and Chassin, 2007). Animal work suggests that adolescent substance use may have neurotoxic effects on the brain, leading to long-term, harmful alterations in brain function (O’Shea et al., 2004; Taffe et al., 2010). Attempts to ameliorate the negative consequences of early substance use initiation may be aided by a better understanding of risk factors that contribute to such use, particularly those operating at the neural level.
Reward system functioning, mediated by the mesolimbic dopaminergic reward pathway, has been identified as a likely neurobiological risk marker for problem substance use (McBride and Li, 1998; Piazza et al., 1991; Volkow and Morales, 2015; Volkow et al., 2002) due to its critical role in incentive processing (Schultz, 1998) and the reinforcing properties of drugs of abuse (Robinson and Berridge, 2000). Drugs of abuse activate the mesolimbic dopamine reward pathway, as do natural rewards such as food and conditioned rewards such as money. Developmental changes in reward system functioning beginning in preadolescence are proposed to contribute to the risk-taking and impulsivity characteristic of this age group (see Casey and Jones, 2010 for a review). As youth transition into adolescence, “developmental mismatch” or “dual systems” models posit that subcortical reward system structures such as the ventral striatum (VS)—including nucleus accumbens (NAcc)—mature earlier and operate with a different trajectory relative to prefrontal control systems (Chambers et al., 2003; Galvan et al., 2006; Shulman et al., 2016; Somerville et al., 2011; Somerville et al., 2010). In addition, compared with both childhood and adulthood, D1 and D2 dopamine receptor density in the striatum is highest in early adolescence (Tarazi and Baldessarini, 2000). Whereas these developmental changes may underlie adolescent-typical behaviors that contribute to substance use risk, not all youth engage in impulsive, sensation-seeking, and risk-taking behaviors such as substance use. Individual variability in reward system functioning assessed before exposure to drugs and alcohol is likely to be informative in determining who is at the highest risk for substance use in adolescence.
Recent evidence suggests that mesolimbic dopamine is specifically related to the anticipatory rather than consummatory component of motivated behavior (Salamone and Correa, 2012; Schott et al., 2008). The monetary incentive delay (MID) task (Knutson et al., 2000) in conjunction with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has been used to characterize the neural correlates of this anticipatory component: Anticipation of monetary gain and loss during this task robustly induces activation of the VS (including NAcc (Carter et al., 2009)), one of the central components of the reward pathway. Importantly, this fMRI signal has been found to reflect phasic dopamine activity (Knutson and Gibbs, 2007). Note that herein we use the term reward anticipation more broadly than gain anticipation. That is, the former reflects the response of the reward system to the anticipation of both positive and negative incentives (Carter et al., 2009), whereas the latter refers specifically to the anticipation of winning money in the context of the MID or similar task.
There are opposing positions, however, regarding the association between pre-morbid reward functioning and risk for problem substance use. One theory asserts that a hyper-responsive reward system is indicative of reward-seeking tendencies, including increased substance use (Hariri et al., 2006; McClure et al., 2004), whereas others propose that a hypo-responsive reward system motivates the desire for drugs and alcohol to normalize low baseline dopamine levels (Blum et al., 2000). Most prior work on reward anticipation processing in addiction has been done in adults with SUDs. For instance, two studies found evidence for hypo-responsive reward system functioning during the anticipation of monetary gains and losses in individuals who had detoxified from alcohol relative to healthy controls (Beck et al., 2009; Wrase et al., 2007). In contrast, other studies have found no differences in VS activation to anticipatory gains and losses between substance users and healthy controls (Bjork et al., 2008b; Jia et al., 2011). Prospective studies involving younger participants have also examined associations between reward circuitry functioning and later problem substance use. For instance, one study of children, adolescents, and young adults found that elevated NAcc activation during MID task gain anticipation was positively correlated with later alcohol problems (Heitzeg et al., 2014).
Studies have also examined the impact of family history of SUD (FH) on reward anticipation due to substantial evidence of higher rates of substance-related problems in the offspring of individuals with a SUD (Russell et al., 1990; Schuckit and Smith, 1996; Sher et al., 1991). For instance, one study (Yau et al., 2012) found a drinking × FH interaction whereby light-drinking FH+ individuals had less NAcc activity during the anticipation of both monetary gains and losses compared to heavy-drinking FH+ and FH-individuals. NAcc activation during gain and loss anticipation positively correlated with drinking in FH+ individuals only. In contrast, (Muller et al., 2015) found no VS activation differences between FH+ and FH-adolescents with low levels of substance use during the anticipation of monetary gains. Finally, Bjork and colleagues (Bjork et al., 2008a) also found no differences between FH+ individuals and controls on VS activation during monetary gain versus non-gain anticipation, but importantly, they did find a significant correlation between self-reported sensation seeking and gain anticipation-induced VS activity, highlighting the potential link between heightened mesolimbic gain anticipation activation and conscious behaviors associated with substance use risk.
These prior studies were conducted in samples which were not substance-naïve (Beck et al., 2009; Bjork et al., 2008b; Heitzeg et al., 2014; Jia et al., 2011; Muller et al., 2015; Wrase et al., 2007; Yau et al., 2012), or the authors did not report level of substance use (Bjork et al., 2008a). Therefore, it is unclear whether the effects were a cause or a consequence of substance use. Indeed, little is known about reward responsivity differences in youth before they initiate substance use. There is mounting evidence that substance use alters reward responsivity—particularly to non-drug rewards such as money (Bechara et al., 2002; Filbey et al., 2016; Martz et al., 2016; Nestor et al., 2010)—suggesting that previous work on past and/or current substance users are ill-equipped to speak specifically to risk. What is needed is the characterization of reward circuitry before drug and alcohol use is initiated. To our knowledge, there are no studies in substance-naïve individuals investigating reward responsivity as a marker for differentiating youth who do and do not go on to use substances.
