Skip to main content
SSM - Population Health logoLink to SSM - Population Health
. 2019 May 30;8:100413. doi: 10.1016/j.ssmph.2019.100413

Social determinants of multimorbidity and multiple functional limitations among the ageing population of England, 2002–2015

Leo Singer a,, Mark Green a, Francisco Rowe a, Yoav Ben-Shlomo b, Karyn Morrissey c
PMCID: PMC6551564  PMID: 31194123

Abstract

This study explores longitudinal relationships between material, psycho-social and behavioural social determinants of health and multimorbidity of people aged 50 years or older in England. We used data from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing collected biannually between 2002 and 2015. Apart from the basic measure of multimorbidity (two or more diseases within a person) we constructed two distinct measures of health in order to take into account the biology of ageing (complex multimorbidity and multiple functional limitations).

We found that the likelihood of multimorbidity and multiple functional limitations was consistently associated with the levels of household wealth, sense of control over one's life, physical activity and loneliness. Larger health inequalities were observed when health was measured as complex multimorbidity and multiple functional limitations than basic multimorbidity. Compared to the population group with the highest wealth, those with the lowest wealth had 47% higher odds of basic multimorbidity (95% C.I. 1.34-1.61), 73% higher odds of complex multimorbidity (95% C.I. 1.52-1.96) and 90% higher odds of having 10 or more functional limitations (95% C.I. 1.59-2.26). We did not find a dose-response relationship between alcohol consumption, smoking and multimorbidity but rather evidence of people in ill health actively moderating their health behaviour.

We suggest that materialist models of multimorbidity and functional limitation at older age can not, on their own, explain the health inequalities as the behavioural and psycho-social factors play an important role. Policies aiming to reduce the risk of multimorbidity and functional limitation should address the issue at these three levels simultaneously, using the existing national infrastructure of General Practices.

Keywords: Multimorbidity, Complex multimorbidity, Multiple functional limitations, Social determinansts of health, Ageing, Health inequalities

Highlights

  • Multimorbidity and functional limitation were associated with household wealth, sense of control over life, physical activity, loneliness.

  • We observed larger health inequalities for complex multimorbidity and multiple functional limitation than basic multimorbidity.

  • Materialist models of multimorbidity do not explain inequalities.

  • Behavioural and psycho-social factors play an important role.

1. Introduction

Multimorbidity, the co-occurrence of two or more diseases within a person, affects over a quarter of primary care patients older than 18 years of age in England (Cassell et al., 2018). Individuals with multimorbidity have higher rates of GP consultations, prescriptions, and hospitalisations compared to people without multimorbidity (Salisbury, Johnson, Purdy, Valderas, & Montgomery, 2011; Cassell et al., 2018). Multimorbidity also leads to lower health-related quality of life (Bayliss et al., 2012; Peters et al., 2018) and decline in physical functioning (Jindai et al., 2016). Multimorbidity among people older than 65 years in England is set to rise with prevalence projected to increase from 54% in 2015 to 67.8% in 2035 (Kingston et al., 2018). People will live longer lives in worse health and this will increase the utilization of health services and the costs of health care (Cassell et al., 2018; Kingston et al., 2018).

While current studies of multimorbidity focus on the impact of biomedical and socio-demographic characteristics on patients’ individual risk (Northwood, Ploeg, Markle-Reid, & Sherifali, 2018), we also need to understand the extra-individual factors contributing to the increase of multiple health problems in the ageing population. Only a few studies have examined simultaneously longitudinal trends in multimorbidity and their relationship with extra-individual factors such as society and environment (Dhalwani et al., 2017; Jackson, Dobson, Tooth, & Mishra, 2015; Mounce et al., 2018; Schäfer et al., 2012). None of them referred to any theoretical framework that would justify the choice of the contextual characteristics. This leads to the risk of omitting relevant factors which might explain more of the outcome variance and to exaggerating effects of the observed characteristics (Frohlich, Corin, & Potvin, 2001). Choosing an appropriate measure should be backed by theory too. For instance, education and income reflect different mechanisms through which socio-economic status operates (Demakakos, Nazroo, Breeze, & Marmot, 2008). We argue that multimorbidity should be studied with the help of the theories of social determinants of health (SDoH). These refer to the social, cultural, economic, and political conditions that influence the health of individuals and populations (De Maio, Mazzeo, & Ritchie, 2013; Lucyk & McLaren, 2017).

Multimorbidity in older people may have a different profile than that found in younger people. For example the prevalence of cardiovascular and neurological patterns (including dementia and Alzheimer's disease) in England increases with age while mental health disorders are more common among younger people (Public Health England, 2018). Further, we suggest that measuring multiple health problems of older people should be consistent with our knowledge of biological ageing. The concept of multimorbidity should reflect the build-up of damage within cells (Austad, 2009; Barnes, 2015; Kirkwood, 2008) that accumulates during the life course and leads to a chronic dysregulation of multiple body systems (Fabbri et al., 2015; Li et al., 2015). Accumulation of diseases is a milestone for system dysregulation, loss of resilience and accelerated ageing (Fabbri et al., 2015). The role of body systems in development of multimorbidity is beginning to receive some attention (Yarnall et al., 2017).

Our study seeks to address these gaps in understanding multimorbidity of ageing people. Along the basic definition of multimorbidity (Van den Akker, Buntinx, Metsemakers, Roos, & Knottnerus, 1998) we propose two measures of health which in our view better reflect the biological process of ageing: complex multimorbidity (Harrison, Britt & Henderson, 2014) and multiple functional limitations. These outcomes should not be omitted when studying multimorbidity as they have implications for quality of life, need for health care and residential care, and premature mortality of old people (Jindai, Nielson, Vorderstrasse, & Quiñones, 2016; Zulman, Pal & Wagner, 2015). Our approach is also novel in that it brings together new measures of multimorbidity and functional limitation with social theory of SDoH in a longitudinal design. The aim of our study is to explore the association of material, psycho-social and behavioural determinants to the probability of developing basic multimorbidity, complex multimorbidity and multiple functional limitations in the ageing population of England over a 14 year period.

1.1. Theoretical framework

Our starting point is the centrality of dysfunction in several body systems that is conducive to multiple impairment, limitation and disease. We postulate that if we can identify diverse social determinants that simultaneously affect an individual, changes could be observed across a number of body systems that will be involved in generating compound health outcomes. Here we follow the Generalized Health Impact model by White, O’Campo, Moineddin, and Matheson (2013) that showed how a combination of social determinants (stress, poverty or quality of housing) generated a range of host responses encompassing more than one health condition. This model informed our approach to the choice of social determinants and for measuring multimorbidity and multiple functional limitations.

1.2. Measuring multimorbidity and multiple functional limitations

Multimorbidity has been measured by a range of methods. In primary care settings, indices based on diagnostic or pharmaceutical data have been used such as Charlson Index, Adjusted Clinical Groups System or Cumulative Illness Index Rating Scale (Diederichs, Berger, & Bartels, 2011). Multimorbidity estimates in general populations are based on a simple unweighted enumeration of the number of diseases. The most common definition is “the co-occurrence of two or more diseases within a person” (Van den Akker et al., 1998) but different cut-off points have been used too (Marengoni et al., 2011). In our study this measure will be called basic multimorbidity in order to distinguish it from two other measures. The limitation of the concept of basic multimorbidity is that it leads to very high estimates among old people (55% to 98% between studies) which may be less informative than other definitions (Marengoni et al., 2011). Neither does it differentiate between the co-occurrence developing within one body system and two or more systems. Multimorbidity may have a larger impact on overall health if it arises out of disparate conditions (such as physical and mental health) rather than closely related comorbidities (Piette & Kerr, 2006; Yarnall et al., 2017).

The construct of complex multimorbidity addresses these issues. It has been defined as “the co-occurrence of three or more chronic conditions affecting three or more different body systems within one person without an index chronic condition” (Harrison, Britt, Miller, & Henderson, 2014, p. 8). Individuals with chronic conditions in 3 + body systems may require more complex care, as chronic conditions in different body systems are likely to compete for treatment, while conditions within the same system are more likely to be complementary (Piette & Kerr, 2006). With regard to the theories of ageing based on dysregulation of body systems described earlier, we argue that complex multimorbidity might be a more appropriate measure for ageing people than basic multimorbidity.

Finally, the measure of multiple functional limitations provides an idea of the impact of multimorbidity on the ageing population. Functional limitations are defined as restrictions in performing vital situation-free physical actions needed in everyday life (Verbrugge & Jette, 1994). This aspect is important as some morbidities (e.g. high blood pressure) may have less of an impact on the quality of life than others (e.g. arthritis). The measure of multiple functional limitations reflects the knowledge that the proportion of old people with physical impairments and limitations in multiple body systems increases with age (Burden of Disease Network Project, 2004; Jindai et al., 2016). Most studies have explored prevalence and effects of single impairments or functional limitations but we know less about the relationships between combined burden of impairments and functional limitations and social determinants (Burden of Disease Network Project, 2004).

1.3. Social determinants

The theoretical approach to health inequalities and the role of SDoH in the UK was shaped by the publication of the Black Report in 1980 that concluded that material conditions were the major determinant of health and premature mortality (Black, 1992). This led to discussions between proponents of the materialist explanations and those who claimed that health inequalities are result of culturally mediated choices and behaviours (Bartley, 2004; Cockerham, 2007). The debate has been enriched by a third perspective, the role of psycho-social factors highlighted in the Whitehall II Study (Marmot et al., 1991). The current approach is to understand these hypotheses as complimentary rather than mutually exclusive and to assess their effects in one model with three groups of determinants (Robertson, Benzeval, Whitley, & Popham, 2015; Van Oort, Van Lenthe, & Mackenbach, 2005).

Material determinants refer to the distribution of income and wealth in society and to resources that allow people to secure goods and services needed for a healthy life, e.g. housing, healthcare (Bartley, 2004; Cockerham, 2007). Attained education can be interpreted as another type of material resource as it mediates health risks on the pathway between childhood conditions and occupational level, income and accumulated wealth in later life (Northwood, Ploeg, Markle-Reid, & Sherifali, 2018). Studies of the ageing population in England and the UK found disparities by socio-economic status (SES) for a range of health outcomes (Nazroo, Zaninotto, and Gjonca, 2008). The few longitudinal studies of multimorbidity showed associations with low education, low household income, difficulties managing on income and total household wealth (Jackson et al., 2015; Mounce et al., 2018; Schäfer et al., 2012). Lower level of education, manual occupation and poor social network predicted higher number of functional limitations in the Swedish population older than 60 years of age (Calderón-Larrañaga et al., 2018). Subjective social status (SSS) has been referred to as a subjective measure of SES as it reflects individual's perceived standing in a social hierarchy and hence can be included in the group of material determinants (Singh-Manoux, Marmot, & Adler, 2005). SSS also reflects perceptions of stress and the sense of social inequality (Charonis et al., 2017). To our knowledge there are no studies of how SSS is related to multimorbidity and only one study examined its association with functional decline (Chen, Covinsky, Cenzer, Adler, & Williams, 2012).

Psycho-social determinants, such as leisure and social activities and social networks and contacts, are increasingly more relevant to older people's idea of healthy ageing (Bowling, 2008; Cosco, Prina, & Perales, 2013). Social networks affect health via pathways such as provision of social support, social influence, social engagement and attachment, and access to resources and goods (Berkman & et al., 2010). Living as a couple, in a family, having a large social network and having a sense of control over one's life were all protective factors reducing the risk of multimorbidity (Marengoni et al., 2011; Melis, Marengoni, Angleman, & Fratiglioni, 2014). Older multimorbid people with a supportive social network have longer survival time compared to those without social support (Olaya et al., 2017). Loneliness has been found positively associated with multimorbidity in England, although the relationship was stronger for people younger than 44 than for people older than 65 (Stickley & Koyanagi, 2018). The stress-buffering hypothesis suggests that social relationships can provide resources that buffer the effect of stress on health (Gellert et al., 2018; Uchino, 2009). The direct effects' model says that social networks can facilitate positive health behaviours and access to health care by providing resources such as material assistance or transportation (Olaya et al., 2017).

Behavioural determinants describe different types of consumption and leisure activities that directly affect health and are, to some extent, subject to individual choice and decision-making (Bartley, 2004). Multimorbidity is associated with levels of physical activity, fruit and alcohol consumption, smoking tobacco and Body Mass Index (Dhalwani et al., 2017). However, while sociology of health has began to describe an interplay between human agency and social structure (Cockerham, 2007), health behaviours are still treated in isolation from other social determinants (Moor, Spallek, & Richter, 2016).

Each type of social determinant can work through any or several of the body systems (Blane, Kelly-Irving, D’Errico, Bartley, & Montgomery, 2013). For instance, occupation can affect respiratory, endocrine or cardiovascular system through toxins at work (Agency for Toxic Substances & Disease Registry, 2018) or nervous system and immune system through stress (Marmot et al., 1991). Smoking tobacco can affect nervous, respiratory, cardiovascular or digestive systems through both inhaled carcinogens and lower self-esteem (Bartley, 2004). These examples illustrate our assumption that the combined long-term impact of material, psycho-social and behavioural determinants should be sufficiently wide to be observable across a range of body systems through our measures of complex multimorbidity and multiple functional limitations.

2. Material and methods

2.1. Data

The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA) is a multidisciplinary panel study of a representative sample of men and women aged 50 years and over living in England. ELSA explores the dynamics between ageing and demographic, socio-economic, psychological and health factors. The study began in 2002 with 12,099 participants and the sample is re-examined every two years. It was replenished at waves 3, 4, 6 and 7 with new participants to maintain the size and representativeness of the study (Steptoe et al., 2013). We used data from the core members. ELSA defines core members as individuals who met the following survey criteria: they fit the age eligibility criteria (aged 50 years or older), took part in the original HSE survey that served as the basis for ELSA, and participated in wave 1 of ELSA or joined later as part of the refreshment samples. The cohabiting partners or household members are not included. Data on psycho-social characteristics come from self-completion interviews and all other data from personal interviews.

2.2. Dependent variables: measures of health

We used data on 25 physical and mental health conditions that were consistently recorded at each wave (Table 1). Respondents were asked whether they still had any of the medically diagnosed conditions or whether they had a new condition. This was coded as a binary variable with the value of 1 meaning the presence of a condition and 0 for its absence. The data were grouped into three categories: individual morbidities, groups representing body systems and functional limitations, a decision based on Verbrugge and Jette's Disablement Process Framework (1994). We decided to enlarge their category ‘functional limitations’ by including instances of impairment (dysfunction and abnormalities in body systems) and disability (difficulty with daily activities) (Table 1).

Table 1.

Health data used in the analysis.

Morbidities Body systems Functional limitations
1 High blood pressure 1. Eye disorders General mobility
2 Angina 1.1. Glaucoma 1 Walking 100 yards
3 Congested heart failure 1.2. Macular degeneration 2 Sitting for 2 hrs
4 Heart murmur 1.4. Cataracts 3 Getting up from chair
5 Abnormal heart rhythm 2. Circulatory disorders 4 Climbing several flights of stairs
6 Heart attack 2.1. High blood pressure 5 Climbing one flight of stairs
7 Diabetes 2.2. Angina 6 Stooping, kneeling or crouching
8 Stroke 2.3. Heart attack 7 Reaching arms above shoulders
9 Lung disease 2.4. Congestive heart failure 8 Pulling or pushing a chair
10 Asthma 2.5. Heart murmur 9 Lifting/carrying weights over 10 pounds
11 Arthritis 2.6. Abnormal heart rhythm 10 Picking up a 5p coin
12 Osteoporosis 2.7. Stroke Activities of daily living
13 Cancer 3. Endocrine, nutritional and metabolic 11 Dressing, including putting on shoes and socks
14 Parkinson's disease 3.1 Diabetic eye disease 12 Walking across a room
15 Dementia 3.2. Diabetes 13 Bathing or showering
16 Alzheimer's disease 4. Musculoskeletal and connective system 14 Eating, such as cutting up your food
17 Hallucinations 4.1. Osteoporosis 15 Getting in or out of bed
18 Anxiety 4.2. Arthritis 16 Using the toilet, including getting up or down
19 Depression 5. Respiratory 17 Using a map to figure out how to get around
20 Emotional problems 5.1. Lung disease 18 Preparing a hot meal
21 Mood swings 5.2. Asthma 19 Shopping for groceries
22 Glaucoma 6. Neoplasms 20 Making telephone calls
23 Diabetic eye disease 6.1. Cancers 21 Taking medications
24 Macular degeneration 7. Nervous disorders 22 Doing work around the house or garden
25 Cataracts 7.1. Parkinson's disease 23 Managing money (paying bills, track of expenses)
7.2. Alzheimer's disease Symptoms
7.3. Hallucinations 24 Difficulty walking 0.25 mile
8. Mental and behavioural 25 Pain in general
8.1. Anxiety 26 Problems with eyesight
8.2. Depression 27 Problems with hearing
8.3. Emotional problems 28 Balance on level surface
8.4. Mood swings 29 Dizzy walking on level surface

2.2.1. Measure 1: basic multimorbidity (MM)

A binary variable was created in order to identify participants at each wave who had two or more morbidities. At each wave this variable was composed of the data fed forward from the previous wave and the data on newly reported morbidities.

2.2.2. Measure 2: complex multimorbidity (CMM)

Individuals with three or more body systems affected by disease were considered as having CMM. Body systems were represented by the Chapters of the International Classification of Diseases 10th Revision (ICD-10) (Table 1). A patient with CMM had one or more chronic conditions within each of three or more different ICD-10 Chapters.

We also checked for potential biases between the basic measure of MM with CMM given that the cutpoint of two morbidities is used in the former while the latter uses three morbidities in distinct body systems. We created a version of basic MM with a cutpoint for 3 morbidities for fairer comparisons and re-ran all of our analyses (see Appendix).

2.2.3. Measure 3: multiple functional limitations (MFLs)

We derived the measure of MFLs from the combination of general mobility variables, Activities of Daily Living (ADL) variables, and data on symptoms of chronic conditions (Table 1). ADL is a common measure of the abilities necessary for basic functioning and for living in a community (Chatterji, Byles, Cutler, & et al., 2015). We counted the number of functional limitations per individual. The frequencies of MFLs per individual were high, reflecting the older age of participants and the large list of 30 limitations. Therefore we decided to specify the measure of MFLs as the presence of 10 or more functional limitations within the same person (MFL10+). This cut-off point allowed us to identify a group of participants with a total high functional limitation, compared to a cut-off point of three or five limitations.

2.3. Explanatory variables

Material SDoH were represented by variables: net household wealth (high, medium, low), subjective social status (high, medium, low), the last occupation (managerial/professional, intermediate, semi/routine), education (A-level or higher, O-level or equivalent and less than O-level). Psycho-social SDoH reflected aspects of social engagement, social support and individual sense of control. Social engagement was measured through individual participation in community organizations. A person was defined as very active if they took part in 3 or more community organizations and active if participated in 1 or 2. Perception of loneliness was a binary variable (yes/no). Social support was represented by variables supportive children (very/some, a little/not at all, no children), supportive friends (very/some, a little/not at all, no friends) and supportive partner (very/some, a little/not at all, no partner). The individual sense of control was measured by how often the respondent felt that what happened to them was out of their control. Four options (never, not often, sometimes, often) were grouped to three categories (never, not often/sometimes, often). Behavioural SDoH were represented by variables physical activity (vigorous, moderate, mild, none), alcohol consumption (never, monthly or less, weekly, daily) and tobacco smoking (never, ex-smoker, current smoker). Confounding variables were: wave (with values 1 to 7), age (categorized in 5-year bands and 90 + years age band) and a binary variable for sex.

3. Methods

To assess the relationships between multimorbidity and material, psycho-social and behavioural factors, we estimate a logistic panel data regression model which captures the temporal sequencing of events and accounts for temporal within-individual correlation. We favour an population-averaged regression model over subject-specific regression model, because these models are more appropriate for estimating the average influence of predictors on outcomes which is our focus: to estimate differences in the risk of multimorbidity between population groups and not between individuals (Muller & MacLehose, 2014). Additionally, population-averaged model do not require assumptions to be made about the distribution of the residuals (Liang & Zeger, 1986). The estimated model is:

logitPr(yit=1|xit)=β0+β1xit+eit (1)

where yit is a binary dependent variable with 1 indicating an individual i experience a MM, CMM or FLM event at time t; 0 otherwise; β0 is the regression constant; xit is a matrix of individual time-varying factors; β1 is a matrix of coefficients capturing the estimated strength of assocition between a multimorbidity event and an explanatory variable; eit is a vector of residuals.

To estimate Equation (1), we use Generalized Estimating Equations (GEEs). GEEs represent an extension of standard regression estimation procedures to allow for autocorrelation (Liang & Zeger, 1986). By allowing correlation for repeated observations on individuals over time, GEEs produce robust estimation of standard errors which are derived from the observed variability in the data, rather than variability predicted by an underlying probability model (Twisk, 2013). GEEs is a two-stage method. The first stage involves specifying and estimating an appropriate correlation structure, and the second stage using the estimated correlations structure to adjust the estimates of the logistic model parameters and standard errors for autocorrelation (Liang & Zeger, 1986). We analysed the correlation structure for each of our three multimorbidity measures and they show a decreasing correlation over time, which justified the choice of an autoregressive correlation structure (Twisk, 2013).

We separately estimate regression models for each of our three multimorbidity measures and also control for confounding factors of time (wave of measurement), age (varying between waves) and sex. Confounding factors and explanatory factors are included as covariates in our regression models. The associations were measured in odds ratios and it is a common measure of health inequalities in large population-based studies (Di Lorenzo et al., 2014). In order to give additional effect to those who dropped out of the analysis, we used longitudinal weights. They calculated the inverse predicted probability of response among respondents who responded to all previous waves and multiplied that weight by the previous wave's longitudinal weight (Banks et al., 2018). Analyses were conducted in Stata version 13.

4. Results

The general characteristics of the studied population are presented in Table 2. The number of participants decreased from 10,331 (wave 1) to 7,130 (wave 7). The retained population are those who remained in the study and took part in the self-completion interviews. The mean age was 66 years (SD 10.9). The proportion of women was 55.3%. All longitudinal studies are subject to problems with non-response and attrition and these problems are starker in studies of ageing where rates of attrition tend to be higher (Banks et al., 2016). In our study respondents who took part in all waves of measurements were different to those who dropped out or refused to fill in the self-completion interview. The retained cohort was slightly older (mean age 67 years compared to 66.2 years), more female (56.2% versus 54.9%) and more affluent (23.3% in the top wealth tertile compared to 21.6%). The core cohort was also more active, with a third conducting vigorous physical activity compared to 27% of those who dropped out at one or more occasions. A quarter of them was very active in their community compared to 17%. The problem of the differences between the two populations was less relevant for our analysis because we focused on all core members rather than those who took part in all waves of measurements.

Table 2.

Descriptive sample characteristics.

