Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2019 May 21;8:e44182. doi: 10.7554/eLife.44182

Ventral hippocampal projections to the medial prefrontal cortex regulate social memory

Mary L Phillips 1, Holly Anne Robinson 1, Lucas Pozzo-Miller 1,
Editors: Laura L Colgin2, Lisa M Monteggia3
PMCID: PMC6542587  PMID: 31112129

Abstract

Inputs from the ventral hippocampus (vHIP) to the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) are implicated in several neuropsychiatric disorders. Here, we show that the vHIP-mPFC projection is hyperactive in the Mecp2 knockout mouse model of the autism spectrum disorder Rett syndrome, which has deficits in social memory. Long-term excitation of mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons in wild-type mice impaired social memory, whereas their long-term inhibition in Rett mice rescued social memory deficits. The extent of social memory improvement was negatively correlated with vHIP-evoked responses in mPFC slices, on a mouse-per-mouse basis. Acute manipulations of the vHIP-mPFC projection affected social memory in a region and behavior selective manner, suggesting that proper vHIP-mPFC signaling is necessary to recall social memories. In addition, we identified an altered pattern of vHIP innervation of mPFC neurons, and increased synaptic strength of vHIP inputs onto layer five pyramidal neurons as contributing factors of aberrant vHIP-mPFC signaling in Rett mice.

Research organism: Mouse

Introduction

Social interactions are a fundamental part of our daily lives, and impairments in social cognition are key features of multiple neuropsychiatric illnesses. A person or animal must reliably recall previous social interactions to make appropriate social responses and then update the memory with each new encounter. Previous studies have identified the hippocampal network as the brain region that tracks social encounters in both human subjects and mouse models (Hitti and Siegelbaum, 2014; Meira et al., 2018; Okuyama et al., 2016; Tavares et al., 2015). Functional neuroimaging in human subjects has revealed that higher covariance between hippocampal activity and changes in social environment reflect better social skills (Tavares et al., 2015). In mouse models, perturbing neuronal activity in both dorsal CA2 and ventral CA1 hippocampal subregions impairs social memory (Hitti and Siegelbaum, 2014; Meira et al., 2018; Okuyama et al., 2016). However, debate remains as to which long-range efferent projections from the hippocampus are required for the formation of social memories.

Autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) are characterized by difficulties in interpreting social situations and a lack of social appropriation (Barendse et al., 2018). A common feature in mouse models of monogenic ASDs is an imbalance in synaptic excitation and inhibition (E/I) within different brain regions (Nelson and Valakh, 2015). Altering the E/I balance in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) of mice mimics autism-like social deficits (Yizhar et al., 2011), and restoring the E/I balance in the CNTNAP2 knockout (KO) and valproate mouse models of ASDs improves their social deficits (Brumback et al., 2018; Selimbeyoglu et al., 2017). Excitatory pyramidal neurons of the ventral hippocampus (vHIP) send long-range projections to the mPFC (Dégenètais et al., 2003; Dembrow et al., 2010; Liu and Carter, 2018; Thierry et al., 2000), and the activity of different populations of mPFC pyramidal neurons are correlated with the novelty of a social target (Liang et al., 2018). Thus, the mPFC is a prime candidate region for the relay of social memory-related signaling from the vHIP. Therefore, we tested whether altering the activity of mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons affects social behavior and memory, and if this long-range projection is dysfunctional in a mouse model of the monogenic syndromic ASD Rett syndrome (RTT). We focused on the Mecp2 KO mouse model of RTT because of the heightened activity in the vHIP (Calfa et al., 2011; Calfa et al., 2015) and the hypoactivity of cortical regions (Durand et al., 2012; Kron et al., 2012; Morello et al., 2018; Sceniak et al., 2016; Tomassy et al., 2014), both resulting from opposite changes in their microcircuit E/I balance.

Using a combination of unbiased behavioral analyses, pathway-specific chemogenetic manipulations with an intersectional genetic approach, high-speed imaging of network activity with voltage-sensitive dyes, trans-synaptic tracing, intracellular recordings, and dual-color optogenetics, we showed that the long-range vHIP-mPFC projection is hyperactive in Mecp2 KO mice, which results in social memory deficits. Furthermore, chemogenetic manipulation of mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons in wild-type (WT) and Mecp2 KO mice correlated with social memory performance in a specific and selective manner. Lastly, these behavioral consequences arose from alterations in the morphology and function of excitatory synapses between vHIP axons and pyramidal neurons in layer 5 of the mPFC.

Results

mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons are selectively activated during social encounters

Because both the vHIP and mPFC have been independently implicated in different aspects of social behavior (Hitti and Siegelbaum, 2014; Liang et al., 2018; Meira et al., 2018; Okuyama et al., 2016; Selimbeyoglu et al., 2017; Yizhar et al., 2011), we first tested if the vHIP projection to the mPFC is selectively engaged during social encounters. To identify vHIP neurons based on their long-range projections, we bilaterally injected green RetroBeads into the prelimbic (PL) region of the mPFC and red RetroBeads into the lateral hypothalamus (LH) of male WT and Mecp2 KO mice at postnatal day 31 (P31), and then we allowed 14 days for RetroBead transfer until P45, when male Mecp2 KO mice are symptomatic (Chen et al., 2001) (Figure 1A and B). Test mice were placed in an open chamber and sequentially exposed for 10 min to either two inanimate objects (novel toy mice) or other live mice (a cage-mate WT littermate and an age-matched non-cage-mate WT mouse), with an interval of 1 hr between exposures (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A). Forty-five minutes after the last interaction, we perfused the mice and prepared their brains for quantitative immunohistochemistry of the immediate early gene c-Fos as an estimate of neuronal activity (Cohen and Greenberg, 2008), measuring the c-Fos intensity of each RetroBead-containing neuron (Figure 1C–D). All vHIP neurons in WT mice that interacted with live mice showed higher c-Fos intensities than those in mice that interacted with toy mice, irrespective of their efferent projections [p<0.0001, Three-Way ANOVA followed by Benjamini and Hochberg Multiple Comparisons (B and H-MC); Figure 1D–E]. Furthermore, mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons in WT mice that interacted with live mice had higher c-Fos intensities than those projecting to the LH (n = 275 mPFC-projecting neurons in six sections from four mice, n = 282/3/2 LH-projecting neurons; p=0.0022, Three-Way ANOVA followed by B and H-MC).

Figure 1. mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons are selectively activated during sequential social encounters with familiar and novel mice.

(A) Schematic of RetroBead injection for labeling mPFC- or LH-projecting vHIP neurons. (B) Injection sites. Scale bar 1 mm. (C) RetroBead labeling and c-Fos immunohistochemistry in vHIP sections. Scale bar 25 μm. (D) Cumulative probability distributions of c-Fos intensities of RetroBead-labeled neurons [n = 163 cells from 3 sections from three mice (163/3/3) WT mPFC-projecting with toy mice; n = 180/3/2 WT LH-projecting with toy mice; n = 275/6/4 WT mPFC-projecting with live mice; n = 271/4/3 WT LH-projecting with live mice; n = 105/6/3 Mecp2 KO mPFC-projecting with toy mice; n = 22/3/2 Mecp2 KO LH-projecting with toy mice; n = 247/4/3 Mecp2 KO mPFC-projecting with live mice; n = 172/5/2 Mecp2 KO LH-projecting with live mice]. (E) Mean c-Fos intensity of RetroBead-labeled neurons, averaged per mouse. [WT mPFC toy mice vs. WT LH toy mice, p 0.6463; WT mPFC toy mice vs. WT mPFC live mice, p<0.0001; WT LH toy mice vs. WT LH live mice, p<0.0001; WT mPFC live mice vs. WT LH live mice, p=0.0022; KO mPFC toy mice vs. KO LH toy mice, p=0.4612; KO mPFC toy mice vs. KO mPFC live mice, p=0.0034; KO LH toy mice vs. KO LH live mice, p=0.4885; KO mPFC live mice vs. KO LH live mice, p=0.1544; WT mPFC toy mice vs. KO mPFC toy mice, p=0.0001; WT mPFC live mice vs. KO mPFC live mice, p=0.1544; Projection p=0.0292; Genotype p<0.0001; Experience p<0.0001; Projection x Genotype p=0.1263; Genotype x Experience p<0.0001; Projection x Genotype x Experience p=0.8545; Three Way ANOVA with Benjamini and Hochberg Multiple Comparisons]. Mean ± SEM; *p<0.05, **p<0.01. Figure 1—source data 1. See also Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

Figure 1—source data 1. mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons are selectively activated during sequential social encounters with familiar and novel mice.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.005

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Higher number of c-Fos-positive neurons in the mPFC following social encounters.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) Schematic of behavioral paradigm, also applicable for Figure 1. (B) Representative images of c-Fos immunostaining in the mPFC. (C) Number of c-Fos positive neurons in the PL region of the mPFC of mice who had interacted with either toy mice or live mice 45 min prior to sacrifice (WT toy mice vs. Mecp2 KO toy mice, p=0.1019; WT toy mice vs. WT live mice, p=0.0077; Mecp2 KO toy mice vs. Mecp2 KO live mice, p=0.0234; WT live mice vs. Mecp2 KO live mice, p=0.0234; Interaction p=0.4001; Experience p=0.0008; Genotype p=0.0063; Two-Way ANOVA with Benjamini and Hochberg Multiple Comparisons; n = 9 mPFC sections from three mice in each condition). Scale bar 50 μm. Mean ± SEM; *p<0.05, **p<0.01.
Figure 1—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Higher number of c-Fos-positive neurons in the mPFC following social encounters.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.004

We obtained similar results in Mecp2 KO mice, with higher c-Fos intensities in mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons of mice interacting with live mice compared to those interacting with toy mice (p=0.0034, Three-Way ANOVA followed by B and H-MC; Figure 1D–E). However, the difference between mPFC- and LH-projecting vHIP neurons from mice that interacted with live mice was not statistically significant, suggesting the loss of selectivity in vHIP signaling in Mecp2 KO mice (p=0.1544, Three-Way ANOVA followed by B and H-MC; Figure 1D–E). In addition, the differences in c-Fos intensities between the toy mice and live mice conditions were smaller in Mecp2 KO mice compared to WT mice, likely due to the higher basal activity in the vHIP of Mecp2 KO mice (Calfa et al., 2011; Calfa et al., 2015). Consistent with the lower levels of neuronal activity reported previously in the mPFC (Kron et al., 2012), the PL region of the mPFC of Mecp2 KO mice had fewer c-Fos-positive neurons than that of WT mice that interacted with live mice (p=0.0234, Two-Way ANOVA followed by B and H-MC; Figure 1—figure supplement 1B). However, both WT and Mecp2 KO mice that interacted with live mice had more c-Fos-positive neurons compared to those that interacted with toy mice, indicating that the mPFC of both genotypes is more robustly activated during a social encounter than by exposure to novel inanimate objects (p<0.05, Two-Way ANOVA followed by B and H-MC; Figure 1—figure supplement 1).

Atypical social behavior and impaired social memory in Mecp2 KO mice

To assess social behaviors, we used a three-chamber interaction arena to sequentially test social preference and then social memory. For social preference, we allowed mice to explore either a chamber containing a novel mouse (stranger 1) restrained under an inverted pencil cup or a chamber containing an empty inverted pencil cup (Figure 2A). Both WT and Mecp2 KO mice spent more time investigating the cup containing stranger one compared to the empty cup. The discrimination index (DI) of this preference is statistically different than chance in both genotypes, and comparable between the two genotypes (n = 12 WT mice; n = 11 Mecp2 KO mice; One-sample t-test against chance p<0.0001 for both genotypes; Two-sample t-test p=0.3523; Figure 2B–C and Figure 2—figure supplement 1A). These data indicate that social preference is intact in Mecp2 KO mice. Immediately following the sociability test (within 2 min), we placed a second novel mouse (stranger 2) under the previously empty pencil cup, and again allowed the test mice to explore both chambers. Indicative of social memory for stranger one and their preference for novel mice, WT mice spent more time investigating the cup containing stranger 2 (n = 12 WT mice, p=0.0002, One-sample t-test; Figure 2B–C and Figure 2—figure supplement 1A). However, the DI of Mecp2 KO mice was significantly different than that of WT mice, and not statistically different than chance (n = 11 Mecp2 KO mice; Two-sample t-test p=0.0140; One-sample t-test p=0.9436; Figure 2B–C and Figure 2—figure supplement 1A), indicating a deficit in the social memory of the stranger one mouse that was encountered 2 min before.

Figure 2. Mecp2 KO mice have impaired social memory and atypical social interactions.

(A) Schematic of three-chamber social test. (B) Representative heat maps of WT and Mecp2 KO mice during the three-chamber social test. (C) Discrimination indices of sociability and memory tests. (n = 12 WT mice; n = 11 Mecp2 KO mice; WT vs. Mecp2 KO sociability, p=0.3523; WT vs. Mecp2 KO, memory p=0.0140; Two-sample Student’s t-test; WT sociability DI vs. chance, p<0.0001; Mecp2 KO sociability DI vs. chance, p<0.0001; WT memory DI vs. chance, p=0.0002; Mecp2 KO memory vs. chance, p=0.9436; One-sample Student’s t-test). (D) Image of unrestricted social memory paradigm. (E) Time spent performing different behaviors using automated scoring of unrestricted social encounters (n = 11 WT mice; n = 11 Mecp2 KO mice; Follow, p=0.0008; Rear Sniff, p=0.0055; Jump On, p=0.0020; Target of Aggressor, p=0.0857; Aggressor, p=0.1513; Digging, p=0.0052; Grooming Front, p=0.0025; Grooming Rear, p=0.1875; Wall Climb, p=0.1713; Wall Jump, p<0.0001; Shuffle, p=0.0007; Walk, p=0.4135; Student’s t-test or Mann-Whitney test dependent on normalcy). (F) Computer confidence scores in following behavior over the course of representative videos separated by target: novel mouse in bold and familiar opaque, first 4 min boxed. (G) Time the test mouse spent following, sniffing rear, or ‘jumping on’ either the novel or familiar mouse during the first 4 min of the video (n = 11 WT; n = 11 KO mice; WT Follow DI vs. chance, p<0.0001; WT Rear Sniff, p<0.0001; WT Jump On, p=0.0279; Mecp2 KO Follow p=0.6683, Mecp2 KO Rear Sniff, p=0.5656; Mecp2 KO Jump On, p=0.5665; Two-sample Student’s t-test;WT vs. Mecp2 KO Follow, p=0.0200; WT vs. Mecp2 KO Sniff, p=0.0229; WT vs. Mecp2 KO Jump On, p=0.0493; One-sample Student’s t-test). Mean ± SD; *p<0.05, **p<0.01. For memory tasks (C and G), * indicate comparisons between genotypes while # indicates differences of the discrimination indices from chance value (0). Figure 2—source data 1. See also Figure 2—figure supplements 12.

Figure 2—source data 1. Mecp2KO mice have impaired social memory and atypical social interactions.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.010

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Impaired social memory in Mecp2 KO mice.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Total investigation times in the three-chamber sociability and social memory tests (n = 12 WT mice; n = 11 Mecp2 KO mice). (B) Total times during unrestricted social behaviors (n = 11 WT mice; n = 11 Mecp2 KO mice). Mean ± SD.
Figure 2—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Impaired social memory in Mecp2KO mice.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.008
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. WT mice show preference for novel mice during the first 4 min of unrestricted interactions.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

(A) Time spent by test mice actively following sentinel mice over the total testing time. (B) Time spent by test mice actively following sentinel mice over the total testing time, plotted vs. the time spent by test mice investigating the chamber. (C) Cumulative percentage of following behavior over time, dotted line indicates 75%. (D) Discrimination index between novel and familiar sentinels over test time. (n = 10 WT mice).

To avoid potential confounds arising from testing social interactions with mice restrained under pencil cups, we implemented a behavioral assay in which WT or Mecp2 KO mice freely interacted simultaneously for 10 min with both a co-housed WT littermate and a novel age-matched WT mouse (Figure 2D). Unbiased scoring of social interactions by the machine-learning based Janelia Automatic Animal Behavior Annotator (JAABA) (Kabra et al., 2013; Ohayon et al., 2013; Robie et al., 2017) revealed that WT mice mainly engaged in following behavior. By contrast, Mecp2 KO mice followed and sniffed other mice significantly less than WT mice, but engaged in an atypical ‘jumping on’ behavior (Figure 2E). In addition, Mecp2 KO mice displayed less digging and facial grooming, more wall jumping, and more shuffled walking (n = 11 WT mice, n = 11 Mecp2 KO mice, p<0.05, Student’s t-test or Mann-Whitney test dependent on normalcy; Figure 2E). As a critical control for social interactions in the unrestricted arena and the three-chamber test, WT and Mecp2 KO mice spent a comparable amount of time walking (p=0.4135, Student’s t-test; Figure 2E). Overall, these data indicate that, while Mecp2 KO mice do display interest in other mice, they do so in an atypical manner.

During the first 4 min of the unrestricted assay, WT mice preferentially interacted with the novel mouse across all measured social behaviors, including following, sniffing, and ‘jumping on’ (n = 11 mice, p<0.05, One-sample t-test; Figure 2G and Figure 2—figure supplement 1B), after which social interaction declined regardless of the target mouse (Figure 2F and Figure 2—figure supplement 2). The discrimination indices between WT mice and Mecp2 KO mice were significantly different (p<0.05, Two-sample t-test; Figure 2G), and indeed Mecp2 KO mice showed no significant preference between the familiar and novel mouse (n = 11 mice, p>0.05, One-sample; Figure 2G and Figure 2—figure supplement 1B), consistent with previously observed deficits in social memory (see Figure 2C).

Increased influence of vHIP inputs on the mPFC network in Mecp2 KO mice

Because mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons are activated during social encounters and Mecp2 KO mice have impaired social memory and atypical social behaviors, we next characterized vHIP inputs to the mPFC in Mecp2 KO mice at the functional level. To identify hippocampal fibers in ex vivo slices of the mPFC, we injected the fluorescent tracer dextran-Alexa-594 into the vHIP and, after 2 weeks to allow its anterograde transfer, prepared brain slices at a 10° angle from the coronal plane (Parent et al., 2010) (Figure 3A). We evoked field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (fEPSPs) with single monopolar current pulses (100 µs) delivered through a theta-glass electrode placed in the fluorescently labeled vHIP fiber bundle and simultaneously imaged voltage-sensitive dye (VSD) signals, which are directly proportional to the slope of individual fEPSPs and follow their rise and decay kinetics (Grinvald et al., 1988; Chang and Jackson, 2003; Bandyopadhyay et al., 2005; Calfa et al., 2011) (Figure 3B and Figure 3—figure supplement 1). The amplitudes of VSD signals evoked by single pulse stimulation of vHIP afferents were larger in mPFC slices from Mecp2 KO mice compared to WT littermates at a range of stimulation intensities [n = 11 slices from 7 WT mice, n = 11/5 Mecp2 KO mice, p=0.0470, Two-way repeated measures (RM) ANOVA; Figure 3D and E]. The spatiotemporal spread of VSD signals throughout the mPFC slice was similar between Mecp2 KO and WT mice (p=0.4529, Two-way RM ANOVA; Figure 3F), although the spatial spread was significantly larger at lower stimulation intensities in slices from Mecp2 KO mice (p=0.0133, Mann-Whitney test; Figure 3G). Stimulating intracortical fibers with another theta glass electrode placed in layer 2/3 of the same cortical column evoked VSD amplitudes of comparable amplitude in slices from Mecp2 KO and WT mice (n = 11 slices from 7 WT mice, n = 11/5 Mecp2 KO mice, p=0.0540; Two-way RM ANOVA; Figure 3H and I). By contrast, the spatiotemporal spread of VSD signals evoked by intracortical stimulation was significantly smaller in slices from Mecp2 KO mice (p=0.0498, Two-way RM ANOVA; Figure 3J and K). The amplitude and spatial spread of VSD signals evoked by vHIP stimulation were 72% and 71%, respectively, of those evoked by intracortical stimulation in mPFC slices from WT mice. However, the amplitude and spatial spread of VSD signals evoked by vHIP stimulation were 95% and 96% of those evoked by intracortical stimulation in Mecp2 KO slices, which reflects both larger vHIP-evoked signals and smaller responses to intracortical stimulation (Figure 3L–3N). These data indicate that vHIP fibers drive hyperactivation of the mPFC network in Mecp2 KO mice, in contrast to the hypoactivation driven by intracortical stimulation, suggesting that vHIP inputs are overrepresented in the mPFC network of Mecp2 KO mice.

Figure 3. Increased influence of vHIP axons in the mPFC of Mecp2 KO mice.

(A) Schematic of dextran injection into the vHIP. (B) Visualization of vHIP fibers in mPFC slices. Scale bar 0.5 mm; inset 100 µm. (C) VSD responses are proportional to the amplitude and follow the kinetics of fEPSPs. (D) Representative VSD responses evoked by stimulation of fluorescently-labeled vHIP fibers. Scale bar 100 µm (D and H). (E–G) Input-output relationship of peak VSD responses (E) p=0.047, Two-way ANOVA), spatiotemporal spread (F) p=0.4529, Two-way ANOVA), and spread over time (G) p=0.0133, Mann-Whitney) (at 75 µA intensity) evoked by vHIP fiber stimulation. (H) Representative VSD responses evoked by intracortical stimulation. (I–K) Input-output relationship of peak VSD responses (I) p=0.4553, Two-way ANOVA), spatiotemporal spread (J) p=0.0498, Two-way ANOVA), and spread over time (K) p=0.1025, Student’s t-test) (at 75 µA intensity) evoked by intracortical stimulation. (L–N) Peak VSD responses (L) p=0.0015, Two-way ANOVA), spatiotemporal spread (M) p=0.0767, Two-way ANOVA), and spread over time (N) p=0.0002, Mann-Whitney) evoked by vHIP fiber stimulation normalized to those evoked by intracortical stimulation. [n = 11 slices from seven mice (11/7) WT mice; n = 11/5 Mecp2 KO]. Spatiotemporal spread = AOC created by spread of the cortical area (% of total) and time (ms). Mean ± SEM; *p<0.05, **p<0.01. Figure 3—source data 1. See also Figure 3—figure supplements 12.

Figure 3—source data 1. Increased influence of vHIP axons in the mPFC of Mecp2KO mice.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.015

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Input-output curves of VSD signal amplitudes and initial slope of fEPSP.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(A) Representative images of VSD signals at different stimulus intensities from a representative experiment. (B) Representative traces of dF/F VSD signals and fEPSPs evoked by different stimulus intensities. (C) Input/output relationships of the initial slope of fEPSPs and VSD signals from a representative experiment.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Impaired LTP at vHIP-mPFC synapses in Mecp2 KO mice.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

(A) Schematic of VSD imaging in an mPFC slice during stimulation of vHIP fibers. (B) Representative images of peak VSD responses to a single pulse stimulation before and after high-frequency stimulation to induce LTP. Scale bar 100 µm. (C) Normalized spatiotemporal spread of single test VSD responses before and after LTP induction; high-frequency stimulation given at arrow. (D–F) Spread over time of single test VSD responses at baseline (1) and 30 min after LTP induction (2), normalized to baseline spread in WT slices (D), Mecp2 KO slices (E), and WT slices in the presence of the NMDA receptor antagonist APV (25 μM) (F) (p=0.0013 Student’s paired t-test, n = 10 slices from 7 WT mice; p=0.2705 Student’s paired t-test, n = 9/5 Mecp2 KO mice; p=0.9205 Student’s paired t-test, n = 4/4 WT mice in APV). Mean ± SEM; *p<0.05, **p<0.01.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2—source data 1. Impaired LTP at vHIP-mPFC synapses in Mecp2KO mice.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.014

Considering the role of long-term synaptic plasticity in memory, we tested the ability of excitatory vHIP-mPFC synapses to undergo long-term potentiation (LTP) in slices from Mecp2 KO mice, because previous studies have described plasticity at these synapses in rats and mice in vivo (Izaki et al., 2003; Izaki et al., 2001; Laroche et al., 2000). In mPFC slices from WT mice, high-frequency stimulation of vHIP afferents evoked a significant potentiation of the spatiotemporal spread of VSD signals, which persisted up to 45 min (n = 10 slices from seven mice, p=0.0013, Student’s paired t-test, Figure 3—figure supplement 1D) and was sensitive to the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor (NMDAR) antagonist APV (100 µM) (n = 4 slices from four mice, p=0.9205; Student’s paired t-test; Figure 3—figure supplement 1F). However, mPFC slices from Mecp2 KO mice showed only a short-term enhancement of the spatiotemporal spread of VSD signals, which quickly decayed back to baseline levels (n = 9 slices from five mice, p=0.2705; Student’s paired t-test; Figure 3—figure supplement 1E). These data demonstrate an impairment of LTP at excitatory vHIP-mPFC synapses, similar to that previously reported at CA3-CA1 synapses in hippocampal slices of Mecp2 KO mice (Li et al., 2016).

Selective chemogenetic manipulation of mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons regulates social memory

To causally link the enhanced vHIP input to the mPFC with the deficits in social behavior in Mecp2 KO mice, we used an intersectional genetic approach to express ‘designer receptors exclusively activated by designer drugs’ (DREADDs) selectively in mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons, and then modulate their activity with the designer ligand clozapine-N-oxide (CNO) (Armbruster et al., 2007; Boender et al., 2014). We injected a retrogradely transported canine adenovirus-2 (CAV-2) expressing Cre recombinase (Cre; CAV-2-Cre) bilaterally into the mPFC. We then injected adeno-associated virus serotype 8 (AAV8) expressing either excitatory (hM3Dq) or inhibitory (hM4Di) DREADDs from a Cre-dependent double-floxed inverse open reading frame (DIO) (Hnasko et al., 2006; Kremer et al., 2000) bilaterally in the vHIP of WT and Mecp2 KO mice at P20 (Figure 4A). Control mice injected with CAV2-Cre and AAV8-DIO-mCherry were also treated with CNO to account for potential peripheral conversion of CNO into clozapine (Gomez et al., 2017). This intersectional approach resulted in sparse labeling of pyramidal neurons in the ventral CA1 region with their axons projecting to the mPFC (Figure 4B). To modulate the vHIP-mPFC circuit in a long-term manner, we delivered CNO via the drinking water (5 mg/kg/day) (Carvalho Poyraz et al., 2016) beginning at P34 and continuing until we used mice for experiments (Figure 4A). Such long-term activation of DREADD receptors via CNO has been validated previously, with neurons expressing the excitatory DREADD hM3Dq in slices from mice exposed to CNO for 14 days still showing an increased firing rate after CNO application (Cheng et al., 2019). At the age of viral injections and the start of CNO treatment, Mecp2 KO mice lack the behavioral and cellular features that will develop into Rett-like symptoms after P45 (Calfa et al., 2011; Durand et al., 2012; Tomassy et al., 2014). In addition, we confirmed that P20-25 Mecp2 KO mice performed at WT levels in terms of social memory (Figure 4—figure supplement 1), VSD signals in mPFC evoked by either vHIP or intracortical stimulation (Figure 4—figure supplement 2A–K), and LTP at vHIP-mPFC synapses (Figure 4—figure supplement 2L–O). Notably, the amplitude of vHIP-evoked VSD signals in mPFC slices from Mecp2 KO mice did not show the typical developmental reduction between P20-25 and P45-50 observed in WT slices, resulting in significantly larger responses in symptomatic Mecp2 KO mice compared to age-matched WT mice (Figure 4—figure supplement 3), similar to CA3-evoked VSD responses in CA1 of hippocampal slices (Calfa et al., 2011; Li et al., 2016).

Figure 4. Activity of mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons modulates social memory in WT and Mecp2 KO mice.

(A) Schematic of CAV2-Cre and DREADD injections and experimental timeline. (B) Injection sites show sparse mCherry labeling of vHIP neurons with identifiable axons in the mPFC. Scale bar 500 µm large, 100 µm inset. (C) Discrimination Index of following familiar versus novel mice in unrestricted social interaction, scored by JAABA (n = 10 mCherry WT mice, p=0.0202; n = 12 hM3Dq WT mice, p=0.6905; n = 9 hM4Di WT mice, p=0.3332; n = 10 mCherry Mecp2 KO, p=0.1322; n = 15 Mecp2 KO mice, p=0.0312; One-sample Student’s t-test against chance. mCherry WT vs. hM3Dq WT mice, p=0.0287; mCherry WT vs. hM4Di WT mice, p=0.0081; mCherry WT vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO mice, p=0.0187; mCherry Mecp2 KO vs. hM3Dq WT mice p=0.4674; mCherry Mecp2 KO vs. hM4Di WT mice, p=0.6108; mCherry Mecp2 KO mice vs. hM4Di Mecp2 KO mice, p=0.0287; ANOVA p=0.0048; One-Way ANOVA with Benjamini and Hochberg Multiple Comparisons). (D) Time spent ‘jumping on’ other mice during unrestricted social interaction (n = 9 mCherry WT mice; n = 12 hM3Dq WT mice; n = 9 hM4Di WT mice; n = 10 mCherry Mecp2 KO; n = 15 hM4Di Mecp2 KO mice; mCherry WT vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO, p=0.0215; hM3Dq WT vs. mCherry WT, p>0.9999; hM3Dq WT vs. hM4Di WT, p>0.9999; hM3Dq WT vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO, p=0.2119; hM4Di WT vs. mCherry WT, p>0.9999; hM4Di WT vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO, p=0.0194; mCherry Mecp2 KO vs. hM4Di (All) Mecp2 KO, p>0.9999; hM4Di (All) Mecp2 KO vs. mCherry WT, p=0.0016; Memory hM4Di vs. mCherry KO, p>0.9999; Memory hM4Di vs. mCherry WT, p=0.0012; No Memory hM4Di vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO, p>0.9999; No Memory hM4Di vs. mCherry WT, p=0.8023; Memory hM4Di vs. No Memory hM4Di, p>0.9999, Kruskal-Wallis test with Dunn’s multiple corrections). (E) Representative VSD responses evoked by vHIP fiber stimulation in CNO-treated mice. Scale bar 200 µm. (F) Input-output relationships of peak VSD responses evoked by vHIP fiber stimulation. (mCherry WT vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO, p<0.001; hM3Dq WT vs. mCherry WT, p<0.0001; hM3Dq WT vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO, p=0.6530; mCherry WT vs. hM4Di, p=0.0642; hM4Di WT vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO, p=0.0013; hM4Di Mecp2 KO vs. mCherry WT, p=0.0333; hM4Di Mecp2 KO vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO, p=0.0016; Interaction p<0.0001; Stim p<0.0001; Group p=0.0193; Two-way RM ANOVA with Benjamini and Hochberg Multiple Comparisons). (G) Representative VSD responses evoked by intracortical stimulation in CNO-treated mice. (H) Input-output relationships of peak VSD responses evoked by intracortical stimulation (mCherry WT vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO, p<0.0001; hM3Dq WT vs. mCherry WT, p<0.0001; hM3Dq WT vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO, p 0.2164; mCherry WT vs. hM4Di WT, p=0.8296; hM4Di WT vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO, p<0.0001; hM4Di Mecp2 KO vs. mCherry WT, p=0.0023; hM4Di Mecp2 KO vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO, p=0.0726; Interaction p=0.2985; Stim p<0.0001; Group p=0.0479; Two-way RM ANOVA with Benjamini and Hochberg Multiple Comparisons). (I) Correlation between social memory DIs and VSD responses evoked by either vHIP fiber (closed circles) or intracortical stimulation (open circles) (n = 7 slices from 7 hM4Di Mecp2 KO mice; Spearman r correlation; r = −0.7895, p=0.0347 vHIP fiber; r = −0.1519, p=0.7451 intracortical). (J–K) Input-output relationships of peak VSD responses evoked by vHIP fiber stimulation in slices from hM4Di Mecp2 KO mice with intact or impaired social memory (Memory vs. mCherry WT, p=0.3043; Memory vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO, p<0.0001; No Memory vs. mCherry WT, p<0.0001; No Memory vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO, p=0.0406; Memory vs. No Memory, p<0.0001; Interaction p<0.0001; Stim p<0.0001; Group p=0.0056; Two-way RM ANOVA with Benjamini and Hochberg Multiple Comparisons). (L–M) Input-output relationships of peak VSD responses evoked by intracortical stimulation in slices from hM4Di Mecp2 KO mice with intact or impaired social memory (Memory vs. mCherry WT, p=0.0002; Memory vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO, p=0.3043; No Memory vs. mCherry WT, p=0.2171, No Memory vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO, p=0.0720; Memory vs. No Memory, p=0.2171; Interaction p=0.2241; Stim p<0.0001; Group p=0.0761; Two-way RM ANOVA with Benjamini and Hochberg Multiple Comparisons). (E–M) n = 18 slices from 9 mCherry (18/9) WT mice; n = 12/8 hM3Dq WT mice; n = 16/9 hM4Di WT mice; n = 17/8 mCherry Mecp2 KO mice; n = 20/10 hM4Di Mecp2 KO mice; n = 11/6 hM4Di Mecp2 KO memory mice; n = 6/4 hM4Di Mecp2 KO no memory mice). (C–D) Mean ± SD; (F,H,K,M) Mean ± SEM; *p<0.05, **p<0.01. Figure 4—source data 1. See also Figure 4—figure supplements 15.

Figure 4—source data 1. VSD responses to vHIP stimulation in mPFC slices and LTP at vHIP-mPFC synapses are not altered in presymptomatic Mecp2KO mice.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.027

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Social memory is not altered in presymptomatic Mecp2 KO mice.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(A) Discrimination index of time spent investigating the empty inverted pencil cup or the cup containing either stranger 1 or stranger 2 (WT sociability, p=0.0001; Mecp2 KO sociability, p=0.0046; WT memory, p=0.0149; Mecp2 KO memory, p=0.0027; One-sample t-test against chance; WT vs.Mecp2 KO sociability, p=0.1394; WT vs. Mecp2 KO memory, p=0.6001; two-sample t-test; n = 8 WT mice; n = 10 Mecp2 KO). * denotes significant differences between groups, # denotes significant difference from chance values. (B) Absolute times for three chamber social task. Mean ± SD; *, # p<0.05, **p<0.01.
Figure 4—figure supplement 1—source data 1. VSD responses to vHIP stimulation in mPFC slices and LTP at vHIP-mPFC synapses are not altered in presymptomatic Mecp2KO mice.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.018
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. VSD responses to vHIP stimulation in mPFC slices and LTP at vHIP-mPFC synapses are not altered in presymptomatic Mecp2 KO mice.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

(A) Schematic of VSD imaging in an mPFC slice during stimulation of vHIP fibers and representative VSD responses evoked by a single vHIP fiber stimulation. Scale bar 100 µm. (B–D) Input-output relationships of VSD peak responses (B) p=0.424, Two-way ANOVA), spatiotemporal spread (C) p=0.625, Two-way RM ANOVA), and spread over time (D) p=0.2918 Student’s t-test) (at 150 µA intensity) evoked by a single vHIP fiber stimulation. (E) Schematic of VSD imaging in an mPFC slice during stimulation of intracortical fibers and representative VSD responses evoked by a single intracortical stimulation. (F–H) Input-output relationships of VSD peak responses (F) p=0.4663, Two-way ANOVA), spatiotemporal spread (G) p=0.3972, Two-Way RM ANOVA), and spread over time (H) p=0.2582, Student’s t-test) (at 150 µA intensity) evoked by a single intracortical stimulation. (I–K) Peak VSD responses (I) p=0.2278, Two-way RM ANOVA), spatiotemporal spread (J) p=0.5857, Two-way ANOVA), and spread over time (K) p=0.9145, Student’s t-test) evoked by a single vHIP fiber stimulation normalized to those evoked by a single intracortical stimulation. (A–K) n = 14 slices from five mice WT; n = 18 5 Mecp2 KO mice). (L) Representative images of peak VSD responses to a single test stimulation before and after high-frequency stimulation to induce LTP. (M) Normalized spatiotemporal spread of single test VSD responses before and after LTP induction; high-frequency stimulation given at arrow. (N–O) Spread over time of single test VSD responses at baseline (1) and 40 min after LTP induction (2), normalized to baseline peak spread in WT slices (N) p=0.0159, Wilcoxon paired test) and Mecp2 KO slices (o) p=2.165×10−5, Wilcoxon paired test) (L–N) n = 5 slices from 5 WT mice; n = 9/5 Mecp2 KO mice). Spatiotemporal spread is the AOC created by spread of the cortical area (% of total) and time (ms). Mean ± SEM; *p<0.05, **p<0.01.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2—source data 1. VSD responses to vHIP stimulation in mPFC slices of Mecp2KO mice do not develop in a typical manner.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.020
Figure 4—figure supplement 3. VSD responses to vHIP stimulation in mPFC slices of Mecp2 KO mice do not develop in a typical manner.

Figure 4—figure supplement 3.

(A–B) Peak VSD responses evoked by either (A) a single vHIP fiber stimulation or (B) a single intracortical stimulation in mPFC slices from WT and Mecp2 KO mice at P20-25 and P45-50 (WT young vs.WT adult vHIP p=0.0151; WT young vs. Mecp2 KO young vHIP p=0.4678; Mecp2 KO young vs. Mecp2 KO adult vHIP p=0.4678; WT adult vs. Mecp2 KO adult vHIP p=0.0157; Interaction p=0.1166; Age p=0.0061; Genotype p=0.0099; Two-Way ANOVA with Benjamini and Hochberg Multiple Comparisons; WT young vs. WT adult intracortical p=0.0020; WT young vs. Mecp2 KO young intracortical p=0.4145; Mecp2 KO young vs. Mecp2 KO adult intracortical p=0.0018; WT adult vs. Mecp2 KO adult intracortical p=0.3699; Interaction p=0.8253; Age p<0.0001; Genotype p=0.1946; Two-Way ANOVA with Benjamini and Hochberg Multiple Comparisons). (C–D) Spatiotemporal spread of VSD signals evoked by (C) vHIP afferent stimulation or (D) intracortical stimulation in mPFC slices from WT and Mecp2 KO mice at P20-25 and P45-50 (WT young vs. WT adult vHIP p<0.0001; WT young vs. Mecp2 KO young vHIP, p=0.9532; Mecp2 KO young vs. Mecp2 KO adult vHIP, p<0.001; WT adult vs. Mecp2 KO adult vHIP, p=0.9532; Interaction p=0.9336; Age p<0.0001; Genotype p=0.8700; Two-Way ANOVA with Benjamini and Hochberg Multiple Comparisons; WT young vs. WT adult intracortical, p<0.0001; WT young vs. Mecp2 KO young intracortical, p=0.2549; Mecp2 KO young vs. Mecp2 KO adult intracortical p<0.0001; WT adult vs. Mecp2 KO adult intracortical, p=0.6539; Interaction p=0.2358; Age p<0.0001; Genotype p=0.5842; Two-Way ANOVA with Benjamini and Hochberg Multiple Comparisons) (n = 14 slices from 5 P20-25 WT mice; n = 18/5 P20-25 Mecp2 KO, n = 11/7 P45-55 WT mice, n = 11/5 P45-55 Mecp2 KO mice; Spatiotemporal spread = AOC created by spread of the cortical area (% of total) and time (ms). Mean ± SEM; *p<0.05, **p<0.01.
Figure 4—figure supplement 3—source data 1. Lack of effects of long-term DREADD stimulation by CNO on anxiety and general (non-social memory) behaviors.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.022
Figure 4—figure supplement 4. Lack of effects of long-term DREADD stimulation by CNO on anxiety and general (non-social memory) behaviors.

Figure 4—figure supplement 4.

(A) Total times spent following novel vs. familiar sentinels. (B) Representative tracks of individual trajectories of test mice during social encounters in an open field. (C) Average time spent in the center of the open field, as a measure of anxiety (mCherry vs. hM3Dq WT p>0.9999; mCherry WT vs. Mecp2 KO, p=0.9793; hM3Dq WT vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO, p=0.9566; mCherry vs. hM4Di WT, p>0.9999; hM4Di WT vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO, p=0.9695; hM3Dq WT vs. hM4Di WT p>0.9999; mCherry vs. hM4Di (All) Mecp2 KO, p=0.8910; hM4Di (All) Mecp2 KO vs. mCherry WT, p>0.9999; mCherry NAc vs. mCherry mPFC WT, p=0.9492; mCherry vs. hM3Dq NAc WT, p=0.9952; Memory vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO, p=0.9727; Memory vs. mCherry WT, p>0.9999; No Memory vs. mCherry Mecp2 KO, p=0.8493; No Memory vs. mCherry WT, p=0.9941; Memory vs. No Memory, p=0.9947; One-way ANOVA with Turkey’s multiple comparisons). (D–F) Time spent engaging in social, aggressive, general, and locomotor behaviors during the unrestricted social interaction assay in (D) DREADD-expressing WT mice, (E) Mecp2 KO mice, and hM4Di Mecp2 KO mice separated by memory performance (F) after long-term CNO treatment (n = 9 mCherry WT mice; n = 12 hM3Dq WT mice; n = 9 hM4Di WT mice; One-Way ANOVA or Kruskal-Wallis test dependent on normalcy; n = 10 mCherry Mecp2 KO; n = 15 hM4Di Mecp2 KO mice; Student’s t-test or Mann-Whitney test dependent on normalcy).
Figure 4—figure supplement 4—source data 1. Memory Discrimination correlates to vHIP input to the mPFC in Mecp2KO mice.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.024
Figure 4—figure supplement 5. Memory Discrimination correlates to vHIP input to the mPFC in Mecp2 KO mice.

Figure 4—figure supplement 5.

(A) Correlation between social memory DI and VSD responses evoked by vHIP fiber stimulation in Mecp2 KO mice expressing mCherry (red) or hM4Di (yellow), with correlation for pooled data (n = 10 slices from 10 Mecp2 KO mice; Spearman r correlation; r = −0.6855, p=0.0287). (B) Correlation between social memory DI and VSD responses evoked by vHIP fiber stimulation in WT KO mice expressing mCherry (blue), hM3Dq (green), or hM4Di (purple), with correlation for pooled data (n = 15 slices from 15 WT KO mice; Spearman r correlation; r = 0.2415, p=0.3859).
Figure 4—figure supplement 5—source data 1. Activity of mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons modulates social memory in WT and Mecp2KO mice.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.026

When tested in the unrestricted social assay, P45 WT mice expressing only the marker mCherry in mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons and treated with CNO for 11 days had a significant preference for the novel mouse (n = 10, p=0.00.0202; One-sample t-test against chance; Figure 4C), similar to naive untreated WT mice. CNO-treated WT mice expressing the excitatory DREADD hM3Dq in mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons (to mimic vHIP hyperactivity in Mecp2 KO mice) had a significantly lower DI, which was not different than chance (n = 12, One-way ANOVA followed by B and H-MC, p=0.0287; One-sample t-test, p=0.6905; Figure 4C), indicating a deficit in social memory resembling that of Mecp2 KO mice. Chronic inhibition of mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons with the inhibitory DREADD hM4Di also caused a significant decrease in the DI and impaired social memory in WT mice (n = 9, One-way ANOVA followed by B and H-MC, p=0.0081; One-sample t-test, p=0.3332;) (Figure 4C), underscoring the role of this long-range projection in social memory.

To further define the consequences of altered vHIP-mPFC signaling in social behaviors, we inhibited mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons in Mecp2 KO mice with the inhibitory DREADD hM4Di and CNO administration from P34 until P45 to selectively reduce the hyperactivation of this long-range circuit. This manipulation was sufficient to increase the time following the novel mouse compared to the familiar littermate in 73% of the treated Mecp2 KO mice, resulting in a significant preference for the novel mouse, and indicating a rescue of social memory (n = 15, One-sample t-test, p=0.0312, Figure 4C and Figure 4—figure supplement 4A). This DI was significantly higher than control CNO-treated Mecp2 KO mice expressing mCherry, which did not discriminate between the novel mouse and the familiar littermate (n = 10, One-way ANOVA followed by B and H-MC, p=0.0287; One-sample t-test, p=0.1322; Figure 4C and Figure 4—figure supplement 4A), similar to naive untreated Mecp2 KO mice. In addition, there were no changes in the types of social behaviors displayed by CNO-treated mice expressing DREADDs in mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons. For example, the amount of time performing the atypical ‘jumping on’ behavior did not differ between hM4Di-expressing and mCherry control Mecp2 KO mice (p>0.9999 within genotypes; p<0.05 between genotypes; Kruskal-Wallis test followed by Dunn’s Multiple Comparisons; Figure 4D), suggesting that the vHIP-mPFC projection plays a specific role in the memory aspect of social interactions. Other than a small reduction of walking time in hM3Dq-expressing WT mice (p=0.0296, One-way ANOVA), there were no differences in grooming behavior, locomotion, or anxiety-like behaviors between DREADD-expressing WT and Mecp2 KO mice and their mCherry-expressing controls after CNO treatment (p>0.05; Figure 4—figure supplement 4B–F).

After allowing 3–4 days for the potential effects of behavioral testing to fade, we prepared ex vivo mPFC slices from CNO-treated DREADD- and mCherry-expressing mice for VSD imaging. Consistent with their deficit in social memory, WT mice expressing the excitatory DREADD hM3Dq in mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons had larger vHIP-induced VSD signals in mPFC slices compared to mCherry-expressing WT controls, resembling those observed in mCherry-expressing Mecp2 KO controls (n = 18 slices from 9 mCherry WT mice; n = 12/8 hM3Dq WT; n = 17/8 Mecp2 KO mice; p<0.0001; p=0.6530; Two-way RM ANOVA; Figure 4E–F). Surprisingly, inhibiting mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons with hM4Di did not affect vHIP-induced VSD signals in mPFC slices of WT mice (p=0.0642; Figure 4E-F). While unexpected, we cannot rule out the possibility that other afferents may have increased their input to the mPFC, as reported previously (Guirado et al., 2016), or that other homeostatic mechanisms might have maintained proper activity levels in the mPFC. Interestingly, we still observed impaired memory performance, suggesting that dysfunction of the vHIP-mFPC pathway is sufficient to impair memory formation in the absence of large-scale changes to the mPFC network. As a control, mCherry expression in mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons followed by CNO treatment did not alter the difference in the amplitude of vHIP-induced VSD signals in mPFC slices between WT and Mecp2 KO mice (p<0.0001; Figure 4F), which resemble those in naive untreated mice (see Figure 3E).

Because 27% of Mecp2 KO mice expressing the inhibitory DREADD hM4Di in mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons did not show an improvement of social memory in the unrestricted test (see Figure 4C), we correlated their social memory DI with the amplitude of vHIP-induced VSD signals in mPFC slices on a mouse-per-mouse basis. This analysis uncovered a statistically significant negative correlation between DI and vHIP-induced mPFC responses (n = 7 pairs, r = −0.7895 p=0.0347, Spearman r correlation; Figure 4I). This correlation was also observed when data from mCherry- and hM4Di-expressing Mecp2 KO mice were pooled together, indicating that the extent of dysfunction in the vHIP-mPFC projection underlies social memory impairments. Interestingly, this correlation is not statistically significant in any of the WT groups, which suggest an intriguing contribution of the altered mPFC microcircuit in Mecp2 KO mice (Figure 4—figure supplement 5). Furthermore, vHIP-induced VSD signals in mPFC slices from Mecp2 KO mice that showed improved social memory after expression of hM4Di in mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons were smaller than those in mCherry-expressing Mecp2 KO controls (n = 11/6, p<0.0001, Two-way ANOVA; Figure 4J–K), resembling those in mCherry-expressing WT mice (p=0.3043). By contrast, vHIP-induced VSD signals in mPFC slices from Mecp2 KO mice that still showed deficits in social memory after expression of hM4Di in mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons were significantly larger than those in mCherry-expressing WT mice (p<0.0001; Two-way ANOVA; Figure 4J–K). In addition, vHIP-evoked VSD signals in the mPFC of hM4Di-expressing Mecp2 KO mice showing improved social memory DI were also larger than mCherry Mecp2 KO controls (p=0.0406). Other than these effects on the social memory DI, no behavioral differences were observed between the hM4Di-expressing Mecp2 KO mice with improved and impaired memory (Figure 4C–D and Figure 4—figure supplement 4).

Selective chemogenetic excitation of mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons in WT mice from P34 to P45 also affected VSD signals evoked by intracortical stimulation in layer 2/3 of mPFC slices. VSD responses in hM3Dq-expressing WT mice were significantly smaller than those in mCherry-expressing WT mice, and resemble those in mCherry-expressing Mecp2 KO mice (n = 18 slices from 9 mCherry WT mice; n = 12/8 hM3Dq WT; n = 17/8 Mecp2 KO mice; p=0.0004; p=0.8522; Two-way RM ANOVA, Figure 4G–H). However, chemogenetic inhibition with hM4Di did not affect VSD signals evoked by intracortical stimulation in WT mice (n = 16/9, p=0.2741, Figure 4G–H). VSD signals evoked by intracortical stimulation in mPFC slices from Mecp2 KO mice expressing hM4Di in mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons were not significantly different than those in mCherry-expressing Mecp2 KO controls, and were smaller than those in mCherry-expressing WT mice (n = 20/10; p=0.0311; p=0.5691; Figure 4G–H). Importantly, there was no significant correlation between the social memory DI and the amplitude of VSD signals evoked by intracortical stimulation in hM4Di-expressing Mecp2 KO mice (n = 7 pairs, r = −0.1519, p=0.7451, Spearman r correlation; Figure 4I), and no difference in these signals between Mecp2 KO mice that showed improvement in social memory and those that did not (n = 11/6 Memory; n = 6/4 No memory; p=0.3359; Two-way RM ANOVA; Figure 4L–M). Combined, these results indicate that selective chemogenetic modulation of mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons has specific consequences on the functional strength of this projection, but has smaller effects on other afferent inputs to the mPFC recruited by intracortical stimulation.

We next acutely manipulated neuronal activity to test whether the vHIP-mPFC projection is required for social memory recall, as opposed to the maintenance or initial formation of a social memory. We removed littermate sentinels from the home cage and administered a single intraperitoneal (i.p.) injection of CNO (1 mL/0.5 mg/100 g body weight) 2 hr before the unrestricted social test (Figure 5A). These i.p. CNO injections did not affect social memory in mCherry-expressing WT mice as their DI was significantly higher than chance (n = 10, p=0.009, One-sample t-test), whereas acute excitation of mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons by CNO activation of hM3Dq impaired social memory in WT mice (n = 10 hM3Dq WT mice, p=0.7775, One-sample t-test; Figure 5B and Figure 5—figure supplement 1). However, the DI was not statistically different between controls and hM3Dq-expressing WT mice in the mPFC projections group due to the large variability in the behavior of hM3Dq group. (p=0.2214, One-Way ANOVA followed by B and H-MC). Intriguingly, acute inhibition of mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons with the inhibitory DREADD hM4Di in Mecp2 KO mice caused them to have a significant preference for the familiar mouse (n = 6, p=0.0004, One-sample t-test Figure 5B and Figure 5—figure supplement 1A) and show a difference in DI compared to mCherry expressing Mecp2 KO mice whose DI was not different than chance (n = 10, p=0.8081, One-sample t-test; mCherry vs. hM4Di Mecp2 KO p=0.0197, One-Way ANOVA followed by B and H-MC). This effect was opposite to that of long-term inhibition of the vHIP-mPFC projection in Mecp2 KO mice (see Figure 4C) and the preference of naive untreated WT mice to follow the novel mouse (see Figure 2G). Despite these differences in social preference with WT mice, acute inhibition of the vHIP-mPFC projection caused Mecp2 KO mice to display a significant preference in targeted social interactions, in contrast to control mCherry-expressing Mecp2 KO mice.

Figure 5. Acute manipulation of activity of vHIP-mPFC projection neurons regulates social memory in a task- and projection-specific manner.

(A) Experimental timeline for acute DREADD manipulation of the vHIP-mPFC projection. (B) Time spent following either familiar or novel mice in unrestricted social interaction, scored by JAABA (n = 8 vHIP-mPFC mCherry WT mice, p=0.0135 Student’s paired t-test; n = 10 vHIP-mPFC hM3Dq WT mice, p=0.6052 Student’s paired t-test; n = 10 vHIP-mPFC mCherry Mecp2 KO mice, p=0.9219 Wilcoxon paired test; n = 8 vHIP-mPFC hM4Di Mecp2 KO mice, p=0.0469 Wilcoxon paired test). (C) Schematic of novel object recognition test. (D) Time spent sniffing either the familiar or novel object, scored by JAABA (n = 8 vHIP-mPFC mCherry WT mice, p=0.006 Student’s paired t-test; n = 10 vHIP-mPFC hM3Dq WT mice, p=0.0266 Student’s paired t-test). (E) Schematic of CAV2-Cre and DREADD injections to manipulate the vHIP-NAc projection. Injection sites show sparse mCherry labeling of vHIP neurons with identifiable axons in the NAc. Time spent following either familiar or novel mice in unrestricted social interaction, scored by JAABA (n = 10 vHIP-NAc mCherry WT mice, p=0.0391 Wilcoxon paired test; n = 11 vHIP-NAc hM3Dq WT mice, p=0.0189 Student’s paired t-test). Mean ± SD; *p<0.05, **p<0.01. Figure 5—source data 1. See also Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

Figure 5—source data 1. Acute manipulation of activity of vHIP-mPFC projection neurons regulates social memory in a task- and projection-specific manner.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.031

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Lack of effects of Acute DREADD stimulation by CNO on anxiety and general (non-social memory) behaviors.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(A and C) Total times spent following novel vs.familiar sentinels in DREADDs expressed in mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons (A) or NAc-Projecting vHIP neurons (C). (B) Total times spent for sniffing novel vs. familiar objects in DREADDs expressed in mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons. (D) Representative tracks of individual trajectories of test mice during social encounters in an open field. (E) Average time spent in the center of the open field, as a measure of anxiety (mCherry mPFC vs. mPFC hM3Dq WT, p>0.9999; mCherry mPFC WT vs. Mecp2 KO, p=0.9937; hM3Dq WT mPFC vs. mCherry mPFC Mecp2 KO, p=0.9718; mCherry vs. hM4Di mPFC Mecp2 KO, p=0.8502; hM4Di mPFC Mecp2 KO vs. mCherry mPFC WT, p=0.9916; One-Way ANOVA). (F–H) Time spent engaging in social, aggressive, general, and locomotor behaviors during the unrestricted social interaction assay in (F) vHIP-mPFC DREADD-expressing WT mice, (G) and vHIP-mPFC Mecp2 KO mice (n = 10 vHIP-mPFC mCherry WT mice; n = 10 vHIP-mPFC hM3Dq WT mice; n = 11 vHIP-mPFC mCherry Mecp2 KO; n = 8 vHIP-mPFC hM4Di Mecp2 KO mice). (H) Time spent engaging in social, aggressive, general, and locomotor behaviors during the unrestricted social interaction assay in DREADD-expressing vHIP-NAc WT mice after acute CNO treatment (n = 8 vHIP-NAc mCherry WT mice; n = 10 vHIP-NAc hM3Dq WT mice; mCherry NAc vs. mCherry mPFC WT, p=0.9492; mCherry vs. hM3Dq NAc WT, p=0.9952; hM3Dq NAc WT vs. mCherry mPFC WT, p=0.9982; hM3Dq NAc WT vs. mCherry mPFC Mecp2 KO, p=0.9009; Student’s t-test or Mann-Whitney Test dependent on Normalcy). Mean ± SD; *p<0.05, **p<0.01.
Figure 5—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Acute manipulation of activity of vHIP-mPFC projection neurons regulates social memory in a task- and projection-specific manner.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.030

We next determined whether the vHIP-mPFC projection encodes novelty in general, or specifically social novelty, by testing the acute effect of its activation on the novel object recognition test. WT mice expressing the excitatory DREADD hM3Dq in mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons showed the same discrimination for the novel object as control mCherry-expressing mice 2 hr after a single i.p. injection of CNO (n = 8 WT mCherry mice, p=0.0058; n = 10 hM3Dq, p=0.0172; One-sample t-test; mCherry vs. hM3Dq p=0.3524; Two-sample t-test; Figure 5C–D and Figure 5—figure supplement 1B), indicating that altering the activity of the long-range vHIP-mPFC projection does not affect hippocampal-dependent novel object recognition.

Do all vHIP projection neurons contribute to social memory, or just those projecting to the mPFC? To address this question, we injected CAV-2-Cre into the nucleus accumbens (NAc) and either control AAV8-DIO-mCherry or AAV8-DIO-hM3Dq into the vHIP of WT mice for selective excitation of NAc-projecting vHIP neurons (Figure 5E). Both hM3Dq-expressing and mCherry-expressing WT mice had a significant preference for the novel mouse compared to the familiar mouse and were not different from each other (n = 10 WT mCherry mice, p=0.00058; n = 11 WT hM3Dq mice, p=0.072; One-sample t-test; mCherry vs. hM3Dq p=0.6294; Two-sample t-test; Figure 5F and Figure 5—figure supplement 1C), indicating that mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons, but not NAc-projecting vHIP neurons, are necessary for the expression of social memory. Other than a small increase of walking time and aggressive behaviors in WT mice with hM3Dq expression in the vHIP-mPFC projection (Walk, p=0.0438, Student's t-test; Aggression, p=0.0214, Mann-Whitney Test due to nonparametric distributions), there were no differences in grooming behavior, locomotion, or anxiety-like behaviors between DREADD-expressing WT and Mecp2 KO mice and their mCherry-expressing controls after CNO treatment (p>0.05; Figure 5—figure supplement 1D–H).

Altered synaptic connectivity of long-range vHIP-mPFC projections in Mecp2 KO mice

Differences in the spatiotemporal spread of VSD signals evoked by stimulation of the vHIP fiber bundle in mPFC slices between WT and Mecp2 KO mice could reflect alterations in the innervation pattern of vHIP axons on different postsynaptic cell types in the mPFC. Morphological and electrophysiological recordings in vivo and in ex vivo slices from rats and WT mice have demonstrated that pyramidal neurons of the ventral CA1 and subiculum form monosynaptic connections with pyramidal neurons in layers 2/3 and 5, as well as with inhibitory interneurons in the PL and infralimbic (IL) regions of the mPFC (Dembrow et al., 2015; Gabbott et al., 2002; Liu and Carter, 2018; Marek et al., 2018). However, a quantitative analysis of the pattern of vHIP innervation onto different postsynaptic cell types was lacking. To identify the first-order postsynaptic neurons innervated by vHIP axons, we injected the trans-synaptic marker wheat germ agglutinin (WGA) into the vHIP (Figure 6A). After 30 hr to allow axonal and trans-synaptic transport in the mPFC (Ruda and Coulter, 1982), we performed immunohistochemistry for WGA and for the neuronal marker NeuN to account for potential WGA injection variability. We identified postsynaptic excitatory neurons by retrograde labeling and classified them based on their axonal projections as pyramidal tract (PT) neurons by injecting FluoroGold (FG) in the dorsal periaqueductal gray (dPAG), or as intratelencephalic (IT) neurons by injecting FG in the contralateral mPFC (c-mPFC) (Figure 6B–D). We identified postsynaptic inhibitory neurons by immunohistochemistry of the markers parvalbumin (PV), calretinin (CAL), and somatostatin (SOM). Regarding the distribution of WGA-positive neurons and neuronal subtypes across the different layers of the mPFC, there were no differences between WT and Mecp2 KO mice for any of the cell types (Figure 6—figure supplement 1). In WT mice, the majority of WGA-labeled, NeuN-positive cells were projection pyramidal neurons, with 52% being IT neurons and 37% PT neurons, followed by 4% PV interneurons, 2% CAL interneurons, and 1% SOM interneurons (Figure 6E). The fraction of PT pyramidal neurons was significantly smaller in Mecp2 KO mice (16%) (n = 9 sections from three mice for both WT and Mecp2 KO mice, p=0.0128, Student’s t-test), whereas the fraction of PV interneurons was significantly larger in Mecp2 KO mice (9%) (p=0.0477, Student’s t-test; Figure 6F). There were no significant differences in the fraction of IT pyramidal neurons (51%), CAL interneurons, (1%), or SOM interneurons (2%) between Mecp2 KO and WT mice (p=0.8695; p=0.0621; p=0.2590; Student’s t-test; Figure 6F).

Figure 6. Trans-synaptic tracing of vHIP target neurons in the mPFC.

(A) WGA injection sites for brains to be used for immunohistochemistry of interneuron markers. Scale bar = 1 mm. (B) Injection sites for brains to be used for identifying contralateral projecting mPFC neurons. Scale bar = 1 mm. (C) Injection sites for brains to be used for identifying dPAG projecting mPFC neurons. Scale bar = 1 mm. (D) Representative examples of WGA identification of inhibitory and excitatory neurons receiving vHIP innervation. Scale bar = 25 µm. (E–F) Breakdown of WGA innervated neurons by subtype in WT (E) and Mecp2 KO mice (F) (IT p=0.8695; PT p=0.0128; CAL p=0.0621; SOM p=0.259; PV p=0.0477, Student’s t-test). (G) Discrimination index of innervated cells, with at ‘at chance’ innervation being 1 (IT p=0.9646; PT p=0.0161; CAL p=0.591; SOM p=0.208; PV p=0.0425, Student’s t-test). (H) Percent of neuron subtype receiving vHIP innervation (IT p=0.9646; PT p=0.0161; CAL p=0.591; SOM p=0.208; PV p=0.0425, Student’s t-test) (n = 9 sections three mice for all groups). Mean ± SEM; *p<0.05, **p<0.01. Figure 6—source data 1. See also Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

Figure 6—source data 1. Trans-synaptic tracing of vHIP target neurons in the mPFC.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.034

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Spatial distribution of different neuronal subtypes in the mPFC that are innervated by vHIP fibers in wild-type and Mecp2 KO mice.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

(A) Schematic of the PL region of the mPFC (left), and representative images of immunostaining for trans-synaptic WGA; PV, SOM, and CAL interneurons; and retrograde FluoroGold labeling of c-mPFC and dPAG pyramidal neurons in the PL-mPFC. Scale bar 50 μm. (B) Percent of each neuronal subtype as a function of distance from the pial surface (inter-hemispheric midline) (n = 15 sections from five mice each for WT and Mecp2 KO WGA staining, n = 9/3 for both WT and Mecp2 KO mice with c-mPFC, dPAG, PV, SOM, and CAL). Mean ± SEM (shaded area).

Although there is no evidence of neuronal cell death in RTT individuals and Mecp2-based mouse models (Chen et al., 2001), we accounted for potential differences in the density of different neuronal cell types by implementing a discrimination index for each mPFC neuron type that was trans-synaptically labeled with WGA. We gave an index of 1 when vHIP axons innervated neurons at chance values (having the same proportion of postsynaptic cell type in NeuN-positive and WGA-positive populations); by contrast, an index higher than one reflected innervation higher than chance and lower than one reflected innervation lower than chance. In the mPFC of WT mice, PT pyramidal neurons were preferentially innervated by vHIP axons (index = 2.1104, n = 9 sections from three mice, p=0.0001, one-sample Student’s t-test; Figure 6G). IT pyramidal neurons and PV-positive interneurons did not have a significant discrimination index (index = 1.1192 and 0.6050, n = 9/3 mice each, p=0.4186 and p=0.2754, one-sample Student’s t-test). Further, vHIP innervation of CAL and SOM interneurons occurred with a probability lower than chance (index = 0.5346 and 0.3165, n = 9/3 each mouse, p=0.0304 and p=0.0322, one-sample Student’s t-test). By contrast, vHIP axons innervated PV cells in the mPFC of Mepc2 KO mice more than in WT mice (n = 9/3, p=0.0425, Student’s t-test), to the detriment of PT pyramidal cells (n = 9/3, p=0.0161, Student’s t-test). We observed similar results when we assessed the percent of each neuronal subtype that received vHIP innervation; PV interneurons were innervated more at the expense of PT pyramidal cells in Mecp2 KO compared to WT mice (Figure 6H). Combined, these results indicate that the pattern of innervation of vHIP axons in the mPFC changes from mainly targeting excitatory projection pyramidal neurons in WT mice to preferentially targeting PV-expressing inhibitory GABAergic interneurons (Figure 6G and H).

Enhanced vHIP-mPFC synaptic strength in Mecp2 KO mice

Larger peak VSD signals evoked by stimulation of the vHIP fiber bundle in mPFC slices from Mecp2 KO mice could reflect either more or stronger excitatory synapses between presynaptic vHIP axons and postsynaptic mPFC neurons. We estimated the numerical density and size of en passant presynaptic terminals along afferent axons within the mPFC by labeling them with mCherry delivered by AAV2 injected into either the vHIP or the c-mPFC, and then performing immunohistochemistry of the presynaptic vesicle marker VGLUT1 (Figure 7A–E). We performed automated detection and size analysis using Bouton Analyzer (Gala et al., 2017) (Figure 7E). The numerical densities of mCherry-expressing en passant presynaptic terminals belonging to vHIP neurons and those belonging to c-mPFC neurons were comparable between WT and Mecp2 KO mice across all cortical layers of the mPFC (p>0.05, Two-way ANOVA followed by B and H-MC; Figure 7F and Figure 7—figure supplement 1). However, the weighted size of individual presynaptic boutons of vHIP axons in layer 5 of the mPFC was significantly larger in Mecp2 KO mice (n = 993 boutons in WT mice, n = 664 Mecp2 KO mice, p<0.0001, Two-way ANOVA followed by B and H-MC), though there was no difference in layer 2/3 (n = 543 boutons in WT mice, n = 720 in Mecp2 KO mice, p=2565, Two-way ANOVA followed by B and H-MC; Figure 7G–H and Figure 7—figure supplement 1C–D). In contrast, the sizes of presynaptic boutons of c-mPFC axons in layers five were significantly smaller in Mecp2 KO mice (n = 782 WT mice, n = 575 Mecp2 KO mice, p=0.0133; Two-way ANOVA followed by B and H-MC, Figure 7G–H), though not in layer 2/3 (n = 698 WT, n = 818 Mecp2 KO, p=8422, Two-way ANOVA followed by B and H-MC, Figure 7—figure supplement 1C–D). Interestingly, presynaptic boutons of vHIP axons in layer 5 of the mPFC were significantly smaller than those of c-mPFC axons in WT mice, a difference absent in Mecp2 KO mice due to larger vHIP boutons and smaller c-mPFC boutons (WT, p<0.0001; Mecp2 KO 0.2320; Figure 7G–H). These results are reminiscent of the amplitude of VSD signals in ex vivo mPFC slices evoked by either vHIP or intracortical stimulation, which were biased towards intracortical stimulation in WT mice, and of comparable amplitude in Mecp2 KO mice due to both larger vHIP-evoked VSD signals and smaller intracortical-evoked responses (see Figure 3L).

Figure 7. The size of presynaptic boutons is altered in layer 5 of the mPFC of Mecp2 KO mice.

(A–B) Schematic and representative examples of AAV2-hSyn-mCherry injection sites for identifying vHIP axons (A) or contralateral mPFC axons (B). Scale bars 1 mm. (C) Schematic of vHIP axons in the mPFC. (D) Axonal swellings identified as presynaptic boutons co-labeled for mCherry and VGLUT1. Scale bar 2 µm. (E) Representative examples of mCherry-filled presynaptic boutons. Scale bar 2 µm. (F) Numerical densities of axonal boutons per length of vHIP and c-mPFC axons located in layer 5 of mPFC (n = 45 axons WT vHIP; n = 36 Mecp2 KO vHIP; n = 49 WT c-mPFC; n = 43 KO c-mPFC; WT vHIP vs. Mecp2 KO vHIP, p=0.9925; WT vHIP vs. WT c-mPFC, p=0.8317 Mecp2 KO vHIP vs. Mecp2 KO c-mPFC, p=0.6765; WT c-mPFC vs. Mecp2 KO c-mPFC, p=0.6765; Interaction p=0.0135; Axon p=0.9592; Genotype p=0.3102; Two-way ANOVA with Benjamini and Hochberg multiple comparisons). Mean ± SD. (G) Cumulative probability distributions of the estimated size of presynaptic boutons comparing vHIP and c-mPFC axons in mPFC layer 5 of WT and Mecp2 KO mice. (H) Average weighted bouton sizes (vHIP WT mice vs. Mecp2 KO mice, p<0.0001; c-mPFC WT mice vs. Mecp2 KO mice, p=2320; vHIP vs. c-mPFC WT mice, p<0.0001; vHIP vs. c-mPFC Mecp2 KO mice, p=0.0133; n = 993 WT vHIP boutons; n = 682 KO vHIP; n = 792 WT c-mPFC; n = 577 KO c-mPFC; Interaction p=0.1193; Axon p=0.3135; Genotype p=0.2967; Two-way ANOVA with Benjamini and Hochberg multiple comparisons). Mean ± SEM; *p<0.05, **p<0.01. Figure 7—source data 1. See also Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

Figure 7—source data 1. The size of presynaptic boutons is altered in layer 5 of the mPFC of Mecp2KO mice.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.038

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. Presynaptic boutons are not altered in Layer 2/3 of the mPFC in Mecp2 KO mice.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

(A) Representative examples of mCherry-filled presynaptic boutons. Scale bar 2 µm. (B) Numerical densities of axonal boutons per length of vHIP and c-mPFC axons located in layer 2/3 of mPFC (n = 37 WT vHIP; n = 41 KO vHIP; n = 49 WT c-mPFC; n = 49 KO c-mPFC; WT vHIP vs. Mecp2 KO vHIP, p=0.4746; WT vHIP vs. WT c-mPFC, p=0.1711; Mecp2 KO vHIP vs. Mecp2 KO c-mPFC, p=0.1711; WT c-mPFC vs. Mecp2 KO c-mPFC, p=0.0601; Two-way ANOVA with Benjamini and Hochberg Multiple Comparisons). Mean ± SD. (C) Cumulative probability distributions of axonal bouton sizes comparing vHIP and c-mPFC axons in layer 2/3 of WT and Mecp2 KO mice. (D) Average axonal bouton sizes. (vHIP WT mice vs. Mecp2 KO mice, p=2565; c-mPFC WT mice vs. Mecp2 KO mice, p=8422; vHIP vs. c-mPFC WT mice, p=0.2565; vHIP vs. c-mPFC Mecp2 KO mice, p=0.8422; n = 543 boutons WT vHIP; n = 733 KO vHIP; n = 543 WT c-mPFC; n = 818 KO c-mPFC; Two-way ANOVA with Benjamini and Hochberg Multiple Comparisons). Mean ± SEM; *p<0.05, **p<0.01.
Figure 7—figure supplement 1—source data 1. The size of presynaptic boutons is altered in layer 5 of the mPFC of Mecp2KO mice.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.037

Because the size of presynaptic terminals is positively correlated with the area of the presynaptic active zone and the number of docked synaptic vesicle (Harris and Sultan, 1995; Murthy et al., 2001), which in turn is positively correlated with the area of the postsynaptic density and the volume of dendritic spines (Harris and Stevens, 1989; Harris and Weinberg, 2012), as well as their content of α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid receptors (AMPAR) (Matsuzaki et al., 2004), we hypothesized that larger VSD responses to vHIP fiber stimulation in the mPFC of Mecp2 KO mice reflect higher synaptic strength. To selectively stimulate different axonal projections onto the same postsynaptic mPFC neuron during whole-cell intracellular recordings, we used two light-sensitive opsins with shifted excitation spectra (Klapoetke et al., 2014). We injected AAV2s expressing the red-shifted opsin Chrimson into the ipsilateral vHIP, and those expressing the blue-shifted opsin Chronos into the c-mPFC (Figure 8A–B and Figure 8—figure supplement 1A–B). In mice expressing Chrimson in the vHIP and Chronos in the c-mPFC, a brief (1–4 ms) pulse of either red (630 nm) or blue (430 nm) light evoked monotonic inward currents in layer five neurons in the presence of 4-AP (100 mM), TTX (1 µM), and 4 mM Ca2+, which represent monosynaptic excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs) (Petreanu et al., 2009). The amplitude of red light vHIP-evoked EPSCs was significantly larger in pyramidal neurons from Mecp2 KO mice compared to those from WT mice at all ranges of light pulse durations (n = 17 cells in 6 slices from 6 WT mice; n = 11/5/5 Mecp2 KO mice; p=0.0118; Two-way RM ANOVA; Figure 8C), whereas the amplitude of vHIP-evoked monosynaptic EPSCs in layer five interneurons (identified by their size, shape, input resistance, and capacitance; see Figure 8—figure supplement 1C–D) was comparable in both genotypes (n = 11 cells in 7 slices from 7 WT mice; n = 12/9/9 Mecp2 KO mice; p=0.4327; Two-way RM ANOVA; Figure 8D). In contrast, blue light stimulation of Chronos-expressing c-mPFC axons evoked EPSCs of comparable amplitude in both layer five pyramidal neurons and interneurons from WT and Mecp2 KO mice at all ranges of light pulse durations (p=0.1617; p=0.6129; Two-way RM ANOVA; Figure 8E and F). The cell-by-cell normalization of the amplitude of EPSCs evoked by red light stimulation of Chrimson-expressing vHIP axons to the amplitude of EPSCs evoked by blue light stimulation of Chronos-expressing c-mPFC axons revealed that vHIP-evoked EPSCs were larger in layer five pyramidal from Mecp2 KO mice compared to WT mice (103% vs. 54%; p=0.00083; Two-way RM ANOVA; Figure 8G). In contrast, normalized EPSC amplitudes in interneurons were not significantly different between Mecp2 KO and WT mice (97% vs. 81%; p=0.3208; Two-way RM ANOVA) (Figure 8H). These results are reminiscent of the normalized amplitude of VSD signals evoked by stimulation of the fluorescently labeled vHIP fiber bundle compared to intracortical stimulation (see Figure 3L).

Figure 8. vHIP synapses on mPFC layer five pyramidal neurons are stronger in Mecp2 KO mice.

(A) Schematic of injection sites for Chrimson in the ipsilateral vHIP and Chronos in the contralateral mPFC. (B) Representative image of an mPFC slice with Chrimson-expressing vHIP afferents and Chronos-expressing c-mPFC afferents. Scale bar 50 µm. (C and D) Input-output relationship of vHIP afferent (red light) evoked responses in pyramidal neurons (C) Interaction p=0.0093; Stim p<0.0001; Genotype p=0.0118, Two-way RM ANOVA) and interneurons (D) Interaction p=0.2692; Stim p<0.0001; Genotype p=0.4327, Two-way RM ANOVA) with representative traces (inset). (E and F) Input-output relationship of c-mPFC afferent (blue light) evoked responses in pyramidal neurons (E) Interaction p=0.69643; Stim p<0.0001; Genotype p=0.1617, Two-way RM ANOVA) and interneurons (F) Interaction p=0.4222; Stim p<0.0001; Genotype p=0.6129, Two-way RM ANOVA) with representative traces (inset) (Scale bars 10 pA/12 ms). (G and H) The amplitude of vHIP afferent (red light)-evoked EPSCs was normalized to the peak EPSCs evoked by blue light stimulation of c-mPFC afferents in pyramidal neurons (G) Interaction p=0.0098; Stim p<0.0001; Genotype p=0.0003, Two-way RM ANOVA) and interneurons (H) Interaction p=0.9644; Stim p<0.0001; Genotype p=0.3208, Two-way RM ANOVA) (n = 17 cells from 6 slices from six mice WT pyramidal neurons, 11/7/7 WT interneurons, 11/5 KO pyramidal cells, 12/9/9 KO interneurons). (I) Representative example trace showing the time windows were measurements were made to calculate the AMPAR/NMDAR ratio (Scale bar 5 pA/10 ms). (J and K) AMPAR/NMDAR ratios of vHIP afferent (red light)-evoked and c-mPFC afferent (blue light)-evoked responses in pyramidal cells (J) WT vs. Mecp2 KO vHIP, p=0.0114, WT vs. Mecp2 KO c-mPFC, p=0.0038; WT vHIP vs. WT c-mPFC, p=0.0114; Mecp2 KO vHIP vs. Mecp2 KO c-mPFC, p=0.0114; Interaction p=0.0002; Input p0.9912; Genotype p=0.8544; Two-way ANOVA with Benjamini and Hochberg multiple comparisons) and interneurons (K) WT vs. Mecp2 KO vHIP, p=0.8843; WT vs. Mecp2 KO c-mPFC, p=0.8121; WT vHIP vs. WT c-mPFC, p=0.8121; Mecp2 KO vHIP vs. Mecp2 KO c-mPFC, p=0.8843; Interaction p=0.4896; Input p=0.2825; Genotype p=0.6265; Two-way ANOVA with Benjamini and Hochberg multiple comparisons) (n = 11 cells from 6 slices from six mice WT pyramidal cells; 9/7/7 WT interneurons; 13/5/5 Mecp2 KO pyramidal cells for both vHIP and c-mPFC stimulation; 11/9/9 Mecp2 KO interneurons). Mean ± SEM; *p<0.05, **p<0.01. Figure 8—source data 1. See also Figure 8—figure supplement 1.

Figure 8—source data 1. vHIP synapses on mPFC layerfivepyramidal neurons are stronger in Mecp2KO mice.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.042

Figure 8.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1. Specific activation of axons expressing opsins with non-overlapping light sensitivity.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1.

(A) EPSCs evoked by red light (630 nm) in a layer 5 mPFC neuron in a slice from a mouse with Chrimson injection in the vHIP; the same neuron did not respond to blue light (430 nm). (B) EPSCs evoked by blue light (430 nm) in a layer 5 mPFC neuron in a slice from a mouse with Chronos injection in the c-mPFC; the same neuron did not respond to red light (630 nm). (C) Input resistance and (D) whole-cell capacitance of visually identified pyramidal neurons and interneurons in layer 5 of mPFC (n = 17 pyramidal neurons from 8 slices from 8 WT mice, 15/8/8 WT interneurons, 14/5/5 Mecp2 KO pyramidal neurons, 14/8/8 Mecp2 KO interneurons; p=0.0002 WT pyramidal vs. interneuron input resistance, p=0.0035 Mecp2 KO pyramidal vs. interneuron; Interaction p=0.5739; Neuron Type p<0.0001; Genotype p=0.4085; Two-Way ANOVA with Benjamini and Hochberg Multiple Comparisons; p<0.0001 WT pyramidal vs. interneuron capacitance, p=0.0018 Mecp2 KO pyramidal vs. interneuron; Interaction p=0.4428; Neuron Type p<0.0001; Genotype p=0.3026; Two-Way ANOVA with Benjamini and Hochberg Multiple Comparisons). The whole-cell capacitance was estimated from the time constant of the exponential decay of the current response to a test voltage step. Mean ± SEM; *p<0.05, **p<0.01.
Figure 8—figure supplement 1—source data 1. vHIP synapses on mPFC layerfivepyramidal neurons are stronger in Mecp2KO mice.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.041

As a direct measure of postsynaptic strength, we calculated the ratio of the AMPAR component of the EPSC (recorded at −70 mV) to that of their NMDAR component (recorded at +40 mV) in the same neuron (Figure 8I). The AMPAR/NMDAR ratio of EPSCs evoked by red light vHIP stimulation was larger in layer five pyramidal neurons from Mecp2 KO mice compared to those from WT mice (n = 11 cells from six slices from WT mice; n = 13/5/5 Mecp2 KO mice; p=0.0114; Two-way ANOVA followed by B and H-MC), whereas the AMPAR/NMDAR ratio of EPSCs evoked by blue light c-mPFC stimulation was smaller in Mecp2 KO mice compared to WT mice (p=0.0114; Two-way ANOVA followed by B and H-MC; Figure 8J). In contrast, the AMPAR/NMDAR ratio of EPSCs evoked by either vHIP or c-mPFC stimulation in interneurons was not significantly different between Mecp2 KO and WT mice (n = 9/7/7 WT mice; n = 11/9/9 Mecp2 KO mice; p=0.8843 vHIP stimulation; p=0.8121 c-mPFC stimulation; Two-way ANOVA followed by B and H-MC; Figure 8K). Combined, these results demonstrate that vHIP excitatory synapses are selectively stronger on layer five pyramidal neurons, but not interneurons, in the mPFC of Mecp2 KO mice.

Discussion

Here, we characterized the projection from the vHIP to the PL region of the mPFC at the structural and functional levels in WT mice, and described its atypical features in the Mecp2 KO model of Rett syndrome. Because the vHIP has been implicated in social memory, and due to the involvement of the mPFC PL subregion in sociability and social novelty encoding, we tested the role of the vHIP projection to the mPFC in social behaviors. By chemogenetically manipulating neuronal activity selectively in mPFC-projecting vHIP CA1 neurons, we demonstrated that these projection neurons regulate social memory in a specific and selective manner, only influencing the discrimination between social targets without affecting other aspects of social interactions.

Coherent, synchronous oscillations between the vHIP and the mPFC underlie working memory tasks in rats (Gordon, 2011). Entrainment of these oscillations occurs in the first postnatal week (Brockmann et al., 2013) and is driven by monosynaptic glutamatergic projections from pyramidal neurons of the ventral CA1 and subiculum to pyramidal neurons and GABAergic interneurons in the IL and PL subregions of the mPFC (Anastasiades et al., 2018; Dégenètais et al., 2003; Dembrow et al., 2010; Thierry et al., 2000). Despite the wealth of information linking this projection to neuropsychiatric disorders (Li et al., 2015), little is known about the synaptic and cellular bases of this long-range projection. To better define the connectivity of this circuit, we performed trans-synaptic tracing to identify postsynaptic targets of vHIP afferents and determined how this innervation pattern is altered in Mecp2 KO mice. Although recent evidence suggests that monosynaptic excitatory vHIP inputs to the mPFC are strongest on pyramidal neurons in layer 2/3 of the IL, but dominated by feed-forward inhibition onto them (Marek et al., 2018), there is also evidence of strong monosynaptic excitatory innervation of IT-projecting pyramidal neurons in layer 5 of the PL (Liu and Carter, 2018). Our trans-synaptic tracing data support the latter observations, with over 50% of WGA-positive neurons being IT-projecting pyramidal neurons. We also identified PV-positive interneurons as the inhibitory subgroup most innervated by vHIP axons, which is consistent with the observation of vHIP-driven feed-forward inhibition in the IL (Marek et al., 2018). In Mecp2 KO mice, there are fewer vHIP-innervated PT-projecting pyramidal neurons, which have been shown to encode social dominance (Franklin et al., 2017). This suggests a basis for the impairments in social memory performance observed in Mecp2 KO mice. However, there are more vHIP-innervated PV-positive interneurons, which may lead to tonic inhibition of the mPFC network due to the hyperactivity of the vHIP in Mecp2 KO mice (Calfa et al., 2011). Interestingly, the density of en passant presynaptic boutons along vHIP axons in the mPFC was not altered in Mecp2 KO mice, indicating a redistribution of excitatory vHIP inputs on different cell types in the mPFC, which results in an atypical wiring pattern.

Although the number of vHIP boutons in the mPFC was not altered, their individual volume was larger in layer 5 of the PL region of the mPFC in Mecp2 KO mice. By contrast, vHIP boutons in layer 2/3 had a more prominent bimodal distribution of volumes compared to WT mice. Because the size of presynaptic boutons is correlated with synaptic strength (Murthy et al., 2001), we tested the strength of vHIP-mPFC synapses onto both pyramidal neurons and interneurons. EPSCs evoked by optogenetic excitation of vHIP fibers in layer 5 mPFC pyramidal cells were larger and had higher AMPAR/NMDAR ratios in Mecp2 KO mice compared to WT littermates. Interestingly, vHIP-evoked EPSC amplitudes and AMPAR/NMDAR ratios in interneurons were not affected in Mecp2 KO mice. Together with the trans-synaptic identification of postsynaptic targets of vHIP terminals in the mPFC, these results indicate that vHIP axons in Mecp2 KO mice innervate fewer pyramidal neurons with stronger synaptic strength, but they have similar innervation strength onto more inhibitory interneurons. Such altered connectivity is reflected in the pattern of vHIP-evoked neuronal depolarizations in mPFC slices revealed by high-speed voltage imaging. In these studies, the larger amplitudes reflect stronger excitatory synapses onto pyramidal neurons, while the spatiotemporal spread throughout the mPFC slice and over time reflects both an altered connectivity pattern and feed-forward inhibition. It is possible that a larger proportion of inhibitory neurons is chronically activated by hyperactive vHIP inputs in Mecp2 KO mice, causing tonic inhibition of the mPFC network, and that vHIP activation of IT-projecting pyramidal neurons overcomes this inhibition.

LTP at vHIP-mPFC synapses is impaired in Mecp2 KO mice (Figure 2—figure supplement 2). Together with larger vHIP-evoked VSD signals (Figure 2) and whole-cell intracellular recordings from layer five pyramidal neurons (Figure 8), these data are similar to those observed in hippocampal CA3-CA1 synapses of Mecp2 KO mice. There, naive excitatory synapses in Mecp2 KO mice have several features of potentiated synapses due to impaired synaptic GluA1 trafficking, suggesting a saturation of the dynamic range available for LTP (Li et al., 2016). A similar mechanism prevents homeostatic synaptic plasticity in Mecp2 KO hippocampal neurons in culture (Xu and Pozzo-Miller, 2017). Current work is aimed at characterizing the mechanism(s) of impaired LTP at vHIP-mPFC synapses in Mecp2 KO mice.

Deficits in the E/I balance within the mPFC have been linked to impaired sociability in WT mice, as well as in mouse models of ASDs (Selimbeyoglu et al., 2017; Yizhar et al., 2011; Brumback et al., 2018). In addition, the hippocampal network is integral to the expression of social memory (Hitti and Siegelbaum, 2014; Meira et al., 2018; Okuyama et al., 2016). Because network activity within the vHIP and the mPFC, as well as the projection pattern of vHIP afferents in the mPFC, are altered in Mecp2 KO mice, we characterized their sociability, social interaction, and social memory. We performed these experiments using computer vision to track multiple freely interacting mice and a computer learning algorithm trained to identify different behaviors (Kabra et al., 2013; Ohayon et al., 2013; Robie et al., 2017). This automatic and unbiased screen of social interactions revealed that, despite showing typical sociability, Mecp2 KO mice displayed an atypical behavior of jumping at other mice more than following or sniffing, which was not associated with aggression. This finding, to our knowledge, is the first description of an atypical social interaction in a mouse model of ASD. We also identified a deficit of social memory in Mecp2 KO mice, which failed to discriminate between a co-housed littermate and a novel mouse as the target of their social interactions under unrestricted conditions.

To demonstrate a causal role of altered vHIP-mPFC inputs on atypical social behaviors in Mecp2 KO mice, we mimicked the characteristic hippocampal hyperactivity of Mecp2 KO mice in WT mice by long-term chemogenetic activation with the excitatory hM3Dq DREADD selectively expressed in mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons. Such long-term excitation impaired social memory, without affecting other types of social interactions. Chronic inhibition of vHIP-mPFC projecting neurons in WT mice also impaired social memory, indicating that there is a set level of proper neuronal activity in this long-range projection, with any deviation resulting in social memory deficits. Even though the vHIP-mPFC projection has been causally tied to anxiety-like behaviors (Padilla-Coreano et al., 2016), long-term chemogenetic manipulation of vHIP-mPFC projection neurons had no major consequences on the time spent in the center of the arena, the time spent engaging in grooming, or the overall locomotor behavior, compared to CNO-treated WT mice expressing Cre-driven mCherry in mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons. This finding demonstrates the selectivity of this manipulation of the vHIP-mPFC projection to social memory. Voltage imaging of network responses in mPFC slices from hM3Dq-expressing mice revealed stronger vHIP inputs and, surprisingly, weaker responses evoked by stimulation of intracortical inputs (which were not chemogenitically manipulated), resembling the responses observed in Mecp2 KO mice. These results may reflect feed-forward homeostatic mechanisms within the mPFC microcircuit, as has been hypothesized to give rise to the dichotomy in the direction of E/I imbalances between limbic and cortical structures in models of neuropsychiatric diseases (Nelson and Valakh, 2015).

Despite the fact that many brain regions are dysfunctional in Mecp2 KO mice, selective long-term inhibition of vHIP-mPFC projection neurons was sufficient to improve their social memory. In addition, social memory discrimination scores were negatively correlated with the amplitude of vHIP-driven depolarizations in mPFC slices: larger voltage dye signals corresponded to worse social memory performance, whereas smaller voltage dye signals corresponded with better expression of social memory. By contrast, long-term inhibition of vHIP-mPFC projection neurons did not affect voltage dye responses evoked by intracortical stimulation, and these responses did not correlate with social memory performance.

Acute manipulation of vHIP-mPFC projection neurons immediately prior to the behavioral test also affected social memory: their excitation with hM3Dq caused a discrimination index not different than chance, whereas inhibition with hM4Di improved social discrimination in Mecp2 KO mice. These data suggest that the vHIP-mPFC projection is necessary for the recall of social memory, as opposed to its maintenance or initial formation. Because acute manipulation impaired social memory in WT mice, we used this paradigm to test the specificity of the vHIP-mPFC projection to the social aspect of memory, as well as its selectivity by testing another vHIP projection target. Acute excitation of hM3Dq-expressing mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons did not affect novel object recognition in WT mice, indication that this manipulation did not affect overall hippocampal function. Furthermore, regulation of social memory was selective to mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons because excitation of hM3Dq-expressing vHIP neurons projecting to the NAc did not affect social memory, which is at odds with a previous report concluding that vHIP-NAc projection is required for social memory (Okuyama et al., 2016). A key difference between these studies is our intersectional viral approach to selectively and exclusively express DREADDs in specific projection neurons of the vHIP.

In summary, we demonstrate that the vHIP-mPFC projection regulates social memory in WT mice, and that its dysfunction causes social memory deficits in Mecp2 KO mice. Defining the synaptic bases of social behaviors provides insight and potential targets for therapies in psychiatric disorders associated with vHIP-mPFC dysfunction, such as autism and schizophrenia.

Materials and methods

Key resources table.