To address this gap, we used a modified MID task during fMRI scanning to investigate the neural correlates of reward anticipation in youth before the initiation of substance use. Participants were recruited from the Michigan Longitudinal Study (MLS), a prospective community study of families enriched for alcohol and other SUDs (Zucker et al., 1996; Zucker et al., 2000). Children were scanned at a mean age of 10.5 years and tracked annually to determine if and when they initiated drug and/or alcohol use. Anatomically defined NAcc activity and logistic regression were used to differentiate the initiation and comparison groups. Specifically, we sought to determine whether, among substance-naïve children, those who go on to initiate substance use at an early age would show hypo- or hyper-active reward responding to incentive cues. Here we define early substance initiation as more than a sip of alcohol or any use of other substances before the age of 16. Based on developmental changes in dopaminergic reward systems and the studies addressing substance use risk (rather than past/current substance use) reviewed above, we hypothesized that hyper-active reward responding would be associated with early substance use initiation. An additional goal was to quantify the predictive utility of neural functioning during reward anticipation in children as an objective marker of early substance use initiation, over and above the risk conveyed by two other major risk factors, FH, and early externalizing behaviors.
2. Material and Methods
2.1. Participants
Participants were 34 youth (25 male) from the MLS (Zucker et al., 1996; Zucker et al., 2000), aged 8.2–12.9 at the time of the fMRI scan (M = 10.5, SD = 1.2). The MLS is a multi-wave, community-recruited study of families with and without parental alcohol use disorder (AUD). As part of the MLS, assessments were conducted every three years starting when the children were aged 3–5; adolescents and young adults were also assessed every year from age 11–26. Families were excluded if the target child displayed evidence of fetal alcohol effects or the mother reported drinking during pregnancy. The majority of participants in the present analyses (82.4%) were FH+ for SUD, making this a high-risk sample. (Although the original recruitment strategy of the MLS involved accessing a community-based sample of parents with AUD, their high other drug comorbidity meant that their offspring would be at elevated risk for both alcohol and other drug use. The MLS has historically focused on understanding these other risk patterns as well [Martel et al., 2009; Martz et al., 2016; Nigg et al., 2006]) Full details on MLS assessment and data collection can be found elsewhere (Zucker et al., 2000).
For the present imaging study, participants were excluded if they had any neurological, acute, uncorrected, or chronic medical illness; current or recent (within 6 months) treatment with centrally active medications; MRI contraindications such as metal implants or claustrophobia; IQ < 70; history of psychosis in self or first-degree relatives; or current, active Axis I disorder according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria. However, disorders that are often comorbid with substance use were not part of the exclusion criteria (so as not to bias the sample against our construct of interest): current or past anxiety, conduct or attention deficit hyperactivity (ADHD) disorder, or past mood disorder. Participants who were taking medication for ADHD were asked to abstain at least 48 hours before the MRI scan. Parents/guardians of participants provided written informed consent and participants provided written informed assent. Study materials and procedures were approved by the University of Michigan Medical School Institutional Review Board.
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Substance use
Drinking and drug use histories were obtained from each participant by trained research staff as part of the regular MLS assessment schedule. Beginning at age 6 and occurring at three-year intervals thereafter, a health and daily living questionnaire was used to assess the use of alcohol (more than a sip), cigarettes, marijuana, and other drugs. If applicable, the age at which use occurred and the quantity/frequency of use was also recorded. At annual assessments beginning at age 11, the Drinking and Drug History Form for Children (Zucker and Fitzgerald, 1994) was used to assess the quantity and frequency of alcohol, nicotine, and illicit drug (i.e., marijuana and 17 other drugs, including inhalants and prescription drugs used non-medically) use. Problems associated with alcohol and drug use, respectively (e.g., missed school because of drinking, had trouble getting along with friends because of drug use), were also assessed.
Previous research has shown that substance use initiation before the age of 16 confers a higher risk for later problems than does initiation in young adulthood (Clapper et al., 1995; Gruber et al., 2012; Strashny, 2013). Therefore, we used age 16 as the threshold for increased risk and selected participants from the ongoing longitudinal study for whom we had follow-up substance use data at age 16 or older (ensuring that all comparison participants were beyond the highest-risk window). Participants also had to have a fMRI scan prior to any substance use in order to be included in the present analyses. Of the 34 participants who met these criteria, 18 eventually initiated substance use (11 male; initiation group), whereas 16 did not (14 male; comparison group). On average, those in the comparison group were 17.9 years old (SD 1.5) at the most recent substance use assessment, whereas those in the initiation group were 13.4 years old (SD 1.5) at substance use initiation.
2.2.2. Other measures
The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-III) (Wechsler, 1991) was used to assess full-scale IQ. Early externalizing behaviors (M = 7.4 years old, SD = 1.0) were assessed via parent-report with T-scores from the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (Achenbach, 1991). For ADHD, conduct disorder, depression, and anxiety diagnoses (i.e., disorders that were not exclusionary during screening), the computerized Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children (C-DISC) (Shaffer et al., 2000) was given, and diagnoses were tallied based on DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria. Finally, FH was defined as the number of parents (0, 1, or 2) with a lifetime diagnosis of drug or alcohol abuse or dependence, according to DSM-IV criteria.
2.3. Stimuli and Task
A modified MID task (Knutson et al., 2000) was used to probe the brain’s response to the anticipation of monetary incentive (i.e., gain and loss). Each trial was 6-s long and consisted of 4 stimuli, presented consecutively: 1) 2000-ms incentive cue (“Win $5.00,” “Win $0.20,” “Don’t Lose $5.00,” “Don’t Lose $0.20,” or “No Money at Stake”), 2) 2000-ms anticipation fixation cross, 3) variable-duration response target (i.e., a solid black shape cuing a button press), and 4) variable-duration feedback (e.g., “Correct Response. You earn $5.00!”). There was no inter-trial interval. Participants were instructed to press the button to the target; responding while the target was on the screen signified a successful trial (i.e., hit). Response target duration was calculated based on each participant’s reaction time during a practice session prior to scanning and recalibrated after the first run for an overall success rate of approximately 60%. Participants were also instructed to respond to neutral targets (i.e., “No Money at Stake”) despite no incentive value. Trials were presented in pseudorandom order in two 5-minute runs of 20 gain trials, 20 loss trials, and 10-neutral trials per run. Participants were paid money won during the task.
