Skip to main content
Sleep logoLink to Sleep
. 2019 Mar 12;42(6):zsz055. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsz055

Rapid eye movement sleep mediates age-related decline in prospective memory consolidation

Michael K Scullin 1,, Chenlu Gao 1, Paul Fillmore 2, R Lynae Roberts 1, Natalya Pruett 1, Donald L Bliwise 3
PMCID: PMC6559169  PMID: 30860593

Abstract

Study Objectives

Prospective memory, or remembering to execute future intentions, accounts for half of everyday forgetting in older adults. Sleep intervals benefit prospective memory consolidation in young adults, but it is unknown whether age-related changes in slow wave activity, sleep spindles, and/or rapid eye movement (REM) sleep mediate hypothesized effects of aging on prospective memory consolidation.

Methods

After an adaptation night, 76 adults aged 18–84 completed two experimental nights of in-laboratory polysomnography recording. In the evening, participants encoded and practiced a prospective memory task and were tested the next morning. On a counterbalanced night, they encoded and practiced a control task, and were tested the following morning.

Results

Increasing age predicted worse prospective memory consolidation (r = −.34), even when controlling for encoding, speed, and control-task performance (all ps < .05). Frontal delta power, slow oscillations, and spindle density were not related to prospective memory consolidation. REM sleep duration, however, explained significant variance in prospective memory consolidation when controlling for age (∆R2 = .10). Bootstrapping mediation showed that less REM sleep significantly mediated the aging effect on prospective memory consolidation [b = −.0016, SE = 0.0009 (95% confidence interval [CI] = −0.0042 to −0.0004)]. REM sleep continued to mediate 24.29% of the total effect of age on prospective memory after controlling for numerous demographic, cognitive, mental health, and sleep variables.

Conclusion

Age-related variance in REM sleep is informative to how prospective memory consolidation changes with increasing age. Future work should consider how both REM sleep and slow wave activity contribute, perhaps in a sequential or dynamic manner, to preserving cognitive functioning with increasing age.

Keywords: intention, prospection, older adults, polysomnography, slow wave activity, sleep spindles, rapid eye movement sleep, spontaneous retrieval, preplay


Statement of Significance.

Whether one needs to remember to take a new medication, deliver a message to a colleague, or pick up milk at the grocery store, the ability to remember to execute delayed intentions is essential to independent living. Because intentions typically cannot immediately be performed, they must undergo memory consolidation. The current study identified an age-related deterioration in the consolidation of delayed intentions. Furthermore, this deterioration might be attributed to reduced REM sleep duration with increasing age. Behavioral and pharmacological interventions that target REM sleep will be critical to establish causality and translation to clinical settings.

Introduction

For 2,000 years, scholars conceptualized memory as functioning to record the past (Plato’s Theaetetus) [1]. Today, however, cognitive neuroscience conceptualizes memory as functioning to prepare us for the future [2]. This future-oriented, “preplay” [3] model of memory is well-captured by the study of prospective memory, that is, the ability to remember to perform delayed intentions. Prospective memory failures account for half of everyday forgetting errors [4] and have been linked to catastrophes such as airplane crashes [5], retained surgical instruments [6], and forgetting that a sleeping child is in the backseat of one’s car [7]. Less dramatic, but more ubiquitous, is the use of prospective memory to return messages, pick up medications from the pharmacy, and take a different route to work that avoids construction [8].

Sleep and prospective memory

Sleep deprivation impairs prospective memory [9–12] whereas normal sleep facilitates prospective memory consolidation [13–18]. However, there exists minimal data on which component of sleep physiology—slow-wave activity (SWA), sleep spindles, or rapid eye movement (REM) sleep—facilitate prospective memory consolidation [15]. To inform predictions for how sleep consolidates prospective memories, it is important to first consider how tests of prospective memory are different from traditional tests of motor/procedural memory and episodic/declarative memory. In the latter tests, participants are placed into an effortful retrieval mode during testing [19], such that the experimenter tells participants to try to perform a motor sequence or to recognize studied words. In prospective memory tests, no reminders are given, and thus, participants must “remember to remember.” [7] The ability to “remember to remember” depends on the associative strength between what must be remembered (intention) and when it must be remembered (cue) [20]. When the cue-intention association is strong (e.g. due to being consolidated during sleep), then processing the retrieval cue will spontaneously/automatically trigger retrieval of the intention [21, 22].

SWA and spindles are typically implicated in supporting episodic memory consolidation, whereas REM sleep is classically implicated in supporting procedural memory consolidation [23]. However, the collective literature indicates that dichotomizing the function of sleep stages by procedural vs. episodic memory systems is less informative than identifying how macro- and micro-features of sleep interact in response to specific memory processes [24]. As one example, while SWA promotes retention of neutral information, REM sleep can enhance emotional processing and binding of future-relevant associations [23–26].

Llewellyn and Hobson’s theoretical model [27] seems particularly relevant to the current work on prospective memory. They proposed that REM sleep evolved to help prepare organisms for future automatic actions. In Llewellyn and Hobson’s words, REM sleep functions “to enable effective preplay, [that is, the future stimulus] can be acted upon unconsciously and rapidly” (p. 81) [27]. Following their conceptualization, because prospective memory is a future-oriented behavior depending on spontaneous/automatic associative processes [28, 29], REM sleep activity may facilitate prospective memory consolidation.

Sleep, aging, and memory consolidation

A broader goal for the sleep and memory consolidation field is to inform cognitive aging [30–33]. A meta-analysis on sleep, aging, and memory consolidation, however, found substantial variability across aging studies in memory-polysomnography correlations and memory outcomes (i.e. age preservation vs. impairment of memory consolidation) [34]. The inconsistencies might reflect small samples, multiple comparisons, and that most studies have not disentangled consolidation deficits from known age-related deficits in processing speed and effortful retrieval [35]. Testing prospective memory provides a potential solution to these processing speed and retrieval challenges. Specifically, in laboratory settings, prospective memory is tested against the background of a speeded ongoing decision-making task to mirror the real-world scenario of having to remember an intention (such as stopping at the grocery store), while performing ongoing activities (such as driving one’s car). Measuring ongoing task performance at the same time as memory is being tested allows for the control of processing speed. In addition to controlling processing speed, tests of “focal” event-based prospective memory (i.e. tests with strong environmental cues) have found that spontaneous/automatic retrieval processes are generally preserved in advancing age [36, 37] (by contrast, tests of “nonfocal” prospective memory that depend on sustained, vigilant monitoring consistently show age-related declines [28]). Utilizing a focal prospective memory test that shows age-preservation of retrieval processes, and statistically controlling for ongoing task performance, provides a closer look at whether aging specifically compromises memory consolidation.

If aging does compromise memory consolidation, then changes in NREM or REM activity might mediate these effects. Some studies found SWA and/or spindle density to mediate age-related memory decline [38, 39], and three experiments that boosted SWA and/or spindles in older adults also improved episodic memory [40–42]. Though NREM processes are certainly important to memory functioning [43], over the past 50 years, greater REM sleep has been a more consistent cross-sectional and longitudinal predictor of cognitive longevity [44–46]. The historical evidence linking REM sleep to better cognitive aging [47], when coupled with theorizing that REM preplays future-relevant associations [27], leads to the prediction that aging will compromise prospective memory to the extent that REM sleep declines.

Methods

Overview of design

Adult participants were recruited for a three-night study. Baseline night data have previously been reported [48]. On night 2 and night 3, participants encoded a prospective memory task and a control task, respectively (night order counterbalanced). In the morning, we tested task performance and evaluated: (1) whether prospective memory task performance declined with increasing age, (2) whether SWA, spindle density, or REM sleep during the preceding night mediated age differences in prospective memory performance, and (3) whether these results were robust when controlling for encoding, control-task, and ongoing task performance.

Participants

Eighty healthy adults between the ages of 18 and 84 completed the prospective memory and polysomnography procedures. Participants were recruited through fliers, local news advertisements, and outreach programs in the central Texas area. Exclusion criteria included taking prescribed medications that were known to affect sleep (selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, sedative-hypnotics, cholinesterase inhibitors); having a history of psychiatric or neurological disorders; or having a history of insomnia, narcolepsy, or an apnea–hypopnea index (AHI) ≥30 during the adaptation night. All participants scored 24 or higher on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), which is a common cutoff for dementia screening [49]. For generalizability, we did not exclude participants with mild-to-moderate sleep apnea. We excluded two participants for not completing all three study nights, one participant due to a protocol deviation (repeated conditions), and one participant for not returning for night 3 until after longer than 1 month (mean number of days between night 2 and night 3 was 1.63 ± 1.36 days). Descriptive data for the remaining 76 participants are presented in Table 1. The study was approved by the Baylor Institutional Review Board and all participants provided informed consent.

Table 1.

Demographic, neuropsychological testing, sleep/circadian questionnaire, and health information

Adults < 30 years
(n = 40)
Adults ≥ 30 years
(n = 36)
Overall sample (N = 76) Correlation with chronological age
Age (in years) 20.28 ± 1.68 62.08 ± 12.83 40.08 ± 22.80
Gender (% female) 60.0% 50.0% 55.3% r(74) = .16, p = .16
Race/ethnicity (% Caucasian) 57.5% 75.0% 65.8% r(74) = .27, p = .02
Education (in years) 14.11 ± 1.16 15.70 ± 3.97 14.85 ± 2.93 r(73) = .21, p = .07
MMSE (of 30) 28.78 ± 1.19 28.03 ± 1.63 28.42 ± 1.45 r(74) = −.21, p = .07
Working memory reading span (of 30) 23.44 ± 4.35 17.32 ± 6.91 20.54 ± 6.45 r(74) = −.51, p < .001
Phonemic fluency (summed FAS) 39.20 ± 8.85 34.69 ± 10.54 37.07 ± 9.89 r(74) = −.24, p = .04
Semantic fluency (summed categories) 47.73 ± 9.84 41.17 ± 9.96 44.62 ± 10.37 r(74) = −.32, p = .01
Mill Hill Vocabulary (proportion correct) 0.70 ± 0.12 0.65 ± 0.13 0.68 ± 0.13 r(74) = −.16, p = .16
PRMQ—total (of 80) 40.05 ± 5.79 37.15 ± 10.25 38.72 ± 8.22 r(72) = −.27, p = .02
PSQI—global score 4.70 ± 2.19 6.19 ± 3.54 5.41 ± 2.99 r(74) = .22, p = .06
PSQI—habitual bedtime 00:18 ± 72.29 min 22:28 ± 68.84 min 23:25 ± 89.32 min r(74) = −.62, p < .001
PSQI—habitual wake time 08:10 ± 75.46 min 06:45 ± 76.82 min 07:30 ± 86.77 min r(74) = −.48, p < .001
ESS—total score 8.87 ± 3.31 8.19 ± 4.41 8.54 ± 3.87 r(72) = −.14, p = .24
MEQ—total score 65.83 ± 9.75 57.81 ± 9.69 62.03 ± 10.46 r(74) = −.26, p = .02
GDS—total score 2.25 ± 2.23 2.67 ± 2.95 2.45 ± 2.58 r(74) = −.09, p = .43

Inferential statistics were conducted using chronological age as a continuous variable, though we also provide descriptive data separated at age 30 for illustrative purposes. There were missing data for years of education (n = 1), PRMQ (n = 2), and ESS (n = 2).

ESS = Epworth Sleepiness Scale; GDS = Geriatric Depression Scale; MEQ = Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire; MMSE = Mini-Mental Status Examination; PSQI = Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; PRMQ = Prospective Retrospective Memory Questionnaire.

Sleep measurement

Overnight polysomnography was recorded in a sound-attenuated sleep laboratory using Grass Comet XL Plus systems. The polysomnography montage consisted of electroencephalography (EEG) from Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4, Fz, C3, C4, P3, P4, Pz, O1, O2, and Oz (grounded at Fpz and Cz locations, referenced to contralateral mastoids), recorded at 200 samples per second. The montage further included left and right electrooculography (EOG), mentalis electromyography (EMG), and breathing measures (nasal pressure, chest and abdomen movements, finger pulse oximetry). Sleep stages were scored in 30-s epochs by a certified polysomnography technician according to AASM guidelines [50], and masked to condition. PSG variables included total sleep time, sleep efficiency, sleep onset latency, wake after sleep onset, AHI, and sleep stages (N1, N2, N3, and REM sleep).

SWA spectral power analysis

Spectral analysis of SWA is considered to be a better measure of slow wave quality than duration of visually scored slow-wave sleep (SWS/N3) [31]. We used Brain Products BrainVision Analyzer 2.0 software to conduct such analyses. Visual inspection for confounding effects of movement or electrode artifact was performed by trained research personnel, and epochs containing artifact (on average 1.82% of all epochs) were excluded from all analyses. The EEG data were band-pass filtered with high- and low-pass cutoffs of 0.3 and 35 Hz and sampling rate was modified to 128 Hz. Each participant file was segmented into equal 4-s segments, and we applied symmetric Hanning window with 50% overlap to decrease edge effects. We excluded EEG data during wake epochs. Using the remaining sleep epochs, we performed fast Fourier transform (FFT) with a resolution of 0.25 Hz to generate spectral power density (µV2/Hz) at all scalp channels for each of the following frequency bands: 0.5–1 Hz (slow oscillations), 1–4 Hz (delta SWA), 4–8 Hz (theta), 8–12 Hz (alpha), 12–16 Hz (sigma), and 16–32 Hz (beta). We analyzed slow oscillations and delta SWA using three approaches: (1) averaged across all electrodes and the entire night, (2) averaged across only frontal electrodes during NREM, and (3) averaged across only frontal electrodes during SWS.

Spindle detection analysis

There are several procedures for counting sleep spindles, but not all methods are equally valid. When comparing six automated spindle analysis methods to expert consensus labeling [51], Wamsley and colleagues’ wavelet-based algorithm [52] produced the best agreement of the automated detectors. In brief, Wamsley’s algorithm uses Morlet wavelet to perform time-frequency transformation on raw EEG data. Then, the algorithm automatically detects EEG events in the 10–16 Hz frequency range and identifies events exceeding 4.5 times the mean signal amplitude of artifact-free epochs that have a minimum duration of 300 ms [51]. We implemented Wamsley’s method in Matlab 9.0 during artifact-free epochs averaged across frontal channels (resampled to 100 Hz) to quantify N2 and SWS spindle density.

Questionnaire and neuropsychological measures

Each night, participants were asked to complete questionnaires during electrode application to keep them awake and engaged. The questionnaires that were relevant to sleep and prospective memory are displayed in Table 1 (additional detailed analyses in Supplementary Table S1), and include the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) [53], Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS) [54], Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire [55], Geriatric Depression Scale [56], and Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire (PRMQ) [57]. We also assessed semantic memory (three vocabulary tests), working memory capacity (automated reading span) [58], semantic fluency (categories of animals, fruits, and cars) [59], and phonemic fluency (FAS—Controlled Oral Word Association Test) [60]. Participants were also asked to maintain a sleep diary before laboratory sessions to track recent sleep durations and napping.

Procedure

Participants arrived before 21:00 h and they completed questionnaires, electrode application, and 20 min of assorted cognitive tasks. After those tasks, participants were randomly assigned to the prospective memory task procedures or the control task procedures (within-subjects design with night order counterbalanced). We used a prospective memory task that typically shows age-preservation of retrieval processes [36, 37] and has previously been used to show sleep-dependent memory consolidation effects in young adults [13].

As illustrated in Figure 1, in laboratory-based tests of prospective memory, participants are first introduced to background, “ongoing” tasks before being given their prospective memory intention [61]. One ongoing task was category decision, in which participants responded yes or no whether two words belonged to the same category (by pressing keys labeled on the number pad). A second ongoing task was lexical decision, in which participants responded yes or no whether a series of letters formed a word or nonword. A third ongoing task was living/nonliving decision in which participants responded yes or no whether a noun represented a living object. Participants completed evening warm-up baseline blocks of each task. The ongoing task order was randomized for each participant.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Prospective memory testing procedure. Participants completed three ongoing tasks with a prospective memory or control recognition task embedded. aOngoing task order was randomized. bProspective memory and control task order were counterbalanced. cTarget word set was counterbalanced across conditions.

Following the evening warm-up baseline blocks, participants were told, via computer instructions, that there was an interest in their ability to remember to perform an action in the future (i.e. prospective memory). In addition to the computerized tasks they would perform the next morning, if they ever saw the words “table” or “horse” then they should remember to press the P key on the keyboard. Following the recommended approach in prospective memory studies [62], participants were told that they could press the P key immediately or on the following few trials, and that no one would remind them to perform this task. To ensure that all participants encoded the prospective memory instructions, they were required to repeat them out loud three times with the experimenter present. They then completed a practice block of pressing the P key in response to a target word.

During the counterbalanced control night, the procedures were identical with two exceptions. First, participants were given the control task of retaining the control words “shape” and “media” for experimenter-prompted recognition tests the following day. Second, the ongoing task stimuli lists, as well as the shape/media and horse/table word sets, were counterbalanced across conditions.

By comparing ongoing task performance across the counterbalanced prospective memory and control task days, one can model the extent to which having a prospective memory task interferes with ongoing task performance. If ongoing task performance consistently suffers on the prospective memory day relative to the control day, then that indicates that participants were vigilantly monitoring (searching) for prospective memory cue words [63]. If performance does not suffer before prospective memory cues, then retrieval is dependent on spontaneous/automatic processes. The efficacy of spontaneous/automatic retrieval processes is known to depend on the strength of the cue-intention association (for detailed theoretical overview, see the Multiprocess Framework of Prospective Memory [7, 20]). Recent work shows that participants will transiently monitor during test sessions, but that during periods in which monitoring is disengaged, participants can still spontaneously retrieve an intention if the cue-intention link is strong (e.g. consolidated) [8, 20]. Previous work on sleep and prospective memory consolidation in young adults indicates that sleep consolidates the cue-intention association, rather than increases vigilant monitoring [13, 14].

Lights out was at approximately 22:30, and next-day lights on was at 07:30. Thus, time in bed was 9 h. In the morning, after being allowed to use the restroom, drink water, and get dressed, participants completed several test blocks (Figure 1). Participants were not reminded of the prospective memory or control tasks. Across the three ongoing task test blocks, there were a total of 450 trials (a 500 ms blank screen occurred between trials). Responding with the yes, no, or P keys advanced the screen to the next trial. Twelve prospective memory trials were interspersed across the three test blocks (in the control condition, there were 12 interspersed recognition tests). It is critical that the prospective memory cue words occur rarely, otherwise participants will continuously monitor, thereby changing the nature of the task from testing spontaneous/automatic prospective memory processes to simply testing vigilance and cue rehearsal [62].

Statistical analyses

First, we used analysis of variance/covariance (ANOVA/ANCOVA) and Pearson product-moment correlations to investigate whether prospective memory performance declined in relation to ongoing task context and chronological age. Chronological age was treated as a continuous variable in all inferential statistical analyses, though in some tables and figures we also reported descriptive data separated by age 30 (e.g. most memory consolidation studies only enroll young adults under the age of 30). Prospective memory consolidation was expressed as morning performance after regressing evening practice performance (standardized residual scores), to protect against the statistical limitations of difference scores (note, however, that the primary findings all replicated when solely using morning performance) [64]. Second, we examined correlations between prospective memory consolidation and hypothesized polysomnography variables (SWA, spindle density, REM), controlling for chronological age, and correcting for multiple comparisons. Third, to test sleep as a mediating variable of age effects on prospective memory consolidation, we conducted a bootstrapping mediation analysis. We conducted the mediation analysis first without adjustment, second after adjusting for performance on the control and ongoing tasks, and third after adjusting for a broader range of variables that have previously been implicated in sleep and aging relationships [65], including demographic, cognitive, and mental health factors (Table 1). Statistical analyses were implemented in SPSS version 23.

Results

Sleep across prospective memory and control nights

Table 2 demonstrates that most sleep variables demonstrated strong inter-night correlations. No polysomnography variable differed significantly across prospective memory and control-task nights (note that data were missing for two control nights). However, as expected, there were widespread age-related changes in polysomnography variables. With increasing age, there was greater sleep fragmentation (sleep efficiency, wake after sleep onset) and higher AHIs. Older participants showed longer N1 sleep, but shorter SWS and REM sleep. Aging severely disrupted SWA and N2 frontal spindle density (Table 3).

Table 2.

Sleep stage scoring on the prospective memory and control nights in relation to aging

Prospective memory night Control night Inferential statistics
Adults
<30
Adults ≥30 Adults
<30
Adults ≥30 Age correlation Inter-night correlation Night effect
Total sleep time (min) 511.50 ± 44.80 426.49 ± 64.48 513.08 ± 41.35 428.63 ± 67.00 r(74) = −.65, p < .001 Average ICC = .81, p < .001 p = .74
Sleep efficiency (%) 92.02 ± 4.81 82.94 ± 10.73 93.20 ± 4.28 83.34 ± 11.46 r(74) = −.58, p < .001 Average ICC = .74, p < .001 p = .67
Sleep onset latency (min) 17.66 ± 14.26 14.79 ± 12.41 13.29 ± 6.78 17.87 ± 22.90 r(74) < .01, p = .97 Average ICC = .37, p = .03 p = .68
Wake after sleep onset (min) 26.88 ± 20.87 72.03 ± 53.36 24.62 ± 25.49 69.80 ± 55.35 r(74) = .59, p < .001 Average ICC = .67, p < .001 p = .87
N1 (min) 26.39 ± 20.17 48.32 ± 34.58 24.86 ± 19.78 37.84 ± 19.00 r(74) = .43, p < .001 Average ICC = .86, p < .001 p = .10
N2 (min) 296.28 ± 43.65 230.62 ± 59.41 300.81 ± 38.24 238.41 ± 51.03 r(74) = −.60, p < .001 Average ICC = .71, p < .001 p = .73
SWS/N3 (min) 78.36 ± 19.78 67.01 ± 40.78 81.14 ± 22.17 70.32 ± 35.63 r(74) = −.27, p = .02 Average ICC = .79, p < .001 p = .50
REM (min) 110.46 ± 24.97 79.38 ± 36.25 106.38 ± 22.24 81.98 ± 31.68 r(74) = −.52, p < .001 Average ICC = .75, p < .001 p = .41
Apnea–Hypopnea Index events/h 0.39 ± 0.91 4.99 ± 6.23 0.36 ± 0.49 4.84 ± 4.77 r(74) = .60, p < .001 Average ICC = .77, p < .001 p = .74

Descriptive data are provided using an age group cutoff of 30 years old, but correlational data relate chronological age as a continuous variable to sleep variables. Sleep variables correlated strongly across the two experimental nights (measured by intraclass correlation [ICC]) and did not differ by condition. Data were missing/corrupted for two control nights.

Table 3.

Quantitative EEG analyses on the prospective memory night in relation to chronological age and prospective memory consolidation

Correlation with chronological age Encoding-adjusted correlation with prospective memory Age and encoding adjusted correlation with prospective memory
SWA (mean for all electrodes, epochs) µV2/Hz r(74) = −.60, p < .001 r(74) = .18, p = .11 r p(73) = .04, p = .73
SO (mean for all electrodes, epochs) µV2/Hz r(74) = −.44, p < .001 r(74) = .10, p = .39 r p(73) = −.01, p = .91
Frontal SWA in NREM µV2/Hz r(74) = −.61, p < .001 r(74) = .26, p = .03 r p(73) = .13, p = .26
Frontal SO in NREM µV2/Hz r(74) = −.55, p < .001 r(74) = .19, p = .11 r p(73) = .06, p = .61
Frontal SWA in SWS µV2/Hz r(73) = −.66, p < .001 r(73) = .23, p = .05 r p(72) = .09, p = .46
Frontal SO in SWS µV2/Hz r(73) = −.68, p < .001 r(73) = .22, p = .06 r p(72) = .07, p = .55
Frontal spindle density in N2 r(74) = −.45, p < .001 r(73) = .15, p = .21 r p(73) = .04, p = .74
Frontal spindle density in N3 r(72) = −.05, p = .66 r(72) = −.01, p = .92 r p(71) = −.03, p = .83

Analyses included delta slow wave activity (SWA, 1–4 Hz), slow oscillations (SO, 0.5–1.0 Hz), and spindle density. Frontal channels included Fp1, Fp2, F3, Fz, and F4. Prospective memory consolidation was operationalized as the standardized residual score of morning prospective memory performance after adjusting for previous night encoding practice block performance.

Ongoing task performance

We first evaluated evening/baseline ongoing task performance, that is, performance before encoding a prospective memory or control task. ANCOVAs on chronological age and condition (prospective memory/control) indicated that evening ongoing task response times and accuracy did not differ across conditions (all Fs ≤ 1.0; Supplementary Table S2). We conducted similar ANCOVAs on morning ongoing task performance, with these data also producing nonsignificant patterns. When averaging ongoing task accuracy across filler/nontarget trials, there were no significant differences between the prospective memory and control conditions in morning ongoing task accuracy (OngoingProspective: M = 0.938, SD = 0.032; OngoingControl: M = 0.940, SD = 0.030; p > .05) or morning ongoing task response times (responses times were averaged for correct filler trials; OngoingProspective: M = 1043 ms, SD = 300; OngoingControl: M = 1001 ms, SD = 308; p > .05).

A more complex pattern emerged for mixed ANCOVA analyses that not only included chronological age and condition (prospective memory/control), but also included quartile segments of each ongoing task context (1–4), and controlled for performance on the corresponding evening/baseline ongoing task. For response times (Supplementary Figure S1), there was a significant condition by quartile interaction in the living/nonliving task [F(3, 210) = 3.32, MSE = 9452.19, p < .001] and the lexical decision task [F(3, 210) = 3.32, MSE = 5844.49, p = .03], but not the category decision task (p > .05) for which there was only a condition main effect [F(1, 207) = 9.12, MSE = 10557.04, p = .004]. For proportion correct accuracy (Supplementary Figure S2), there was a significant condition by quartile interaction for the lexical decision task [F(3, 210) = 3.02, MSE = .001, p = .03] and category decision task [F(3, 207) = 4.93, MSE = .002, p = .003], and a three-way interaction between chronological age, condition, and quartile for the living/nonliving task [F(3, 207) = 4.78, MSE = .002, p = .003]. The data are illustrated in Supplementary Figures S1 and S2, and collectively they show that there was a mixture of reliance on spontaneous/automatic retrieval and monitoring, consistent with the Multiprocess Framework’s account of spontaneous retrieval processes sometimes initiating transient monitoring processes [8, 20].

Prospective memory and control task performance

A mixed ANCOVA on condition and chronological age resulted in a significant interaction for performance on morning control trials versus morning prospective-memory trials, F(1, 72) = 7.47, MSE = .06, p = .008. Accuracy on the morning control recognition task was high (proportion correct: M = 0.97, SD = 0.06) and age invariant [r(71) = −.10, p = .43], demonstrating that participants of all ages could easily retain the shape/media and horse/table control words. By contrast, “remembering to remember” to perform an action when seeing the prospective memory cue words was much more difficult for participants (proportion correct: M = 0.53, SD = 0.37), and declined as a function of increasing age, r(74) = −.34, p = .003 (Figure 2). Prospective memory performance did not significantly differ as a function of ongoing task context or night order counterbalance (Fs < 2.2, ps > .10).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Prospective memory performance decreased with increasing age in all ongoing task contexts (category decision, r = −.29, p = .01, lexical decision, r = −.21, p = .07, living/nonliving decision, r = −.39, p < .001). Morning prospective memory performance is the proportion correct averaged for four trials in each of the three contexts. Error bars reflect standard errors.

The age effect on prospective memory was still significant when covarying participants’ performance during the evening encoding-practice-block to produce a standardized residual score of prospective memory consolidation, F(1, 73) = 5.48, MSE = 1.93, p = .02 [64]. Furthermore, the age decline in prospective memory consolidation was maintained when statistically controlling for performance on the control recognition task, F(1, 71) = 7.96, MSE = 3.04, p = .006, ongoing task response times, F(1, 73) = 4.64, p = .03, and ongoing task accuracy, F(1, 73) = 9.23, p = .003. Thus, prospective memory consolidation processes decline in older age, even when accounting for age variability in quickly and accurately completing cognitive tasks.

Sleep and prospective memory correlations

When controlling for chronological age, prospective memory consolidation was significantly correlated with REM sleep duration, r(73) = .32, p = .005 (even following Bonferroni correction). Figure 3 shows the scatterplot between REM sleep duration and prospective memory consolidation, with similar effect sizes across young adults and middle-to-older aged adults. More REM sleep did not simply mean greater vigilant monitoring during the test phase (e.g. ongoing task speed). When controlling for chronological age and evening/baseline ongoing task performance, there was no association between REM sleep duration and morning ongoing task response times during the category decision task [r(72) = .06, p = .62], lexical decision task [r(72) = −.08, p = .50], or living/nonliving task [r(72) = −.04, p = .75; see also Supplementary Table S3). Furthermore, the REM–memory relationship did not reflect variability in recent sleep durations. We examined sleep diary and PSG data from participants the night before the prospective memory condition (data available for 64 participants, M = 7.28 h, 15.9% reported napping). After controlling for age, prospective memory performance was not significantly associated with previous-day sleep duration [r(62) = .15] or nap duration [r(62) = −.14]. Thus, recent sleep history did not explain the association between REM sleep and prospective memory consolidation.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Prospective memory consolidation was associated with previous-night REM sleep duration. Prospective memory consolidation is operationalized as the standardized residuals of morning prospective memory performance after adjusting for evening encoding practice block performance.

Prospective memory consolidation was selectively associated with REM sleep. Table 3 shows that SWA delta power, frontal slow oscillations, NREM spindle density, total sleep time, and AHI were each unrelated to prospective memory consolidation after controlling for chronological age and evening encoding (all ps > .05). Topographic analysis of spindle density from different electrode sites resulted in no significant associations with prospective memory consolidation (Supplementary Figure S3).

Mediation analyses

To test REM sleep duration as a mediator of the effect of aging on prospective memory consolidation, we took a bootstrap estimation approach using 5,000 samples [65]. The unadjusted analysis is illustrated in Figure 4. Consistent with full mediation, when including REM sleep as a mediator, chronological age was no longer a significant predictor of prospective memory performance, b = −.0024, SE = 0.0020, p = 0.25. Bootstrapping analysis showed the indirect coefficient to be significant, b = −.0031, SE = 0.0011 (95% CI = −0.0056 to −0.0012), supporting the hypothesis that REM sleep was a mediator between chronological age and prospective memory performance.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

REM sleep duration mediated the effect of age on prospective memory. The values are unstandardized regression coefficients (top) and p values (bottom).

Next, we tested for mediation after controlling for control task and ongoing task performance. Prospective memory was significantly associated with both chronological age, b = −.0056, SE = 0.0022, p = .01, and with REM sleep, b = .0035, SE = 0.0015, p = 0.02. When controlling for REM sleep, chronological age was no longer a significant predictor of prospective memory performance, b = −.0040, SE = 0.0022, p = .08. Bootstrapping showed the indirect coefficient to be significant, b = −.0016, SE = 0.0009 (95% CI = −0.0042 to −0.0004). Furthermore, these results generally replicated even when additionally controlling for several other variables that have been implicated in sleep, cognition, and aging [33], including gender, race/ethnicity, education, MMSE, PRMQ, PSQI, ESS, working memory, fluency, semantic memory, circadian preference, and depression. In this adjusted analysis, prospective memory was significantly associated with REM sleep, b = .0031, SE = 0.0015, p = .046, and age, b = −.0070, SE = 0.0027, p = .01 (after controlling for REM sleep, b = −.0053, SE = 0.0028, p = .06). Even when controlling for these numerous covariates, bootstrapping still showed the indirect coefficient to be marginally significant, b = −.0017, SE = 0.0013 (95% CI = −0.0051 to 0.0000), such that REM sleep accounted for 24.29% of the overall effect [66].

Discussion

Prospective memory consolidation is expected to decline with increasing age, with a significant amount of that decline being mediated by REM sleep. Whereas some previous work found the association between sleep and neurocognitive measures to be weakened with advancing age [33, 67–69], we found that REM sleep duration was associated with prospective memory consolidation across age groups. Interestingly, we continued to observe this pattern even when controlling for a myriad of variables related to general cognitive functioning, depression, circadian preference, recent sleep quality, and polysomnography-defined total sleep time. In the present work, REM sleep did not simply boost one’s attentional ability to vigilantly monitor for prospective memory cues (e.g. ongoing task speed); instead, the collective findings indicated that REM sleep helped preserve the association between what needed to be remembered and when it needed to be remembered. These findings are consistent with recent theorizing that REM sleep functions to preplay future scenarios so as to promote next-day automatic cognitive processes [27].

There are only two published studies in young adults that examined how sleep physiology related to prospective memory consolidation. In one study, prospective memory did not significantly correlate with any traditional sleep stage parameters [14], and in another study, prospective memory was better following a 3-h early-night sleep interval (SWS-rich) than a 3-h late-night sleep interval (REM-rich) [15]. Rather than contradicting those findings, the differences between our study and previous studies can be understood by the differences in time-in-bed opportunity. The current study included a 9-h time-in-bed opportunity, resulting in an average of 110.5 min of REM sleep. The two published studies included briefer opportunities for sleep, resulting in averages of approximately 75 and 50 min of REM, respectively. Perhaps REM sleep will emerge as a more consistent predictor of memory consolidation if future studies allow for greater variability in REM sleep by extending the time-in-bed opportunity.

Reconsideration of the two-stage/sequential hypothesis of memory processing [70, 71] may unite some of the findings in the sleep and cognitive aging literature. On the one hand, the 50+ year history of neuropsychological studies that related cognitive functioning to polysomnography variables have often reported correlations with REM sleep [33]. In the largest [45] and longest [46] longitudinal studies of polysomnography and cognitive outcomes, low REM sleep predicted more rapid cognitive aging (and SWS did not). Yet, on the other hand, recent cross-sectional studies have suggested that SWA or spindle density mediate age-related decline in memory consolidation [32, 38, 39, 72]. Even more provocative are the findings that experimentally increasing SWA—spindle activity improved memory functioning, at least temporarily, in some older adults [40–42]. In our study of adults aged 18–84, REM sleep was robustly associated with prospective memory, as was frontal SWA (however, SWA was only associated before controlling for chronological age). In weighing all of these findings, our view is that taking an either-REM-or-SWA approach may be less useful for explaining and remedying the numerous cognitive deficits associated with aging than recognizing that both REM and SWA contribute, perhaps in a sequential manner [70, 71].

Limitations of this work included a cross-sectional design and a modest sample size (e.g. relatively few middle-aged adults). Sleep and memory studies typically include many statistical tests (as did the current study) [73], but we minimized the chance for false positives by focusing on three empirically based aspects of sleep (REM, SWA, spindles). The REM-memory correlation was significant even with Bonferroni correction, and similar effect sizes were observed in young adults and middle-to-older aged adults [33]. The strength of the age and prospective memory correlation may have been influenced by age differences in optimal time of learning/testing, though statistically controlling for circadian preference did not impact the primary finding that REM sleep duration mediated this age–memory association.

Conclusions

NREM and REM processes may interact dynamically to help preserve a range of cognitive processes [74]. In the current work, REM sleep duration was significantly associated with prospective memory consolidation in young adults and middle-to-older aged adults, which adds to a developing literature on prospective memory being impacted by sleep disruption [9–12, 75–77] and by clinical sleep disorders [78–80]. The current findings fit the theoretical view that prospective memory consolidation occurs via reactivation processes followed by cue–intention association processes, but alternative accounts should also be tested. One alternative account is that cholinergic activity, which promotes both REM sleep [81] and prospective memory [82], is known to decline with aging [44]. Another account is that REM sleep functions to process emotional content [24], and to the extent that an unfinished prospective memory intention is deemed stressful [48], prospective memories may undergo emotional processing during REM sleep. Though there is still much to be learned about how NREM and REM activity interact to preserve brain health and memory functioning [74, 83], the current data indicate that experimentally increasing REM sleep may be a viable next target for combating human errors in “remembering to remember.”

Funding

This work was supported by a Sleep Research Society Foundation Early Career Development Award. Additional support was provided by the National Institutes of Health (AG053161 to M.K.S.).

Conflict of interest statement. D.L.B. is a consultant for Merck, Jazz, Ferring, Eisai, Respicardia and a speaker for Merck. There are no other financial disclosures.

Supplementary Material

zsz055_suppl_Supplementary_Materials

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful for the assistance of Hannah Ballard, Mericyn Daunis, Mary High, Madison Krueger, Sanna Lokhandwala, Stacy Nguyen, Cole Rowley, Kiersten Scott, Claudina Tami, Taylor Terlizzese, Ali Villagran, and Matthew Willis. We are additionally grateful to Simon Warby and Karine Lacourse for providing sample data to validate the spindle detection performance, and Javier Lopez-Calderon and Steven Luck for EEG analysis suggestions.

References

  • 1. Cornford FM. Plato’s Theory of Knowledge: The Theaetetus and the Sophist. North Chelmsford, MA: Courier Corporation; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Szpunar KK, et al. . A taxonomy of prospection: Introducing an organizational framework for future-oriented cognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111(52):18414–18421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Dragoi G, et al. . Preplay of future place cell sequences by hippocampal cellular assemblies. Nature. 2011;469(7330):397–401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Kliegel M, et al. . Prospective memory research: why is it relevant? Intl J Psychol. 2003;38(4):193–194. [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Loft S. Applying psychological science to examine prospective memory in simulated air traffic control. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2014;23(5):326–331. [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Dismukes RK. Prospective memory in workplace and everyday situations. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2012;21(4):215–220. [Google Scholar]
  • 7. McDaniel MA, et al. . Prospective Memory: An Overview and Synthesis of an Emerging Field. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Shelton JT, et al. . The dynamic interplay between bottom-up and top-down processes supporting prospective remembering. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2017;26(4):352–358. [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Esposito MJ, et al. . Sleep deprivation and time-based prospective memory. Sleep. 2015;38(11):1823–1826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Grundgeiger T, et al. . Effects of sleep deprivation on prospective memory. Memory. 2014;22(6):679–686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Kyle SD, et al. . Sleep and cognitive performance: cross-sectional associations in the UK biobank. Sleep Med. 2017;38:85–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Dement WC. The Stanford Sleep Book. Author; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Scullin MK, et al. . Remembering to execute a goal: Sleep on it! Psychol Sci. 2010;21(7):1028–1035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Diekelmann S, et al. . Sleep improves prospective remembering by facilitating spontaneous-associative retrieval processes. PLoS One. 2013;8(10):e77621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Diekelmann S, et al. . Sleep to implement an intention. Sleep. 2013;36(1):149–153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Barner C, et al. . Consolidation of prospective memory: effects of sleep on completed and reinstated intentions. Front Psychol. 2016;7:2025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Barner C, et al. . Effects of sleep on the realization of complex plans. J Sleep Res. 2019; 28: 1–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Cunningham TJ, et al. . Prospective memory performance negatively correlates with slow-wave sleep. Sleep. 2017;40(Suppl):A83–A84. [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Tulving E. Ecphoric processes in episodic memory. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1983;302(1110):361–371. [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Scullin MK, et al. . The dynamic multiprocess framework: evidence from prospective memory with contextual variability. Cogn Psychol. 2013;67(1–2):55–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Scullin MK, et al. . Control of cost in prospective memory: evidence for spontaneous retrieval processes. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2010;36(1):190–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Scullin MK, et al. . Focal/nonfocal cue effects in prospective memory: monitoring difficulty or different retrieval processes? J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2010;36(3):736–749. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Diekelmann S, et al. . The memory function of sleep. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11(2):114–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Stickgold R, et al. . Sleep-dependent memory triage: evolving generalization through selective processing. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16(2):139–145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Rasch B, et al. . In search of a role of REM sleep in memory formation. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2015;122:1–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Boyce R, et al. . Causal evidence for the role of REM sleep theta rhythm in contextual memory consolidation. Science. 2016;352(6287):812–816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Llewellyn S, et al. . Not only … but also: REM sleep creates and NREM Stage 2 instantiates landmark junctions in cortical memory networks. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2015;122:69–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Anderson FT, McDaniel MA, Einstein GO. Remembering to remember: an examination of the cognitive processes underlying prospective memory. In: Byrne JH, ed. Reference Module in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Psychology, Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference. 2nd ed. Oxford, UK: Oxford Centre for Computational Neuroscience; 2017:451–463. [Google Scholar]
  • 29. McDaniel MA, Einstein GO. Spontaneous retrieval in prospective memory. In: Nairne JS, ed. The Foundations of Remembering: Essays in Honor of Henry L. Roediger III. London, UK: Psychology Press; 2007:227–242. [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Spencer RM, et al. . Age-related decline of sleep-dependent consolidation. Learn Mem. 2007;14(7):480–484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Mander BA, et al. . Sleep and human aging. Neuron. 2017;94(1):19–36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Fogel S, et al. . Sleep spindles: a physiological marker of age-related changes in gray matter in brain regions supporting motor skill memory consolidation. Neurobiol Aging. 2017;49:154–164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Scullin MK, et al. . Sleep, cognition, and normal aging: integrating a half century of multidisciplinary research. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015;10(1):97–137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Gui WJ, et al. . Age-related differences in sleep-based memory consolidation: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia. 2017;97:46–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Scullin MK, et al. . The effects of an afternoon nap on episodic memory in young and older adults. Sleep. 2017;40(5):zsx035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Mullet HG, et al. . Prospective memory and aging: evidence for preserved spontaneous retrieval with exact but not related cues. Psychol Aging. 2013;28(4):910–922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Scullin MK, et al. . Prospective memory and aging: preserved spontaneous retrieval, but impaired deactivation, in older adults. Mem Cogn. 2011;39(7):1232–1240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Mander BA, et al. . Prefrontal atrophy, disrupted NREM slow waves and impaired hippocampal-dependent memory in aging. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16(3):357–364. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Varga AW, et al. . Effects of aging on slow-wave sleep dynamics and human spatial navigational memory consolidation. Neurobiol Aging. 2016;42:142–149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Ladenbauer J, et al. . Promoting sleep oscillations and their functional coupling by transcranial stimulation enhances memory consolidation in mild cognitive impairment. J Neurosci. 2017;37(30):7111–7124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Papalambros NA, et al. . Acoustic enhancement of sleep slow oscillations and concomitant memory improvement in older adults. Front Hum Neurosci. 2017;11:109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Westerberg CE, et al. . Memory improvement via slow-oscillatory stimulation during sleep in older adults. Neurobiol Aging. 2015;36(9):2577–2586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Rasch B, et al. . About sleep’s role in memory. Physiol Rev. 2013;93(2):681–766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Scullin MK, et al. . Is cognitive aging associated with levels of REM sleep or slow wave sleep? Sleep. 2015;38(3):335–336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Song Y, et al. ; Osteoporotic Fractures in Men (MrOS) Study Group Relationships between sleep stages and changes in cognitive function in older men: the MrOS sleep study. Sleep. 2015;38(3):411–421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Pase MP, Himali JJ, Grima N, et al. . REM sleep mechanisms predict incident dementia in the framingham heart study. Alzheimers Dement. 2017;13(7):910–911. [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Feinberg I, et al. . EEG sleep patterns as a function of normal and pathological aging in man. J Psychiatr Res. 1967;5(2):107–144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Scullin MK, et al. . The effects of bedtime writing on difficulty falling asleep: a polysomnographic study comparing to-do lists and completed activity lists. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2018;147(1):139–146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Folstein MF, et al. . “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Iber C, et al. . The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology and Technical Specifications. Westchester, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Warby SC, et al. . Sleep-spindle detection: crowdsourcing and evaluating performance of experts, non-experts and automated methods. Nat Methods. 2014;11(4):385–392. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Wamsley EJ, et al. . Reduced sleep spindles and spindle coherence in schizophrenia: mechanisms of impaired memory consolidation? Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71(2):154–161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Buysse DJ, et al. . The pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28(2):193–213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep. 1991;14(6):540–545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Horne JA, et al. . A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol. 1976;4(2):97–110. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Yesavage JA, et al. . Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) recent evidence and development of a shorter version. Clin Gerontol. 1986;5(1–2):165–173. [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Crawford JR, et al. . The Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire (PRMQ): normative data and latent structure in a large non-clinical sample. Memory. 2003;11(3):261–275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Oswald FL, et al. . The development of a short domain-general measure of working memory capacity. Behav Res Methods. 2015;47(4):1343–1355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Spreen O, Strauss E.. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests. 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Loonstra AS, et al. . COWAT metanorms across age, education, and gender. Appl Neuropsychol. 2001;8(3):161–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Einstein GO, et al. . Normal aging and prospective memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1990;16(4):717–726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. McDaniel MA, et al. . Dual pathways to prospective remembering. Front Neurosci. 2015;9:392–401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Smith RE. The cost of remembering to remember in event-based prospective memory: investigating the capacity demands of delayed intention performance. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2003;29(3):347–361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Scullin MK. Sleep, memory, and aging: the link between slow-wave sleep and episodic memory changes from younger to older adults. Psychol Aging. 2013;28(1):105–114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Hayes AF, et al. . The relative trustworthiness of inferential tests of the indirect effect in statistical mediation analysis: does method really matter? Psychol Sci. 2013;24(10):1918–1927. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Preacher KJ, et al. . Effect size measures for mediation models: quantitative strategies for communicating indirect effects. Psychol Methods. 2011;16(2):93–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Liu YR, et al. . Sleep-related brain atrophy and disrupted functional connectivity in older adults. Behav Brain Res. 2018;347:292–299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Miller MA, et al. . Cross-sectional study of sleep quantity and quality and amnestic and non-amnestic cognitive function in an ageing population: the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA). PLoS One. 2014;9(6):e100991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Edinger JD, et al. . Slow-wave sleep and waking cognitive performance II: findings among middle-aged adults with and without insomnia complaints. Physiol Behav. 2000;70(1–2):127–134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Giuditta A, et al. . The sequential hypothesis of the function of sleep. Behav Brain Res. 1995;69(1–2):157–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Giuditta A. Sleep memory processing: the sequential hypothesis. Front Syst Neurosci. 2014;8:219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Helfrich RF, et al. . Old brains come uncoupled in sleep: slow wave-spindle synchrony, brain atrophy, and forgetting. Neuron. 2018;97(1):221–230.e4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Mantua J. Sleep physiology correlations and human memory consolidation: where do we go from here? Sleep. 2018;41(2):zsx204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Scullin MK, Gao C. Dynamic contributions of slow wave sleep and REM sleep to cognitive longevity. Curr Sleep Med Reports. 2018;4:284–293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Leong RLF, et al. . Multiple nights of partial sleep deprivation do not affect prospective remembering at long delays. Sleep Med. 2018;44:19–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Occhionero M, et al. . The effect of sleep loss on dual time-based prospective memory tasks. Am J Psychol. 2017;130(1):93–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Fine L, Weinborn M, Ng A, et al. . Sleep disruption explains age-related prospective memory deficits: implications for cognitive aging and intervention. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2018:1–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Bezdicek O, et al. . Prospective memory impairment in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. Clin Neuropsychol. 2018;32(5):1019–1037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Marcone S, Gagnon JF, Desjardins C, et al. . Prospective memory in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder with or without mild cognitive impairment: a preliminary study. Clin Neuropsychol. 2018:1–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Li X, et al. . The prospective memory of patients with idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. Sleep Med. 2018;47:19–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81. Hornung OP, et al. . The relationship between REM sleep and memory consolidation in old age and effects of cholinergic medication. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61(6):750–757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82. Rusted JM, et al. . Positive effects of nicotine on cognition: the deployment of attention for prospective memory. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2009;202(1–3):93–102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83. Scullin MK. Do older adults need sleep? A review of neuroimaging, sleep, and aging studies. Curr Sleep Med Rep. 2017;3(3):204–214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

zsz055_suppl_Supplementary_Materials

RESOURCES