Abstract
Culture (behaviour based on socially transmitted information) is present in diverse animal species, yet how it interacts with genetic evolution remains largely unexplored. Here, we review the evidence for gene–culture coevolution in animals, especially birds, cetaceans and primates. We describe how culture can relax or intensify selection under different circumstances, create new selection pressures by changing ecology or behaviour, and favour adaptations, including in other species. Finally, we illustrate how, through culturally mediated migration and assortative mating, culture can shape population genetic structure and diversity. This evidence suggests strongly that animal culture plays an important evolutionary role, and we encourage explicit analyses of gene–culture coevolution in nature.
Subject terms: Evolutionary ecology, Coevolution, Cultural evolution, Evolutionary genetics
The reciprocal interaction between genetic and cultural evolution is well recognised in humans. Here, Whitehead and colleagues review the growing body of evidence that culture is also a major driver of both neutral and adaptive genetic evolution in non-human animals.
Introduction
Genes and culture are both inheritance systems that transmit information between organisms and generate phenotypic change (see Box 1 for definitions). Social learning is the key ingredient of culture. Animals learn important skills from each other, including what to eat and where to find it, how to recognise and escape predators, and which migratory pathways to take through their environments. These cultural adaptations affect population structures as well as the physical and social environment that elicits genetic evolution. Thus, cultural behaviour may select for particular functional genes, influence patterns of genetic diversity, and spark speciation. When cultural activity is an important determinant of fitness, it can generate selection for traits that further enhance cultural competencies, allowing genes and culture to coevolve reciprocally (Fig. 1).
Early reviews assumed that non-human cultures were insufficiently stable to affect genetic evolution1,2. However, recent research has established that animal culture is present in insects, birds, fishes and mammals3,4, that it can have important impacts on fitness (e.g., ref. 5), that it can be stable over many generations (e.g., refs. 6,7), and that it can affect evolutionary dynamics even when transient (e.g., refs. 8,9). Here, we review evidence from across the animal kingdom that the strength and direction of selection, as well as population genetic structure, are influenced by cultural activities in a range of species, not just humans. We define gene–culture coevolution inclusively, as occurring when cultural evolution shapes genetic evolution, often but not always entailing reciprocal interactions between the two. We compare genetics and culture as systems of inheritance, and explain the different ways in which culture modifies genetic selection. These include how culture may select for particular functional genes, whether in populations, species-wide, or in different species, and how culture shapes the structure and diversity of variation in neutral genes.
Box 1 Glossary.
Conformity: A disproportionate adherence to majority behaviour (positive frequency dependence)2, which may override personal adherence to an alternative.
Cultural evolution: Evolutionary change in behaviour, knowledge or constructed artefacts arising from social learning and transmission.
Cultural hitchhiking: Neutral genetic variants that happen to be carried by individuals who transmit selective cultural traits ‘hitchhike’ along the same cross-generational pathways; matrilineal cultural inheritance can hence constrain mitochondrial genetic inheritance, leading to a reduction in variance93.
Cultural intelligence: Cognitive processes supporting and/or dependent on cultural transmission.
Cultural transmission: Diffusion of behaviour patterns or knowledge via social learning from others’ actions or their consequences.
Culture: (a) broad sense—equivalent to ‘Tradition’ below123; (b) more elaborate sense–a communal complex of multiple traditions56.
Ecotypes: Communities within the same species exploiting different ecological niches.
Gene–culture (or Culture-gene) coevolution: Cultural processes shape genetic evolution by modifying the selection of genes, often entailing reciprocal interactions and feedbacks.
Horizontal transmission: Cultural transmission of behaviour between members of the same generation13.
Phenotypic plasticity: The ability of a genotype to produce different phenotypes in different environments.
Social learning: Learning that is facilitated by observation of, or interaction with, another animal or its products4,17.
Tradition: A behaviour pattern shared by members of a community that relies on socially learned and transmitted information.
Vertical transmission: Cultural transmission of behaviour from parent to offspring13.
Evolutionarily relevant properties of culture
Parallels between genetic and cultural inheritance are well-established, but cultural transmission is mechanistically different from genetic transmission and cannot simply be treated as ‘another class of gene’3,10,11. Although genes are transmitted at conception from parents to offspring, social learning occurs throughout the lifespan, from many different individuals. Culture thereby allows for the propagation of phenotypic variants among unrelated individuals, often within timespans significantly shorter than a generation (e.g., social transmission of predator recognition in minnows, Pimphales promelas12; socially learned mating preferences in grouse, Centrocercus urophasianus9). Such ‘horizontal cultures’, comprising learning among similarly aged peers, can be highly labile; conversely, ‘vertical cultures’, in which offspring learn from parents, can be very stable13,14. The stability of cultural transmission can be enhanced through conformity (i.e., a disproportionate tendency to adopt the most common behaviour), as in the foraging traditions of great tits, Parus major15. This stability allows cultural traits to be maintained as Nash equilibria, generating a ‘cultural inertia’ that can hinder adaptation to changing environmental conditions, and leading to population-specific traditions in behaviour (e.g., migratory pathways in reef fishes16). Through eliciting change in behaviour, often across an entire population, culture can transform the social environment, whereas cultural activities (e.g., foraging) also modify ecological circumstances. Culture thus encompasses a range of temporal scales, pathways of information flow and impacts on selection.
Cultural transmission has a number of additional properties that affect its role in genetic evolution. Culture provides a highly flexible means to adjust to novel conditions and modify selection. Much of social learning is reliant on phylogenetically ancient and widespread associative learning processes, such as classical and operant conditioning17. This kind of learning can be applied in an extremely flexible and open-ended manner, including learning from both conspecifics and heterospecifics, which means that animals are not restricted to learning only about environmental features previously encountered by the lineage (e.g., established predators or foods). Instead, animals can also learn about entirely novel stimuli or events, and devise appropriate responses to them (e.g., birds learn to evade a novel predator18,19). Via learning, animals can therefore generate adaptive responses to conditions without prior evolution of dedicated traits with suitable reaction norms. In addition to responding appropriately to changing features of their environments, such as the dangers presented by a novel predator, social learning can also generate opportunities for phenotypic change in the absence of any immediate environmental change or stressor (e.g., when orangutans, Pongo pygmaeus, proactively devise new food-processing techniques, social learning allows others to access hitherto-unexploited foods, such as palm heart20).
Culture typically leads to the production and propagation of adaptive behaviour. Learned behavioural innovations (the analogue of mutation) are usually not random but rather novel functional solutions tailored to new challenges or hitherto-unexploited opportunities21. For instance, among the most-celebrated examples of animal innovation are the invention by primates and cetaceans of new food-processing methods, and new dominance and courtship displays, which spread because of their perceived beneficial qualities and/or are associated with a rise in status or reproductive success22,23. Learned behavioural innovation is now extensively documented in animals21, and many such innovations are propagated through social learning (e.g., ref. 24). Animal social learning is also typically non-random and strategic, with evidence that individuals often disproportionately copy successful individuals and high-payoff behaviour25,26, enhancing the spread of adaptive variants (e.g., some insects and birds are known to copy the nest-site decisions of successful conspecifics and heterospecifics27,28). Good information, supporting fitness-enhancing behaviour, is far more likely to be propagated than bad information.
These features, most of which are particularly well-documented in vertebrates, mean that phenotypic accommodation through culture has the potential to be common, rapid and powerful. Cultures can quickly accumulate adaptive features29 and introduce novelty into phenotype space, generating diverse selection on genes30. Yet, cultural systems are not typically well-captured by standard population genetic or quantitative genetic models of trait evolution, but instead require dedicated theory2,3,13,31,32.
Culture modifies the strength of selection
A prima facie confusing feature of culture is that it can both speed up and slow down genetic evolution, but these contrasting effects are now well-understood, thanks to theoretical work2,13,33. In stationary, or slowly changing, unimodal fitness landscapes, learning typically slows evolution by reducing phenotypic differences between genotypes34–36. This explains how cultural species such as humans and, possibly, bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops spp.) can live in extremely diverse habitats without major genetic adaptations37. However, learning usually accelerates evolution in dynamic environments that cannot be tracked effectively by selection of genes2,13, or in static multimodal fitness landscapes, where the existence of multiple optima means that populations can become trapped on suboptimal fitness peaks. In the latter case, learning smooths the fitness landscape, increasing the likelihood of a directly increasing path of fitness to the global optimum33,36,38 and helping genotypes to locate otherwise difficult-to-find fitness peaks39.
In cases where learning accelerates evolution, phenotypic change (henceforth ‘phenotypic accommodation’) precedes, and then facilitates genetic adaptation by modifying selection on genetic variation (‘genetic accommodation’)40. Although, as described above, social learning can buffer selection on genetic variation that would otherwise lead to genetic adaptation, this buffering is unlikely to be perfect, and hence will not preclude selection of alleles that increase the probability of producing the phenotype, or improve it further (a process known as ‘genetic assimilation’41). For instance, the dietary traditions of killer whales, Orcinus orca, have favoured population-specific genes influencing morphology and digestion42,43.
Social learning can also elicit the selection of genetic adaptation in other traits (genetic accommodation). For instance, mate-choice copying, where the choice of mating partner is influenced by the mate-choice decisions of other individuals, is found in fruit flies, fishes, birds and mammals. Mate-choice copying propagates mating preferences over short time periods, such as a single season, yet population genetic models have shown that it can substantially enhance the strength of sexual selection on male traits8. Birdsong provides another illustration of how cultural change, which is rapid at least with respect to rates of genetic change, can nonetheless be consequential for genetic evolution, influencing patterns of migration and assortative mating, and facilitating speciation44–46. In sum, theoretical work leads to the expectation that genetic accommodation and genetic assimilation in response to culture could be widespread in animal populations, but this has been little investigated. Further examples of how cultural plasticity can precede and facilitate genetic evolution, and affect evolutionary rates, are considered in more detail in the following section.
Culture creates new selection pressures
Culture changes selection on functional genes
Among the processes of gene–culture coevolution, attention has overwhelmingly focussed on relationships between the distributions of functional genes within a population and cultural variants. Correlations are expected if cultural innovations alter the selection regimes for particular genes, as individuals with the same genes can have different fitness in different cultural contexts. Given it is impossible to demonstrate the causes of past episodes of adaptive evolution experimentally, the strongest evidence comes from humans, where extensive genetic sampling can be combined with historical and archaeological data. For example, the coevolutionary relationship between dairy farming and adult lactase production is well-established47. More generally, diverse agricultural practices are thought to have inadvertently selected for alleles expressed in enhanced metabolism of the increased starch, carbohydrates, alcohol and so forth, found in agricultural diets31,48. Genomic studies have identified over 100 other variants subject to recent selection for which cultural practices are thought to be the primary source of selection, although the difficulty of demonstrating their causal role precludes certainty48.
In non-humans, the most-compelling evidence comes from killer whales, in which recent population-genomic studies show that functional genes associated with digestion differ between ecotypes (Box 1) in ways that appear adaptive, and show evidence of recent selection42,43. Genes associated with the methionine cycle, which is involved in protein synthesis, differ systematically between mammal-eating and fish-eating ecotypes, a contrast presumed to result from different patterns of dietary protein intake42. Although North Pacific and Antarctic mammal-eating ecotypes both had strong signatures of selection in these groups of genes, the precise locations of the variations were different in the two ecotypes, suggesting independent genetic routes to phenotypic change. Given the culturally mediated variation in inter-population variation in chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) diets49, similar culturally initiated genetic variation in digestion may well be found in this species too.
Culturally transmitted foraging preferences might also have influenced the evolution of functional genes in great tits15,50. Providing food for these birds is popular in the United Kingdom and recent genomic comparisons of British great tits with those from the Netherlands, where bird feeding is less common, suggest there has been selection on genes involved in beak morphology50. Birds that use feeding stations have larger beaks, perhaps because these individuals are more effective at breaking open the provided seeds. Parid tits became famous for their foraging innovations when they learned to pierce open milk bottles to drink the cream in 1940’s Britain; this behaviour spread too rapidly to be the result of individual learning alone51. Artificial seeding of foraging behaviours in great tits has subsequently demonstrated that detecting and learning how to access bird feeders can spread horizontally and vertically through populations via social learning, enhancing individual learning15. It is therefore probable that social transmission of information about seed feeders increased their use quickly, and consequently altered selection pressure for beak size in great tits50. Furthermore, genetic differences within and between populations could affect culture reciprocally; through their positions in social foraging networks50,52, larger-beaked individuals may be more likely to spread knowledge of new feeding opportunities53.
Although genomic scans suggest large-scale culturally driven selection for genes in recent human evolution, with a few exceptions, such as the dairy farming-lactase case, further work is required to pin down particular instances46,54. This is also the case with non-humans where, for instance, it is necessary to exclude the theoretical possibility that ‘allele-surfing’ (i.e., increases in allele frequencies during post-bottleneck population growth) could have contributed to the patterns found in killer whales55.
Culture favours genes enhancing adaptations for culture
A substantial reliance on social learning has been predicted to select for genetic variants that enhance such learning species-wide, shaping supportive neural traits such as encephalisation, energy production or plasticity, or modifying life-history traits such as a longer juvenile period available for learning or enhanced parental support. 'Cultural drive’ or ‘cultural intelligence’ hypotheses2,37,56–62 cite a range of evidence that cultural inheritance can enhance fitness through the development of greater competence in key behaviours such as foraging and predator avoidance56,59, and they propose that this in turn will enhance selection pressures for genetically coupled phenotypic traits particularly the neural and life-history variables mentioned above. These effects may in turn lead to more reliance on culture, potentially creating positive ontogenetic-evolutionary feedback loops shaping gene–culture coevolution.
Tests of the cultural intelligence hypotheses have been of two main types. Most common have been cross-species, comparative analyses, addressing the prediction that the scale of cultural inheritance in a species will be associated with selection on supportive phenotypic characters. For example, Street et al.63 applied phylogenetic comparative analysis techniques to published databases that span 55 primate genera and 184 species to address relationships between records of social learning and predictor variables. Evidence of greater proclivity for social learning was predicted by both measures of brain size and of reproductive lifespan. The authors concluded that results are ‘consistent with the hypothesis that the evolution of large brains, sociality, and long lifespans has promoted reliance on culture …. in turn driving further increases in brain volume, cognitive abilities, and lifespan in some primate lineages’63.
Culture relies not only on social learning but also on intermittent behavioural innovation, and similar comparative analyses have identified relationships between records of innovation and brain size in both primates64 and birds65. Reinforcing these correlational analyses, a recent mechanistic model of brain evolution concluded that ‘our results are consistent with aspects of various cultural hypotheses for brain evolution’66.
A second approach is to compare closely related species differing in cultural richness, recently explored in a comparison between orangutan species, in which a more extensive cultural repertoire has been described for the Sumatran (Pongo abilii) than for the Bornean species (P. pygmaeus)67. Consistent with the cultural intelligence hypothesis, the Sumatran species have brains reported to be 2–10% larger and showed superior performance in cognitive tests conducted in comparable captive environments67.
Evolutionary effects of culture may explain a further life-history phenomenon, the existence of menopause not only in humans but in whales, where females of matrilineal species may live long after their reproductive span68. Modelling studies have concluded that menopause can evolve through inclusive fitness benefits69. Menopause is predicted to be favoured when females’ relatedness to the group and ability to assist relatives (e.g., by providing a highly competent model from whom to learn) increase with age, but continued reproduction would reduce their capacity to assist relatives69. Older female killer whales are known to be repositories of such extensive adaptive knowledge70.
Correlational tests of the cultural intelligence hypothesis outlined above have, however, been constrained by relatively crude measures of the scope of cultural intelligence in any given species, often resting on post hoc analysis of publications that report non-standardised measures of social learning. In future, the field will benefit from the development of more comparable and systematic variables to be applied in such analyses.
Culture generates selection across species
One difficulty with demonstrating gene–culture coevolution arises when genes and culture are both spread predominantly through transmission from parents to offspring. When culture changes how species interact, however, it can also influence genetic evolution across species boundaries, removing this potential confound. For example, experimental studies with brood parasitic cuckoos and hosts have established that culture may alter the strength of selection across time differently to when culture is absent. Knowledge of the threat of brood parasites (‘cuckoos’) is maintained in groups via social learning among naïve fairy-wren (Malurus cyaneus) hosts71,72, and reed warblers (Acrocephalus scirpaceus) learn about the identity of cuckoos that mimic more dangerous enemies horizontally from peers18,19,73. In reed warblers at least, this socially learned and transmitted information leads to increased detection of cuckoos and strengthens selection against the parasite74. However, common cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) females have a colour polymorphism, likely to be an MC1R variant75, that defeats host culture. Social learning about cuckoos is not generalised across morphs, so knowledge spreads quickly only about the common form. In this case, host culture generates a stronger selective advantage for cuckoos that appear different (i.e., negative frequency dependent selection) than if hosts only learned individually18.
Novel behaviours that spread and are maintained through culture also have the potential to initiate, or intensify, selection on interacting species. For example, recent experiments with great tits show that social transmission of foraging preferences can switch a prey species’ defence strategy from crypsis to aposematism76. Another candidate for interspecies gene–culture coevolution lies in the wide array of complex socially learned foraging techniques of killer whales77. Corkeron and Connor78 suggest that the seasonal migrations of baleen whales to the tropics may function to avoid killer whale predation. Although the details of baleen whale migrations seem socially learned79, the drive to migrate is likely to have a genetic basis that may have been influenced by the predatory cultures of killer whales.
There are many familiar examples where humans are the cultural species; these include the industrial revolution increasing selection for melanic forms of peppered moths (Biston betularia)80, and the rise of agriculture facilitating coevolution of novel pathogens with humans81,82. Although evidence for non-human culture altering selection in other species is at present limited, in theory this could influence any type of interaction where social learning in (at least) one party occurs. For example, previously ‘honest’ foraging bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) learn to rob nectar from flowers by observing others83—if this knowledge transmits across generations84, it could shape pollination efficiency and selection on the plant83. The onset of a new foraging culture could also rapidly shift selection on host microbiomes as they adapt to a novel resource, or influence transmission of microbial communities85. Culture influencing genetic change across species boundaries has great potential to demonstrate how gene–culture coevolution can operate.
Culture shapes population genetic structure and diversity
Culture shapes neutral genetic variation in space
When cultural knowledge strongly affects habitat use or patterns of dispersal through the social learning of habitat preferences, migration routes or foraging methods that select for different habitats, then cultural knowledge can be unevenly distributed across the range of a species. If neutral genes are being transmitted in parallel with these cultural traits—which would necessitate relatively stable vertically transmitted cultures—persistent geographical patterns of genetic variation can result. For example, in Shark Bay, Australia, foraging techniques of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus) that are transmitted from mother to daughter have set up a distinctive spatial structure of mitochondrial haplotypes over scales of a few kilometres, which the dolphins could traverse in minutes. Haplotypes characteristic of a feeding type that is used in deep water map onto the greater depths, whereas in the neighbouring shallows other haplotypes predominate86.
Through culturally facilitated dispersal, adaptation to culturally modified environments, and the effects of cultural practices on admixture, culture has directly or indirectly shaped much of the current geography of human genes48,87. Similar effects may arise from culture in other species32. Migration routes, or navigation strategies (e.g. ‘follow the coast’), learned from parents can set up geographical patterns in neutral genetic variation at either or both ends of the route88. Cultural transmission of migration routes is common among birds89, especially long-lived species, and cetaceans, especially baleen whales; in both cases, geographical patterns of genetic variation result79,90. A particularly remarkable case is that of the beluga whales (Delphinapterus leucas), where strict matrilineal migration cultures have retained distinct mitochondrial haplotype distributions on summering grounds in eastern and western Hudson Bay, reflecting different glacial refuges of maternal ancestors, even though the whales from each side of the bay meet and mate in winter and during migration91. In contrast, some species, such as barnacle geese (Branta leucopsis) and southern right whales (Eubalaena australis), tend to mate within migratory cultures, leading to spatial patterns in nuclear as well as mitochondrial DNA79,90. Geographical effects reappear in other sections of this review, where we consider how culture may segregate populations, leading to population structure and in some cases seeding speciation, reducing diversity, or selecting for particular functional genes (Fig. 1).
Culture can reduce genetic diversity
Culture can potentially reduce genetic diversity in socially structured populations through two related processes: cultural hitchhiking and culturally mediated migration92. In cultural hitchhiking, culture creates heritable variation in reproductive success or survival between different groups, so that the diversity of neutral genes transmitted in parallel with the cultural traits (i.e., vertically) is reduced to essentially those genes in the original groups with successful cultures93. In culturally mediated migration, culture sets up barriers or divisions within a population, inhibiting dispersal and mating. Then, the diversity of neutral as well as functional genes is reduced through processes such as bottlenecks and selection94. Cultural hitchhiking, as well as culturally mediated migration, require cultures that affect fitness or dispersal, respectively, that are quite stable, and that do not frequently transfer between population segments.
Both cultural hitchhiking and culturally mediated migration have been considered drivers of the very-low mitochondrial DNA diversity of five species of whale that have matrilineal social systems in which daughters typically stay grouped with their mothers while both are alive95. Female sperm whale (Physeter macrocephalus) populations are partitioned into sympatric, matrilineally based clans with distinct cultures and fitness differences, so that their exceptionally low mitochondrial diversity is consistent with cultural hitchhiking95. The division of killer whales into ecotypes is much deeper77. A strong mutual antipathy of proto-ecotypes is supported by genomic results, suggesting that a form of culturally mediated migration may be behind the killer whales’ low genetic diversity95.
In all these cases, it is difficult to rule out all non-cultural drivers of low genetic diversity (such as bottlenecks, population divisions, background selection or selective sweeps without cultural input), and several processes may have contributed92. However, it is telling that both hominins and those matrilineal whale species with particularly clear evidence that culture affects behaviour and fitness10,37 have remarkably low genetic diversity. Similar processes may also have affected genetic diversity in birds92,96,97, as described below.
Culture may drive the early phases of speciation
For speciation there are two central requirements—the separation of an existing species into groups, and reproductive isolation in the face of secondary contact. As culture can establish behavioural differences among initial population divisions more rapidly than genetic inheritance and local adaptation77,88,98, the role culture plays in influencing divergent selection has attracted theoretical and empirical attention, particularly concerning traits and preferences involved in assortative mating99.
Several researchers have argued100,101 (supported by population genetic models98,102,103) that the influence of culture on speciation depends on whether only the trait involved in mate choice is learned socially, or whether the preference is also learned. Learned-trait models (where the preference is genetically determined but the signal is socially transmitted) indicate that if culture reduces stabilising selection against new alleles affecting learning predispositions, then genetic divergence in mating preferences will arise more quickly than when traits are genetically inherited98,102. However, if mating traits are learned obliquely and independently of the tutor’s fitness, then in theory selection will be inhibited as the trait is learned from both mated and unmated tutors103. Cultural transmission of traits may even break down incipient speciation if secondary contact occurs because learning can mask genotypic variation103,104; the trait can be rapidly adjusted through learning (e.g., conformity of song dialect) to match the preferences of either population. For this reason, cultural transmission is primarily thought to inhibit sympatric speciation100.
However, where mating signals and receiver preferences are both culturally transmitted (e.g., male songbirds learn a dialect and females learn a preference from the same tutors) culture will instead promote speciation under both allopatric98 and secondary contact102 scenarios. Learned preferences will increase the strength of divergent selection acting on the trait and prevent recombination, amplifying both cultural and genetic evolution of associated loci102. Biases to learning (e.g., songs and preferences are learned during a sensitive period) may restrict individuals shifting phenotypes to match local preferences and further limit gene flow99.
Studies of culturally transmitted song in birds have generated the best lines of evidence for the role of culture in speciation105, with recent work demonstrating that song learning increases rates of species diversification across clades106. White-crowned sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys) song dialects vary strongly over small spatial scales, with restricted gene flow between dialect groups97. Recently, genomic studies of neighbouring subspecies (Z.l. nutalli and Z.l. pugetensis) demonstrated that song dialects likely act as a barrier to gene flow, with the strength of song discrimination correlated with genetic distance between sites96. Among indigobirds (Vidua), which are brood parasites, cultural transmission occurs across species, as males learn the song of their host species and females imprint upon that same song107. Switching host species therefore creates strong isolating barriers that have led to rapid recent speciation in sympatry108. Finally, culture-shaping speciation may even have been captured in action with the recent establishment of an apparently isolated lineage of Galápagos finches (Geospizinae) derived from the hybrid mating of a cactus finch (G. conirostris) immigrant male with a medium ground finch (G. fortis)—termed the ‘Big Bird’ lineage109. Twenty years ago, Grant and Grant44 showed that Darwin’s finches acquire their song-type and preference from their father (or foster-father). Although females do not sing, they mate assortatively according to their culturally inherited song type as well as by imprinting on parental morphology; culture therefore facilitates pre-zygotic reproductive isolation by constraining female mate choice, even in the absence of genetic penalties110. Together with changes in beak size, in only three generations this song culture has most probably contributed to allele segregation and incipient speciation of the Big Bird lineage109, although experiments demonstrating this mechanism remain to be done.
Finally, by coupling changes in population structure and modified selection on functional genes, cultural transmission of foraging traits or preferences could also help facilitate genetic divergence. Killer whale population structure is strongly influenced by culturally transmitted specialisations on particular prey resources, giving rise to different killer whale ‘ecotypes’ that can be distinguished behaviourally and morphologically77. There are significant genetic divergences between these lineages both in mitochondrial111 and nuclear42 genomes. It is believed that the ecotypes arose from cultural niche specialisation by matrilineal groups, which later developed reproductive barriers, culturally (e.g., through learned aggression towards outgroups) and/or genetically77. In killer whales, culturally transmitted behaviour has therefore triggered the evolution of multiple lineages considered to be currently undergoing incipient ecological speciation77. Although some have suggested they should now be considered different species111, these ecotypes are not currently recognised as such. The genetic evidence suggests that the present day global diversity of killer whales has arisen in the last 250 kya, or ~8000 generations42, but the genus Orcinus has been present in the fossil record for up to 5 My—so why are estimates of ecotype divergence so relatively recent? The most likely explanation is that the formation of ecotype lineages is a dynamic process, with learning and cultural transmission at its heart, both driving the exploitation of new niches as they arise and leading to increased specialisation risk, resulting in lineage extirpation112. Thus, in killer whales, cultural transmission seemingly underlies adaptive radiation into ecotypes and then incipient species, but can also increase local extirpation risk. The social learning of foraging techniques or habitat preferences by young animals has the potential to trigger genetic radiation in a variety of taxa, but this has been little investigated beyond killer whales.
The precise nature of the interactions between individuals with alternative cultures may be key to speciation, and to other processes of gene–culture coevolution. However, such interactions, almost inevitably, are rarely observed. How conspecifics whose cultures have begun to differentiate respond to each other, for example, by conformist convergence or alternatively by avoidance and conflicts, is a topic that can benefit from complementary observations in the field and experiments.
Conclusions and future directions
Until now, consideration of gene–culture coevolution has largely focussed on the evolution of functional genes in human populations1,31,54. Our review extends the reach of this discussion by suggesting that culture may change the nature of the evolutionary process in diverse ways, and for many animals. Culture provides a form of inheritance that is additional to genes and our review indicates it is far from trivial in its consequences for genetic evolution; moreover the two inheritance streams can interact to influence each other’s evolution3,11,30,46,113.
Human culture, so prolific and dominant across the globe, has had a dramatic impact on genetic evolution in other species114, a process for which we now have preliminary evidence at least, in a diversity of other species. There is good evidence for both humans and non-humans that culture can shape geographic patterns of genes, and comparative phylogenetic evidence that it has driven species-wide genetic traits, such as aspects of brains and life-histories. The cultures of killer whales offer a case study of how culture may drive the evolution of functional genes within population segments, and the ecotype-fissured killer whales offer a simpler system than human culture within which to delineate fundamentals of such processes.
Two processes of gene–culture coevolution were first considered for non-humans, with their relevance for our own species explored later. Cultural hitchhiking was conceived to explain low genetic diversity in cetaceans93, and was later extended to humans94,115. Culturally mediated speciation was a topic first introduced for birds44 and whales77, but might also apply to hominins. It is now well-established that Neanderthals, Denisovans and Homo floresiensis coexisted with modern humans and possessed distinct behavioural repertoires116. Culturally transmitted ecological specialisations may have led to radiations comparable to that of killer whales in our own evolutionary past, possibly on multiple occasions.
A sizeable body of experimental data, mathematical theory and comparative analyses collectively attests to the potential significance of gene–culture coevolution in animals. However, at this stage conclusions must often be regarded as tentative. This is primarily because few studies have genetic and cultural data that are directly comparable in terms of the organisms or groups of organisms sampled, or in the spatial and temporal scopes of the data. Second, our evidence comes predominantly from studies of non-human primates, birds and cetaceans, taxa for which cultural transmission has received the most attention to date5,37,117 (Table 1; Fig. 2) but which limits scope for generalisations. Comparing across taxonomic groups is further complicated by the differing foci on various aspects of culture, such as song for birds, foraging for primates, and mate-copying for insects. It would be helpful if more systematic and comparable measures were adopted to capture cultural content (foraging, navigation, song etc.), social transmission mechanisms (copying, teaching, etc.) and the distribution patterns of cultural variants as they evolve regionally and over generations. Ideally, future studies would systematically assemble coupled data on both genomics and the diverse dimensions of cultural complexity to facilitate comparative analyses of the gene–culture coevolutionary hypotheses that we have outlined above.
Table 1.
Process of gene–culture coevolution | Typical or relevant characteristics of culture (not necessarily sufficient) | Stronger evidence | Weaker evidence | Circumstantial evidence |
---|---|---|---|---|
Culture selects functional genes | Transgenerational transmission of phenotypes typically stable; strong selection derives from behavioural homogeneity; conformity. | Humans (e.g., LCT, AMY1, HbS…)31,48 Killer whales (e.g., methionine-expressed alleles, …)42 | Birds50 | Non-human primatesa |
Culture favours adaptations for culture | Complex and diverse learning environment created by culture; adaptive advantages of strategic and high-fidelity copying; socially transmitted technology; conformity. | Humans2,48,54,66,124 | Cetaceans37 Non-human primates59,61,63,64 Birds46 | |
Culture generates selection across species | Transgenerational transmission of phenotypes modifies selection on other species. | Humans48,80,114 | Birds18,74,76 | Killer whalesa |
Culture shapes neutral genetic variation in space | Stable, transgenerational transmission of phenotypes. | Humans48,125 Long-lived birds90 Beluga whales91 Baleen whales79 Bottlenose dolphins86 | ||
Culture can reduce genetic diversity | Stable, transgenerational transmission of phenotypes; behavioural homogeneity. | Sperm whales95 Killer whales95 Humans48,94 | Pilot whales95 False killer whales95 Birds92 | |
Culture may drive the early phases of speciation | Stable, transgenerational transmission of phenotypes; behavioural homogeneity; conformity. | Birds44,48,98,126 Killer whales77 Humans48 | Homininsa |
Cases are designated as ‘stronger evidence’ where there exists compelling experimental, theoretical or correlational data that imply gene–culture coevolution. Cases with ‘weaker evidence’ are those where experimental, theoretical or correlational data are consistent with gene–culture coevolution but where plausible alternative explanations have not been ruled out. Cases described as ‘circumstantial’ are those in which gene–culture coevolution has been proposed but not yet investigated
aProposal made in the text
Another limitation of the current state of knowledge is that a large portion of available evidence is correlational. Experimental studies would add clarity, but many of the best-studied taxa tend not to be well suited for manipulative experiments. Birds are an exception15, but the extensive evidence now accruing for social learning in insects118 makes this taxonomic group a promising area for development of the field119. For example, a recent study of a parasitoid wasp species (Lariophagus distinguendus) shows that early learning of host preferences has facilitated the evolution of two isolated, potentially speciating, lineages120, and a recent finding that fruit flies (Drosophila melanogaster) can carry culturally transmitted mate preferences across multiple cultural generations121 suggests great potential for experimental evolution of both culture and genes122. The study of gene–culture coevolution beyond humans may be in its infancy, but sufficient evidence now exists to see it as an exciting and significant avenue for further focused empirical research.
Acknowledgements
We thank Katrina Falkenberg for help with design and production of the figures.
Author contributions
All authors wrote the manuscript.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Journal peer review information: Nature Communications thanks the anonymous reviewers for their contribution to the peer review of this work.
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Feldman MW, Laland KN. Gene-culture coevolutionary theory. Trends Ecol. Evol. 1996;11:453–457. doi: 10.1016/0169-5347(96)10052-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. Culture and the evolutionary process. (Chicago University Press, Chicago, IL, 1985).
- 3.Whiten A, Ayala F, Feldman MD, Laland KN. The extension of biology through culture. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2017;114:7775–7781. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1707630114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Hoppitt, W. & Laland, K. N. Social learning: An introduction to mechanisms, methods, and models. (Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ, 2013).
- 5.Slagsvold T, Wiebe KL. Social learning in birds and its role in shaping a foraging niche. Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. B. 2011;366:969–977. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Mercader J, et al. 4,300-year-old chimpanzee sites and the origins of percussive stone technology. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:3043–3048. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0607909104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Lachlan RF, Ratmann O, Nowicki S. Cultural conformity generates extremely stable traditions in bird song. Nat. Commun. 2018;9:2417. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-04728-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kirkpatrick M, Dugatkin LA. Sexual selection and the evolutionary effects of copying mate choice. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1994;34:443–449. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Gibson RM, Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL. Mate choice in lekking sage grouse revisited: the roles of vocal display, female site fidelity, and copying. Behav. Ecol. 1991;2:165–180. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Richerson, P. J. & Boyd, R. Not by genes alone: how culture transformed human evolution. (Chicago University Press, Chicago, IL, 2005).
- 11.Whiten A. A second inheritance system: the extension of biology through culture. Interface Focus. 2017;7:20160142. doi: 10.1098/rsfs.2016.0142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Chivers DP, Smith RJF. Chemical recognition of risky habitats is culturally transmitted among flathead minnows, Pimephales promelas (Osteichthyes, Cyprinidae) Ethology. 1995;99:286–296. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. & Feldman, M. W. Cultural transmission and evolution: a quantitative approach. (Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ, 1981). [PubMed]
- 14.Henrich J, Boyd R. The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. Evol. Hum. Behav. 1998;19:215–241. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Aplin LM, et al. Experimentally induced innovations lead to persistent culture via conformity in wild birds. Nature. 2015;518:538–541. doi: 10.1038/nature13998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Warner RR. Traditionality of mating-site preferences in a coral reef fish. Nature. 1988;335:719–721. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Heyes CM. Social learning in animals: categories and mechanisms. Biol. Rev. 1994;69:207–231. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185x.1994.tb01506.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Thorogood R, Davies NB. Cuckoos combat socially transmitted defenses of reed warbler hosts with a plumage polymorphism. Science. 2012;337:578–580. doi: 10.1126/science.1220759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Davies NB, Welbergen JA. Social transmission of a host defense against cuckoo parasitism. Science. 2009;324:1318–1320. doi: 10.1126/science.1172227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Russon, A. E. Innovation and creativity in forest-living rehabilitant orang-utans In:Animal Innovation (eds Reader, S. M. & Laland, K. N.). 279–306 (Oxford University Press, New York, NY, 2003).
- 21.Reader, S. M. & Laland, K. N. Animal innovation. (Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, 2003).
- 22.Goodall, J. The chimpanzees of Gombe: patterns of behavior. (Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, 1986).
- 23.Sargeant BL, Mann J. Developmental evidence for foraging traditions in wild bottlenose dolphins. Anim. Behav. 2009;78:715–721. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Allen J, Weinrich M, Hoppitt W, Rendell L. Network-based diffusion analysis reveals cultural transmission of lobtail feeding in humpback whales. Science. 2013;340:485–488. doi: 10.1126/science.1231976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Laland KN. Social learning strategies. Learn. Behav. 2004;32:4–14. doi: 10.3758/bf03196002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Rendell L, et al. Cognitive culture: theoretical and empirical insights into social learning strategies. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011;15:68–76. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Sarin S, Dukas R. Social learning about egg-laying substrates in fruitflies. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2009;276:4323–4328. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.1294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Seppanen JT, Forsman JT, Monkkonen M, Krams I, Salmi T. New behavioural trait adopted or rejected by observing heterospecific tutor fitness. Proc. Roy. Soc. B. 2011;278:1736–1741. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.1610. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Boyd R, Richerson PJ. Why culture is common, but cultural evolution is rare. Proc. Brit. Acad. 1996;88:77–93. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Laland KN, et al. The extended evolutionary synthesis: its structure, assumptions and predictions. Proc. Roy. Soc. B. 2015;282:20151019. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2015.1019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Creanza N, Kolodny O, Feldman MW. Cultural evolutionary theory: how culture evolves and why it matters. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2017;114:7782–7789. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1620732114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Whitehead H. Gene–culture coevolution in whales and dolphins. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2017;114:7814–7821. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1620736114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Borenstein E, Meilijson I, Ruppin E. The effect of phenotypic plasticity on evolution in multipeaked fitness landscapes. J. Evol. Biol. 2006;19:1555–1570. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01125.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Anderson RW. Learning and evolution: a quantitative genetics approach. J. Theor. Biol. 1995;175:89–101. doi: 10.1006/jtbi.1995.0123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Ancel LW. Undermining the Baldwin expediting effect: does phenotypic plasticity accelerate evolution? Theor. Popul. Biol. 2000;58:307–319. doi: 10.1006/tpbi.2000.1484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Frank SA. Natural selection. II. Developmental variability and evolutionary rate. J. Evol. Biol. 2011;24:2310–2320. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2011.02373.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Whitehead, H. & Rendell, L. The cultural lives of whales and dolphins. (Chicago University Press, Chicago, IL, 2015).
- 38.Mills, R. &Watson, R. A. On crossing fitness valleys with the Baldwin effect (Proceedings of the Tenth International Conference on the Simulation and Synthesis of Living Systems, MIT Press, 2006).
- 39.Hinton GE, Nowlan SJ. How learning can guide evolution. Complex Syst. 1987;1:495–502. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Levis NA, Pfennig DW. Evaluating ‘plasticity-first’ evolution in nature: key criteria and empirical approaches. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2016;31:563–574. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2016.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.West-Eberhard, M. J. Developmental plasticity and evolution. (Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, 2003).
- 42.Foote AD, et al. Genome-culture coevolution promotes rapid divergence of killer whale ecotypes. Nat. Commun. 2016;7:11693. doi: 10.1038/ncomms11693. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Hoelzel A, Moura A. Killer whales differentiating in geographic sympatry facilitated by divergent behavioural traditions. Heredity. 2016;117:481–482. doi: 10.1038/hdy.2016.112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Grant BR, Grant PR. Cultural inheritance of song and its role in the evolution of Darwin's finches. Evolution. 1996;50:2471–2487. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1996.tb03633.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Beltman J, Haccou P, Ten Cate C. The impact of learning foster species' song on the evolution of specialist avian brood parasitism. Behav. Ecol. 2003;14:917–923. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Lachlan RF, Slater PJ. The maintenance of vocal learning by gene–culture interaction: the cultural trap hypothesis. Proc. R. Soc. B. 1999;266:701–706. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Beja-Pereira A, et al. Gene-culture coevolution between cattle milk protein genes and human lactase genes. Nat. Genet. 2003;35:311. doi: 10.1038/ng1263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Laland KN, Odling-Smee J, Myles S. How culture shaped the human genome: bringing genetics and the human sciences together. Nat. Rev. Genet. 2010;11:137–148. doi: 10.1038/nrg2734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Whiten A, et al. Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature. 1999;399:682–685. doi: 10.1038/21415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Bosse M, et al. Recent natural selection causes adaptive evolution of an avian polygenic trait. Science. 2017;358:365–368. doi: 10.1126/science.aal3298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Lefebvre L. The opening of milk bottles by birds: evidence for accelerating learning rates, but against the wave-of-advance model of cultural transmission. Behav. Process. 1995;34:43–53. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(94)00051-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Pruitt JN, Pinter-Wollman N. The legacy effects of keystone individuals on collective behaviour scale to how long they remain within a group. Proc. Roy. Soc. B. 2015;282:20151766. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2015.1766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Aplin LM, Farine DR, Morand-Ferron J, Sheldon BC. Social networks predict patch discovery in a wild population of songbirds. Proc. Roy. Soc. B. 2012;279:4199–4205. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.1591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Richerson PJ, Boyd R, Henrich J. Gene-culture coevolution in the age of genomics. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:8985–8992. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0914631107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Edmonds CA, Lillie AS, Cavalli-Sforza LL. Mutations arising in the wave front of an expanding population. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2004;101:975–979. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308064100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Whiten A, van Schaik CP. The evolution of animal ‘cultures’ and social intelligence. Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. B. 2007;362:603–620. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Moll H, Tomasello M. Cooperation and human cognition: the Vygotskian intelligence hypothesis. Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. B. 2007;362:639–648. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Burkart JM, Schubiger MN, van Schaik CP. Future directions for studying the evolution of general intelligence. Behav. Brain Sci. 2017;40:e195. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X17000024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.van Schaik CP, Burkart JM. Social learning and evolution: the cultural intelligence hypothesis. Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. B. 2011;366:1008–1016. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Wilson AC. The molecular basis of evolution. Sci. Am. 1985;253:164–173. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican1085-164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Reader SM, Laland KN. Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2002;99:4436–4441. doi: 10.1073/pnas.062041299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Wilson, A. C. From molecular evolution to body and brain evolution In:Perspectives on cellular regulation: from bacteria to cancer (eds Campisi, J. & Pardee, A. B.). 331–340 (J. Wiley & Sons, Pardee, NY, 1991).
- 63.Street SE, Navarrete AF, Reader SM, Laland KN. Coevolution of cultural intelligence, extended life history, sociality, and brain size in primates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2017;114:7908–7914. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1620734114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Navarrete, A. F., Reader, S. M., Street, S. E., Whalen, A. & Laland, K. N. The coevolution of innovation and technical intelligence in primates. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. B371, 10.1098/rstb.2015.0186 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 65.Overington SE, Morand-Ferron J, Boogert NJ, Lefebvre L. Technical innovations drive the relationship between innovativeness and residual brain size in birds. Anim. Behav. 2009;78:1001–1010. [Google Scholar]
- 66.González-Forero M, Gardner A. Inference of ecological and social drivers of human brain-size evolution. Nature. 2018;557:554. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0127-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Forss SI, Willems E, Call J, van Schaik CP. Cognitive differences between orang-utan species: a test of the cultural intelligence hypothesis. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:30516. doi: 10.1038/srep30516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Ellis S, et al. Analyses of ovarian activity reveal repeated evolution of post-reproductive lifespans in toothed whales. Sci. Rep. 2018;8:12833. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-31047-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Johnstone RA, Cant MA. The evolution of menopause in cetaceans and humans: the role of demography. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 2010;277:3765–3771. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Brent LJN, et al. Ecological knowledge, leadership, and the evolution of menopause in killer whales. Curr. Biol. 2015;25:746–750. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Langmore N, et al. Learned recognition of brood parasitic cuckoos in the superb fairy-wren Malurus cyaneus. Behav. Ecol. 2012;23:798–805. [Google Scholar]
- 72.Feeney WE, Langmore NE. Social learning of a brood parasite by its host. Biol. Lett. 2013;9:20130443. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2013.0443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Campobello D, Sealy SG. Use of social over personal information enhances nest defense against avian brood parasitism. Behav. Ecol. 2011;22:422–428. [Google Scholar]
- 74.Thorogood R, Davies NB. Combining personal with social information facilitates host defences and explains why cuckoos should be secretive. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:19872. doi: 10.1038/srep19872. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.San-Jose LM, et al. Effect of the MC1R gene on sexual dimorphism in melanin-based colorations. Mol. Ecol. 2015;24:2794–2808. doi: 10.1111/mec.13193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Thorogood R, Kokko H, Mappes J. Social transmission of avoidance among predators facilitates the spread of novel prey. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 2018;2:254–261. doi: 10.1038/s41559-017-0418-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Riesch R, Barrett-Lennard LG, Ellis GM, Ford JKB, Deecke VB. Cultural traditions and the evolution of reproductive isolation: ecological speciation in killer whales? Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 2012;106:1–17. [Google Scholar]
- 78.Corkeron PJ, Connor RC. Why do baleen whales migrate? Mar. Mamm. Sci. 1999;15:1228–1245. [Google Scholar]
- 79.Carroll EL, et al. Cultural traditions across a migratory network shape the genetic structure of southern right whales around Australia and New Zealand. Sci. Rep. 2015;5:16182. doi: 10.1038/srep16182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Cook LM, Grant BS, Saccheri IJ, Mallet J. Selective bird predation on the peppered moth: the last experiment of Michael Majerus. Biol. Lett. 2012;8:609–612. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2011.1136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Pearce-Duvet JM. The origin of human pathogens: evaluating the role of agriculture and domestic animals in the evolution of human disease. Biol. Rev. 2006;81:369–382. doi: 10.1017/S1464793106007020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.O’Brien MJ, Laland KN. Genes, culture, and agriculture: An example of human niche construction. Curr. Anthropol. 2012;53:434–470. [Google Scholar]
- 83.Leadbeater E, Chittka L. Social transmission of nectar-robbing behaviour in bumble-bees. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 2008;275:1669–1674. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.0270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Alem S, et al. Associative mechanisms allow for social learning and cultural transmission of string pulling in an insect. PLoS Biol. 2016;14:e1002564. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002564. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Tung J, et al. Social networks predict gut microbiome composition in wild baboons. Elife. 2015;4:e05224. doi: 10.7554/eLife.05224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Kopps AM, et al. Cultural transmission of tool use combined with habitat specializations leads to fine-scale genetic structure in bottlenose dolphins. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2014;281:20133245. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.3245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Creanza N, Feldman MW. Worldwide genetic and cultural change in human evolution. Curr. Opin. Genet. Dev. 2016;41:85–92. doi: 10.1016/j.gde.2016.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Turbek SP, Scordato ESC, Safran RJ. The role of seasonal migration in population divergence and reproductive isolation. Trends. Ecol. Evol. 2018;33:164–175. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2017.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Mueller T, O’Hara RB, Converse SJ, Urbanek RP, Fagan WF. Social learning of migratory performance. Science. 2013;341:999–1002. doi: 10.1126/science.1237139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Jonker R, et al. Genetic consequences of breaking migratory traditions in barnacle geese Branta leucopsis. Mol. Ecol. 2013;22:5835–5847. doi: 10.1111/mec.12548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Brown Gladden JG, Ferguson MM, Clayton JW. Matriarchal genetic population structure of North America beluga whales Delphinapterus leucas (Cetacea: Monodontidae) Mol. Ecol. 1997;6:1033–1046. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-294x.1997.00275.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Premo LS. Hitchhiker’s guide to genetic diversity in socially structured populations. Curr. Zool. 2012;58:287–297. [Google Scholar]
- 93.Whitehead H. Cultural selection and genetic diversity in matrilineal whales. Science. 1998;282:1708–1711. doi: 10.1126/science.282.5394.1708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Premo LS, Hublin J. Culture, population structure, and low genetic diversity in Pleistocene hominins. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2009;106:33–37. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809194105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Whitehead H, Vachon F, Frasier TR. Cultural hitchhiking in the matrilineal whales. Behav. Genet. 2017;47:324–334. doi: 10.1007/s10519-017-9840-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Lipshutz SE, Overcast IA, Hickerson MJ, Brumfield RT, Derryberry EP. Behavioural response to song and genetic divergence in two subspecies of white‐crowned sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys) Mol. Ecol. 2017;26:3011–3027. doi: 10.1111/mec.14002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.MacDougall‐Shackleton EA, MacDougall‐Shackleton SA. Cultural and genetic evolution in mountain white‐crowned sparrows: song dialects are associated with population structure. Evolution. 2001;55:2568–2575. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2001.tb00769.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Lachlan RF, Servedio MR. Song learning accelerates allopatric speciation. Evolution. 2004;58:2049–2063. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2004.tb00489.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Verzijden MN, et al. The impact of learning on sexual selection and speciation. Trends. Ecol. Evol. 2012;27:511–519. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2012.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Verzijden MN, et al. The impact of learned mating traits on speciation is not yet clear: response to Kawecki. Tr.ends Ecol. Evol. 2013;28:69–70. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2012.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Kawecki TJ. The impact of learning on selection-driven speciation. Trends. Ecol. Evol. 2013;28:68–69. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2012.10.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Yeh DJ. Assortative mating by an obliquely transmitted local cultural trait promotes genetic divergence: a model. Am. Nat. 2019;193:81–92. doi: 10.1086/700958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Yeh DJ, Servedio MR. Reproductive isolation with a learned trait in a structured population. Evolution. 2015;69:1938–1947. doi: 10.1111/evo.12688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Olofsson H, Servedio MR. Sympatry affects the evolution of genetic versus cultural determination of song. Behav. Ecol. 2008;19:596–604. [Google Scholar]
- 105.Edwards SV, et al. Speciation in birds: Genes, geography, and sexual selection. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2005;102:6550–6557. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0501846102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Mason NA, et al. Song evolution, speciation, and vocal learning in passerine birds. Evolution. 2017;71:786–796. doi: 10.1111/evo.13159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Payne RB, Payne LL, Woods JL, Sorenson MD. Imprinting and the origin of parasite–host species associations in brood-parasitic indigobirds, Vidua chalybeata. Anim. Behav. 2000;59:69–81. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1999.1283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Sorenson MD, Sefc KM, Payne RB. Speciation by host switch in brood parasitic indigobirds. Nature. 2003;424:928. doi: 10.1038/nature01863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Lamichhaney S, et al. Rapid hybrid speciation in Darwin's finches. Science. 2018;359:224–228. doi: 10.1126/science.aao4593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Grant PR, Grant BR. Role of sexual imprinting in assortative mating and premating isolation in Darwin’s finches. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2018;115:E10879–E10887. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1813662115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Morin PA, et al. Complete mitochondrial genome phylogeographic analysis of killer whales (Orcinus orca) indicates multiple species. Genome Res. 2010;20:908–916. doi: 10.1101/gr.102954.109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Whitehead H, Ford JKB. Consequences of culturally-driven ecological specialization: killer whales and beyond. J. Theor. Biol. 2018;456:279–294. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2018.08.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Lachlan R, Feldman M. Evolution of cultural communication systems: the coevolution of cultural signals and genes encoding learning preferences. J. Evol. Biol. 2003;16:1084–1095. doi: 10.1046/j.1420-9101.2003.00624.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Alberti M, et al. Global urban signatures of phenotypic change in animal and plant populations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2017;114:8951–8956. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1606034114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Whitehead H, Richerson PJ, Boyd R. Cultural selection and genetic diversity in humans. Selection. 2002;3:115–125. [Google Scholar]
- 116.Scerri EM, et al. Did our species evolve in subdivided populations across Africa, and why does it matter? Trends Ecol. Evol. 2018;33:582–594. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2018.05.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Whiten A. The scope of culture in chimpanzees, humans and ancestral apes. Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. B. 2011;366:997–1007. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Leadbeater E, Chittka L. Social learning in insects—from miniature brains to consensus building. Curr. Biol. 2007;17:R703–R713. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Leadbeater E, Dawson EH. A social insect perspective on the evolution of social learning mechanisms. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2017;114:7838–7845. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1620744114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.König K, et al. Does early learning drive ecological divergence during speciation processes in parasitoid wasps? Proc. R. Soc. B. 2015;282:20141850. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.1850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Danchin E, et al. Cultural flies: Conformist social learning in fruitflies predicts long-lasting mate-choice traditions. Science. 2018;362:1025–1030. doi: 10.1126/science.aat1590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Whiten A. Culture and conformity shape fruitfly mating. Science. 2018;362:998–999. doi: 10.1126/science.aav5674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Laland KN, Janik VM. The animal cultures debate. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2006;21:542–547. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2006.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Kaplan H, Hill K, Lancaster J, Hurtado AM. A theory of human life history evolution: diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evol. Anthr. 2000;9:156–185. [Google Scholar]
- 125.Allentoft ME, et al. Population genomics of Bronze Age Eurasia. Nature. 2015;522:167. doi: 10.1038/nature14507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Laland KN. On the evolutionary consequences of sexual imprinting. Evolution. 1994;48:477–489. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1994.tb01325.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]