2.4. fMRI Data Acquisition
Whole-brain BOLD, functional images were acquired on a 3.0T GE MR750 scanner (Milwaukee, WI) using T2*-weighted single-shot combined spiral in/out sequences (Glover and Law, 2001) (TR 2000 ms, TE 30 ms, flip angle 90 degrees, field-of-view 200 mm, matrix size 64 × 64, slice thickness 4 mm, in-plane resolution 3.12 × 3.12 mm, 29 slices). High-resolution anatomical T1 scans were also obtained for spatial normalization (TR 12.2 ms, TE 5.2 ms, flip angle 15 degrees, field-of-view 26 mm, matrix size 256 × 192, slice thickness 1.2 mm, in-plane resolution 1.02 × 1.08 mm, 124 slices). Motion was minimized with foam padding around the head and instructing participants on the importance of keeping still.
2.5. Data processing and analytic strategy
2.5.1. Preprocessing
Functional images were reconstructed using an iterative algorithm (Noll et al., 2005; Sutton et al., 2003) and motion corrected using FSL v5.0.2.2 (FMRIB, Oxford, UK). Runs exceeding 3 mm translation or 3° rotation were excluded. The difference between initiation and comparison groups on the number of runs excluded was not significant: initiation = 16.7%, comparison = 15.6%, t(32) = 0.13, p = .901. Image preprocessing was completed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM8; Wellcome Institute of Cognitive Neurology, Oxford, UK). Functional images were slice-time corrected, spatially normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template using SPM8 defaults, and smoothed with a 6-mm full-width at half-maximum (FWHM) smoothing kernel. Low-frequency noise was removed with a high-pass filter (128 sec).
2.5.2. Individual-level analyses
Image processing was done in SPM8, and individual-level analyses were carried out using the general linear model. Two general events were modeled: 1) incentive cue + anticipation fixation, and 2) response target + feedback. The incentive cue + anticipation fixation event was modeled separately for each cue type: large gain, small gain, large loss, small loss, and neutral. The response + feedback event was modeled separately for each cue (except neutral) and feedback type: positive (i.e., hit) and negative (i.e., miss) for each of large gain, small gain, large loss, and small loss. These were convolved with the standard hemodynamic response function, along with six realignment parameters and white matter signal intensity as nuisance variables. Owing to our focus in this study on NAcc activation during the anticipation of gaining and losing money, NAcc spherical anatomical masks of 5 mm in diameter and centered on the Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates of [x = −10, y = 13, z = −8] and [x = 10, y = 13, z = −8] (Bjork et al., 2008b) were used to extract mean parameter estimates with MarsBaR (Brett et al., 2002) for two contrasts of interest: large gain anticipation vs. neutral anticipation (i.e., Win $5.00 vs. No Money at Stake) and large loss anticipation vs. neutral anticipation (i.e., Don’t Lose $5.00 vs. No Money at Stake). This task was originally developed with different incentive values so that the parametric effect of different monetary amounts could be investigated. Our interest here was in the function of reward processing overall rather than the difference between large and small incentives, and previous work has found that large incentives elicit a more robust signal than do small incentives (Bjork et al., 2008a). Thus, we focused on large gain and loss for the present analyses. After extraction, comparisons of the left and right NAcc using paired samples t-tests (large gain: t(33)=0.94, p=.353; large loss: t(33)=0.09, p=.928) and correlational analyses (large gain: r=.84, p<.001; large loss: r=.73, p<.001) indicated that a single averaged left/right NAcc variable for each of the two contrasts of interest could be used.
2.5.3. Prediction of substance use initiation using logistic regression
Hierarchical logistic regression was used to test the predictive utility of substance use risk factors on the initiation of substance use by age 16. Group membership (initiation or comparison) was the dependent variable. Phenotypic variables (externalizing behaviors [continuous variable] and FH [i.e., 0, 1, or 2 parents]) were entered as predictors in the first block (restricted model). NAcc variables (large gain anticipation and large loss anticipation [continuous variables]) were added in the second block (full model). No other covariates were included in either block. Model comparison was performed using a likelihood ratio test to determine whether the full model (i.e., phenotypic and NAcc variables) showed a significantly better fit than the restricted model (i.e., phenotypic variables alone). In addition, beta coefficients were individually tested for significance. Alpha was set at .05 for both of these analyses.
2.5.4. Demographic, Psychometric, and Task Performance Measures
Independent samples t-tests or Fisher’s exact tests were used to evaluate differences between initiation and comparison groups on demographic, psychometric, and task performance measures.
3. Results
3.1. Demographic variables and participant characteristics
There were no significant differences (i.e., all ps > .05) between groups on sex, age at fMRI scan, IQ, or FH. There were also no significant differences in early externalizing behaviors or lifetime conduct disorder, ADHD, anxiety, or depression diagnoses. No participants met criteria for SUD at follow-up. See Table 1 for statistics.
Table 1.
Initiation Group (n = 18) |
Comparison Group (n = 16) |
Statistic | Significance | |
---|---|---|---|---|
% Female | 38.9% | 12.5% | — | p = .125a |
Age at fMRI Scan | 10.7 (1.2) | 10.2 (1.1) | t(32) = 1.26 | p = .216b |
Age at First Substance Use | 13.4 (1.5) | n/a | — | — |
Full-Scale IQ* | 106.8 (15.0) | 107.3 (12.4) | t(31) = 0.11 | p = .910b |
Phenotypic Variables | ||||
Family History of SUD (n; 0/1/2 parents) | 2/5/11 | 4/7/5 | — | p = .233a |
Early Externalizing Behaviors** | 51.1 (10.2) | 46.0 (10.3) | t(32) = 1.46 | p = .155b |
Nucleus Accumbens Variables | ||||
Large Gain Anticipation vs. Neutral Anticipation | 1.26 (0.78) | 0.87 (0.83) | t(32) = 1.40 | p = .170 |
Large Loss Anticipation vs. Neutral Anticipation | 0.17 (0.60) | 0.41 (0.67) | t(32) = 1.11 | p = .274 |
Substance Use at Most Recent Follow-up | ||||
Used Alcohol (n) | 17 | 0 | — | p < .001c |
Used Cannabis (n) | 15 | 0 | — | p < .001c |
Used Nicotine (n) | 8 | 0 | — | p = .003c |
Used Other Drugs (n) | 12 | 0 | — | p = .020c |
Used Multiple Substances (n) | 17 | 0 | — | p < .001c |
Alcoholic Drinks Per Year*** | 75.90 (78.66) | 0 | t(17) = 3.88 | p < .001d |
Occasions of Cannabis Use Per Year*** | 47.90 (86.70) | 0 | t(17) = 2.10 | p = .026d |
Days of Nicotine Use Per Year*** | 136.74 (134.24) | 0 | t(16) = 2.34 | p = .016d |
Number of Other Drugs Used Per Year*** | 1.24 (0.39) | 0 | t(17) = 5.16 | p < .001d |
Lifetime Diagnosis (n; present/absent)**** | ||||
Conduct Disorder | 2/15 | 0/15 | — | p = .486a |
ADHD | 2/15 | 3/12 | — | p = .645a |
Generalized Anxiety Disorder | 1/16 | 0/15 | — | p > .999a |
Depressive Disorder | 2/15 | 0/15 | — | p = .486a |
Note. fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; SUD, substance use disorder; ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Numbers represent means, with standard deviations in parentheses, unless otherwise noted.
Two-tailed Fisher’s exact test.
Two-tailed independent samples t-test.
One-tailed Fisher’s exact test.
One-tailed independent samples t-test. Degrees of freedom reduction reflects equal variances not assumed.
One participant in the initiation group was missing an IQ score.
Early externalizing data are T-scores from the Child Behavior Checklist – Parent Report.
Calculated only for those who reported using the indicated substance, beginning with the age of initiation of that substance.
One participant in each group was missing diagnostic information.
3.2. Task performance
There were no significant differences between initiation and comparison groups on task performance measures (i.e., success rate and reaction time). See Table 2 for statistics.
Table 2.
Initiation Group |
Comparison Group |
Statistic | Significance | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Hit Rate (%) | ||||
Large Gain | 64.7 (25.0) | 62.5 (18.3) | t(32) = 0.29 | p = .772 |
Neutral | 42.2 (20.7) | 45.6 (21.4) | t(32) = 0.47 | p = .642 |
Large Loss | 56.1 (23.4) | 64.4 (14.4) | t(29) = 1.26a | p = .220 |
Hit Reaction Time (ms) | ||||
Large Gain | 203.5 (46.4) | 226.0 (40.5) | t(32) = 1.50 | p = .144 |
Neutral | 202.8 (45.9) | 216.5 (66.6) | t(32) = 0.70 | p = .488 |
Large Loss | 202.6 (52.1) | 234.9 (43.4) | t(32) = 1.95 | p = .060 |
Note. ms, milliseconds. For Initiation and Comparison columns, the numbers given are means, with standard deviations in parentheses. Statistic and significance columns refer to two-tailed independent samples t-tests.
Degrees of freedom reduction reflects equal variances not assumed.
3.3. Brain imaging
To confirm that the MID task elicited the expected NAcc activations, we computed one-sample t-tests in the whole sample (N = 34) for each contrast of interest. See Supplemental Material1for these task main effects (Table S1 and Figure S1) as well as for analyses demonstrating our measurement of reward anticipation (and not general response anticipation) and our disentangling of the effects of anticipation from those of feedback.
3.3.1. Prediction of substance use initiation
The restricted model (i.e., comprising externalizing behaviors and FH) did not significantly predict group membership (χ22 = 4.41, p = .110) and had an overall classification accuracy of 61.8%. The full model (i.e., comprising NAcc variables in addition to externalizing behaviors and FH) significantly predicted group membership (χ24 = 11.80, p = .019). The full model was also a significantly better predictor of group membership than the restricted model (χ22 = 7.38, p = .025) according to a log likelihood test. The overall classification accuracy of the full model was 73.5%, and the area under the curve was .826. Of the four predictors, only anticipation of large gain was significant (B = 1.76, p = .023). The odds ratio for large gain anticipation was 5.81, indicating the increase in the odds of initiating substance use for a one-unit increase in the predictor, holding externalizing behaviors, FH, and large loss anticipation constant. See Table 3 for statistics.
Table 3.
Model χ2 | Sig | Nagelkerke R2 | Classification Accuracy (overall, sensitivity, specificity) | B | SE | Sig | OR | 95% CI for the OR | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Restricted Model | 4.41 | .110 | .16 | 61.8%, 66.7%, 56.3% | |||||
Early Externalizing Behaviors | 0.04 | 0.04 | .244 | 1.05 | 0.97 – 1.13 | ||||
Family History of SUD | 0.74 | 0.51 | .149 | 2.09 | 0.77 – 5.68 | ||||
Constant | −3.01 | 1.95 | .123 | 0.05 | |||||
Full Model | 11.80 | .019 | .39 | 73.5%, 72.2%, 75.0% | |||||
Early Externalizing Behaviors | 0.06 | 0.04 | .156 | 1.06 | 0.98 – 1.15 | ||||
Family History of SUD | 0.54 | 0.61 | .377 | 1.71 | 0.52 – 5.67 | ||||
NAcc: Large Gain Anticipation vs. Neutral Anticipation | 1.76 | 0.77 | .023 | 5.81 | 1.28 – 26.42 | ||||
NAcc: Large Loss Anticipation vs. Neutral Anticipation | −1.93 | 1.04 | .063 | 0.15 | 0.02 – 1.11 | ||||
Constant | −4.66 | 2.28 | .041 | 0.01 |
Note. SUD, substance use disorder; NAcc, nucleus accumbens; Sig, significance; B, beta coefficient; SE, standard error; OR, odds ratio; CI, confidence interval
4. Discussion
An aim of this prospective study was to determine whether activation during reward anticipation in substance-naïve children can predict who will go on to use substances in adolescence, above and beyond other risk factors. Results revealed that greater NAcc activity during the anticipation of winning money was associated with subsequent substance initiation, controlling for early externalizing behaviors and parental SUD. This finding is in line with the hypothesis that hyperactivation of reward circuitry may be a risk factor for later substance use problems.
The NAcc is one of the central structures in the mesolimbic dopaminergic pathway, receiving projections from the ventral tegmental area. Together these regions are critically involved in motivated behavior, including reward seeking and SUD. Convergent evidence has demonstrated that all drugs of abuse increase synaptic dopamine in the NAcc (e.g., (Dichiara and Imperato, 1988; Johnson and North, 1992)). The NAcc is also implicated in non-drug reward (e.g., (Beck et al., 2009; Ernst et al., 2005; Knutson et al., 2001)). The present study adds to the extant literature by demonstrating that (a) substance-naïve children who express greater NAcc activation during the processing of non-drug (i.e., monetary) gain anticipation are more likely to use substances in adolescence, and (b) neural circuitry regulating a specific aspect of reward processing (i.e., gain anticipation) appears to play at least as strong a role as do previously identified phenotypic measures.
Adolescence is a developmental period marked by heightened risk-taking (Spear, 2000), including substance use. Heightened sensitivity to reward has been identified as a possible mechanism (e.g., (Casey and Jones, 2010)). In support of this, increased magnitude of gain-related NAcc activation has been found in adolescents relative to adults (Galvan et al., 2006; Heitzeg et al., 2014). Not all adolescents engage in risky behavior, however, and here we found that differences during childhood in NAcc activation to the anticipation of winning money predicted who would go on to use drugs and/or alcohol at an early age. This finding is consistent with prior work by Galvan and colleagues (Galvan et al., 2007) that found the likelihood of engaging in risky behavior was significantly positively correlated with NAcc activity to large vs. small monetary gain. Associations between VS activity and risky decisions have also been found in adults (Kuhnen and Knutson, 2005; Matthews et al., 2004). The present findings are consistent with these adult and adolescent studies and extend the potential association between VS activity and risk-taking behavior, such as early substance use, to children.
As described previously, the MID task is characterized by an incentive cue followed by an anticipatory phase, a response target, and then feedback. The present study focused on the anticipation of large gains and large losses compared to neutral anticipation (i.e., no money at stake). Some reports indicate that activation of the VS to the anticipation of both monetary gains and losses supports the role of motivational salience in addiction risk (Robinson and Berridge, 2008; Yau et al., 2012). Other research suggests that two different systems underlie brain activation to the anticipation of gains and losses. According to this latter view, the anticipation of gain elicits a dopaminergic response from the mesolimbic reward pathway, whereas loss anticipation is thought to be associated to a greater extent with serotonergic responding as a function of behavioral inhibition and risk aversion (Beck et al., 2009; Daw et al., 2002). Although it was not possible in the present study to test the biochemical processes associated with brain activation, our findings demonstrating a significant positive effect of NAcc activation to monetary gain and a negative effect trending towards significance for NAcc activation to monetary loss point to the possibility of different neural mechanisms involved in gain versus loss in terms of substance use outcomes.
A number of strengths of this study are worth noting. First, we scanned subjects at a very early age (i.e., 8.2–12.9 years), ensuring that we would be able to characterize the neural correlates of reward anticipation before any experimentation with drugs or alcohol took place. This is particularly important given evidence that substance use may have diffuse effects on reward processing and extend to other types of rewards (e.g., (Nestor et al., 2010)). Second, because the initiation of alcohol (Clapper et al., 1995), marijuana (Gruber et al., 2012), or substance use in general (Strashny, 2013) before the age of 16 conveys greater risk for later social, emotional, and physical problems, we only included subjects for whom we had substance use data after the age of 16 in order to ensure that they had aged out of the window of highest risk for these problems. Finally, we controlled for other common predictors of substance use initiation, namely FH and externalizing behaviors. These constructs have been shown to convey a significant risk for later SUDs and problems (Chassin et al., 1999; Hussong et al., 2012; Merikangas et al., 1998). For instance, one study found that family history conferred an 8-fold risk for SUD (Merikangas et al., 1998). Another study found significantly increased odds of regular alcohol, nicotine, and cannabis use at age 14 among those with high externalizing at age 11 (King et al., 2004). Based on these previous observations, it was important to account for the influence of both FH and externalizing when determining the extent to which brain activity predicts substance use.
Interestingly, in this sample, neither FH nor externalizing behaviors were significantly different between the initiation and comparison groups when tested is isolation with independent samples t-tests. In addition, neither variable was a significant predictor of substance use initiation. With regard to FH, one explanation for the nonsignificant finding could be due to the manner in which it was measured (i.e., number of parents with a SUD). Future work should consider including second-degree relatives and/or family density scores in the measurement of FH in order to provide a more fine-grained measure of the predictive power of this variable. It is possible that externalizing behaviors did not emerge as significantly different between groups because of reduced variance as a product of participants’ relatively young age as well as the fact that none of them had started using alcohol or drugs at the time of the scan. Finally, as this was a high-risk sample with only 6 FH-participants, externalizing and FH may not have been predictive due to reduced variance in these variables. FH and externalizing behaviors may be predictive of substance use initiation in a healthy, community sample.
