Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Jun 3.
Published in final edited form as: Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2012 Jul 7;18(10):1073–1078. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2012.06.004

Health comorbidities and cognition in 1948 patients with idiopathic Parkinson’s disease

JD Jones a,c, I Malaty b,c, CC Price a,c, MS Okun b,c, D Bowers a,b,c,*
PMCID: PMC6545886  NIHMSID: NIHMS1023866  PMID: 22776043

Abstract

Background:

Health comorbidities, particularly cardiovascular factors, are well known to pose risks for cognitive decline in older adults. This study examined the prevalence and contribution of comorbidities on cognitive performance in a large cohort of Parkinson patients.

Methods:

Data on 1948 PD patients were obtained from the National Parkinson Foundation Quality Improvement Initiative (NPF-QII) registry, a multi-site initiative from NPF Centers of Excellence. Available comorbidity data included six common conditions (heart/circulation problems, diabetes, arthritis, cancer, respiratory disease, and other neurologic disease) that were clinician-rated for presence and severity. Available cognitive measures included semantic fluency and a 5-word recall memory task. The unique effects of comorbidities on cognition were analyzed (multiple hierarchical regression) controlling for demographic, PD disease severity (duration, Hoehn-Yahr), and medication status.

Results:

The two most reported comorbidities were arthritis (46.6%) and heart/circulation problems (36.3%), with diabetes affecting 9% of the sample. Severity of heart/circulation problems independently contributed to worse delayed recall performance (p = 0.03). A trend emerged for more severe diabetes as contributing to worse semantic fluency scores (p = 0.06).

Conclusions:

This study with a large cohort of PD patients provides evidence for a small detrimental influence of specific health comorbidities, particularly heart/circulatory and diabetes, on general measures of cognition. This effect is present, above and beyond the influences of basic demographic information (age), duration and staging of PD, and medication status. Future studies involving more refined cognitive indices and direct assessment of comorbidities are warranted.

Keywords: Parkinson’s disease/Parkinsonism, Neuropsychology, Cardiovascular, Comorbidities, Prevalence

1. Introduction

Parkinson’s disease (PD) is the second most common neurodegenerative disorder of late life and involves progressive changes in a family of segregated motor and nonmotor circuits influencing multiple neurotransmitter systems [1]. Although commonly treated as a dopaminergic depletion disorder, PD is a multisystem disorder with classic motor symptoms (tremors, rigidity, bradykinesia), but also autonomic, mood, and cognitive changes. Typical cognitive changes include slowed thinking, increased forgetfulness, and problems with working memory and multi-tasking [2]. Cognitive changes can occur early in the disease course [3,4], and have been attributed to disruption of dopamine-dependent frontalstriatal circuitry [5]. Superimposed on this prototypical frontal profile, subsets of Parkinson patients develop additional memory and visuospatial deficits that may reflect broad based cholinergic disruption [4,6]. Over time, up to 75−80% of Parkinson patients eventually transition to dementia, particularly those surviving longer than 10 years after disease onset [7].

While PD-related neural degeneration is the primary contributor of cognitive decline, it is unclear whether additional factors, like co-occurring health comorbidities, might also be contributory. Indeed, a wealth of literature over the past decade has pointed to cardiovascular conditions, particularly hypertension and diabetes, as posing risks for cognitive decline in older adults [8,9]. These comorbidities have been associated with small vessel vascular disease observed on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans as leukoaraiosis (LA) or white matter abnormalities [10]. Hypertension-related alterations in white matter, particularly those connecting frontal and subcortical regions, have been linked to changes in set-shifting, working memory, and processing speed [11]. A recent quantitative study using semi-automated volumetric techniques, found that LA affecting as little as 3−13% of white matter volume detrimentally influenced cognition [12].

The question arises as to whether cardiovascular comorbidities negatively influence cognition in Parkinson patients, similar to that observed in normal elderly. Previous studies have failed to show a detrimental affect of cardiovascular disease on dementia status [1317]. The proposed reasons why cardiovascular risk factors may not relate to cognitive status were a possible protective factor from PD related to lower blood pressure from levodopa medication and reduced likelihood of smoking among individuals with PD [18]. However, while cardiovascular risk factors were not related to cognition, LA, a proposed mechanism mediating hypertension and cognition, did predict cognitive status [16,17]. A possible explanation for the non-significant findings includes being underpowered (no sample sizes were larger than 171 among the previously mentioned studies) to detect subtle differences which may precede dementia onset.

In the present study, we aimed to determine the unique relationship that common comorbidities may exert on simple measures of cognition in a large sample of almost 2000 Parkinson patients who were part of the National Parkinson Foundation Quality Improvement Initiative (NPF-QII). This initiative was a multi-center effort that has resulted in a large standardized clinical practice and longitudinal registry for the purpose of comparative effectiveness research and quality improvement studies [19]. Variables collected in this database were chosen for their ability to quickly capture a broad range of symptoms with minimal interruption to routine clinical care. Of relevance to the current study, this database included information about the occurrence of six common comorbidities: heart-circulatory, diabetes, arthritis, cancer, respiratory diseases, and other neurologic conditions. Based on findings with normal elderly, we hypothesized that co-occurring “vascular” comorbidities (i.e., heart/circulation problems, diabetes) would be associated with worsened cognitive status in patients with PD even when controlling for disease severity and duration.

2. Methods

2.1. Design and standard protocol approvals, registrations, and patient consents

A cross-sectional study design was used, whereby data were obtained from the NPF-QII registry [19]. The data in this registry were drawn from 17 participating NPF Centers of Excellence. A diagnosis of idiopathic PD was made by a movement disorders specialist using the UK Brain Bank criteria [20]. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of each of the following participating centers: University of Pennsylvania, Oregon Health and Science University, Northwestern University (Illinois), Struthers Parkinson’s Center (Minnesota), Markham-Stouffville Hospital (Ontario, Canada), the Parkinson’s Institute and Clinical Center (California), Muhammad Ali Parkinson Center (Arizona), University of Kansas, Baylor College of Medicine (Texas), Georgia Health and Sciences University, University of South Florida, John Hopkins University (Maryland), Vanderbilt University (Tennessee), Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center (New York), Nijmegen Medical Center (Nijmegen, Netherlands), Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center (Tel Aviv, Israel) and the University of Florida. Informed consent was obtained prior to administration of any study procedure.

2.2. Participants

Participants included all patients registered in the NPF-QII registry between 2009 and 2010 with a diagnosis of idiopathic PD but no history of undergoing deep brain stimulation (DBS) surgery. The database query from NPF-QII yielded 2646 PD patients without DBS. Individuals were further excluded if one or more of the following coded variables were unavailable: comorbidity ratings (described below), disease duration, Hoehn and Yahr stage [21], PD type (tremor predominant vs other), cognitive data (see below), gender, age, living conditions (living at home independently, with skilled care, or other), and information about medication category. Medication category consisted of whether the patient was receiving the following treatments: levodopa, dopamine agonists, antidepressants, cognitive enhancers, or stimulants. Within these treatment categories, there was no further breakdown in terms of specific dosage or specific type of medication. Additionally, height and weight were obtained in order to compute a body mass index (BMI). After meeting all inclusion/exclusion criteria, the final N was 1948 participants.

2.3. Cognitive measures and health comorbidities from NPF-QII

Scores from two cognitive measures were available from the NPF registry: semantic verbal fluency and verbal memory. The “semantic” verbal fluency task involved naming as many animal exemplars as possible during a 1 min period (i.e., Animal fluency; dependent variable = total number of correct animal names) [22]. The verbal memory task involved oral presentation of the five-word list from the Montreal Cognitive Assessment [23], with an immediate recall and a 90-s delayed recall by the patients. For the present study, we focused on delayed recall, a more sensitive memory index, with total number of words correctly recalled as the final variable.

Six categories of health comorbidities were available and rated by the NPF clinician for presence and severity of impact using a 5-point scale: 0 = absent; 1 = asymptomatic/minimal; 2 = moderate; 3 = severe; 4 = very severe. These comorbidities included: heart/circulation problems, diabetes, arthritis, cancer, respiratory disease and “other neurological disorders.” Note that hypertension was subsumed under the heart/circulation category, which also included past heart attack, heart failure, irregular heartbeat and peripheral artery disease. It was not possible to isolate independent effects of hypertension vs. cardiac disease in this dataset. The “other neurologic” category included conditions such as stroke, neuropathy, etc. The “arthritis” category included pain in joints hips, knees, shoulder and spine, and did not distinguish between osteoarthritis and rheumatoid arthritis.

2.4. Statistical analyses

To assess the independent contribution of health comorbidities on cognitive status, we conducted separate multiple hierarchical regression analyses on each of the two cognitive measures (Animal fluency, 90” delayed recall of the 5-item word list). Scores from each cognitive measure were normally distributed. Each hierarchical regression analysis included four blocks of “predictor” variables consisting of 1) available demographic information (age, sex, living condition); 2) disease-related variables (duration of PD, Hoehn and Yahr (H&Y) stage, PD subtype); 3) medication types; and 4) health comorbidities including a computed measure of BMI. Health comorbidities were entered as the last block into the hierarchical regression, so that “variance” associated with other variables (disease, demographic, etc.) could be extracted prior to considering the influence of comorbidities. Statistical significance was evaluated at the a = 0.05 level. Variation Inflation Factor (VIF) revealed low multicollinearity (VIF < 5) among predictors for all analyses.

3. Results

Table 1 shows the characteristics of the final sample of 1948 PD patients. The cohort was approximately 60% male with an average age of 66.6 years and average symptom duration of approximately 9 years (range 1−45 years). Disease severity as indexed by Hoehn and Yahr staging ranged from 1 to 5 with an overall mean of 2.49. Almost half the sample were taking an antidepressant and nearly one third were on a cognitive enhancer.

Table 1.

Participant characteristics and cognitive data.

Age (yrs)
 Mean (±SD) 66.6 ± 10.2
 Range (min-max) 25–95
Sex
 Male:female 1169:779
Disease duration (yrs)
 Mean (±SD) 9.25 ± 6.1
 Range (min-max) 1–45
Tremor predominant, % sample 72%
Living situation (% sample)
 Living at home 98.1%
 Skilled care 1.9%
Hoehn-Yahr staging
 Mean (±SD) 2.49 ± 0.8
Staging (% sample)
  Stage 1 13.9
  Stage 2 59.0
  Stage 3 21.1
  Stage 4 3.6
  Stage 5 .4
Medications (% sample taking)
− Levodopa 82
− Dopamine agonists 41
− Antidepressants 46
− Cognitive enhancers 29
− Stimulant 2
Cognitive measures (mean ± SD)
− Semantic fluency (animals) 19.1 ± 6.2
− 90″ Delayed recall (max = 5) 3.0 ± 1.3
Body mass index, mean ± SD 27.3 ± 5.4

Variables are reported as means ± standard deviations (SD’s) with additional range data for certain variables.

3.1. Prevalence of comorbidities

As shown in Table 2, arthritis and heart/circulation problems were the most common comorbidities (i.e., 47% and 36%, respectively), whereas diabetes was least frequently reported (i.e., 9%). The occurrence of comorbidities was also examined as a function of age, by dividing the sample into three groups: 59 years (N = 451), 60−69 yrs (N = 726) and 70 yrs (N = 771). As shown in Fig. 1, comorbidity prevalence significantly increased with age for four of the six recorded conditions: heart/circulation (p < 0.001), diabetes (p < 0.001), cancer (p < 0.001) and arthritis (p < 0.001). There was a trend for neurologic conditions to increase with age (p < 0.06) (Table 3).

Table 2.

Prevalence of six common comorbidities among the Parkinson sample (N = 1852).

Comorbidity rating by physician
Comorbidity Absent
Minimal
Moderate
Severe
Very severe
N % N % N % N % N %
Heart/circulation 1240 63.7 496 25.5 175 9.0 33 1.7 4 0.2
Respiratory 1659 85.2 189 9.7 78 4.0 18 0.9 4 0.2
Diabetes 1772 91.0 108 5.5 55 2.8 12 0.6 1 0.1
Other neurologic 1751 89.9 118 6.1 58 3.0 15 0.8 6 0.3
Cancer 1688 86.7 187 9.6 43 2.2 28 1.4 2 0.1
Arthritis 1041 53.4 473 24.3 328 16.8 86 4.4 20 1.0

Heart/circulation category included: hypertension, past heart attack, heart failure, irregular heartbeat, peripheral artery disease; Respiratory include: asthma, COPD, etc. Other neurologic included stroke, neuropathy, head injury, etc. Arthritis category included pain in joints, hips, knees, shoulder, and spine, with no distinction between osteoarthritis and rheumatoid arthritis.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Comorbidity prevalence in Parkinson sample according to age, Percent of comorbidities present by age group. **p < 0.001. Heart/Circulation by age group (young to old): 26.2%, 36.6%, 42.0%, (χ2 (2) = 32.3, p < 0.001); Respiratory by age group: 12.2%, 15.4%, 15.8%, (χ2 (2) = 3.6, p < 0.163), Diabetes: 5.3%, 9.0%, 11.3%, (χ2 (2) = 12.3, p < 0.001), Neurologic: 7.5%, 9.9%, 11.8%, (χ2 (2) = 5.6, p < 0.061), Cancer: 6.0%, 9.9%, 20.9%, (χ2 (2) = 66.2, p < 0.001), Arthritis: 29.7%, 43.8%, 59.0%, (χ2 (2) = 103.7, p < 0.001).

Table 3.

Cognition.

Predictors
Outcomes
Variables
Animal fluency
Delayed 90″ recall
FΔ R2 Δ Beta Sig. F Δ R2 Δ Beta Sig.
Final model 19.498 0.162 <0.001 11.534 0.103 <0.001
Demographics 85.575 0.117 <0.001 47.562 0.069 <0.001
Age −0.252 <0.001 −0.162 <0.001
Sex  0.002  0.927  0.149 <0.001
Living −0.021  0.340 −0.009  0.690
PD severity 17.693 0.024 <0.001 12.180 0.017 <0.001
Model  Resting tremor −0.05  0.018  0.024  0.266
H&Y stage −0.115 <0.001 −0.081  0.001
Disease duration −0.008  0.733 −0.057  0.017
Medication 6.631 0.015 <0.001 4.653 0.011 <0.001
Model   Levodopa −0.067  0.004 −0.047  0.048
Dopamine agonist  0.021  0.332  0.017  0.462
Anti depressant −0.017  0.434 −0.008  0.731
Stimulant  0.012  0.562  0.025  0.245
Cognitive enhancer −0.093 <0.001 −0.092 <0.001
Comorbidity 1.869 0.007  0.061 1.503 0.006  0.151
Model Heart/circulation −0.002  0.918 −0.050  0.030
Respiratory −0.017  0.430  0.025  0.260
Diabetes −0.041  0.061 −0.018  0.417
Neurological −0.035  0.098  0.000  0.992
Cancer −0.035  0.100  0.001  0.979
Arthritis −0.032  0.150  0.036  0.120
BMI squared −0.009  0.677 −0.018  0.414
BMI  0.006  0.802 −0.031  0.176

Results from separate hierarchical regression analyses examining the relationship between comorbidities and cognitive tasks (animal fluency and delayed memory recall). Variables pertaining to demographics, PD severity and medication status were statistically controlled for in preceding blocks of predictors.

3.2. Cognitive outcomes

Table 3 shows the results of the hierarchical regression analyses. For delayed recall, the overall regression model was significant (F (19,1927) = 11.534, p < 0.001, R Squared =.103). Specifically, being older, being female, having more severe PD (i.e. higher HY stage), having longer PD duration, and being on levodopa or cognitive enhancers were all related to lower delayed memory scores. Comorbidities as a whole failed to produce a unique effect on memory performance (F change (8,1927) 1.503, p = 0.151). However, when looking at individual comorbidities, we found that heart/circulation problems significantly contributed to memory performance (p = 0.03). Namely, PD patients with greater heart/ circulation problems had lower performance on the delayed memory task. None of the remaining comorbidities (i.e., diabetes, arthritis, cancer, other neurologic) significantly contributed to memory performance.

For semantic fluency, the overall regression model was also significant (F (19,1927) 19.49, p < 0.001, R Squared .162). Age, presence of tremor at rest, PD severity, and medication status (levodopa and cognitive enhancers) significantly influenced semantic fluency scores. Thus, being older, having a non-tremor presentation, being in more advanced stages of PD, and being on cognitive enhancers or levodopa were associated with worse fluency scores. The addition of comorbidities as a whole into the regression model tended to account for a significant amount of unique variance in our model (F change (8,1927) 1.869, p = 0.061). When looking at individual comorbidities, we also found trend relationships, with the presence of diabetes (p = 0.06) and “other neurologic” (p = 0.098) each tending to be associated with lower semantic fluency scores.

Post hoc analyses were conducted with 1802 PD individuals who were not on cognitive enhancers (i.e. Donepezil, Rivastigmine, etc.) in order to minimize inclusion of possible individuals for whom dementia or cognitive difficulties were of concern. Repeat analyses on this participant subset revealed findings consistent with our total sample. Heart/circulation problems were found to be a unique significant predictor of delayed memory recall (p = 0.029) and diabetes showed a trend pattern with semantic fluency scores (p = 0.084).

4. Discussion

The present study provides evidence that co-occurring health conditions, cardio/circulatory and diabetes, negatively influence the cognitive status of Parkinson patients. While demographic and clinical PD variables accounted for a majority of variance in cognitive domains, heart/circulation problems were associated with worse delayed verbal memory recall, whereas the presence of diabetes tended to be associated with worse semantic fluency scores. Importantly, this association was independent of disease severity, age, gender, and medication status (i.e., dopaminergic, psychotropic, cognitive enhancers). Although the model for overall comorbidities failed to reach significance, individual comorbidities contributed to cognitive scores. This implies that some of the variance in cognitive deficits in PD is explained by specific disease states rather than reflecting a nonspecific global burden of increased comorbidities.

These findings roughly parallel those with normal elderly [8,9]. However, they contrast with those of five prior studies in Parkinson patients. In these previous studies, cardiovascular risk factors have been examined in relation to categorical classification of PD patients as “demented” or not. One study evaluated 173 newly diagnosed PD patients at baseline, re-assessed them 4 years later for presence of dementia, and identified 43 incident cases of dementia [13]. The major finding was that subjective report of cardiovascular risk factors at baseline (i.e., hypertension, stroke, diabetes, coronary artery disease, etc.) did not predict transition to dementia over the 4 year period. Four other studies were crosssectional [1417]; three studies categorized their PD cohort into three cognitive subgroups: demented, mild cognitive impairment, and unimpaired groups. None of these studies found a differential occurrence of vascular comorbidities across the different cognitive subgroupings.

Our study differs from prior PD comorbidity-cognition research in several important ways. First, rather than using presence/ absence of dementia or MCI as the final endpoint, we examined parametric variation in cognitive scores due to comorbidity status in our Parkinson cohort. Parametric approaches may be more sensitive in detecting differences than are those using categorical outcome classifications [24]. Second, our sample size of 1948 was a great deal larger than previous studies. Because our findings indicated that the variance uniquely explained by heart/circulation comorbidities (as defined by NPF guidelines) was small, it is possible that previous studies with Parkinson patients lacked sufficient power to find small relationships between cardiovascular risk factors and cognition. Finally, although we do not know the influence of lifestyle/cultural factors, our study was primarily based on a large multi-center sample, whereas previous studies involved PD cohorts from a Norwegian epidemiologic study [13,15,17], from Poland [14] and from Korea [16].

In the present study, the most prevalent comorbidity was arthritis, at approximately 46%, and followed by heart/circulation problems in approximately 36% of the PD patients. Due to the manner in which the data were originally obtained through the NPF cohort, we were unable to determine the relative occurrence of hypertension vs cardiac problems (or both) to the heart/circulation category due. Regardless, the occurrence of hypertension in our sample is clearly lower than the 60−65% typically reported in prevalence studies in normal elderly over the age of 65 years [25]. The lower prevalence of hypertension in PD has been attributed, in part, to iatrogenic effects of levodopa medications [18]. The remaining comorbidities appear to more closely approximate those observed in the normal elderly. We also observed significant agerelated increases for arthritis, cancer, heart-circulatory, and diabetes.

While our study did not investigate possible mechanisms underlying the comorbidity−cognition relationships in PD patients, we presume that they are similar to those hypothesized in the normal elderly, specifically small vessel vascular disease. A number of large scale studies in normal older adults have provided support for the relationship between vascular risk factors (hypertension, pulse pressure, diabetes), white matter changes as observed on MRI, and cognitive status [26,27]. There is some suggestion that hypertension may interact with genetic factors to increase likelihood of vascular changes [28], that diabetes may influence specific hippocampal regions [29] and that hypertension and diabetes have different influences with respect to vessel pathophysiology [10]. By contrast, studies in PD have not consistently shown a relationship between white matter abnormalities on brain MRI and cognition. Some studies have found that white matter abnormalities are related to an increased risk of dementia and impairment on general measures of cognition [16,17,30], whereas others have not [14,15]. The bases for these differences are unclear, however, a variety of methodologic factors may be contributory. The latter include variability in the assessment of risk factors (subclinical risk factors vs. presence of stroke), use of newly diagnosed versus older cohorts, variability in techniques for quantifying white matter lesions, and a lack of power to detect the small differences as demonstrated in the current study. It has further been suggested that the cognitive effects of white matter lesions may be more detectable in advanced stages of PD [15,17].

This study is not without limitations. First, due to the nature of how data were collected as part of the NPF-QII, we were unable to isolate the influence of hypertension on cognition, independent of cardiac disease. The use of direct measures of hypertension (i.e., blood pressure, pulse pressure, etc.) may be beneficial in light of frequent co-occurrence of both hypertension and hypotension in PD. Second, implementation of a more detailed and comprehensive neurocognitive assessment battery could better differentiate which cognitive domains are most sensitive to the influence of comorbidities. Additionally, both measures used in the current study have a strong verbal component which could confound the interpretation. Third, we could not address the contribution of education or mood (i.e., depression, apathy) as these variables were not specifically obtained as part of the NPF-QII. Further investigation into the relationship between comorbidities and mood in PD could be of interest given the high percentage of individuals on antidepressants. The addition of age match controls could provide information as to whether or not health comorbidities have a differential impact on cognition in PD. Lastly independent information about the “dementia” status of the participants was not obtained as part of the NPF-QII. Regarding the latter, however, when we removed individuals taking cognitive enhancers from our dataset and reran the analyses, identical findings were obtained.

This current study adds to the literature on cognitive changes in Parkinson disease by providing evidence that comorbidities related to cardiovascular health are associated with reduced cognitive performance on simple measures of memory recall and verbal fluency. While these documented effects are small, they are present even when age, PD-related disease severity and medication use are “controlled for” (i.e., covaried). Given the high prevalence of cardiovascular risk factors that increase with age, both in Parkinson patients in the current study as well as normal elderly, further research may be important to better understand how these factors interact with and potentially exacerbate the phenotypic expression and rate of cognitive decline. Future studies should be performed using more direct measures of comorbidities, an assessment of multiple cognitive domains in a sample of non-demented PD patients, and coupled with investigation of possible mechanisms mediating this relationship. In sum, cardiovascular comorbidities may play a detrimental role on cognition in PD as they do among the normal elderly and further research is needed to better understand the nature of this relationship in individuals who are already at risk for significant cognitive decline.

Acknowledgments

Study support

University of Florida and the UF National Parkinson Foundation Center of Excellence, Gainesville, FL. The data for this study were provided, free of charge, by the National Parkinson FoundationQuality Improvement PF-QII Longitudinal Study. The NPF QII investigators and advisory team include: M. Brandabur, B. Bloem, C. Cho, A.Santiago, T. Gurevich, M. Guttman, R. Hauser, J. Jankovic, K. Lyons, Z. Mari, I. Malaty, J. Morgan, J. Nutt, S. Parashos, A. Sideroff, T. Simuni, D. Tarsy, T. Davis, J. Miyasaki, E. Cheng, N. Giladi, L. Marsh, M.S. Okun, E. Nelson, P. Schmidt, and J. Zamudio.

Disclosures

This study involved data collected by the National Parkinson Foundation and was provided to the authors free of charge. No financial compensation was received for conducting this study.

Mr. Jones reports no disclosures.

Dr. Malaty reports no disclosures relevant to this publication. In the past year, Dr. Malaty has received support from the National Parkinson Foundation and Michael J. Fox Foundation as well as Tourette Syndrome Association for Parkinson and Tourette syndrome related research. She also participates in research funded by IPSEN, Acadia, Solvay and TEVA, all relevant to Parkinson disease.

Dr. Okun serves as a consultant for the National Parkinson Foundation, and has received research grants from NIH, NPF, the Michael J. Fox Foundation, the Parkinson Alliance, Smallwood Foundation, and the UF Foundation. Dr. Okun has previously received honoraria, but in the past 24 months has received no support from industry including travel. Dr. Okun has received royalties for publications with Demos, Manson, and Cambridge (movement disorders books). Dr. Okun has participated in CME activities on movement disorders sponsored by the USF CME office, PeerView, and by Vanderbilt University. The institution and not Dr. Okun receives grants from Medtronic and ANS/St. Jude, and the PI has no financial interest in these grants. Dr. Okun has participated as a site PI and/or co-I for several NIH, foundation, and industry sponsored trials over the years but has not received honoraria. Dr. Price has received research support from the NIH and the National Parkinson Foundation.

Dr. Bowers has received research support from the NIH, Michael J. Fox Foundation, the National Parkinson Foundation, and the McKnight Research Foundation.

References

  • [1].Alexander GE, DeLong MR, Strick PL. Parallel Organization of Functionally segregated circuits linking Basal Ganglia and Cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience 1986;9(1):357–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [2].Zgaljardic DJ, Borod JC, Foldi NS, Mattis PJ. A review of the coGnitive and behavioral sequelae of Parkinson’s disease. Relationship to Frontostriatal Circuitry 2003;16(4):193–210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [3].Aarsland D, Brønnick K, Larsen JP, Tysnes OB, Alves G. Cognitive impairment in incident, untreated Parkinson disease: the Norwegian ParkWest study. Neurology 2009;72(13):1121–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [4].Williams-Gray CH, Evans JR, Goris A, Foltynie T, Ban M, Robbins R, et al. The distinct cognitive syndromes of Parkinson’s disease: 5 year followup of the CamPaIGN cohort. Brain 2009;132(11):2958–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [5].Owen AM, Sahakian BJ, Hodges JR, Summers BA, Polkey CE, Robbins TW. Dopamine-dependent fronto-striatal planning deficits in early Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology 1995;9:126–40. [Google Scholar]
  • [6].Bohnen NI, Kaufer DI, Ivanco LS, Koeppe RS, Dais JG, Mathis CA, et al. Cortical cholinergic function is more severely affected in Parkinsonian dementia than in Alzheimer disease: an in vivo positron emission tomographic study. Archives of Neurology 2003;60:1745–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [7].Hely MA, Reid WG, Adena MA, Halliday GM, Morris JGL. The Sydney multicenter study of Parkinson’s disease: the inevitability of dementia at 20 years. Movement Disorders 2008;23:837–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [8].Verdelho A, Madureira S, Ferro JM, Basile AM, Chabriat H, Erkinjuntti T, et al. Differential impact of cerebral white matter changes, diabetes, hypertension and stroke on cognitive performance among non-disabled elderly. The LADIS study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2007;78:1325–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [9].Ruitenberg A, Skoog I, Ott A, Witteman JCM, van Harskamp F, Hofman A, et al. Blood pressure and risk of dementia: results from the Rotterdam study and the Gothenburg H-70 Study. Demen Geriatr Cogn Disord 2001;12:33–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [10].Bezerra DC, Sharrett AR, Matsushita K, Gottesman RF, Shibata D, Mosley TH, et al. Risk factors for lacune subtypes in the Atherosclerosis risk in Communities (ARIC) study. Neurology 2012;78:102–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [11].Kennedy KM. Raz N.Aging, white matter, and cognition: differential effects of regional variations in diffusion properties on memory, executive functions, and speed. Neuropsychologia 2009;47:916–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [12].Price CC, Mitchell S, Brumback B, Tanner J, Schmalfuss I, Lamar M, et al. The Leukoaraiosis (LA) 25% threshold and the phenotypic expression of dementia. Neurology 2012, in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • [13].Haugarvoll K, Aarsland D, Wentzel-Larsen T, Larsen JP. The influence of cerebrovascular risk factors on incident dementia in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica 2005;112(6):386–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [14].Slawek J, Wieczorek D, Dubaniewicz M, Lass P. The influence of vascular risk factors and hyperintensities on the degree of cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Neurol Neuorchir Pol 2008;42(6):505–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [15].Dalaker TO, Larsen JP, Dwyer MG, Aarsland D, Beyer MK, Alves G, et al. White matter hyperintensities do not impact cognitive function in patients with newly diagnosed Parkinson’s disease. Neuroimage 2009;47:2083–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [16].Lee SJ, Kim JS, Yoo JY, Song IU, Kim B, Jung SL, et al. Influence of white matter hyperintensities on the cognition of patients with Parkinson disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2010;24:227–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [17].Beyer MK, Aarsland D, Greve OJ, Larsen JP. Visual rating of white matter hyperintensities in Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders 2006;23: 223–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [18].Nanhoe-Mahabier W, de Laat K, Visser JE, Zijlmans J, de Leeuw FE, Bloem BR. Parkinson disease and comorbid cerebrovascular disease. Nature Reviews Neurology 2009;5:533–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [19].Okun MS, Siderowf A, Nutt JG, O’Conner GT, Bloem BR, Olmstead EM, et al. Piloting the NPF data-driven quality improvement initiative. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders 2010;16(8):517–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [20].Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, Lees AJ. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. Journal Neurology. Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 1992;55(3):181–4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [21].Hoehn M, Yahr M. Parkinsonism: onset, progression, and mortality. Neurology 1967;17:738–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [22].Tombaugh T, Kozak J, Rees L. Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1999;14(2):167–77. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [23].Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bedirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MOCA): a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005;53: 695–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [24].Hays WL. Statistics 5th ed. Clifton Park, NY: Cengage Learning; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • [25].Aronow WS, Fleg JL, Pepine CJ, Artinian NT, Bakris G, Brown A, et al. Expert Consensus Documenta ACCF/AHA 2011 Expert Consensus Document oh Hypertension in the Elderly. J Am Soc Hypertension 2011;5(4):259–352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [26].Raz N, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Acker JD. Vascular health and longitudinal changes in brain and cognition in middle-aged and older adults. Neuropsychology 2007;21:149–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [27].Debette S, Seshadri S, Beiser A, Au R, Himali JJ, Palumbo C, et al. Midlife vascular risk factor exposure accelerates structural brain aging and cognitive decline. Neurology 2011;77(5):461–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [28].Schmidt H, Zeginigg M, Wiltgen M, Himali JJ, Palumbo C, Wolf PA, et al. Genetic variants of the NOTCH3 gene in the elderly and and magnetic resonance imaging correlates of age-related cerebral small vessel disease. Brain 2011;134:3384–97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [29].Wu W, Brickman AM, Luchsinger J, Ferrazzano P, Pichiule P, Yoshita M, et al. The brain in the age of old: the hippocampal formation is targeted differentially by diseases of late life. Annals of Neurology 2008;64(6):698–706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [30].Choi SA, Evidente VG, Caviness JN. Are there differences in cerebral white matter lesion burdens between Parkinson’s disease patients with and without dementia? Acta Neuopathol 2010;119:147–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES