Abstract
Other people can profoundly affect our opinions and decisions. In the current study, we compared the effects of peer influence on responses to a primary reward – food – in both young adolescents and adults. Food is critical for survival, and in addition to its rewarding properties, habits and practices surrounding eating are heavily influenced by social and cultural norms. To address the impact of peer influence on food valuations, young adolescents ages 10–14 and young adults ages 18–22 rated the desirability of foods before and after seeing peer opinions about those foods. We then compared the degree to which participants changed their ratings of food desirability as a function of the type of social information received (e.g. peers liking a food more or less than the participant). We found that all participants’ ratings conformed to the peer ratings, and that adolescents had less stable valuations across all conditions over time. These results provide evidence for the effectiveness of peer influence in shifting valuations of appetitive stimuli, and can inform interventions aimed at improving healthy eating choices.
Keywords: conformity, adolescence, reward, social influence, food
For better or for worse, the opinions of other people shape our beliefs and actions. This phenomenon – known as social influence – has been heavily documented in social and evolutionary psychology, behavioral economics, cultural anthropology, sociology, and more recently, social neuroscience. Studies have shown, for example, that social influence, including knowledge of social norms, can change a variety of behaviors, ranging from basic perceptual judgments (Asch, 1952) to more complex attitudes and moral beliefs (Borsari & Carey, 2003). In addition to changing individuals at the behavioral level, a growing body of work has demonstrated how social influence facilitates robust changes at the neural level as well in regions and networks associated with conflict monitoring, valuation, and reinforcement learning (Campbell-Meiklejohn & Frith, 2012; Cascio, Scholz, & Falk, 2015; Izuma, 2017; Klucharev, Hytönen, Rijpkema, Smidts, & Fernández, 2009; Welborn et al., 2015; Zaki, Schirmer, & Mitchell, 2011). Importantly, social influence can have important consequences for our physical health. Indeed, work in applied psychology has shown that social norms can influence a wide variety of health-related behaviors from decreasing heavy drinking, smoking and drug use to increasing safe sex practices (Hansen & Graham, 1991; Perkins, Linkenbach, Lewis, & Neighbors, 2010; Sheeran & Taylor, 1999).
Some work in developmental psychology suggests that social influence may be especially salient during adolescence, a time period that has been characterized by increased sensitivity to social information (Blakemore & Mills, 2014; Chein, Albert, O’Brien, Uckert, & Steinberg, 2011; Nelson, Leibenluft, McClure, & Pine, 2005). Adolescents are believed to be more likely to take risky actions as a result of social influence, particularly by their peers (Gardner & Steinberg, 2005), including smoking, drinking, drug use, unprotected sex, and reckless driving (Chein et al., 2011; David, Catalano, & Miller, 1992; Krosnick & Judd, 1982; Mizuno, Seals, Kennedy, & Myllyluoma, 2000; Steinberg, 2011). This heightened propensity to engage in risky behaviors in conjunction with social influence has also been observed in animal models. For instance, adolescent rats are more likely to consume alcohol and nicotine in the presence of peers relative to adults (Logue, Chein, Gould, Holliday, & Steinberg, 2014; Thiel, Sanabria, & Neisewander, 2009).
While it is known that social influence may lead teens to make riskier choices, one area that has been less explored – yet is a significant public health concern – is how knowledge of others’ attitudes toward food may shape our own eating choices. At its most basic level, food is a primary reward that is critical for survival (Schultz, 2000); and habits and practices surrounding eating and food choice are heavily mediated by social and cultural norms (Robinson, Thomas, Aveyard, & Higgs, 2014). As with other reward-inducing behaviors, eating is prone to abuse and dysregulation. One third of children and adolescents and an even larger proportion of adults are overweight or obese, which carries heavy health costs (Lobstein et al., 2015; Ogden, Carroll, Kit, & Flegal, 2012). Obesity can contribute to heart disease – the leading cause of death in the United States - as well as other leading causes of death including diabetes and certain types of cancers (National Institutes of Health, 1998).
Despite these facts, little work has asked how peer influence impacts responses to primary rewards such as food during adolescence and even fewer studies have directly compared adolescents with adults. While some survey-based and field research is consistent with the idea that social factors, including one’s community and social network contribute to obesity (de la Haye, Robins, Mohr, & Wilson, 2010; Valente, Fujimoto, Chou, & Spruijt-Metz, 2009), to our knowledge, no experimental studies have directly examined the effects of peer influence on adolescents’ food preferences in comparison with adults.
