Abstract
Exposure to ambient air pollution and noise is ubiquitous globally. A strong body of evidence links air pollution, and recently noise, to cardiovascular conditions that eventually may also affect cognition in the elderly. Data that support a broader influence of these exposures on cognitive function during aging is just starting to emerge. This review summarizes current findings and discusses methodological challenges and opportunities for research. Although current evidence is still limited, especially for chronic noise exposure, high exposure has been associated with faster cognitive decline either mediated through cerebrovascular events or resulting in Alzheimer’s disease. Ambient environmental exposures are chronic and affect large populations. While they may yield relatively modest-sized risks, they nevertheless result in large numbers of cases. Reducing environmental pollution is clearly feasible, though lowering levels requires collective action and long-term policies such as standard setting, often at the national level as well as at the local level.
Keywords: air pollution, noise, cognition, cognitive decline, dementia, Alzheimer’s disease
INTRODUCTION
Exposure to ambient air pollution is ubiquitous globally and has been associated with various adverse health outcomes in children and adults, including asthma, cardiovascular disease, respiratory disorders, adverse pregnancy outcomes, and all-cause mortality (52, 79). A strong body of evidence links air pollution and, more recently, noise to cardiovascular conditions. But data that support an influence of these exposures on cognitive function across the life course and on late-life rates of cognitive decline and dementia risk are just beginning to accumulate. It is commonly thought that a mixture of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and vascular neuropathology synergize, contributing to cognitive decline and impairment and dementia late in life (3, 74, 75). Cardiovascular risk factors and disease are linked with cognitive decline and dementia risk, suggesting that air pollution and noise exposure may contribute indirectly to cognitive decline and dementia risk by influencing vascular neuropathology. The extent to which air pollution or noise exposures directly affect cognitive decline and incidence of dementia is less well understood.
Cognitive decline and dementia are major concerns for both older adults and communities. Estimates vary, but among adults over 65, the prevalence of mild cognitive impairment and of dementia is thought to be 30–40% and 10%, respectively. The incidence and prevalence of both strongly increase with age (3). In the coming decades, virtually all countries will face challenges related to the growth of aging populations and the subsequent economic and social burden of caring for those with dementia (88).
Significance
Possible modifiable risk factors for dementia and cognitive impairment identified thus far include early-life education, physical activity, smoking, social isolation, and a number of health-related factors, such as diabetes, hypertension, traumatic brain injury, and hearing loss (5, 7, 53). While these factors could all be targets for intervention and prevention, some are more amenable to medical or public health strategies than others. Ambient environmental exposures such as air pollution, ambient noise, or ambient pesticides represent potential risk factors that could be changed by public policy (53). Many of the dementia-related risk factors may be influenced by—or arise from—environmental pollution. In fact, we argue that modification of proximal individual-level risk factors [see Livingston (53)] may achieve limited success as long as the causative upstream environmental exposure remains in place.
Ambient environmental exposures are long term and often affect large populations. While these exposures may yield relatively modest risk increases, they nevertheless result in large numbers of cases. Reducing environmental pollution is clearly feasible, though lowering levels requires collective action and long-term policies such as standard setting, often at the national level as well as the local level.
Pathways from Environment to the Brain
Tau and β-amyloid (Aβ) deposited in the brain are thought to be the proximate pathological mechanisms underlying most nonvascular dementias and cognitive impairment. However, little to no research is available that assesses how pollutants affect the development of tau or Aβ protein aggregates. The dominant paradigm (and much of the evidence) presently frames cardiovascular risk factors and conditions as possible mediators of the association between pollutants and cognitive change/dementia incidence (71, 84). For example, increasing global cortical Aβ deposition has been linked with an increasing number of conventional cardiovascular disease (CVD) risk factors at midlife such as obesity, smoking, hypertension, diabetes, and cholesterol (37), and an extensive literature links air pollution to stroke (8). Particles have been found to reduce heart rate variability; in addition, high levels of air pollution have been associated with chronic brain inflammation, which has in turn been linked to neurodegeneration in AD (9, 15, 16, 41, 58, 69, 76, 81). Animal studies have confirmed that ultrafine particles (<0.1 μm in aerodynamic diameter) can reach the brain, cause inflammation, and act as neurotoxins (33, 62, 89). Particles may access the brain directly from the nose: Olfactory bulb neurons have been found loaded with particles, impaired olfactory function is a precursor of AD (80), and pathogens may enter the brain via anterograde transport (42). In studies of autopsied brains from deceased residents of metropolitan areas with high particulate and ozone air pollution, investigators found the inflammatory mediator cyclo-oxygenase-2 to be more highly expressed in the frontal cortex and hippocampus and found more neuronal and astrocytic accumulation of AD-specific protein aggregates in neurons (13). Even more surprising is that tau hyperphosphorylation with pretangle-like material and diffuse Aβ plaques were found at younger ages in half of the exposed urbanites but in none of the unexposed urbanites (13).
Biologic pathways linking ambient noise exposure and dementia are less well studied. Some animal evidence indicates that long-term high-level noise exposure is associated with persistent tau pathology, accelerated overproduction of Aβ, and induction of abnormal auditory input to the brain, resulting in aberrant changes in the hippocampus and the cortex (22, 27–29, 56). A number of studies have found increased levels of neuronal oxidative stress in response to noise. Although evidence of neurodegenerative events from lower but chronic levels of noise exposure is generally lacking, epidemiologic studies have linked chronic noise to cardiovascular health, especially chronic hypertension (60, 61), and possibly to stroke occurrence as well (32).
STUDY REVIEWS
Cognitive Change and Dementia Incidence as Outcomes in Studies of Environmental Exposures
Most longitudinal studies of change in cognitive function report a decline in test scores over time and a more rapid decline with increasing age. Such studies have reported baseline differences with numerous risk factors, but far fewer risk factors have been firmly associated with longitudinal change in cognition. The English Longitudinal Study on Aging, an example of a well-executed longitudinal normative aging study, has suggested that such factors are age, gender, education, depression, alcohol, and physical exercise (99). While generally related to cognitive outcomes, these factors are not necessarily associated with environmental exposures, such that adjusting for them as potential confounders may not be necessary or appropriate. However, they may also be potential effect measure modifiers for environmental exposures. For a conceptual framework between environmental exposures and cognitive decline and dementia, see Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Ambient environmental exposures and cognition conceptual model. Proposed pathways between ambient environmental exposures and late-life cognitive decline: indirectly through cardiovascular disease (CVD) and health factors related to CVD, and directly through pathways such as systemic inflammation and neuroinflammation, beta amyloid (Aβ) and tau protein aggregation, or changes to structural white matter integrity. It should be noted, though, that the other pathways, such as systemic inflammation, may be related to cardiovascular health, as well.
Assessment of predictors of dementia incidence and cognitive change is most effective with longitudinal study designs that include high-quality, repeated measurements of both exposures and outcomes. Repeated measures of exposure allow investigators to observe time-dependent effects of environmental factors on cognitive decline. Most studies that attempt to link environmental exposures to cognitive change and dementia incidence to date, however, do not meet these criteria. This review highlights studies that are longitudinal with neuropsychological examination for cognitive outcomes, have adequate sample size, are population based, assess established cognitive risk factors for potential confounder control, and investigate incident dementia or change in cognitive function repeatedly (Table 1).
Table 1.
Prospective, population-based studies, with adequate sample size, investigating the association between air pollution and late-life cognitive outcomes
Study | Country | Study type | Exposure measurement | Outcome measurement | Cognitive domains | Sample size | Outcome size | Summary | Outcomes |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
AD and dementia incidence | |||||||||
Oudin et al.2016 (63); Betula Study | Sweden | Longitudinal 1993–2010 | Monitors and LUR; baseline year residential mean, NOx | AD and VaD: clinical examination | NA | 1,806 | 191 AD; 111 VaD | ↑ Risk | NOx quartile 4 versus 1: allcause dementia HR = 1.43 (1.00, 2.05); AD HR = 1.38 (0.87, 2.19); VaD HR = 1.47 (0.83, 2.61) |
Cacdottolo et al. 2017(12); WHIMS | United States | Longitudinal 1995–2010 | Spatiotemporal models and EPAAQS; PM2.5 | All-cause dementia: clinical examination | NA | 3,647 | 173 | ↑ Risk | High PM2.5 (versus low) HR = 1.92 (1.32, 2.80) |
Cognitive impairment | |||||||||
Loop et al. 2013 (54); REGARDS | United States | Longitudinal 2003–2007 | Satellite sensing and EPA monitors/spatiotemporal model; PM2.5 | Six-item telephone screener ≤ 4 | Short memory Orientation | 20,150 | 1,633 | Suggestive NS ↑ Risk | Per PM2.5 10 μg/m3, OR = 1.26 (0.97, 1.64) |
Cacdottolo et al. 2017(12); WHIMS | United States | Longitudinal 1995–2010 | Spatiotemporal models and AQS; PM2.5 | Eight-point 3MS decline | Global | 3,647 women | 329 | ↑ Risk | High PM2.5 (versus low) HR = 1.81 (1.42, 2.32) |
Cognitive decline | |||||||||
Weuve et al. 2012 (93); NHS Cognitive Cohort | United States | Longitudinal 1995–2008 | EPA monitors/spatiotemporal model; PM2.5 and PM2.5–10 | TICS and EBMT (telephone) | Global speed processing Verbal recall Memory | 19,409 women | NA | Faster decline | Cognitive score change, per 10 μg/m3: PM2.5 β=−0.018 (−0.035, −0.002); PM2.5–10 β = −0.020 (−0.032,−0.008) |
Tonne et al. 2014(85); Whitehall II | United Kingdom | Longitudinal 2002–2009 | London monitors/KCLurban dispersion model and postal code; PM2.5 and PM10 | Cognitive tests: cognitive tests in person | Memory score Reasoning Verbal fluency | 2,867 | NA | Suggestive NS faster decline | Five-year standardized memory test decline, per IQR: PM2.5 β=−0.030 (−0.062, 0.002); PM10 β=−0.029 (−0.065, 0.007) |
Oudin et al. 2017 (64); Betula Study | Sweden | Longitudinal 1988–2010 | Monitors and LUR; NOx | Cognitive tests: episodic memory summary score in person | Memory score (episodic, semantic, working, attention, speed) Executive function | 1,469 | NA | No associations | No association between five-year change on episodic memory measure and NOx β = 0.01 (−0.02, 0.03) |
Abbreviations: 3MS,Modified Mini-Mental State Exam; AD, Alzheimer’s disease; AQS, EPA Air Quality System; EBMT, East Boston Memory Test; EPA, US Environmental Protection Agency; HR, hazard ratio; IQR, interquartile range; LUR, land use regression; NA, not applicable/outcome is a continuous measure of cognitive decline; NHS, Nurses’ Health Study; NOx, nitrogen oxides; NS, nonsignificant; OR, odds ratio; PM, particulate matter; REGARDS, Reasons for Geographic And Racial Differences in Stroke; TICS, Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status; VaD, vascular dementia;WHIMS,Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study.
Ambient Air Pollution
Air pollution is a complex mixture of compounds from different sources, including combustion, industrial, agricultural, or natural sources, and includes particulate matter (PM), ozone, carbon monoxide (CO), sulfur and nitrogen oxides, methane, volatile organic compounds (VOCs; e.g., benzene, toluene, and xylene), and metals (e.g., lead, manganese, vanadium, iron) (10). Certain exposures are more typical for urban environments, such as traffic-related air pollution (TRAP), whereas in rural communities much of the contributions to air pollution may originate from diesel equipment or burning of biological matter. The components of air pollution have also changed over time; for instance, the lead content in TRAP decreased drastically after the banning and phasing out of leaded gasoline in the United States between the 1980s and 1995. We do not know whether a most relevant exposure period exists for late-life cognitive function, but long-term exposures extending over decades or even the entire life course likely affect these outcomes.
Air pollution is routinely monitored according to US Environmental Protection Agency (EPA) directions for pollutants referred to as criteria pollutants [CO, PM, SO2, nitrogen dioxide (NO2), lead] or as air toxics (such as benzene or other VOCs). More recently, researchers are developing complex spatiotemporal models that integrate such routine monitoring data with land use, emissions, and even satellite data and special research-driven monitoring protocols. Some pollutants are commonly thought of as surrogate markers for specific sources of air pollution. Common markers include particles of different sizes [aerodynamic diameter of <10 μm (PM10) or <2.5 μm (PM2.5)], carbon monoxide (CO), and nitric oxides (NO2, NOx), which have also been considered to represent TRAP in urban areas. This view may be oversimplified, however, as these pollutants often do not arise from a single source but from many. For example, PM10 or PM2.5 may include car and diesel emissions, as well as industry, power plant, and refinery emissions, depending on the region or area under study.
In recent years, several major epidemiologic studies have reported on associations between air pollutants and cognitive function. Table 1 outlines the findings from the studies deemed of highest quality. For completeness, a summary of other air pollution and cognition studies, mostly record-based or cross-sectional, is included in Supplemental Table 1, but we do not discuss these in depth.
Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias.
Two large cohorts, with long-term prospective follow-up and in-person clinical evaluations, have reported on air pollution and incident dementia. The Betula study in Northern Sweden enrolled and followed 1,806 participants for dementia incidence over a 15-year period, with 5 exams every 5 years between 1993 and 2014 (63). Over follow-up, 191 participants were diagnosed with AD and 111 with vascular dementia (VaD). The researchers modeled annual mean NOx levels at the residential addresses at baseline using a landuse regression model trained on 4 weeklong measurements at 36 sites (November 2009 through June 2010). Participants living in homes that received the highest quartile NOx exposure were at an increased risk of developing dementia relative to those in the lowest quartile [hazard ratio (HR) = 1.43; 95% confidence interval (CI): 1.00, 2.05] (Table 1). The analysis stratified by AD and VaD diagnosis yielded estimates of similar magnitude for both outcomes (AD HR = 1.38, 95% CI: 0.87, 2.19; VaD HR = 1.47, 95% CI: 0.83, 2.61). The researchers estimated the population attributable fraction of exposure to these dementias as 16% (95% CI: 4, 26) (63).
The risk of dementia from particulate air pollutants was examined in a US-wide cohort of 3,647 older, community-dwelling (>95% urban) women from the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study (WHIMS) (12). Ambient PM2.5 concentration at all WHIMS residential locations from 1999 to 2010 was estimated using spatiotemporal chemical transport models derived from monitoring data provided by the US EPA Air Quality System (AQS). The time-varying three-year average exposure preceding either diagnosis or censoring date was used in analysis. Over an average of 9.9 years of follow-up, 173 participants were classified as incident cases of all-cause dementia at annual exams. Residing in locations with high PM2.5, i.e., exceeding current National Ambient Air Quality Standards (NAAQS) standards, increased the risk for all-cause dementia by 92% (HR = 1.92, 95% CI:1.32, 2.80) (12). Similar to the results of the Betula study, this study estimated that ~21% of all-cause dementia documented by these researchers was attributable to residential exposure to high ambient PM2.5.
Large sample size, long follow-up with clinical exams, collection of in-depth covariate data, and fine-scaled spatiotemporal exposure modeling are notable strengths of these studies. However, NOx in the Betula study was measured (in 2009) 16 years after the first follow-up (in 1993), and baseline residential addresses were used for exposure assessment. While one could argue that relative levels of air pollution may be generally similar over a certain amount of time, population and traffic density or major roadway construction may have changed substantially, leading to some non-differential exposure misclassification. The WHIMS-constructed spatiotemporal chemical transport model exposure assessments may have established temporality more effectively in this cohort, but it allowed solely for the estimation of late-life exposure to PM2.5 from 1999 to 2010. Exposures before 1999, when WHIMS participants were aged less than 65 years, could not be modeled well because nationwide monitoring data prior to this time did not have adequate quality or sufficient geographic coverage.
Cognitive decline and incident impairment.
A small number of longitudinal studies with repeated cognitive exams have investigated the influence of air pollution using a variety of measures (Table 1). Two studies of incident cognitive impairment provide some support for an adverse association with PM2.5 exposure. A biracial US cohort study of 20,150 participants (REGARDS) measured incident cognitive impairment with a telephone six-item screener and integrated both EPA AQS monitoring data and satellite measurements of aerosol optical depth to estimate PM2.5 concentration at the participants’ baseline residence (2003–2007). A 10 μg/m3 increase in 1-year mean baseline PM2.5 concentration was associated with a 26% increase in risk [OR = 1.26 (0.97, 1.64)]. The risk was slightly stronger in participants living in urban areas [OR = 1.40 (1.06–1.85)], but the estimated effect sizes attenuated considerably after additional adjustments for demographic, behavioral, and health-related risk factors (54). In the same WHIMS cohort discussed above, high PM2.5 was also associated with an 81% [HR = 1.81 (1.42, 2.32)] increased risk for incident cognitive impairment [8-point loss on the Modified Mini-Mental State Exam (3MS); n = 329] (12).
Three additional cohort studies examined cognitive decline over a period of follow-up. The Nurses’ Health Study cognitive cohort (n = 19,409 older women) relied on three cognitive exams and assessed long-term PM exposure (7–14 years) at participants’ residences using spatiotemporal models and AQS monitoring. Exposures to PM2.5 and PM2.5–10 were associated with faster cognitive decline, as measured by a composite global score [per 10 μg/m3: PM2.5 β = −0.018 (−0.035, − 0.002); PM2.5–10 β = −0.020 (−0.032, −0.008)] (93). A study in the United Kingdom (Whitehall II, n = 2,867) reports suggestive associations between a five-year decline on a standardized memory test and PM exposure, modeled at the baseline residence using a dispersion modeling approach and emissions data, but found no associations with reasoning or verbal fluency decline (85). In contrast, the Betula cohort from Northern Sweden, which observed an increased risk of NOx exposure for incident dementia (discussed above) (63), did not see any association between NOx and five-year change in episodic memory (64).
These studies expand beyond the WHIMS and Betula cohorts and somewhat corroborate the results for air pollution and incident all-cause dementia and AD. However, all air pollution measures were relatively short term and represented only exposures during middle to older age.
Other studies.
Other published air pollution and cognition studies have a number of weaknesses (Supplemental Table 1). A series of four large, retrospective medical record–based studies in Taiwan and North America, using data from national air quality monitoring databases, all reported increased air pollution risks for dementia (19, 20, 44, 47). Medical record databases provide notable benefits, including large, representative, and often nationwide samples and the statistical power to detect weak to modest, but meaningful, effect sizes. However, dementia is not always documented well in medical records. For example, whether outpatient visits are recorded or dementia is diagnosed and recorded and at what stage varies widely by medical system. In addition, exposure assessment is generally limited to using the residential address or sometimes only the medical clinic address on file, limiting accurate or longer-term exposure assessment. Also, potential confounders beyond age, sex, and possibly some medical factors, including education, smoking, body mass index, and physical activity, are usually unavailable in record-based approaches. These should all be considered when researching cognitive outcomes, especially if socioeconomic status (SES) is related to air pollution in the areas under investigation.
Analyzing cognitive performance of a geographically diverse sample of individuals from the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center, researchers recently found that increased levels of ozone, but not PM2.5, were associated with an increased rate of cognitive decline, as measured by both the Mini–Mental State Examination (MMSE) and the Clinical Dementia Rating Scale Sum of Boxes (CDR-SB) (25). However, the population was selected from patients of a number of neurologic clinics and was not representative of the general population.
To date, most studies examining the ambient environment and study participants’ cognitive function were cross-sectional, based on prevalent cognitive function. Although most studies report at least one association between an air pollutant and a cognitive outcome, these findings are not necessarily seen in the primary analyses. For instance, positive associations may have been seen only in subanalyses, such as those restricted to urban populations or participants over 75 years of age. These studies are summarized in Supplemental Table 1.
Neuroimaging.
A series of studies with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans have associated elevated air pollution exposure with smaller brain volume (Table 2). Analyzing MRIs from 943 participants of the Framingham Offspring Study (FOS), investigators associated increasing PM2.5 exposure with lower cerebral brain volume, a marker of age-associated brain atrophy (96). Specifically, a 2 μg/m3 increase in PM2.5 exposure, modeled at the residential address with Moderate Resolution Imaging Spectroradiometer satellite-derived aerosol optical density measurements, was associated with smaller total cerebral brain volume [β = −0.32% (−0.59, −0.05)] and higher odds of covert brain infarcts [OR = 1.46 (1.10, 1.94)]. Living further away from a major roadway was also associated with greater log-transformed white matter hyperintensity (WMH) volume (96). However, in analyzing data from 236 participants in the Massachusetts Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center Longitudinal Cohort, researchers did not find any association between residential proximity to major roads or average PM2.5, modeled in the same manner with satellite data as done for the FOS, and greater burden of small vessel disease or neurodegeneration. However, a 2-μg/m3 increment in PM2.5 in this cohort was associated with a −0.19 (−0.37, −0.005) lower natural log-transformed WMH volume (95).
Table 2.
Studies investigating air pollution and brain structurea
Study | Country | Exposure measurement | Outcome measurement | Sample size | Summary | Outcomes |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Chen et al. 2015 (21); WHIMS | United States | Spatiotemporal models and EPA AQS; PM2.5 | MRI (volumetric GM and WM) | 1,403 women | ↓ WM | Per PM2.5 IQR: WM volume β= 6.23 cm3 smaller (3.72, 8.74) in total brain; β= 4.47 cm3 smaller (2.72, 6.67) in association areas |
Wilker et al. 2015(96); FOS | United States | Satellite sensing (PM2.5) and distance to major roadways | MRI (TCBV, HV, WMHV, and CBI) | 929 cognitively intact | ↓ TCBV, ↑ CBI Risk | Per 2-μg/m3 PM2.5 increase: TCBV β= −0.32 (−0.59, −0.05) and CBI risk OR = 1.46 (1.10, 1.94). Living further from roadways:WMHV βper IQR = 0.10 (0.01, 0.19) |
Wilker 2016 (95); MADRC | United States | Distance to major roadways; PM2.5 | MRI (BPF and WMHV) | 236 cognitively impaired | ↓ WMHV | No association between PM2.5 and BPF or microbleeds. Per 2-μg/m3 PM2.5 increase, lower log transformed WMHV β=−0.19 (−0.37, −0.005) |
Casanova 2016 (17); WHIMS | United States | Spatiotemporal models and EPA AQS; PM2.5 | MRI (volumetric GM and WM) | 1,365 women | ↓ WM and GM | Higher PM2.5 associated with smaller GM and WM volume |
Power 2018 (67); ARIC | United States | Spatiotemporal models and EPA AQS; PM2.5 and PM10 | MRI (volumetric GM and WMH) | 1,753 | ↓ Deep-gray brain volume | Higher mean PM2.5 and PM10 exposures were associated with smaller deep-gray brain volumes; higher PM2.5 exposures were associated with smaller total and regional brain volumes in Minnesota |
Abbreviations: AQS, EPA Air Quality System; ARIC, Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities; BPF, brain parenchymal fraction; CBI, covert brain infarcts; EPA, US Environmental Protection Agency; FOS, Framingham Offspring Study; GM, gray matter; HV, hippocampal volume; IQR, interquartile range; MADRC, Massachusetts Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center Longitudinal Cohort; MRI, magnetic resonance imaging; OR, odds ratio; TCBV, total cerebral brain volume; WHIMS,Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study;WM, white matter; WMHV, white matter hyperintensity volume.
Note, all studies are cross-sectional (brain structure based on a single exam).
Two studies from the WHIMS also reported on MRIs. Among 1,403 women without dementia at enrollment in the WHIMS, women who had greater PM2.5 exposures had smaller white matter volume [per interquartile range (IQR), β = 6.23 cm3 smaller (3.72, 8.74) in total brain and β = 4.47 cm3 smaller (2.72, 6.67) in association areas], but not gray matter volumes (21). A recent follow-up to this study, analyzing MRIs from 1,365 WHIMS women using voxel-based morphometry, showed that higher PM2.5 exposure was associated with smaller volumes in both cortical gray and subcortical white matter areas (17). And recent findings from the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study also suggest smaller brain volume with higher PM exposure (67). In the ARIC study, researchers modeled PM2.5 and PM10 exposures at participants’ residences over the 5–20 years prior to MRI using a spatiotemporal model and monitoring data (1990–2007). In a pooled analysis of four study sites, higher mean PM2.5 and PM10 exposures were associated with smaller deep-gray regional brain volumes [e.g., for mean PM2.5 from 1990 to 2007, β = −0.03 standard deviation (SD) units per 1 μg/m3 higher exposure (−0.08, 0.00)]. Higher PM2.5 exposures 5–14 years prior were marginally associated with smaller frontal lobe volumes [β = −0.02 SD units per 1–μg/m3 higher exposure (−0.04, 0.00)] (67).
These studies provide important evidence that air pollution influences not only old age brain function but also brain structure. Yet, they also rely on relatively short-term air pollution exposure estimates from later in life, and there are currently no studies with repeated measurements to assess change over time.
Ambient Noise
Vehicular traffic, an important source of urban air pollution, also emits much of the ambient noise pollution found in urban areas. Thus, owing to their shared sources, air pollution and ambient noise often occur simultaneously. The association between chronic exposure to ambient noise and cognitive function has hardly been investigated. Most studies of ambient noise examined short-term effects and psychological outcomes, such as anxiety and annoyance. There is a long history of experimental studies associating administered noise with worse neuropsychiatric performance, and a few occupational studies have implicated high-noise occupations with cognitive deficits (6, 24, 36, 87). Only the Heinz Nixdorf Recall study has published observational findings on the relationship between traffic-related ambient noise at each participant’s residence and cognitive performance. Among 4,086 community-dwelling participants in Germany, a 10-dB(A) (A-weighted decibels) elevation in traffic noise was associated with a lower global cognitive score (24-h noise measure β = −0.32, 95% CI: −0.62, −0.20) (86). Associations between ambient noise and global cognitive function were stronger in former and current smokers (β = −0.57, 95% CI: − 0.95, −0.19) compared with nonsmokers (β = −0.24, 95% CI: −0.55, −0.07; p for interaction = 0.04). These cross-sectional findings are intriguing, but more high-quality studies are needed.
Susceptibility
Genetic susceptibility.
Genetic risk factors represent a person’s endogenous characteristics that may change susceptibility to environmental exposures and cognitive decline or dementia. Reported associations to date are based largely on outcomes of global functioning. Currently, the most salient genetic risk factor for cognitive impairment and dementia is apolipoprotein E4 (APOE). This genotype is present in about 25% of European ancestry populations and has been shown to influence (or modify) the associations between various risk factors and cognitive functioning and dementia (11, 38–40, 57, 78). Approximately 20 genes have been identified as increasing or decreasing risk for AD (30). However, as a group, these genetic variants have only a small effect on dementia/AD risk, and they remain largely unexplored in terms of modifying the effects of environmental exposures.
The WHIMS longitudinal cohort is the only study that met our criteria for a high-quality investigation that also reported on the role of APOE. Adverse PM2.5 associations were exacerbated among the ε4 carriers for both incident all-cause dementia (ε3/3: HR = 1.68; ε3/4: HR = 1.91; ε4/4: HR = 2.95) and cognitive impairment (ε3/3: HR = 1.65; ε3/4: HR = 1.93; ε4/4: HR = 3.95) (12). These results are corroborated in a cross-sectional analysis of the SALIA (Study of the influence of Air pollution on Lung function, Inflammation and Aging) women’s cohort, where cognitive function was affected by measures of traffic load and PM2.5 absorbance only in those with at least one APOE ε4 risk allele (73).
More high-quality research is necessary to elucidate genetic susceptibility to ambient air pollution exposures. New understandings of environmental influences on DNA methylation and other epigenetic modes may also arise from future research and potentially even shed some light on the upstream environmental causes of cognitive impairment/dementia as well as elucidate transgenerational effects (23, 51, 65).
Social factors, life course, and social environment.
Race/ethnicity, SES, and neighborhood physical and socioeconomic features are reported to co-vary with air pollution and/or other environmental exposures (4, 34, 66). Although abundant evidence supports race/ethnicity and neighborhood differential vulnerability to a wide array of diseases, little work has assessed the aging brain’s vulnerability to toxins being enhanced by race, ethnicity, or low social status at the individual or neighborhood level.
The Americans’ Changing Lives study (n = 779) recently reported cross-sectional adverse associations between PM2.5 and cognitive function, and the estimated effect was stronger among adults living in what they considered high-stress neighborhoods [per 1 μg/m3 increase, interaction incidence-rate ratio (IRR) = 1.09 (1.01, 1.18)] (1). The authors suggest that those living in socioeconomically disadvantaged neighborhoods, where both social stressors and environmental hazards are more common, may be particularly susceptible to adverse health effects owing to both social and physical environmental exposures. Yet, this type of vulnerability has not been widely researched, let alone in a longitudinal design.
Important work has suggested that early childhood conditions, e.g., parental SES, food deprivation, stress in childhood, social and physical environment, may affect cognitive functioning in older ages (35, 55). In Mexico City, associations were reported between childhood brain pathology and air pollution (14), but long-term brain changes that start in childhood and extend to adulthood and older age have not yet been studied. The paucity of data obtained across the life course limits what one can say about upstream risk factors that may influence cognitive function and decline before adult middle age or older ages. There has been some scientific reluctance to studying exposure effects earlier in the life course owing to the belief that cognitive decline does not begin, or alternatively is not measurable, until late-middle to older age; i.e., detectable effects can be estimated only when a point is reached at which cognitive decline occurs more rapidly and can be measured with existing, often insensitive, tools. This position limits possibilities of identifying earlier upstream risk factors that could be useful for prevention. Any causal chain linking early-life exposures to late-life disease/function is inevitably long and influenced by a host of different factors. It may also be important to determine whether early life/midlife exposures to environmental toxicants contribute to cognitive decline later in life because this period constitutes sensitive or critical periods of brain development and maturation or because they simply represent a cumulative, long-term risk (26, 70, 82).
METHODOLOGICAL CHALLENGES
A critical first step in estimating effects of environmental exposures on cognitive decline and dementia incidence is to define the specific research question clearly and then to identify the appropriate statistical analysis to estimate the target parameter. This review is focused on whether environmental exposures have causal effects on cognitive decline and dementia incidence; thus, we focus on methodological challenges related to causal, rather than predictive, research questions. Major methodological challenges in research estimating the effects of environmental exposures on cognitive decline and dementia incidence include identifying the most relevant time of exposure for the outcome of interest; exposure assessment challenges, especially exposure measurement error due to spatial misalignment of monitoring data and study participants; challenges in measuring and modeling cognitive function and cognitive change; unmeasured confounding; and selection bias arising from selection into the sample and selective attrition. We briefly describe these challenges below but refer readers to more in-depth discussions elsewhere. On challenges in measuring cognitive outcomes, we refer the readers to a more detailed review of measurement, including uncertainty in diagnostic criteria and measurement error in neuropsychological assessments (92); on measuring air pollution and noise exposure, we refer readers to a review of confounding and exposure measurement error in air pollution epidemiology (77) and to work on measurement error methods corrections (72).
Timing of Exposure
Neuropathological challenges thought to be relevant to cognitive decline and dementia may begin 20 or more years prior to the onset of clinical symptoms (2). Thus, exposures in early life and midlife are probably quite relevant for the development of neuronal damage related to cognitive decline and dementia. Current evidence on environmental exposures and cognitive decline and dementia incidence measures environmental exposures in late life. Nevertheless, information about the residential mobility among older adults in the study population of interest could provide insights into the extent to which residential environmental exposures measured in late life may represent exposure in midlife.
Exposure Measurement
One of the main challenges in estimating air pollution exposure from ambient stations is the spatial misalignment of monitoring data and study participants. Monitoring data are generally not available at the subject locations, and one must assume that a subject’s ambient exposures are well represented by a monitoring station. Approaches to spatial prediction of pollutants vary across citywide or census-tract averages, to nearest monitor interpolation, to kriging, and land-use regression (LUR) (77). For pollutants with relatively homogeneous spatial structures (e.g., PM2.5 or ozone), less refined spatial models may provide adequate estimates of exposure at residences or workplaces. However, for pollutants with a shorter range from the source (e.g., NOx), city averages, nearest monitor, and even kriging will likely result in substantial exposure misclassification (46). LURs are designed to predict such spatially varying concentrations from sources such as traffic. Most methods integrate regulatory monitoring data. However, the number of fixed location monitors may be limited and may be preferentially sited close to or far from pollutant sources, leading to bias (31). Satellite-based chemical transport models of exposure are currently being developed to help alleviate this issue. Finally, there are trade-offs between using measures of ambient pollution exposure and using personal air pollution exposure measures. The latter remains very expensive to collect in terms of costs and subject burden, limiting the feasibility of measurement over longer periods of time. Ambient exposure measures have advantages in that they can serve as instrumental variables for personal exposures and thus are not impacted by confounders in the same manner that actual personal exposure measures might be (see 91).
Outcome Measurement
Major challenges in outcome measurement include validity and reliability of measuring cognitive function, cognitive change, and incident dementia. Distinguishing between true cognitive change and measurement error is critical for estimating the effects of exposures on cognitive decline. Cognitive measures with unequal interval scaling, including ceilings and floors, can either attenuate or inflate effect estimates in longitudinal studies (59, 68). Challenges in modeling cognitive decline to minimize biased estimates of the effect of exposures on the rate of cognitive decline include accounting for practice effects (90) and for nonlinear cognitive trajectories.
Confounding
Compared with studies examining the rate of cognitive decline, studies examining incident dementia are more susceptible to confounding by life course exposures that may influence environmental exposures and premorbid cognitive function, for example, childhood socioeconomic disadvantage or education. Thus, high-quality studies examining the rate of cognitive decline provide stronger evidence for causality of exposures. However, studies of cognitive decline are not immune to confounding. Environmental conditions correlated with the exposure of interest are an important potential source of confounding. For example, estimated daytime and nighttime noise tend to be higher for census block groups with lower SES in the United States (18).
Selection Bias
Selection bias arising from selection into the sample and selective attrition potentially induces spurious exposure–outcome associations if selection is a common effect of exposure and an unmeasured determinant of cognitive decline (and related causal structures) (43, 94). This selective loss to follow-up and survival is relevant particularly for air pollution studies because environmental exposures are linked with mortality, cognitive decline predicts mortality among older adults, and mortality rates are high in older adult populations (48, 49, 83, 97, 98). Selection into study participation among people who are alive is another potential source of selection bias (45, 50).
CONCLUSION
Research linking ambient air pollution and noise with cognitive decline and dementia is slowly accumulating. However, the current evidence is far from definitive, especially for chronic noise exposures. It is increasingly recognized that a life course perspective is likely relevant to identifying important determinants of cognitive decline that accompany aging. Thus, the evidence that environmental exposures play a role will be substantially strengthened by studies that measure exposures to environmental factors in early life and midlife and that record cognitive decline in a standardized and repeated fashion. Cheaper and noninvasive neuroimaging or blood-based biomarkers of neurodegeneration may be important future contributors to such research. Current longitudinal research is stymied both by the lack of address history prior to enrollment in many existing cohort studies as well as by a lack of dense enough air pollution monitoring networks before 1990. Continued follow-up of current adult cohorts into the coming decades for the assessment of late-life health, including cognition, will provide new opportunities for longer-term concomitant exposure assessment. Large, population-representative studies would help us gain a more complete picture of the potential link between environmental exposures and cognitive aging in diverse populations and settings, i.e., will help elucidate the burden of environmental factors for vulnerable aging populations in high-risk or high-pollution contexts. Reducing environmental pollution will require collective action and long-term policies at the national and local levels. Given the magnitude of exposure, there is great potential for prevention.
Supplementary Material
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This work was supported by the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences (F32ES028087, R01ES023451, R01ES10544, R21ES022391) and the National Institute on Aging (R01AG12975, R00AG053410).
DISCLOSURE STATEMENT
The authors are not aware of any affiliations, memberships, funding, or financial holdings that might be perceived as affecting the objectivity of this review.
Glossary
- Particulate matter (PM)
delineated by <2.5 μm (fine), 2.5–10 μm (coarse), and <10 μm
- CO
Carbon monoxide
- NO2
nitrogen dioxide
- NOx
nitrogen oxides
LITERATURE CITED
- 1.Ailshire J, Karraker A, Clarke P. 2017. Neighborhood social stressors, fine particulate matter air pollution, and cognitive function among older U.S. adults. Soc. Sci. Med 172:56–63 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Alzheimer’s Assoc. 2017. Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimer’s Dement. 13:325–73 [Google Scholar]
- 3.Alzheimer’s Assoc. 2018. Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimer’s Dement. 14(3):367–429 [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ard K 2015. Trends in exposure to industrial air toxins for different racial and socioeconomic groups: a spatial and temporal examination of environmental inequality in the U.S. from 1995 to 2004. Soc. Sci. Res 53:375–90 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Barnes D, Yaffe K. 2013. The projected effect of risk factor reduction on Alzheimer’s disease prevalence. Lancet Neurol. 10(9):819–28 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Basner M, Babisch W, Davis A, Brink M, Clark C, et al. 2014. Auditory and non-auditory effects of noise on health. Lancet 383:1325–32 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Baumgart M, Snyder HM, Carrillo MC, Fazio S, Kim H, Johns H. 2015. Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: a population-based perspective. Alzheimer’s Dement. 11(6):718–26 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Béjot Y, Reis J, Giroud M, Feigin V. 2018. A review of epidemiological research on stroke and dementia and exposure to air pollution. Int. J. Stroke 10.1177/1747493018772800 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Block ML, Calderón-Garcidueñas L. 2009. Air pollution: mechanisms of neuroinflammation and CNS disease. Trends Neurosci. 32:506–16 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Block ML, Elder A, Auten RL, Bilbo SD, Chen H, et al. 2012. The outdoor air pollution and brain health workshop. Neurotoxicology 33:972–84 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Blue EE, Bis JC, Dorschner MO, Tsuang DW, Barral SM, et al. 2018. Genetic variation in genes underlying diverse dementias may explain a small proportion of cases in the Alzheimer’s Disease Sequencing Project. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord 45:1–17 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Cacciottolo M, Wang X, Driscoll I, Woodward N, Saffari A, et al. 2017. Particulate air pollutants, APOE alleles and their contributions to cognitive impairment in older women and to amyloidogenesis in experimental models. Transl. Psychiatry 7(1):e1022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Calderón-Garcidueñas L, Kavanaugh M, Block M, D’Angiulli A, Delgado-Chávez R, et al. 2012. Neuroinflammation, hyperphosphorylated tau, diffuse amyloid plaques, and down-regulation of the cellular prion protein in air pollution exposed children and young adults. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 28(1):93–107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Calderón-Garcidueñas L, Reynoso-Robles R, Vargas-Martínez J, Gómez-Maqueo-Chew A, Pérez-Guillé B, et al. 2016. Prefrontal white matter pathology in air pollution exposed Mexico City young urbanites and their potential impact on neurovascular unit dysfunction and the development of Alzheimer’s disease. Environ. Res 146:404–17 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Calderón-Garcidueñas L, Vincent R, Mora-Tiscareño A, Franco-Lira M, Henríquez-Roldán C, et al. 2007. Elevated plasma endothelin-1 and pulmonary arterial pressure in children exposed to air pollution. Environ. Health Perspect 115(8):1248–53 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Campbell A, Oldham M, Becaria A, Bondy SC, Meacher D, et al. 2005. Particulate matter in polluted air may increase biomarkers of inflammation in mouse brain. Neurotoxicology 26(1):133–40 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Casanova R, Wang X, Reyes J, Akita Y, Serre ML, et al. 2016. A voxel-based morphometry study reveals local brain structural alterations associated with ambient fine particles in older women. Front. Hum. Neurosci 10:495. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Casey JA, Morello-Frosch R, Mennitt DJ, Fristrup K, Ogburn EL, James P. 2017. Race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, residential segregation, and spatial variation in noise exposure in the contiguous United States. Environ. Health Perspect 125(7):077017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Chang K-H, Chang M-Y, Muo C-H, Wu T-N, Chen C-Y, Kao C-H. 2014. Increased risk of dementia in patients exposed to nitrogen dioxide and carbon monoxide: a population-based retrospective cohort study. PLOS ONE 9(8):e103078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Chen H, Kwong JC, Copes R, Tu K, Villeneuve PJ, et al. 2017. Living near major roads and the incidence of dementia, Parkinson’s disease, and multiple sclerosis: a population-based cohort study. Lancet 389:718–26 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Chen J-C, Wang X, Wellenius GA, Serre ML, Driscoll I, et al. 2015. Ambient air pollution and neurotoxicity on brain structure: evidence from Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study. Ann. Neurol 78(3):466–76 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Cheng L, Wang SH, Chen QC, Liao XM. 2011. Moderate noise induced cognition impairment of mice and its underlying mechanisms. Physiol. Behav 104(5):981–88 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Chi GC, Liu Y, MacDonald JW, Barr RG, Donohue KM, et al. 2016. Long-term outdoor air pollution and DNA methylation in circulating monocytes: results from the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (MESA). Environ. Health 5(1):119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Chiovenda P, Pasqualetti P, Zappasodi F, Ercolani M, Milazzo D, et al. 2007. Environmental noiseexposed workers: event-related potentials, neuropsychological and mood assessment. Int. J. Psychophysiol 65(3):228–37 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Cleary EG, Cifuentes M, Grinstein G, Brugge D, Shea TB. 2018. Association of low-level ozone with cognitive decline in older adults. J. Alzheimer’s Dis 61:67–78 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Clifford A, Lang L, Chen R, Anstey KJ, Seaton A. 2016. Exposure to air pollution and cognitive functioning across the life course—a systematic literature review. Environ. Res 147:383–98 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Cui B, Wu M, She X. 2009. Effects of chronic noise exposure on spatial learning and memory of rats in relation to neurotransmitters and NMDAR2B alteration in the hippocampus. J. Occup. Health 51(2):152–58 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Cui B, Wu M, She X, Liu H. 2012. Impulse noise exposure in rats causes cognitive deficits and changes in hippocampal neurotransmitter signaling and tau phosphorylation. Brain Res. 1427:35–43 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Cui B, Zhu L, She X, Wu M, Ma Q, et al. 2012. Chronic noise exposure causes persistence of tau hyperphosphorylation and formation of NFT tau in the rat hippocampus and prefrontal cortex. Exp. Neurol 238(2):122–29 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.DeKosky ST, Gandy S. 2014. Environmental exposures and the risk for Alzheimer disease: Can we identify the smoking guns? JAMA Neurol. 71:273–75 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Diggle PJ, Menezes R, Su T-L. 2010. Geostatistical inference under preferential sampling. J. R. Stat. Soc. C Appl. Stat 59(2):191–232 [Google Scholar]
- 32.Dzhambov AM, Dimitrova DD. 2016. Exposure-response relationship between traffic noise and the risk of stroke: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Arch. Ind. Hyg. Toxicol 67(2):136–51 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Elder A, Gelein R, Silva V, Feikert T, Opanashuk L, et al. 2006. Translocation of inhaled ultrafine manganese oxide particles to the central nervous system. Environ. Health Perspect 114(8):1172–78 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Fecht D, Fischer P, Fortunato L, Hoek G, de Hoogh K, et al. 2015. Associations between air pollution and socioeconomic characteristics, ethnicity and age profile of neighbourhoods in England and the Netherlands. Environ. Pollut 198:201–10 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Fors S, Lennartsson C, Lundberg O. 2009. Childhood living conditions, socioeconomic position in adulthood, and cognition in later life: exploring the associations. J. Gerontol. - B 64(6):750–57 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Gomes LM, Martinho Pimenta AJ, Castelo Branco NA. 1999. Effects of occupational exposure to low frequency noise on cognition. Aviat. Space. Environ. Med 70(3 Pt. 2):A115–18 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Gottesman RF, Schneider ALC, Zhou Y, Coresh J, Green E, et al. 2017. Association between midlife vascular risk factors and estimated brain amyloid deposition. JAMA 317(14):1443–50 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Haan M, Espeland MA, Klein BE, Casanova R, Gaussoin SA, et al. 2012. Cognitive function and retinal and ischemic brain changes: The Women’s Health Initiative. Neurology 78(13):942–49 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Haan MN, Miller JW, Aiello AE, Whitmer RA, Jagust WJ, et al. 2007. Homocysteine, B vitamins, and the incidence of dementia and cognitive impairment: results from the Sacramento Area Latino Study on Aging. Am. J. Clin. Nutr 85(2):511–17 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Haan MN, Shemanski L, Jagust WJ, Manolio TA, Kuller L. 1999. The role of APOE ∈4 in modulating effects of other risk factors for cognitive decline in elderly persons. JAMA 282(1):40–46 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Hartz AMS, Bauer B, Block ML, Hong J-S, Miller DS. 2008. Diesel exhaust particles induce oxidative stress, proinflammatory signaling, and P-glycoprotein up-regulation at the blood-brain barrier. FASEB J. 22(8):2723–33 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Hawkes CH, Del Tredici K, Braak H. 2009. Parkinson’s disease: the dual hit theory revisited. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci 1170:615–22 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Hernán MA, Hernández-Diaz S, Robins JM. 2004. A structural approach to selection bias. Epidemiology 15(5):615–25 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Jung C-R, Lin Y-T, Hwang B-F. 2015. Ozone, particulate matter, and newly diagnosed Alzheimer’s disease: a population-based cohort study in Taiwan. J. Alzheimers Dis 44(2):573–84 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Keyes KM, Rutherford C, Popham F, Martins SS, Gray L. 2017. How healthy are survey respondents compared with the general population? Using survey-linked death records to compare mortality outcomes. Epidemiology 29:299–307 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Kim SY, Sheppard L, Kim H. 2009. Health effects of long-term air pollution: influence of exposure prediction methods. Epidemiology 20(3):442–50 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kioumourtzoglou M-A, Schwartz JD, Weisskopf MG, Melly SJ, Wang Y, et al. 2016. Long-term PM2.5 exposure and neurological hospital admissions in the northeastern United States. Environ. Health Perspect 124(1):23–29 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Lavery LL, Dodge HH, Snitz B, Ganguli M. 2009. Cognitive decline and mortality in a community-based cohort: the Monongahela Valley Independent Elders Survey. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc 57(1):94–100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Lelieveld J, Evans JS, Fnais M, Giannadaki D, Pozzer A. 2015. The contribution of outdoor air pollution sources to premature mortality on a global scale. Nature 525:367–71 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Lewinn KZ, Sheridan MA, Keyes KM, Hamilton A, McLaughlin KA. 2017. Sample composition alters associations between age and brain structure. Nat. Commun 8(1):874. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Lichtenfels AJDFC, Van Der Plaat DA, De Jong K, Van Diemen CC, Postma DS, et al. 2018. Long-term air pollution exposure, genome-wide DNA methylation and lung function in the Lifelines Cohort Study. Environ. Health Perspect 126(2):027004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Lim SS, Vos T, Flaxman AD, Danaei G, Shibuya K, et al. 2012. A comparative risk assessment of burden of disease and injury attributable to 67 risk factors and risk factor clusters in 21 regions, 1990–2010: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet 380:2224–60 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V, Costafreda SG, Huntley J, et al. 2017. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet 390:10113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Loop MS, Kent ST, Al-Hamdan MZ, Crosson WL, Estes SM, et al. 2013. Fine particulate matter and incident cognitive impairment in the REasons for Geographic and Racial Differences in Stroke (REGARDS) cohort. PLOS ONE 8(9):e75001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Luo Y, Waite LJ. 2005. The impact of childhood and adult SES on physical, mental, and cognitive wellbeing in later life. J. Gerontol. B 60(2):S93–101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Manikandan S, Padma MK, Srikumar R, Jeya Parthasarathy N, Muthuvel A, Sheela Devi R. 2006. Effects of chronic noise stress on spatial memory of rats in relation to neuronal dendritic alteration and free radical-imbalance in hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex. Neurosci. Lett 399(1–2):17–22 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Matura S, Prvulovic D, Hartmann D, Scheibe M, Sepanski B, et al. 2016. Age-related effects of the apolipoprotein E gene on brain function. J. Alzheimers Dis 52:317–31 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Mills NL, Donaldson K, Hadoke PW, Boon NA, MacNee W, et al. 2009. Adverse cardiovascular effects of air pollution. Nat. Clin. Pract. Cardiovasc. Med 6(1):36–44 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Morris MC, Evans DA, Hebert LE, Bienias JL. 1999. Methodological issues in the study of cognitive decline. Am. J. Epidemiol 149(9):789–93 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Münzel T, Gori T, Babisch W, Basner M. 2014. Cardiovascular effects of environmental noise exposure. Eur. Heart J 35(13):829–36 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Ndrepepa A, Twardella D. 2011. Relationship between noise annoyance from road traffic noise and cardiovascular diseases: a meta-analysis. Noise Health 13(52):251–59 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Oberdörster G, Sharp Z, Atudorei V, Elder A, Gelein R, et al. 2004. Translocation of inhaled ultrafine particles to the brain. Inhal. Toxicol 16(6–7):437–45 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Oudin A, Forsberg B, Adolfsson AN, Lind N, Modig L, et al. 2016. Traffic-related air pollution and dementia incidence in Northern Sweden: a longitudinal study. Environ. Health Perspect 124(3):306–12 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Oudin A, Forsberg B, Lind N, Nordin S, Åström DO, et al. 2017. Is long-term exposure to air pollution associated with episodic memory? A longitudinal study from Northern Sweden. Sci. Rep 7(1):12789. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Paul KC, Chuang YH, Cockburn M, Bronstein JM, Horvath S, Ritz B. 2018. Organophosphate pesticide exposure and differential genome-wide DNA methylation. Sci. Total Environ 645:1135–43 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Pinault L, Crouse D, Jerrett M, Brauer M, Tjepkema M. 2016. Spatial associations between socioeconomic groups and NO2 air pollution exposure within three large Canadian cities. Environ. Res 147(2):373–82 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Power MC, Lamichhane AP, Liao D, Xu X, Jack CR, et al. 2018. The association of long-term exposure to particulate matter air pollution with brain MRI findings: The ARIC study. Environ. Health Perspect 126(2):027009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Proust-Lima C, Dartigues J-F, Jacqmin-Gadda H. 2011. Misuse of the linear mixed model when evaluating risk factors of cognitive decline. Am. J. Epidemiol 174(9):1077–88 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Rozemuller AJM, Jansen C, Carrano A, Van Haastert ES, Hondius D, et al. 2012. Neuroinflammation and common mechanism in Alzheimer’s disease and prion amyloidosis: amyloid-associated proteins, neuroinflammation and neurofibrillary degeneration. Neurodegener. Dis 10(1–4):301–4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Saenen ND, Plusquin M, Bijnens E, Janssen BG, Gyselaers W, et al. 2015. In utero fine particle air pollution and placental expression of genes in the brain-derived neurotrophic factor signaling pathway: an ENVIRONAGE birth cohort study. Environ. Health Perspect 123(8):834–40 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Saito S, Ihara M. 2016. Interaction between cerebrovascular disease and Alzheimer pathology. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 29:168–73 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Samoli E, Butland BK. 2017. Incorporating measurement error from modeled air pollution exposures into epidemiological analyses. Curr. Environ. Health Rep 4:472–80 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Schikowski T, Vossoughi M, Vierkötter A, Schulte T, Teichert T, et al. 2015. Association of air pollution with cognitive functions and its modification by APOE gene variants in elderly women. Environ. Res 142:10–16 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Schneider JA, Arvanitakis Z, Bang W, Bennett DA. 2007. Mixed brain pathologies account for most dementia cases in community-dwelling older persons. Neurology 69(24):2197–204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Schneider JA, Arvanitakis Z, Leurgans SE, Bennett DA. 2009. The neuropathology of probable Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. Ann. Neurol 66(2):200–8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Schwartz J, Litonjua A, Suh H, Verrier M, Zanobetti A, et al. 2005. Traffic related pollution and heart rate variability in a panel of elderly subjects. Thorax 60(6):455–61 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Sheppard L, Burnett RT, Szpiro AA, Kim SY, Jerrett M, et al. 2012. Confounding and exposure measurement error in air pollution epidemiology. Air Qual. Atmos. Health 5:203–16 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Shih I-F, Paul K, Haan M, Yu Y, Ritz B. 2017. Physical activity modifies the influence of apolipoprotein E ε4 allele and type 2 diabetes on dementia and cognitive impairment among older Mexican Americans. Alzheimer’s Dement. 14:1–9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Stieb DM, Chen L, Eshoul M, Judek S. 2012. Ambient air pollution, birth weight and preterm birth: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Environ. Res 117:100–11 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Sun GH, Raji CA, MacEachern MP, Burke JF. 2012. Olfactory identification testing as a predictor of the development of Alzheimer’s dementia: a systematic review. Laryngoscope 122:1455–62 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Sun Q, Wang A, Jin X, Natanzon A, Duquaine D, et al. 2005. Long-term air pollution exposure and acceleration of atherosclerosis and vascular inflammation in an animal model. JAMA 294(23):3003–10 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Szilagyi M, Halfon N. 2015. Pediatric adverse childhood experiences: implications for life course health trajectories. Acad. Pediatr 15(5):467–68 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Thorvaldsson V, Hofer SM, Berg S, Skoog I, Sacuiu S, Johansson B. 2008. Onset of terminal decline in cognitive abilities in individuals without dementia. Neurology 71(12):882–87 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Toledo JB, Arnold SE, Raible K, Brettschneider J, Xie SX, et al. 2013. Contribution of cerebrovascular disease in autopsy confirmed neurodegenerative disease cases in the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Centre. Brain 136(9):2697–706 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Tonne C, Elbaz A, Beevers S, Singh-Manoux A. 2014. Traffic-related air pollution in relation to cognitive function in older adults. Epidemiology 25(5):674–81 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Tzivian L, Dlugaj M, Winkler A, Hennig F, Fuks K, et al. 2016. Long-term air pollution and traffic noise exposures and cognitive function: a cross-sectional analysis of the Heinz Nixdorf Recall study. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health - Part A Curr. Issues 79(22–23):1057–69 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Tzivian L, Winkler A, Dlugaj M, Schikowski T, Vossoughi M, et al. 2015. Effect of long-term outdoor air pollution and noise on cognitive and psychological functions in adults. Int. J. Hyg. Environ. Health 218:1–11 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.U. N. (United Nations). 2002. Report of the Second World Assembly on Ageing: Madrid, 8–12 April 2002. Rep. A/CONF.197/9, United Nations, New York [Google Scholar]
- 89.Van Berlo D, Albrecht C, Knaapen AM, Cassee FR, Gerlofs-Nijland ME, et al. 2010. Comparative evaluation of the effects of short-term inhalation exposure to diesel engine exhaust on rat lung and brain. Arch. Toxicol 84(7):553–62 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Vivot A, Power MC, Glymour MM, Mayeda ER, Benitez A, et al. 2016. Jump, hop, or skip: modeling practice effects in studies of determinants of cognitive change in older adults. Am. J. Epidemiol 183(4):302–14 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Weisskopf MG, Webster TF. 2017. Trade-offs of personal versus more proxy exposure measures in environmental epidemiology. Epidemiology 28(5):635–43 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Weuve J, Proust-Lima C, Power MC, Gross AL, Hofer SM, et al. 2015. Guidelines for reporting methodological challenges and evaluating potential bias in dementia research. Alzheimer’s Dement. 11(9):1098–109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Weuve J, Puett RC, Schwartz J, Yanosky JD, Laden F, Grodstein F. 2012. Exposure to particulate air pollution and cognitive decline in older women. Arch. Intern. Med 172(3):219–27 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Weuve J, Tchetgen Tchetgen EJ, Glymour MM, Beck TL, Aggarwal NT, et al. 2012. Accounting for bias due to selective attrition: the example of smoking and cognitive decline. Epidemiology 23(1):119–28 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Wilker EH, Martinez-Ramirez S, Kloog I, Schwartz J, Mostofsky E, et al. 2016. Fine particulate matter, residential proximity to major roads, and markers of small vessel disease in a memory study population. J. Alzheimer’s Dis 53(4):1315–23 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Wilker EH, Preis SR, Beiser AS, Wolf PA, Au R, et al. 2015. Long-term exposure to fine particulate matter, residential proximity to major roads and measures of brain structure. Stroke 46:1161–66 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Wilson RS, Beckett LA, Bienias JL, Evans DA, Bennett DA. 2003. Terminal decline in cognitive function. Neurology 60(11):1782–87 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Yaffe K, Lindquist K, Vittinghoff E, Barnes D, Simonsick EM, et al. 2010. The effect of maintaining cognition on risk of disability and death. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc 58(5):889–94 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Zaninotto P, Batty GD, Allerhand M, Deary IJ. 2018. Cognitive function trajectories and their determinants in older people: 8 years of follow-up in the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 10.1136/jech-2017-210116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.