Several points are important to keep in mind when interpreting these results in the context of the broader literature on reward anticipation and substance use. Those who initiated substance use in the present study did not reach the threshold for a SUD, indicating that the contrast is between adolescents who try drugs and alcohol at a relatively early age and those who do not. Thus, the differences in brain function are related to substance use initiation rather than something more serious such as dependence. At the same time, adolescents who initiate alcohol use at age 15 have nearly four times the risk for later alcohol dependence compared to those who start at 21 or older (Grant and Dawson, 1997). Similarly, the risk for later drug dependence is almost twice as high in those who start using at age 15 relative to those who start at 21 or older (Grant and Dawson, 1998). Additionally, over 94% of the initiation group from the present sample reported using more than one substance, indicating a substantially increased risk for problem use later in life (Moss et al., 2014). It will be important to continue following these youth into late adolescence and young adulthood in order to track their substance use trajectories as a function of the neurobiological differences observed here. In addition, some limitations should be noted. First, we were not able to investigate sex differences in reward responsivity, owing to the sample being primarily male and to the modest sample size due to the practical constraints of scanning a high-risk sample of substance-naïve youth and following them annually. Second, this sample included children with conduct disorder, ADHD, and anxiety and depressive disorder diagnoses within their lifetime. That is, they display the variations in level of psychopathology that would be anticipated from a community sample whose families are elevated on substance use and related comorbidities (e.g., antisocial behavior, depression). Thus, these results may not generalize to children at different levels of risk, such as samples with higher rates of disruptive behavior disorders (who would be expected to have an even greater risk) and those without any lifetime DSM diagnoses (who would be expected to have a lower risk). Finally, we did not have data on pubertal status, which may be important when the participant age range covers late childhood and early adolescence, as it did here. These issues should be considered in future studies.
4.1. Conclusions
To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate reward anticipation processing in substance-naïve children as a predictor of adolescent substance use initiation. This work is important in order to differentiate the risk factors from the consequences of substance use. Given how malleable the brain is at this age, these results highlight the feasibility and importance of assessing youth quite early and focusing treatment efforts on neurobiological deficits that are directly related to risk before they transition to more problematic use in adulthood. Heightened responsivity to gain anticipation in the NAcc may predispose individuals to early substance use, beyond the risk conveyed by other factors. Despite the difficulty in scanning subjects at a young age and following them over a period of several years in order to characterize their substance use patterns, it will be critical to conduct further studies of this sort. Indeed, the National Institutes of Health’s Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development study is currently underway and will undoubtedly produce other important information about substance abuse risk.
Supplementary Material
Highlights.
Substance-naïve children scanned with fMRI during a monetary incentive delay task
Childhood nucleus accumbens activity predicted adolescent substance use initiation
The model with neural variables performed better than the model without them
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Leon Puttler, Katherine Baisden, Mary Soules, and Bailey Ferris for their contributions to data collection, analysis, or management.
Role of Funding Sources
This work was supported by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (R01 AA007065, T32 AA007477, and K01 AA024804), the National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01 DA027261), and the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (UL1 TR000433). The funding sources had no role in the design or conduct of the study, the writing of the manuscript, or the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Conflict of Interest
The authors have no conflicts of interest.
Supplementary material can be found by accessing the online version of this paper at http://dx.doi.org and by entering doi:
Supplementary material can be found by accessing the online version of this paper at http://dx.doi.org and by entering doi:
References
- Achenbach TM, 1991. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 and 1991 Profile. University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry, Burlington, VT: https://www.unc.edu/depts/sph/longscan/pages/measures/Age16/writeups/Age%2016%20Child%20Behavior%20Checklist%20EXTERNAL%20%20SITE%20MM%20FINAL.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association, 1994. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth ed APA, Washington, D.C. [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Dolan S, Hindes A, 2002. Decision-making and addiction (part II): Myopia for the future or hypersensitivity to reward? Neuropsychologia 40, 1690–1705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck A, Schlagenhauf F, Wustenberg T, Hein J, Kienast T, Kahnt T, Schmack K, Hagele C, Knutson B, Heinz A, Wrase J, 2009. Ventral striatal activation during reward anticipation correlates with impulsivity in alcoholics. Biol. Psychiat 66, 734–742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bjork JM, Knutson B, Hommer DW, 2008a. Incentive-elicited striatal activation in adolescent children of alcoholics. Addiction 103, 1308–1319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bjork JM, Smith AR, Hommer DW, 2008b. Striatal sensitivity to reward deliveries and omissions in substance dependent patients. Neuroimage 42, 1609–1621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blum K, Braverman ER, Holder JM, Lubar JF, Monastra VJ, Miller D, Lubar JO, Chen TJH, Comings DE, 2000. Reward deficiency syndrome: a biogenetic model for the diagnosis and treatment of impulsive, addictive, and compulsive behaviors. J. Psychoactive Drugs 32, 1–112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brett M, Anton JL, Valabregue R, Poline JB, 2002. Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox [abstract] Presented at the 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain, June 2–6, 2002, Sendai, Japan https://matthew.dynevor.org/research/abstracts/marsbar/marsbar_abstract.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Carter RM, MacInnes J, Huettel S, Adcock RA, 2009. Activation in the VTA and nucleus accumbens increases in anticipation of both gains and losses. Front. Behav. Neurosci 3, 1–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casey BJ, Jones RM, 2010. Neurobiology of the adolescent brain and behavior: Implications for substance use disorders. J. Am. Acad. Child Psy 49, 1189–1201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chambers RA, Taylor JR, Potenza MN, 2003. Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: a critical period of addiction vulnerability. Am. J. Psychiat 160, 1041–1052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chassin L, Pitts SC, DeLucia C, Todd M, 1999. A longitudinal study of children of alcoholics: predicting young adult substance use disorders, anxiety, and depression. J. Abnorm. Psychol 108, 106–119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clapper RL, Buka SL, Goldfield EC, Lipsitt LP, Tsuang MT, 1995. Adolescent problem behaviors as predictors of adult alcohol diagnoses. Int. J. Addict 30, 507–523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daw ND, Kakade S, Dayan P, 2002. Opponent interactions between serotonin and dopamine. Neural Netw. 15, 603–616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dawson DA, Goldstein RB, Chou SP, Ruan WJ, Grant BF, 2008. Age at first drink and the first incidence of adult-onset DSM-IV alcohol use disorders. Alcohol. Clin. Eep. Res 32, 2149–2160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dichiara G, Imperato A, 1988. Drugs abused by humans preferentially increase synaptic dopamine concentrations in the mesolimbic system of freely moving rats. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A 85, 5274–5278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ernst M, Nelson EE, Jazbec S, McClure EB, Monk CS, Leibenluft E, Blair J, Pine DS, 2005. Amygdala and nucleus accumbens in responses to receipt and omission of gains in adults and adolescents. Neuroimage 25, 1279–1291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Filbey FM, Dunlop J, Ketcherside A, Baine J, Rhinehardt T, Kuhn B, DeWitt S, Alvi T, 2016. fMRI study of neural sensitization to hedonic stimuli in long-term, daily cannabis users. Hum. Brain Mapp 37, 3431–3443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galvan A, Hare T, Voss H, Glover G, Casey BJ, 2007. Risk-taking and the adolescent brain: who is at risk? Developmental Sci. 10, F8–F14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galvan A, Hare TA, Parra CE, Penn J, Voss H, Glover G, Casey BJ, 2006. Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behavior in adolescents. J. Neurosci 26, 6885–6892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glover GH, Law CS, 2001. Spiral-in/out BOLD fMRI for increased SNR and reduced susceptibility artifacts. Magn. Reson. Med 46, 515–522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant BF, Dawson DA, 1997. Age at onset of alcohol use and its association with DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence: Results from the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey. J. Subst. Abuse 9, 103–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant BF, Dawson DA, 1998. Age of onset of drug use and its association with DSM-IV drug abuse and dependence: results from the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey. J. Subst. Abuse 10, 163–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gruber SA, Dahlgren MK, Sagar KA, Gönenc A, Killgore WDS, 2012. Age of onset of marijuana use impacts inhibitory processing. Neurosci. Lett 511, 89–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hariri AR, Brown SM, Williamson DE, Flory JD, de Wit H, Manuck SB, 2006. Preference for immediate over delayed rewards is associated with magnitude of ventral striatal activity. J. Neurosci 26, 13213–13217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heitzeg MM, Villafuerte S, Weiland BJ, Enoch MA, Burmeister M, Zubieta JK, Zucker RA, 2014. Effect of GABRA2 genotype on development of incentive-motivation circuitry in a sample enriched for alcoholism risk. Neuropsychopharmacol. 39, 3077–3086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hussong AM, Huang WJ, Serrano D, Curran PJ, Chassin L, 2012. Testing whether and when parent alcoholism uniquely affects various forms of adolescent substance use. J. Abnorm. Child Psych 40, 1265–1276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jia Z, Worhunsky PD, Carroll KM, Rounsaville BJ, Stevens MC, Pearlson GD, Potenza MN, 2011. An initial study of neural responses to monetary incentives as related to treatment outcome in cocaine dependence. Biol. Psychiat 70, 553–560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson SW, North RA, 1992. Opioids excite dopamine neurons by hyperpolarization of local interneurons. J. Neurosci 12, 483–488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King KM, Chassin L, 2007. A prospective study of the effects of age of initiation of alcohol and drug use on young adult substance dependence. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 68, 256–265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King SM, Iacono WG, McGue M, 2004. Childhood externalizing and internalizing psychopathology in the prediction of early substance use. Addiction 99, 1548–1559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knutson B, Fong GW, Adams CM, Varner JL, Hommer D, 2001. Dissociation of reward anticipation and outcome with event-related fMRI. Neuroreport 12, 3683–3687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knutson B, Gibbs SEB, 2007. Linking nucleus accumbens dopamine and blood oxygenation. Psychopharmacology 191, 813–822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knutson B, Westdorp A, Kaiser E, Hommer D, 2000. fMRI visualization of brain activity during a monetary incentive delay task. Neuroimage 12, 20–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhnen CM, Knutson B, 2005. The neural basis of financial risk taking. Neuron 47, 763–770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martel MM, Pierce L, Nigg JT, Jester JM, Adams K, Puttler LI, Buu A, Fitzgerald HE, Zucker RA, 2009. Temperament pathways to childhood disruptive behavior and adolescent substance abuse: Testing a cascade model. J. Abnorm. Child Psych 37, 363–373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martz ME, Trucco EM, Cope LM, Hardee JE, Jester JM, Zucker RA, Heitzeg MM, 2016. Association of marijuana use with blunted nucleus accumbens response to reward anticipation. JAMA Psychiat. 73, 838–844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews SC, Simmons AN, Lane SD, Paulus MP, 2004. Selective activation of the nucleus accumbens during risk-taking decision making. Neuroreport 15, 2123–2127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McBride WJ, Li TK, 1998. Animal models of alcoholism: neurobiology of high alcohol-drinking behavior in rodents. Crit. Rev. Neurobiol 12, 339–369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McClure SM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD, 2004. Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science 306, 503–507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merikangas KR, Stolar M, Stevens DE, Goulet J, Preisig MA, Fenton B, Zhang HP, O’Malley SS, Rounsaville BJ, 1998. Familial transmission of substance use disorders. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 55, 973–979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miech RA, Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE, Patrick ME, 2018. Monitoring the Future national survey results on drug use, 1975–2017: Volume I, Secondary school students. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan, 566 pp. http://monitoringthefuture.org/pubs/monographs/mtf-vol1_2017.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Moss HB, Chen CM, Yi HY, 2014. Early adolescent patterns of alcohol, cigarettes, and marijuana polysubstance use and young adult substance use outcomes in a nationally representative sample. Drug Alcohol Depend. 