Abstract
Alexithymia, or the inability to identify and describe one’s emotions, is significantly higher in bipolar disorder (BD) and schizophrenia (SZ), compared to healthy controls (HC). Alexithymia has also been observed to predict psychosocial functioning in SZ. We investigated whether alexithymia predicted social and everyday functioning in BD, as well as transdiagnostically in HC, BD, and SZ patients. 56 BD, 45 SZ, and 50 HC were administered and compared on tests measuring neurocognition, social cognition, functioning and alexithymia. We conducted linear regressions assessing whether alexithymia predicted functional outcomes in BD. Next, we conducted hierarchical stepwise linear regressions investigating the predictive ability of neurocognition, social cognition and alexithymia on everyday and social functioning in our overall sample. BD and SZ patients were comparable on most demographics and demonstrated higher alexithymia compared to HCs. In BD, alexithymia predicted social functioning only. In the overall sample, difficulty identifying and describing feelings predicted everyday functioning; difficulty describing feelings predicted social functioning. Results suggest that aspects of alexithymia significantly predict functioning among these psychiatric groups, above and beyond the contributions of previously identified factors such as neurocognition and social cognition. Results may aid in developing proper interventions aimed at improving patients’ ability to articulate their feelings.
Keywords: bipolar, schizophrenia, neurocognition, social cognition, functioning, alexithymia, psychosis
1. INTRODUCTION
Bipolar disorder (BD) primarily features chronic and recurrent affective episodes that negatively affect functional outcome in at least two-thirds of patients (Huxley and Baldessarini, 2007). Additionally, BD patients also present with impaired psychosocial functioning (MacQueen, Young, and Jaffe, 2008; Sanchez-Moreno et al., 2009), even during affective remission. Initial clinical and demographic predictors of functional status have included older age, depressive symptoms, number of previous mixed episodes and hospitalizations (Rosa et al., 2009). However, subsequent research assessing everyday functioning in BD have demonstrated limited predictive ability of clinical and demographic factors (Martinez-Aran et al., 2007; Tabarés-Seisdedos et al., 2008), suggesting that additional factors significantly contribute to functional status.
Poor overall functioning, including work, social, and everyday performance, is common among individuals with major psychiatric disorders such as BD and schizophrenia (SZ) (Ormel et al., 2008). Consistently neurocognitive deficits have been associated with lower overall functioning for both BD and SZ patients (Martinez-Aran et al., 2002; Tabarés-Seisdedos et al., 2008). Many BD patients present with impaired performance on neurocognitive domains of attention, memory, and executive function (Martinez-Aran et al., 2004; Burdick et al. 2007), deficits that are associated with poorer psychosocial and everyday functioning (Martino et al., 2009; Sanchez-Moreno et al., 2009; Burdick et al 2010). Similarly, SZ patients are impaired on most neurocognitive domains (Keefe et al., 2006) and these deficits are also associated with impaired social and occupational functioning (Bowie et al., 2010; Martinez-Aran et al., 2002). Impaired social cognition is also another predictor of poorer functional outcome, especially for SZ patients (Couture et al., 2006); this relationship exists for certain social cognitive domains, particularly emotion recognition, theory of mind, and social perception. Social cognitive deficits have also been observed in BD, particularly domains of emotion recognition and theory of mind (Bora et al., 2016; Samamé et al., 2012), although its relationship to functioning has been inconclusive (Lahera et al., 2012; Thaler et al., 2014). Current consensus supports the notion that neurocognition and social cognition are overlapping, yet partially separable constructs which contribute to functional status in a non-redundant manner (Mehta et al., 2013). Studies which have assessed the relationship between neurocognition, social cognition and functioning have observed a mediating effect of social cognition on the neurocognitive-functioning relationship in SZ, such that neurocognition predicts functional status through potential underlying social cognitive mechanisms (Schmidt et al., 2011). In BD, interestingly, social cognition has been shown to moderate this same relationship, suggesting the presence of social cognitive heterogeneity (i.e., differential neurocognitive-functioning relationships depending on the individual’s social cognitive level) (Ospina et al., 2018). Overall, these relationships suggest that social cognitive ability may be dependent on more basic neurocognitive processes, both of which partially contribute to functional outcome.
While some domains of social cognition, such as theory of mind and emotion recognition, have been commonly examined in the psychiatric literature, alexithymia remains largely unexplored. Alexithymia is defined as a difficulty in recognizing and articulating the emotional experiences of the self (Sifneos, 1972), and may relate to domains of emotion self-regulation and self-awareness (Taylor et al., 1999). Aspects of alexithymia include: 1) difficulty in identifying and describing feelings, 2) difficulty distinguishing feelings from bodily sensations, 3) a deficit in symbolic thinking, and 4) a tendency to focus attention externally. Clinically, individuals with alexithymia avoid speaking about their feelings; instead they describe the logic of their cognitive and behavioral actions. Their speech is monotonous, stilted, and lacks richness. While they may not admit to feeling clinical symptoms, such as depression or anxiety, they may complain about physical symptoms. They are also characterized by an impoverished fantasy life, impaired emotional functioning, and present with difficulty in interpersonal relationships (Taylor, 1984). Given this inability to recognize self-referential cognitive states, alexithymia has been theorized to partially represent deficits in metacognition (Dimaggio et al., 2009), with certain metacognitive strategies correlating with aspects of alexithymia (Babei et al., 2016). Studies in BD have generally shown that BD patients present with higher alexithymia scores, particularly difficulty in identifying and describing feelings, compared to healthy controls (HCs) (Herold et al., 2017; Yilmaz et al., 2016). Interestingly, studies comparing psychotic versus non-psychotic axis I disorders (as well as one study comparing BD to major depressive disorder) revealed no difference in alexithymia between the diagnostic groups (Heshmati et al., 2010; Karayağiz et al., 2016; Picardi et al., 2012), suggesting that alexithymia may not reliably differentiate between mood and psychotic disorders and may in fact be a characteristic of major psychiatric illnesses in general.
Studies in SZ have reported higher alexithymia scores compared to HCs (Cedro et al., 2001; van’t Wout et al., 2007). Furthermore, recent studies have also found alexithymia to predict psychosocial functioning in SZ patients (Kimhy et al., 2012) as well as in individuals at high risk for psychosis (Kimhy et al., 2016), wherein difficulty in describing feelings accounted for a significant amount of variance in predicting psychosocial functioning above and beyond the predictive ability of neurocognitive and other social cognitive domains. Measures of metacognition have also been shown to predict psychosocial and everyday functioning in SZ (Arnon-Ribenfeld et al., 2017; Fogley et al., 2014), and like social cognition, deficits in metacognitive processes have also been shown to mediate the relationship between neurocognition and social functioning in SZ patients (Lysaker et al., 2010). However, while they may share common underlying mechanisms, social cognition and metacognition may represent distinct constructs which relate to social functioning in differing ways (Fogley et al., 2014). To date, only one study in BD has evaluated the effect of alexithymia on functioning, focusing on quality of life; higher alexithymia scores predicted lower quality of life in both BD and depressed patients (Karayağiz et al., 2016). However, it remains unclear whether alexithymia predicts other functional outcomes in BD, as has been shown in SZ.