N % N %
Measurement occasions 56,202 Sense of control
Participants 15,046 26,179 46.6 High 15,513 27.6
Basic MM
Complex MM 9,663 17.19 Some 35,708 63.54
MFL10+ 6,319 11.24 Low 3,608 6.42



Mean age (SD) 66(10.9) Partner support
Age group (years) A lot/some 37,399 66.54
50-54 5,576 9.92 A little/not at all 1,549 2.76
55-59 10,074 17.92 No partner 16,953 30.16
60-64 10,434 18.57
65-69 9,569 17.03 Children support
70-74 8,166 14.53 A lot/some 43,693 77.74
75-79 6,233 11.09 A little/not at all 4,230 7.53
80-84 3,791 6.75 No children 7,895 14.05
85-89 1,789 3.18
90 plus 570 3.18 Friends' support
A lot/some 42,705 75.98
Females 31,106 55.3 A little/not at all 9,076 16.15
No friends 3,946 7.02
Physical Activity
Vigorous 16,327 29.05 Loneliness
Moderate 27,124 48.26 Yes 6,720 11.96
Mild 8,319 14.8
No 4,416 7.86 Household wealth
Top 12,244 21.79
Alcohol consumption Medium 32,358 57.57
Never 6,635 11.81 Low 10,490 18.66
Monthly or less 15,661 27.87
Weekly 14,111 25.11 Subjective social status
Daily 18,661 33.2 Top 9,080 16.16
Medium 38,025 67.66
Smoking Low 6,756 12.02
Never 21,041 37.44
Ex-smoker 27,526 48.98 Occupational level
Smoker 7,564 13.46 Managers/professionals 18,555 33.01
Intermediate 13,895 24.72
Participation Semi/routine 22,334 39.74
Very active 11,162 19.86
Active 26,940 47.93 Educational level
Not active 15,142 26.94 A-level+ 18,418 32.77
O-level or equiv. 14,436 25.69
Less than O-level 23,088 41.08

The following results in Table 3 – 6 were extracted from three logistic regression analyses, one per each health outcome, using the GEE method. Each model took account of all explanatory and confounding variables. The probability of people aged 50 or older in England to develop multimorbidity, complex multimorbidity and 10 or more functional limitations has increased between 2002 and 2014–15 (Table 3). Compared to 2002/03, the odds of having multimorbidity in 2014/15 were 2.33 times larger (95% CI 2.14–2.54), the odds of having complex multimorbidity 2.57 times larger (95% CI 2.29-2.88) and the odds of having ten or more MFLs twice larger (95% CI 1.77-2.31). The probability of having multimorbidity increases with age across the three measures. The increase peaked in multimorbidity at the age 85-89 years and in complex multimorbidity at the age 80-84 years. Female respondents were more likely to have basic MM (OR 1.31, 95% CI 1.21 – 1.41), complex MM (OR 1.26, 95% CI 1.14 – 1.38) and MFL10+ (OR 1.27, 95% CI 1.14-1.41).

Table 3.

Basic multimorbidity, complex multimorbidity and functional limitation by year of measurement, age and sex.


Basic MM

Complex MM

MFL10+

Odds ratio 95% CI Odds ratio 95% CI Odds ratio 95% CI
Year
2002/03 1 1 1
2004/05 0.87 0.82-0.92 1.06 0.96-1.17 1.34 1.17-1.52
2006/07 1.07 1.01-1.13 1.17 1.05-1.29 1.63 1.43-1.84
2008/09 1.42 1.33-1.52 1.79 1.62-1.99 1.33 1.15-1.49
2010/11 1.61 1.51-1.73 2.07 1.86-2.29 1.47 1.28-1.67
2012/13 1.69 1.56-1.82 2.26 2.03-2.52 1.35 1.18-1.54
2014/15
2.33
2.14-2.54
2.57
2.29-2.88
2.02
1.77-2.31
Age group (years)
50-54 1 1 1
55-59 1.23 1.11-1.37 1.36 1.09-1.68 1.15 0.99-1.33
60-64 1.61 1.43-1.81 1.97 1.57-2.48 1.27 1.07-1.50
65-69 2.16 1.90-2.45 2.63 2.08-3.33 1.31 1.10-1.57
70-74 2.95 2.59-3.38 3.52 2.76-4.48 1.52 1.27-1.83
75-79 4.14 3.59-4.79 4.37 3.41-5.60 1.72 1.48-2.16
80-84 4.98 4.24-5.86 5.41 4.19-7.01 2.23 1.82-2.71
85-89 5.89 4.81-7.23 4.56 3.42-6.00 2.86 2.27-3.59
90 plus
4.70
3.40-6.51
3.33
2.33-4.75
3.62
2.65-4.91
Females 1.31 1.21-1.41 1.26 1.14-1.38 1.27 1.14-1.41

4.1. Material determinants

We observed a health gradient across the three levels of household wealth in basic multimorbidity, complex multimorbidity and multiple functional limitations (Table 4). Compared to the population group with the highest wealth, those with the lowest wealth had 47% higher odds of basic MM, 73% higher odds of complex MM and 90% higher odds of 10 + functional limitations (Table 4). Low subjective social status was associated with higher odds of having all three outcomes compared to reporting high SSS, with odds ratios at 1.14 (95% CI 1.04-1.24), 1.2 (95% CI 1.07-1.35) and 1.37 (95% CI 1.26-1.70) respectively. People in routine or semi-routine occupations had higher odds of having basic multimorbidity and MFL10 + than people in the managerial and professional group, with odds ratios at 1.07 (95% CI 1.04-1.24) and 1.28 (95% CI 1.14-1.46) respectively. People with basic education had higher odds of having MFL10 + than people with at least A-levels (OR 1.12, 95% CI 1.01-1.22).

Table 4.

Basic multimorbidity, complex multimorbidity, functional limitation and material determinants.

Basic MM

Complex MM

MFL10+

Odds ratio 95% CI Odds ratio 95% CI Odds ratio 95% CI
Household wealth
High 1 1 1
Medium 1.13 1.10-1.19 1.20 1.09-1.31 1.28 1.12-1.47
Low 1.47 1.34-1.61 1.73 1.52-1.96 1.90 1.59-2.26
Subjective social status
High 1 1 1
Medium 1.04 0.98-1.10 1.11 1.00-1.20 1.15 1.02-1.29
Low 1.14 1.04-1.24 1.21 1.07-1.35 1.37 1.26-1.70
Occupation
Manager/prof. 1 1 1
Intermediate 0.93 0.85-1.01 0.92 0.81-1.03 1.04 0.91-1.20
Semi/routine 1.07 1.04-1.24 1.03 0.92-1.15 1.28 1.14-1.46
Education
A-level+ 1 1 1
0-Level or equiv. 0.93 0.86-1.00 0.92 0.81-1.03 0.89 0.80-1.02
Less than 0-Level 1.02 0.97-1.07 1.04 0.92-1.16 1.12 1.01-1.22

4.2. Psycho-social determinants

The relationship between the predictors of social support and our outcomes was mixed (Table 5). We observed that on average people without friends had 14% higher odds of basic multimorbidity than people whose friends were very supportive or supportive to some degree. Similarly people with no partner had odds of having basic multimorbidity higher than those who reported having supportive partner, with OR 1.15 (95% CI 1.06-1.26). The perception of loneliness was positively associated with all three outcomes: for basic MM OR 1.18 (95% CI 1.10-1.26), for complex multimorbidity OR 1.21 (95% CI 1.11-1.32) and for MFL10 + OR 1.32 (95% CI 1.20-1.46). The relationship between the sense of control and the probability of having each of our health outcomes was graded by the degree of the perceived control. The odds ratios were higher for MFL10 + than for the other two outcomes. Participation in community was not associated with multimorbidity and complex multimorbidity (Table 5). The odds of having MFL10 + increased the less people participated, with those not active in community having 25% higher odds (95% CI 1.10-1.42) than those who were very active.

Table 5.

Basic multimorbidity, complex multimorbidity, functional limitation and psycho-social determinants.