136, 51–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muller KU, Gan G, Banaschewski T, Barker GJ, Bokde ALW, Buchel C, Conrod P, Fauth-Buhler M, Flor H, Gallinat J, Garavan H, Gowland P, Heinz A, Ittermann B, Lawrence C, Loth E, Mann K, Martinot JL, Nees F, Paus T, Pausova Z, Rietschel M, Strohle A, Struve M, Schumann G, Smolka MN, IMAGEN Consortium, 2015. No differences in ventral striatum responsivity between adolescents with a positive family history of alcoholism and controls. Addict. Biol 20, 534–545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nestor L, Hester R, Garavan H, 2010. Increased ventral striatal BOLD activity during non-drug reward anticipation in cannabis users. Neuroimage 49, 1133–1143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nigg JT, Wong MM, Martel MM, Jester JM, Puttler LI, Glass JM, Adams KM, Fitzgerald HE, Zucker RA, 2006. Poor response inhibition as a predictor of problem drinking and illicit drug use in adolescents at risk for alcoholism and other substance use disorders. J. Am. Acad. Child Psy 45, 468–475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noll DC, Fessler JA, Sutton BP, 2005. Conjugate phase MRI reconstruction with spatially variant sample density correction. IEEE T. Med. Imaging 24, 325–336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Shea M, Singh ME, McGregor IS, Mallet PE, 2004. Chronic cannabinoid exposure produces lasting memory impairment and increased anxiety in adolescent but not adult rats. J. Psychopharmacol 18, 502–508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piazza PV, Rougepont F, Deminiere JM, Kharoubi M, Lemoal M, Simon H, 1991Dopaminergic activity is reduced in the prefrontal cortex and increased in the nucleus-accumbens of rats predisposed to develop amphetamine self-administration. Brain Res. 567, 169–174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson TE, Berridge KC, 2000. The psychology and neurobiology of addiction: An incentive-sensitization view. Addiction 95, S91–S117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson TE, Berridge KC, 2008. Review. The incentive sensitization theory of addiction: Some current issues. Philos. T. Roy. Soc. B 363, 3137–3146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russell M, Cooper ML, Frone MR, 1990. The influence of sociodemographic characteristics on familial alcohol problems: Data from a community sample. Alcohol Clin. Eep. Res 14, 221–226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salamone JD, Correa M, 2012. The mysterious motivational functions of mesolimbic dopamine. Neuron 76, 470–485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schott BH, Minuzzi L, Krebs RM, Elmenhorst D, Lang M, Winz OH, Seidenbecher CI, Coenen HH, Heinze HJ, Zilles K, Duzel E, Bauer A, 2008. Mesolimbic functional magnetic resonance imaging activations during reward anticipation correlate with reward-related ventral striatal dopamine release. J. Neurosci 28, 14311–14319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schuckit MA, Smith TL, 1996. An 8-year follow-up of 450 sons of alcoholic and control subjects. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 53, 202–210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W, 1998. Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. J. Neurophysiol 80, 1–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaffer D, Fisher P, Lucas CP, Dulcan MK, Schwab-Stone ME, 2000. NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): Description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. J. Am. Acad. Child Psy 39, 28–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sher KJ, Walitzer KS, Wood PK, Brent EE, 1991. Characteristics of children of alcoholics: Putative risk factors, substance use and abuse, and psychopathology. J. Abnorm. Psychol 100, 427–448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shulman EP, Smith AR, Silva K, Icenogle G, Duell N, Chein J, Steinberg L, 2016. The dual systems model: Review, reappraisal, and reaffirmation. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci 17, 103–117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Somerville LH, Hare T, Casey BJ, 2011. Frontostriatal maturation predicts cognitive control failure to appetitive cues in adolescents. J. Cognitive Neurosci 23, 2123–2134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Somerville LH, Jones RM, Casey BJ, 2010. A time of change: Behavioral and neural correlates of adolescent sensitivity to appetitive and aversive environmental cues. Brain Cognition 72, 124–133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spear LP, 2000. The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci. Biobehav. R 24, 417–463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strashny A, 2013. Age of substance use initiation among treatment admissions aged 18 to 30, The CBHSQ Report Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (US), Rockville, MD: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK384841/ [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sutton BP, Noll DC, Fessler JA, 2003. Fast, iterative image reconstruction for MRI in the presence of field inhomogeneities. IEEE T. Med. Imaging 22, 178–188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taffe MA, Kotzebue RW, Crean RD, Crawford EF, Edwards S, Mandyam CD, 2010. Long-lasting reduction in hippocampal neurogenesis by alcohol consumption in adolescent nonhuman primates. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A 107, 11104–11109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tarazi FI, Baldessarini RJ, 2000. Comparative postnatal development of dopamine D-1, D-2 and D-4 receptors in rat forebrain. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci 18, 29–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow ND, Morales M, 2015. The brain on drugs: from reward to addiction. Cell 162, 712–725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Maynard L, Fowler JS, Jayne B, Telang F, Logan J, Ding YS, Gatley SJ, Hitzemann R, Wong C, Pappas N, 2002. Effects of alcohol detoxification on dopamine D2 receptors in alcoholics: A preliminary study. Psychiat. Res.-Neuroim 116, 163–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D, 1991. The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, third ed The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX. [Google Scholar]
- Wrase J, Schlagenhauf F, Kienast T, Wustenberg T, Bermpohl F, Kahnt T, Beck A, Strohle A, Juckel G, Knutson B, Heinz A, 2007. Dysfunction of reward processing correlates with alcohol craving in detoxified alcoholics. Neuroimage 35, 787–794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yau WY, Zubieta JK, Weiland BJ, Samudra PG, Zucker RA, Heitzeg MM, 2012. Nucleus accumbens response to incentive stimuli anticipation in children of alcoholics: relationships with precursive behavioral risk and lifetime alcohol use. J. Neurosci 32, 2544–2551. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zucker RA, Ellis DA, Fitzgerald HE, Bingham CR, Sanford K, 1996. Other evidence for at least two alcoholisms II. Life course variation in antisociality and heterogeneity of alcoholic outcome. Dev. Psychopathol 8, 831–848. [Google Scholar]
- Zucker RA, Fitzgerald HE, 1994. Drinking and Drug History Form for Children University of Michigan Department of Psychiatry, Addiction Research Center, Ann Arbor, MI. [Google Scholar]
- Zucker RA, Fitzgerald HE, Refior SK, Puttler LI, Pallas DM, Ellis DA, 2000. The clinical and social ecology of childhood for children of alcoholics: description of a study and implications for a differentiated social policy, in: Fitzgerald HE, Lester BM, Zucker RA (Eds.), Children of Addiction: Research, Health and Policy Issues; Routledge Falmer, New York, pp. 109–141 [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.