SUMMARY
Sensory perception arises from the integration of externally and internally driven representations of the world. Disrupted balance of these representations can lead to perceptual deficits and hallucinations. The serotonin-2A receptor (5-HT2AR) is associated with such perceptual alterations, both in its role in schizophrenia and in the action of hallucinogenic drugs. Despite this powerful influence on perception, relatively little is known about how serotonergic hallucinogens influence sensory processing in the neocortex. Using widefield and two-photon calcium imaging and single-unit electrophysiology in awake mice, we find that administration of the hallucinogenic selective 5-HT2AR agonist DOI (2,5-dimethoxy-4-iodoamphetamine) leads to a net reduction in visual response amplitude and surround suppression in primary visual cortex, as well as disrupted temporal dynamics. However, basic retinotopic organization, tuning properties, and receptive field structure remain intact. Our results provide support for models of hallucinations in which reduced bottom-up sensory drive is a key factor leading to altered perception.
Graphical Abstract
In Brief
Activation of serotonin-2A receptors (5-HT2ARs) is associated with hallucinations, but impacts on sensory processing are largely unknown. Michaiel et al. demonstrate that the 5-HT2AR agonist DOI strongly reduces sensory-evoked activity and disrupts temporal dynamics. These results support models of hallucinations that propose reduced bottom-up sensory drive.
INTRODUCTION
Both externally (bottom-up) and internally (top-down) driven representations of the world contribute to sensory perception. Disruption of accurate sensory perception, as occurs during hallucination, is hypothesized to result from increased top-down and/or decreased bottom-up signaling, leading to excessive reliance on prediction at the expense of sensory input (Cassidy et al., 2018; Grossberg 2000). Abnormal serotonin-2A receptor (5-HT2AR) activity is implicated in sensory hallucination, defined as the misinterpretation of sensory stimuli in space or time or the perception of an absent external stimulus. In particular, hallucinations and altered perception resulting from both schizophrenia and psychedelic drug administration are prevented by antagonism of 5-HT2ARs, supporting a central role of this receptor in mediating hallucinations (Schmidt et al., 1995; Vollenweider et al., 1998).
The cognitive and perceptual effects of 5-HT2AR modulation have been extensively studied, particularly in the context of psychedelic drugs (reviewed in Nichols, 2016). Recent studies have begun to elucidate the action of serotonergic hallucinogens on large-scale brain activity in humans using neuroimaging methods (Preller et al., 2018; Carhart-Harris et al., 2016). However, the impact on sensory information processing at the level of single neurons and populations of neurons is largely unknown. To our knowledge, measures of visually evoked responses after 5-HT2AR agonist administration in humans are limited to one study, which showed large reductions in pre-stimulus alpha-band LFP synchronization (Kometer et al., 2013). There have been few studies of individual V1 neuron responses to visual stimuli following administration of 5-HT2AR agonists, yielding varying findings of suppression, facilitation, or bidirectional changes in firing rate (Rose and Horn, 1977; Fox and Dray, 1979; Dray et al., 1980; Watakabe et al., 2009). Furthermore, these studies were conducted in anesthetized animals, did not measure individual neuron-tuning properties, and did not address cell type or layer specificity.
The selective 5-HT2AR agonist DOI (2,5-dimethoxy-4-iodoamphetamine) is known to be a powerful hallucinogen in humans (Shulgin and Shulgin, 1991) and has been used extensively to study 5-HT2AR function in animal models, particularly of schizophrenia and psychedelic drug action (for reviews, see Hanks and González-Maeso, 2013; Nichols 2016). In this study, we assessed the impact of DOI on visual processing at multiple scales, from retinotopic maps to individual neurons, using widefield and two-photon calcium imaging and single-unit electrophysiology in awake, head-fixed mice. Our results demonstrating how a serotonergic hallucinogen disrupts sensory processing should provide a deeper understanding of how cortical circuits generate a representation of the world based on sensory input.
RESULTS
To measure the effects of 5-HT2AR activation on spatial and temporal processing in visual cortex, we measured visual responses in mice head-fixed on a spherical treadmill (Dombeck et al., 2007) using widefield imaging and two-photon calcium imaging, and single-unit electrophysiology with silicon probes (Figure 1A). Following presentation of a set of visual stimuli, mice received a subcutaneous injection of either saline (control) or the 5-HT2AR agonist DOI (10 mg/kg; see STAR Methods and Figure S1 for an explanation of dose choice), and after a 15-to 20-min waiting period, the stimulus set was repeated. To explore how previous experience with visual stimuli may influence effects of 5-HT2AR signaling, we performed a subset of these passive viewing experiments with animals previously trained on a visually guided task, in addition to standard non-trained animals. As visual responses and surround suppression are modulated by behavioral state (Niell and Stryker, 2010; Ayaz et al., 2013), we separated data into stationary or running periods for statistical comparison. Notably, neither pupil size nor fraction of time running was different following drug administration (Figure S2), suggesting that changes observed were not due to differences in behavioral state.
Widefield imaging of cortical excitatory neurons in CaMKIIa-tTA:tetO-GCaMP6s mice (GCaMP6s mice; Wekselblatt et al., 2016) revealed no change in the retinotopic map of azimuth and elevation in visual cortex (Figures 1B and 1C; p = 0.999, Kruskal-Wallis; see also Movie S1) but a dramatic reduction in responses to grating patches in visual areas after DOI, but not saline, administration during stationary periods (Figures 1D and 1E). Interestingly, this reduction was larger in animals that had previously received training on a visual task than in animals naive to training (Figure 1F; p = 0.012, Kruskal-Wallis; paired t test: DOI trained: p = 0.031, n = 5; DOI naive: p = 0.049, n = 6; saline trained: p = 0.192, n = 5, saline naive: p = 0.917, n = 5). Passive stimuli used here were similar to those used in previous behavioral experiments (circular grating patches) but were different in size and location in visual space (see STAR Methods for further details), arguing against effects of perceptual learning. Furthermore, baseline responses between trained and naive animals were not statistically different (Figure S3).
Given that widefield signals represent the summed activity in cell bodies, dendrites, and axons from many different excitatory cortical neurons, we next used two-photon calcium imaging to study the effect at the level of individual neurons, focusing on spatial integration. A key mechanism by which V1 neurons integrate information across space is through surround suppression, where larger stimuli tend to decrease V1 responses. This phenomenon can be explained by divisive normalization of ‘‘driving’’ classical receptive field (CRF) responses by ‘‘suppressive’’ responses in the extra-CRF (eCRF). We performed two-photon imaging in L2/3 of V1 in GCaMP6s mice while showing grating patches of varying sizes (5°–50°), which revealed clear surround suppression in the neuropil responses (Figure 2A; see also Movie S2). Consistent with widefield imaging, DOI reduced the magnitude of visual responses at the level of neuropil, as well as the visual responses of individual neurons (Figure 2B). We computed size tuning curves from the individual neuron data (Figure 2C), fit these with a divisive normalization model (Ayaz et al., 2013; see STAR Methods), and measured the coefficients of the driving (RD) and suppressive (RS) fields. Both RD and RS were reduced after administration of DOI, but not saline (Figure 2D). These DOI-induced changes in RD and RS were significant for both naive and trained animals during stationary periods (Figure 2E; RD p = 0.021, RS p = 0.010, Kruskal-Wallis; paired t test: DOI trained: RD p = 0.003, RS p = 0.002 n = 9/215; DOI naive: RD p = 0.020, RS p = 0.012, n = 8/144; saline trained: RD p = 0.201, RS p = 0.730 n = 11/197; saline naive: RD p = 0.159, RS p = 0.317, n = 1½69; where n = animals/cells; alpha = 0.025 corrected for multiple comparisons). DOI also reduced RD during running bouts in trained, but not naive animals (not shown; RD p = 0.023, RS p = 0.032, Kruskal-Wallis; paired t test: DOI trained: RD p = 0.015, RS p = 0.026; DOI naive: RD p = 0.084, RS p = 0.357; saline trained: RD p = 0.773, RS p = 0.031; saline naive: RD p = 0.744, RS p = 0.559; alpha = corrected for multiple comparisons). Consistent with these changes in RD and RS, DOI reduced the suppression index in naive (stationary only) and trained (stationary and running) animals (Figures 2D and 2E; suppression index paired t test before versus after: DOI trained: pstat = 0.005, prun = 0.014; DOI naive: pstat = 0.034, prun = 0.814; saline trained: pstat = 0.285, prun = 0.150; saline naive: pstat = 0.261, prun = 0.390).
In order to determine how 5-HT2AR activation affects temporal dynamics of population activity, we recorded local field potentials (LFPs) using silicon probes and found the average LFP power in all cortical layers was reduced across a wide frequency range following administration of DOI in both spontaneous (not shown) and visually evoked activity (Figure 3A; paired t test, corrected for multiple comparisons: stationary saline: pdelta = 0.184, ptheta = 0.531, palpha = 0.254, pbeta = 0.065, pgamma = 0.0361, n = 12 animals; stationary DOI: pdelta = 0.127, ptheta = 0.0015, palpha = 0.002, pbeta = 0.0001, pgamma = 0.0001, n = 12 animals; running saline: pdelta = 0.072, ptheta = 0.995, palpha = 0.572, pbeta = 0.616, pgamma = 0.287, n = 12 animals; running DOI: pdelta = 0.766, ptheta = 0.0077, palpha = 0.02, pbeta = 0.0003, pgamma = 0.0005, n = 12 animals; alpha = 0.01). Interestingly, the visual stimulus-evoked increase in gamma power (28–35 Hz) was completely abolished after DOI administration. These results are consistent with findings from studies of hallucinogenic drug effects in humans using electroencephalography (EEG) and magnetoencephalography (MEG) (Kometer et al., 2013; Carhart-Harris et al., 2016), which also show an overall reduction in oscillatory synchronization.
We next aimed to examine how individual V1 neuron activity is affected by 5-HT2AR activation by analyzing responses of isolated single units to drifting sinusoidal gratings. We focused this analysis on L2/3 and L5 because they display distinct response properties (Niell and Stryker, 2008), and both excitatory and inhibitory neurons in these layers contain the highest 5-HT2AR density in mouse neocortex (Weber and Andrade, 2010). Units were classified as putative excitatory or narrow-spiking inhibitory based on spike waveform (Niell and Stryker, 2008). As such, inhibitory neurons in this study are likely fast-spiking parvalbumin (PV) cells and not somatostatin (SOM)-expressing cells. Following DOI administration, the peak visually evoked firing rate of excitatory V1 neurons was bidirectionally modulated (Figure 3B; saline: r2 = 0.74, p = 0.679, n = 155; DOI: r2 = 0.44, p = 0.181, n = 187; paired t test). Interestingly, we observed rate-specific modulation of responses; neurons with initially low firing rates were facilitated, and neurons with initially high firing rates were suppressed (Figure 3C), similar to observations with 5-HT2AR activation in anesthetized non-human primate and cat V1 (Watakabe et al., 2009; Rose and Horn, 1977). In contrast to the excitatory neuron population, inhibitory neurons did not change their peak evoked firing rate (saline: r2 = 0.73, p = 0.103, n = 26; DOI: r2 = 0.93, p = 0.812, n = 17; paired t test). The same pattern was observed during locomotive states (not shown; saline excitatory: r2 = 0.55, p = 0.057, inhibitory: r2 = 0.75, p = 0.215; DOI excitatory: r2 = 0.44, p = 0.7.15e-05, inhibitory r2 = 0.93, p = 0.577; paired t test).
To determine how strongly each cortical layer was affected by DOI, we calculated modulation indices of stationary peak firing rate across the neural population, where negative (positive) values represent neurons that reduced (increased) their rate following drug administration (Figures 3C and 3D). The distributions were shifted overall toward suppression; however, because these distributions were bidirectional, we calculated the mean absolute value for each layer to determine the strength of modulation independent of sign. This revealed visually evoked responses in L2/3 were more affected by DOI than saline (t test: p = 0.005, corrected for multiple comparisons), whereas spontaneous rate was not affected (Figure 3E). Thus, the effects of DOI are specific for layer and cell type and differ for spontaneous versus evoked activity.
We next determined how DOI affected the time course of V1 responses based on the peristimulus time histogram (PSTH) of responses to drifting gratings (Figure 4A). Following DOI administration, we saw layer-specific changes in the mean PSTH of visually responsive cells (neurons with peak visually evoked rate greater than 2 Hz in either the pre- or post-recording block). The mean response of both L2/3 and L5 was significantly reduced (two-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov test; L2/3: pstat = 0.0001, n = 37, prun = 0.004, n = 61; L5: pstat = 0.001, n = 13, prun = 0.026, n = 19), consistent with more neurons being suppressed than enhanced, whereas inhibitory units were not affected (inhibitory [inh.]: pstat = 0.878, n = 7, prun = 0.878, n = 10).
The time course of the mean PSTH showed a transient response at stimulus onset followed by a smaller sustained response, which was most pronounced in L2/3 neurons (Figure 4A). Notably, the transient (first 500 ms after stimulus onset) and sustained (500 ms preceding stimulus offset) components were differentially affected by DOI. We separated the two temporal components and found that L2/3 was strongly suppressed during the transient component (pstat = 0.0002, prun = 0.0035) and was only affected during the sustained component when animals were running (p = 0.0007; Figure 4B). L5 and inhibitory units, in contrast, did not show a significant net change in either temporal component (L5trans pstat = 0.0471; L5trans prun = 0.864; L5sus pstat = 0.436; L5sus prun = 0.727; inhtrans pstat = 0.587; inhtrans prun = 0.875; inhsus pstat = 0.964; inhsus prun = 0.852). Thus, DOI administration disrupts temporal dynamics of visual responses in L2/3 by strongly reducing the onset transient.
We next determined if DOI affected the encoding of low-level stimulus features and feature selectivity. Across the recorded population of neurons, we found no change for the preferred grating orientation following DOI administration (Figure 4C; saline: r2 = 0.92, p = 0.346, n = 37; DOI: r2 = 0.87, p = 0.639, n = 33; not shown; saline running: r2 = 0.87, p = 0.425, n = 27; DOI running: r2 = 0.81, p = 0.873, n = 43; paired t test). The mean orientation selectivity index was also unaffected by DOI and saline administration (Figure 4D; Wilcoxon rank sum test on mean of stationary and running; saline: p = 0.362, n = 100; DOI: p = 0.214, n = 91). Preferred direction of grating motion and mean direction selectivity index (DSI) were also unchanged (not shown; Wilcoxon rank sum test; preferred direction saline stationary: r2 = 0.95p= 0.912 n = 33, running: r2 = 0.5 p = 0.929 n = 25; DOI stationary: r2 = 0.57 p = 0.861 n = 56; running: r2 = 0.46 p = 0.486 n = 62; DSI saline stationary: p = 0.95 n = 71; running: p = 0.25 n = 71; DOI stationary: p = 0.33, n = 64; running: p = 0.70, n = 64). We also found no change in the distribution of spatial frequency preference for responsive cells, as the same proportions were selective to either low (0.01–0.02 cycles per degree [cpd]; paired t test mean of running and stationary; saline: p = 0.435; DOI: p = 0.823), medium (0.04–0.08 cpd; saline: p = 0.334; DOI: p = 0.397), or high (0.16–0.32 cpd; saline: p = 0.640; DOI: p = 0.485) spatial frequencies or to full-field flicker (saline: p = 0.267, DOI: p = 0.577) following DOI treatment (Figure 4E; saline: n = 100; DOI: n = 93). The observed changes in firing rate did not correlate with tuning properties or selectivity (not shown; saline preferred [pref] orientation [ori] stationary [stat]: r2 = 0.012, pref ori moving [mv]: r2 = 0.034, orientation selectivity index [OSI] stat: r2 = 0.005, OSI mv: r2 = 0.063; DOI: pref ori stat: r2 = 0.0001; pref ori mv: r2 = 0.0096; OSI stat: r2 = 0.021; OSI mv: r2 = 0.053).
To determine the similarity in receptive field structure before and after treatment, we calculated 2D correlation coefficients between raw spike-triggered average receptive fields (STAs) computed from pre- and post-recording sessions. We found no significant differences between the distributions of coefficients calculated from saline and DOI recording blocks (Figure 4F; two-sample t test: p = 0.348, nsaline = 28, nDOI = 41). Thus, despite significant changes in temporal dynamics and spatial contextual modulation, basic tuning properties and receptive field structure of individual V1 neurons were un-changed after 5-HT2AR activation.
DISCUSSION
Using widefield and two-photon calcium imaging and single-unit electrophysiology in awake mouse V1, we investigated how systemic administration of the hallucinogenic 5-HT2AR agonist DOI affects cortical processing of visual information. We found reductions in response gain and surround suppression and altered temporal dynamics but no changes in basic tuning properties. Together, this study provides a systematic measurement of the effects of hallucinogenic 5-HT2AR agonist administration on visual coding of cortical sensory neurons in awake animals.
It remains to be determined whether the observed effects are due to action on 5-HT2ARs within V1 or elsewhere. Watakabe et al. (2009) administered DOI locally through microinfusions in V1 and also observed bidirectional firing rate modulation, suggesting that 5-HT2AR activation in V1 is sufficient to drive neurophysiological changes consistent with systemic DOI administration. It is unknown, however, if local action of 5-HT2ARs in V1 alone is sufficient to drive perceptual changes. Furthermore, the circuit mechanisms by which these 5-HT2AR-mediated changes occur are unclear. Evidence also suggests that other members of the 5-HT2 receptor family are activated by DOI, albeit with significantly lower efficiency, and DOI is more selective for 5-HT2AR than LSD (Knight et al., 2004). Given that our dose is comparable to most studies of 5-HT2AR function (see STAR Methods for discussion), we do not expect this to be the case; however, we cannot rule out that 5-HT2B or 5-HT2C receptors contribute to our results. These issues will be important to address in future studies of psychedelic drug influence on sensory cortical processing.
5-HT2AR Activation Reduces Sensory Drive
Models of hallucination suggest that reductions in bottom-up sensory drive can lead to a misinterpretation of sensory information. We observed reduced visually evoked widefield calcium activity, a measure of bulk activity in excitatory neurons, suggesting 5-HT2AR activity reduces sensory drive in cortex. At the level of individual neurons, DOI administration bidirectionally modulated firing rates, but the overall effect was a decrease in V1 responses, which has also been observed in anesthetized primates and cats (Watakabe et al., 2009; Dray et al., 1980; Rose and Horn, 1977). Reduced sensory drive may lead to increased dependence on top-down expectations, leading to misinterpretation of sensory information, as hypothesized by current models of hallucination (Cassidy et al., 2018; Grossberg, 2000).
Previous in vivo studies have not discriminated between excitatory or inhibitory cell types or cortical layers in the context of 5-HT2AR modulation of V1 response properties. Both excitatory and inhibitory populations showed bidirectional changes after DOI administration, though inhibitory neuron changes were not significant, possibly due to small sample size. Furthermore, 5-HT2AR activation resulted in layer-specific modulation of excitatory neuron activity, decreasing evoked responses in L2/3. Given that subsets of excitatory and inhibitory neurons express 5-HT2ARs, with a majority in L5 (Weber and Andrade, 2010), it is possible that the directionality of DOI-induced change in a neuron’s visual response is determined by whether it expresses 5-HT2AR rather than its excitatory or inhibitory identity. Current evidence points toward increased excitability in 5-HT2AR-expressing neurons (Avesar and Gulledge, 2012; Stephens et al., 2014), suggesting that non-expressing neurons, the majority of V1, may be suppressed via network mechanisms.
A recent study (Seillier et al., 2017) observed changes in visual responses after local iontophoresis of 5-HT into macaque V1 that were quite similar to those seen here, including a net decrease in response gain without change in selectivity, despite the fact that 5-HT itself acts on multiple receptor subtypes in cortex. Together, our findings suggest that at least in visual cortex the effect of 5-HT is dominated by the 2A subtype; however, future studies could further examine how different serotonin receptor subtypes contribute to modulation of sensory processing. Addressing these questions will require reliable genetic access to 5-HTR-family expressing neurons, which would also permit manipulations to determine the specific circuits mediating the effects observed here (Gong et al., 2007).
5-HT2AR Activation Alters Visual Contextual Modulation in Excitatory V1 Neurons
Beyond CRF properties, contextual influences are critical components of visual processing. Lateral and top-down connections are thought to be key mediators of contextual processing, which is important for perceptual functions such as attention and figure-ground segregation. Disrupted contextual processing, including decreased visual surround suppression at the psycho-physical and physiological levels, has been reported in patients with schizophrenia (Butler et al., 2008; Tibber et al., 2013; Zenger-Landolt and Heeger, 2003). Failure to appropriately incorporate contextual information could also underlie altered visual perception observed with psychedelic drugs. We found reduced surround suppression in V1 neurons resulting from decreased strength of driving and suppressive field coefficients after 5-HT2AR activation, consistent with studies in patients with schizophrenia. This suggests these receptors may be important for adjusting the influence of context in visual cortical processing.
The magnitude of DOI-induced change in some measures was larger for trained than naive animals, including the amplitude of responses measured with widefield imaging and the suppressive field measured with two-photon imaging. Given that pupil diameter and time spent running did not consistently change after DOI administration (Figure S2), we do not anticipate changes seen here reflect solely changes in behavioral state or depth of field. Training on a visual task can result in a variety of changes in visual cortical processing, such as stimulus prediction or expectation, attention, stimulus encoding, and perceptual learning (for review, see Khan and Hofer, 2018). These learning-induced changes can be context specific and dependent on either bottom-up or top-down inputs. The various inputs to V1 that are modified by different learning paradigms could be differentially affected by neuromodulators, and untangling the logic of 5-HT2AR modulation of specific V1 inputs may lend insight into the mechanisms of learning-induced changes in V1.
5-HT2AR Activation Disrupts Temporal Dynamics of Visual Responses
DOI disrupted temporal dynamics at the population level, where we observed decreases in LFP power, and at the single-unit level, where we observed strong suppression of the transient onset response in L2/3 neurons. Previous studies suggest the transient component of visual responses are more weakly tuned than sustained responses (Ringach et al., 1997) and that transient responses may encode behaviorally relevant signals such as salience, novelty, or expectation (Homann et al., 2017; Fiser et al., 2016). Sustained responses may more accurately encode stimulus identity. We found that 5-HT2AR activation differentially affected these response components in a cell-type- and layer-specific manner. Specifically, DOI altered transient responses in excitatory, but not inhibitory, L2/3 neurons, whereas sustained responses were unaffected. Given the relatively small effects of DOI on sustained relative to transient responses, along with the maintenance of feature selectivity in V1 neurons, these data suggest 5-HT2AR activation does not disrupt stimulus encoding at the level of individual neurons but rather alters integration of top-down with bottom-up sensory information.
We also observed changes in temporal dynamics at the population level as a dramatic 5-HT2AR-mediated decrease in visually evoked LFP power across V1 layers. Patients with schizophrenia (Uhlhaas and Singer, 2010; Moran and Hong, 2011) and subjects administered psychedelic drugs (Liechti, 2017) display reduced oscillatory power specifically in the gamma frequency band, which is associated with neuronal responses to visual stimuli (Liechti 2017; Sedley and Cunningham, 2013) and communication across neural populations through co-ordinated activity (Jia et al., 2013). Additionally, animal models of hallucination show reduced oscillatory synchronization across various brain areas (prefrontal cortex [PFC]: Wood et al., 2012; Celada et al., 2008; nucleus accumbens: Goda et al., 2013; hippocampus, striatum, and reticular formation: Dimpfel et al., 1989).
Implications for Models of Hallucination and Sensory Processing
Despite these DOI-mediated changes in V1 sensory drive and temporal dynamics, CRF tuning properties and stimulus encoding remained unchanged. This suggests that altered visual perception related to 5-HT2AR function results not from changes in V1 stimulus encoding but from impaired downstream integration due to changes in gain and temporal dynamics. Consistent with these findings, many perceptual deficits in patients with schizophrenia are attributed to reduced gain of sensory responses (Butler et al., 2008; Phillips and Silverstein, 2013).
Understanding the action of 5-HT2ARs may provide insight into the general principles of cortical sensory processing, particularly given the potent impact of hallucinogenic 5-HT2AR agonists on perception and cognition. There is increased urgency for under-standing the neurophysiological effects of 5-HT2AR modulation given the recent resurgence in use of psychedelic drugs in the treatment of mental health disorders (Johnson and Griffiths, 2017; Carhart-Harris and Goodwin, 2017). Our results provide a basis for investigating circuit-specific actions of these drugs in cortical function.
STAR★METHODS
CONTACT FOR REAGENT AND RESOURCE SHARING
Further information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the Lead Contact, Cristopher Niell (cniell@uoregon.edu).
EXPERIMENTAL MODEL AND SUBJECT DETAILS
Animals:
All procedures were conducted in accordance with the guidelines of the National Institutes of Health and were approved by the University of Oregon Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Two-to eight-month old adult mice [C57BL/6J for electro-physiology, CaMKII-tTA:tetO-GCaMP6s (Jackson Laboratories stock numbers 007004 and 024742) for imaging (Mayford et al., 1996; Wekselblatt et al., 2016)] were initially implanted with a steel headplate over primary visual cortex to allow for head-fixation during electrophysiology (Niell and Stryker, 2008) or imaging (Wekselblatt et al., 2016) experiments. In total, 26 male and 39 female mice were used for this study. Animals were handled by the experimenter for several days before surgical procedures, and subsequently habituated to the spherical treadmill for several days before experiments. Some mice in imaging experiments were previously trained on a two-alternative forced choice task, where they were water restricted and given water rewards based on leftward or right-ward movements of the spherical treadmill during luminance discrimination and orientation/spatial discrimination of a grating patch (for details, see Wekselblatt et al., 2016). The grating patches presented during passive viewing in this study were similar in quality (45 deg, 0.16 cycles/degree for behavior, see below for passive parameters) but presented in a different location in visual space compared to the previous behavioral training. These mice were not water restricted during the current experiments, and imaging experiments performed under identical conditions as naive groups.
METHOD DETAILS
Surgical procedures
Animals were anesthetized with isoflurane (3% induction, 1.5%–2% maintenance, in O2) and body temperature was maintained at 37.5°C using a feedback-controlled heating pad. Fascia was cleared from the surface of the skull following scalp incision and a custom steel headplate containing a circular well was attached to the skull using Vetbond (3M) and dental acrylic. The headplate well was centered over V1 (2.5–3 mm lateral of the midline and 1 mm anterior of Lambda). Carprofen (10 mg/kg) and lactated Ringer’s solution were administered subcutaneously, and animals were monitored for three days following surgery.
For widefield imaging, a protective layer of cyanoacrylate adhesive (Loctite) was applied to the skull within the headplate well (10 mm diameter) during headplate attachment. For two-photon experiments, a second surgery was performed at least 3 days after headplate attachment, whereby a section of skull ~5 mm in diameter was removed via dental drill, artificial dura (Dow-Corning 3–4680 Silicone Gel) was applied in the craniotomy, and a 5 mm glass coverslip was glued into place over the craniotomy. Antibiotics (cefazolin, 10 mg/kg) were administered in the week surrounding the surgery, and an anti-inflammatory (dexamethasone, 10 mg/kg) was administered 18h and 2h prior to surgery to prevent brain swelling.
For electrophysiology experiments, at least two days following headplate attachment a craniotomy (1 mm diameter) was made the night before or several hours prior to the recording session. The cortical surface was covered with a layer of 1.5% agarose in 0.9% saline and a layer of Kwik-Sil (WPI) to prevent drying and provide structural support.
Experiments
Mice were head-fixed above a spherical treadmill and locomotion was measured via an optical mouse placed on the side of the spherical treadmill using a custom MATLAB script. Visual stimuli were generated in MATLAB using the Psychophysics toolbox extensions (Brainard 1997; Pelli 1997), and presented on gamma-corrected LED monitors oriented tangentially 20–25 cm from the contralateral eye (plus ipsilateral eye for widefield retinotopic mapping). Saline (0.9% NaCl) or DOI (Sigma, 10 mg/kg in saline) was then administered subcutaneously, and visual responses to the same stimulus set (presented in reverse order) were recorded again after a waiting period of 15–20 min. Mice were monitored for front paw stereotypy, which DOI reliably induced within 5–7 min following injection.
This dose of 10 mg/kg was chosen based on standards in the literature, which range from 1–10 mg/kg intraperitoneal (Freo et al., 1992; Aulakh et al., 1992; Garcia et al., 2007; González-Maeso et al., 2007). Subcutaneous injection was used rather than intraperitoneal to prevent having to remove the animal from the head-fixed setup between pre and post stimulus presentations. We estimate that our effective dose is approximately equivalent to 2.5 mg/kg intraperitoneal based on previous comparisons of the two injection methods, where serum levels tend to rise more slowly and peak at significantly lower concentrations after subcutaneous injection (Porter et al., 1985; Turner et al., 2011; Turner et al., 2011; Hirota and Shimizu, 2012). Previous work revealed LSD elicited head bob behavior in rabbits occurs independent of the route of administration (Schindler et al., 2012). However, to confirm that we were not using an excessively high dose, we tested a lower dose in a subset of widefield experiments (2 mg/kg subcutaneous) and saw no significant change in response amplitude relative to baseline (see Figure S1).
Widefield Imaging
A widefield microscope (Scimedia, Inc.) equipped with a sCMOS camera (PCO, 10 Hz acquisition) was used to measure GCaMP6s signal though the skull during blue LED excitation (Luxeon Rebel 470 nm, 0.1 mW/mm2 at the sample). In a subset of experiments, a green LED (Luxeon Rebel 530 nm, 0.1 mW/mm2 at the sample) was used for excitation every four frames to measure hemodynamic signals, which were subtracted from the blue frames (Wekselblatt et al., 2016). The change in fluorescence relative to baseline (ΔF/F) was calculated for each pixel individually using its mean value as F. Visual areas were first mapped using a topographic stimulus consisting of a bar of 1/f noise sweeping in either azimuth or elevation, and the amplitude and phase of the Fourier component of the ΔF/F signal were calculated at the stimulus frequency (0.1 Hz), which were later used to align sessions across animals (Kalatsky and Stryker, 2003). Vertical and horizontal stationary grating patches (0.16 cpd, 30 deg) were presented to the right eye with 1 s duration and 1 s inter-stimulus interval. For each animal, a central point in V1 corresponding to the approximate response peak was selected, and the pixels around this point in a 5 × 5 region were averaged to create ΔF/F traces. To analyze spatial spread of responses, an elliptical meshgrid was generated around this central point, with a 2:1 ratio of the major:minor axes aligned in the anteroposterior:mediolateral dimensions to account for cortical magnification factor, and the points along this meshgrid radiating out from the center were averaged along these concentric ellipses to create an average ΔF/F for a given distance from the center point along the minor axis of the ellipse. Normalized ΔF/F change was calculated as (post-pre)/mean(post,pre).
Two-photon imaging
A two-photon microscope (Neurolabware, 16X Nikon CFI75 LWD objective) was used to measure GCaMP6s signal through the cranial window at 920 nm laser excitation (Mai-Tai, Spectra-Physics). ~800 μm by 800 μm frames were acquired at 10 Hz using Scanbox software. Visual areas were first mapped using widefield imaging (described above), then V1 was targeted and the stimulus screen and field of view were adjusted to center the visual response. A mapping stimulus (see widefield imaging methods) was first used to measure spatial receptive fields, followed by a period of darkness (5 min) to measure spontaneous activity. Then a ~22 min stimulus was shown to measure surround suppression, which consisted of binarized grating patches at various sizes (5, 10, 20, 30, 40, and 50 deg of visual field), spatial frequencies (0.04, 0.16 cycles/deg), and orientations (0, 90°) at a 2 Hz temporal frequency and full contrast, with 0.5 s duration and 0.5 s inter-stimulus interval.
Cell footprints were extracted using constrained nonnegative matrix factorization, with a spatially homogeneous neuropil response factored out (Pnevmatikakis et al., 2016). ΔF/F was calculated for all pixels using the 10th percentile as F, and then traces for all cells were deconvolved using constrained foopsi. Data for each specific stimulus were then analyzed using custom MATLAB scripts. For surround suppression data, ΔF/F within each inter-trial-interval was averaged and subtracted from the ensuing trial, and the ΔF/F traces during blank stimuli (mean luminance gray identical to inter-trial-interval) were averaged across the experiment and subtracted from all trials (separately for stationary and running trials). Only neurons whose somata were within the region of neuropil activated by the 10 deg stimulus were included in the analysis, constrained within an elliptical region with a 2:1 major:minor axis ratio along the anteroposterior:mediolateral dimensions to account for cortical magnification factor, with a manual rotational offset and overall size chosen to closely match each individual animal’s response pattern. Within this region, only neurons with responses to any one of the stimulus types (combination of size, spatial frequency, orientation) greater than 10% ΔF/F for both pre and post drug injection were included in analyses. For all two-photon group analyses, averages were taken across cells (within animal) and these values were then used to calculate group mean/standard error. For surround suppression experiments, a divisive normalization model (Ayaz et al., 2013) was used to fit the data for pre and post injection periods separately, then all variables except RD and RS were constrained as the average of pre/post values, and the fits were run again. The equation fit to each animal’s size tuning curve was:
where RD and RS are the strengths of the driving and suppressive fields, σD and σS are the extents of the driving and suppressive fields, m is an exponent, d is the diameter of the stimulus, and erf is the error function. The coefficient of determination for each group was: saline naive r2 = 0.939, saline trained r2 = 0.928, DOI naive r2 = 0.867, DOI trained r2 = 0.941, and there was no significant difference between pre and post fit r2 for any group (paired t test). Suppression index was calculated as (RMAX - R50d)/(RMAX + R50d) where RMAX is the largest response across all sizes, and R50d is the response to the largest stimulus (50 deg). Normalized change for RD, RS, and suppression index was calculated as (post-pre)/mean(post,pre).
Extracellular Multichannel Electrophysiology
Multisite silicon probes (NeuroNexus, A2×32–5mm25–200-177) coated with a small amount of the lipophilic dye DiO (Invitrogen) were inserted through the overlaying agarose and into monocular V1 using a microdrive (Siskiyou Designs). Electrode penetrations were done over the course of 30 min – 1 h and the probe was allowed to settle in its final position for at least 30 min before data collection began. Hand-mapped receptive fields were used to approximately center the screen position on receptive field centers. Contrast-modulated noise movies (Gaussian 1/f) were presented and spike-triggered averaging (STA) was utilized to estimate spatial receptive fields as in Niell and Stryker (2008). Full-field drifting sinusoidal gratings were presented at twelve evenly spaced directions of motion, six spatial frequencies (0.01, 0.02, 0.04, 0.08, 0.16, and 0.32 cpd), and full-field flicker (0 cpd) with temporal frequency of 2 Hz. Stimulus presentations were randomly interleaved for 1.5 s duration, with 1 s inter-stimulus interval. To estimate spontaneous firing rate, a gray blank condition (mean luminance) was also presented. For darkness recordings, the computer monitor was turned off and other sources of light in the room were covered.
At the end of the experiment, animals were euthanized by deep anesthesia and cervical dislocation. Following removal, brains were immersed in 4% PFA (Electron Microscopy Sciences) in PBS at 4°C. 100 μm coronal sections were cut with a vibratome and mounted using Vectashield with DAPI (Vector Laboratories) then imaged on a Zeiss Axio Imager 2 to determine the depth of electrode penetrations. Each site along the electrode was given a layer assignment based on its position on the probe relative to the depth of the probe tip and geometry of the penetration angle. In addition to histology, current source density was also used to identify cortical layers in neural recordings (Hoy and Niell, 2015).
Data acquisition was performed as described by Niell and Stryker (2008). Signals were acquired using a System 3 workstation (Tucker-Davis Technologies) and analyzed with custom software in MATLAB (MathWorks). Extracellular signals were filtered from 0.7 to 7 kHz and sampled at 25 kHz. We detected spiking events on-line by voltage threshold crossing, and a 1 ms waveform sample on four adjacent recording sites was acquired, creating a virtual tetrode. Single-unit clustering and spike waveform analyses were performed using a combination of custom software in MATLAB and Klusta-Kwik (Harris et al., 2000), as described previously (Niell and Stryker, 2008). Quality of unit separation was based on a clear refractory period of less than 0.01% of spikes within a 1 ms interspike interval and by the computed L ratio (Schmitzer-Torbert et al., 2005). Units were also checked for stability by confirming that their peak amplitude remained consistent over the course of the recording session. Units that were found by histology to be outside of V1 were excluded from subsequent analysis.
Movement signals from the optical mouse were acquired at up to 300 Hz and integrated at 100 ms intervals (Mx310; Logitech), as originally described by Niell and Stryker (2010). By using these measurements, we calculated animals’ mean speed for every stimulus presentation. Trials with mean speed above 0.5 cm/s were considered movement trials.
For LFP analysis, the extracellular signal was filtered from 1 to 300 Hz and sampled at 1.5 kHz. The power spectrum was computed using multi-taper estimation in MATLAB with the Chronux package (Mitra and Pesaran, 1999; Mitra and Bokil, 2007), with a sliding window and three to five tapers. Spectra were normalized for presentation by applying a 1/f correction (Sirota et al., 2008). Traces of individual experiments were normalized to the range of either the pre or post recording block across all experiments before averaging.
Units were classified as narrow or broad spiking based on properties of their average waveforms at the electrode site with largest amplitude. As detailed in Niell and Stryker (2008), two parameters—(1) height of the positive peak relative to the initial negative trough and (2) time from the minimum of the initial trough to maximum of the following peak—were sufficient to generate two linearly separable clusters corresponding to narrow spiking (putative inhibitory) and broad-spiking (putative excitatory) neurons. These clusters were separated using K-means.
Average evoked firing rate was calculated following a baseline subtraction of the spontaneous rate during 1 s inter-stimulus intervals. Modulation indices were calculated for evoked (1 Hz threshold) and spontaneous (0.5 Hz threshold) rates where MI = (Rpost-Rpre)/(Rpost+Rpre). Peri-stimulus time histograms were calculated using 100 ms time bins over the 1.5 s duration of each stimulus presentation and 1 s ISI. Visually responsive units included in the analysis were defined as units with an average firing rate above 2 spikes/s after baseline subtraction for either pre or post recording blocks.
We calculated preferred angle of orientation by finding the stimulus orientation that elicited the peak response for each cell on average, regardless of spatial frequency. The OSI was calculated as the depth of modulation from the preferred orientation to its orthogonal orientation θortho = θpref + π/2, as (Rpref - Rortho)/(Rpref + Rortho). Preferred spatial frequency was determined by finding the spatial frequency that elicited the largest response, on average. We used STAs of individual units recorded before and after drug administration to calculate 2-D correlation coefficients as a measure of similarity of receptive field structure.
QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Two-tailed paired t tests or Wilcoxon Rank sum tests were used to compare data before versus after drug administration. For comparisons between saline and DOI, two-sample tests such as Kolmogorov-Smirnov or two-sample two-tailed t tests were used. For comparison of trained and naive saline and DOI groups, Kruskal-Wallis and post hoc Tukey-Kramer tests were used. Significance was defined as p < 0.05, and in the case of multiple comparisons a Bonferroni correction was implemented.
DATA AND SOFTWARE AVAILABILITY
Datasets and custom MATLAB codes used for analysis are available upon request to Lead Contact, Cristopher Niell (cniell@uoregon.edu).
Supplementary Material
REAGENT or RESOURCE | SOURCE | IDENTIFIER |
---|---|---|
Experimental Models: Organisms/Strains | ||
Mouse: C57 BI/6J | JAX | JAX: 000664 |
Mouse: CaMKII-tTA:tetO-GCaMP6s | Mayford et al., 1996; Wekselblatt et al., 2016 | JAX: 007004 and 024742 |
Chemicals, Peptides, and Recombinant Proteins | ||
(±)-DOI hydrochloride | Sigma-Aldrich | Cat#: D101–10MG |
Software and Algorithms | ||
2-p Cell Extraction | Pnevmatikakis et al., 2016 | N/A |
Highlights.
The 5-HT2A R agonist DOI reduces sensory drive in mouse V1
DOI exerts layer and cell-type specific effects on temporal processing
DOI alters contextual modulation by reducing surround suppression
The results support models of hallucination involving reduced bottom-up sensory drive
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank Dr. David Fitzpatrick and Dr. Matt Smear for comments on the manuscript and members of the Niell lab for helpful discussions. This work was supported by NIH grants R01 EY023337 (C.M.N.), 2T32HD007348–26 (A.M.M.), and F32 EY027696–02 (P.R.L.P.)
Footnotes
DECLARATION OF INTERESTS
The authors declare no competing interests.
SUPPLEMENTAL INFORMATION
Supplemental Information can be found with this article online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2019.02.104.
REFERENCES
- Aulakh CS, Hill JL, Yoney HT, and Murphy DL (1992). Evidence for involvement of 5-HT1C and 5-HT2 receptors in the food intake suppressant effects of 1-(2,5-dimethyoxy-4-iodophenyl)-2-aminopropane (DOI). Psycho-pharmacology 109, 444–448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Avesar D, and Gulledge AT (2012). Selective serotonergic excitation of callosal projection neurons. Front. Neural Circuits 6, 12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ayaz A, Saleem AB, Schölvinck ML, and Carandini M (2013). Locomotion controls spatial integration in mouse visual cortex. Curr. Biol 23, 890–894. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brainard DH (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spat. Vis 10, 433–436. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butler PD, Silverstein SM, and Dakin SC (2008). Visual perception and its impairment in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 64, 40–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carhart-Harris RL, and Goodwin GM (2017). The therapeutic potential of psychedelic drugs: past, present, and future. Neuropsychopharmacology 42, 2105–2113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carhart-Harris RL, Muthukumaraswamy S, Roseman L, Kaelen M, Droog W, Murphy K, Tagliazucchi E, Schenberg EE, Nest T, Orban C, et al. (2016). Neural correlates of the LSD experience revealed by multi-modal neuroimaging. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 113, 4853–4858. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cassidy CM, Balsam PD, Weinstein JJ, Rosengard RJ, Slifstein M, Daw ND, Abi-Dargham A, and Horga G (2018). A perceptual inference mechanism for hallucinations linked to striatal dopamine. Curr. Biol 28, 503–514.e4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Celada P, Puig MV, Díaz-Mataix L, and Artigas F (2008). The hallucinogen DOI reduces low-frequency oscillations in rat prefrontal cortex: reversal by antipsychotic drugs. Biol. Psychiatry 64, 392–400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dimpfel W, Spüler M, and Nichols DE (1989). Hallucinogenic and stimulatory amphetamine derivatives: fingerprinting DOM, DOI, DOB, MDMA, and MBDB by spectral analysis of brain field potentials in the freely moving rat (Tele-Stereo-EEG). Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 98, 297–303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dombeck DA, Khabbaz AN, Collman F, Adelman TL, and Tank DW (2007). Imaging large-scale neural activity with cellular resolution in awake, mobile mice. Neuron 56, 43–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dray A, Fox PC, Hilmy M, and Somjen GG (1980). The effects of LSD and some analogues on the responses of single cortical neurons of the cat to optical stimulation. Brain Res 200, 105–121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fiser A, Mahringer D, Oyibo HK, Petersen AV, Leinweber M, and Keller GB (2016). Experience-dependent spatial expectations in mouse visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci 19, 1658–1664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox PC, and Dray A (1979). Iontophoresis of LSD: Effects on responses of single cortical neurons to visual stimulation. Brain Res 161, 167–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freo U, Holloway HW, Kalogeras K, Rapoport SI, and Soncrant TT (1992). Adrenalectomy or metyrapone-pretreatment abolishes cerebral metabolic responses to the serotonin against 1-(2,5-dimethoxy-4-iodophenyl)-2-aminopropoane (DOI) in the hippocampus. Brain Res 586, 256–264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garcia EE, Smith RL, and Sanders-Bush E (2007). Role of G(q)protein in behavioral effects of the hallucinogenic drug 1-(2,5-dimethoxy-4-iodo-phenyl)-2-aminopropane. Neuropharmacology 52, 1671–1677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goda SA, Piasecka J, Olszewski M, Kasicki S, and Hunt MJ (2013). Serotonergic hallucinogens differentially modify gamma and high frequency oscillations in the rat nucleus accumbens. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 228, 271–282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gong S, Doughty M, Harbaugh CR, Cummins A, Hatten ME, Heintz N, and Gerfen CR (2007). Targeting Cre recombinase to specific neuron populations with bacterial artificial chromosome constructs. J. Neurosci 27, 9817–9823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- González-Maeso J, Weisstaub NV, Zhou M, Chan P, Ivic L, Ang R, Lira A, Bradley-Moore M, Ge Y, Zhou Q, et al. (2007). Hallucinogens recruit specific cortical 5-HT(2A) receptor-mediated signaling pathways to affect behavior. Neuron 53, 439–452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grossberg S (2000). How hallucinations may arise from brain mechanisms of learning, attention, and volition. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc 6, 583–592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanks JB, and González-Maeso J (2013). Animal models of serotonergic psychedelics. ACS Chem. Neurosci 4, 33–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris KD, Henze DA, Csicsvari J, Hirase H, and Buzsáki G (2000). Accuracy of tetrode spike separation as determined by simultaneous intracellular and extracellular measurements. J. Neurophysiol 84, 401–414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirota J, and Shimizu S (2012). Routes of administration. In The Laboratory Mouse, Second Edition, Hedrick HJ, ed. (Elsevier), pp. 709–725. [Google Scholar]
- Homann J, Koay SA, Glidden AM, Tank DW, and Berry MJ II. (2017). Predictive coding of novel versus familiar stimuli in the primary visual cortex. bioRxiv 10.1101/197608. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hoy JL, and Niell CM (2015). Layer-specific refinement of visual cortex function after eye opening in the awake mouse. J. Neurosci 35, 3370–3383. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jia X, Tanabe S, and Kohn A (2013). g and the coordination of spiking activity in early visual cortex. Neuron 77, 762–774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson MW, and Griffiths RR (2017). Potential therapeutic effects of psilocybin. Neurotherapeutics 14, 734–740. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalatsky VA, and Stryker MP (2003). New paradigm for optical imaging: temporally encoded maps of intrinsic signal. Neuron 38, 529–545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khan AG, and Hofer SB (2018). Contextual signals in visual cortex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol 52, 131–138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knight AR, Misra A, Quirk K, Benwell K, Revell D, Kennett G, and Bickerdike M (2004). Pharmacological characterisation of the agonist radioligand binding site of 5-HT(2A), 5-HT(2B) and 5-HT(2C) receptors. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch. Pharmacol 370, 114–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kometer M, Schmidt A, Jäncke L, and Vollenweider FX (2013). Activation of serotonin 2A receptors underlies the psilocybin-induced effects on a oscillations, N170 visual-evoked potentials, and visual hallucinations. J. Neurosci 33, 10544–10551. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liechti ME (2017). Modern Clinical Research on LSD. Neuropsychopharmacology 42, 2114–2127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayford M, Bach ME, Huang YY, Wang L, Hawkins RD, and Kandel ER (1996). Control of memory formation through regulated expression of a CaMKII transgene. Science 274, 1678–1683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitra P, and Bokil H (2007). Observed Brain Dynamics (Oxford University Press; ). [Google Scholar]
- Mitra PP, and Pesaran B (1999). Analysis of dynamic brain imaging data. Biophys. J 76, 691–708. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moran LV, and Hong LE (2011). High vs low frequency neural oscillations in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull 37, 659–663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nichols DE (2016). Psychedelics. Pharmacol. Rev 68, 264–355. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niell CM, and Stryker MP (2008). Highly selective receptive fields in mouse visual cortex. J. Neurosci 28, 7520–7536. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niell CM, and Stryker MP (2010). Modulation of visual responses by behavioral state in mouse visual cortex. Neuron 65, 472–479. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelli DG (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat. Vis 10, 437–442. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips WA, and Silverstein SM (2013). The coherent organization of mental life depends on mechanisms for context-sensitive gain-control that are impaired in schizophrenia. Front. Psychol 4, 307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pnevmatikakis EA, Soudry D, Gao Y, Machado TA, Merel J, Pfau D, Reardon T, Mu Y, Lacefield C, Yang W, et al. (2016). Simultaneous denoising, deconvolution, and demixing of calcium imaging data. Neuron 89, 285–299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Porter WP, Bitar YM, Strandberg JD, and Charache PC (1985). A comparison of subcutaneous and intraperitoneal oxytetracycline injection methods for control of infectious disease in the rat. Lab. Anim 19, 3–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preller KH, Burt JB, Ji JL, Schleifer CH, Adkinson BD, Stämpfli P, Seifritz E, Repovs G, Krystal JH, Murray JD, et al. (2018). Changes in global and thalamic brain connectivity in LSD-induced altered states of consciousness are attributable to the 5-HT2A receptor. eLife 7, e35082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ringach DL, Hawken MJ, and Shapley R (1997). Dynamics of orientation tuning in macaque primary visual cortex. Nature 387, 281–284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rose D, and Horn G (1977). Effects of LSD on the response of single units in cat visual cortex. Exp. Brain Res 27, 71–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schindler EAD, Dave KD, Smolock EM, Aloyo VJ, and Harvey JA (2012). Serotonergic and dopaminergic distinctions in the behavioral pharmacology of (±)-1-(2,5-dimethoxy-4-iodophenyl)-2-aminopropane (DOI) and lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD). Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav 101, 69–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt CJ, Sorensen SM, Kehne JH, Carr AA, and Palfreyman MG (1995). The role of 5-HT2A receptors in antipsychotic activity. Life Sci 56, 2209–2222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmitzer-Torbert N, Jackson J, Henze D, Harris K, and Redish AD (2005). Quantitative measures of cluster quality for use in extracellular recordings. Neuroscience 131, 1–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sedley W, and Cunningham MO (2013). Do cortical gamma oscillations promote or suppress perception? An under-asked question with an over-assumed answer. Front. Hum. Neurosci 7, 595. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seillier L, Lorenz C, Kawaguchi K, Ott T, Nieder A, Pourriahi P, and Nienborg H (2017). Serotonin decreases the gain of visual responses in awake macaque V1. J. Neurosci 37, 11390–11405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shulgin A, and Shulgin A (1991). Pihkal: A Chemical Love Story (Transform Press) [Google Scholar]
- Sirota A, Montgomery S, Fujisawa S, Isomura Y, Zugaro M, and Buzsáki G (2008). Entrainment of neocortical neurons and gamma oscillations by the hippocampal theta rhythm. Neuron 60, 683–697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stephens EK, Avesar D, and Gulledge AT (2014). Activity-dependent serotonergic excitation of callosal projection neurons in the mouse prefrontal cortex. Front. Neural Circuits 8, 97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tibber MS, Anderson EJ, Bobin T, Antonova E, Seabright A, Wright B, Carlin P, Shergill SS, and Dakin SC (2013). Visual surround suppression in schizophrenia. Front. Psychol 4, 88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner PV, Pekow C, Vasbinder MA, and Brabb T (2011). Administration of substances to laboratory animals: equipment considerations, vehicle selection, and solute preparation. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci 50, 614–627. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uhlhaas PJ, and Singer W (2010). Abnormal neural oscillations and synchrony in schizophrenia. Nat. Rev. Neurosci 11, 100–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vollenweider FX, Vollenweider-Scherpenhuyzen MF, Bäbler A, Vogel H, and Hell D (1998). Psilocybin induces schizophrenia-like psychosis in humans via a serotonin-2 agonist action. Neuroreport 9, 3897–3902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watakabe A, Komatsu Y, Sadakane O, Shimegi S, Takahata T, Higo N, Tochitani S, Hashikawa T, Naito T, Osaki H, et al. (2009). Enriched expression of serotonin 1B and 2A receptor genes in macaque visual cortex and their bidirectional modulatory effects on neuronal responses. Cereb. Cortex 19, 1915–1928. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weber ET, and Andrade R (2010). Htr2a gene and 5-HT(2A) receptor expression in the cerebral cortex studied using genetically modified mice. Front. Neurosci 4, 36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wekselblatt JB, Flister ED, Piscopo DM, and Niell CM (2016). Largescale imaging of cortical dynamics during sensory perception and behavior. J. Neurophysiol 115, 2852–2866. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood J, Kim Y, and Moghaddam B (2012). Disruption of prefrontal cortex large scale neuronal activity by different classes of psychotomimetic drugs. J. Neurosci 32, 3022–3031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zenger-Landolt B, and Heeger DJ (2003). Response suppression in v1 agrees with psychophysics of surround masking. J. Neurosci 23, 6884–6893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Datasets and custom MATLAB codes used for analysis are available upon request to Lead Contact, Cristopher Niell (cniell@uoregon.edu).