Most studies have generally shown greater alexithmyia in psychiatric populations compared to HCs, with little to any distinction between specific major psychiatric disorders. Some studies have also demonstrated an association between alexithymia and functioning, particularly in SZ. However, alexithymia research in BD is scant, particularly regarding diagnostic comparisons of alexithymia between BD and other psychiatric disorders, as well as determining alexithymia’s predictive ability of functional outcomes in BD. The current study aimed to extend the literature regarding alexithymia in a BD sample, specifically: 1) to compare BD, SZ, and HC groups on alexithymia domains, and 2) assess whether alexithymia domains predict functional outcomes within a BD group. First, we hypothesized that BD would not be distinguishable from SZ on alexithymia domains, given prior research (Karayağiz et al., 2016; Picardi et al., 2012). Second, given commonalities (i.e., clinical, genetic, and neurobiological) between BD and SZ, we posited that alexithymia would predict functioning in BD patients, since this relationship has been observed in SZ individuals (Kimhy et al., 2012). Finally, we aimed to explore the predictive ability of alexithymia above and beyond previously identified factors, such as neurocognition and social cognition, transdiagnostically in our overall sample. Since other social cognitive domains (and to some degree, metacognition) have demonstrated modulating effects on the neurocognitive-functioning relationship (Lysaker et al., 2010; Ospina et al., 2018; Schmidt et al., 2011), we hypothesized that alexithymia would account for some of the predictive variance on functional outcome in our overall sample.
2. METHODS
2.1. Participants
The sample consisted of 151 participants diagnosed with: either BD I (n=46) or BD II (n=10), schizophrenia (n=23) or schizoaffective disorder (n=22), and HC (n=50). All participants were recruited at Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai in an R01-funded study between 2012 to 2017; recruitment advertisements were posted throughout the metropolitan, NYC area. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board and we obtained written informed consent from all participants. Inclusion criteria for participants included: 1) diagnosis of BD I, BD II (BD), schizophrenia, or schizoaffective disorder (SZ) using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-IV) (First et al., 2002), 2) and age 18 to 65 years. We recruited HCs separately as presenting without evidence of any Axis I disorder. Exclusion criterion for HCs included presence of an Axis I disorder among the participants’ first-degree relatives based on self-report. Exclusion criteria for all participants were: 1) history of central nervous system trauma, neurological disorder, or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, 2) recent severe substance abuse/dependence disorder in the past 3 months, determined using the SCID, 3) electroconvulsive therapy in the past 12 months, 4) an active, unstable medical problem (e.g., a diagnosis of metastatic brain cancer, multiple sclerosis), 5) an estimated, premorbid IQ<70 (using the Wide Range Achievement Test-3rd Edition [WRAT] Reading subtest (Wilkinson, 1993), and 6) individuals taking medications with known adverse cognitive affects or cognitive enhancers.
2.2. Measures
Alexithymia
Alexithymia was assessed using the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) (Bagby et al., 1994), a 20-item self-report measure that evaluates 3 subscales: 1) difficulty identifying feelings, 2) difficulty describing feelings, and 3) externally-oriented thinking. Each item is rated on a 5-point scale (from 1=‘strongly disagree’ to 5=‘strongly agree’); subscales are computed by summing relevant items, and a total alexithymia score is computed by summing responses to all 20 items, with greater TAS scores representing greater alexithymia. The TAS-20 has demonstrated solid internal consistency and reliability.
Clinical
DSM-IV BD or SZ diagnoses (or lack of Axis I diagnosis in HCs), presence of lifetime psychotic features, illness length in years, and psychiatric medication use were ascertained from the SCID-IV by highly trained psychologists. Current manic and depressive symptomatology were evaluated using the Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS) (Young et al., 1978) and Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD) (Hamilton, 1960), respectively.
Neurocognition
We assessed neurocognition using the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB) (Nuechterlein and Green, 2006). The MCCB contains 10 tests measuring 7 domains: 1) processing speed (Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia, Trail Making Test Part A, and semantic fluency), 2) attention and vigilance (Continuous Performance Test-Identical Pairs), 3) working memory (Weschler Memory Scale spatial and letter number span), 4) verbal learning (Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised [HVLT-R]), 5) visual learning (Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised), 6) reasoning and problem solving (Neuropsychological Assessment Battery Mazes subtest), and 7) social cognition (Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test [MSCEIT]). We replaced the HVLT-R with the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT), as it has demonstrated better sensitivity in determining verbal learning deficits in a BD (i.e., less impaired) population (Yatham et al., 2010; Burdick et al. 2011). The battery generally takes 70 minutes to complete in a single session. Scores are expressed in T-scores with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10. A global neurocognitive composite score was calculated by averaging the T-scores of all MCCB domains and CVLT, with the exception of the MSCEIT.
Social Cognition
The MCCB assesses social cognition using the MSCEIT Managing Emotions Subtest, which measures emotion management and emotion regulation by presenting vignettes of various social situations; participants are instructed to choose the most appropriate social response to achieve preferred outcomes. We assessed additional social cognitive domains, such as theory of mind, using the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET) (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001). The RMET consists of 36 black-and-white photographs of pairs of eyes with the rest of the face obscured, each associated with a forced-choice emotion label; each correct answer scored one point. We evaluated facial affect recognition by administering the Emotion Recognition Task (ERT), a computer-based subtest of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) (Robbins et al., 1994). The ERT presents 90 images of actors mimicking one of the 6 universal emotional expressions in two blocks, for a total of 180 stimuli. After each stimulus presentation (200 ms), the participant is instructed to choose among the emotional labels displayed on the screen: happiness, sadness, anger, disgust, fear and surprise. The ERT provides the number and percentage of correctly identified facial expressions. All neurocognitive and social cognitive measures were administered by highly trained clinical research coordinators under extensive supervision.
Functioning
We evaluated everyday functioning using the World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule (WHODAS 2.0) (Üstün, 2010), a 36-item measure that assesses disability severity across 6 domains in the past 30 days: 1) understanding and communicating, 2) getting around, 3) self-care, 4) getting along with others, 5) life activities, and 6) participation in society. Given low employment rates in our sample, we utilized the alternate 32-item calculation omitting employment. We computed domain scores by summing the relevant item responses, where greater WHODAS scores represented worse functioning. We also assessed social functioning using the Social Adjustment Scale-Self Report (SASSR) (Weissman, 1999), a 54-item questionnaire designed to measure performance over the past 2 weeks in 6 role areas: 1) work (either for paid work, unpaid housework, or student), 2) social and leisure activities, 3) relationships with extended family, 4) marital or intimate partner role, 5) parental role, and 6) role in the familial unit, including perceptions of financial support. Each question is rated on a 5-point scale, and means are computed for individual role areas as well as an overall functioning mean; higher SAS scores represent worse social functioning. Role areas not relevant to the participant may be skipped; therefore, role area means are computed for all items completed by the participant.
2.3. Statistical Analyses
We first compared BD, SZ, and HCs on demographics, symptoms, and premorbid IQ using chi-squares and ANOVAs, as appropriate. We compared TAS scales between diagnostic groups using ANCOVAS, controlling for significant clinical and demographic variables. Additional analyses included diagnostic group comparisons using ANCOVAs and MANCOVAs to compare neurocognitive, social cognitive and functioning scores, controlling for: WRAT-3, YMRS, HRSD, and age. Follow-up comparisons for ANOVAs were corrected using the Tukey criterion, while follow-up multiple comparisons for ANCOVA/MANCOVAs were corrected using the Least Significant Difference method. To assess the relationship between illness course and our variables of interest, we conducted correlations between age, age of onset, length of illness (computed by subtracting age of onset from age), and number of mood episodes with TAS and functioning variables. In order to evaluate whether alexithymia predicts functioning (both WHODAS and SAS total scores as dependent variables) in a BD sample, we conducted separate hierarchical linear regression models with demographic and clinical covariates (WRAT-3, YMRS, HRSD, age, psychosis history) in block 1, and the alexithymia variables (TAS-20) in block 2. In order to discover which factors best predict functioning (both WHODAS and SAS total scores as dependent variables) in our overall sample, we conducted separate hierarchical stepwise linear regressions with demographic and clinical covariates (WRAT-3, YMRS, HRSD, age, sex, race (i.e., Caucasian vs. non-Caucasian), diagnosis, psychosis history) in block 1, neurocognitive variables (MCCB domains, CVLT, and the composite neurocognitive score) in block 2, social cognitive variables (MSCEIT, RMET, and ERT) in block 3, and the alexithymia variables (TAS-20) in block 4. Neurocognitive variables were T-scored based on a normative sample, while the remaining variables represented raw scores. All statistical analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics 23 (IBM corp., Armonk, NY, USA). Alpha was set at 0.05; all analyses were two-tailed.
3. RESULTS
3.1. Sample Characteristics
Diagnostic group comparisons for demographics, clinical and alexithymia scores are presented in Table 1. BD, SZ and HC groups performed comparably on most demographics; however, HCs were significantly younger than SZs. Also, SZ patients completed fewer years of education compared to the BD and HC groups. Both BD and SZ groups reported higher depressive and manic symptoms and worse premorbid IQ compared to HCs. Regarding alexithymia, both BD and SZ groups had more difficulty in describing and identifying feelings, with no difference in externally-oriented thinking. Overall, both BD and SZ patients presented with greater alexithymia total scores compared to HCs (see Table 1).
Table 1.
Statistics | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
BD (n=56) | SZ (n=45) | HC (n=50) | F or χ2 | p | |
Diagnosis, n | |||||
BD I | 46 | - | - | ||
BD II | 10 | - | - | ||
Schizophrenia | - | 23 | - | ||
Schizoaffective | - | 22 | - | ||
Sex, n (%) | |||||
Males | 21 (37.5) | 24 (53.3) | 21 (42.0) | 2.63 | 0.27 |
Females | 35 (62.5) | 21 (46.7) | 29 (58.0) | ||
Race, n (%) | |||||
Caucasian | 21 (37.5) | 10 (22.2) | 14 (28.0) | 2.90 | 0.24 |
Non-Caucasian | 35 (62.5) | 35 (77.8) | 36 (72.0) | ||
Age, years | 38.89 (12.83) | 44.51 (12.40) | 38.06 (12.84) | 3.59 | 0.03 |
HC v. SZ; p=0.04 | |||||
Age of onset, years | 18.52 (6.72) | 21.04 (10.18) | - | 2.24 | 0.14 |
Education, years | 14.88 (2.86) | 13.47 (2.29) | 15.38 (1.91) | 7.96 | 0.001 |
BD v. SZ; p=0.01 | |||||
HC v. SZ; p<0.001 | |||||
Depressive symptoms: HRSD | 5.91 (5.96) | 6.53 (5.27) | 0.48 (1.22) | 25.06 | <0.001 |
HC v. SZ; p<0.001 | |||||
BD v. HC; p<0.001 | |||||
Manic symptoms: YMRS | 2.02 (3.50) | 2.29 (4.30) | 0.38 (1.26) | 4.98 | <0.01 |
HC v. SZ; p=0.01 | |||||
BD v. HC; p=0.003 | |||||
Premorbid IQ: WRAT-3 | 104.45 (12.51) | 98.24 (13.89) | 105.60 (12.73) | 4.35 | 0.02 |
BD v. SZ; p=0.05 | |||||
HC v. SZ; p=0.02 | |||||
Alexithymia | BD (n=56) | SZ (n=43) | HC (n=50) | F | p |
TAS-20 DIF | 16.61 (7.78) | 18.02 (6.71) | 9.48 (4.06) | 10.88 | <0.001 |
BD v. HC; p<0.001 | |||||
HC v. SZ; p<0.001 | |||||
TAS-20 DDF | 13.61 (4.93) | 14.41 (4.66) | 9.54 (4.36) | 5.35 | 0.006 |
BD v. HC; p=0.007 | |||||
HC v. SZ; p=0.003 | |||||
TAS-20 EOT | 18.32 (5.00) | 19.67 (4.86) | 16.38 (4.46) | 2.27 | 0.11 |
TAS-20 Total Score | 48.54 (13.86) | 52.12 (12.34) | 35.40 (10.42) | 10.26 | <0.001 |
BD v. HC; p<0.001 | |||||
HC v. SZ; p<0.001 |
Note: Data are given as mean (standard deviation).
BD, Bipolar Disorder I/II; SZ, Schizophrenia/Schizoaffective; HC, Healthy Control; HRSD, Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression; YMRS, Young Mania Rating Scale; WRAT-3, Wide Range Achievement Test-3rd Edition; TAS, Toronto Alexithymia Scale; DIF, Difficulty identifying feelings; DDF, Difficulty describing feelings; EOT, Externally-oriented thinking. Only significant comparisons are listed.
3.2. Diagnostic Comparisons of Cognition and Functioning Measures
Diagnostic group comparisons for cognitive and functioning scores are presented in Table 2. Overall, BD and SZ groups performed worse on most neurocognitive domains and the composite neurocognition score, compared to HCs. The SZ group scored significantly lower on the MSCEIT than the BD and HC groups; no other social cognitive measures differed between the diagnostic groups. Diagnostic group comparisons for both WHODAS and SAS total scores showed that both BD and SZ patients scored worse on the WHODAS (F(2, 138)=15.56, p<.001), and the SAS (F(2, 140)= 11.90, p<.001); there were no differences between BD and SZ groups for either functioning measure total scores. Given no difference in overall everyday and social functioning between BD and SZ groups, subsequent regression models using the overall sample (i.e., BD, SZ, and HC) included diagnosis as a dichotomous predictor variable (i.e., psychiatric case [BD/SZ] vs. control [HC] group).
Table 2.
Statistics | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
BD | SZ | HC | F | p | |
Neurocognition | n=53 | n=42 | n=49 | ||
Processing Speed | 44.47 (11.50) | 40.14 (7.93) | 49.51 (12.80) | 4.79 | 0.01 |
BD v. HC; p=0.05 | |||||
HC v. SZ; p=0.002 | |||||
Attention/Vigilance | 39.02 (12.08) | 39.05 (11.19) | 45.90 (11.56) | 5.18 | 0.007 |
BD v. HC; p=0.002 | |||||
HC v. SZ; p=0.03 | |||||
Working Memory | 40.77 (11.08) | 38.86 (12.20) | 45.37 (12.0) | 2.20 | 0.12 |
Visual Learning | 42.32 (11.79) | 39.71 (10.99) | 44.73 (14.08) | 1.34 | 0.27 |
Verbal Learning | 47.30 (12.79) | 40.58 (8.69) | 48.55 (11.24) | 4.88 | 0.01 |
BD v. SZ; p=0.01 | |||||
HC v. SZ; p=0.004 | |||||
Reasoning/Problem-Solving | 46.08 (11.39) | 40.76 (8.64) | 43.71 (9.39) | 2.44 | 0.09 |
Composite Score | 43.33 (8.66) | 39.85 (7.11) | 46.30 (8.43) | 4.29 | 0.02 |
HC v. SZ; p=0.004 | |||||
Social Cognition | n=53 | n=41 | n=50 | ||
MSCEITa | 46.48 (10.17) | 38.91 (11.93) | 49.22 (10.17) | 3.17 | 0.05 |
BD v. SZ; p=0.02 | |||||
HC v. SZ; p=0.03 | |||||
RMET | 25.68 (4.39) | 23.20 (5.41) | 25.50 (5.45) | 1.09 | 0.34 |
ERT (% correct) | |||||
Happy | 69.06 (19.24) | 66.34 (16.85) | 64.15 (19.22) | 0.06 | 0.94 |
Sad | 71.67 (18.50) | 67.76 (20.84) | 70.81 (20.0) | 0.15 | 0.86 |
Anger | 78.71 (17.79) | 70.81 (19.61) | 75.97 (22.12) | 0.41 | 0.66 |
Disgust | 62.94 (19.78) | 60.57 (23.84) | 63.13 (25.31) | 0.46 | 0.64 |
Fear | 50.26 (22.0) | 45.20 (21.13) | 49.24 (25.14) | 0.12 | 0.89 |
Surprise | 55.13 (14.02) | 53.47 (16.54) | 53.81 (16.08) | 0.77 | 0.46 |
Functioning | |||||
WHODAS | n=56 | n=41 | n=50 | ||
Communicating | 23.07 (17.84) | 24.80 (18.80) | 6.00 (10.14) | 10.36 | <0.001 BD v. HC; p<0.001 HC v. SZ; p<0.001 |
Getting Around | 18.57 (18.85) | 21.10 (22.18) | 4.10 (7.80) | 2.71 | 0.07 |
Self-Care | 12.28 (14.98) | 8.08 (12.67) | 0.63 (2.28) | 4.95 | 0.01 BD v. SZ; p=0.03 BD v. HC; p=0.004 |
Getting Along | 23.48 (20.00) | 25.61 (18.51) | 5.10 (8.42) | 8.04 | <0.001 BD v. HC; p=0.002 HC v. SZ; p<0.001 |
Life Activities-Home | 29.46 (26.29) | 16.62 (16.45) | 7.87 (13.53) | 8.96 | <0.001 BD v. SZ; p=0.002 BD v. HC; p<0.001 |
Participation in Society | 28.91 (17.64) | 24.54 (18.24) | 4.50 (7.99) | 13.94 | <0.001 BD v. HC; p<0.001 HC v. SZ; p=0.001 |
Overall Score | 22.63 (12.90) | 20.12 (12.00) | 4.70 (6.52) | 15.56 | <0.001 BD v. HC; p<0.001 HC v. SZ; p<0.001 |
SAS-SR | n=56 | n=43 | n=50 | ||
Social Leisure | 2.45 (0.77) | 2.86 (0.72) | 1.96 (0.51) | 8.02 | 0.001 BD v. SZ; p=0.004 HC v. SZ; p<0.001 |
Relationships-extended family | 1.97 (0.78) | 2.07 (0.61) | 1.46 (0.46) | 4.73 | 0.01 BD v. HC; p=0.02 HC v. SZ; p=0.004 |
Total Score | 2.21 (0.59) | 2.38 (0.51) | 1.64 (0.35) | 11.90 | <0.001 BD v. HC; p<0.001 HC v. SZ; p<0.001 |
Note: Data are given as mean (standard deviation).
BD, Bipolar Disorder I/II; SZ, Schizophrenia/Schizoaffective; HC, Healthy Control; MSCEIT, Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test; RMET, Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test; ERT, Emotion Recognition Test; WHODAS, World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule 2.0; SAS-SR, Social Adjustment Scale-Self Report.
BD (n=54), SZ (n=45), HC (n=50). Only significant comparisons are listed.
3.3. Relationships between illness course, alexithymia and functioning
Correlations between age, age of onset, illness length in years, total number of mood episodes (for BD patients only), alexithymia, and functioning variables are presented in Table 3. In our BD sample, age negatively correlated with TAS items of difficulty identifying and describing feelings, while age of onset negatively correlated with the TAS domain of externally-oriented thinking and the total TAS score. Also, age of onset correlated negatively with social functioning, specifically, relationships with extended family and the SAS total score. In the SZ group, only participant age correlated with everyday functioning, specifically activities in the home; no other illness course factors associated with alexithymia or functioning.
Table 3.
BD (n = 56) |
SZ (n = 45) |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | Age of Onset | Illness Length | Total # of Mood Episodes | Age | Age of Onset | Illness Length | |
TAS: DIF | −0.27* | −0.26 | −0.12 | 0.31 | 0.01 | 0.07 | −0.05 |
TAS: DDF | −0.28* | −0.17 | −0.18 | 0.26 | 0.26 | −0.08 | 0.29 |
TAS: EOT | 0.12 | −0.26* | 0.25 | 0.14 | −0.12 | 0.00 | −0.11 |
TAS: Total Score | −0.20 | −0.30* | −0.04 | 0.32 | 0.05 | 0.01 | 0.04 |
WHODAS | |||||||
Communicating | −0.26 | 0.13 | −0.32* | 0.05 | −0.07 | 0.05 | −0.10 |
Getting Around | 0.20 | 0.14 | 0.12 | −0.05 | 0.19 | 0.04 | 0.14 |
Self-Care | −0.11 | 0.01 | −0.12 | 0.09 | 0.12 | 0.04 | 0.08 |
Getting Along | −0.23 | −0.14 | −0.15 | −0.05 | 0.19 | 0.08 | 0.12 |
Life Activities-Home | 0.06 | 0.08 | 0.01 | −0.12 | 0.30* | 0.26 | 0.08 |
Participation in Society | −0.02 | −0.08 | 0.02 | 0.32 | 0.07 | 0.09 | −0.00 |
Overall Score | −0.08 | 0.04 | −0.10 | 0.04 | 0.26 | 0.11 | 0.16 |
SAS-SR | |||||||
Social Leisure | −0.14 | −0.11 | −0.08 | −0.04 | −0.08 | 0.29 | −0.29 |
Relationships-extended family | −0.24 | −0.33* | −0.07 | 0.12 | −0.12 | 0.02 | −0.12 |
BD, Bipolar Disorder I/II; SZ, Schizophrenia/Schizoaffective; TAS, Toronto Alexithymia Scale; DIF, Difficulty identifying feelings; DDF, Difficulty describing feelings; EOT, Externally-oriented thinking; WHODAS, World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule 2.0; SAS-SR, Social Adjustment Scale-Self Report
p<.05;
p<0.01;
p<0.001.
3.4. Regression Models Predicting Functioning
3.4.1. Regression predicting functioning in BD only
The hierarchical linear regression evaluating whether alexithymic factors predict everyday functioning (i.e., WHODAS) scores in our BD group yielded a significant model accounting for 46% of the variance (F(8, 55)=4.90, p<0.001). However, no alexithymia domains significantly predicted everyday functioning, with only externally-oriented thinking achieving trend-level significance (β=0.21, p=0.09). Significant covariates in this model included only depressive symptomatology (β=0.39, p=0.004). Results for the hierarchical linear regression evaluating whether alexithymia domains predict social functioning (i.e., SAS) in our BD group revealed an overall significant model accounting for 42.5% of the variance (F(8, 55)=4.34, p=0.001). Significant alexithymic predictors included the ability to describe feelings ((β=0.53, p=0.002) and externally-oriented thinking ((β=0.26, p=0.04). The only significant covariate in this model included premorbid IQ (β=0.36, p=0.01).
3.4.2. Regression predicting functioning in the overall sample
Hierarchical stepwise regressions investigating predictive factors of everyday and social functioning scores in our overall sample are shown in Table 4. For the model predicting global everyday functioning (i.e., WHODAS), significant clinical predictors included depressive symptomatology and diagnosis. Among neurocognitive domains, processing speed significantly predicted everyday functioning; for social cognitive variables, only the % correct of happy faces on the ERT predicted everyday functioning. Finally, the alexithymia dimensions measuring difficulty identifying feelings and difficulty describing feelings both predicted WHODAS scores; this model was significant, accounting for 69% of the variance (F(19, 127)=12.34, p<0.001). For the model predicting overall social functioning (i.e., SAS) in our total sample, significant clinical predictors included depressive symptoms and diagnosis. Working memory was the only significant neurocognitive predictor of social functioning, while none of the social cognitive measures added significantly to the model. Finally, the alexithymia dimension indexing difficulty describing feelings predicted the SAS total score; this model was significant, accounting for 52% of the variance (F(19, 131)=6.34, p<0.001).
Table 4.
WHODAS Total Score | SAS-SR Total Score | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
N=128 | N=132 | |||
Predictors | B (SE) | β (p) | B (SE) | β (p) |
HRSD | 1.11 (0.19) | 0.41 (<0.001) | 0.03 (0.01) | 0.30 (0.001) |
Diagnosis | −6.41 (2.06) | −0.23 (0.002) | −0.33 (0.11) | −0.28 (0.002) |
Processing Speed | 0.18 (0.09) | 0.16 (0.04) | 0.00 (0.01) | 0.05 (0.56) |
Attention/Vigilance | 0.04 (0.09) | 0.04 (0.62) | 0.00 (0.01) | 0.07 (0.44) |
Working Memory | −0.12 (1.00) | −1.04 (0.24) | 0.01 (0.01) | 0.20 (0.05) |
Visual Learning | −0.05 (0.08) | −0.05 (0.51) | −0.01 (0.00) | −0.14 (0.13) |
Verbal Learning | −0.08 (0.08) | −0.07 (0.34) | −0.01 (0.00) | −0.10 (0.27) |
Reasoning/Problem-Solving | 0.02 (0.09) | 0.12 (0.86) | −0.00 (0.01) | −0.03 (0.76) |
MSCEIT | 0.09 (0.09) | 0.07 (0.98) | −0.00 (0.01) | −0.03 (0.76) |
RMET | 0.20 (0.20) | 0.09 (0.23) | 0.01 (0.01) | 0.09 (0.32) |
ERT% Happy | 0.12 (0.05) | 0.16 (0.02) | 0.00 (0.00) | 0.02 (0.76) |
ERT% Sad | 0.02 (0.05) | 0.02 (0.74) | 0.00 (0.00) | 0.07 (0.37) |
ERT% Anger | −0.09 (0.06) | −0.12 (0.12) | −0.00 (0.00) | −0.06 (0.53) |
ERT% Disgust | 0.00 (0.05) | 0.00 (0.94) | −0.00 (0.00) | −0.12 (0.20) |
ERT% Fear | 0.01 (0.04) | 0.02 (0.79) | −0.00 (0.00) | −0.06 (0.49) |
ERT% Surprise | −0.06 (0.07) | −0.07 (0.38) | 0.00 (0.00) | 0.09 (0.34) |
TAS-20 DIF | 0.35 (0.17) | 0.19 (0.04) | −0.00 (0.01) | −0.03 (0.80) |
TAS-20 DDF | 0.53 (0.24) | 0.20 (0.03) | 0.03 (0.01) | 0.26 (0.02) |
TAS-20 EOT | 0.14 (0.18) | 0.05 (0.43) | 0.02 (0.01) | 0.13 (0.10) |
F | 12 34*** | 6.40*** |
WHODAS, World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule 2.0; SAS-SR, Social Adjustment Scale-Self Report; HRSD, Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression; MSCEIT, Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotion Intelligence Test; RMET, Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test; ERT, Emotion Recognition Test; TAS-20, Toronto Alexithymia Scale; DIF, Difficulty identifying feelings; DDF, Difficulty describing feelings; EOT, Externally-oriented thinking; R2, Variance.
p<.05;
p<0.01;
p<0.001.
4. CONCLUSION
The present study is the first to investigate differences in alexithymia between BD, SZ, and HC groups, as well as examine the predictive ability of alexithymia on functioning in BD. Additionally, we aimed to identify demographic, clinical, neurocognitive and social cognitive (including alexithymia) predictors of everyday and social functioning for all diagnostic groups simultaneously. Our diagnostic groups were comparable on most demographics, with BD and SZ groups presenting with higher depressive and manic symptoms and worse premorbid IQ compared to HCs. We also found that BD and SZ groups scored higher on alexithymia, particularly describing and identifying feelings, as well as the total alexithymia score, compared to HCs. Further, age and age of onset correlated with aspects of alexithymia and social functioning in the BD group only. Difficulty in describing feelings and externally-oriented thinking were found to predict social functioning only in our BD group. Finally, hierarchical stepwise regression models predicting everyday and social functioning suggest that certain alexithymia domains, specifically difficulty describing feelings, independently contributed to prediction models of functioning (in addition to diagnostic status and depressive symptomatology), with more severe alexithymia predicting a lower level of functioning.
Diagnostic comparisons of neurocognitive domains generally demonstrated comparable performance between SZ and BD groups, who both performed worse on domains of processing speed, attention/vigilance, verbal learning and the overall neurocognitive composite score, compared to HCs. However, evaluation of group means demonstrated slightly better performance in the BD group compared to the SZ group on these domains, which has been found previously (Altschuler et al., 2004; Daban et al., 2006); limited sample sizes may offer one explanation for not achieving statistical significance on these comparisons. Regarding social cognition, the SZ group performed worse on the emotion regulation task (i.e., MSCEIT) compared to BD and HC groups, a result which is supported by prior work (Lee et al., 2013). Current results showed comparable performance on social cognition between BD and HC groups, as has been previously evidenced (Lee et al., 2013). Interestingly, the SZ group performed as well as the BD and HC groups on the theory of mind and emotion recognition tasks. However, reviewing group means demonstrated lower performance for the SZ group on these measures; possible explanations for this result include lack of statistical power, as well as the inclusion of age, IQ, and clinical symptomatology as covariates, which have been shown to moderate impairment in both emotion recognition (Kohler et al., 2009) and theory of mind (Bora, Yucel, and Pantelis, 2009). Additionally, comparable neuro- and social cognitive performance between SZ and BD groups may be explained by the presence of cognitive heterogeneity within these patient samples. Recent studies propose that BD may be characterized by several cognitive subgroups, with some patients demonstrating intact neurocognition (similar to HCs), some patients with impairments on select neurocognitive domains, and other patients with severe impairment in most domains (similar to SZ patients) (Burdick et al., 2014). Further research also suggests social cognitive (Ospina et al., 2018) and functional (Solé et al. 2018) heterogeneity in BD. Likewise, there is some evidence of cognitive heterogeneity in SZ as well (Joyce et al., 2005). It is possible that either our BD sample is primarily comprised of lower-functioning individuals or that the SZ patients are higher functioning than is typically seen in other samples, rendering these groups indistinguishable from one another. Overall, our results are generally in line with previous research, indicating convergent evidence of neurocognitive and emotion regulation deficits as well as impaired functioning in BD and SZ, relative to HCs.
Previous studies have consistently shown greater alexithymia for BD and SZ patients, compared to HCs (Herold et al., 2017; Yilmaz et al., 2016; Cedro et al., 2001; van’t Wout et al., 2007). Also, studies comparing psychotic versus non-psychotic disorders revealed no difference in alexithymia (Heshmati et al., 2010; Karayağiz et al., 2016; Picardi et al., 2012). The current results support these past studies, with BD and SZ groups performing comparably on all alexithymia domains. This finding suggests that alexithymia may be a non-specific characteristic of certain mental illnesses, particularly psychiatric disorders distinguished by deficits in cognitive processing and emotion regulation. Alternatively, these findings may also imply that certain social cognitive impairments, such as unawareness of one’s own affective state, may be equally affected across specific mental illnesses. Interestingly, alexithymia scores remain stable even in remission for both mood and psychosis disorders (Picardi et al., 2012), suggesting that alexithymia may be characterized as a constant, personality trait (Martinez-Sanchez et al., 2003). One theoretical perspective classifies alexithymia into two subtypes, with type I characterized by the absence of emotional experience (including the experience’s associated cognitive appraisal) and type II characterized by a selective deficit of emotional cognition with sparing of emotional experience (Bermond, 1995). Considering cognitive-emotional deficits specific to BD and SZ as well as their similar alexithymia scores, the TAS may in fact be assessing both subtypes of alexithymia, with subtype I most commonly found in SZ and subtype II in BD. Therefore, development of a subtype-specific alexithymia assessment may potentially yield differential scores between BD and SZ populations.
Past studies have found that alexithymia predicts functioning, beyond what is contributed by other factors such as neurocognition and other aspects of social cognition, particularly in SZ (Kimhy et al., 2012; Kimhy et al., 2015). In BD, no studies to date have assessed the predictive ability of alexithymia on functioning (although alexithymia has been shown to predict worse quality of life in BD [Karayağiz et al., 2016]). Our results generally support this finding; within our BD sample, the alexithymic domains including difficulty describing feelings and externally-oriented thinking predicted social functioning; however, alexithymia was not observed to predict everyday functioning in this same group. Within our entire sample (i.e., BD, SZ and HC combined), difficulty describing feelings remained as the only significant alexithymia factor that predicted both everyday and social functioning. This would suggest that this emotional awareness deficit plays a significant role in predicting functional status among both psychiatric and non-psychiatric populations. In addition to psychiatric illness, alexithymia has also been observed to predict socio-emotional functioning in non-clinical populations (Ciarrochi et al., 2008; Mattila et al., 2009), highlighting the importance of developing therapeutic interventions targeting improvements in emotional awareness. Common neuroanatomical substrates have been found in relation to alexithymia, specifically the medial prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate, for BD (Herold et al., 2017) and SZ (Harrison et al., 2007) patients, as well as HCs (Moriguchi et al., 2006). As such, alexithymia assessment may generally reflect deficits in these brain regions, which are critical for cognitive and affective processing. Also, alexithymia has been demonstrated to associate with specific emotion regulation strategies, such as suppression, which represents an attempt to inhibit emotion-expressive behavior (Gross, 1998). Suppression is common in psychiatric disorders such as SZ (van der Meer et al., 2009) and BD (Gruber, Harvey and Gross, 2012), which consequently has been associated with poorer social functioning (Kimhy et al., 2012). Subsequent research, therefore, is necessary to understand the potential mechanisms linking alexithymia, emotion regulation and overall functioning in SZ and BD populations. Finally, additional predictors of social and everyday functioning included diagnosis and depressive symptoms, consistent with prior work (Bonnin et al., 2010; Tabarés-Seisdedos et al., 2008).
The limitations of the current study include: the use of a cross-sectional design, which limits the ability to make causal inferences between the variables of interest and a relatively limited sample, which may affect generalizability of results. While we assessed some aspects of functioning using two commonly used measures, additional functioning tests measuring other aspects such as adaptive functioning would be useful to gain a more comprehensive understanding of the relationship between neurocognition and social cognition on functioning status. Also, while our HC group exhibits lower neurocognition (MCCB) scores compared to the normative sample, our HC group is better matched to our psychiatric groups on demographic characteristics (e.g. race) and as such, a more appropriate comparator group. We did not specifically assess for the presence of personality disorders in our participants. Alexithymia may be a typical feature of personality disorders (Grabe et al., 2004); additionally, BD patients comorbid with personality disorders may present with worse functional outcomes than those with BD alone (Dunayevich et al., 2000). Therefore, assessing for comorbid personality disorders may better aid in understanding the predictive relationships between alexithymia and functioning in BD. Finally, our functioning and alexithymia scales were self-reports; it is possible that poor scores for these measures relate to negative attitudes about the self or reflect other aspects of psychopathology. Relatedly, functioning measures were assessed “in the laboratory” and so may have limited applicability to the patient’s “real-world” functional status. Future studies would benefit from including objective, informant-based measures, real-world measures, as well as assessing patient self-attitudes.
Our results help further our understanding of functional status in psychiatric populations. While most social cognitive research has consistently focused on specific subdomains, such as theory of mind and emotion recognition, the current study emphasizes the importance of considering alexithymia as an additional factor in determining functional status. Assessing emotional awareness, specifically difficulty in describing feelings, may serve as a promising avenue in investigating social cognition and functioning in mood and psychotic disorders, as well as non-clinical populations. Also, developing treatment strategies such as psychoeducational approaches specifically targeting alexithymia may be useful in promoting recovery and improved functional outcome.
Highlights.
Bipolar and schizophrenia groups scored lower on cognition compared to controls.
Bipolar and schizophrenia groups scored lower on functioning compared to controls.
Bipolar and schizophrenia groups scored higher on alexithymia compared to controls.
Difficulties in identifying and describing feelings predicted everyday functioning.
Difficulty in describing feelings predicted social functioning.
Acknowledgements:
This study was funded by Grants from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) to KEB (R01 MH 100,125; R34 MH101267; R01 MH102257) and the Veterans Administration (VA) Health system to KEB (I01CH000995).
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Conflict of Interest: Dr. Burdick has served as an advisory board member for Dainippon Sumitomo Pharmaceutical, Otsuks Pharmaceuticals, Neuralstem, and Takeda Lundbeck, which had no impact upon the work presented in this manuscript. All other authors report no competing interests regarding the present study.
References
- Altshuler LL, Ventura J, van Gorp WG, Green MF, Theberge DC, & Mintz J (2004). Neurocognitive function in clinically stable men with bipolar I disorder or schizophrenia and normal control subjects. Biological psychiatry, 56(8), 560–569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnon-Ribenfeld N, Hasson-Ohayon I, Lavidor M, Atzil-Slonim D, & Lysaker PH (2017). The association between metacognitive abilities and outcome measures among people with schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. European Psychiatry, 46, 33–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Babaei S, Varandi SR, Hatami Z, & Gharechahi M (2016). Metacognition beliefs and general health in predicting alexithymia in students. Global journal of health science, 8(2), 117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bagby RM, Taylor GJ, & Parker JD (1994). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale—II. Convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity. Journal of psychosomatic research, 38(1), 33–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, & Plumb I (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42(2), 241–251. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bermond B (1995). Alexithymie, een neuropsychologische benadering. Tijdschrift voor Psychiatrie, 37, 717–727. [Google Scholar]
- Bonnin CM, Martínez-Arán A, Torrent C, Pacchiarotti I, Rosa AR, Franco C, … & Vieta E (2010). Clinical and neurocognitive predictors of functional outcome in bipolar euthymic patients: a long-term, follow-up study. Journal of affective disorders, 121(1), 156–160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bora E, Bartholomeusz C, & Pantelis C (2016). Meta-analysis of Theory of Mind (ToM) impairment in bipolar disorder. Psychological medicine, 46(2), 253–264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bora E, Yucel M, & Pantelis C (2009). Theory of mind impairment in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Schizophrenia research, 109(1–3), 1–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowie CR, Depp C, McGrath JA, Wolyniec P, Mausbach BT, Thornquist MH, … & Pulver AE (2010). Prediction of real-world functional disability in chronic mental disorders: a comparison of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(9), 1116–1124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burdick KE, Russo M, Frangou S, Mahon K, Braga RJ, Shanahan M, & Malhotra AK (2014). Empirical evidence for discrete neurocognitive subgroups in bipolar disorder: clinical implications. Psychological medicine, 44(14), 3083–3096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burdick KE, Goldberg TE, Cornblatt BA, Keefe RS, Gopin CB, DeRosse P, … & Malhotra AK (2011). The MATRICS consensus cognitive battery in patients with bipolar I disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(8), 1587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burdick KE, Goldberg JF, & Harrow M (2010). Neurocognitive dysfunction and psychosocial outcome in patients with bipolar I disorder at 15‐year follow‐up. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 122(6), 499–506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burdick KE, Braga RJ, Goldberg JF, & Malhotra AK (2007). Cognitive dysfunction in bipolar disorder. CNS drugs, 21(12), 971–981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cedro A, Kokoszka A, Popiel A, & Narkiewicz-Jodko W (2001). Alexithymia in schizophrenia: an exploratory study. Psychological reports, 89(1), 95–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ciarrochi J, Heaven PC, & Supavadeeprasit S (2008). The link between emotion identification skills and socio-emotional functioning in early adolescence: a 1-year longitudinal study. Journal of Adolescence, 31(5), 565–582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Couture SM, Penn DL, & Roberts DL (2006). The functional significance of social cognition in schizophrenia: a review. Schizophrenia bulletin, 32(suppl_1), S44–S63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daban C, Martinez-Aran A, Torrent C, Tabarés-Seisdedos R, Balanzá-Martínez V, Salazar-Fraile J, … & Vieta E (2006). Specificity of cognitive deficits in bipolar disorder versus schizophrenia. Psychotherapy and psychosomatics, 75(2), 72–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dimaggio G, Vanheule S, Lysaker PH, Carcione A, & Nicolò G (2009). Impaired self-reflection in psychiatric disorders among adults: a proposal for the existence of a network of semi independent functions. Consciousness and cognition, 18(3), 653–664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balanzá-Martínez V, Tabarés-Seisdedos R, Selva-Vera G, Martínez-Arán A, Torrent C, Salazar-Fraile J, … & Gómez-Beneyto M (2005). Persistent cognitive dysfunctions in bipolar I disorder and schizophrenic patients: a 3-year follow-up study. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 74(2), 113–119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunayevich E, Sax KW, Keck JP, McElroy SL, Sorter MT, McConville BJ, & Strakowski SM (2000). Twelve-month outcome in bipolar patients with and without personality disorders. The Journal of clinical psychiatry, 61(2), 134–139. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, & Williams JBW (2002). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-TR axis I disorders, research version, patient edition. (SCID-I/P). New York: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute. [Google Scholar]
- Grabe HJ, Spitzer C, & Freyberger HJ (2004). The relationship between alexithymia, personality and psychopathology. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 1299–1301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gross JJ (1998). Antecedent-and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of personality and social psychology, 74(1), 224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gruber J, Harvey AG, & Gross JJ (2012). When trying is not enough: Emotion regulation and the effort–success gap in bipolar disorder. Emotion, 12(5), 997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton M (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry, 23(1), 56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrison BJ, Yücel M, Pujol J, & Pantelis C (2007). Task-induced deactivation of midline cortical regions in schizophrenia assessed with fMRI. Schizophrenia research, 91(1), 82–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herold D, Usnich T, Spengler S, Sajonz B, Bauer M, & Bermpohl F (2017). Decreased medial prefrontal cortex activation during self-referential processing in bipolar mania. Journal of Affective Disorders, 219, 157–163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heshmati R, Jafari E, Hoseinifar J, & Ahmadi M (2010). Comparative study of alexithymia in patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders, non-psychotic disorders and normal people. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 5, 1084–1089. [Google Scholar]
- Huxley N, & Baldessarini RJ (2007). Disability and its treatment in bipolar disorder patients. Bipolar disorders, 9(1–2), 183–196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joyce EM, Hutton SB, Mutsatsa SH, & Barnes TR (2005). Cognitive heterogeneity in first-episode schizophrenia. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 187(6), 516–522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karayağız Ş, & Baştürk M (2016). Alexithymia levels in patients with unipolar and bipolar depression and the effect of alexithymia on both severity of depression symptoms and quality of life. Anatolian Journal of Psychiatry, 17(5), 362–368. [Google Scholar]
- Keefe RS, Bilder RM, Harvey PD, Davis SM, Palmer BW, Gold JM, … & Adler LW (2006). Baseline neurocognitive deficits in the CATIE schizophrenia trial. Neuropsychopharmacology, 31(9), 2033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimhy D, Gill KE, Brucato G, Vakhrusheva J, Arndt L, Gross JJ, & Girgis RR (2016). The impact of emotion awareness and regulation on social functioning in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis. Psychological medicine, 46(14), 2907–2918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimhy D, Vakhrusheva J, Jobson-Ahmed L, Tarrier N, Malaspina D, & Gross JJ (2012). Emotion awareness and regulation in individuals with schizophrenia: implications for social functioning. Psychiatry research, 200(2), 193–201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohler CG, Walker JB, Martin EA, Healey KM, & Moberg PJ (2009). Facial emotion perception in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Schizophrenia bulletin, 36(5), 1009–1019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lahera G, Benito A, Montes JM, Fernandez-Liria A, Olbert CM, & Penn DL (2013). Social cognition and interaction training (SCIT) for outpatients with bipolar disorder. Journal of affective disorders, 146(1), 132–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee J, Altshuler L, Glahn DC, Miklowitz DJ, Ochsner K, & Green MF (2013). Social and nonsocial cognition in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: relative levels of impairment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(3), 334–341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lysaker PH, Shea AM, Buck KD, Dimaggio G, Nicolò G, Procacci M, … & Rand KL (2010). Metacognition as a mediator of the effects of impairments in neurocognition on social function in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 122(5), 405–413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacQueen GM, Young LT, & Joffe RT (2001). A review of psychosocial outcome in patients with bipolar disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 103(3), 163–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez-Aran A, Vieta E, Torrent C, Sanchez-Moreno J, Goikolea JM, Salamero M, … & Fountoulakis K (2007). Functional outcome in bipolar disorder: the role of clinical and cognitive factors. Bipolar disorders, 9(1–2), 103–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martínez‐Arán A, Vieta E, Colom F, Torrent C, Sánchez‐Moreno J, Reinares M, … & Salamero M (2004). Cognitive impairment in euthymic bipolar patients: implications for clinical and functional outcome. Bipolar disorders, 6(3), 224–232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez-Aran A, Penades R, Vieta E, Colom F, Reinares M, Benabarre A, Salamero M & Gasto C (2002). Executive function in patients with remitted bipolar disorder and schizophrenia and its relationship with functional outcome. Psychotherapy and psychosomatics, 71(1), 39–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martínez-Sánchez F, Ato-García M, & Ortiz-Soria B (2003). Alexithymia—state or trait?. The Spanish journal of psychology, 6(1), 51–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martino DJ, Marengo E, Igoa A, Scápola M, Ais ED, Perinot L, & Strejilevich SA (2009). Neurocognitive and symptomatic predictors of functional outcome in bipolar disorders: a prospective 1 year follow-up study. Journal of affective disorders, 116(1), 37–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mattila AK, Saarni SI, Salminen JK, Huhtala H, Sintonen H, & Joukamaa M (2009). Alexithymia and health-related quality of life in a general population. Psychosomatics, 50(1), 59–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehta UM, Thirthalli J, Subbakrishna DK, Gangadhar BN, Eack SM, & Keshavan MS (2013). Social and neuro-cognition as distinct cognitive factors in schizophrenia: a systematic review. Schizophrenia research, 148(1–3), 3–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moriguchi Y, Ohnishi T, Lane RD, Maeda M, Mori T, Nemoto K, … & Komaki G (2006). Impaired self-awareness and theory of mind: an fMRI study of mentalizing in alexithymia. Neuroimage, 32(3), 1472–1482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nuechterlein KH, & Green MF (2006). MATRICS consensus cognitive battery manual. Los Angeles, CA: MATRICS Assessment Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Ormel J, Petukhova M, Chatterji S, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, Alonso J, Angermeyer MC, … & Haro JM (2008). Disability and treatment of specific mental and physical disorders across the world. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 192(5), 368–375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ospina LH, Nitzburg GC, Shanahan M, Perez-Rodriguez MM, Larsen E, Latifoglu A, & Burdick KE (2018). Social cognition moderates the relationship between neurocognition and community functioning in bipolar disorder. Journal of affective disorders, 235, 7–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Picardi A, Caroppo E, Porcelli P, Di Maria G, Munittola G, & Martinotti G (2012). Alexithymia and severe psychopathology: a study on psychiatric inpatients. Psychopathology, 45(3), 159–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robbins TW, James M, Owen AM, Sahakian BJ, McInnes L, & Rabbitt P (1994). Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB): a factor analytic study of a large sample of normal elderly volunteers. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 5(5), 266–281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosa AR, Reinares M, Franco C, Comes M, Torrent C, Sánchez-Moreno J, … & Vieta E (2009). Clinical predictors of functional outcome of bipolar patients in remission. Bipolar Disorders, 11(4), 401–409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samamé C, Martino DJ, & Strejilevich SA (2012). Social cognition in euthymic bipolar disorder: systematic review and meta-analytic approach. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 125(4), 266–280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanchez-Moreno J, Martinez-Aran A, Tabares-Seisdedos R, Torrent C, Vieta E, & Ayuso-Mateos JL (2009). Functioning and disability in bipolar disorder: an extensive review. Psychotherapy and psychosomatics, 78(5), 285–297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt SJ, Mueller DR, & Roder V (2011). Social cognition as a mediator variable between neurocognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: empirical review and new results by structural equation modeling. Schizophrenia bulletin, 37(suppl_2), S41–S54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sifneos PE (1972). Short-term psychotherapy and emotional crisis. Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Solé B, Bonnin CM, Jiménez E, Torrent C, Torres I, Varo C, … & Vieta E (2018). Heterogeneity of functional outcomes in patients with bipolar disorder: a cluster-analytic approach. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 137(6), 516–527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tabarés-Seisdedos R, Balanzá-Martínez V, Sánchez-Moreno J, Martinez-Aran A, Salazar-Fraile J, Selva-Vera G, Rubio C, Mata I, Gómez-Beneyto M & Vieta E (2008). Neurocognitive and clinical predictors of functional outcome in patients with schizophrenia and bipolar I disorder at one-year follow-up. Journal of affective disorders, 109(3), 286–299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor GJ, Bagby RM, & Parker JD (1999). Disorders of affect regulation: Alexithymia in medical and psychiatric illness. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Taylor GJ (1984). Alexithymia: concept, measurement, and implications for treatment. The American Journal of Psychiatry. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thaler NS, Sutton GP, & Allen DN (2014). Social cognition and functional capacity in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Psychiatry research, 220(1), 309–314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Üstün TB (Ed.). (2010). Measuring health and disability: Manual for WHO disability assessment schedule WHODAS 2.0. World Health Organization. [Google Scholar]
- van der Meer L, van’t Wout M, & Aleman A (2009). Emotion regulation strategies in patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry research, 170(2–3), 108–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van’t Wout M, Aleman A, Bermond B, & Kahn RS (2007). No words for feelings: alexithymia in schizophrenia patients and first-degree relatives. Comprehensive psychiatry, 48(1), 27–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weissman MM (1999). Social Adjustment Scale-self Report (SAS-SR): User’s Manual. Multi-Health Systems. [Google Scholar]
- Wilkinson GS (1993). WRAT-3: Wide range achievement test administration manual. Wide Range, Incorporated. [Google Scholar]
- Yatham LN, Torres IJ, Malhi GS, Frangou S, Glahn DC, Bearden CE, … & Ozerdem A (2010). The International Society for Bipolar Disorders–Battery for Assessment of Neurocognition (ISBD-BANC). Bipolar disorders, 12(4), 351–363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yilmaz O, Ates MA, Semiz UB, Tutuncu R, Bez Y, Algul A, … & Cetin M (2016). Childhood traumas in patients with bipolar disorder: association with alexithymia and dissociative experiences. ANADOLU PSIKIYATRI DERGISI-ANATOLIAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHIATRY, 17(3), 188–195. [Google Scholar]
- Young RC, Biggs JT, Ziegler VE, & Meyer DA (1978). A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 133(5), 429–435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]