Here, we used a variant of an established paradigm (Klucharev, Hytönen, Rijpkema, Smidts, & Fernández, 2009) to ask how social norms influence valuation of a variety of palatable foods in young adolescents and young adults. Two questions were of primary interest. The first was whether young adults and adolescents differ in their baseline food preferences. Establishing baselines for food preference as a function of age is a necessary precursor to addressing the second question, which was whether and how social influence can shift an individual’s food valuations. This is because differences in the average level of desire for foods – or the amount of variability or instability of these desires – could potentially mask age-related differences in social influence. Turning to the second question, emerging work suggests that social influence can impact food preferences in adults (Croker, Whitaker, Cooke, & Wardle, 2009; Nook & Zaki, 2015), although little is known about adolescent susceptibility to such influence. As such, our aim was to assess how social influence shapes responses to primary appetitive food cues in adolescents, and compare how their responses might compare to those of adults. Specifically, in light of work showing increases in risky decision-making and reward seeking during adolescence with peer influence, we were interested in whether adolescents would also be more sensitive than adults to peer influence over non-risky reward-based decisions. Finally, a third exploratory question of secondary interest was whether there would be individual difference factors, such as gender or body mass index (BMI), that would cross-cut answers to the first two questions such that some individuals would show greater or lesser susceptibility to social-influence.
Method
Participants
We tested 94 participants, 47 young adults ages 18–22 (23 female, M=20.98, SD=1.58) and 47 young adolescents ages 10–14 (24 female, M=12.38, SD=1.40) recruited from the New York City metropolitan area. These two age groups were carefully chosen based on previous work suggesting that conformity behaviors may peak in the late childhood to early adolescent period and taper off linearly from later adolescence into adulthood (Costanzo & Shaw, 1966; Steinberg & Monahan, 2007). By choosing an early adolescent cohort, we sought to capture a developmental period where individuals are believed to be most susceptible to peer influence and contrast it with an adult comparison group with whom the effects of peer influence may be more stabilized. On the basis of a prior study in adults using the paradigm we used in the current study (Zaki et al., 2011) we expected that a sample size of 47 participants per age group would achieve approximately 98% power to detect a social influence effect of comparable size (d = 0.62) assuming an α of .05. The Columbia University Institutional Review Board approved the study. All participants gave informed consent. Participants were screened to exclude for psychiatric, developmental, and eating disorders prior to participating in the experiment. Five additional participants were excluded: three for not following the instructions correctly, and two because of task interruptions related to computer malfunctions.
Stimuli
Food stimuli were collected and normed from prior studies on food craving (Kober et al., 2010; Silvers et al., 2014) as well as from public online sources. Care was taken to ensure that the images were selected to be palatable, to depict a variety of foods, both savory and sweet, and to span the spectrum of energy density and healthiness (e.g. from fruit and salads to desserts and fried foods).
Procedure
Social Influence Task
Participants were told they were taking part in a study on food preference and that a sample of approximately 100 people in their respective age group had rated a set of food pictures. Participants were then told that they would rate their preferences for the same foods, and in most cases would be shown the average rating made by the participants who had already completed the study.
For each trial, participants viewed a food image and then were asked to rate their food preference using a 7-point scale (1 = not at all, 7 = a lot). The participant’s rating was highlighted by a green square. Participants then saw the average group rating for that food, highlighted by a red square. They also saw a number indicating the difference between their rating and the group rating (See Figure 1).
Group ratings were generated by a pseudorandom adaptive algorithm that assigned each trial to one of four peer response conditions based on initial ratings: Peers Want More, Peers Want Less, Peers Agree, and the control condition in which no feedback was given (“No Feedback”). In each participant, approximately 25% of the trials were assigned to each peer condition. In the Peers Want More and Peers Want Less trials, peer responses could either be one or two points above or below participant ratings. Trials were run in 15 item blocks where within each block feedback based on the participant’s response would include approximately 3 No Feedback responses, 4 Agree responses, 4 responses 1 point above or below participants’ response and 4 responses 2 points above or below participant’ response. To counteract potential floor and ceiling effects (e.g. individuals who consistently gave 1 ratings and thus had fewer Peers Want Less responses), we chose a multilevel model analysis approach in order to best capture both within and between subject variance, and we also included initial ratings as a covariate in all of our regression analyses. After rating the complete set of images, participants took a thirty-minute break. They then re-rated the same images a second time, this time without seeing peer ratings. These procedures are modeled after prior studies using similar methods of social influence on preferences for other types of stimuli in adults (Klucharev et al., 2009; Zaki et al., 2011).
Individual Difference Measures
We collected a battery of assessments on a subset of the sample measuring potential factors we thought might affect participants’ responses to peer influence. The sample subset consisted of participants who were able to stay for an extended testing session following the experiment. Additionally, we added questionnaires to our study after the first cohort of adults was tested (N=25), and therefore our adult sample is reduced for some measures (see supplementary materials for Ns).
These measures covered three main domains: general individual differences, social behaviors, and health behaviors. When adolescent-specific scales were available they were used on our young adolescent sample and z-scores were used in age group comparisons. General individual difference measures included gender, age, IQ (Canivez, Konold, Collins, & Wilson, 2009), pubertal status (Tanner & Whitehouse, 1976), and socioeconomic status (McLoyd, 1998), as well as standard assessments of mental health including depression (Beck, Steer, & Carbin, 1988; Helsel & Matson, 1984) and anxiety (Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970; Spielberger & Edwards, 1973). Social measures included resistance to peer influence (Steinberg & Monahan, 2007), rejection sensitivity (Downey & Feldman, 1996; McLachlan, Zimmer-Gembeck, & McGregor, 2010), need to belong (Baumeister & Leary, 1995), and social desirability (Crandall, Crandall, & Katkovsky, 1965; Crowne & Marlowe, 1960; Reynolds, 1982). Food-related measures included hunger levels at time of test and time participants last ate, as well as body mass index (BMI; percentiles for young adolescents Kuczmarski et al., 2002, BMI for young adults, categorical weight status for group comparisons; see supplement for more details), disordered eating using the SCOFF eating disorder screening test (Morgan, Reid, & Lacey, 1999; Walsh, Wheat, & Freund, 2000), body image with the Body Esteem Scale (Mendelson, Mendelson, & White, 2001), media influence (Cusumano & Thompson, 2001), body weight and healthy eating subscales from the Youth Risk Behavior Survey (Eaton et al., 2012) which included questions about self-described weight, dieting status, and healthy eating. See supplement for further description of measures.
Analysis
We used the R statistical software language (R Core Team, 2014), and in particular, its lme4 (Bates, Maechler, Bolker, & Walker, 2014) and lmerTest (Kuznetsova, Brockhoff, & Christensen, 2013) packages, to model the effects of peer influence on food preference within age groups. Using lme4, we estimated multilevel models that allowed for subject-specific random intercepts and peer influence slopes. To account for potential effects of regression to the mean, we used initial ratings as a covariate in our models testing social influence (Yu & Chen, 2015). Additionally, we conducted secondary exploratory analyses (described in supplement) to test individual differences that might impact one’s likelihood of being socially influenced (e.g. gender, hunger level, question type, age, body weight, and dieting status).
Results
Age-related Differences in Baseline Food Preferences
Young adolescent participants showed different patterns than young adults in their initial food preferences, the distribution of their ratings, and the stability of those preferences across phases.
Average Valuations for Foods
Young adolescents and young adults demonstrated different baseline patterns of food valuations (Figure 2A). Age group was a significant predictor of initial ratings, with young adolescents reporting lower levels of craving as compared to young adults, b = −.60, 95% CI[−.91, −.28], p = .003 (mean initial ratings: young adolescents 4.28(1.02), young adults 4.87(.85)).
Distribution of Food Valuations
We found that the distribution of food valuations differed between age groups: two-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, D= .13, p =2.2 X 10−16 (Figure 2B). Additionally, young adolescents rated a higher proportion of foods on the negative end of the scale compared to young adults (percentage of negative ratings: young adolescents 39%, young adults 25%), t(92) = 3.53, p = .0006, d = .73). Conversely, young adults showed a stronger positive skew and rated a larger proportion of foods on the positive end of the scale (percent positive ratings: young adolescents 50%, young adults 62%), t(92) = 2.88, p = .005, d = .59.
Stability of Baseline Food Valuations
In assessing the overall stability of valuations over time, we measured how age group predicted absolute change in ratings. Using the absolute value measurement of the change score allowed us to see how much individuals changed their ratings, regardless of sign. We found young adolescents demonstrated more volatility in general, and changed their ratings to a greater degree than young adults across all conditions including the No Feedback and Peers Agree conditions (Figs. 3A, 3B), b = .11, 95% CI [.00, .21], p =.03.
Social Influence on Food Valuations
Overall, we found a conformity effect in both age groups such that when peers preferred foods more or less, participants changed their ratings in the direction of the peer ratings (Fig. 4), young adolescents: b = .14, 95% CI [.08, .19], p =.0001, young adults: b = .16, 95% CI [.12, .20], p = 8.04 X 10−14. We did not find a significant interaction with age group suggesting that neither group was more likely to conform than the other, b = −.009, 95% CI [−.06, .08], p =.80.
Interaction with Individual Difference Factors
Individual difference factors including age and gender did not strongly account for differences in susceptibility to social influence between subjects. First, a full regression model including all measured individual differences (i.e. gender, age, depression, anxiety, resistance to peer influence, rejection sensitivity, social desirability, hunger level, time participants last ate, body mass index, body image and healthy and disordered eating habits) as covariates did not reduce the degree to which social influence changed ratings, b = .16, 95% CI [.10, .22], p = 2.42 X 10−06. Second, we looked at each individual difference measure separately and assessed how they related to one’s likelihood of conforming during the task. At an alpha level of .05, we found that for young adolescents, higher anxiety correlated with greater conformity, r(64) = .30, p = .01; however, no results from this exploratory analysis survived Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (Table 1; see supplement for more summary statistics and plots).
Table 1.
Teens | Adults | Full Sample | |
---|---|---|---|
General | |||
Age | -.01 | .02 | .03 |
Gender | .09 | .03 | .06 |
IQ | -.13 | ---- | ---- |
Pubertal Status | .07 | ---- | ---- |
Parent Education Level | -.13 | -.03 | -.07 |
Depression | .15 | .09 | .13 |
Anxiety (State-Trait Combined) | .32* | .26 | .30* |
Social Cognition | |||
Resistance to Peer Influence | -.19 | .14 | -.05 |
Need to Belong | .06 | -.11 | -.01 |
Rejection Sensitivity | .18 | .36† | .22† |
Social Desirability | -.06 | .13 | -.01 |
Eating Behavior | |||
Hunger Level at time of test | -.15 | -.02 | -.10 |
Last Eating Time prior to test | .04 | -.27† | -.12 |
Body Mass Index | -.01 | -.02 | -.00 |
Disordered Eating | .12 | -.09 | .02 |
Body Image | -.17 | ---- | ---- |
Media Influence | .12 | -.35 | .03 |
Self-Described Weight | -.05 | -.24 | -.08 |
Dieting Status | .29† | -.18 | .20 |
Healthy Eating | -.20 | .26 | -.10 |
Note:
p < .10.
p < .05. (2-tailed.).
Discussion
The study began with the question of how social influence shapes appetitive valuations in young adolescents and young adults. To address it, both groups of participants first expressed their ratings for a set of food stimuli, received feedback on normative peer ratings for them, and later re-valued their own ratings. Three key findings were obtained. First, analysis of the initial baseline (i.e. pre-influence), ratings demonstrated that young adolescents had stronger negative initial opinions about more foods. Young adults had more positive initial ratings of more foods as demonstrated by their left-skewed distribution of ratings toward the positive end of the scale. Second, adolescents changed their food ratings more across all conditions, irrespective of whether they were socially influenced. Third, comparing pre-influence ratings to post-influence ratings showed that exposure to group norms changed reported food valuations in both age groups to a similar extent. Finally, we found that social influence effects were robust to individual differences including gender and other variables related to social processing and health behaviors. These data have significant implications for basic research on adolescent appetitive reactivity and social behavior as well as for translational research on improving health behaviors.
First, with respect to age-related differences in baseline food valuation, there could be at least three factors at play. First, young adults may have wanted to eat more foods by virtue of the fact that they may have had more exposure to the foods over time because of their age (Ventura & Worobey, 2013). Future work could assess how familiarity and experience with food interacts with one’s ability to be influenced by others’ food valuations across different developmental time points. Second, young adolescents may have more sensitive palates and may generally prefer the tastes of fewer foods (Birch, 1999). Finally, differences in initial ratings may be a more general phenomenon of younger individuals having a negative response bias compared to adults as a function of their cognitive maturity. This theory, however, has been tested mainly in children and may not generalizable to young adolescents (Chambers & Johnston, 2002; Marsh, 1986).
Second, with respect to the finding that young adolescents were more likely to change their ratings across all conditions including when peers agreed and when no feedback was given, the fact that this effect was not specific to the social influence conditions suggests young adolescents’ food valuations show more volatility and less stability than adult food valuations, even if they are not more susceptible to peer influence. While young adolescents started out with more extreme ratings in both the positive and negative direction, their rate of change was also greater than that of the young adults. This instability has been described in research on adolescent valuations more generally (Campbell, 1961) and also specifically with food habits (Nu, MacLeod, & Barthelemy, 1996; von Post-Skagegård et al., 2002). As noted above, this could be because young adolescents have had less experience with the foods presented, and thus have less stable opinions about the foods compared to adults. This begs the question of whether future studies could further tease apart the effects of social influence and food valuation stability by examining social influence across different types of stimuli – including some that are equivalently or even more familiar to adolescents than adults, such as social media – or across a broader age range, spanning continuously from children to older adults.
Third, with respect to susceptibility to social influence, we found that the effects of social influence on appetitive valuations were robust in both young adolescents and young adults across a wide range of food types both healthy and unhealthy. This finding replicates prior work in adults using similar experimental manipulations (Klucharev et al., 2009; Nook & Zaki, 2015; Zaki et al., 2011) and extends such work by demonstrating that young adolescent populations are also similarly affected by such influence. This finding also supports a host of studies that have found an effect of social influence on food choices (Neumark-Sztainer, Story, Perry, & Casey, 1999; Robinson, Blissett, & Higgs, 2013). However, our study is unique in that rather than being survey or focus group based, we assessed real-time behavior in the lab, and while most studies examined one age group or the other, we directly compared young adolescents to young adults.
Notably, while both young adolescents and young adults showed robust social influence effects, it could be argued that this study diverges from the theory that adolescence may be a period of increased sensitivity to social influence (Blakemore & Mills, 2014). One reason that adolescents did not show larger social influence effects in this task compared to adults may be because of the social manipulation used. Most social influence studies on adolescents include the presence or the belief of a presence of one to five same sex peers (Chein et al., 2011; Jones et al., 2014; Masten et al., 2009; Silva, Shulman, Chein, & Steinberg, 2016). Our task used a majority influence, norms-based manipulation where participants believed that about 100 peers in the same age group had previously rated the food images. Indeed, while there are many studies on majority influence effects in adults, studies with adolescents have been mixed (Berndt, 1979; Costanzo & Shaw, 1966; Walker & Andrade, 1996) and developmental patterns of social conformity effects from adolescence to adulthood have yet to be discovered. Two studies assessing the effects of social influence on judgments of risky behaviors found that young adolescents were more susceptible to peer influence compared to older adolescents and adults (Knoll, Magis-Weinberg, Speekenbrink, & Blakemore, 2015; Knoll, Leung, Foulkes, & Blakemore, 2017). These differing results from ours and others (Lourenco et al., 2015; Rosenblau, Korn, & Pelphrey, 2018) may be due to the stimulus type used – risk judgments. Food is a stimulus all participants have had experience with on a day-today basis whereas risk assessments may be more novel and less familiar. While future studies could test further boundary conditions of the adolescent social sensitivity hypothesis, our findings suggest that appetitive reactivity to food cues can be shifted by peers to a similar degree in both adolescents and adults. Furthermore, future work could measure developmental differences in the degree to which adolescents are swayed by few versus many influencers.
Fourth, with respect to individual differences in susceptibility to social influence, we found that social influence effects were robust even when accounting for numerous factors. This finding is in concert with a recent meta-analysis on a similar social construct - social modeling of food consumption - which in addition to not finding interactions with age and social modeling, also found that evidence of interactions with factors such as sex, hunger, weight, and eating goals were limited to inconclusive (Cruwys, Bevelander, & Hermans, 2015).
Finally, the present study has implications for translational and more applied work aimed at improving health behaviors. Indeed, it suggests that a social norms-based approach may be an effective strategy in improving eating choices. This norms-based approach was successful in a study aimed at reducing bullying in middle schools (Perkins, Craig, & Perkins, 2011), and drinking and driving in college-aged adults (Perkins et al., 2010). Another study aimed at seventh graders designed to prevent drinking, marijuana and tobacco use found that a social norms approach was the most effective in reducing onset time and prevalence of use after a one year follow-up (Hansen & Graham, 1991). Employing social influence to improve eating choices could be a strong addition to health interventions such as Michelle Obama’s “Let’s Move” campaign, the Coordinated Approach to Child Health program, and others.
Several limitations in this study are important to address. First, our study was designed to measure social influence effects starting at an age when social influence susceptibility is believed to be high (age 10), decrease linearly, and then asymptote around early adulthood (Steinberg & Monahan, 2007). As such, we excluded the middle adolescent period, which could have revealed more insights into the change trajectory of the volatility we observed in young adolescents’ food preferences. We also acknowledge that our young adult group is still undergoing neural maturation processes and transitional stages in social interactions and environments (i.e. most of these adults were still in college). An open question is whether a difference in social influence may be seen between young adolescents and older adults whose social lives and environments have experienced more stability over a longer period of time. Second, to make the task feasible in our lab environment, we assessed appetitive reactivity using food pictures and self-report, which may yield different or weaker results compared to measurements involving the presence of real food and subsequent food consumption (e.g. see Giuliani, Merchant, Cosme, & Berkman, 2018). Third, the first cohort of adults we tested did not complete the full battery of individual difference measures and as a result we may have been underpowered to detect group-level differences in relationships between these measures and social influence.
This study investigated how social influence shapes reactivity to appetitive food cues in early adolescence and adulthood. Beyond the answers this laboratory study can provide, future studies in an intervention context could address how social influence impacts actual food consumption, and how the addition of repeated exposure of specific types of social influence may impact individuals over time. Overeating and obesity is a major public health concern. These results highlight the effect other people have on one’s eating choices and underscore the potential impact of harnessing the power of social influence to improve eating decisions and habits.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
Funding Sources: This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health R01 HD069178 and R01 AG043463 to K. Ochsner, and F31 NIMH 107119 to R. Martin.
The authors wish to thank Jochen Weber and Jamil Zaki for help with task design, Suneet Goraya and Athena Bochanis for help with data collection, Niall Bolger, Greg Jensen, and Jared P. Lander for advice on statistical analyses, and Daphna Shohamy, Nim Tottenham, Jennifer Silvers, Katherine Duncan, and members of the SCAN and Learning Labs at Columbia University for helpful discussions throughout the project.
Footnotes
This work was presented at the Society for Research on Adolescence Biennial Meeting, 2014 and the Flux Congress Annual Meeting, 2014. Analysis code for this article is available at https://github.com/beckmart/SocialInfluenceBehavioralStudy. Data is available by request.
References
- Asch SE. Groups, leadership and men; research in human relations. Oxford, England: Carnegie Press; 1952. Effects of group pressure upon the modification and distortion of judgments; pp. 177–190. [Google Scholar]
- Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. 2014 Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.
- Baumeister RF, Leary MR. The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;117(3):497–529. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT, Steer RA, Carbin MG. Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review. 1988;8(1):77–100. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Berndt TJ. Developmental changes in conformity to peers and parents. Developmental Psychology. 1979;15(6):608–616. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.15.6.608. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge KC, Robinson TE. Parsing reward. Trends in Neurosciences. 2003;26(9):507–513. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00233-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birch LL. Development of Food Preferences. Annual Review of Nutrition. 1999;19(1):41–62. doi: 10.1146/annurev.nutr.19.1.41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blakemore S-J, Mills KL. Is Adolescence a Sensitive Period for Sociocultural Processing? Annual Review of Psychology. 2014;65(1) doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borsari B, Carey KB. Descriptive and Injunctive Norms in College Drinking: A Meta-Analytic Integration. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2003;64(3):331–341. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2003.64.331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell-Meiklejohn D, Frith CD. Chapter 8 - Social Factors and Preference Change. In: Dolan R, Sharot T, editors. Neuroscience of Preference and Choice. San Diego: Academic Press; 2012. pp. 177–206. Retrieved from http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780123814319000085. [Google Scholar]
- Campbell DT. Conformity in Psychology’s Theories of Acquired Behavioral Dispositions. In: Berg IA, Bass BM, editors. Conformity and deviation. New York, NY, US: Harper and Brothers; 1961. pp. 101–142. [Google Scholar]
- Canivez GL, Konold TR, Collins JM, Wilson G. Construct validity of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence and Wide Range Intelligence Test: Convergent and structural validity. School Psychology Quarterly. 2009;24(4):252–265. doi: 10.1037/a0018030. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cascio CN, Scholz C, Falk EB. Social influence and the brain: persuasion, susceptibility to influence and retransmission. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2015;3:51–57. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.01.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Chambers CT, Johnston C. Developmental Differences in Children’s Use of Rating Scales. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2002;27(1):27–36. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/27.1.27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chein J, Albert D, O’Brien L, Uckert K, Steinberg L. Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain’s reward circuitry. Developmental Science. 2011;14(2):F1–10. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01035.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costanzo PR, Shaw ME. Conformity as a Function of Age Level. Child Development. 1966;37(4):967–975. doi: 10.2307/1126618. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Crandall VC, Crandall VJ, Katkovsky W. A Children’s Social Desirability Questionnaire. Journal of Consulting Psychology. 1965;29:27–36. doi: 10.1037/h0020966. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Croker H, Whitaker KL, Cooke L, Wardle J. Do social norms affect intended food choice? Preventive Medicine. 2009;49(2–3):190–193. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2009.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crowne DP, Marlowe D. A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology. 1960;24(4):349–354. doi: 10.1037/h0047358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cruwys T, Bevelander KE, Hermans RCJ. Social modeling of eating: A review of when and why social influence affects food intake and choice. Appetite. 2015;86:3–18. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.08.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cusumano DL, Thompson JK. Media influence and body image in 8–11-year-old boys and girls: A preliminary report on the multidimensional media influence scale. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2001;29(1):37–44. doi: 10.1002/1098-108X(200101)29:1<37::AID-EAT6>3.0.CO;2-G. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- David J, Catalano RF, Miller JY. Risk and protective factors for alcohol and other drug problems in adolescence and early adulthood: Implications for substance abuse prevention. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;112(1):64–105. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Haye K, Robins G, Mohr P, Wilson C. Obesity-related behaviors in adolescent friendship networks. Social Networks. 2010;32(3):161–167. doi: 10.1016/j.socnet.2009.09.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Downey G, Feldman SI. Implications of rejection sensitivity for intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1996;70(6):1327–1343. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Downey G, Lebolt A, Rincón C, Freitas AL. Rejection sensitivity and children’s interpersonal difficulties. Child Development. 1998:1074–1091. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eaton DK, Kann L, Kinchen S, Shanklin S, Flint KH, Hawkins J … Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) Youth risk behavior surveillance - United States, 2011. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. Surveillance Summaries (Washington, D.C.: 2002) 2012;61(4):1–162. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finlayson G, King N, Blundell JE. Liking vs. wanting food: Importance for human appetite control and weight regulation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2007;31(7):987–1002. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gardner M, Steinberg L. Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky decision making in adolescence and adulthood: an experimental study. Developmental Psychology. 2005;41(4):625–635. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.4.625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giuliani N, Merchant JS, Cosme D, Berkman E. Neural predictors of eating behavior and dietary change. PsyArXiv. 2018 doi: 10.17605/OSF.IO/7T9XD. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Hansen WB, Graham JW. Preventing alcohol, marijuana, and cigarette use among adolescents: Peer pressure resistance training versus establishing conservative norms. Preventive Medicine. 1991;20(3):414–430. doi: 10.1016/0091-7435(91)90039-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helsel WJ, Matson JL. The assessment of depression in children: The internal structure of the child depression inventory (CDI) Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1984;22(3):289–298. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(84)90009-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izuma K. Decision Neuroscience. San Diego: Academic Press; 2017. Chapter 16 - The Neural Bases of Social Influence on Valuation and Behavior; pp. 199–209. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Jones RM, Somerville LH, Li J, Ruberry EJ, Powers A, Mehta N, … Casey BJ. Adolescent-specific patterns of behavior and neural activity during social reinforcement learning. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014;14(2):683–697. doi: 10.3758/s13415-014-0257-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klucharev V, Hytönen K, Rijpkema M, Smidts A, Fernández G. Reinforcement Learning Signal Predicts Social Conformity. Neuron. 2009;61(1):140–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.11.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knoll LJ, Leung JT, Foulkes L, Blakemore SJ. Age-related differences in social influence on risk perception depend on the direction of influence. Journal of Adolescence. 2017;60:53–63. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2017.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knoll LJ, Magis-Weinberg L, Speekenbrink M, Blakemore S-J. Social Influence on Risk Perception During Adolescence. Psychological Science. 2015 doi: 10.1177/0956797615569578. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Kober H, Mende-Siedlecki P, Kross EF, Weber J, Mischel W, Hart CL, Ochsner KN. Prefrontal-striatal pathway underlies cognitive regulation of craving. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2010;107(33):14811–14816. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1007779107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krosnick JA, Judd CM. Transitions in social influence at adolescence: Who induces cigarette smoking? Developmental Psychology. 1982;18(3):359–368. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.18.3.359. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Guo SS, Grummer-Strawn LM, Flegal KM, Mei Z, … Johnson CL. 2000 CDC Growth Charts for the United States: methods and development. Vital and Health Statistics. Series 11, Data from the National Health Survey. 2002;(246):1–190. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuznetsova A, Brockhoff PB, Christensen RHB. R Package Version, 2–0. 2013. lmerTest: Tests for random and fixed effects for linear mixed effect models (lmer objects of lme4 package) [Google Scholar]
- Leary MR, Kelly KM, Cottrell CA, Schreindorfer LS. Construct Validity of the Need to Belong Scale: Mapping the Nomological Network. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2013;95(6):610–624. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2013.819511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lobstein T, Jackson-Leach R, Moodie ML, Hall KD, Gortmaker SL, Swinburn BA, … McPherson K. Child and adolescent obesity: part of a bigger picture. The Lancet. 2015;385(9986):2510–2520. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)61746-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Logue S, Chein J, Gould T, Holliday E, Steinberg L. Adolescent mice, unlike adults, consume more alcohol in the presence of peers than alone. Developmental Science. 2014;17(1):79–85. doi: 10.1111/desc.12101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lourenco FS, Decker JH, Pedersen GA, Dellarco DV, Casey BJ, Hartley CA. Consider the Source: Adolescents and Adults Similarly Follow Older Adult Advice More than Peer Advice. PLOS ONE. 2015;10(6):e0128047. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0128047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marsh HW. Negative item bias in ratings scales for preadolescent children: A cognitive-developmental phenomenon. Developmental Psychology. 1986;22(1):37–49. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.22.1.37. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Masten CL, Eisenberger NI, Borofsky LA, Pfeifer JH, McNealy K, Mazziotta JC, Dapretto M. Neural correlates of social exclusion during adolescence: understanding the distress of peer rejection. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2009;4(2):143–157. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLachlan J, Zimmer-Gembeck MJ, McGregor L. Rejection Sensitivity in Childhood and Early Adolescence: Peer Rejection and Protective Effects of Parents and Friends. Journal of Relationships Research. 2010;1(01):31–40. doi: 10.1375/jrr.1.1.31. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McLoyd VC. Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American Psychologist. 1998;53(2):185–204. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.53.2.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mendelson BK, Mendelson MJ, White DR. Body-esteem scale for adolescents and adults. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2001;76(1):90–106. doi: 10.1207/S15327752JPA7601_6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mizuno Y, Seals B, Kennedy M, Myllyluoma J. Predictors of Teens’ Attitudes Toward Condoms: Gender Differences in the Effects of Norms. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2000;30(7):1381–1395. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2000.tb02526.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan JF, Reid F, Lacey JH. The SCOFF questionnaire: assessment of a new screening tool for eating disorders. BMJ. 1999;319(7223):1467–1468. doi: 10.1136/bmj.319.7223.1467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- National Institutes of Health. Clinical Guidelines on the Identification, Evaluation, and Treatment of Overweight and Obesity in Adults. Obes Res. 1998 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson EE, Leibenluft E, McClure EB, Pine DS. The social reorientation of adolescence: a neuroscience perspective on the process and its relation to psychopathology. Psychological Medicine. 2005;35(02):163–174. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704003915. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neumark-Sztainer D, Story M, Perry C, Casey MA. Factors Influencing Food Choices of Adolescents: Findings from Focus-Group Discussions with Adolescents. Journal of the American Dietetic Association. 1999;99(8):929–937. doi: 10.1016/S0002-8223(99)00222-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nook EC, Zaki J. Social Norms Shift Behavioral and Neural Responses to Foods. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2015;27(7):1412–1426. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00795. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nu CT, MacLeod P, Barthelemy J. Effects of age and gender on adolescents’ food habits and preferences. Food Quality and Preference. 1996;7(3–4):251–262. doi: 10.1016/S0950-3293(96)00023-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Kit BK, Flegal KM. Prevalence of obesity and trends in body mass index among us children and adolescents, 1999–2010. JAMA. 2012;307(5):483–490. doi: 10.1001/jama.2012.40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perkins HW, Craig DW, Perkins JM. Using social norms to reduce bullying: A research intervention among adolescents in five middle schools. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 2011 doi: 10.1177/1368430210398004. 1368430210398004. [DOI]
- Perkins HW, Linkenbach JW, Lewis MA, Neighbors C. Effectiveness of social norms media marketing in reducing drinking and driving: A statewide campaign. Addictive Behaviors. 2010;35(10):866–874. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2014. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org. [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds WM. Development of reliable and valid short forms of the marlowe-crowne social desirability scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1982;38(1):119–125. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198201)38:1<119::AID-JCLP2270380118>3.0.CO;2-I. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson E, Blissett J, Higgs S. Social influences on eating: implications for nutritional interventions. Nutrition Research Reviews. 2013;26(02):166–176. doi: 10.1017/S0954422413000127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson E, Thomas J, Aveyard P, Higgs S. What Everyone Else Is Eating: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of the Effect of Informational Eating Norms on Eating Behavior. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics. 2014;114(3):414–429. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2013.11.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenblau G, Korn CW, Pelphrey KA. A Computational Account of Optimizing Social Predictions Reveals That Adolescents Are Conservative Learners in Social Contexts. Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;38(4):974–988. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1044-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W. Multiple reward signals in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2000;1(3):199–207. doi: 10.1038/35044563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheeran P, Taylor S. Predicting Intentions to Use Condoms: A Meta- Analysis and Comparison of the Theories of Reasoned Action and Planned Behavior1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1999;29(8):1624–1675. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1999.tb02045.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Silva K, Shulman EP, Chein J, Steinberg L. Peers Increase Late Adolescents’ Exploratory Behavior and Sensitivity to Positive and Negative Feedback. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2016;26(4):696–705. doi: 10.1111/jora.12219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silvers JA, Insel C, Powers A, Franz P, Weber J, Mischel W, … Ochsner KN. Curbing Craving Behavioral and Brain Evidence That Children Regulate Craving When Instructed to Do So but Have Higher Baseline Craving Than Adults. Psychological Science. 2014 doi: 10.1177/0956797614546001. 0956797614546001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Silvers JA, McRae K, Gabrieli JDE, Gross JJ, Remy KA, Ochsner KN. Emotion. Washington, D.C: 2012. Age-Related Differences in Emotional Reactivity, Regulation, and Rejection Sensitivity in Adolescence. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger CD, Edwards CD. STAIC preliminary manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children (“ How I feel questionnaire”) Consulting Psychologists Press; 1973. [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. 1970 Retrieved from http://ubir.buffalo.edu/xmlui/handle/10477/2895.
- Steinberg L. Adolescents’ Risky Driving in Context. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2011;49(6):557–558. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg L, Monahan KC. Age differences in resistance to peer influence. Developmental Psychology. 2007;43(6):1531–1543. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sumter SR, Bokhorst CL, Steinberg L, Westenberg PM. The developmental pattern of resistance to peer influence in adolescence: Will the teenager ever be able to resist? Journal of Adolescence. 2009;32(4):1009–1021. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanner JM, Whitehouse RH. Clinical longitudinal standards for height, weight, height velocity, weight velocity, and stages of puberty. Archives of Disease in Childhood. 1976;51(3):170–179. doi: 10.1136/adc.51.3.170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thiel KJ, Sanabria F, Neisewander JL. Synergistic interaction between nicotine and social rewards in adolescent male rats. Psychopharmacology. 2009;204(3):391–402. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1470-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valente TW, Fujimoto K, Chou CP, Spruijt-Metz D. Adolescent Affiliations and Adiposity: A Social Network Analysis of Friendships and Obesity. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2009;45(2):202–204. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ventura AK, Worobey J. Early influences on the development of food preferences. Current Biology: CB. 2013;23(9):R401–408. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.02.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Post-Skagegård M, Samuelson G, Karlström B, Mohsen R, Berglund L, Bratteby LE. Changes in food habits in healthy Swedish adolescents during the transition from adolescence to adulthood. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2002;56(6):532–538. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1601345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walker MB, Andrade MG. Conformity in the Asch Task as a Function of Age. The Journal of Social Psychology. 1996;136(3):367–372. doi: 10.1080/00224545.1996.9714014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walsh JM, Wheat ME, Freund K. Detection, evaluation, and treatment of eating disorders. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2000;15(8):577–590. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2000.02439.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Welborn BL, Lieberman MD, Goldenberg D, Fuligni AJ, Galvan A, Telzer EH. Neural Mechanisms of Social Influence in Adolescence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2015:nsv095. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Yu R, Chen L. The need to control for regression to the mean in social psychology studies. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement. 2015;5:1574. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01574. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zaki J, Schirmer J, Mitchell JP. Social influence modulates the neural computation of value. Psychological Science. 2011;22(7):894–900. doi: 10.1177/0956797611411057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.