Abstract
Objective:
Sleep is unequally distributed in the US population. People with low socioeconomic status (SES) report worse quality and shorter sleep than people with high SES. Past research hypothesized that a potential reason for this link could be exposure to material hardship. This study examines the associations between several material hardships and sleep outcomes.
Methods:
We use population-representative cross-sectional data (n = 730) from the Michigan Recession and Recovery Study (MRRS) collected in 2013 and examine the associations between six indicators of material hardship (employment instability, financial problems, housing instability, food insecurity, forgone medical care, and the total number of material hardships reported) and three sleep outcomes (short sleep, sleep problems, and nonrestorative sleep). We build multivariable logistic regression models controlling for respondents’ characteristics and light pollution near their residence.
Results:
In unadjusted models, all material hardships were associated with negative sleep outcomes. In adjusted models, forgone medical care was a statistically significant predictor of nonrestorative sleep (average marginal effect 0.16) as was employment instability (average marginal effect 0.12). The probability of sleep problems and nonrestorative sleep increased with a greater number of hardships overall (average marginal effects of 0.02 and 0.05 respectively). We found marginally statistically significant positive associations between food insecurity and short sleep and sleep problems.
Conclusions:
This study finds that, except when considering foregone medical care, employment instability, and total count of material hardships, associations between material hardship and negative sleep outcomes are not statistically significant after adjusting for a robust set of sociodemographic and health characteristics.
Keywords: Material hardship, SES, disparities, population survey
INTRODUCTION
Inadequate sleep has been linked to multiple negative health outcomes, including an increased risk of diabetes, cardiovascular disease, mental health disorders, and mortality (Grandner, 2017). Poor sleep is not equally distributed in the US population; those of lower socioeconomic status (SES) are more likely to report suboptimal sleep duration and worse sleep quality (Grandner et al., 2010; Krueger & Friedman, 2009), and are therefore more likely to suffer their negative health consequences. However, relatively little is known about the causes of the observed SES disparity in sleep (Lallukka, Arber, Rahkonen, & Lahelma, 2010). This study builds on the neomaterial theoretical tradition in social epidemiology, focusing on the pathways between SES and adverse health outcomes (Lynch et al., 2004; Lynch, Smith, Kaplan, & House, 2000). We hypothesize that low SES is linked to poor sleep outcomes through material hardships, such as hunger, housing insecurity, and lack of needed medical care, which translates to physical and psychological barriers to sleep.
Material hardship may erect physical as well as physiological barriers to optimal sleep duration and quality. The preceding literature has addressed two specific material hardships and their relationship to sleep: food insecurity and housing instability. Food insecurity, the better documented of the two, has been linked to poor sleep outcomes in several studies. Research based on nationally representative population surveys in the United States and Mexico has found that the food insecure were more likely to report insufficient or short sleep, and that this association was not explained by their sociodemographic characteristics (Ding, Keiley, Garza, Duffy, & Zizza, 2015; Jordan, Perez-Escamilla, Desai, & Shamah-Levy, 2016; Liu, Njai, Greenlund, Chapman, & Croft, 2014). Researchers have speculated that this association could be due to the negative effect of food insecurity on mental health or by a lack of nutrients such as vitamin B-12 or folic acid, which can alter mood and impair immunity, though these explanatory pathways have not been tested directly (Ding et al., 2015).
Unstable or inadequate housing could similarly jeopardize sleep quantity and duration by preventing people from finding sleeping quarters shielded from excessive heat or cold, light, noise, and danger. In a study of a population sample of US adults, Liu and colleagues (2014) found housing insecurity, defined as worry about affording rent or mortgage, to be associated with frequent insufficient sleep. Homelessness, the most extreme form of housing instability, typically means a frequent change in sleeping arrangements and the absence of a private and secure sleeping area. A recent study of homeless US adults found that during the previous month they experienced inadequate sleep on 13 nights and unintentionally falling asleep on five days, on average (Reitzel et al., 2017).
Most past studies have not been able to differentiate between different types of material hardships, and thus could not speak to the specific pathways through which low economic resources could potentially translate to negative sleep outcomes. Often, they have relied on general combined measures of financial and material difficulties, which combine multiple heterogeneous indicators. For example, Perales and Plage (2017) found an association between material hardship and shorter sleep in a population sample of Australian adults. In another study using a population sample of US midlife women, difficulty “making ends meet” was linked to restless sleep, but not sleep difficulties (Hall, Bromberger, & Matthews, 1999).
In addition to creating physical obstacles to sleep, material hardship could stimulate psychological distress, which may also contribute to poor sleep outcomes. Material hardship has been shown to be associated with depression (Heflin & Iceland, 2009; Manuel, Martinson, Bledsoe-Mansori, & Bellamy, 2012) and other mood and anxiety disorders, independently of SES (Kiely, Leach, Olesen, & Butterworth, 2015). Depression, worry, and rumination have all been linked to bedtime agitation, insomnia, or delayed sleep onset (Baglioni, Spiegelhalder, Lombardo, & Riemann, 2010; Watts, Coyle, & East, 1994; Zoccola, Dickerson, & Lam, 2009). However, researchers who examined the material hardship-psychological distress pathway directly have found that it mediates, but does not fully explain the association between material hardship and poor sleep outcomes (Liu et al., 2014). Taken together, past evidence suggests a complex combination of physiological and psychological elements underlying the relationship between material hardship and sleep outcomes.
Our study builds on the emerging material hardship and sleep literature. We argue that a more complete understanding of their connection requires a deeper examination of not only the outcome, sleep, but also of material hardship: its various types, and how their differing natures may relate to sleep outcomes. Such a deeper examination will advance our theoretical grasp of the relationship, as well as inform interventions to improve the sleep outcomes of low SES populations. We supply some of the missing pieces of the link between sleep and material hardship by analyzing the relationship between sleep and six separate indicators of material hardship: employment instability, financial problems, housing instability, food insecurity, forgone medical care, and total number of material hardships in a population-representative sample of working-age adults living in Southeast Michigan. We ask two research questions: First, are material hardships associated with short sleep, sleep problems, and nonrestorative sleep? Second, do the associations between poor sleep outcomes and material hardships persist after accounting for a robust set of sociodemographic, health, and environmental factors? Our results complicate the conclusions drawn from prior research and show that many associations between material hardship and poor sleep outcomes are not statistically significant after adjusting for a robust set of sociodemographic and health characteristics.
DATA AND METHODS
Data
The Michigan Recession and Recovery Study (MRRS) is a population survey of a stratified random sample of adults aged 19–64 living in the Detroit metropolitan area (Macomb, Oakland, and Wayne counties). The first wave of interviews was conducted from October 2009 to April 2010 (n = 914), the second from April to August of 2011 (n = 847), and the third and final wave was conducted from June to October of 2013 (n = 751). We relied on data collected in the third wave of MRRS. Although Wave 3 has fewer observations than the preceding waves, respondents in Wave 3 were asked a more extensive set of questions about their sleep quantity and quality. Wave 3 also has the most accurate measurement of light pollution because nighttime light conditions in this wave are approximated by the earliest suitable data from the Visible Infrared Imaging Radiometer Suite (VIIRS). The VIIRS instrument records nighttime satellite imagery of greater resolution and dynamic range than other sources. It is able to capture variation in the nighttime light conditions of densely-populated urban areas, which was not previously possible (Miller et al., 2013).
Approximately 15% respondents did not respond to one or more questions required to construct our analytic variables. We performed multiple imputation using Stata -ice- imputation routine (Royston, 2006) and created twenty additional datasets with imputed responses for all respondents. After imputation, we constructed an analytic sample that excluded 18 respondents who moved out of the original sampling area and therefore did not have valid local light pollution measures. We additionally excluded from our final analytic sample three respondents who did not respond to questions about their sleep duration or quality. Our total analytic sample was therefore comprised of 730 respondents. All measures were constructed based on the Wave 3 data collection except for gender and race, which were collected at baseline.
Measures
Sleep Outcomes
We constructed three sleep measures: short sleep, sleep problems, and nonrestorative sleep. To measure sleep duration, participants were asked, “How much sleep do you get in a 24 hour period?” Those who reported less than six hours were classified as having short sleep (14%). Sleep problems were measured by the question “Over the last two weeks, how often have you been bothered by trouble falling asleep or sleeping too much?” Respondents could select from “None at all,” “Several days,” “More than half the days,” or “Nearly every day.” Those who responded “More than half the days” or “Nearly every day” were categorized as having sleep problems (17%). Nonrestorative sleep was measured by an item “How often do you feel unrested during the day, no matter how many hours of sleep you had?” Respondents could choose from “Never,” “Rarely (once a month),” “Sometimes (2–4 times per month),” “Often (2–3 times per week),” or “Almost always (4 or more times per week). Those who responded “Often (2–3 times per week)” or “Almost always (4 or more times per week)” were classified as having nonrestorative sleep (38%).
Material Hardship
We constructed six indicators of material hardship: employment instability, financial problems, housing instability, food insecurity, forgone medical care, and a combined measure which recorded the number of material hardships respondents experienced in total out of these five. We classified respondents as having experienced employment instability if they were unemployed or temporarily laid off during the past 12 months, or if their work hours were shortened, their wages were reduced, or they were furloughed since the time of their last interview1 (23%). Respondents who were not in the labor force at the time of their interview were assigned to a third category (33%).
We classified financial problems as any of the following: being behind on utility payments (electricity, gas, or water and sewer), receiving a loan or cash advance from a payday lender or check casher, having a credit card cancelled, or filing for personal bankruptcy (27%). Housing instability was defined as any of the following: being behind on rent currently or at any time since their last interview, having moved because they could no longer afford their previous home, having moved in with others to share household expenses, being behind on their mortgage payments, in the process of foreclosure, having experienced a foreclosure within the last 12 months, or being recently evicted or homeless (14%).
Food insecurity was measured with the six-item short form of the USDA Food Security Survey Module (Economic Research Service 2012). Respondents were asked questions such as “In the last 12 months, did you ever eat less than you felt you should because there wasn’t enough money to buy food?” and “In the last 12 months were you ever hungry but didn’t eat because you couldn’t afford enough food?” We dichotomized the variable as either food secure (answering affirmatively to 0 or 1 questions) or food insecure (answering affirmatively to 2 or more questions) (22%), as defined in the guide to measuring food security by the USDA (2000). Forgone medical care was measured with items asking respondents whether since their last interview they could not afford to see a doctor or dentist or had skipped or reduced doses of prescription medication to save money. We classified respondents who responded positively to any of these experiences as having forgone medical care (22%). Our final measure of material hardship recorded the total number of measured hardships for each individual, with a range from 0 to 5. More than half of our respondents (54%) reported experiencing one or more material hardships.
Light Pollution
Nighttime radiance, a measure of light pollution, could prevent optimal sleep, and living in a light polluted area may be correlated with material hardship. Suburban areas around Detroit are generally more affluent than the more densely populated and light polluted urban areas. People experiencing material hardship may be forced to opt for less desirable housing options near highways and other sources of light pollution. We therefore control for average nighttime radiance which captures the amount of light pollution in respondents’ neighborhoods. Monthly radiance was calculated using satellite imagery from the Visible Infrared Imaging Radiometer Suite (VIIRS). While there are other sources of nighttime satellite imagery, namely the Defense Meteorological Satellite Program (DMSP), those data are limited by low spatial resolution and low dynamic range (Elvidge, Baugh, Zhizhin, & Hsu, 2013). In densely lit urban areas like Southeast Michigan, only the VIIRS data are able to capture spatial variations in light (Elvidge et al., 2013). However, due to this sensitivity, the VIIRS instrument is vulnerable to stray light entering the satellite and distorting its radiance measurements (Mills, Weiss, & Liang, 2013). Because stray-light-corrected monthly composites are only available from 2014 on, we used the radiance values from 2014 as proxies for light conditions during the third wave of interviews, conducted in 2013. Average monthly radiance, measured in nanowatts per steradian per square meter (nW·sr−1·m−2), was calculated at the census block level, and respondents were assigned the average radiance from the month in 2014 that corresponded to the month of their Wave 3 interview.
Other Covariates
In multivariable models, we adjusted for sociodemographic, socioeconomic, and health characteristics that may be correlated with both sleep and material hardship, including age, gender, race (dichotomized as Black or non-Black), marital status (married/cohabiting or not), children in the household (having children five years old and younger, having children all older than five, or having no children), education (less than high school, high school or some college, a bachelor’s degree or more), log-transformed household income, employment status, self-rated health (dichotomized as poor/fair or good/very good/excellent), obesity (a BMI of 30 and above), smoking cigarettes, harmful or hazardous alcohol use (measured by the AUDIT (Reinert & Allen, 2007)), an anxiety attack within the last four weeks, and depressive symptoms measured by the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9). When scoring the PHQ-9, we excluded the third item which asked respondents about sleep problems. A score of 10 or more is often used to distinguish clinically significant symptoms in the full PHQ-9 (Kroenke et al. 2010), and we used the same cutoff in our analysis. Cutoffs at 8, 9, and 11 yielded results that were not substantively different.
Statistical Analysis
We first examined bivariate relationships between each sleep outcome and all covariates. We then estimated logistic regression models to assess the relationship between each material hardship indicator and sleep outcome. After estimating unadjusted associations, control variables were added in blocks: beginning with sociodemographic controls (Models “A”), then socioeconomic variables (Models “B”), then health variables (Models “C”), and finally light pollution (Models “D”). The results of these logistic regression models are reported as average marginal effects to facilitate comparability across models. We applied survey weights to make our results representative of adults aged 19 to 64 from Southeastern Michigan and to account for attrition. All analyses were performed using Stata 15 with -mi- and -svy- commands that apply Rubin’s rules to generate combined estimates from the 20 multiply imputed datasets (StataCorp, 2017). We display average marginal effects of the main predictors in the tables included in the text. Full tables are included as an Appendix.
RESULTS
Sample Characteristics
Table 1 presents weighted sample characteristics stratified by sleep outcomes. Those with short sleep were more likely to report financial problems, housing instability, food insecurity, forgone medical care, and a greater number of total hardships. Respondents with sleep problems were more likely to report financial problems, housing instability, food insecurity, forgone medical care, and a greater number of total hardships. Respondents who reported nonrestorative sleep were more likely to report employment instability, financial problems, food insecurity, forgone medical care, and a greater number of total hardships. Across outcomes, respondents reporting any sleep issues had lower education, worse self-rated health, and a greater share of them was classified as depressed.
Table 1.
Sample Overall | Short Sleep | Sleep Problems | Nonrestorative Sleep | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Yes | No | p-value | Yes | No | p-value | Yes | No | p-value | ||
n | 730 | 113 | 617 | 143 | 587 | 279 | 451 | |||
Employment instability % | ||||||||||
Yes | 22.6 | 15.6 | 23.7 | 29.1 | 21.2 | * | 30.3 | 17.9 | ** | |
No | 44.6 | 41.2 | 45.2 | 33.1 | 47.0 | 39.2 | 47.9 | |||
Not in labor force | 32.8 | 43.2 | 31.1 | 37.8 | 31.8 | 30.5 | 34.2 | |||
Financial problems % | 27.0 | 45.0 | 24.2 | *** | 47.8 | 22.7 | *** | 34.0 | 22.8 | * |
Housing instability % | 14.1 | 27.6 | 11.9 | * | 25.2 | 11.8 | ** | 15.5 | 13.2 | |
Food insecurity % | 21.9 | 42.1 | 18.7 | ** | 45.7 | 17.0 | *** | 34.0 | 14.6 | *** |
Forgone medical care % | 21.8 | 42.6 | 18.5 | *** | 45.6 | 16.9 | *** | 32.5 | 15.3 | *** |
Mean number of hardships | 1.07 | 1.73 | 0.97 | ** | 1.93 | 0.90 | *** | 1.46 | 0.84 | *** |
[95% CI] | [0.95, 1.20] | [1.28, 2.17] | [.86, 1.08] | [1.59, 2.28] | [0.79, 1.00] | [1.23, 1.70] | [0.69, 0.99] | |||
Mean age | 46.3 | 45.6 | 46.4 | 45.9 | 46.4 | 44.4 | 47.5 | * | ||
[95% CI] | [44.9, 47.7] | [40.4, 50.7] | [44.6, 48.2] | [42.5, 49.2] | [44.6, 48.1] | [42.4, 46.4] | [45.6, 49.3] | |||
Woman % | 51.8 | 35.2 | 54.5 | * | 59.0 | 50.4 | 57.5 | 48.4 | ||
Black % | 25.0 | 35.4 | 23.3 | * | 30.6 | 23.8 | 21.6 | 27.0 | ||
Married or cohabiting % | 66.0 | 60.0 | 67.0 | 57.5 | 67.8 | * | 58.1 | 70.9 | ** | |
Children % | ||||||||||
No children | 49.3 | 45.4 | 49.9 | 41.8 | 50.8 | 39.9 | 55.0 | ** | ||
5 years old or less | 19.8 | 23.8 | 19.2 | 15.6 | 20.7 | 21.8 | 18.7 | |||
All children older than 5 | 30.9 | 30.8 | 30.9 | 42.5 | 28.5 | 38.3 | 26.4 | |||
Education % | ||||||||||
Less than high school | 7.0 | 7.1 | 7.0 | † | 10.8 | 6.2 | * | 6.0 | 7.6 | * |
HS or some college | 61.7 | 76.2 | 59.3 | 67.3 | 60.5 | 67.3 | 58.3 | |||
Bachelor’s or more | 31.3 | 16.6 | 33.7 | 21.9 | 33.3 | 26.7 | 34.1 | |||
Mean household income | $77,483 | $76,265 | $77,679 | $49,063 | $83,362 | *** | $66,365 | $84,232 | ** | |
[95% CI] | [$65,488, $89,477] | [$41,194, $111,337] | [$66,223, $89,135] | [$39,583, $58,542] | [$70,143, $96,582] | [$56,422, $76,307] | [$69,900, $98,563] | |||
Employment status % | ||||||||||
Employed | 61.2 | 53.7 | 62.4 | 51.3 | 63.2 | 62.1 | 60.6 | |||
Unemployed | 6.0 | 3.1 | 6.5 | 10.9 | 5.0 | 7.4 | 5.2 | |||
Not in labor force | 32.8 | 43.2 | 31.1 | 37.8 | 31.8 | 30.5 | 34.2 | |||
Poor/fair self-rated health % | 17.3 | 46.4 | 12.6 | *** | 43.0 | 12.0 | *** | 25.6 | 12.2 | * |
Obese (BMI ≥ 30) % | 33.4 | 34.6 | 33.2 | 36.9 | 32.7 | 33.0 | 33.6 | |||
Hazardous alcohol use % | 9.3 | 9.9 | 9.2 | 16.9 | 7.7 | † | 13.4 | 6.8 | * | |
Smokes cigarettes % | 32.8 | 35.8 | 32.3 | 50.2 | 29.2 | *** | 41.6 | 27.5 | * | |
Depression % | 10.5 | 27.3 | 7.8 | *** | 44.0 | 3.5 | *** | 21.0 | 4.1 | *** |
Anxiety attack in last mo. % | 9.9 | 18.2 | 8.6 | † | 36.5 | 4.4 | *** | 20.9 | 3.2 | *** |
Mean nighttime radiance | 22.5 | 25.4 | 22.0 | 26.4 | 21.7 | ** | 22.4 | 22.6 | ||
[95% CI] | [18.0, 27.0] | [20.6, 30.1] | [17.3, 26.8] | [22.5, 30.2] | [17.0, 26.4] | [18.8, 26.0] | [17.4, 27.7] |
p < 0.001,
p < 0.01,
p < 0.05,
p < 0.10
Logistic Regression Models
Short Sleep
Table 2 reports unadjusted average marginal effects capturing the association between the six material hardship variables and each sleep outcome. Financial problems, housing instability, food insecurity, forgone medical care, and a greater number of reported material hardships were associated with an increased probability of reporting short sleep. While these associations persisted after adjusting for sociodemographic and socioeconomic controls (Models A and B, Table 3), the inclusion of health and environmental variables fully attenuated the relationship between short sleep and all material hardship measures but employment instability (Models D, Table 3). Employment instability was associated with 7 percentage point lower probability of short sleep. In supplementary sensitivity analysis, where we excluded respondents sleeping longer than 9 hours from the reference group, we found our results for short sleep to be substantively unchanged.
Table 2.
Short Sleep | Sleep Problems | Nonrestorative Sleep | |
---|---|---|---|
Employment instability (ref: no instability) | |||
Experienced employment instability | −0.032 | 0.094 | 0.176** |
[−0.081, 0.017] | [−0.032, 0.219] | [0.066, 0.285] | |
Not in labor force | 0.055 | 0.070* | 0.02 |
[−0.043, 0.152] | [0.005, 0.136] | [−0.067, 0.106] | |
Financial problems | 0.126** | 0.180*** | 0.134* |
[0.044, 0.208] | [0.114, 0.246] | [0.031, 0.237] | |
Housing instability | 0.155* | 0.158** | 0.044 |
[0.001, 0.308] | [0.042, 0.274] | [−0.116, 0.204] | |
Food insecurity | 0.164** | 0.238*** | 0.266*** |
[0.059, 0.268] | [0.155, 0.321] | [0.125, 0.407] | |
Forgone medical care | 0.168** | 0.238*** | 0.237*** |
[0.060, 0.277] | [0.128, 0.348] | [0.132, 0.342] | |
Number of material hardships | 0.044*** | 0.069*** | 0.082*** |
[0.020, 0.068] | [0.051, 0.086] | [0.049, 0.114] |
p < 0.001,
p < 0.01,
p < 0.05,
p < 0.10
Table 3.
Short Sleep | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Models A | Models B | Models C | Models D | |
Employment instability (ref: no instability) | ||||
Experienced hardship | −0.035 | −0.046* | −0.069** | −0.071** |
[−0.083, 0.013] | [−0.084, −0.008] | [−0.117, −0.020] | [−0.119, −0.022] | |
Not in labor force | 0.082† | 0.064 | −0.006 | −0.007 |
[−0.012, 0.176] | [−0.027, 0.156] | [−0.059, 0.046] | [−0.061, 0.047] | |
Financial problems | 0.109* | 0.098* | 0.046 | 0.045 |
[0.024, 0.194] | [0.022, 0.175] | [−0.031, 0.124] | [−0.029, 0.119] | |
Housing instability | 0.135† | 0.123* | 0.051 | 0.050 |
[−0.016, 0.286] | [0.008, 0.238] | [−0.031, 0.132] | [−0.030, 0.130] | |
Food insecurity | 0.148* | 0.127** | 0.062 | 0.063 |
[0.029, 0.267] | [0.037, 0.217] | [−0.020, 0.144] | [−0.022, 0.147] | |
Forgone medical care | 0.144* | 0.130* | 0.059 | 0.058 |
[0.036, 0.252] | [0.023, 0.237] | [−0.022, 0.139] | [−0.023, 0.139] | |
Number of hardships | 0.040** | 0.043** | 0.019 | 0.018 |
[0.011, 0.069] | [0.018, 0.068] | [−0.008, 0.046] | [−0.008, 0.045] |
p < 0.001,
p < 0.01,
p < 0.05,
p < 0.10
Four models were estimated for each material hardship measure, with control variables added in stages. The A models included controls for sociodemographic factors: age, gender, race, marital status, and children under five years old. The B models included further controls for socioeconomic status: education, household income, and employment status; except for models of employment instability where employment status was not included. The C models included further controls for health: poor or fair health, obesity, alcohol use, cigarette smoking, depression, and anxiety. The D models are fully adjusted models, which included a final control for light pollution. For all hardship measures, the difference between the average marginal effects in the C and D models is not statistically significant.
Sleep Problems
Not being in the labor force, financial problems, housing instability, food insecurity, forgone medical care, and a greater number of total material hardships were all associated with a greater probability of reporting sleep problems in unadjusted models in Table 2. Though all the associations remained statistically significant after controlling for sociodemographic and socioeconomic factors (Models A and B, Table 4), in the fully adjusted models, only the number of material hardships was statistically significant at conventional levels (Models D, Table 4). Each additional material hardship was associated with a 2 percentage point greater probability of sleep problems. We also found a marginally statistically significant association between food insecurity and sleep problems, where food insecure people had approximately 7 percentage point greater probability of sleep problems. Depression was the only covariate statistically significantly associated with sleep problems across the fully adjusted models. In supplementary sensitivity analysis, we varied the cut-off for categorizing respondents as having sleep problems. With a less conservative coding of “several days” or more of sleep problems as opposed to “more than half the days” used in the main analysis, financial problems, food insecurity, forgone medical care, and the total number of material hardships were all statistically significant predictors of sleep problems in the fully adjusted models.
Table 4.
Sleep Problems | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Models A | Models B | Models C | Models D | |
Employment instability (ref: no instability) | ||||
Experienced hardship | 0.078 | 0.031 | −0.004 | −0.002 |
[−0.048, 0.205] | [−0.084, 0.146] | [−0.097, 0.090] | [−0.097, 0.093] | |
Not in labor force | 0.077** | 0.043† | −0.014 | −0.013 |
[0.022, 0.132] | [−0.008, 0.094] | [−0.081, 0.053] | [−0.080, 0.054] | |
Financial problems | 0.180*** | 0.149*** | 0.040 | 0.043 |
[0.103, 0.258] | [0.067, 0.232] | [−0.038, 0.118] | [−0.033, 0.119] | |
Housing instability | 0.150** | 0.122* | −0.004 | −0.004 |
[0.044, 0.257] | [0.025, 0.219] | [−0.057, 0.050] | [−0.057, 0.050] | |
Food insecurity | 0.224*** | 0.196*** | 0.067† | 0.066† |
[0.140, 0.308] | [0.122, 0.270] | [−0.002, 0.135] | [−0.001, 0.133] | |
Forgone medical care | 0.234*** | 0.212*** | 0.081 | 0.082 |
[0.122, 0.347] | [0.099, 0.324] | [−0.029, 0.191] | [−0.027, 0.191] | |
Number of hardships | 0.072*** | 0.067*** | 0.022* | 0.023* |
[0.050, 0.094] | [0.050, 0.083] | [0.001, 0.043] | [0.002, 0.044] |
p < 0.001,
p < 0.01,
p < 0.05,
p < 0.10
Four models were estimated for each material hardship measure, with control variables added in stages. The A models included controls for sociodemographic factors: age, gender, race, marital status, and children under five years old. The B models included further controls for socioeconomic status: education, household income, and employment status; except for models of employment instability where employment status was not included. The C models included further controls for health: poor or fair health, obesity, alcohol use, cigarette smoking, depression, and anxiety. The D models are fully adjusted models, which included a final control for light pollution. For all hardship measures, the difference between the average marginal effects it the C and D models is not statistically significant.
Nonrestorative Sleep
In unadjusted models (Table 2), we found statistically significant associations between nonrestorative sleep and employment instability, financial problems, food insecurity, forgone medical care, and a greater number of total material hardships. After adjusting for sociodemographic and socioeconomic variables (Models A and B, Table 5), the relationship between nonrestorative sleep and financial problems was no longer statistically significant. In the fully adjusted models (Models D, Table 5), food insecurity became marginally statistically significant with an average marginal effect of 0.15; employment instability, forgone medical care, and the number of material hardships remained statistically significant, each associated with a 12, 16, and 5 percentage point greater probability of nonrestorative sleep, respectively.
Table 5.
Nonrestorative Sleep | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Models A | Models B | Models C | Models D | |
Employment instability (ref: no instability) | ||||
Experienced hardship | 0.164** | 0.146* | 0.126* | 0.120* |
[0.052, 0.275] | [0.026, 0.265] | [0.013, 0.239] | [0.003, 0.237] | |
Not in labor force | 0.053 | 0.042 | −0.001 | −0.005 |
[−0.038, 0.144] | [−0.047, 0.131] | [−0.072, 0.070] | [−0.075, 0.065] | |
Financial problems | 0.129* | 0.097 | 0.016 | 0.009 |
[0.027, 0.232] | [−0.025, 0.218] | [−0.105, 0.137] | [−0.106, 0.124] | |
Housing instability | 0.036 | 0.027 | −0.079 | −0.076 |
[−0.113, 0.185] | [−0.123, 0.176] | [−0.175, 0.017] | [−0.175, 0.023] | |
Food insecurity | 0.253** | 0.245** | 0.147 | 0.151† |
[0.109, 0.396] | [0.099, 0.390] | [−0.033, 0.328] | [−0.029, 0.331] | |
Forgone medical care | 0.230*** | 0.243*** | 0.159** | 0.160** |
[0.130, 0.330] | [0.137, 0.348] | [0.057, 0.261] | [0.058, 0.263] | |
Number of hardships | 0.083*** | 0.084** | 0.049† | 0.048* |
[0.048, 0.119] | [0.037, 0.131] | [−0.000, 0.098] | [0.000, 0.096] |
p < 0.001
p < 0.01,
p < 0.05,
p < 0.10
Four models were estimated for each material hardship measure, with control variables added in stages. The A models included controls for sociodemographic factors: age, gender, race, marital status, and children under five years old. The B models included further controls for socioeconomic status: education, household income, and employment status; except for models of employment instability where employment status was not included. The C models included further controls for health: poor or fair health, obesity, alcohol use, cigarette smoking, depression, and anxiety. The D models are fully adjusted models, which included a final control for light pollution. For all hardship measures, the difference between the average marginal effects in the C and D models is not statistically significant.
DISCUSSION
Sleep is essential for a healthy life. Accordingly, one of the objectives of Healthy People 2020 (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2018) is to increase the proportion of adults who obtain sufficient sleep. However, this key ingredient of health is not equitably distributed in the US population. People with low SES tend to enjoy fewer of the benefits of good sleep because they are less likely to obtain it (Krueger & Friedman, 2009). One possible explanation for the observed SES disparity is material hardship, which might disrupt the sleep environment, cause stress or worry, and translate into sleeplessness. Prior scholarship in this field has focused on food insecurity and housing instability, both of which were found to be positively associated with poor sleep outcomes (Liu et al., 2014). The present study adds to the emerging literature by examining the associations between multiple forms of material hardship and sleep, while employing a robust set of sociodemographic, SES, health, and environmental controls in a cross-sectional sample of working-age adults who lived in Southeast Michigan in 2013.
In contrast to past work, our results did not show strong evidence of associations between housing instability and sleep after adjusting for a wide range of sociodemographic and health characteristics and light pollution. Moreover, the associations between food insecurity and negative sleep outcomes were weaker than we anticipated based on past literature. Food insecurity was a marginally statistically significant predictor of sleep problems and nonrestorative sleep. In sensitivity analysis where we specified food insecurity as a continuous scale as opposed to using the USDA recommended food insecurity threshold, we found food insecurity to be statistically significantly associated with short sleep (AME 0.02), but not with the two other negative sleep outcomes. The difference between our findings and the findings reported by prior studies may be attributed to our more extensive set of health and sociodemographic controls than were used in prior analysis. Our results suggest that the associations between housing instability, sleep insecurity and negative sleep outcomes could be explained by other physical and mental health conditions. Thus, while these material hardships are correlated with worse sleep outcomes, the association may be accounted for by other negative changes in health linked to material hardship. Alternatively, poor health may be a cause of both material hardship and poor sleep. We encourage future research using larger datasets with longitudinal measurements of health, sleep, and material hardship to examine these potential mediating and confounding pathways.
In addition, we found that respondents who experienced employment instability were less likely than those with stable jobs to report short sleep, but more likely to feel unrested during the day. This finding aligns with prior work showing that unemployed people sleep longer hours than their employed counterparts, likely because they have more free time available to devote to sleep (Antillón, Lauderdale, & Mullahy, 2014). We speculate that the nonrestorative nature of sleep reported by those with employment instability could be ascribed to stress (Linn, Sandifer, & Stein, 1985), but we are unable to test this directly because there is no stress measure available in our data.
Our study shows a previously undocumented link between forgone medical care and sleep quality. Forgone medical care, defined as not seeking professional medical attention when one feels it is needed or skipping doses of medication to save money, was associated with nonrestorative sleep. There are several mechanisms through which forgone medical care could contribute to poor sleep. Like other material hardships, it may operate through worry and anxiety. One may be kept up at night by concerns about not being able to access medical care, but the absence of needed care or medicine could also result in poor sleep outcomes more directly. Untreated medical conditions can lead to pain and other physical symptoms that could prevent sleep and hamper its quality. Our study is unable to adjudicate between the two possible pathways because we do not know the nature of the medical conditions for which respondents wished to seek medical attention. It is also possible the respondents may have wished to consult a medical professional about their sleep issues. If that is the case, the measured association between foregone medical care and sleep is due to endogeneity, that is, inadequate sleep was the ailment for which the care was foregone.
Finally, we discovered that a greater number of material hardships was associated with a greater probability of both sleep problems and nonrestorative sleep. It may be that each material hardship additively increases the amount of stress individual experiences and the increasing stress levels hampers their ability to enjoy needed restorative sleep. In sensitivity analysis, we found that the documented relationship is likely driven by a threshold effect, where the probability of negative outcomes is substantially increased for respondents with one or more hardships compared to those with no hardship, but does not grow monotonically as the hardship burden rises. However, the reliability of the inferences that can be drawn based on this sensitivity analysis is compromised by the small number of respondents (16%) who reported more than two hardships. Further research is necessary to understand the nature of the relationship between cumulative material hardship experience and sleep.
Limitations
The data used are cross-sectional. Although it is more plausible that material hardship leads to poor sleep outcomes, the reverse may be true in certain circumstances. For example, a person with severe sleep issues may find it difficult to hold a steady job due to daytime fatigue. As sleeplessness is associated with impaired cognitive function (Alhola & Polo-Kantola, 2007), the lack of quality sleep could also lead to poor financial decisions that translate into material hardships or to the inability to earn enough resources.
All our measures except light pollution are self-reported, including measures of sleep quantity and quality. Methodological literature has demonstrated that people tend not to be the best judges of their own sleep. Objective sleep data, for example measured through actigraphy, may have provided more accurate measures of sleep duration than self-reported data. Prior evidence, for example, suggests that self-reported sleep tends to be longer than sleep duration recorded by actigraphy (Lauderdale, Knutson, Yan, Liu, & Rathouz, 2008). Moreover, actigraphy may be able to capture even those sleep disturbances that respondents are not themselves aware of.
We use a population sample of working-age adults living in Southeast Michigan. These results may not be generalizable to older adults whose sleep patterns differ and need not be correlated with material hardship in a manner similar to that of working age adults. Because the region was strongly affected by the Great Recession and the prevalence of material hardship was and remains particularly high in the area (Burgard et al., 2012; Gould-Werth & Seefeldt, 2012), it may be that material hardship and sleep do not have the same relationship in our sample as they do in other places, where material hardship may be rarer. The unique context of the study could explain why we did not detect any associations between housing instability and negative sleep outcomes, as this form of material hardship may have become normalized in a region where one in 3 residents, or a total of 139,699 homes, went through a foreclosure between 2005 and 2015 (Kurth & MacDonald, 2015).
Conclusion
This study is among the first to examine associations between multiple material hardship indicators and sleep. While prior research has established material hardship as a possible pathway through which SES contributes to disparities in sleep, little is known about how specific hardships are associated with sleep and how their relationships to sleep might differ. We address this gap and discover that, after accounting for individual sociodemographic and health characteristics, material hardship measured in our study is not associated with short sleep. We find that forgone medical care and employment instability are associated with nonrestorative sleep. We find only weak evidence for the association between food insecurity and sleep problems and nonrestorative sleep. For both sleep problems and nonrestorative sleep, higher hardship burden was associated with a greater probability of an adverse sleep outcome. Our results present suggestive evidence that interventions that address material hardships, and especially forgone medical care, such as free community clinics and prescription drugs subsidies, may positively affect sleep quality in disadvantaged populations and consequently help remedy some of the persistent disparities in health in the United States. We encourage further examination of these relationships with longitudinal data on both sleep and material hardship.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements:
The data used in this study were collected with funds from the National Poverty Center (NPC) at the University of Michigan, the MacArthur Foundation (09–94176-HCD) the Russell Sage Foundation (92–11-08), and the Ford Foundation (N011667–0). The authors gratefully acknowledge use of the services and facilities of the Population Studies Center at the University of Michigan, funded by NICHD Center Grant R24 HD041028.
Footnotes
The average time between Wave 2 and Wave 3 interviews was approximately 27 months
Contributor Information
Lucie Kalousová, Nuffield College, University of Oxford.
Brian Xiao, University of Michigan.
Sarah A. Burgard, University of Michigan
References
- Alhola P, & Polo-Kantola P (2007). Sleep deprivation: Impact on cognitive performance. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 3(5), 553–567. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antillón M, Lauderdale DS, & Mullahy J (2014). Sleep Behavior and Unemployment Conditions. Economics and human biology, 14, 22–32. doi: 10.1016/j.ehb.2014.03.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baglioni C, Spiegelhalder K, Lombardo C, & Riemann D (2010). Sleep and emotions: A focus on insomnia. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 14(4), 227–238. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgard SA, Danziger S, Seefeldt KS, Allard S, Danziger SK, Engler T, . . . Wathen M (2012). Persisting Hardships in Southeast Michigan After the Great Recession. Retrieved from https://poverty.umich.edu/files/2017/02/policybrief33.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Ding M, Keiley MK, Garza KB, Duffy PA, & Zizza CA (2015). Food Insecurity Is Associated with Poor Sleep Outcomes among US Adults1–3. The Journal of Nutrition, 145(3), 615–621. doi: 10.3945/jn.114.199919 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elvidge C, Baugh KE, Zhizhin M, & Hsu F-C (2013). Why VIIRS data are superior to DMSP for mapping nighttime lights. Proceedings of the Asia-Pacific Advanced Network, 35, 62–69. [Google Scholar]
- Gould-Werth A, & Seefeldt KS (2012). Material Hardships During the Great Recession: Findings From the Michigan Recession and Recovery Study (35). Retrieved from Ann Arbor, MI: [Google Scholar]
- Grandner MA (2017). Sleep, Health, and Society. Sleep Med Clin, 12(1), 1–22. doi: 10.1016/j.jsmc.2016.10.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grandner MA, Patel NP, Gehrman PR, Xie D, Sha D, Weaver T, & Gooneratne N (2010). Who gets the best sleep? Ethnic and socioeconomic factors related to sleep complaints. Sleep Med, 11(5), 470–478. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2009.10.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall M, Bromberger J, & Matthews K (1999). Socioeconomic Status as a Correlate of Sleep in African-American and Caucasian Women. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 896(1), 427–430. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08161.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heflin CM, & Iceland J (2009). Poverty, Material Hardship, and Depression*. Social Science Quarterly, 90(5), 1051–1071. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6237.2009.00645.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jordan ML, Perez-Escamilla R, Desai MM, & Shamah-Levy T (2016). Household Food Insecurity and Sleep Patterns Among Mexican Adults: Results from ENSANUT-2012. J Immigr Minor Health, 18(5), 1093–1103. doi: 10.1007/s10903-015-0246-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiely KM, Leach LS, Olesen SC, & Butterworth P (2015). How financial hardship is associated with the onset of mental health problems over time. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 50(6), 909–918. doi: 10.1007/s00127-015-1027-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krueger PM, & Friedman EM (2009). Sleep duration in the United States: a cross-sectional population-based study. Am J Epidemiol, 169(9), 1052–1063. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwp023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kurth J, & MacDonald C (2015). Volume of abandoned homes ‘absolutely terrifying. The Detroit News. Retrieved from https://www.detroitnews.com/story/news/special-reports/2015/05/14/detroit-abandoned-homes-volume-terrifying/27237787/ [Google Scholar]
- Lallukka T, Arber S, Rahkonen O, & Lahelma E (2010). Complaints of insomnia among midlife employed people: the contribution of childhood and present socioeconomic circumstances. Sleep Med, 11(9), 828–836. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.04.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lauderdale DS, Knutson KL, Yan LL, Liu K, & Rathouz PJ (2008). Self-reported and measured sleep duration: how similar are they? Epidemiology, 19(6), 838–845. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e318187a7b0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linn MW, Sandifer R, & Stein S (1985). Effects of unemployment on mental and physical health. American Journal of Public Health, 75(5), 502–506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu Y, Njai RS, Greenlund KJ, Chapman DP, & Croft JB (2014). Relationships Between Housing and Food Insecurity, Frequent Mental Distress, and Insufficient Sleep Among Adults in 12 US States, 2009. Preventing Chronic Disease, 11, E37. doi: 10.5888/pcd11.130334 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynch JW, Smith GD, Harper S, Hillemeier M, Ross N, Kaplan GA, & Wolfson M (2004). Is income inequality a determinant of population health? Part 1. A systematic review. Milbank Quarterly, 82(1), 5–99. doi: 10.1111/j.0887-378X.2004.00302.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynch JW, Smith GD, Kaplan GA, & House JS (2000). Income inequality and mortality: importance to health of individual income, psychosocial environment, or material conditions. British Medical Journal, 320(7243), 1200–1204. doi: 10.1136/bmj.320.7243.1200 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manuel JI, Martinson ML, Bledsoe-Mansori SE, & Bellamy JL (2012). The influence of stress and social support on depressive symptoms in mothers with young children. Soc Sci Med, 75(11), 2013–2020. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.07.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller SD, Straka W, Mills SP, Elvidge CD, Lee TF, Solbrig J, . . . Weiss SC (2013). Illuminating the Capabilities of the Suomi National Polar-Orbiting Partnership (NPP) Visible Infrared Imaging Radiometer Suite (VIIRS) Day/Night Band. Remote Sensing, 5(12), 6717–6766. doi: 10.3390/rs5126717 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mills S, Weiss S, & Liang C (2013, 2013). VIIRS day/night band (DNB) stray light characterization and correction. [Google Scholar]
- Perales F, & Plage S (2017). Losing ground, losing sleep: Local economic conditions, economic vulnerability, and sleep. Social Science Research, 62, 189–203. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2016.08.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reinert DF, & Allen JP (2007). The alcohol use disorders identification test: An update of research findings. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 31(2), 185–199. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00295.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reitzel LR, Short NA, Schmidt NB, Garey L, Zvolensky MJ, Moisiuc A, . . . Businelle MS (2017). Distress Tolerance Links Sleep Problems with Stress and Health in Homeless. American Journal of Health Behavior, 41(6), 760–774. doi: 10.5993/AJHB.41.6.10 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Royston, P. (2006). ICE: Stata module for multiple imputation of missing values: Boston College Department of Economics.
- StataCorp. (2017). Stata Statistical Software: Release 15 (Version Release 15). College Station, TX: StataCorp LLC. [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, O.o. D. P. a. H. P. (2018). Healthy People 2020 Retrieved from https://www.healthypeople.gov/2020/topics-objectives/topic/sleep-health/objectives
- USDA. (2000). Guide to measuring household food security : revised March, 2000. Retrieved from Alexandria, VA: http://hdl.handle.net/10113/38369
- Watts FN, Coyle K, & East MP (1994). The contribution of worry to insomnia. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 33(2), 211–220. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1994.tb01115.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zoccola PM, Dickerson SS, & Lam S (2009). Rumination Predicts Longer Sleep Onset Latency After an Acute Psychosocial Stressor. Psychosomatic Medicine, 71(7), 771–775. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181ae58e8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.