Basic MM

Complex MM

MFL10+

Odds ratio 95% CI Odds ratio 95% CI Odds ratio 95% CI
Participation
Very active 1 1 1
Active 0.97 0.92-1.03 1.03 0.95-1.12 1.12 1.01-1.27
Not active 1.04 0.96-1.12 1.01 0.91-1.12 1.25 1.10-1.42
Sense of control
High 1 1 1
Some 1.21 1.16-1.27 1.28 1.20-1.37 1.79 1.63-1.96
Low 1.57 1.41-1.74 1.70 1.51-1.91 3.29 2.87-3.76
Supportive children
Very/some 1 1 1
A little/not at all 1.02 0.94-1.11 1.02 0.90-1.15 1.14 1.00-1.28
No children 0.96 0.86-1.07 0.98 0.86-1.11 0.98 0.86-1.12
Supportive friends
Very/some 1 1 1
A little/not at all 0.99 0.94-1.06 0.96 0.89-1.04 1.04 0.95-1.14
No friends 1.14 1.02-1.26 1.07 0.95-1.2 1.05 0.92-1.19
Supportive partner
Very/some 1 1 1
A little/not at all 0.93 0.81-1.07 0.98 0.81-1.18 0.99 0.79-1.22
No partner 1.15 1.06-1.26 1.03 0.94-1.14 1.04 0.94-1.16
Loneliness
Yes 1.19 1.11-1.28 1.22 1.12-1.33 1.32 1.20-1.46

4.3. Behavioural determinants

The relationship between physical activity and the probability of having each of our health measures was graded by the level of intensity of physical activity (Table 6). Compared to those who exercised vigorously, people who were physically inactive had 1.6 times larger odds of having basic MM (95% CI 1.21 – 1.41), twice larger odds of having complex MM (95% CI 1.78-2.27) and 8 times larger odds of having MFL10+ (95% CI 7.00-9.43). The frequency of alcohol consumption was associated with our health outcomes but not in the expected way. The probability of developing each of our health outcomes increased with decreasing frequency of drinking (Table 6). A history of smoking was related to the health outcomes. Ex-smokers had higher odds ratios compared to people who never smoked: 1.27 (95% CI 1.17 – 1.37) for basic MM, OR 1.29 (95% CI 1.17 – 1.42) for complex MM and OR 1.37 (95% CI 1.23-1.54) for ten or more multiple functional limitations.

Table 6.

Basic multimorbidity, complex multimorbidity, functional limitation and behavioural determinants.

Basic MM

Complex MM

MFL10+

Odds ratio 95% CI Odds ratio 95% CI Odds ratio 95% CI
Physical activity
Vigorous 1 1 1
Moderate 1.22 1.16-1.28 1.32 1.22-1.42 1.83 1.64-2.04
Mild 1.57 1.46-1.68 1.92 1.75-2.12 4.26 3.73-4.86
None 1.60 1.45-1.76 2.01 1.78-2.27 8.13 7.00-9.43
Alcohol consumption
Don't drink 1 1 1
Monthly or less 0.90 0.83-0.99 0.79 0.71-0.87 0.67 0.60-0.75
Weekly 0.82 0.75-0.91 0.68 0.61-0.77 0.51 0.45-0.58
Daily 0.81 0.73-0.88 0.65 0.58-0.74 0.47 0.40-0.53
Smoking
Never 1 1 1
Ex-smoker 1.27 1.17-1.37 1.29 1.17-1.42 1.37 1.23-1.54
Current 1.03 0.91-1.14 1.04 0.90-1.21 1.46 1.26-1.70

4.4. Sensitivity analysis

In a separate sensitivity analysis we explored how the alternative measure of three or more diseases (MM3+) reflected the health inequalities compared to CMM. The results in the Appendix show that the magnitude of the odds ratios using MM3+ lay for most of the variables between the ORs of basic MM and CMM. We conclude that although the differences in risk between CMM and basic MM with the higher cutpoint are narrower, the former measure still indicates the highest inequality which we believe justifies its use.

5. Discussion

5.1. Main results

Our results originate from a representative sample of an English population, hence they can be generalized for England. We have explored longitudinal association between the material, psycho-social and behavioural determinants and measures of multimorbidity and multiple functional limitation in an English population of people aged 50 years or older. We found a consistent association between the outcome measures and the levels of household wealth, the sense of control over one's life, physical activity and loneliness. The relationship with smoking and alcohol consumption indicated possible health selection, where individuals were actively regulating their health behaviours. Multiple functional limitation and complex multimorbidity captured larger inequalities than basic multimorbidity.

5.2. Interpretation

We present evidence that a variety of material, psycho-social and behavioural determinants are to varying extents related to basic multimorbidity, complex multimorbidity and multiple functional limitations. We found consistent inequalities in multimorbidity and multiple functional limitation across the levels of household wealth, the sense of control over one's life, physical activity and loneliness. These inequalities appeared larger when measured as multiple functional limitation and complex multimorbidity than basic multimorbidity. Our results suggest that solely materialist models of multimorbidity at older age are insufficient as behavioural and psycho-social factors play an important role. Behavioural patterns in smoking and alcohol consumption suggest that while health inequality accumulates during the life course, psycho-social resources and active human agency also contribute to shaping the population health profiles.

Among material determinants the strongest health disparities were captured by household wealth. Compared to the population group with the highest wealth, those with the lowest wealth had 47% higher odds of basic MM, 73% higher odds of complex MM and 90% higher odds of ten or more functional limitations. The stark disparities support the evidence that the amount of available household wealth or assets constrains individuals' consumption choices on quality of housing, usable outdoor space, type of residential area or quality of health care (Joseph Rowntree Foundation, 2014). Savings also act as a buffer against unexpected loss of income due to ill health in later life thus reducing the exposure to stress. We observed that individuals with the lowest subjective social status had 14% larger odds of having basic MM, 20% larger odds of having complex MM and 37% larger odds of having ten or more functional limitations than those with the top SSS. These differences might reflect two-way effects: a very low subjective perception of one's status contributes to worse health but having more complex issues (with simultaneously affected body systems and limited in everyday lives) additionally reinforces the negative rating of individual status (O’Brien, Wyke, & Watt, 2014).

Occupational status was weakly associated with basic MM and MFL10 + and educational qualifications were weakly related to MFL10+. In comparison to the measure of household wealth which reflects a process of life-long accumulation, the indicators of education and occupation reflect periods of time from a more distant past. This might explain stronger and more consistent effect of household wealth and suggest that it is a better indicator of an older person's status (Adena & Myck, 2014; McGovern & Nazroo, 2015).

Psycho-social determinants produced mixed results. We found a clear gradient between individuals with the strongest and the weakest sense of control over their lives. The more people felt in charge of their lives the less likely they were to develop ill health. Low control beliefs can affect health in several ways. They may lead to anxiety and aggression which facilitates chronic stress response, smoking and drinking. Feeling low control over destiny can also lead to passive responses such as low self-esteem which induce depression (Whitehead et al., 2016). Loneliness was the other factor consistently related to all of our measures. The feeling of being socially isolated is relatively common among the elderly because some relationships are lost as people get older (Singh & Misra, 2009).

Participation in community groups was associated to a reduced probability of developing 10 or more functional limitations but not to reduced multimorbidity. Participating in at least three community groups presupposes certain level of health and physical functioning which acts as a clear barrier for those with at least 10 functional limitations. This health selection process might explain why we can see a social gradient for multiple functional limitations but not for multimorbidities. Other measures of social support showed either no significant relationship or a limited relationship. We found an association with friendship among people with basic MM. The effects of support on health of older people depend on the source of support and the quality of relationship. For example relationships with friends can be beneficial to one's health while relationship with family members not (Huxhold, Miche, & Schüz, 2014).

In the group of behavioural determinants we found a dose-response relationship between all levels of physical activity and probability of the three health outcomes. This may suggest that lack of physical activity is an important factor increasing probability of chronic and complex health problems and limitations in the ageing population (Cimarras-Otal et al., 2014; Dhalwani et al., 2017). However, reverse causation is also very likely so that worse health outcomes were driving reduced physical activity. Both possibilities could operate together leading to a positive feedback loop of deteriorating health and reduced physical activity. Increasing frequency of alcohol consumption seems to be associated to reduced odds of multimorbidity and multiple functional limitation. Evidence has indicated that moderate alcohol consumption brings benefits to health and mortality reduction but the discussion is biased by the common problem of the inclusion of ex-drinkers with life-time abstainers in the same category (Stockwell et al., 2016). Part of this discussion explores the role of human agency in regulating alcohol consumption, thus balancing the negative health effects with experiences of social inclusion beneficial for mental health (Kelly, Olanrewaju, Cowan, Brayne, & Lafortune, 2018). Holdsworth, Mendonça, Pikhart et al. (2016) found that older people with good or improving self-reported health were increasing their drinking over time while people with bad or worsening health moderated their drinking.

The relationship between smoking and both types of multimorbidity is ambiguous. Compared to people who never smoked, ex-smokers were more likely to develop any one of the three outcomes but there was no relationship between current smokers and those who never smoked for basic or complex MM. Cross-tabulating smoking, age and prevalence of multimorbidity, we found that ex-smokers were more prevalent among older age groups with higher morbidities and current smokers were younger and healthier. The explanation of similar findings by Nazroo et al. (2008) is that when people become ill they might stop smoking. Unlike morbidities, the odds ratios between people with multiple functional limitation formed a consistent gradient. This difference might be related to the fact that the prevalence of current smokers was higher and the prevalence of non-smokers smaller among people with MFL10 + than among people with MM and CMM. The results for alcohol consumption and smoking exemplify how people in later life continue making active choices within their social context (Elder, 1994).

Comparing health inequalities between our three outcomes deserves a note of caution. Whilst the odds ratios for complex MM and multiple functional limitation show bigger associations with SES than basic MM, the latter is more common and this may to some degree limit the increase across categories, as it starts off at a higher level. This fact still allows us to make the claim that multiple functional limitation and complex multimorbidity captured larger inequalities than basic multimorbidity. Working with only the basic measure of multimorbidity might limit our ability to see the social heterogeneity of ageing population. But apart from improving the measure we also need to try to explain why different measures lead to different inequalities. Functional limitation and decline in the elderly is a consequence of chronic disease, with a greater effect among people with a higher number of morbidities (Jindai et al., 2016; Ryan, Wallace, O'Hara, & Smith, 2015). Complex multimorbidity results from dysfunction in three or more body systems. Both outcomes demand complex and long-term care but we know that patients’ responses are socially differentiated (Bartley, 2004; Cockerham, 2007). People from higher social backgrounds are capable to use resources such as power, money, knowledge, prestige or social support to protect themselves from health risks or mitigate the consequences of multimorbidity (Link & Phelan, 1995). Taking into account this socially patterned human agency might help to explain why inequalities in complex multimorbidity and multiple functional limitations are stronger than in basic MM.

5.3. Limitations

There are several limitations to our study related to methodology and the scope of analyses. Our study was exploratory and based on GEE method with population-averaged data. It allowed us to observe averaged distribution of certain characteristics between individuals. However this study design does not enable building explanatory analyses or drawing conclusions on both within-individual and between-individual variance or change in outcomes.

Recent studies reported that social determinants do not only influence health simultaneously but also influence each other (Moor et al., 2016; Short & Mollborn, 2015). For instance, social support can mitigate the effect of stress on people with low social status (Gellert et al., 2018). We have not examined these interaction effects but they could lead to modification of some effects.

We constrained our classification to a generic count of single diseases, ICD-10 chapters and functional limitations without identifying the most frequent combinations. These have been studied either as pairs and triplets or as clusters with the highest degree of association and due to their synergistic effects are of special interest to clinicians (Ng et al., 2018). Unpacking the associational heterogeneity might shed some light on the relationships between these patterns of multimorbidity and specific determinants or groups of determinants.

6. Conclusions

Our study was the first study to comprehensively explore materialist, psycho-social and behavioural determinants of health in relation to multimorbidity and multiple functional limitations. Policies aiming to reduce the risk of multimorbidity and functional limitation should address the issue at several levels, as a socio-economic and behavioural intervention. Behavioural and therapeutic approaches in the community can help to compensate for social isolation, reduced self-esteem or to regain more sense of control over people's lives (Public Health England and UCL Institute of Health Equity, 2015). This strategy should be based around local primary care centres. They could be provided with additional resources to spend more time as the frontline assessors of multimorbidity and consistent coordinators acting as a link between patients and the specialist health care services (World Health Organization, 2016).

Ethical statement

Ethical approval was not required for the analysis of anonymised secondary data, which is made available to all researchers freely.

Declarations of interest

None.

Acknowledgement

The study was funded by the School of Environmental Sciences, University of Liverpool.

Appendix. Different definitions of MM and social determinants

Table A1.

Basic multimorbidity, complex multimorbidity, MM3+ and material determinants

Basic MM
95% CI Complex MM
95% CI MM3+
95% CI
Odds ratio Odds ratio Odds ratio
Household wealth
High 1 1 1
Medium 1.13 1.10-1.19 1.20 1.09-1.31 1.18 1.1-1.28
Low 1.47 1.34-1.61 1.73 1.52-1.96 1.66 1.49-1.86
Subjective social status
High 1 1 1
Medium 1.04 0.98-1.10 1.11 1.00-1.20 1.06 0.98-1.14
Low 1.14 1.04-1.24 1.21 1.07-1.35 1.16 1.04-1.28
Occupation
Manager/prof. 1 1 1
Intermediate 0.93 0.85-1.01 0.92 0.81-1.03 0.93 0.84-1.04
Semi/routine 1.07 1.04-1.24 1.03 0.92-1.15 1.05 0.95-1.16
Education
A-level+ 1 1 1
0-Level or equiv. 0.93 0.86-1.00 0.92 0.81-1.03 0.94 0.86-1.05
Less than 0-Level 1.02 0.97-1.07 1.04 0.92-1.16 1.01 0.95-1.09

Table A2.

Basic multimorbidity, complex multimorbidity, MM3+ and psycho-social determinants


Basic MM

Complex MM

MM3+

Odds ratio 95% CI Odds ratio 95% CI Odds ratio 95% CI
Participation
Very active 1 1 1
Active 0.97 0.92-1.03 1.03 0.95-1.12 1.03 0.96-1.11
Not active
1.04
0.96-1.12
1.01
0.91-1.12
1.04
0.99-1.24
Sense of control
High 1 1 1
Some 1.21 1.16-1.27 1.28 1.20-1.37 1.28 1.21-1.35
Low
1.57
1.41-1.74
1.70
1.51-1.91
1.62
1.44-1.92
Supportive children
Very/some 1 1 1
A little/not at all 1.02 0.94-1.11 1.02 0.90-1.15 1.05 0.95-1.16
No children
0.96
0.86-1.07
0.98
0.86-1.11
0.98
0.87-1.09
Supportive friends
Very/some 1 1 1
A little/not at all 0.99 0.94-1.06 0.96 0.89-1.04 0.99 0.93-1.06
No friends
1.14
1.02-1.26
1.07
0.95-1.2
1.06
0.95-1.18
Supportive partner
Very/some 1 1 1
A little/not at all 0.93 0.81-1.07 0.98 0.81-1.18 0.91 0.78-1.07
No partner
1.15
1.06-1.26
1.03
0.94-1.14
1.11
1.02-1.21
Loneliness
Yes 1.19 1.11-1.28 1.22 1.12-1.33 1.20 1.14-1.33

Table A3.

Basic multimorbidity, complex multimorbidity, MM3+ and behavioural determinants

Basic MM

Complex MM

MM3+

Odds ratio 95% CI Odds ratio 95% CI Odds ratio 95% CI
Physical activity
Vigorous 1 1 1
Moderate 1.22 1.16-1.28 1.32 1.22-1.42 1.29 1.21-1.37
Mild 1.57 1.46-1.68 1.92 1.75-2.12 1.79 1.65-1.94
None
1.60
1.45-1.76
2.01
1.78-2.27
1.78
1.60-1.99
Alcohol consumption
Don't drink 1 1 1
Monthly or less 0.90 0.83-0.99 0.79 0.71-0.87 0.80 0.73-0.87
Weekly 0.82 0.75-0.91 0.68 0.61-0.77 0.73 0.65-0.81
Daily
0.81
0.73-0.88
0.65
0.58-0.74
0.69
0.62-0.77
Smoking
Never 1 1 1
Ex-smoker 1.27 1.17-1.37 1.29 1.17-1.42 1.31 1.20-1.44
Current 1.03 0.91-1.14 1.04 0.90-1.21 1.11 0.97-1.26

Table A4.

Correspondence between CMM and MFL10 if MM=1

MFL10+
CMM 0 1 Total
0 14,276 2,246 16,522
1 6,638 3,018 9,656
Total 20,914 5,264 26,178

Chi-square(1) = 1.200, p = .000.

Table A5.

Correspondence between CMM and MFL10 if MM=0

MFL10+
CMM 0 1 Total
0 28,965 1,051 30,016
1 2 3 5
Total 28,967 1,054 30,021

Chi-square(1) = 47.106, p = .000.

References

  1. Adena M., Myck M. Poverty and transitions in health in later life. Social Science & Medicine. 2014;116:202–210. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.06.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Agency for Toxic Substances, Disease Registry . 2018. Taking an exposure history: Which organ systems are affected by toxic exposure(s)?https://www.atsdr.cdc.gov/csem/csem.asp?csem=33&po=6 [Google Scholar]
  3. Austad S. Making sense of biological theories of aging. In: Bengtson V., editor. Handbook of theories of aging. Springer Publishing Company; New York: 2009. pp. 147–162. [Google Scholar]
  4. Banks J., Blake M., Clemens S. 25th ed. UK Data Service; 2018. English longitudinal study of ageing: Waves 0-8, 1998-2017. [data collection] SN: 5050. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barnes P. Mechanisms of development of multimorbidity in the elderly. European Respiratory Journal. 2015;45:790–806. doi: 10.1183/09031936.00229714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bartley M. Polity Press; Cambridge: 2004. Health inequality: An introduction to theories, concepts and methods. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bayliss M., Rendas-Baum R., White M.K., Maruish M., Bjorner J., Tunis S.L. Health-related quality of life (HRQL) for individuals with self-reported chronic physical and/or mental health conditions: Panel survey of an adult sample in the United States. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes. 2012;10:154. doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-10-154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Berkman L. From social integration to health: Durkheim in the new millennium. Social Science & Medicine. 2000;51(6):843–857. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00065-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Black D. The Black Report. In: Townsend P., Davidson N., editors. Inequalities in health: The Black Report. 2nd ed. Penguin Books; London: 1992. [Google Scholar]
  10. Blane D., Kelly-Irving M., D'Errico A., Bartley M., Montgomery S. Socio-biological transitions: How does the social become biological? Longitudinal and Life Course Studies. 2013;4(2):136–146. doi: 10.14301/llcs.v4i2.236. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bowling A. Enhancing later life: How older people perceive active ageing? Aging & Mental Health. 2008;12(3):293–301. doi: 10.1080/13607860802120979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Burden of Disease Network Project . 2004. Disability at old age: Final report.https://ju.se/download/18.3783220012d8f123ca58000115/1520578695703/DISABILITY%20IN%20OLD%20AGE.pdf [Google Scholar]
  13. Calderón-Larraňaga A., Santoni G., Wang H.X. Rapidly developing multimorbidity and disability in older adults: Does social background matter? Journal of Internal Medicine. 2018;283(5):489–499. doi: 10.1111/joim.12739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cassell A., Edwards D., Harshfield A. The Epidemiology of multimorbidity in primary care. British Journal of General Practice. 2018;68(669):e245–e251. doi: 10.3399/bjgp18X695465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Charonis A., Kyriopoulos I., Spanakis M. Subjective social status, social network and health disparities: Empirical evidence from Greece. International Journal for Equity in Health. 2017;16(40) doi: 10.1186/s12939-017-0533-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Chatterji S., Byles J., Cutler D. Health, functioning, and disability in older adults—present status and future implications. Lancet. 2015;385:563–575. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)61462-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Chen B., Covinsky K.E., Cenzer I.S., Adler N., Williams B.A. Subjective social status and functional decline in older adults. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2012;27(6):693–699. doi: 10.1007/s11606-011-1963-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cimarras-Otal C., Calderón-Larraňaga A., Poblador-Plou B. Association between physical activity, multimorbidity, self-rated health and functional limitation in the Spanish population. BMC Public Health. 2014;14:1170. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-14-1170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cockerham W. Polity Press; Cambridge: 2007. Social causes of health and disease. [Google Scholar]
  20. Cosco T., Prina A., Perales J. Lay perspectives of successful ageing: A systematic review and meta-ethnography. BMJ Open. 2013;3 doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2013-002710. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. De Maio F., Mazzeo J., Ritchie D. Social determinants of health: A view on theory and measurement. Rhode Island Medical Journal. 2013;96(7):15–19. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23819135 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Demakakos P., Nazroo J., Breeze E., Marmot M. Socioeconomic status and health: The role of subjective social status. Social Science & Medicine. 2008;67(2):330–340. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.03.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dhalwani N., Zaccardi F., O'Donovan G., Carter P., Hamer M., Yates T. Association between lifestyle factors and the incidence of multimorbidity in an older English population. Journal of Gerontology: Series A. 2017;72(4):528–534. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Di Lorenzo L., Coco V., Forte F., Trinchese G.F., Forte A.M., Pappagallo M. The use of odds ratio in the large population-based studies: Warning to readers. Muscles, Ligaments and Tendons Journal. 2014;4(1):90–92. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Diederichs C., Berger K., Bartels D.B. The Measurement of multiple chronic Diseases - a systematic review on existing multimorbidity indices. The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences. 2011;66:301–311. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glq208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Elder G. Time, human agency, and social change: Perspectives on the life course. Social Psychology Quarterly. 1994;57(1):4–15. http://www.jstor.org/stable/2786971 [Google Scholar]
  27. Fabbri E., Zoli M., Gonzalez-Freire M., Salive M.E., Studenski S.A., Ferrucci L. Aging and multimorbidity: New tasks, priorities, and frontiers for integrated gerontological and clinical research. Journal of the American Medical Directors Association. 2015;16(8):640–647. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2015.03.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Frohlich K., Corin E., Potvin L. A theoretical proposal for the relationship between context and disease. Sociology of Health & Illness. 2001;23(6):776–797. doi: 10.1111/1467-9566.00275. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Gellert P., Häusler A., Suhr R. Testing the stress-buffering hypothesis of social support in couples coping with early-stage dementia. PLoS One. 2018;13(1) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0189849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Harrison C., Britt H., Miller G., Henderson J. Examining different measures of multimorbidity, using a large prospective cross-sectional study in Australian general practice. BMJ Open. 2014;4(7) doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2013-004694. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Holdsworth C., Mendonça M., Pikhart H. Is regular drinking in later life an indicator of good health? Evidence from the English longitudinal study of ageing. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health. 2016;70:764–770. doi: 10.1136/jech-2015-206949. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Huxhold O., Miche M., Schüz B. Benefits of having friends in older ages: Differential effects of informal social activities on well-being in middle-aged and older adults. Journal of Gerontology: Serie Bibliographique. 2014;69(3):366–375. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jackson C., Dobson A., Tooth L., Mishra G. Body mass index and socioeconomic position are associated with 9-year trajectories of multimorbidity: A population-based study. Preventive Medicine. 2015;81:92–98. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2015.08.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Jindai K., Nielson C.M., Vorderstrasse B.A., Quiñones A.R. Multimorbidity and functional limitations among adults 65 or older, NHANES 2005-2012. Preventing Chronic Disease. 2016;13:E151. doi: 10.5888/pcd13.160174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Joseph Rowntree Foundation . 2014. How does money influence health?https://www.jrf.org.uk/report/how-does-money-influence-health [Google Scholar]
  36. Kelly S., Olanrewaju O., Cowan A., Brayne C., Lafortune L. Alcohol and older people: A systematic review of barriers, facilitators and context of drinking in older people and implications for intervention design. PLoS One. 2018;13(1) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0191189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kingston A. Projections of multi-morbidity in the older population in England to 2035: Estimates from the population ageing and care simulation (PACSim) model. Age and Ageing. 2018;47(3):374–380. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afx201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kirkwood T.B.L. Understanding ageing from an evolutionary perspective. Journal of Internal Medicine. 2008;263:117–127. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2007.01901.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Liang K.-Y., Zeger S.L. Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models. Biometrika. 1986;73(1):13–22. doi: 10.1093/biomet/73.1.13. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Link B., Phelan J. Social conditions as fundamental causes of disease. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1995;35(Extra issue):80–94. http://www.jstor.org/stable/2626958 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Li Q., Wang S., Milot E., Bergeron P., Ferrucci L., Fried L.P. Homeostatic dysregulation proceeds in parallel in multiple physiological systems. Aging Cell. 2015;14(6):1103–1112. doi: 10.1111/acel.12402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Lucyk K., McLaren L. Taking stock of the social determinants of health: A scoping review. PLoS One. 2017;12(5) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0177306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Marengoni A., Angleman S., Melis R. Aging with multimorbidity: A systematic review of the literature. Ageing Research Reviews. 2011;10:430–439. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2011.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Marmot M.G., Smith G.D., Stansfield S., Patel C. Health inequalities among British civil servants: The Whitehall II study. The Lancet. 1991;337(8754):1387–1393. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(91)93068-K. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. McGovern P., Nazroo J. Patterns and causes of health inequalities in later life: A bourdieusian approach. Sociology of Health & Illness. 2015;37(1):143–160. doi: 10.1111/1467-9566.12187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Melis R., Marengoni A., Angleman S., Fratiglioni L. Incidence and predictors of multimorbidity in the elderly: A population-based longitudinal study. PLoS One. 2014;9(7) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0103120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Moor I., Spallek J., Richter M. Explaining socioeconomic inequalities in self-rated health: A systematic review of the relative contribution of material, psychosocial and behavioural factors. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health. 2016;0:1–11. doi: 10.1136/jech-2016-207589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Mounce L.T.A., Campbell J.L., Henley W.E., Tejerina Arreal M.C., Porter I., Valderas J.M. Predicting incident multimorbidity. The Annals of Family Medicine. 2018;16:322–329. doi: 10.1370/afm.2271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Muller C.J., MacLehose R.F. Estimating predicted probabilities from logistic regression: Different methods correspond to different target populations. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2014;43(3):962–970. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyu029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Nazroo J., Zaninotto P., Gjonca E. Mortality and healthy life expectancy. In: Banks J., editor. Living in the 21stcentury: Older people in England. The 2006 English longitudinal Study of ageing. The Institute of Fiscal Studies; London: 2008. [Google Scholar]
  51. Northwood M., Ploeg J., Markle‐Reid M., Sherifali D. Integrative review of the social determinants of health in older adults with multimorbidity. Journal of Advanced Nursing. 2018;74(1):45–60. doi: 10.1111/jan.13408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Olaya B., Domènech-Abella J., Moneta M.V., Lara E. All-cause mortality and multimorbidity in older adults: The role of social support and loneliness. Experimental Gerontology. 2017;99:120–126. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2017.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. O'Brien R., Wyke S., Watt G.C.M. The ‘everyday work’ of living with multimorbidity in socioeconomically deprived areas of Scotland. Journal of Comorbidity. 2014;4:1–10. doi: 10.15256/joc.2014.4.32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Peters M., Kelly L., Potter C.M., Jenkinson C., Gibbons E., Forder J. Quality of life and burden of morbidity in primary care users with multimorbidity. Patient Related Outcome Measures. 2018;9:103–113. doi: 10.2147/PROM.S148358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Piette J., Kerr E. The impact of comorbid chronic conditions on diabetes care. Diabetes Care. 2006;29(3):725–731. doi: 10.2337/diacare.29.03.06.dc05-2078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Public Health England . 2018. Health profile for England.https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/health-profile-for-england-2018 2018. [Google Scholar]
  57. Public Health England, UCL Institute of Health Equity . 2015. Reducing social isolation across the life course: Local action on health inequalities.https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/461120/3a_Social_isolation-Full-revised.pdf [Google Scholar]
  58. Robertson T., Benzeval M., Whitley E., Popham F. The role of material, psychosocial and behavioral factors in mediating the association between socioeconomic position and allostatic load (measured by cardiovascular, metabolic and inflammatory markers) Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2015;45:41–49. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2014.10.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Ryan A., Wallace E., O'Hara P., Smith S.M. Multimorbidity and functional decline in community-dwelling adults: A systematic review. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes. 2015;13:168. doi: 10.1186/s12955-015-0355-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Salisbury C., Johnson L., Purdy S., Valderas J.M., Montgomery A.A. Epidemiology and impact of multimorbidity in primary care: A retrospective cohort study. British Journal of General Practice: The Journal of the Royal College of General Practitioners. 2011;61(582):e12–21. doi: 10.3399/bjgp11X548929. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Schäfer I., Hansen H., Schön G. The influence of age, gender and socio-economic status on multimorbidity patterns in primary care. First results from the multicare cohort study. BMC Health Services Research. 2012;12:89. doi: 10.1186/1472-6963-12-89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Short S.E., Mollborn S. Social determinants and health behaviors: Conceptual frames and empirical advances. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2015;5:78–84. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Singh-Manoux A., Marmot M., Adler N. Does subjective social status predict health and change in health status better than objective status? Psychosomatic Medicine. 2005;67(6):855–861. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000188434.52941.a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Singh A., Misra N. Loneliness, depression and sociability in old age. Industrial Psychiatry Journal. 2009;18(1):51–55. doi: 10.4103/0972-6748.57861. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Steptoe A., Breeze E., Banks J. Cohort profile: The English longitudinal study of ageing. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2013;42(6):1640–1648. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Stickley A., Koyanagi A. Physical multimorbidity and loneliness: A population-based study. PLoS One. 2018;13(1) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0191651. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Stockwell T., Zhao J., Panwar S., Roemer A., Naimi T., Chikritzhs T. Do "moderate" drinkers have reduced mortality risk? A systematic review and meta-analysis of alcohol consumption and all-cause mortality. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2016;77(2):185–198. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2016.77.185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Twisk J. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 2013. Applied longitudinal data analysis for epidemiology. [Google Scholar]
  69. Uchino B. Understanding the links between social support and physical health: A life-span perspective with emphasis on the separability of perceived and received support. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2009;4(3) doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01122.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Van Oort F.V.A., Van Lenthe F.J., Mackenbach J. Material, psychosocial, and behavioural factors in the explanation of educational inequalities in mortality in The Netherlands. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health. 2005;59(3):214–220. doi: 10.1136/jech.2003.016493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Van den Akker M., Buntinx F., Metsemakers J.F.M., Roos S., Knottnerus A. Multimorbidity in general practice: Prevalence, incidence and determinants of co-occurring chronic and recurrent diseases. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 1998;51(5):367–370. doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(97)00306-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Verbrugge L.M., Jette A.M. The disablement process. Social Science & Medicine. 1994;38(1):1–14. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(94)90294-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Whitehead M., Pennington A., Orton L. How could differences in “control over destiny” lead to socio-economic inequalities in health? A synthesis of theories and pathways in the living environment. Health & Place. 2016;39:51–61. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2016.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. White H.L., O'Campo P., Moineddin R., Matheson F.I. Modeling the cumulative effects of social exposures on health: Moving beyond disease-specific models. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2013;10(4):1186–1201. doi: 10.3390/ijerph10041186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. World Health Organization . 2016. Multimorbidity: Technical series on safer primary care.http://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/252275/9789241511650-eng.pdf;jsessionid=C9DD416571A6180B893AD22890B7C90B?sequence=1 [Google Scholar]
  76. Yarnall A.J., Sayer A., Clegg A., Rockwood K., Parker S., Hindle J. New horizons in multimorbidity in older adults. Age and Ageing. 2017;46(6):882–888. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afx150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Zulman D.M., Pal Chee C., Wagner T.H. Multimorbidity and healthcare utilisation among high-cost patients in the US veterans affairs health care system. BMJ Open. 2015 doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-007771. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from SSM - Population Health are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES