Abstract
Dopamine is hypothesized to convey error information in reinforcement learning tasks with explicit appetitive or aversive cues. However, during motor skill learning feedback signals arise from an animal’s evaluation of sensory feedback resulting from its own behavior, rather than any external reward or punishment. It has previously been shown that intact dopaminergic signaling from the ventral tegmental area/substantia nigra pars compacta (VTA/SNc) complex is necessary for vocal learning when songbirds modify their vocalizations to avoid hearing distorted auditory feedback (playbacks of white noise). However, it remains unclear whether dopaminergic signaling underlies vocal learning in response to more naturalistic errors (pitch-shifted feedback delivered via headphones). We used male Bengalese finches (Lonchura striata var. domestica) to test the hypothesis that the necessity of dopamine signaling is shared between the two types of learning. We combined 6-hydroxydopamine (6-OHDA) lesions of dopaminergic terminals within Area X, a basal ganglia nucleus critical for song learning, with a headphones learning paradigm that shifted the pitch of auditory feedback and compared their learning to that of unlesioned controls. We found that 6-OHDA lesions affected song behavior in two ways. First, over a period of days lesioned birds systematically lowered their pitch regardless of the presence or absence of auditory errors. Second, 6-OHDA lesioned birds also displayed severe deficits in sensorimotor learning in response to pitch-shifted feedback. Our results suggest roles for dopamine in both motor production and auditory error processing, and a shared mechanism underlying vocal learning in response to both distorted and pitch-shifted auditory feedback.
Keywords: basal ganglia, Bengalese finch, dopamine, sensorimotor adaptation, songbird, vocal learning
Significance Statement
Dopamine has been hypothesized to convey a reward prediction error signal in learning tasks involving external reinforcement. However, the role dopamine plays in tasks involving self-guided error correction in the absence of external reinforcement is much less clear. To address this question, we studied the role of dopamine in sensorimotor adaptation using male Bengalese finches, which spontaneously produce a complex motor behavior (song) and are capable of modulating their behavioral output in response to induced auditory errors. Our results reveal that in addition to conveying errors in motor performance, dopamine may also have a role in modulating effort and in choosing a corrective response to the auditory error.
Introduction
Complex organisms perform sensorimotor learning to modulate behavior in response to sensory feedback. This process uses feedback from past performances arising from either explicit reward/punishment cues (e.g., food reward, electric shocks) or from self-evaluation of the performance (e.g., hearing one’s own voice during speech or song). While prior work has taken a number of approaches to taxonomizing different forms sensorimotor learning, including distinguishing model-based and model-free learning (Wolpert et al., 1995; Mohan et al., 2011; Haith and Krakauer, 2013) and habitual versus goal-directed behavior (Balleine and O'doherty, 2010; Redgrave et al., 2010), here we focus on an orthogonal distinction into two broad components: error-based learning that relies on self-evaluation and reinforcement learning that relies on cues from the environment (Wolpert et al., 2011). Classic studies have linked dopamine to reinforcement learning as a reward prediction error signal that conveys information about explicit rewards and punishments (Schultz et al., 1997; Glimcher, 2011). However, the question of whether dopamine is also involved in error-based learning in the absence of external rewarding or aversive cues has been harder to address. Some studies have reported deficits in error-based learning in patients with Parkinson’s disease (Paquet et al., 2008; Mollaei et al., 2013), but since Parkinson’s disease is associated with cognitive and executive deficits in addition to larger motor deficits (Lees and Smith, 1983; Cooper et al., 1991; Dubois and Pillon, 1996; Jankovic, 2008), the specific role of dopamine has been difficult to isolate.
Songbirds have emerged as an effective model system in which to study the role of dopamine in sensorimotor learning. Songbirds spontaneously produce songs hundreds of times per day. Like human speech, song is learned during development (Wilbrecht and Nottebohm, 2003; Lipkind et al., 2013) and actively maintained by auditory feedback through adulthood (Sakata and Brainard, 2006, 2008; Sober and Brainard, 2009; Kuebrich and Sober, 2015). Additionally, songbirds have a well-defined neural circuitry dedicated to song production and song learning (Sohrabji et al., 1990; Scharff and Nottebohm, 1991; Brainard and Doupe, 2000). Dopaminergic neurons from the ventral tegmental area/substantia nigra pars compacta (VTA/SNc) complex innervate Area X, a basal ganglia nucleus essential for song learning, and have been hypothesized as a way for auditory error information to enter the song system (Bottjer, 1993; Soha et al., 1996; Mandelblat-Cerf et al., 2014; Peh et al., 2015; Fig. 1). Researchers examining vocal control employ two primary methods to induce song learning in adult songbirds: through distorted auditory feedback (Tumer and Brainard, 2007) and through pitch shifts played through custom-made headphones (Sober and Brainard, 2009). It remains unclear to what extent the two paradigms share underlying neural mechanisms. Dopamine has been shown to be involved in changing the pitch of the song in response to distorted auditory feedback. Specifically, birds display deficits in learning to avoid distorted feedback under dopamine depleted conditions (Hoffmann et al., 2016; Hisey et al., 2018), neural recordings of dopaminergic neurons revealed prediction error type responses when birds were required to avoid such auditory distortions while singing (Gadagkar et al., 2016), and pitch-contingent optical stimulation of dopaminergic terminals in Area X evoked changes in the pitch of the birds’ song (Hisey et al., 2018; Xiao et al., 2018). Here, we tested the hypothesis that there are common neural mechanisms underlying both learning paradigms by studying the role of dopamine in birds when they respond to a pitch shifted version of their own auditory feedback (Sober and Brainard, 2009).
We tested the role of dopamine in error-based learning by selectively lesioning dopaminergic terminals in Area X using 6-hydroxydopamine (6-OHDA). Since the cell bodies of dopaminergic neurons in VTA/SNc that innervate Area X are intermingled with those projecting to the rest of the songbird basal ganglia (Person et al., 2008), we injected 6-OHDA directly into Area X to avoid introducing general motor or song production deficits. We fitted the birds with custom-built headphones through which we introduced a shift in pitch (either upwards or downwards) of the bird’s auditory feedback (Sober and Brainard, 2009; Hoffmann et al., 2012) to measure how birds changed their pitch over time in response to this induced sensory error and how self-guided error correction was affected by dopamine manipulations.
Materials and Methods
All animals used for this study were adult (range of ages: 105–217 days post hatch; median age: 141 days post hatch) male Bengalese finches (Lonchura striata var. domestica). Throughout the study, the animals were housed in isolated sound attenuating chambers (referred to as sound boxes) on a 14/10 h light/dark cycle. All singing analyzed for this paper was undirected song, i.e., songs sung in the absence of a female. All procedures were approved by Emory University’s Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
Experimental design
Songbirds display significant bird-by-bird variability in amount of learning displayed, and so most experimental designs include a within-bird control to measure the amount of learning within a bird before and after a manipulation of interest (Hoffmann et al., 2016; Hisey et al., 2018). However, in the case of headphones as we use here (described in Headphones Attachment and Assembly below), the only way to secure the headphones to the birds for the duration of the experiment is to cement them to the skull. Although this method ensures that the headphones fit comfortably around the ear canals and remain in place for the duration of the experiment, cementing the headphones to the skull prevents access to the brain, thereby preventing us from examining learning in the same animals before and after lesion. As a result, we designed a group comparison study to test the role of dopamine in sensorimotor adaptation. We performed pitch shift experiments on six unlesioned birds (three each for upward shifts and downward shifts) and eight lesioned birds (four for upward pitch shift and four for downward pitch shift). As detailed below, virtual auditory feedback through the headphones was delivered almost in real time and was meant to replace the natural auditory feedback that birds would otherwise receive. All pitch shifts were one semitone in magnitude (equally split between +1 and –1 semitone shifts). Each experiment consisted of 3 d of baseline (unshifted auditory feedback through headphones) followed by 14 d of pitch shifted auditory feedback. At the end of the shift period, we turned off the shift in pitch (i.e., set the pitch shift to zero semitones as in the baseline epoch) and recorded the birds’ activity for 6–7 d. During this period, unlesioned birds typically reverse the effects of the pitch shift (Sober and Brainard, 2009). We refer to this period as “washout.” Washout data were collected for all six unlesioned birds. Due to technical difficulties associated with keeping the headphones attached for extended periods of time, washout data were collected for only four out of the eight lesioned birds (two for upward pitch shifts and two for downward). In addition, we performed control experiments with two unlesioned birds fitted with headphones and no pitch shift and eight lesioned birds without any pitch shifts (five with headphones and zero pitch shift throughout; three with no headphones). To minimize the number of animals we used, our unlesioned bird group consisted of data reanalyzed from Sober and Brainard, 2009. All data that have not been labeled explicitly as “data reanalyzed from a previous study” are new data collected for the purpose of this study. Furthermore, since we showed previously (Hoffmann et al., 2016) that animals injected with saline instead of 6-OHDA were statistically indistinguishable from unlesioned birds, we did not include a saline-injected control group in this study. Note that of the eight birds whose data were reanalyzed from Sober and Brainard (2009), the raw data for two animals, the unlesioned birds with no headphones shift, were unavailable. However, we were able to extract the daily mean pitch values from each animal’s data from an eps version of the original figure summarizing the data. The resulting figure that shows the mean change in pitch and error bars for the group was produced from the two data points for each day.
For our lesioned group, we reduced the dopaminergic innervation of Area X (Fig. 1), a song specific nucleus of the basal ganglia, using 6-OHDA microinjections as described in detail previously (Hoffmann et al., 2016). Following 6-OHDA surgery, the birds were allowed to recover in their sound boxes for 4–5 d. This coincides with the period over which 6-OHDA is known to cause degeneration of striatal innervation (Jeon et al., 1995). Subsequently, the headphones (Hoffmann et al., 2012) were fitted to the birds and set to initially provide unshifted auditory feedback (zero pitch shift). Following headphones attachment, the birds typically did not sing for 2–4 d (for a timeline schematic, see Fig.1C). Once they started singing again (defined as at least 30 song bouts produced over the entire day), we began recording a 3-d baseline period. Following the 3 d of baseline, the birds were recorded for 14 d during a period of shift. As described previously (Sober and Brainard, 2009; Kelly and Sober, 2014), the pitch shift was a one semitone shift (either upwards or downwards) played back to the bird through the headphones. The auditory feedback through the headphones was almost real time (delay of ∼10 ms) and was intended to replace the bird’s natural auditory feedback. In order to do so, the volume is set to be at least 2 log units greater in sound intensity than the bird’s own feedback. For the birds that had no pitch shift through the headphones, they continued with zero shift as they were in baseline for the equivalent 14 d. Following this 14-d period, we recorded the birds’ activity for 6–7 d of washout. Owing to the difficulties of keeping the headphones attached and functional for long periods of time, we were not able to collect washout data for every animal. Analysis of washout was therefore necessarily limited to birds that did have data collected for the washout period.
Note that one of our 6-OHDA lesioned birds in the –1 semitone shift group was subjected to an extended baseline period of 6 d rather than the 3-d period used for all other animals. Excluding data from this bird did not change any of our results significantly. Therefore, all results reported include this bird, treating the last 3 d of baseline equivalent to days 1 through 3 of baseline for every other bird.
Birds with lesions that were not fitted with headphones were returned to their sound boxes postsurgery and were recorded for the duration of the experiment. In this case, since they did not have a break in singing due to placement of fully assembled headphones, the baseline was defined as days 6 through 8 after lesion and the “shift” period was defined as day 9 through 22 after lesion to keep the timelines comparable between groups.
6-OHDA lesions
We performed the lesions using stereotactic surgeries as described in detail previously (Hoffmann et al., 2016). Briefly, birds were anesthetized using ketamine and midazolam and positioned at a beak angle of 20° below horizontal. Isoflurane was used to sustain anesthesia following the first hour of surgery. All stereotactic coordinates were relative to the landmark Y0, the posterior border to the divergence of the central sinus in songbirds. Small craniotomies were performed above the coordinates AP 4.75–6.4 mm; ML 0.75–2.3 mm on both sides. 6-OHDA (Tocris; conjugated with HBr) was injected bilaterally in a 4 × 3 mm grid at AP coordinates 5.1, 5.5, 5.9, and 6.3 mm and ML coordinates 0.9, 1.55, and 2.2 mm with a DV coordinate between 3.08 and 3.18 mm from the surface of the brain. For each injection, the glass pipette was lowered into the brain slowly allowing for time for rebounding of tissue, and following the injection, the pipette was left in place for at least 30 s before withdrawal at a similarly slow pace. Additionally, we initially performed one final injection at AP 4.8 mm, ML 0.8 mm, and DV 2.6 mm from the surface of the brain targeting the tail portion of Area X but dropped this injection in later birds as the targeting was not reliable and the injection required a larger craniotomy to perform. 13.8 nl of 6-OHDA was injected in the slow setting (23 nl/s) at each injection site using a Drummond Scientific Nanoject II auto-nanoliter injector.
Headphones attachment and assembly
The methodology is described in detail in Hoffmann et al. (2012). Briefly, each set of headphones was custom-fit to an individual bird under anesthesia. If attached on a bird that also had a 6-OHDA lesion, both lesion and headphones fit adjustment were performed back-to-back in the same surgery. Once the headphones had been successfully fitted for the bird, the electronics (a speaker on each side and a miniature microphone on one side to record headphones output and calibrate volume) were assembled offline. The fully assembled headphones were then refitted to the bird 4–5 d after surgery. We used a flexible tether with a commutator to power the headphones and read the electronic signals.
Histology
Following the end of the experiment, headphones were removed and the birds were deeply anesthetized with ketamine and midazolam before performing perfusions using 10% formalin. The brains were postfixed overnight in formalin and then cryoprotected in 30% sucrose for 1–4 d before slicing into 40 µm sections on a freezing sliding microtome. Alternating sections were either immunoreacted with tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) antibody and visualized with diaminobenzidine (TH-DAB) or Nissl-stained. TH-DAB was used to quantify the extent of lesions in the 6-OHDA birds, while Nissl was used to verify that there had been no necrosis and to assist in identifying boundaries of Area X in adjacent TH-DAB sections. For the TH-DAB reaction, all incubations were conducted on a shaker at room temperature and all chemicals were dissolved in 0.1 M phosphate buffer (PB) unless otherwise noted. Fixed sections were treated sequentially with 0.3% hydrogen peroxide to suppress endogenous peroxidases and 1% sodium borohydride to reduce exposed aldehydes and improve background staining before incubating overnight in a TH antibody solution (Millipore catalog #MAB318, RRID:AB_2201528, 1:4000; 0.3% Triton X-100; and 5% normal horse serum). Tissue was then incubated in biotinylated anti-mouse secondary antibody (Vector Laboratories catalog #BA-2000, RRID:AB_2313581, 1:200 and 0.3% Triton X-100) followed by avidin-biotin-complex (ABC) solution (Vector Laboratories catalog #PK-4000, RRID:AB_2336818). Tissue was exposed to DAB solution (Amresco E733; 5 mg DAB per tablet; two tablets in 20 ml of purified water) for ∼5 min. Sections were mounted, air-dried, delipidized with ethanol and citrisolv, and coverslipped with Permount (Fisher Scientific, SP15-500). For the Nissl-stained sections, Nissl stain was applied on mounted, air-dried tissue, which was delipidized with ethanol and citrisolv, and coverslipped with Permount. Stained sections were imaged using a slide scanner (Meyer Instruments PathScan Enabler IV; 24-bit color, 7200 dpi, “sharpen more” filter, brightness, and contrast level 50) and the resulting images were analyzed using ImageJ (RRID:SCR_003070).
Image and lesion analysis
TH-DAB-stained sections were used for lesion quantification by analysis through a custom written macro in ImageJ. The analysis was based on a metric of optical density (OD) described in detail in Hoffmann et al. (2016). Briefly, the macro allowed us to demarcate the boundary of Area X in every section that it is present. We also used a circle of diameter 0.5 mm to mark a section of representative striatum outside of Area X in the same section. We then defined the OD ratio as the ratio between the OD of Area X in the section to that of striatum in the section as follows:
One of the established ways of identifying Area X in songbirds has been that Area X is darker than the surrounding striatum when stained with TH-DAB (Bottjer, 1993; Soha et al., 1996; Hoffmann et al., 2016). Due to this property, we used the cumulative distribution of the OD ratio in saline-injected birds to define our threshold for lesions. Any section in our group of 6-OHDA lesioned birds with an OD ratio less than the 5th percentile of the saline-injected birds sections counted toward the overall proportion of lesioned sections. Additionally, we used a two-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov test to test whether the lesioned and saline populations were indeed drawn from separate distributions. We also used the threshold procedure described above to quantify lesion extent for individual animals. We then asked whether lesion extent was significantly correlated with vocal behavior metrics such as baseline variance, change in variance from baseline to end of shift and change in pitch at the end of shift. Note however that while this metric is robust at the population level, it is less so for individual birds.
Pitch quantification
All our analysis was performed using an extracted value of pitch for every instance in which a bird sings a particular syllable. Briefly, birds have multiple syllables within their song and they typically repeat their song hundreds of times per day during the course of the experiment. We call each time they sing a particular syllable an iteration of that syllable. We restricted our analysis to roughly 30 song files per day between 10 A.M. to 12 P.M. and have shown earlier that the choice of time window does not qualitatively affect our results (Sober and Brainard, 2009; Hoffmann and Sober, 2014; Kelly and Sober, 2014). To quantify pitch, for each syllable we specify a time during the syllable (relative to syllable onset) during which the syllable is relatively flat and clear in the frequency versus time space and can be reliably quantified across iterations across days. The pitch we extract represents a weighted average of the frequencies with the highest power in the lowest harmonic of the syllable. In order to make comparisons between different syllables whose base frequency can vary widely, we convert the pitches into semitones as shown below:
where s is the change in pitch in semitones, pitch is the observed pitch, and baseline is the average pitch across the 3 d of baseline for that particular syllable. For all group analysis, the means reported are the means over all birds and over all syllables weighted by the proportion of times they sang each syllable. This was chosen to account for the fact that syllables that are sung more often are exposed a greater number of times to the shifted auditory feedback. Pitch quantification was performed using custom-written scripts in MATLAB (RRID:SCR_001622).
Error quantification
For each of our groups, we had between four and eight birds, each bird performed between four and 12 different syllables whose pitch could be quantified, and each syllable was repeated between 40 and 600 times per day. As a result, while we have several thousands of data points toward establishing the position of the mean pitch change per group for each day, the structure of the data is hierarchical and error accumulates at different levels (birds, syllables and iterations). Grouping all the data together and estimating the standard error of the mean underestimates the error by ignoring the non-independence between data points due to the hierarchical structure. On the other extreme, aggregating points and simply using individual birds or syllables does not allow us to use all of our data effectively. This is a complex problem that different studies, including our own prior efforts have used varying methods to address (Galbraith et al., 2010; Sober and Brainard, 2012; Aarts et al., 2014; Tian and Brainard, 2017). To more accurately quantify the error in our groups and better account for the variance arising from finite data samples, we use a hierarchical bootstrapping approach (Crowley, 1992; Efron and Tibshirani, 1994). In its simplest form, bootstrapping involves generating N (N = 104 throughout this paper) random subsamples of the dataset by sampling with replacement from the original data and computing a metric of interest for each subsample. This results in having a distribution of the metric of interest, the 67% confidence interval of which provides an accurate estimate of the uncertainty in measurement of that metric in the original dataset (Efron, 1981, 1992; Efron and Tibshirani, 1994). For example, if one wanted to obtain the uncertainty in measuring the kurtosis of the data, one would generate bootstrap subsamples and calculate the kurtosis for each subsample. The standard deviation of the population of kurtosis values so obtained gives an accurate estimate of the uncertainty of the kurtosis in the original data. In the special case of estimating a population of means (which is the metric of interest in all instances in this paper), the uncertainty in measurement referred to above corresponds to the standard error of the mean of the dataset. However, bootstrapping by itself does not solve the problem of non-independence in hierarchical data. Crucially, to address this issue the resampling described above has to be done separately over each level of the hierarchy. This means that to generate a single subsample, we first resampled among the birds, then for each selected bird, we resampled among its syllables and finally for each syllable, we resampled among its iterations. Finally, we acknowledged that Bengalese finches can vary greatly in their syllable repertoires from one bird to the next. While all birds typically have an order of 10 syllables, some birds repeat one or two syllables with a much higher frequency than any other syllable while others represent each syllable equally. Since the bootstrapping procedure was used to calculate uncertainty of measurement due to sampling from a limited number of birds, we posited that each syllable would be equally likely in hypothetical new birds. Therefore, we set the number of iterations of a particular syllable that could occur in a bootstrapped subsample to be independent of the frequency of occurrence of that syllable in the actual data. All the data for the subsample were then combined and their mean was calculated for the subsample. Note that this procedure only applies to our estimate of measurement uncertainty (not the mean pitch values), since the means reported in the results are calculated from the actual data collected. This process was then repeated N times. In order to also account for the error in estimation of the mean of each syllable during baseline, the resampling was performed on pitch measurements recorded in hertz (Hz) and the measurements were converted to semitones just before calculating the mean pitch for each subsample. A similar procedure was followed for quantifying error during washout. To account for the error in estimation of pitch on the last day of pitch shift, the subtraction of the mean pitch on the final day of shift through the washout period was performed following the resampling. Our error quantification was performed using custom written scripts in MATLAB (all analysis scripts will be made available on GitHub postpublication; https://github.com/soberlab/Dopamine_Headphones_Paper_code).
Hypothesis testing with bootstrap
In addition to using bootstrapping to compute error estimates as described above, we also used a bootstrapping approach to test whether vocal pitches were significantly different across time or experimental conditions by computing direct posterior probabilities for individual hypotheses. Hence, we report our results in terms of direct probabilities of a sample being greater than or equal to another sample or fixed value in lieu of p values. Specifically, we resample the distribution for each group and calculate the mean 104 times to produce a distribution of resampled means to calculate the variance associated with having a finite number of samples.
These resampled distributions were used to compute whether the two distributions of vocal pitches were significantly different. For all instances in this paper, we use two-way tests with α = 0.05. This means that a probability is significant if the probability supporting the hypothesis, p < α/2 or if p > (1 – α/2), i.e., if p < 0.025 or if p > 0.975. In the case of computing the probability of the mean of a group being different from a constant, one can calculate the proportion of the population of bootstrapped means (as defined above, Error quantification) being greater than or equal to said constant. For example, to compute the probability that the mean pitch of a particular group is significantly different from zero, one would compute the proportion of the population of bootstrapped means that are greater than or equal to zero. If this proportion is <0.025 then the pitch of the group of interest is significantly below zero while if the proportion is >0.975 then the pitch of the group is significantly above zero.
We used a similar approach to compute significant differences between two groups of interest. In this case, we compute a population of bootstrapped means for each group. From these two bootstrapped populations, we compute a joint probability distribution between the bootstrapped means of the two groups. The null hypothesis representing no difference between the two groups would correspond to a circle centered about the unity line. Therefore, to test the difference between the two groups, we compute the volume of the joint probability distribution on one side of the unity line (including the unity line itself) to quantify the probability of one group being greater than or equal to the other group. If the probability computed is >0.975, then the first group is statistically greater than the second group. Alternatively, if the probability computed is <0.025, then the first group is statistically less than the second group. We computed multiple comparisons between groups by computing differences between two groups at a time and applied a Bonferroni correction to the threshold for significance. Our statistical tests were performed using custom scripts written in MATLAB which will also be made available on GitHub postpublication; https://github.com/soberlab/Dopamine_Headphones_Paper_code.
Validating our results with linear mixed models (LMMs)
To ensure that our results were robust to our choice of error quantification and design, we also separately reported frequentist statistical tests on our results. Since our data are hierarchical (see above, Error quantification), the recommended way to perform frequentist statistics on our data are through LMMs (Aarts et al., 2014, 2015). Specifically, we built LMMs by using bird identity and syllable identity within a bird as variable effects and tested for significance of fixed effect factors. Concretely, our LMMs were of the form:
where xij refers to the condition of the shift (±1 semitone or 0 semitone) and is the fixed effect while b0jk accounts for the bird identity and c00k accounts for syllable identities within a bird which are both variable effects. The code for hypothesis testing using LMMs was also done in MATLAB and will be available on GitHub postpublication; https://github.com/soberlab/Dopamine_Headphones_Paper_code.
Results
We performed pitch shift experiments on six unlesioned birds (three each for upward shifts and downward shifts) and eight lesioned birds (four for upward pitch shift and four for downward pitch shift). Following the end of the pitch shift, we also collected data during the washout period, i.e., when the pitch shift is set back to zero and the bird typically reverts its pitch back to baseline. All six unlesioned birds had washout data collected for 6 d following the end of shift. Of the eight 6-OHDA lesioned birds, four had data for washout for 7 d each (we were unable to record washout data for the other four lesioned animals due to technical problems associated with long-term use of the headphones). In addition, we performed control experiments with two unlesioned birds fitted with headphones who heard unshifted (zero pitch shift) auditory feedback and eight birds who received 6-OHDA lesions but did not undergo any pitch shifts (for complete details, see Materials and Methods).
6-OHDA lesions reduce dopaminergic innervation of Area X
We quantified the lesion extent using a metric developed as part of our prior work (Hoffmann et al., 2016). Specifically, we used sections of Area X stained with DAB, a chromogen that conjugates to antibodies specific for TH, the rate limiting enzyme involved in catecholamine synthesis and a reliable marker for dopaminergic and noradrenergic innervation (Fig. 2). TH-DAB does not follow the Beer–Lambert law and varies in stain intensity even within the same animal (Van Eycke et al., 2017). As a result, quantification is typically performed between hemispheres within one section comparing a lesioned to an unlesioned hemisphere. However, we had to perform bilateral lesions for our experiments since song learning is not known to be lateralized in Bengalese finches. To quantify lesion extent, we used the fact that Area X has denser dopaminergic innervation and thus stains darker by TH-DAB than the surrounding striatum (Bottjer, 1993; Soha et al., 1996). Specifically, we quantified an OD ratio for a batch of birds that had been injected with saline into Area X (N = 4 birds; data reanalyzed from Hoffmann et al., 2016) and produced a cumulative distribution plot of the ratio across all sections for these birds. We then defined the 5th percentile of that distribution as the threshold for defining lesioned sections (see Materials and Methods). When we produced a similar cumulative distribution plot of the OD ratio for all 16 of our 6-OHDA lesioned birds, ∼37.5% of all sections were below the threshold defined above (Fig. 2B). This was somewhat smaller than the lesion extent for the cohort of birds in (Hoffmann et al., 2016) in which 50% of lesioned sections were below the threshold. However, the lesions were qualitatively similar between the two groups. In addition, the population of OD ratios for the 6-OHDA lesioned birds was consistently below that for the saline-injected birds as verified by a two-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov test (K = 0.3467; p = 5.75 × 10−9). We have also previously shown that such 6-OHDA lesions have no discernible effect on the existing low levels of noradrenergic innervation of Area X (Hoffmann et al., 2016).
6-OHDA lesioned birds reduce pitch even in the absence of auditory error
We showed earlier that in unlesioned animals, the headphones do not cause changes in vocal pitch in the absence of any shifts in feedback pitch (Sober and Brainard, 2009). As shown in Figure 3A, the mean pitch across days 12 through 14 of the experiment for these birds was found to be 0.02 ± 0.07 semitones (all measures of mean pitch reported are mean ± SEM). Since this particular dataset only consists of six data points, it did not make sense to perform a bootstrap analysis (here SEM is measured across six data points; see Materials and Methods). Instead, we used a one sample t test and found that this distribution was not significantly different from zero (t = 0.35; df = 5; p = 0.74).
Data from eight birds with 6-OHDA lesions but without any pitch shift revealed an unexpected systematic lowering of vocal pitch after dopamine depletion. Of those, five birds had headphones that conveyed unshifted auditory feedback (i.e., no pitch shift) and three birds had no headphones attached. When we analyzed the mean pitch change for each day for these two groups, we found them within error bars of each other for all 14 d of the experiment, and their pitch change across days 12 through 14 (–0.20 ± 0.14 with headphones; –0.16 ± 0.06 without headphones) were statistically indistinguishable (probability of resampled mean pitch with headphones greater than that without headphones was p = 0.098; see Materials and Methods, Hypothesis testing with bootstrap). As a result, we combined the data from the two groups to compute the mean shift in pitch over the course of the experiment as shown in Figure 3B (the means for individual groups and traces for individual birds are shown in Extended Data Fig. 3-1). The overall shift in pitch over days 12 through 14 for this combined group was –0.19 ± 0.08 semitones. This decrease in pitch was statistically significant (probability of resampled mean pitch greater than or equal to zero was p = 0.0029), demonstrating, unexpectedly, that 6-OHDA lesions of Area X impacted song production by reducing the average pitch over time even in the absence of pitch-shifted auditory feedback.
6-OHDA lesioned birds do not respond adaptively to pitch-shifted auditory error
In unlesioned animals, birds respond to a pitch shift through the headphones in an adaptive manner. Specifically, when subjected to a +1 semitone pitch shift through the headphones, the unlesioned birds compensate adaptively by lowering their pitch (mean pitch change over days 12–14 for N = 3 birds was –0.40 ± 0.07 semitones; probability of resampled mean pitch greater than or equal to zero was p < 10−4; limit due to resampling 104 times; Fig. 4A, blue trace) and when subjected to a –1 semitone shift in pitch, the unlesioned birds increase their pitch [mean pitch change over days 12–14 for N = 3 birds was 0.36 ± 0.11 semitones (Fig. 4A, red trace); probability of resampled mean pitch greater than or equal to zero was p = 0.9996, recall that in our bootstrapping analysis we conclude that distributions are significantly different if the probability that one is greater than or equal to the other is <0.025 or >0.975 (see Materials and Methods); traces for individual birds are shown in Extended Data Fig. 4-1A]. The plot depicting adaptive change in pitch (inverting y-axis for +1 semitone shift birds) for unlesioned birds is shown in Figure 4C, black trace. A direct comparison between the populations of –1 semitone shift and +1 semitone shift birds revealed a complete non-overlap among posterior distributions of sampled means (probability of resampled mean pitch for +1 semitone shift greater than or equal to that for –1 semitone shift was p < 10−4; limit due to resampling 104 times). This resampling-based analysis reaffirms our initial finding (Sober and Brainard, 2009) that unlesioned birds respond adaptively to pitch-shifted auditory errors and compensate accordingly for them, despite the fact that this earlier paper did not take into account the hierarchical nature of the data and the resulting propagation of uncertainty when computing statistical significance.
For 6-OHDA lesioned birds, however, all birds decreased their pitch over time regardless of the direction of pitch shift through the headphones (Fig. 4B), similar to what we observed in lesioned birds with no pitch shifts (Fig. 3B). The +1 semitone shift group had a final pitch change of –0.38 ± 0.16 semitones (probability of resampled mean pitch greater than or equal to zero was p = 0.0040) while the –1 semitone shift group changed to a final pitch of –0.46 ± 0.19 semitones (probability of resampled mean pitch greater than or equal to zero was p = 0.0747) relative to the baseline (traces for individual birds are shown in Extended Data Fig. 4-1B). The two groups were not statistically different from each other (probability of resampled mean pitch of +1 semitone shift group being greater than or equal to that of –1 semitone shift group was p = 0.26). We also compared each group to the no shift group and did not find statistically significant results (probability of resampled mean pitch of no shift group being greater than or equal to that of –1 semitone shift group was p = 0.62; probability of resampled mean pitch of no shift group being greater than or equal to that of +1 semitone shift group was p = 0.91). All statistical comparisons have been summarized in Table 1. Furthermore, when we quantified the adaptive change in pitch for this group, the final change in pitch was close to zero (Fig. 4C, gray trace). This suggests that following 6-OHDA lesions, birds do not respond adaptively to the auditory error. Instead, the birds seem to reduce their pitch over time regardless of the direction or presence of pitch-shifted auditory error. Note that as was mentioned above and shown in Table 1, there was not a statistically significant difference between the Lesioned –1 semitone shift group and zero. This was due to the fact that while birds subjected to the –1 semitone shift did reduce their pitch on average, a few syllables for each bird increased their pitch, resulting in a group effect that fell short of significance. Since our error quantification treats the contribution from each syllable equally, the effects of individual syllables add up resulting in a not statistically significant difference (see Materials and Methods, Error quantification).
Table 1.
Hypothesis tested, Bayesian probability of group on left being >= column heading (see Materials and Methods, Hypothesis testing with bootstrap) | |||
---|---|---|---|
Groups compared | Zero | Lesioned +1 semitone shift | Lesioned –1 semitone shift |
Lesioned 0 shift | 0.0029 | 0.91 | 0.62 |
Lesioned +1 semitone shift | 0.0040 | 0.26 | |
Lesioned –1 semitone shift | 0.0747 |
Results of statistical tests for ±1 semitone shift and 0 shift lesioned groups. The probabilities for each hypothesis are reported by testing the probability of the group on the left being greater than or equal to the various column headings. Blank spaces represent tests that either do not make sense to make or have been reported on another row. The probabilities that are statistically significant at α = 0.05 are depicted in bold.
Since the hierarchical bootstrapping as we have performed here to calculate statistical tests and standard errors has not been widely applied to such datasets in neuroscience previously, we also analyzed our data using hierarchical LMMs (Aarts et al., 2014, 2015). LMMs have been widely applied to datasets involving large numbers of samples from a small number of subjects such as non-human primate studies (Arlet et al., 2015; Pleil et al., 2016) and rodent studies (Liang et al., 2015) or to analyze repeated measures or time series data (Wykes et al., 2012; Howe et al., 2013). Specifically, we built LMMs to test the effects of the shift condition while controlling bird identity and specific syllables within each bird as variable effects (see Materials and Methods, Validating our results with linear mixed models). For the unlesioned birds the LMM revealed a strong effect of the shift condition (t = 7.17; p = 7.92 × 10−13) on final pitch at the end of the shift period. For the 6-OHDA lesioned birds, the effect of the shift condition (+1 semitone shift vs –1 semitone shift versus no shift) was not significant (t = 1.91; p = 0.056). Also, when we combined the shift groups and compared them to the no shift groups, the effect was not statistically significant (t = 1.47; p = 0.14). That these models give us the same statistically significant results as our bootstrapping procedure gives us an independent verification of our error calculation and statistics.
No correlations between lesion extent and changes in pitch
We measured the extent of 6-OHDA lesions by quantifying the proportion of histologic sections that fell below the 5th percentile of section OD ratio for saline-injected birds (see Materials and Methods). We can use this same threshold to obtain a rough metric of the lesion extent for each bird. Using this lesion extent, we computed correlations between the lesion extent and a variety of metrics of changes in pitch during the experiment and these have been summarized in Table 2.However, we saw no significant correlations.
Table 2.
Lesion extent versus | Pearson’s correlation, r | Correlation significance, p |
---|---|---|
Final pitch change | 0.4261 | 0.1466 |
Baseline variance | 0.296 | 0.3261 |
Final variance | –0.0498 | 0.8716 |
Percent increase in variance | –0.4272 | 0.1454 |
The lesion extent for each bird was defined as the proportion of sections with OD ratio below the 5th percentile of OD ratios for the population of saline-injected birds. A Pearson’s correlation coefficient (r) and the associated p value is reported for this lesion extent versus changes in song metrics. Variances were computed across either 3 d of baseline or the final 3 d of the shift period.
Washout is impaired by dopamine depletion
Following the end of the shift period, we turned the pitch shift through the headphones back to zero and recorded the birds’ songs for an additional 6–7 d. During this period, birds without lesions typically revert their pitch back toward baseline levels (Sober and Brainard, 2009). Hence, we refer to this period as washout. We first collected washout data from the birds that had 6-OHDA lesions and headphones but no shifts. As stated earlier, by days 12 through 14 of the shift period, these birds had a mean pitch of –0.20 ± 0.13 semitones. By days 6 and 7 of the washout period, their pitch had changed to –0.34 ± 0.15 semitones (Fig. 5A; traces for individual birds are shown in Extended Data Fig. 5-1A). The probability of the resampled mean pitch during the end of the shift period being greater than or equal to that during the end of the washout period was p = 0.67. Therefore, although the change was not statistically significant, the mean pitch did drop further during washout. In order to quantify how much the pitch changes in response to the end of the sensory perturbation (pitch shift), we subtracted the mean pitch for each syllable on the last day of pitch shift throughout the entire washout period and quantified the resulting deviation in pitch (Fig. 6A). This emphasizes the dynamics of how the pitch changes or Δ(Pitch) over time during washout in response to the end of the shift. The resulting change in pitch was found to be –0.12 ± 0.11 semitones (probability of resampled mean pitch greater than or equal to zero was p = 0.22).
Unlesioned birds displayed a robust return to baseline following the end of the pitch shift period as shown in Figure 5B (see traces for individual birds in Extended Data Fig. 5-1B). For birds subjected to a –1 semitone shift, they reduced their pitch from 0.36 ± 0.11 semitones at the end of shift to 0.17 ± 0.08 semitones during the last 2 d of washout (probability of mean resampled pitch during washout being greater than or equal to that at the end of shift was p = 0.08). Equivalently, birds subjected to a +1 semitone shift increased their pitch from –0.40 ± 0.07 semitones at the end of the shift period to –0.20 ± 0.05 semitones by the end of the washout period (probability of mean resampled pitch during washout being greater than or equal to that at the end of shift was p = 0.98). We also computed the dynamics underlying the Δ(Pitch) over time during the washout period by subtracting the pitch for each syllable on the last day of shift through the washout period (Fig. 6B). Birds subjected to a +1 semitone shift, having reduced their pitch during the shift increased their pitch during washout. The last 2 d of washout had a mean change relative to the last day of shift of 0.17 ± 0.07 semitones (probability of resampled mean pitch lesser than or equal to zero was p = 0.0003). Similarly, birds subjected to a –1 semitone shift reduced their pitch back toward baseline during washout by –0.22 ± 0.11 semitones relative to the last day of shift (probability of resampled mean pitch greater than or equal to zero was p = 0.0064).
For our 6-OHDA lesioned birds, only four out of eight birds had data for 7 d of washout due to difficulties in keeping the headphones attached (two each for upward and downward shifts). We repeated the analysis for washout for these birds as described above for lesioned no shift and unlesioned birds. First, the mean change in pitch from the last day of shift through the washout period is shown in Figure 5C. Birds subjected to a +1 semitone shift returned their pitch back toward baseline increasing their pitch from –0.31 ± 0.19 semitones at the end of the shift period to –0.20 ± 0.14 semitones by the end of the washout period (probability of mean resampled pitch during washout being greater than or equal to that at the end of shift was p = 0.75; Fig. 5C, blue trace). Contrary to expectations however, the birds subjected to a –1 semitone shift drifted further away from baseline reducing their pitch from –0.16 ± 0.22 semitones at the end of the shift to –0.38 ± 0.30 semitones by the end of the washout period (probability of mean resampled pitch during washout being greater than or equal to that at the end of shift was p = 0.35; Fig. 5C, red trace). The traces for individual birds are shown in Extended Data Figure 5-1C.
Curiously, when we quantified the change in pitch in response to the end of the sensory perturbation subtracting the pitch change through the last day of shift through the washout period as before (i.e., measured the direction of pitch changes during washout, without considering the magnitude or direction of the pitch changes at the end of the shift period), the dynamics of the change in pitch was very similar to that seen in unlesioned birds (Fig. 6C). Lesioned birds subjected to a +1 semitone shift, averaging across the last 2 d of washout, shifted their pitch 0.24 ± 0.06 semitones with respect to the last day of shift (probability of resampled mean pitch lesser than or equal to zero was p = 0.0003). Lesioned birds subjected to a –1 semitone shift on the other hand, changed their pitch by –0.28 ± 0.11 semitones with respect to the last day of shift (probability of resampled mean pitch greater than or equal to zero was p = 0.0182). This result once again shows the dual effects we are observing following dopamine depletion. First, while not statistically significant, the pitch continued to drop for birds with unshifted auditory feedback. Second, washout was severely impaired in lesioned birds as evidenced by the fact that the birds subjected to a –1 semitone shift drifted further away from the baseline following the end of the shift instead of back toward baseline. Confusingly though, both lesioned and unlesioned birds followed the same dynamics for the Δ(Pitch) over time following the end of the pitch shifted auditory feedback.
Discussion
Our results reveal two key effects of dopamine manipulation on the control of birdsong. First, all birds subjected to a 6-OHDA lesion of Area X displayed a drop in average vocal pitch which appeared between a week and two weeks after lesion (Figs. 3B, 4B). Second, 6-OHDA lesioned birds displayed a severe deficit in sensorimotor learning as is evidenced by the lack of difference in response to a +1 or –1 semitone shift in pitch (Fig. 4B,C, gray trace).
While our primary finding seems to be one that implicates a role for dopamine in motor production, i.e., ability to produce higher pitched renditions of syllables in a bird’s repertoire, there is also a clear role for dopamine in learning the adaptive response to a sensory perturbation. It is true that when subjected to a +1 semitone pitch shift, there was no difference in mean change of pitch between lesioned (–0.38 ± 0.16 semitones) and unlesioned (–0.40 ± 0.07 semitones) birds (Fig. 4A,B, blue traces). However, when subjected to a –1 semitone pitch shift, while the adaptive response would be to raise their pitch, lesioned birds lowered their pitch (Fig. 4B, red trace). In addition, even for the lesioned birds subjected to a +1 semitone shift, their final change in pitch was not statistically different from the pitch drift seen in lesioned birds with no pitch shift (compare Fig. 3B, black trace, and Fig. 4B, blue trace). This impairment in sensorimotor learning is reminiscent of deficits in learning in persons with Parkinson’s disease (Paquet et al., 2008; Mollaei et al., 2013) and rodent models of dopamine depletion in striatum and motor cortex (Shiotsuki et al., 2010; Hosp et al., 2011; Hosp and Luft, 2013). Hence our results suggest two factors at play, namely, motor production and sensorimotor learning. Disentangling these has been a hard problem in neuroscience (Beninger, 1983; Wise, 2004) since manipulations that affect motor learning also degrade motor production, complicating efforts to isolate learning mechanisms (Ungerstedt, 1968; Iancu et al., 2005; Cenci and Lundblad, 2007). Here, we isolated the lesions’ effects on motor production by including the lesioned no shift group.
We have previously reported that 6-OHDA lesions of Area X do not produce any changes in number of songs produced or in any general motor behavior (Hoffmann et al., 2016). We similarly did not observe any qualitative difference in song quality or motor behavior between lesioned birds reported in this study and the birds reported in the 2016 study except the systematic drop in average pitch of songs sung after lesion. Note however that the lesioned birds reported in this study were recorded from for two to three weeks longer after lesion than those from the 2016 study due to differences in time required to complete the behavioral experiments after lesion. It therefore seems likely that this extended timeframe was necessary to observe the aforementioned pitch drop.
Vigor has been characterized as motivation (Salamone et al., 2007; Salamone and Correa, 2012), speed of movements, or both (Mazzoni et al., 2007; Turner and Desmurget, 2010). A reduction in motor vigor following dopamine depletion could explain the systematic drop in pitch we observed. Dopamine has been shown to be associated with vigor in humans and other mammalian systems (Niv et al., 2007; Beierholm et al., 2013; Panigrahi et al., 2015; Berke, 2018). In our experiments, we found that following 6-OHDA lesions of Area X the average pitch across all syllables for each bird dropped by roughly 11–13 d after lesion. Higher pitched syllables require a combination of greater muscle activation and higher air sac pressure to be produced (Goller and Suthers, 1996; Elemans et al., 2008, 2015; Riede et al., 2010), suggesting that higher pitched renditions of a particular syllable are more effortful to produce than lower pitched ones. We thus hypothesize that while unlesioned birds are capable of flexibly changing their pitch in a bidirectional fashion, dopamine lesioned birds will display a deficit in raising their pitch due to the increased effort required to do so. A related observation supporting our interpretation of our results is that birds sing at an elevated pitch when singing directed songs to females (Sakata et al., 2008; Leblois et al., 2010). Since it has also been reported that dopamine levels in Area X are elevated during directed song (Sasaki et al., 2006), this fits with the overall trend in our results.
Studies that have targeted individual syllables for pitch changes following dopamine depletions have not reported a systematic drop in pitch after lesion (Hoffmann et al., 2016; Hisey et al., 2018). Our study does not necessarily contradict these results since those studies reported a deficit in learning after lesion by either combining upwards and downwards shifts (Hoffmann et al., 2016) or only driving pitch changes in one direction (Hisey et al., 2018). Additionally, for the birds reported in this study, while the average pitch across all syllables for each bird dropped, some individual syllables did increase their pitch. Furthermore, as noted above the birds in the present study were recorded for a longer period of time after lesion than those reported previously.
The results from our washout data from the 6-OHDA lesioned birds are challenging to interpret. It is true that the lesioned birds subjected to a +1 semitone shift did return their pitch toward baseline and washout seemed to be unaffected for these birds (Fig. 5C, blue trace). Previous studies have reported that washout was not affected by dopamine depletion in tasks where birds shifted the pitch of a single syllable to avoid distorted auditory feedback (Hoffmann et al., 2016; Hisey et al., 2018). However, the birds subjected to a –1 semitone shift reduced their pitch resulting in their mean pitch moving further away from the baseline pitch (Fig. 5C, red trace). This suggests that washout is severely impaired in dopamine depleted birds. On the other hand, curiously, the change in pitch over time analyzed during washout in response to the end of the shift period was very similar between lesioned and unlesioned birds (compare Fig. 6B,C). We speculate that the lesion effects reported above could reflect either an inability to adaptively modulate motor output in response to error signals or from miscalculations in computing the error in the first place.
Adaptive sensorimotor learning in songbirds in response to induced auditory pitch shifts has been an effective paradigm to study the computational principles underlying sensorimotor learning (Sober and Brainard, 2009, 2012; Kelly and Sober, 2014). Bayesian inference works well to explain how unlesioned birds respond to auditory errors based on their prior experience of singing (Hahnloser and Narula, 2017; Zhou et al., 2018). However, since 6-OHDA lesioned birds exhibit drops in vocal pitch regardless of the direction of feedback pitch shift, any model that performs an adaptation to an error signal will fail to replicate the data without an additional mathematical mechanism to drive pitch downward in the presence of a reduced dopamine signal. One potential modification to the model would be to add a “relaxation state” into which the system relaxes in the absence of dopamine (Shadmehr and Arbib, 1992; Shadmehr and Mussa-Ivaldi, 1994). However, apart from the mean pitch, which did drop consistently across groups following 6-OHDA lesions, we did not find any other consistent relationships among other moments such as variance, skewness and kurtosis or overall probability distributions of produced pitch that could be used to constrain a revised Bayesian model to explain our results. Future work might therefore investigate the hypothesis that dopamine lesions disrupt sensorimotor learning by degrading the brain’s ability to perform Bayesian inference.
To conclude, our experiments show that dopamine plays a critical role in the brain’s ability to modulate vocal production in response to auditory errors. Future experiments will focus on disentangling specific roles for dopamine in sensorimotor learning by manipulating the dopamine signal at a faster temporal resolution. Results from such experiments could help fill gaps regarding the roles of tonic and phasic dopamine (Grace, 1991) for example and the timeline of error correction. Eventually, results from such experiments can be used to impose mathematical constraints on a computational model detailing the quantitative role of dopamine in such sensorimotor learning.
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgements: We thank David Hercules and Connor G. Gallimore for their role in assisting Amanda L. Jacob with tissue processing and imaging for some of the histology presented in this paper.
Synthesis
Reviewing Editor: Darcy Kelley, Columbia University
Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: NONE. Note: If this manuscript was transferred from JNeurosci and a decision was made to accept the manuscript without peer review, a brief statement to this effect will instead be what is listed below.
The author's have been responsive to the reviews of a previous version of the manuscript transferred from J. Neurosci, The implanted headphones paradigm should be of interest to researchers in the birdsong fiels, the statistical analyses are thorough and thoughtful and the revised manuscript a useful contribution to the literature.
References
- Aarts E, Verhage M, Veenvliet JV, Dolan CV, van der Sluis S (2014) A solution to dependency: using multilevel analysis to accommodate nested data. Nat Neurosci 17:491–496. 10.1038/nn.3648 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aarts E, Dolan CV, Verhage M, van der Sluis S (2015) Multilevel analysis quantifies variation in the experimental effect while optimizing power and preventing false positives. BMC Neurosci 16:94. 10.1186/s12868-015-0228-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arlet M, Jubin R, Masataka N, Lemasson AJ (2015) Grooming-at-a-distance by exchanging calls in non-human primates. Biol Lett 11:20150711 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0711 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balleine BW, O'doherty JP (2010) Human and rodent homologies in action control: corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology 35:48 10.1038/npp.2009.131 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beierholm U, Guitart-Masip M, Economides M, Chowdhury R, Düzel E, Dolan R, Dayan P (2013) Dopamine modulates reward-related vigor. Neuropsychopharmacology 38:1495–1503. 10.1038/npp.2013.48 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beninger RJ (1983) The role of dopamine in locomotor activity and learning. Brain Res 287:173–196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berke JD (2018) What does dopamine mean? Nat Neurosci 21:787–793. 10.1038/s41593-018-0152-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bottjer SW (1993) The distribution of tyrosine hydroxylase immunoreactivity in the brains of male and female zebra finches. J Neurobiol 24:51–69. 10.1002/neu.480240105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brainard MS, Doupe AJ (2000) Interruption of a basal ganglia-forebrain circuit prevents plasticity of learned vocalizations. Nature 404:762–766. 10.1038/35008083 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cenci MA, Lundblad M (2007) Ratings of L‐DOPA‐induced dyskinesia in the unilateral 6‐OHDA lesion model of Parkinson's disease in rats and mice. Curr Protoc Neurosci Chapter 9:Unit 9.25. 10.1002/0471142301.ns0925s41 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper JA, Sagar HJ, Jordan N, Harvey NS, Sullivan EV (1991) Cognitive impairment in early, untreated Parkinson's disease and its relationship to motor disability. Brain 114:2095–2122. 10.1093/brain/114.5.2095 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crowley PH (1992) Resampling methods for computation-intensive data analysis in ecology and evolution. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 23:405–447. [Google Scholar]
- Dubois B, Pillon BJ (1996) Cognitive deficits in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol 244:2–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Efron B (1981) Nonparametric estimates of standard error: the jackknife, the bootstrap and other methods. Biometrika 68:589–599. 10.2307/2335441 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Efron B (1992) Bootstrap methods: another look at the jackknife In: Breakthroughs in statistics, pp 569–593. New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Efron B, Tibshirani RJ (1994) An introduction to the bootstrap. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. [Google Scholar]
- Elemans CP, Mead AF, Rome LC, Goller F (2008) Superfast vocal muscles control song production in songbirds. PLoS One 3:e2581. 10.1371/journal.pone.0002581 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elemans C, Rasmussen JH, Herbst CT, Düring DN, Zollinger SA, Brumm H, Srivastava K, Svane N, Ding M, Larsen ON (2015) Universal mechanisms of sound production and control in birds and mammals. Nat Commun 6:8978. 10.1038/ncomms9978 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gadagkar V, Puzerey PA, Chen R, Baird-Daniel E, Farhang AR, Goldberg JH (2016) Dopamine neurons encode performance error in singing birds. Science 354:1278–1282. 10.1126/science.aah6837 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galbraith S, Daniel JA, Vissel B (2010) A study of clustered data and approaches to its analysis. J Neurosci 30:10601–10608. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0362-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glimcher PW (2011) Understanding dopamine and reinforcement learning: the dopamine reward prediction error hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108 [Suppl 3]:15647–15654. 10.1073/pnas.1014269108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goller F, Suthers RA (1996) Role of syringeal muscles in controlling the phonology of bird song. J Neurophysiol 76:287–300. 10.1152/jn.1996.76.1.287 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grace AA (1991) Phasic versus tonic dopamine release and the modulation of dopamine system responsivity: a hypothesis for the etiology of schizophrenia. Neuroscience 41:1–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahnloser RH, Narula G (2017) A Bayesian account of vocal adaptation to pitch-shifted auditory feedback. PLoS One 12:e0169795. 10.1371/journal.pone.0169795 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haith AM, Krakauer JW (2013) Model-based and model-free mechanisms of human motor learning. Adv Exp Med Biol 782:1–21. 10.1007/978-1-4614-5465-6_1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hisey E, Kearney MG, Mooney R (2018) A common neural circuit mechanism for internally guided and externally reinforced forms of motor learning. Nat Neurosci 21:589–597. 10.1038/s41593-018-0092-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffmann LA, Sober SJ (2014) Vocal generalization depends on gesture identity and sequence. J Neurosci 34:5564–5574. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5169-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffmann LA, Kelly CW, Nicholson DA, Sober SJ (2012) A lightweight, headphones-based system for manipulating auditory feedback in songbirds. J Vis Exp. Advance online Publication. Retrieved Nonember 26, 2012. doi: 10.3791/50027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffmann LA, Saravanan V, Wood AN, He L, Sober SJ (2016) Dopaminergic contributions to vocal learning. J Neurosci 36:2176–2189. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3883-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hosp JA, Luft AR (2013) Dopaminergic meso-cortical projections to M1: role in motor learning and motor cortex plasticity. Front Neurol 4:145. 10.3389/fneur.2013.00145 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hosp JA, Pekanovic A, Rioult-Pedotti MS, Luft AR (2011) Dopaminergic projections from midbrain to primary motor cortex mediate motor skill learning. J Neurosci 31:2481–2487. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5411-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howe WM, Berry AS, Francois J, Gilmour G, Carp JM, Tricklebank M, Lustig C, Sarter M (2013) Prefrontal cholinergic mechanisms instigating shifts from monitoring for cues to cue-guided performance: converging electrochemical and fMRI evidence from rats and humans. J Neurosci 33:8742–8752. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5809-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iancu R, Mohapel P, Brundin P, Paul G (2005) Behavioral characterization of a unilateral 6-OHDA-lesion model of Parkinson's disease in mice. Behav Brain Res 162:1–10. 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.02.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jankovic J (2008) Parkinson’s disease: clinical features and diagnosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 79:368–376. 10.1136/jnnp.2007.131045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeon BS, Jackson-Lewis V, Burke RE (1995) 6-Hydroxydopamine lesion of the rat substantia nigra: time course and morphology of cell death. Neurodegeneration 4:131–137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly CW, Sober SJ (2014) A simple computational principle predicts vocal adaptation dynamics across age and error size. Front Integr Neurosci 8:75. 10.3389/fnint.2014.00075 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuebrich BD, Sober SJ (2015) Variations on a theme: songbirds, variability, and sensorimotor error correction. Neuroscience 296:48–54. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.09.068 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leblois A, Wendel BJ, Perkel DJ (2010) Striatal dopamine modulates basal ganglia output and regulates social context-dependent behavioral variability through D1 receptors. J Neurosci 30:5730–5743. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5974-09.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lees AJ, Smith E (1983) Cognitive deficits in the early stages of Parkinson's disease. Brain 106:257–270. 10.1093/brain/106.2.257 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liang Z, Watson GD, Alloway KD, Lee G, Neuberger T, Zhang NJN (2015) Mapping the functional network of medial prefrontal cortex by combining optogenetics and fMRI in awake rats. Neuroimage 117:114–123. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipkind D, Marcus GF, Bemis DK, Sasahara K, Jacoby N, Takahasi M, Suzuki K, Feher O, Ravbar P, Okanoya K, Tchernichovski O (2013) Stepwise acquisition of vocal combinatorial capacity in songbirds and human infants. Nature 498:104–108. 10.1038/nature12173 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mandelblat-Cerf Y, Las L, Denisenko N, Fee MS (2014) A role for descending auditory cortical projections in songbird vocal learning. Elife 3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazzoni P, Hristova A, Krakauer JW (2007) Why don't we move faster? Parkinson's disease, movement vigor, and implicit motivation. J Neurosci 27:7105–7116. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0264-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mohan V, Morasso P, Metta GJN (2011) The distribution of rewards in sensorimotor maps acquired by cognitive robots through exploration. Neurocomputing 74:3440–3455. 10.1016/j.neucom.2011.06.009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mollaei F, Shiller DM, Gracco VL (2013) Sensorimotor adaptation of speech in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord 28:1668–1674. 10.1002/mds.25588 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niv Y, Daw ND, Joel D, Dayan P (2007) Tonic dopamine: opportunity costs and the control of response vigor. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 191:507–520. 10.1007/s00213-006-0502-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panigrahi B, Martin KA, Li Y, Graves AR, Vollmer A, Olson L, Mensh BD, Karpova AY, Dudman JT (2015) Dopamine is required for the neural representation and control of movement vigor. Cell 162:1418–1430. 10.1016/j.cell.2015.08.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paquet F, Bedard MA, Levesque M, Tremblay PL, Lemay M, Blanchet PJ, Scherzer P, Chouinard S, Filion J (2008) Sensorimotor adaptation in Parkinson’s disease: evidence for a dopamine dependent remapping disturbance. Exp Brain Res 185:227–236. 10.1007/s00221-007-1147-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peh WY, Roberts TF, Mooney R (2015) Imaging auditory representations of song and syllables in populations of sensorimotor neurons essential to vocal communication. J Neurosci 35:5589–5605. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2308-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Person AL, Gale SD, Farries MA, Perkel DJ (2008) Organization of the songbird basal ganglia, including area X. J Comp Neurol 508:840–866. 10.1002/cne.21699 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pleil KE, Helms CM, Sobus JR, Daunais JB, Grant KA, Kash T (2016) Effects of chronic alcohol consumption on neuronal function in the non‐human primate BNST. Addict Biol 21:1151–1167. 10.1111/adb.12289 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Redgrave P, Rodriguez M, Smith Y, Rodriguez-Oroz MC, Lehericy S, Bergman H, Agid Y, DeLong MR, Obeso JA (2010) Goal-directed and habitual control in the basal ganglia: implications for Parkinson's disease. Nat Rev Neurosci 11:760. 10.1038/nrn2915 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riede T, Fisher JH, Goller F (2010) Sexual dimorphism of the zebra finch syrinx indicates adaptation for high fundamental frequencies in males. PLoS One 5:e11368. 10.1371/journal.pone.0011368 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakata JT, Brainard MS (2006) Real-time contributions of auditory feedback to avian vocal motor control. J Neurosci 26:9619–9628. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2027-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakata JT, Brainard MS (2008) Online contributions of auditory feedback to neural activity in avian song control circuitry. J Neurosci 28:11378–11390. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3254-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakata JT, Hampton CM, Brainard MS (2008) Social modulation of sequence and syllable variability in adult birdsong. J Neurophysiol 99:1700–1711. 10.1152/jn.01296.2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salamone JD, Correa M (2012) The mysterious motivational functions of mesolimbic dopamine. Neuron 76:470–485. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salamone JD, Correa M, Farrar A, Mingote SM (2007) Effort-related functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine and associated forebrain circuits. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 191:461–482. 10.1007/s00213-006-0668-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sasaki A, Sotnikova TD, Gainetdinov RR, Jarvis ED (2006) Social context-dependent singing-regulated dopamine. J Neurosci 26:9010–9014. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1335-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scharff C, Nottebohm F (1991) A comparative study of the behavioral deficits following lesions of various parts of the zebra finch song system: implications for vocal learning. J Neurosci 11:2896–2913. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR (1997) A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275:1593–1599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shadmehr R, Arbib MA (1992) A mathematical analysis of the force-stiffness characteristics of muscles in control of a single joint system. Biol Cybern 66:463–477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shadmehr R, Mussa-Ivaldi FA (1994) Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task. J Neurosci 14:3208–3224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shiotsuki H, Yoshimi K, Shimo Y, Funayama M, Takamatsu Y, Ikeda K, Takahashi R, Kitazawa S, Hattori N (2010) A rotarod test for evaluation of motor skill learning. J Neurosci Methods 189:180–185. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.03.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sober SJ, Brainard MS (2009) Adult birdsong is actively maintained by error correction. Nat Neurosci 12:927–931. 10.1038/nn.2336 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sober SJ, Brainard MS (2012) Vocal learning is constrained by the statistics of sensorimotor experience. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109:21099–21103. 10.1073/pnas.1213622109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soha JA, Shimizu T, Doupe AJ (1996) Development of the catecholaminergic innervation of the song system of the male zebra finch. J Neurobiol 29:473–489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sohrabji F, Nordeen EJ, Nordeen KW (1990) Selective impairment of song learning following lesions of a forebrain nucleus in the juvenile zebra finch. Behav Neural Biol 53:51–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tian LY, Brainard MS (2017) Discrete circuits support generalized versus context-specific vocal learning in the songbird. Neuron 96:1168–1177.e5. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.10.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tumer EC, Brainard MS (2007) Performance variability enables adaptive plasticity of 'crystallized' adult birdsong. Nature 450:1240–1244. 10.1038/nature06390 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner RS, Desmurget M (2010) Basal ganglia contributions to motor control: a vigorous tutor. Curr Opin Neurobiol 20:704–716. 10.1016/j.conb.2010.08.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ungerstedt U (1968) 6-Hydroxy-dopamine induced degeneration of central monoamine neurons. Eur J Pharmacol 5:107–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Eycke YR, Allard J, Salmon I, Debeir O, Decaestecker C (2017) Image processing in digital pathology: an opportunity to solve inter-batch variability of immunohistochemical staining. Sci Rep 7:42964. 10.1038/srep42964 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilbrecht L, Nottebohm F (2003) Vocal learning in birds and humans. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 9:135–148. 10.1002/mrdd.10073 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise RA (2004) Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nat Rev Neurosci 5:483–494. 10.1038/nrn1406 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z, Jordan MI (1995) An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science 269:1880–1882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolpert DM, Diedrichsen J, Flanagan JR (2011) Principles of sensorimotor learning. Nat Rev Neurosci 12:739–751. 10.1038/nrn3112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wykes RC, Heeroma JH, Mantoan L, Zheng K, MacDonald DC, Deisseroth K, Hashemi KS, Walker MC, Schorge S, Kullmann DM (2012) Optogenetic and potassium channel gene therapy in a rodent model of focal neocortical epilepsy. Sci Transl Med 4:161ra152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xiao L, Chattree G, Oscos FG, Cao M, Wanat MJ, Roberts TF (2018) A basal ganglia circuit sufficient to guide birdsong learning. Neuron 98:208–221.e5. 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.02.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou B, Hofmann D, Pinkoviezky I, Sober SJ, Nemenman I (2018) Chance, long tails, and inference in a non-Gaussian, Bayesian theory of vocal learning in songbirds. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 115:E8538–E8546. 10.1073/pnas.1713020115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.