Reagent type
(species) or
resource
Designation Source or
reference
Identifiers Additional
information
Strain, strain background (Mus musculus) Mecp2tm1.1Jae Mutant Mouse
Resource and
Research Center
MMRRC:000415-UCD
Genetic reagent (Virus) AAV-hSyn-Chronos-GFP UNC Vector Core UNC Vector Core AAV2
Genetic reagent (Virus) AAV-hSyn-ChrimsonR-tdTomato UNC Vector Core UNC Vector Core AAV2
Genetic reagent (Virus) AAV-hSyn-mCherry UNC Vector Core UNC Vector Core AAV2
Genetic reagent (Virus) CAV-2-Cre-GFP CNRS Biocampus Montpellier CNRS Biocampus Montpellier
Genetic reagent (Virus) pAAV8-DIO-hSyn-HM4D(Gi)-mCherry Addgene 44362 AAV8
Genetic reagent (Virus) pAAV-DIO-hSyn-HM3D(Gq)-mCherry Addgene 44361 AAV8
Genetic reagent (Virus) pAAV-DIO-hSyn-mCherry Addgene 50459 AAV8
Antibody Anti-mCherry (Rabbit polyclonal) Abcam ab167453 (1:500)
Antibody Anti-vGlut1 (Guinea Pig polyclonal) Synaptic Systems 135304 (1:500)
Antibody Anti-GFP (chicken polyclonal) Abcam ab13970 (1:500)
Antibody Anti-PV (Rabbit polyclonal) Abcam ab11427 (1:500)
Antibody Anti-PV (Guinea Pig polyclonal) Synaptic Systems 195004 (1:750)
Antibody Anti-cFos (Guinea Pig polyclonal) Synaptic Systems 226004 (1:500)
Antibody Anti-CAL (Chicken polyclonal) Synaptic Systems 214106 (1:750)
Antibody Anti-SST (Guinea Pig polyclonal) Synaptic Systems 366004 (1:750)
Antibody Anti-NeuN (Mouse monoclonal) Synaptic Systems 266011 (1:500)
Antibody Anti-NeuN (Guinea Pig polyclonal Synaptic Systems 266004 (1:1000)
Antibody Anti-Lectin (Rabbit polyclonal) Sigma T4144-1VL (1:2000)
Antibody Alexa Fluor 488 Goat Anti-Rabbit Jackson ImmunoResearch 111-545-003 (1:500)
Antibody Alexa Fluor 594 Goat Anti-Rabbit Jackson ImmunoResearch 111-585-003 (1:500)
Antibody Alexa Fluor 594 Goat Anti-Guinea Pig Jackson ImmunoResearch 106-585-003 (1:500)
Antibody Alexa Fluor 594 Goat Anti-Chicken Jackson ImmunoResearch 103-585-155 (1:500)
Antibody Alexa Fluor 647 Goat Anti-Mouse Jackson ImmunoResearch 115-605-003 (1:500)
Antibody Biotinylated Goat Anti-Rabbit Vector Laboratories BA-1000 (1:200)
Antibody Biotinylated Goat Anti-Guinea Pig Vector Laboratories BA-7000 (1:200)
Antibody Streptavidin, Alexa Fluor 405 Thermo Fisher Scientific S-32351 (1:1500)
Antibody Streptavidin, Alexa Fluor 488 Thermo Fisher Scientific S-32354 (1:1500)
Antibody Streptavidin, Alexa Fluor 594 Thermo Fisher Scientific S-32356 (1:1500)
Chemical compound, drug Dextran-Alexa-594 10,000 MW Thermo Fisher Scientific D22913
Chemical compound, drug Wheat Germ Agglutinin Vector Laboratories L-1020
Chemical compound, drug FluoroGold Fluorochrome
Chemical compound, drug RetroBeads Lumafluor Inc
Chemical compound, drug Tetrodotoxin Alomone Labs
Chemical compound, drug 4-Aminopyridine Sigma 275875
Chemical compound, drug D-AP5 Tocris 106
Software, algorithm Motr Janelia research center https://github.com/motr/motr
Software, algorithm JAABA Janelia research center https://github.com/kristinbranson/JAABA
Software, algorithm Novel Object Recognition Add-on for JAABA This paper https://github.com/PhillipsML/NOR
Software, algorithm Voltage Sensitive Dye Analysis Code This paper https://github.com/PhillipsML/VoltageDyeAnalysis
Software, algorithm GraphPad Prism, version 8 GraphPad RRID:SCR_002798
Software, algorithm TIWB DOI: 10.1093/jmicro/dfy015.
Software, algorithm Neuroplex Red Shirt Imaging
Software, algorithm ImageJ, FIJI DOI: 10.1038/nmeth.2019
Software, algorithm Matlab, 2015b & 2017b Mathworks
Other Born Blonde Maxi Clairol

Contact for reagent and resource sharing

Further information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the Lead Contact, Dr. Lucas Pozzo-Miller (lucaspm@uab.edu).

Experimental model and subject details

Female mice with deletions of exon three in the Mecp2 gene (Mecp2tm1.1Jae; Chen et al., 2001) were obtained from the Mutant Mouse Regional Resource Center (University of California, Davis), and maintained in a pure C57/BL6 background by crossing them with male WT C57/BL6 mice. All experimental subjects were male hemizygous Mecp2tm1.1Jae mice, referred to as Mecp2 KO mice. Subjects classified as presymptomatic were tested between P20 and P24. Subjects that exhibited Rett-like symptoms, such as hypoactivity, hind limb clasping, resting tremors, and reflex impairments (Guy et al., 2001), were classified as symptomatic (between P45-P60). Age-matched male WT littermates were used as controls. Mice were handled and housed according to the Committee on Laboratory Animal Resources of the National Institutes of Health. All experimental protocols were reviewed and approved annually by the Institutional Animals Care and Use Committee of the University of Alabama at Birmingham.

Method details

Intracranial injections

Mice were anesthetized with 4% isoflurane vapor in 100% oxygen gas and maintained with 1–2.5% isoflurane vapor in 100% oxygen gas mixtures. Mice were aligned in a stereotactic frame (Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA), and their body temperature was measured with a rectal probe and maintained with a heating pad. A midline incision was made down the scalp, and a dental drill used to perform a small craniotomy. A 2.5 µL syringe (Hamilton Company, Reno, NV) was used to inject solutions (viruses, RetroBeads, fluorescent dextran, FluoroGold, or WGA) at a rate of 0.25 µL/min using a microsyringe pump (UMP3 UltraMicroPump, Micro4; World Precision Instruments, Sarasota, FL). The needle was slowly extracted from the injection site over 10 min, after which the incision was closed with surgical glue. All ages and coordinates for each experiment are relative to the bregma and listed as anterior/posterior (A/P), medial/lateral (M/L), and dorsal/ventral (D/V).

Immunohistochemistry

Mice were anesthetized with an i.p. injection of ketamine (100 mg/kg) and transcardially perfused with ice-cold 1X phosphate-buffered saline (PBS), followed by ice-cold 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in 1X PBS. The brain was dissected and postfixed in 4% PFA overnight. Brains were sectioned at 30 μm thickness with a vibratome (PELCO 100, model 3000; Ted Pella Inc, Redding, CA) and stored at 4°C in 1X PBS containing 0.01% sodium azide. Free-floating sections were permeabilized using 0.25% Triton-100X for 15 min and subsequently incubated in blocking solution (0.01% sodium azide, 2% bovine serum albumin (BSA), 0.1% Triton-100X, 2M glycine, and 10% goat serum in 1X PBS) for 1 hr. Antibody diluent consisted of 0.01% sodium azide, 2% BSA, 0.1% Triton-100X, and 5% goat serum in 1X PBS. Primary antibodies were diluted in antibody diluent at concentrations listed below and incubated for 36 hr at room temperature. After washing 3 times for 5 min in 1X PBS, secondary antibodies were diluted in antibody diluent and incubated for 4 hr at room temperature. Sections were washed 3 times for 5 min in 1X PBS before mounting with Vectashield mounting media (Vector Biolabs, Malvern, PA).

Amplification

For primary antibodies requiring further amplification (c-Fos, mCherry, and GFP), a biotinylation step was added following the primary antibody incubation. Sections were incubated in biotinylated anti-host of the primary antibody (Vector Biolabs, Malvern, PA) at a concentration of 1:200 in antibody diluent for 2 hr. After washing 3 times for 5 min in 1X PBS, sections were incubated in streptavidin-conjugated fluorophore (Alexa-405, Alexa-488, Alexa-594; Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA) diluted in antibody diluent at a concentration of 1:1600 for 4 hr. Sections were washed 3 times for 5 min before mounting with Vectashield mounting media (Vector Biolabs, Malvern, PA).

Injection of WGA

For identification of mPFC neurons innervated by the vHIP, P45 mice received a 500 nL injection of 4% WGA (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) (Ruda and Coulter, 1982) into the vHIP (3.8 A/P, 3.3 M/L, 3.5 D/V from the bregma). Mice were sacrificed 30 hr later, a time point that our pilot tests confirmed the transsynaptic transfer to the 1 st order neurons, and not to other neurons down the synaptic chain. To avoid cross-reactivity between the anti-WGA primary antibody and other antibodies, sections were first incubated with anti-WGA and then underwent subsequent biotinylation and streptavidin steps. Following the last wash, sections were again blocked for 1 hr. Immunohistochemitry for neuronal subtypes and NeuN was subsequently performed following the standard protocol.

Injection of FluoroGold

For identification of mPFC pyramidal neurons based on their projections, P30 mice received a 250 nL injection of 2% FluoroGold (Fluorochrome, Denver, CO) (Schmued and Fallon, 1986) either in the right mPFC (1.45 A/P, 0.5 M/L, 1.45 D/V from the bregma) or in the dPAG (1.18 A/P, 4.2 M/L, 2.36 D/V from the bregma with a needle angle of 26°). At P45, the same mice were injected with 500 nL of 4% WGA into the vHIP (3.8 A/P, 3.3 M/L, 3.5 D/V from the bregma) and sacrificed 30 hr later.

Identification of socially-activated neurons

For identification of vHIP projection neurons, P35 mice received injections of red RetroBeads (Lumafluor, Durham, NC) (Quattrochi et al., 1989) in the LH (1.34 A/P, 1.1 M/L, 5.3 D/V from the bregma) and green RetroBeads in the mPFC (1.45 A/P, 0.5 M/L, 1.45 D/V from the bregma). Two weeks after surgery, mice underwent 3 days of testing acclimation (3 min of handling and 10 min inside the 16 × 10 in test box. On day 4, mice were placed in the test box containing either a littermate sentinel (social condition) or a toy mouse (object condition) and allowed to interact for 10 min before being returned to the home cage. After 1 hr, the same test mouse was placed back in the same box, which now included either a novel sentinel mouse (social condition) or a novel toy mouse (object condition), and allowed to interact for 10 min before being returned to the home cage. Previous reports indicate the vHIP neurons are activated by social encounter regardless of novelty (Okuyama et al., 2016). We therefore chose to present a strong stimulus to recruit as many vHIP neurons as possible by using sequential interactions with both a littermate and novel mouse. Test mice were anesthetized with ketamine (100 mg/kg, i.p.) 45 min after the last interaction and perfused for subsequent immunostaining. vHIP sections from mice containing RetroBeads were stained for the immediate early gene c-Fos and NeuN. mPFC sections from non-surgical mice were stained for c-Fos. All sections were processed for immunohistochemistry at the same time, and images were taken consecutively using identical imaging settings in a confocal microscope. For quantification of c-Fos fluorescence intensity in RetroBead labeled neurons, the soma of NeuN-positive cells co-labeled with RetroBeads were manually circled using FIJI (ImageJ, NIH). The measurement was then redirected to the c-Fos channel and fluorescence intensity calculated. All RetroBead labeled neurons contained within the ventral subiculum and ventral CA1 of 6 stained sections each from four mice were measured. Sections which had atypically high background fluorescence were excluded; the remaining were normalized to background fluorescence (determined by measuring the intensity of NeuN negative region). Because all RetroBead-labeled neurons were labeled, we did not set a threshold for considering a neuron c-Fos positive and instead report the fluorescence intensity for each individual neuron in a cumulative frequency distribution (Figure 1D) and the intensity average per mouse (Figure 1E). Statistical tests were conducted on averaged data. For c-Fos positive cells in the mPFC, FIJI was used to make images binary and automatically detect particles and measure fluorescence. Identical settings were used during this process. Because this experiment necessitated the use of a threshold for binarization and therefore not all neurons were measured, we used a fluorescence intensity cutoff of 80 arbitrary units to define cells as c-Fos positive. These data were reported statistically tested as number of c-Fos positive neurons per section, averaged by mouse (Figure 1—figure supplement 1).

Imaging and quantification of presynaptic terminals

P20 mice received injections of AAV2-Syn-mCherry (UNC Vector Core, Chapel Hill, NC) either in the vHIP (3.8 A/P, 3.3 M/L, 3.5 D/V from the bregma; 500 nL) or in the right mPFC (1.45 A/P, 0.5 M/L, 1.45 D/V from the bregma; 250 nL). They were perfused 4 weeks later, and 30 μm sections were cut from the mPFC and stained with anti-mCherry antibodies. All images were acquired using a 63X (1.4 NA) oil immersion objective in an LSM-800 Airyscan confocal microscope (Zeiss, Oberkochen, Germany) using identical settings (laser power, pinhole, photomultiplier tube, current, gain, and offset). Axons were semi-manually traced using NCTracer in FIJI (Longair et al., 2011), and boutons were quantified using BoutonAnalyzer (Gala et al., 2017) running in Matlab (MathWorks, Natick, MA). Statistical tests were conducted on density per axon and size per bouton.

Electrophysiology

Ex vivo brain slices

Mice were anesthetized with ketamine (100 mg/kg, i.p.) and transcardially perfused with an ice-cold modified ‘cutting’ artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF) containing 87 mM NaCl, 2.5 mM KCl, 0.5 mM CaCl2, 7 mM MgCl2, 1.25 mM NaH2PO4, 25 mM NaHCO3, 25 mM glucose, and 75 mM sucrose, bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2. Coronal sections (300 µm thick) of the mPFC cut at an angle of 10° from the coronal plane were prepared using a vibratome (VT1200S; Leica Biosystems, Wetzlar, Germany), transferred to a submerged recovery chamber filled with ‘recording’ aCSF (125 mM NaCl, 2.5 mM KCl, 2 mM CaCl2, 1 mM MgCl2, 1.25 mM NaH2PO4, 25 mM NaHCO3, and 25 mM glucose, bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2), kept at 30°C for 30 min, and then allowed to recover for 1 hr at room temperature before use.

High-speed imaging of voltage-sensitive dye signals

For visualization of vHIP afferent fibers in mPFC slices, P30 mice received bilateral injections of 0.4 µL of Dextran-Alexa-594 (10,000 MW; Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA) in the vHIP, at 3.88 A/P, 3.3 M/L, 3.5 D/V from the bregma. After 2 weeks, mice were sacrificed and individual ex vivo slices were stained with 30 µM of the voltage-sensitive fluorescent dye RH414 (Anaspec, Fremont, CA) in aCSF for 1 hr at room temperature, and transferred to an immersion chamber continuously perfused (2 mL/min) with ‘recording’ aCSF saturated with 95%O2/5% CO2 and kept at 30°C. A theta glass electrode filled with ‘recording’ aCSF was positioned within the fluorescently labeled vHIP fiber bundle, and another theta glass electrode was placed in line with the cortical column across from the vHIP fiber electrode. In some experiments, an extracellular electrode filled with ‘recording’ aCSF (1–3 MΩ) was positioned in layer 2/3 to record fEPSPs with an Axoclamp amplifier (Molecular Devices, San Jose, CA) in current-clamp mode; signals were sampled at 2 kHz, amplified 10 times with a pre-amplifier (Model 210; Brownlee Precision, now NeuroPhase, Palo Alto, CA), digitized at 10 kHz (ITC-18; InstruTech, Longmont, CO), and acquired with custom-written software (TI WorkBench) (Inoue, 2018) in a MacMini computer (Apple, Cupertino, CA). RH414 was excited at 530 ± 50 nm with a phosphor-pumped LED (Heliophor; 89 North, Williston, VT), and its filtered fluorescence (535 ± 50 nm band-pass, 580 nm beam-splitter, 594 nm long-pass; Semrock, Rochester, NY) was imaged in an inverted microscope (IX71; Olympus, Tokyo, Japan) through a 10 × 0.5 NA objective (Fluar; Zeiss, Oberkochen, Germany) and acquired with a scientific CMOS camera running at 2500 frames-per-second at full 128 × 128 pixel resolution (NeuroCMOS-SM128; RedShirt Imaging, Decatur, GA), controlled by NeuroPlex software (RedShirt Imaging, Decatur, GA) in a Windows computer (Dell, Round Rock, TX); the electrophysiology computer was synchronized with the imaging computer by TTL pulses. The input-output relationship of the amplitude of VSD signals was obtained by delivering 12 different stimulus intensities with 30 µA increments. The input-output relationship of VSD amplitudes matched that of the the slope of fEPSPs, and VSD signals followed the kinetics of individual fEPSPs (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). LTP of VSD signals and fEPSPs was induced by high-frequency stimulation of vHIP afferent fibers, which consisted of trains of pulses at 300 Hz for 0.5 s repeated 10 times with 3 min intervals (Huang et al., 2004). Induction of LTP in mPFC slices from symptomatic Mecp2 KO mice required partial antagonism of GABAA receptors with 5 µM picrotoxin to reduce the hyperactivity driven by vHIP stimulation; LTP induction in mPFC slices from presymptomatic Mecp2 KO mice only required 1 µM picrotoxin.

VSD signals were analyzed using custom written Matlab codes (https://github.com/PhillipsML/VoltageDyeAnalysisPhillips, 2019b, copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/VoltageDyeAnalysis), as following: VSD responses in layer 2/3 were determined in a semi-automated manner with boundaries based on the percent of cortical thickness; the boundary between layers 5 and 2/3 was 20% of the cortical thickness from the white matter, and the boundary between layers 2/3 and 1 was 10% of the cortical thickness from the pial surface. Maximum responses were reported as the mean dF/F from a 3 × 3 pixel Region Of Interest (ROI) placed within layer 2/3 with the highest response in the 6 ms following stimulation, which corresponds to the peak of the fEPSPs. The spatiotemporal spread was calculated as the area under the curve (AOC) of the spread in the cortical area (% of total) and time (ms). For the correlation between VSD signals and social memory performance, only one slice per mouse was used (the slice with the median response). Statistical tests were performed on slices as the replicate.

Intracellular whole-cell recordings

For optogenetic activation of vHIP and contralateral mPFC afferents, a total of 0.5 µL of AAV2-Syn-Chrimson-tdTomato (UNC Vector Core, Chapel Hill, NC) was injected into the left vHIP (250 nL at 3.5 A/P, 3.3 M/L, 3.5 D/V from the bregma and 250 nL at 3.1 A/P, 3.3 M/L, 3.0 D/V from the bregma) and 250 nL of AAV2-Syn-Chronos-GFP (UNC Vector Core, Chapel Hill, NC) into the right mPFC (1.45 A/P, 0.5 M/L, 1.45 D/V from the bregma). After allowing 4 weeks for opsin expression, mPFC ex vivo slices were transferred to an immersion chamber continuously perfused (2 mL/min) with ‘recording’ aCSF (120 mM NaCl, 2.5 mM KCl, 2 mM CaCl2, 1 mM MgCl2, 25 mM NaHCO3, 1.4 mM NaH2PO4, 21 mM glucose, 0.4 mM Na-ascorbate, and 2 mM Na-pyruvate bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2) and kept at 30°C. Addition of 1 µM TTX and 100 mM 4-AP, and increasing Ca2+ to 4 mM ensured recordings of monosynaptic responses (Petreanu et al., 2009). Whole-cell recording electrodes were pulled from borosilicate glass (World Precision Instruments, Sarasota, FL) and filled with 120 mM Cs-gluconate, 17.5 mM CsCl, 10 mM Na-HEPES, 4 mM Mg-ATP, 10 mM NA2-creatine phosphate, and 0.2 mM Na-EGTA; this yielded a resistance of 4 ± 0.5 MΩ in aCSF. Whole-cell currents were recorded in voltage-clamp mode with an Axopatch-200B amplifier (Molecular Devices, San Jose, CA), filtered at 2 kHz, digitized at 10 kHz (ITC-18; InstruTech, Longmont, CO), and acquired with TI WorkBench in a G5 PowerMac computer (Apple, Cupertino, CA). Opsins were excited with monochromatic light from a Laser-LED illumination system (Lumen Dynamics, now Excelitas Technologies, Waltham, MA) attached to the epifluorescence port of a DM-LFS fixed-stage upright microscope (Leica Biosystems, Wetzlar, Germany), and focused onto the slice through a Zeiss 63X (1.0 NA) water immersion lens (Zeiss, Oberkochen, Germany), which was also used for visualized whole-cell recordings under infrared-differential interference contrast. TI WorkBench (Inoue, 2018) controlled and synchronized the Laser-LED (Lumen Dynamics, now Excelitas Technologies, Waltham, MA) stimulation with electrophysiological recordings and fluorescence imaging with an electron multiplying charge-coupled device (QuantEM 512SC; Photometrics, Tucson, AZ). Chronos was excited with 430 nm light, and Chrimson was excited with 630 nm light. In control experiments, 630 nm light evoked inward currents in layer five pyramidal neurons in slices expressing only Chrimson in vHIP axons, but not those expressing Chronos in c-mPFC axons (Figure 8—figure supplement 1A–B). Conversely, 430 nm light of an intensity that evoked inward currents in slices expressing only Chronos in c-mPFC axons, did not evoke any responses in slices expressing only Chrimson in vHIP axons (Figure 8—figure supplement 1B). Neurons were classified as pyramidal neurons or interneurons based on their morphology, input resistance, and whole-cell capacitance (estimated from the exponential decay of the current response to a test voltage step) (Figure 8—figure supplement 1c-d). Neurons whose input resistance changed more than 20% during the recordings or had unclamped spikes (i.e., action currents) were excluded. Statistical tests considered neuron the biological replicate.

Behavioral assays

Three-chamber social assay

Mice were acclimated to being handled during 3 min each for 3 days prior to the testing day at the same time as testing. All handling and testing were done in the dark phase of the 12 hr light/12 hr dark cycle, with the experimenter wearing a red headlamp and infrared illumination for digital videography. Mice were placed in the center chamber of a three-chambered box that contained empty inverted pencil cups in the two side chambers, and were allowed to freely explore. After 5 min of acclimation, mice were shepherded back to the center chamber, and blocks were put in place over the side openings. A novel mouse was put under one of the pencil cups on one of the two sides, with the side being interleaved between trials. The blocks were lifted and the test mouse allowed to freely explore the chambers for 10 min. After this time, the test mouse was again shepherded into the middle compartment and blocked there. A second novel mouse was placed under the previously empty pencil cup. The side blocks were removed and the test mouse was allowed to freely explore for another 10 min. After testing each mouse, the apparatus was thoroughly cleaned with isopropanol. Test mice spending more than 75% of the acclimation time in one compartment were removed from the study. The amount of time the test mouse spent actively sniffing each pencil cup (either empty or containing sentinel mice) was quantified and the discrimination index was calculated as [(Time investigating Mouse Cup - Time investigating Empty Cup) / (Time investigating Mouse Cup + Time investigating Empty Cup) * 100] for the sociability test and [(Time investigating Novel Mouse Cup - Time investigating Familiar Mouse Cup) / (Time investigating Novel Mouse Cup + Time investigating Familiar Mouse Cup) * 100] for the memory test. Statistical tests were preformed on the discrimination indices per mouse.

Unrestricted social Assay

One week prior to testing, the back of sentinel mice was dyed with blond hair dye (Born Blonde Maxi, Clairol) with differing patterns for tracking by computer vision. Mice were acclimated to handling and to the testing box containing clean bedding for 3 days prior to the testing day. All acclimation and testing (3 min handling, 10 min in the testing box) were done in the dark phase with experimenter wearing a red headlamp, and video acquisition using infrared illumination. At the beginning of the testing day, sentinel mice from different cages were placed in a neutral cage to acclimate to one another. The test mice were placed in the testing box and were videotaped for 10 min. Then, sentinel mice (one cage-mate of the test mouse and an unknown from a different cage) were placed in the testing box with the test mouse, and allowed to freely interact while being videotaped for 10 min. After this time, the sentinel mice were placed back in the neutral cage and the test mouse returned to the home cage. The test box was cleaned and filled with new bedding between each test mouse. Each sentinel mice interacted with a maximum of 5 test mice, and were discarded if they fought with other sentinels or were excessively grooming. After all mice had been tested, sentinel mice were individually videotaped for 10 min for computer training. Individual and test videos were fed to the Motr program (https://github.com/motr/motr; Ohayon et al., 2013) to create tracks that were sent to JAABA (https://github.com/kristinbranson/JAABA; Kabra et al., 2013; Robie et al., 2017Branson, 2019) for unbiased computer identification of behaviors. JAABA classifiers were trained on pilot data sets. Behavioral scores for social memory assessment were taken from the first 4 min of the trial (Figure 2), and separated based on the mouse target (cage-mate or novel). The rationale of using the first 4 min of the total 10 min of the unrestricted social interaction assay is based on the observation that more than 75% of the social interactions occur during the first 4 min. After that time, there is no longer a difference in time spent following the target mice compared to the time spent exploring the environment, as the test mouse becomes acclimated to their presence. As a result of this, the Discrimination Index after the first 4 min become a less reliable estimate preference in the social interactions (Figure 2—figure supplement 2). Behavioral scores for social memory and other behaviors were taken from the entire video, as different behaviors emerged at later times during the trial, and social behavior times pooled between novel and cage-mate mouse. Memory discrepancy scores were calculated as (Time Following Novel - Time Following Familiar) / (Time Following Novel + Time Following Familiar) * 100. Mice not interacting with sentinels for more than 3 s (out of 240) were excluded. Preference for the novel was considered a value greater than 0. Statistical tests were performed on the discrimination indices per mouse for memory, or for all behaviors, time per behavior for each mouse.

Novel object recognition

Mice were acclimated to handling and to the testing box containing clean bedding and two identical objects for 7 days prior to the testing day (handled for 3 min, placed in the testing box for 10 min). On the 8th day, mice were returned to the testing chamber where one of the objects had been replaced with a novel object. Time spent interacting with each object was scored using a custom code addition (https://github.com/PhillipsML/NORPhillips, 2019a, copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/NOR) to JAABA (https://github.com/kristinbranson/JAABABranson, 2019), which was taught to identify active sniffing behavior, and allow quantification of the sniffing time within a user-defined region of interest around each object. Memory scores were calculated for the first 4 min of the test day. Memory discrepancy scores were calculated as (Time Following Novel - Time Following Familiar) / (Time Following Novel + Time Following Familiar) * 100. Statistical tests were performed on the discrimination indices per mouse.

Chemogenetic manipulation with DREADDs and CNO

Long-term treatment

P20 WT and Mecp2 KO mice were injected with CAV-2-Cre (Biocampus, Institute of Molecular Genetics, Montpellier, France) into the mPFC; 250 nL at 1.45 AP, 0.5 ML, and 1.45 DV, and with either AAV8-hSyn-DIO-mCherry, AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM4Di(Gi)-mCherry, or AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3Dq(Gq)-mCherry into the vHIP; 500 nL at 3.5 AP, 3.3 ML, and 4.0 DV (all viruses from the UNC Vector core). CNO (Tocris Bioscience) was first dissolved in DMSO (5 mg in 200 µL), and then diluted in 200 mL of water with 5 mM saccharine (Sigma Aldrich) (Carvalho Poyraz et al., 2016). Mice were allowed ad libitum access to this solution in a standard drinking bottle starting at P34. Mice were tested using the unrestricted social interaction assay at P45. Mice were kept on CNO and sacrificed 3–5 days after behavioral testing for the preparation of ex vivo mPFC slices for voltage-sensitive dye imaging. Previous reports have validated the efficacy of long-term activity modulation using CNO activation of DREADDs, though all potential adaptations were not tested (Cheng et al., 2019).

Acute treatment

P20 WT mice were injected with CAV-2-Cre either into the mPFC; 250 nL at 1.45 AP, 0.5 ML, and 1.45 DV; or the NAc; 250 nL at 0.9 AP, 0.9 ML, and 3.8 DV; and AAV8-hSyn-DIO-mCherry or AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM3Dq(Gq)-mCherry into the vHIP; 500 nL at 3.5 AP, 3.3 ML, and 4.0 DV. P20 Mecp2 KO mice were injected with CAV-2-Cre into the mPFC; 250 nL at 1.45 AP, 0.5 ML, and 1.45 DV; and AAV8-hSyn-DIO-mCherry or AAV8-hSyn-DIO-hM4Di(Gi)-mCherry into the vHIP; 500 nL at 3.5 AP, 3.3 ML, and 4.0 DV. Mice were acclimated to IP needle pokes for 2 weeks prior to behavioral tests, though were not injected with any substance. On testing day, mice received a single IP injection of CNO (3 mg per kg of body weight) 2 hr before testing (Smith et al., 2015).

Quantification and statistical analysis

All experiments were performed blinded to genotype, with the exception of AAV injections expressing DREADDs. Because DREADD-mediated long-term excitation of the already hyperactive vHIP of Mecp2 KO mice will not reveal new information neither on Rett syndrome nor typical social behaviors, we did not include that experimental group to reduce the number of experimental mice. Thus, the experimenters were not blinded to genotype when injecting mice in order to optimize the number of animals to be used in these experiments. The experimenters were blinded during behavioral testing, though some Mecp2 KO mice were easy to identify due to their Rett-like symptoms. The implementation of computer vision to score all behavioral data, and of Matlab codes to analyze all VSD data were additional steps taken to ensure unbiased acquisition and data analyses. In addition, all statistical analyses were performed blinded to the genotype and treatment groups using Prism (GraphPad) and Matlab. Power analyses were performed using G*Power (Faul et al., 2009). Statistical tests used in each experiment are provided in main text, within associated figure legend, or within the statistical table in supplemental information. Data first underwent a test for normalcy: either D’Agostino and Pearson’s or, in the case that the number of replicates was not sufficient for this test, Shapiro-Wilk normalcy test. The decision to use either parametric or non-parametric tests were dependent on this outcome. ANOVAs were conducted in tandem with Benjamani and Hochberg Multiple Comparisons, unless otherwise stated. Memory discrepancy indices were first tested using t-tests (two groups) or ANOVAs (more than two groups) to determine differences between groups and secondly tested in a one-sample t-test against chance value (0) to determine if the preference was significant. Analysis of input-output relationships used Two-Way Repeated Measures (RM) ANOVA. Significance was defined as p<0.05, with the specific statistical test provided in main text or within associated figure legend in addition to the statistical table in supplemental information. Sample sizes (n) refer to number of cells, number of slices, or number of animals, with the specific convention provided in the main text or within the associated figure legend. Significance conventions are as follows: *: p<0.05; **: p<0.01; ***: p<0.001. Sample sizes are provided in main text, within associated figure legend, or within the statistical table in supplemental information. Behavioral data are presented as mean ± SD, all other data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by NIH grant R56-NS103089-01A1 (LP-M), the Civitan International Foundation (MLP), NIH training grant T32-NS061788-04 (MLP), and Rettsyndrome.org (LP-M). We thank Dr. Wei Li for comments on the manuscript, Ms. Karen Ayala-Baylon for mouse colony maintenance, and Dr. Takafumi Inoue (Waseda University, Tokyo, Japan) for electrophysiology and imaging software. We thank the Biostatistics Clinic of UAB’s Center for Clinical and Translational Science for assistance with statistical analyses. We also thank Mr. Rahul Gaini, Ms. Allison Fusilier, and Ms. Melissa Bentley for their contributions to data acquisition and analyses in early phases of this project.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Lucas Pozzo-Miller, Email: lucaspm@uab.edu.

Laura L Colgin, University of Texas at Austin, United States.

Lisa M Monteggia, Vanderbilt University, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institutes of Health R56-NS103089-01A1 to Lucas Pozzo-Miller.

  • Civitan International Civitan Emerging Scholar: W John Rynearson Award to Mary L Phillips.

  • International Rett Syndrome Foundation to Lucas Pozzo-Miller.

  • National Institutes of Health T32-NS061788-04 to Mary L Phillips.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: Mice were handled and housed according to the Committee on Laboratory Animal Resources of the National Institutes of Health. All experimental protocols were reviewed and approved annually by the Institutional Animals Care and Use Committee of the University of Alabama at Birmingham (IACUC-20114).

Additional files

Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.043

Data availability

All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files. Custom code used is available at https://github.com/PhillipsML/VoltageDyeAnalysis and https://github.com/PhillipsML/NOR (copies archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/VoltageDyeAnalysis and https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/NOR).

References

  1. Anastasiades PG, Marlin JJ, Carter AG. Cell-Type specificity of callosally evoked excitation and feedforward inhibition in the prefrontal cortex. Cell Reports. 2018;22:679–692. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2017.12.073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Armbruster BN, Li X, Pausch MH, Herlitze S, Roth BL. Evolving the lock to fit the key to create a family of G protein-coupled receptors potently activated by an inert ligand. PNAS. 2007;104:5163–5168. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0700293104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bandyopadhyay S, Gonzalez-Islas C, Hablitz JJ. Dopamine enhances spatiotemporal spread of activity in rat prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2005;93:864–872. doi: 10.1152/jn.00922.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barendse EM, Hendriks MPH, Thoonen G, Aldenkamp AP, Kessels RPC. Social behaviour and social cognition in high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorder (ASD): two sides of the same coin? Cognitive Processing. 2018;19:545–555. doi: 10.1007/s10339-018-0866-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Boender AJ, de Jong JW, Boekhoudt L, Luijendijk MC, van der Plasse G, Adan RA. Combined use of the canine adenovirus-2 and DREADD-technology to activate specific neural pathways in vivo. PLOS ONE. 2014;9:e95392–e95396. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0095392. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Branson KJ. GitHub. 2019 https://github.com/kristinbranson/JAABA
  7. Brockmann MD, Kukovic M, Schönfeld M, Sedlacik J, Hanganu-Opatz IL. Hypoxia-ischemia disrupts directed interactions within neonatal prefrontal-hippocampal networks. PLOS ONE. 2013;8:e83074. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brumback AC, Ellwood IT, Kjaerby C, Iafrati J, Robinson S, Lee AT, Patel T, Nagaraj S, Davatolhagh F, Sohal VS. Identifying specific prefrontal neurons that contribute to autism-associated abnormalities in physiology and social behavior. Molecular Psychiatry. 2018;23:2078–2089. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Calfa G, Hablitz JJ, Pozzo-Miller L. Network hyperexcitability in hippocampal slices from Mecp2 mutant mice revealed by voltage-sensitive dye imaging. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011;105:1768–1784. doi: 10.1152/jn.00800.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Calfa G, Li W, Rutherford JM, Pozzo-Miller L. Excitation/inhibition imbalance and impaired synaptic inhibition in hippocampal area CA3 of Mecp2 knockout mice. Hippocampus. 2015;25:159–168. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Carvalho Poyraz F, Holzner E, Bailey MR, Meszaros J, Kenney L, Kheirbek MA, Balsam PD, Kellendonk C. Decreasing striatopallidal pathway function enhances motivation by energizing the initiation of Goal-Directed action. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:5988–6001. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0444-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chang PY, Jackson MB. Interpretation and optimization of absorbance and fluorescence signals from voltage-sensitive dyes. Journal of Membrane Biology. 2003;196:105–116. doi: 10.1007/s00232-003-0629-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chen RZ, Akbarian S, Tudor M, Jaenisch R. Deficiency of methyl-CpG binding protein-2 in CNS neurons results in a Rett-like phenotype in mice. Nature Genetics. 2001;27:327–331. doi: 10.1038/85906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cheng J, Umschweif G, Leung J, Sagi Y, Greengard P. HCN2 channels in cholinergic interneurons of nucleus accumbens shell regulate depressive behaviors. Neuron. 2019;101:662–672. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.12.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cohen S, Greenberg ME. Communication between the synapse and the nucleus in neuronal development, plasticity, and disease. Annual Review of Cell and Developmental Biology. 2008;24:183–209. doi: 10.1146/annurev.cellbio.24.110707.175235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dégenètais E, Thierry AM, Glowinski J, Gioanni Y. Synaptic influence of hippocampus on pyramidal cells of the rat prefrontal cortex: an in vivo intracellular recording study. Cerebral Cortex. 2003;13:782–792. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.7.782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dembrow NC, Chitwood RA, Johnston D. Projection-specific neuromodulation of medial prefrontal cortex neurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:16922–16937. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3644-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dembrow NC, Zemelman BV, Johnston D. Temporal dynamics of L5 dendrites in medial prefrontal cortex regulate integration versus coincidence detection of afferent inputs. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:4501–4514. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4673-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Durand S, Patrizi A, Quast KB, Hachigian L, Pavlyuk R, Saxena A, Carninci P, Hensch TK, Fagiolini M. NMDA receptor regulation prevents regression of visual cortical function in the absence of Mecp2. Neuron. 2012;76:1078–1090. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG. Statistical power analyses using G*power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods. 2009;41:1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Franklin TB, Silva BA, Perova Z, Marrone L, Masferrer ME, Zhan Y, Kaplan A, Greetham L, Verrechia V, Halman A, Pagella S, Vyssotski AL, Illarionova A, Grinevich V, Branco T, Gross CT. Prefrontal cortical control of a brainstem social behavior circuit. Nature Neuroscience. 2017;20:260–270. doi: 10.1038/nn.4470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gabbott P, Headlam A, Busby S. Morphological evidence that CA1 hippocampal afferents monosynaptically innervate PV-containing neurons and NADPH-diaphorase reactive cells in the medial prefrontal cortex (Areas 25/32) of the rat. Brain Research. 2002;946:314–322. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(02)02487-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gala R, Lebrecht D, Sahlender DA, Jorstad A, Knott G, Holtmaat A, Stepanyants A. Computer assisted detection of axonal bouton structural plasticity in in vivo time-lapse images. eLife. 2017;6:e29315. doi: 10.7554/eLife.29315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gomez JL, Bonaventura J, Lesniak W, Mathews WB, Sysa-Shah P, Rodriguez LA, Ellis RJ, Richie CT, Harvey BK, Dannals RF, Pomper MG, Bonci A, Michaelides M. Chemogenetics revealed: dreadd occupancy and activation via converted clozapine. Science. 2017;357:503–507. doi: 10.1126/science.aan2475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gordon JA. Oscillations and hippocampal-prefrontal synchrony. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2011;21:486–491. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.02.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Grinvald A, Frostig RD, Lieke E, Hildesheim R. Optical imaging of neuronal activity. Physiological Reviews. 1988;68:1285–1366. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1988.68.4.1285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Guirado R, Umemori J, Sipilä P, Castrén E. Evidence for competition for target innervation in the medial prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 2016;26:1287–1294. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Guy J, Hendrich B, Holmes M, Martin JE, Bird A. A mouse Mecp2-null mutation causes neurological symptoms that mimic rett syndrome. Nature Genetics. 2001;27:322–326. doi: 10.1038/85899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Harris KM, Stevens JK. Dendritic spines of CA 1 pyramidal cells in the rat hippocampus: serial electron microscopy with reference to their biophysical characteristics. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1989;9:2982–2997. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-08-02982.1989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Harris KM, Sultan P. Variation in the number, location and size of synaptic vesicles provides an anatomical basis for the nonuniform probability of release at hippocampal CA1 synapses. Neuropharmacology. 1995;34:1387–1395. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(95)00142-S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Harris KM, Weinberg RJ. Ultrastructure of synapses in the mammalian brain. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology. 2012;4:a005587. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a005587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hitti FL, Siegelbaum SA. The hippocampal CA2 region is essential for social memory. Nature. 2014;508:88–92. doi: 10.1038/nature13028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hnasko TS, Perez FA, Scouras AD, Stoll EA, Gale SD, Luquet S, Phillips PE, Kremer EJ, Palmiter RD. Cre recombinase-mediated restoration of nigrostriatal dopamine in dopamine-deficient mice reverses hypophagia and bradykinesia. PNAS. 2006;103:8858–8863. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0603081103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Huang Y-Y, Simpson E, Kellendonk C, Kandel ER. Genetic evidence for the bidirectional modulation of synaptic plasticity in the prefrontal cortex by D1 receptors. PNAS. 2004;101:3236–3241. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308280101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Inoue T. TI workbench, an integrated software package for electrophysiology and imaging. Microscopy. 2018;67:129–143. doi: 10.1093/jmicro/dfy015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Izaki Y, Takita M, Nomura M. Mouse hippocampo-prefrontal paired-pulse facilitation and long-term potentiation in vivo. Neuroreport. 2001;12:1191–1193. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200105080-00028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Izaki Y, Takita M, Nomura M, Akema T. Differences between paired-pulse facilitation and long-term potentiation in the dorsal and ventral hippocampal CA1-prefrontal pathways of rats. Brain Research. 2003;992:142–145. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(03)03538-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kabra M, Robie AA, Rivera-Alba M, Branson S, Branson K. JAABA: interactive machine learning for automatic annotation of animal behavior. Nature Methods. 2013;10:64–67. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Klapoetke NC, Murata Y, Kim SS, Pulver SR, Birdsey-Benson A, Cho YK, Morimoto TK, Chuong AS, Carpenter EJ, Tian Z, Wang J, Xie Y, Yan Z, Zhang Y, Chow BY, Surek B, Melkonian M, Jayaraman V, Constantine-Paton M, Wong GK, Boyden ES. Independent optical excitation of distinct neural populations. Nature Methods. 2014;11:338–346. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kremer EJ, Boutin S, Chillon M, Danos O. Canine adenovirus vectors: an alternative for adenovirus-mediated gene transfer. Journal of Virology. 2000;74:505–512. doi: 10.1128/JVI.74.1.505-512.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kron M, Howell CJ, Adams IT, Ransbottom M, Christian D, Ogier M, Katz DM. Brain activity mapping in Mecp2 mutant mice reveals functional deficits in forebrain circuits, including key nodes in the default mode network, that are reversed with ketamine treatment. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:13860–13872. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2159-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Laroche S, Davis S, Jay TM. Plasticity at hippocampal to prefrontal cortex synapses: dual roles in working memory and consolidation. Hippocampus. 2000;10:438–446. doi: 10.1002/1098-1063(2000)10:4&#x0003c;438::AID-HIPO10&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Li M, Long C, Yang L. Hippocampal-prefrontal circuit and disrupted functional connectivity in psychiatric and neurodegenerative disorders. BioMed Research International. 2015;2015:1–10. doi: 10.1155/2015/810548. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Li W, Xu X, Pozzo-Miller L. Excitatory synapses are stronger in the hippocampus of rett syndrome mice due to altered synaptic trafficking of AMPA-type glutamate receptors. PNAS. 2016;113:E1575–E1584. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1517244113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Liang B, Zhang L, Barbera G, Fang W, Zhang J, Chen X, Chen R, Li Y, Lin DT. Distinct and dynamic ON and OFF neural ensembles in the prefrontal cortex code social exploration. Neuron. 2018;100:700–714. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.08.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Liu X, Carter AG. Ventral hippocampal inputs preferentially drive corticocortical neurons in the infralimbic prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;38:7351–7363. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0378-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Longair MH, Baker DA, Armstrong JD. Simple neurite tracer: open source software for reconstruction, visualization and analysis of neuronal processes. Bioinformatics. 2011;27:2453–2454. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btr390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Marek R, Jin J, Goode TD, Giustino TF, Wang Q, Acca GM, Holehonnur R, Ploski JE, Fitzgerald PJ, Lynagh T, Lynch JW, Maren S, Sah P. Hippocampus-driven feed-forward inhibition of the prefrontal cortex mediates relapse of extinguished fear. Nature Neuroscience. 2018;21:384–392. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0073-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Matsuzaki M, Honkura N, Ellis-Davies GC, Kasai H. Structural basis of long-term potentiation in single dendritic spines. Nature. 2004;429:761–766. doi: 10.1038/nature02617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Meira T, Leroy F, Buss EW, Oliva A, Park J, Siegelbaum SA. A hippocampal circuit linking dorsal CA2 to ventral CA1 critical for social memory dynamics. Nature Communications. 2018;9:4163. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-06501-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Morello N, Schina R, Pilotto F, Phillips M, Melani R, Plicato O, Pizzorusso T, Pozzo-Miller L, Giustetto M. Loss of Mecp2 causes atypical synaptic and molecular plasticity of Parvalbumin-Expressing interneurons reflecting rett Syndrome-Like sensorimotor defects. Eneuro. 2018;5:ENEURO.0086-18.2018. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0086-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Murthy VN, Schikorski T, Stevens CF, Zhu Y. Inactivity produces increases in neurotransmitter release and synapse size. Neuron. 2001;32:673–682. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00500-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Nelson SB, Valakh V. Excitatory/Inhibitory balance and circuit homeostasis in autism spectrum disorders. Neuron. 2015;87:684–698. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.07.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Ohayon S, Avni O, Taylor AL, Perona P, Roian Egnor SE. Automated multi-day tracking of marked mice for the analysis of social behaviour. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2013;219:10–19. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.05.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Okuyama T, Kitamura T, Roy DS, Itohara S, Tonegawa S. Ventral CA1 neurons store social memory. Science. 2016;353:1536–1541. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf7003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Padilla-Coreano N, Bolkan SS, Pierce GM, Blackman DR, Hardin WD, Garcia-Garcia AL, Spellman TJ, Gordon JA. Direct ventral Hippocampal-Prefrontal input is required for Anxiety-Related neural activity and behavior. Neuron. 2016;89:857–866. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Parent MA, Wang L, Su J, Netoff T, Yuan LL. Identification of the hippocampal input to medial prefrontal cortex in vitro. Cerebral Cortex. 2010;20:393–403. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Petreanu L, Mao T, Sternson SM, Svoboda K. The subcellular organization of neocortical excitatory connections. Nature. 2009;457:1142–1145. doi: 10.1038/nature07709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Phillips ML. NOR. E41f712GitHub. 2019a https://github.com/PhillipsML/NOR
  60. Phillips ML. VoltageDyeAnalysis. 189b41eGitHub. 2019b https://github.com/PhillipsML/VoltageDyeAnalysis
  61. Quattrochi JJ, Mamelak AN, Madison RD, Macklis JD, Hobson JA. Mapping neuronal inputs to REM sleep induction sites with carbachol-fluorescent microspheres. Science. 1989;245:984–986. doi: 10.1126/science.2475910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Robie AA, Seagraves KM, Egnor SE, Branson K. Machine vision methods for analyzing social interactions. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 2017;220:25–34. doi: 10.1242/jeb.142281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Ruda M, Coulter JD. Axonal and transneuronal transport of wheat germ agglutinin demonstrated by immunocytochemistry. Brain Research. 1982;249:237–246. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(82)90057-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Sceniak MP, Lang M, Enomoto AC, James Howell C, Hermes DJ, Katz DM. Mechanisms of functional hypoconnectivity in the medial prefrontal cortex of Mecp2 null mice. Cerebral Cortex. 2016;26:1938–1956. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Schmued LC, Fallon JH. Fluoro-Gold: a new fluorescent retrograde axonal tracer with numerous unique properties. Brain Research. 1986;377:147–154. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(86)91199-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Selimbeyoglu A, Kim CK, Inoue M, Lee SY, Hong ASO, Kauvar I, Ramakrishnan C, Fenno LE, Davidson TJ, Wright M, Deisseroth K. Modulation of prefrontal cortex excitation/inhibition balance rescues social behavior in CNTNAP2-deficient mice. Science Translational Medicine. 2017;9:eaah6733. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.aah6733. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Smith RS, Hu R, DeSouza A, Eberly CL, Krahe K, Chan W, Araneda RC. Differential muscarinic modulation in the olfactory bulb. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:10773–10785. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0099-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Tavares RM, Mendelsohn A, Grossman Y, Williams CH, Shapiro M, Trope Y, Schiller D. A map for social navigation in the human brain. Neuron. 2015;87:231–243. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.06.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Thierry AM, Gioanni Y, Dégénétais E, Glowinski J. Hippocampo-prefrontal cortex pathway: anatomical and electrophysiological characteristics. Hippocampus. 2000;10:411–419. doi: 10.1002/1098-1063(2000)10:4&#x0003c;411::AID-HIPO7&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Tomassy GS, Morello N, Calcagno E, Giustetto M. Developmental abnormalities of cortical interneurons precede symptoms onset in a mouse model of rett syndrome. Journal of Neurochemistry. 2014;131:115–127. doi: 10.1111/jnc.12803. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Xu X, Pozzo-Miller L. EEA1 restores homeostatic synaptic plasticity in hippocampal neurons from rett syndrome mice. The Journal of Physiology. 2017;595:5699–5712. doi: 10.1113/JP274450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Yizhar O, Fenno LE, Prigge M, Schneider F, Davidson TJ, O'Shea DJ, Sohal VS, Goshen I, Finkelstein J, Paz JT, Stehfest K, Fudim R, Ramakrishnan C, Huguenard JR, Hegemann P, Deisseroth K. Neocortical excitation/inhibition balance in information processing and social dysfunction. Nature. 2011;477:171–178. doi: 10.1038/nature10360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Lisa M Monteggia1
Reviewed by: Jeffrey L Neul2

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Ventral hippocampal projections to the medial prefrontal cortex regulate social memory" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, and the evaluation has been overseen by Laura Colgin as the Senior Editor. The following individuals involved in review of your submission have agreed to reveal his identity: Jeffrey L Neul (Reviewer #3).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another, and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

This study uses a large array of techniques to examine the properties of vHIP – mPFC connections in wild type and MeCP2KO animal to establish the role of this circuit in regulation of social behavior. Although a large number of synaptic and cellular deficits have been characterized in RTT models, the circuit deficits associated with this disorder remain poorly understood. Therefore, the reviewers overall agree that the current study is in principle an important addition to existing studies on RTT pathophysiology. However, the authors need to revise several aspects of their paper in order to validate the conclusions. More rigorous data analyses (in particular statistical tests) are needed to strengthen this rather extensive study.

Essential revisions:

1) As stated in the individual reviewers' comments below, statistical analysis in each figure should strengthened. In particular the authors should present better justifications for the statistical tests they used and also validate the sample sizes (i.e., perform power analyses). If all claims hold up under statistical scrutiny, then many of the other criticisms in the individual reviews become minor.

2) The authors should comment on the potential caveats of their experimental design in several places (use of cFOS signals and long term use of DREADD-CNO system). Experimental design details require better justification overall.

3) Related to the above point, the design of the social interaction tests requires justification. It is unclear why they authors chose to only assess the first 4 minutes of the 10 minute encounter for the behavioral test. A reasonable rationale should be provided to explain this choice.

The individual reviews are included below for your information.

Reviewer #1:

This study uses a large array of techniques to examine the properties of vHIP – mPFC connections in wild type and MeCP2KO animal to establish the role of this circuit in regulation of social behavior. Although a large number of synaptic and cellular deficits have been characterized in RTT models, the circuit deficits associated with this disorder remain poorly understood. Therefore, the current study is in principle a welcome addition to existing canon of studies on RTT pathophysiology. However, the authors need to clarify several aspects of their experiments and provide more technical details to make this rather extensive study more accessible to a wider audience.

1) Figure 1: cFOS staining should use histograms to better present the distribution of fluorescence intensities? What do these intensities present, cells, clusters? Clarify the relationship between neuronal activity and cFOS staining. Why count neurons, rather than quantify staining intensity per neuron?

2) Figure 3: The relationship between fEPSPs and voltage sensitive dye signals. EPSP slope is a more reliable reporter for synaptic responses, whereas fEPSP amplitudes are rather non-linear reporters for postsynaptic activity. What do the authors think they are monitoring using voltage sensitive dyes (synaptic responses, firing or a loosely defined measure of "activity")?

3) The authors should be commended for the well-controlled DREADD-CNO experiments. That said, the authors primarily perform long term experiments using this system. It is important to validate the DREADD system works as expected under the conditions of long-term (11 days?) exposure to CNO. It is plausible to expect some level of adaptation and plasticity in the system. However, the authors' interpretation of their result does not take this possibility into account.

4) It is extremely difficult to interpret the experiments presented in Figure 8 in terms of a change in presynaptic efficacy. The authors use an extremely artificial setting (4-AP + TTX+ 4 mM Ca2+) to evaluate synaptic efficacy via optogenetic stimulation. It is difficult ascertain if the differences seen in these experiments are indeed due to bigger boutons and increased presynaptic release probability.

Reviewer #2:

The authors demonstrate that the projection from ventral hippocampus to mPFC is hyperactive in symptomatic Rett mice, yet activity driven by intracortical stimulation is decreased. The electrophysiological findings in Figures 3 and Figure 4, and anatomical findings in Figure 6, are convincing and compelling. However, the link to behavior is tenuous, largely because of issues with the statistics. It is difficult to gauge the claims made in Figure 1, Figure 2, Figure 4, Figure 5. Although this is a potentially very interesting manuscript, the statistical issues make it difficult to assess the impact of the paper, as well as what additional experiments or analyses may be needed.

Specifically:

Figure 1 represents an extremely ambitious experiment, as the authors compare c-Fos expression across two experimental conditions, two genotypes, and neurons that project to two regions. I have a number of concerns about the experimental design and analysis. In sum, the data, as presented, do not fully support the claim that mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons are selectively activated during social encounters.

The rationale for using the behavioral task in Figure 1C is a bit unclear. (If this is a commonly used and well-accepted protocol, please cite its prior use.) On the control arm of the task, the subject gets two novel objects consecutively (two fake mice). On the experimental arm, the subject gets a familiar mouse and then a novel mouse. It is not clear why two steps are needed here. It seems one of the following designs may better assess social encounters:

a) Two arms being a familiar mouse, then sac, or a novel mouse, then sac.b) Two arms being a novel fake mouse, then sac, or a novel mouse, then sac.

The methods for quantifying images (as in Figure 1D) are not described.

The authors report number of cells as "n" in Figure 1E, but in this case it seems as though the correct biological replicate here should be number of animals (and c-Fos expression should be averaged within animal).

Statistically, the authors report a Kruskal-Wallis test, which is a non-parametric one-way ANOVA. But a one-way ANOVA is not appropriate for such an ambitious experiment, where three conditions are being compared simultaneously (genotype, behavioral condition, and projection). For example, the conclusion that "the differences in c-Fos intensities between the object and social conditions were smaller in Mecp2 KO mice" (subsection “mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons are selectively activated during social encounters”) appears to be true, but is not supported by a statistically significant interaction between genotype and behavioral condition. Overall, the study is likely under-powered (2-4 animals per group) for a 3x2 design and a three-way ANOVA.

I have a similar concern for Figure 2. On the whole, the data in Figure 2 seem to suggest impairments in social behavior and social memory in Rett mice, but it is not clear if these findings are supported statistically. The authors conclude that "Mecp2 KO mice spent similar amounts of time in both chambers" (Subsection “Atypical social behavior and impaired social memory in Mecp2 KO mice”), whereas WT mice spent more time in the chamber with stranger 2." This result, compared with the left panel of Figure 2C, leads the authors to conclude that Rett mice have normal social preference, but impaired social memory. However, paired t-tests are not sufficient to make across-group comparisons. A two-way RM ANOVA would be needed for both the left and right panels in Figure 2C. A statistically significant interaction in the right panel would provide statistical support to the claim that Rett mice have impaired social memory. The same point also applies to Figure 2G.

Subsection “Atypical social behavior and impaired social memory in Mecp2 KO mice”: "During the first four minutes of the assay, WT mice preferentially interacted with the novel mouse across all measured social behaviors, including following, sniffing, and "jumping on", after which social interaction declined regardless of target (n=6 mice, p<0.05, Student's paired t-test Figure 2F)." Two questions here:

a) It is not clear what the statistics are referring to here. It could be either the finding that WT mice interacted with the novel mouse preferentially in social behaviors (in which case the callout should be 2G), or it could be the claim that social interaction declined after the first four minutes. If the latter, the summarized data should be plotted in a panel in addition to the example trace in Figure 2F.

b) Why do the authors extract the first 4 minutes out of 10 for analysis? Has this been done by previous groups, and/or was this cutoff determined a priori? This is an important cutoff to justify, as it is used in multiple analyses throughout the paper.

Overall, Figure 3 nicely demonstrates that vHIP stimulation drives increased excitability in mPFC slices, but intracortical stimulation suggests that the local network is hypoexcitable in Rett. I have only a few minor questions about Figure 3:

The authors claim that VSD signals in ex vivo slices are directly proportional to the amplitude of field EPSPs and follow their kinetics (subsection “Increased influence of vHIP input on the mPFC network in Mecp2 KO mice”, Figure 3B-C). The only demonstration of this is a single example traces in Figure 3C. This point would be made much more strongly with an example I/O curve.

How is dF/F calculated for VSD signals in I/O curves (such as Figure 3E)? I apologize if I missed this in the methods. The example in Figure 3D includes both a time dimension and a spatial dimension of the VSD signal. Do the points in Figure 3E represent summated dF/F in the whole window at the peak time? Or the most intense peak in the window at the peak time?

In Figure 3—figure supplement 2, the authors demonstrate that Rett slices have impaired LTP. They present normalized data, likely because the Rett slices are hyperactive at baseline (visible in example trace and demonstrated in Figure 3). Is it possible that the lack of LTP might reflect a ceiling effect?

Figures 4—figure supplement 1, Figure 4—figure supplement 2, Figure 4—figure supplement 3 are mentioned in the text as being related to Figure 4, but they seem more related to Figure 3. These data show that social* and electrophysiological** impairments in Rett mice are not seen at the presymptomatic stage. This is an important point, and could perhaps even be highlighted in its own section in the text.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1Figure should use RM two-way ANOVAs. Also related to Figure 4—figure supplement 1Figure: do presymptomatic mice also show impaired social behavior in the unrestricted social assay?

Figure 4—figure supplement 3Figure should use two-way ANOVAs to compare genotype and age. A significant interaction between genotype and age would be needed to make the claim in subsection “Selective chemogenetic manipulation of mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons regulates social memory”.

Figure 4C-D presents another ambitious study: bidirectional DREADD manipulation of the vHIP -> mPFC pathway in WT and Rett mice. I have a few questions:

Were the experimenters blind to genotype for these experiments? If so, why is there an excitatory DREADD group only for the WT condition?

Student's and nonparametric paired t-tests are not appropriate to compare three conditions: genotype, familiar vs. novel, and DREADD. Therefore it is difficult to assess whether inhibitory DREADDs do indeed improve performance in Rett mice, or whether excitatory DREADDs do indeed mimic altered performance. One idea that perhaps could reduce the number of dimensions from 3 to 2 for this and other studies would be to plot "time following" as a proportion within animal (similar to "memory discrepancy" in Figure 4I). This way, a 2-way ANOVA would work instead of a 3-way ANOVA. Then perhaps a supplement could show the absolute following times in a descriptive way to the familiar and novel animal, because it is nice to see those raw data as well.

The way the electrophysiological data are presented in Figure 4 appear much cleaner, but again, these experiments were done at the same time. It is not clear how statistics were done here: were two-way ANOVAs done individually on each panel in Figures 4F and 4H? Because each panel contains WT mCherry and KO mCherry, plus a different condition. I like that it is plotted this way to make things easier to understand, but it is not clear whether the statistical analysis is done on the entire dataset (genotype X DREADD X stim. intensity)? Again, perhaps the stim. intensity dimension could be collapsed if need be to a single value if a three-way test is prohibitive.

Figure 4I is potentially very interesting, showing a within-animal correlation between behavioral performance and long-range electrophysiology, three days later. But this is only shown for the Mecp2 KO + inhibitory DREADD group. Is this correlation seen in other experimental groups?

In Figure 4K, what is the cutoff to decide if an animal has "improved social memory" or "no memory"?

The statistical questions presented earlier also apply to Figure 5 and make it difficult to interpret these results.

In Figure 6, the "n" used is not clear – the authors report an "n" of 9 sections from 3 mice. But the figures show only 3 data points. It seems as though mice would be more appropriate here than sections, and sections can be pooled.

Similar statistical concerns about the lack of two-way ANOVA for Figure 7, though it is clear there is no difference in Figure 7F. Statistical comparisons in 7G-H also should account for multiple comparisons.

Figure 8 nicely demonstrates what would be expected based on the results in Figure 3. The electrophysiological results overall are very convincing between these two figures: hyperactive long-range vHIP->mPFC and hypoactive local or c-mPFC->mPFC. Figure 8 also extends this result to show that long-range hyperexcitability is seen in excitatory neurons but not inhibitory neurons. However, the sample size is much larger in pyramidal cells (n=15 or n=11, versus n=6 in inhibitory cells in 8D). Increasing the sample size to ~10-15 neurons for Figure 8D-H would be useful in helping to understand if the trend seen in 8D and 8H (p=0.06) would reach statistical significance with equivalent power.

Reviewer #3:

This is an excellent, and quite thorough, body of work analyzing the role of the ventral hippocampus to medial prefrontal cortex circuit in social behavior, and specifically looking at the role in mice with MECP2 mutations. I think the work stands nicely on its own and provides a clear story. I have no major concerns and do not feel any additional experiments are needed.

A minor concern is that the statistical testing performed should be consistent. In some of the behavior analysis, non-parametric tests (Wilcoxon, Mann-Whitney, etc) are used and in other cases parametric (t-test, anova) are used. This is problematic when there are different tests used for different components of the same overall analysis, for example in Figure 2E for free moving behavior all the comparisons used student t-test except for aggressor which used Kolomogorov-Smirnov testing, which is probably not an appropriate test in this case anyway. It would appear that the test was cherry picked to identify significance, rather than choosing appropriate statistical methods before analysis. A non-parametric rank test like Mann-Whitney is probably appropriate, and ultimately if no significance exists for aggressor it will in no way change the overall manuscript.

A question is why behavior scores for social memory assessment were only taken for the first 4 minutes. Some rationale, hopefully generated a priori, for restricting the analysis to the first 4 minutes should be provided.

eLife. 2019 May 21;8:e44182. doi: 10.7554/eLife.44182.046

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) As stated in the individual reviewers' comments below, statistical analysis in each figure should strengthened. In particular the authors should present better justifications for the statistical tests they used and also validate the sample sizes (i.e., perform power analyses). If all claims hold up under statistical scrutiny, then many of the other criticisms in the individual reviews become minor.

We appreciate and reciprocate the reviewers’ goals to ensure that the proper statistical tests were used for the comparisons of all data sets. To this extent, we consulted with the Biostatistics Clinic of UAB’s Center for Clinical and Translational Science, and followed their advice to address the reviewers’ questions, and to re-analyze all data sets in the manuscript. As stated below for each case, these analyses did not change the conclusions reached in the original manuscript.

2) The authors should comment on the potential caveats of their experimental design in several places (use of cFOS signals and long term use of DREADD-CNO system). Experimental design details require better justification overall.

We have addressed the potential caveats of the long-term expression of DREADD and CNO administration, and better described the rationale for using c-Fos as a surrogate of neuronal activity. In addition, we now include additional details of experimental design in the Materials and methods section.

3) Related to the above point, the design of the social interaction tests requires justification. It is unclear why they authors chose to only assess the first 4 minutes of the 10 minute encounter for the behavioral test. A reasonable rationale should be provided to explain this choice.

In the revised manuscript and below, we provide an explanation for the use of the first 4 minutes of social interactions. In addition, we include a new supplementary figure to justify this analysis design.

The individual reviews are included below for your information.

Reviewer #1:

This study uses a large array of techniques to examine the properties of vHIP – mPFC connections in wild type and MeCP2KO animal to establish the role of this circuit in regulation of social behavior. Although a large number of synaptic and cellular deficits have been characterized in RTT models, the circuit deficits associated with this disorder remain poorly understood. Therefore, the current study is in principle a welcome addition to existing canon of studies on RTT pathophysiology. However, the authors need to clarify several aspects of their experiments and provide more technical details to make this rather extensive study more accessible to a wider audience.

1) Figure 1: cFOS staining should use histograms to better present the distribution of fluorescence intensities? What do these intensities present, cells, clusters? Clarify the relationship between neuronal activity and cFOS staining. Why count neurons, rather than quantify staining intensity per neuron?

We apologize for our lack of clarity: as reviewer 1 suggests, we have quantified the cFos intensity per neuron. In the original submission, each “n” in the bar graphs was an individual neuron. The new panel D in Figure 1 shows the cumulative probability distribution of c-Fos intensity per neuron, which presents the same information as a histogram but without relying on the statistical mean. In the new panel E of Figure 1 each “n” is an individual mouse, which allowed performing a 3-Way ANOVA, as requested by reviewer 2. We now extended the description of this experiment in the Results section and Materials and methods section of the manuscript.

Our rationale to quantify c-Fos intensity per neuron instead of counting the number of c-Fos-positive neurons is based on the fact that c-fos expression is a graded reflection of neuronal activity mediated by different intracellular Ca2+ levels (Cohen and Greenberg, 2009). In addition, we consider that choosing an arbitrary c-Fos intensity value as a threshold cut-off is more biased than presenting all c-Fos intensity values displayed by all neurons. Note that every c-Fos-positive cell was confirmed to be a neuron based on its NeuN staining, and that their projection target identified by retread labeling (i.e. three fluorescence channels: dual immunostaining for c-Fos and NeuN, plus retread fluorescence).

2) Figure 3: The relationship between fEPSPs and voltage sensitive dye signals. EPSP slope is a more reliable reporter for synaptic responses, whereas fEPSP amplitudes are rather non-linear reporters for postsynaptic activity. What do the authors think they are monitoring using voltage sensitive dyes (synaptic responses, firing or a loosely defined measure of "activity")?

The VSD signals reported in our manuscript occur during single subthreshold excitatory postsynaptic potentials recorded with extracellular electrodes (field EPSPs), not action potential firing, which is evident as faster rising and larger population spikes. We use this VSD imaging approach to estimate the depolarization evoked throughout the network by stimulation of selected afferent fibers. VSD imaging is complementary to electrophysiological recordings because it provides spatial information from sites beyond the recording site (e.g. Chang and Jackson, 2003).

We agree with the reviewer that the slope of the field EPSP reflects more accurately the underlying postsynaptic current without confounding dendritic depolarizations. In addition to following their kinetics and being directly proportional to the amplitude of field EPSPs in input-output curves, the ∆F/F of evoked VSD signals is also directly proportional to the initial slope of field EPSPs.The peak of VSD aptitudes reflects the largest synaptic depolarization throughout the network. The spatial spread of VSD signals reflects the proportion of the network with postsynaptic responses at a set afferent stimulus strength, while the temporal spread combines the proportion of the responding network with the duration of postsynaptic depolarization across the cortical network. We now include the figure below as a supplementary figure to better describe the relationship between VSD signals and postsynaptic responses, and endeavor to make these concepts more clear in the revised manuscript

3) The authors should be commended for the well-controlled DREADD-CNO experiments. That said, the authors primarily perform long term experiments using this system. It is important to validate the DREADD system works as expected under the conditions of long-term (11 days?) exposure to CNO. It is plausible to expect some level of adaptation and plasticity in the system. However, the authors' interpretation of their result does not take this possibility into account.

We apologize for our lack of clarity describing our rationale for these experiments. Neurons expressing the excitatory DREADD hM3Dq in slices from mice exposed to CNO for 14 days still show an increased firing rate after CNO application (Cheng et al., 2018). We now include this rationale and its justification in the revised manuscript.

4) It is extremely difficult to interpret the experiments presented in Figure 8 in terms of a change in presynaptic efficacy. The authors use an extremely artificial setting (4-AP + TTX+ 4 mM Ca2+) to evaluate synaptic efficacy via optogenetic stimulation. It is difficult ascertain if the differences seen in these experiments are indeed due to bigger boutons and increased presynaptic release probability.

Regarding the issue of synaptic strength, we did not intend to interpret our results as reflecting a purely presynaptic difference between Mecp2 KO and wild-type mice.The larger volume of presynaptic boutons of vHIP axons in the mPFC of Mecp2 KO mice suggest higher synaptic strength, but not necessarily due solely to a presynaptic mechanism. Larger presynaptic boutons are correlated to larger active zones and thus higher numbers of docked vesicles and higher Pr (e.g. Murthy et al., 2001), but also to larger spine heads with larger postsynaptic densities containing more glutamate receptors (Matsuzaki et al., 2004). The data presented in Figure 8 supports a difference in synaptic strength due to a postsynaptic mechanism:higher AMPA/NMDA ratio at vHIP-evoked optical EPSCs in mPFC layer 5 pyramidal neurons from Mecp2 KO mice. The observation of larger presynaptic boutons and higher AMPA/NMDA ratios reflect mechanisms underlying synaptic strength that are not mutually exclusive. In addition, preliminary analyses of optical EPSCs evoked by paired-pulse stimulation in standard recording conditions (i.e. normal extracellular Ca2+, no TTX, no 4-AP) suggest a higher Pr at vHIP synapses on mPFC pyramidal neurons in Mecp2 KO mice, but not on interneurons. The parsimonious interpretation is that vHIP synapses on mPFC pyramidal neurons are stronger in Mecp2 KO mice due to both higher presynaptic efficacy and higher postsynaptic responsiveness. We consider that the full characterization of presynaptic and postsynaptic function at vHIP-mPFC synapses is beyond the scope of the current manuscript.

We apologize for our lack of clarity describing our rationale for the design of these experiments. The reason to perform optogenetic stimulation in the presence of TTX, 4-AP, and 4mM extracellular Ca2+ was to isolate monosynaptic EPSCs for accurate measurements of their amplitude without the confounding contribution of polysynaptic responses (Petreneau et al., 2009).

Reviewer #2:

The authors demonstrate that the projection from ventral hippocampus to mPFC is hyperactive in symptomatic Rett mice, yet activity driven by intracortical stimulation is decreased. The electrophysiological findings in Figure 3 and Figure 4, and anatomical findings in Figure 6, are convincing and compelling. However, the link to behavior is tenuous, largely because of issues with the statistics. It is difficult to gauge the claims made in Figure 1, Figure 2, Figure 4, Figure 5. Although this is a potentially very interesting manuscript, the statistical issues make it difficult to assess the impact of the paper, as well as what additional experiments or analyses may be needed.

Specifically:

Figure 1 represents an extremely ambitious experiment, as the authors compare c-Fos expression across two experimental conditions, two genotypes, and neurons that project to two regions. I have a number of concerns about the experimental design and analysis. In sum, the data, as presented, do not fully support the claim that mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons are selectively activated during social encounters.

The rationale for using the behavioral task in Figure 1C is a bit unclear. (If this is a commonly used and well-accepted protocol, please cite its prior use.) On the control arm of the task, the subject gets two novel objects consecutively (two fake mice). On the experimental arm, the subject gets a familiar mouse and then a novel mouse. It is not clear why two steps are needed here. It seems one of the following designs may better assess social encounters:

a) Two arms being a familiar mouse, then sac, or a novel mouse, then sac.b) Two arms being a novel fake mouse, then sac, or a novel mouse, then sac.

We thank the reviewer for this opportunity to clarify the rationale of the these experiments. The two experimental designs suggested by the reviewer address two different issues: (a) is designed to test the hypothesis that vHIP neurons are activated by social interaction with a novel mouse (as opposed to a familiar one), while (b) tests the hypothesis that vHIP neurons are activated by social interaction with a novel mouse (as opposed to a toy mouse).

Our experimental design aims to test the second issue: whether mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons are activated by a social encounter, as opposed to exposure to a toy mouse. Okuyama et al., (2016) describe that exposure to mice caused activation of vHIP neurons, regardless of novelty; however, these authors did not differentiate between vHIP neurons that project to different brain areas. Because we were unsure of the sensitivity of the c-Fos intensity assay, we choose to present a strong stimulation to recruit as many mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons as possible to increase the signal-tonoise of the assay. To ensure maximal activation of as many mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons as possible we performed sequential interactions, first with a littermate mouse followed by a non-littermate mouse 1 hour later as the "social condition” Please note that this experiment was not performed to identify the vHIP neurons that encode social memory (as in the reviewer’s (a) design), which we think requires the application of a different experimental approach other than just c-Fos labeling.

The methods for quantifying images (as in Figure 1D) are not described.

We apologize for this oversight, we now include all methodological details.

The authors report number of cells as "n" in Figure 1E, but in this case it seems as though the correct biological replicate here should be number of animals (and c-Fos expression should be averaged within animal).

Statistically, the authors report a Kruskal-Wallis test, which is a non-parametric one-way ANOVA. But a one-way ANOVA is not appropriate for such an ambitious experiment, where three conditions are being compared simultaneously (genotype, behavioral condition, and projection). For example, the conclusion that "the differences in c-Fos intensities between the object and social conditions were smaller in Mecp2 KO mice" (subsection “mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons are selectively activated during social encounters”) appears to be true, but is not supported by a statistically significant interaction between genotype and behavioral condition. Overall, the study is likely under-powered (2-4 animals per group) for a 3x2 design and a three-way ANOVA.

Following the suggestion from reviewer 1 and consultation with biostatisticians at UAB (see above), panel D in Figure 1 in the revised manuscript shows the cumulative probability distributions of c-Fos intensities from individual neurons. In addition, panel D in the new Figure 1 shows the same data as bar graphs of c-Fos intensity in each cell averaged per mouse as the sampling number “n”. We performed a three-way ANOVA using genotype, behavioral condition, and projection as the conditions, which yielded statistically significant differences between the groups as in the original analyses.

I have a similar concern for Figure 2. On the whole, the data in Figure 2 seem to suggest impairments in social behavior and social memory in Rett mice, but it is not clear if these findings are supported statistically. The authors conclude that "Mecp2 KO mice spent similar amounts of time in both chambers" (Subsection “Atypical social behavior and impaired social memory in Mecp2 KO mice”), whereas WT mice spent more time in the chamber with stranger 2." This result, compared with the left panel of Figure 2C, leads the authors to conclude that Rett mice have normal social preference, but impaired social memory. However, paired t-tests are not sufficient to make across-group comparisons. A two-way RM ANOVA would be needed for both the left and right panels in Figure 2C. A statistically significant interaction in the right panel would provide statistical support to the claim that Rett mice have impaired social memory. The same point also applies to Figure 2G.

Following reviewer 2’s suggestion in Figure 4 question 2, we have changed all the panels illustrating the social memory results to now show a memory Discrimination Index as the variable for statistical comparisons, which yields a single value per group. The memory Discrimination Index is the difference in the time spent following a novel mouse compared to the time spent following a familiar mouse. For comparisons between 2 groups, we used either a Student’s t-test for parametric data or a MannWhitney test for non-parametric data (data distribution was tested with the D’Agostino and Pearson’s test for normalcy). For comparisons between 3 or more groups, we used a two-way ANOVA. In addition, we now report a post-hoc one sample t-test comparing the Discrimination Index values against chance values to determine if each group has a significant preference in following times. In addition, we now include all the raw data of following times in supplemental figures.

Subsection “Atypical social behavior and impaired social memory in Mecp2 KO mice”: "During the first four minutes of the assay, WT mice preferentially interacted with the novel mouse across all measured social behaviors, including following, sniffing, and "jumping on", after which social interaction declined regardless of target (n=6 mice, p<0.05, Student's paired t-test Figure 2F)." Two questions here:

We apologize for the typographical mistake: the statistics reported in 2G are about the memory component.

The rationale of using the first 4 minutes of the total 10 minutes of the unrestricted social interaction assay is based on the observation that more than 75% of the social interactions occur during the first 4 minutes. After that time, there is no longer a difference in time spent interacting with the target the mice compared to the time spent exploring the environment, as the test mouse becomes acclimated to their presence. As a result of this, the Discrimination Index after the first 4 minutes become a less reliable estimate of the social interactions. The entirety of the 10 minute videos are used for quantification of non-target specific behaviors, as different behaviors emerged at later times in the movies. We have plotted the analyses of the entire 10-minute videos and included as Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

a) It is not clear what the statistics are referring to here. It could be either the finding that WT mice interacted with the novel mouse preferentially in social behaviors (in which case the callout should be 2G), or it could be the claim that social interaction declined after the first four minutes. If the latter, the summarized data should be plotted in a panel in addition to the example trace in Figure 2F.

b) Why do the authors extract the first 4 minutes out of 10 for analysis? Has this been done by previous groups, and/or was this cutoff determined a priori? This is an important cutoff to justify, as it is used in multiple analyses throughout the paper.

Overall, Figure 3 nicely demonstrates that vHIP stimulation drives increased excitability in mPFC slices, but intracortical stimulation suggests that the local network is hypoexcitable in Rett. I have only a few minor questions about Figure 3:

The authors claim that VSD signals in ex vivo slices are directly proportional to the amplitude of field EPSPs and follow their kinetics (subsection “Increased influence of vHIP input on the mPFC network in Mecp2 KO mice”, Figure 3B-C). The only demonstration of this is a single example traces in Figure 3C. This point would be made much more strongly with an example I/O curve.

Please see our answer to question 2 by reviewer 1. Briefly, we now include the input output relationships of VSD signals and the initial slope of field EPSPs in a

supplementary figure, including representative images of color coded ∆F/F ratios, and ∆F/F traces from 4 stimulation intensities. Similar direct proportionality has been consistently reported in papers using organic VSD dyes for imaging subthresold voltage responses in ex vivo brain slices (e.g. Chang and Jackson, 2003; Bandyopadhyay et al., 2005; Calfa et al., 2011).

How is dF/F calculated for VSD signals in I/O curves (such as Figure 3E)? I apologize if I missed this in the methods. The example in Figure 3D includes both a time dimension and a spatial dimension of the VSD signal. Do the points in Figure 3E represent summated dF/F in the whole window at the peak time? Or the most intense peak in the window at the peak time?

We apologize for the lack of detail in the Materials and methods section, which are now included in the revised manuscript. The maximum VSD signals are the mean ∆F/F ratios within a 3x3 pixel region of interest (ROI) placed on the location with the highest ∆F/F ratio at 6 ms after afferent stimulation, which always occurred within layer 2/3 of mPFC slices. We chose 6 ms after stimulation because that coincides with the peak of the extracellularly recorded field EPSP. We now have uploaded our code to the GitHub repository and provide the link in the manuscript.

In Figure 3—figure supplement 2, the authors demonstrate that Rett slices have impaired LTP. They present normalized data, likely because the Rett slices are hyperactive at baseline (visible in example trace and demonstrated in Figure 3). Is it possible that the lack of LTP might reflect a ceiling effect?

The reviewer is correct, we think that the lack of LTP at vHIP-mPFC synapses is due to a “ceiling” effect. We have demonstrated that hippocampal CA3-CA1 synapses of Mecp2 KO mice are potentiated at baseline due to impaired synaptic AMPAR trafficking, which prevents further LTP (Li et al., 2016); a similar mechanism impairs homeostatic synaptic plasticity in Mecp2 KO hippocampal neurons in culture (Xu and Pozzo-Miller, 2017). We consider that performing the long list of experiments required to demonstrate the same mechanism at vHIP-mPFC synapses is beyond the scope of the present manuscript, and is the focus of our immediate efforts in the lab.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1, Figure 4—figure supplement 2, Figure 4—figure supplement 3 are mentioned in the text as being related to Figure 4, but they seem more related to Figure 3. These data show that social* and electrophysiological** impairments in Rett mice are not seen at the presymptomatic stage. This is an important point, and could perhaps even be highlighted in its own section in the text.

We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We agree that this is an important observation because it confirms other reports of typical behaviors and synaptic function in young presymptomatic Mecp2 KO mice (including our own work in the hippocampus), and have described it in the Results section, including a supplemental figure. However, presenting these data in a main figure will significantly lengthen the manuscript and distract from the main conclusions we wish to present.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1 should use RM two-way ANOVAs. Also related to Figure 4—figure supplement 1: do presymptomatic mice also show impaired social behavior in the unrestricted social assay?

We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We consider these experiments beyond the scope of this manuscript, because they are better suited for a separate study of the postnatal development of the vHIP-mPFC projection and its role in social behaviors, as well as of the developmental progression of social behavior deficits in Mecp2 KO mice.

Figure 4—figure supplement 3should use two-way ANOVAs to compare genotype and age. A significant interaction between genotype and age would be needed to make the claim in subsection “Selective chemogenetic manipulation of mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons regulates social memory”.

We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We have reanalyzed these data using a twoway ANOVA and find a significant interaction between Age and Genotype, with the outcome of the multiple comparisons analyses matching our original results.

Figure 4C-D presents another ambitious study: bidirectional DREADD manipulation of the vHIP -> mPFC pathway in WT and Rett mice. I have a few questions:

Were the experimenters blind to genotype for these experiments? If so, why is there an excitatory DREADD group only for the WT condition?

We decided to only test the effects of the excitatory DREADD in WT mice because making the vHIP more hyperactive in Mecp2 KO mice will surely affect behavioral responses that are already affected. Since the outcome of these experiments may not reveal novel information about Rett syndrome, we decided not to use more animals than those strictly necessary. The experimenters were not blinded to genotype when injecting mice in order to optimize the number of animals to be used in these experiments. The experimenters were blinded during behavioral testing, though some Mecp2 KO mice were profoundly symptomatic and easy to identify. The implementation of computer vision to score all behavioral data, and of Matlab codes to analyze all VSD data are additional steps taken to ensure unbiased acquisition and analyses of data.

Student's and nonparametric paired t-tests are not appropriate to compare three conditions: genotype, familiar vs. novel, and DREADD. Therefore it is difficult to assess whether inhibitory DREADDs do indeed improve performance in Rett mice, or whether excitatory DREADDs do indeed mimic altered performance. One idea that perhaps could reduce the number of dimensions from 3 to 2 for this and other studies would be to plot "time following" as a proportion within animal (similar to "memory discrepancy" in Figure 4I). This way, a 2-way ANOVA would work instead of a 3-way ANOVA. Then perhaps a supplement could show the absolute following times in a descriptive way to the familiar and novel animal, because it is nice to see those raw data as well.

The way the electrophysiological data are presented in Figure 4 appear much cleaner, but again, these experiments were done at the same time.

Regarding the changes made to the statistical analyses of behavioral data, please refer to our response to Figure 2 question 1. To briefly restate, following advice from biostatisticians at UAB (see above) and the reviewer’s suggestion, we have consolidated the time spent following familiar and novel mice into a Discrimination Index; the raw data of following times is now presented in supplementary figures. We performed a two-way ANOVA (Group x Discrimination Index) with multiple comparisons and a post-hoc one-sample t-test against chance to determine significant preferences. The outcome of these tests match the results presented in the original manuscript. We could not perform a three-way ANOVA (Genotype x DREADD x Memory) because the 2 genotypes did not receive the same DREADD treatment (as justified above). We have therefore referred to the Genotype x DREADD as “Group”, as per the suggestion of biostatisticians at UAB.

It is not clear how statistics were done here: were two-way ANOVAs done individually on each panel in Figures 4F and 4H? Because each panel contains WT mCherry and KO mCherry, plus a different condition. I like that it is plotted this way to make things easier to understand, but it is not clear whether the statistical analysis is done on the entire dataset (genotype X DREADD X stim. intensity)? Again, perhaps the stim. intensity dimension could be collapsed if need be to a single value if a three-way test is prohibitive.

We apologize for our lack of clarity. We performed a two-way repeated measures ANOVA (Group X Stimulation Intensity, with Stimulation Intensity being the repeated measure). We cannot do a three-way ANOVA because the genotypes received different DREADD treatments as stated above.

Figure 4I is potentially very interesting, showing a within-animal correlation between behavioral performance and long-range electrophysiology, three days later. But this is only shown for the Mecp2 KO + inhibitory DREADD group. Is this correlation seen in other experimental groups?

We now include the analyses of within-animal correlation of social memory

Discrimination Index with the amplitude of vHIP-evoked VSD signals in mPFC slices in the other experimental groups. Unfortunately, we have only 3 surviving control Mecp2 KO mice expressing mCherry that yielded both social behaviors and VSD imaging data in ex vivo slices. However, this group shows a trend of a correlation of vHIP-evoked VSD signals and memory Discrimination Index similar to that of Mecp2 KO mice expressing the inhibitory DREADD. When these data from all Mecp2 KO mice are pooled, there is a statistically significant correlation between social memory and vHIPdriven mPFC activity. Interestingly, this correlation is not statistically significant in any of the WT groups, which suggest an intriguing contribution of the altered mPFC microcircuit in Mecp2 KO mice. The correlation between social memory and mPFC activity evoked by intra-cortical stimulation is not statistically significant in any of the groups. The figure is included in the revised manuscript as Figure 4—figure supplement 5.

In Figure 4K, what is the cutoff to decide if an animal has "improved social memory" or "no memory"?

Throughout the manuscript we used a social memory Discrimination Index (DI), which was calculated as:

DI = (tfollowing unknown – tfollowing known/ tfollowing unknown + tfollowing known) * 100

Using this ratio, “Improved social memory” is DI > 0, and “No memory” is DI ≤ 0. This information has been added to the Materials and methods section.

The statistical questions presented earlier also apply to Figure 5 and make it difficult to interpret these results.

Please see our responses above regarding to new statistical analyses of behavioral data. Comparisons of the Discrimination Index between groups yielded the same outcome for the acute Mecp2 group and the NAc projection group. However, the DI was not statistically different between controls and hM3Dq-expressing WT mice in the mPFC projections group due to the large variability in the hM3Dq group. The control group had a DI significantly different than chance, whereas the hM3Dq did not. These new results do not change the main conclusion of our study.

In Figure 6, the "n" used is not clear – the authors report an "n" of 9 sections from 3 mice. But the figures show only 3 data points. It seems as though mice would be more appropriate here than sections, and sections can be pooled.

We apologize for our lack of clarity. The reviewer is correct, the brain sections were pooled and individual data points are the statistical averages across mice.

Similar statistical concerns about the lack of two-way ANOVA for Figure 7, though it is clear there is no difference in Figure 7F. Statistical comparisons in 7G-H also should account for multiple comparisons.

We moved the data of vHIP presynaptic boutons within mPFC Layer 2/3 to a supplemental figure. In addition, we performed a two-way ANOVA (Genotype x Projection) for the comparison of vHIP presynaptic boutons. We now show both the cumulative probability of vHIP bouton sizes and a bar graph with the statistical averages. We performed a two-way ANOVA to compare these data, which yielded the same outcome as in the original results.

Figure 8 nicely demonstrates what would be expected based on the results in Figure 3. The electrophysiological results overall are very convincing between these two figures: hyperactive long-range vHIP->mPFC and hypoactive local or c-mPFC->mPFC. Figure 8 also extends this result to show that long-range hyperexcitability is seen in excitatory neurons but not inhibitory neurons. However, the sample size is much larger in pyramidal cells (n=15 or n=11, versus n=6 in inhibitory cells in 8D). Increasing the sample size to ~10-15 neurons for Figure 8D-H would be useful in helping to understand if the trend seen in 8D and 8H (p=0.06) would reach statistical significance with equivalent power.

Following the reviewer’s suggestion, we have increased the sample size of the wholecell recordings from mPFC interneurons during optical stimulation of either vHIP or contralateral mPFC axons to n=11 from WT mice and n=12 from Mecp2 KO mice. Statistical comparisons yielded no significant differences in either vHIP-evoked synaptic currents or mPFC-evoked synaptic currents, as well as in the normalized data. We also have a larger sample size of AMPA/NMDA ratio evoked by optical stimulation of vHIP axons in mPFC interneurons, which also show no significant differences between WT and Mecp2 KO mice. These results with larger sample sizes yielded the same outcome as in the original results.

Reviewer #3:

This is an excellent, and quite thorough, body of work analyzing the role of the ventral hippocampus to medial prefrontal cortex circuit in social behavior, and specifically looking at the role in mice with MECP2 mutations. I think the work stands nicely on its own and provides a clear story. I have no major concerns and do not feel any additional experiments are needed.

A minor concern is that the statistical testing performed should be consistent. In some of the behavior analysis, non-parametric tests (Wilcoxon, Mann-Whitney, etc) are used and in other cases parametric (t-test, anova) are used. This is problematic when there are different tests used for different components of the same overall analysis, for example in Figure 2E for free moving behavior all the comparisons used student t-test except for aggressor which used Kolomogorov-Smirnov testing, which is probably not an appropriate test in this case anyway. It would appear that the test was cherry picked to identify significance, rather than choosing appropriate statistical methods before analysis. A non-parametric rank test like Mann-Whitney is probably appropriate, and ultimately if no significance exists for aggressor it will in no way change the overall manuscript.

We apologize for our lack of clarity and for giving the impression that we selected statistical tests to hunt for p value significance (not our practice). All statistical tests in the revised manuscript have been deemed appropriate by experts of the Biostatistics Clinic of UAB’s Center for Clinical and Translational Science. For all statistical comparisons, we first tested if the data fitted a normal distribution using either the D’Agostino and Pearson’s test or the Shapiro-Wilk test when the number of replicates was below the requirement for D’Agostino & Pearson test. Depending on the result of this normalcy test, the data were compared using either a parametric (normal distribution) or a non-parametric test (non-normal distribution). We list all the statistical tests and the pvalues yielded in each comparison in the statistical table. We re-analyzed all nonparametric data sets with two samples using the Mann-Whitney test as suggested, which yielded a lack of statistical significance for the aggression phenotype shown in Figure 2E.

A question is why behavior scores for social memory assessment were only taken for the first 4 minutes. Some rationale, hopefully generated a priori, for restricting the analysis to the first 4 minutes should be provided.

The rationale of using the first 4 minutes of the total 10 minutes of the unrestricted social interaction assay is described above in our responses to reviewer 2, including a figure that is also a supplemental figure in the revised manuscript.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—source data 1. mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons are selectively activated during sequential social encounters with familiar and novel mice.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.005
    Figure 1—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Higher number of c-Fos-positive neurons in the mPFC following social encounters.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.004
    Figure 2—source data 1. Mecp2KO mice have impaired social memory and atypical social interactions.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.010
    Figure 2—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Impaired social memory in Mecp2KO mice.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.008
    Figure 3—source data 1. Increased influence of vHIP axons in the mPFC of Mecp2KO mice.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.015
    Figure 3—figure supplement 2—source data 1. Impaired LTP at vHIP-mPFC synapses in Mecp2KO mice.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.014
    Figure 4—source data 1. VSD responses to vHIP stimulation in mPFC slices and LTP at vHIP-mPFC synapses are not altered in presymptomatic Mecp2KO mice.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.027
    Figure 4—figure supplement 1—source data 1. VSD responses to vHIP stimulation in mPFC slices and LTP at vHIP-mPFC synapses are not altered in presymptomatic Mecp2KO mice.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.018
    Figure 4—figure supplement 2—source data 1. VSD responses to vHIP stimulation in mPFC slices of Mecp2KO mice do not develop in a typical manner.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.020
    Figure 4—figure supplement 3—source data 1. Lack of effects of long-term DREADD stimulation by CNO on anxiety and general (non-social memory) behaviors.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.022
    Figure 4—figure supplement 4—source data 1. Memory Discrimination correlates to vHIP input to the mPFC in Mecp2KO mice.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.024
    Figure 4—figure supplement 5—source data 1. Activity of mPFC-projecting vHIP neurons modulates social memory in WT and Mecp2KO mice.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.026
    Figure 5—source data 1. Acute manipulation of activity of vHIP-mPFC projection neurons regulates social memory in a task- and projection-specific manner.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.031
    Figure 5—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Acute manipulation of activity of vHIP-mPFC projection neurons regulates social memory in a task- and projection-specific manner.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.030
    Figure 6—source data 1. Trans-synaptic tracing of vHIP target neurons in the mPFC.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.034
    Figure 7—source data 1. The size of presynaptic boutons is altered in layer 5 of the mPFC of Mecp2KO mice.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.038
    Figure 7—figure supplement 1—source data 1. The size of presynaptic boutons is altered in layer 5 of the mPFC of Mecp2KO mice.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.037
    Figure 8—source data 1. vHIP synapses on mPFC layerfivepyramidal neurons are stronger in Mecp2KO mice.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.042
    Figure 8—figure supplement 1—source data 1. vHIP synapses on mPFC layerfivepyramidal neurons are stronger in Mecp2KO mice.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.041
    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.44182.043

    Data Availability Statement

    All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files. Custom code used is available at https://github.com/PhillipsML/VoltageDyeAnalysis and https://github.com/PhillipsML/NOR (copies archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/VoltageDyeAnalysis and https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/NOR).


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES