Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: Prev Sci. 2019 Jul;20(5):776–787. doi: 10.1007/s11121-019-0984-z

Sensation Seeking and Impulsivity Can Increase Exposure to Risky Media and Moderate its Effects on Adolescent Risk Behaviors

Atika Khurana a, Amy Bleakley b, Morgan E Ellithorpe c, Michael Hennessy b, Patrick E Jamieson d, Ilana Weitz d
PMCID: PMC6565384  NIHMSID: NIHMS1524961  PMID: 30659453

Abstract

Media exposure to risky behaviors (e.g., alcohol use, violence) has been associated with adolescent engagement in risk-taking behaviors, but not all adolescents are equally at risk. Here we focus on individual differences in impulsivity and sensation seeking and assess their effects on the relation between media risk exposure and adolescent risk behavior. Survey data from 1,990 Black and White U.S. adolescents (Mean age = 15.6±1.10 years; 48% female) and content analysis of top-grossing films and popular TV shows were analyzed using linear regression models. High levels of impulsivity and sensation seeking were associated with greater exposure to risky media content, and also operated as moderators, exacerbating the impact of media risk exposure on adolescent risk behaviors. Prevention efforts targeting negative effects of media on adolescent health should prioritize youth with high levels of impulsivity and sensation seeking.

Keywords: Media effects, Adolescent risk behaviors, Impulsivity, Sensation Seeking


Adolescents are spending increasing amounts of time with media (Lenhart, 2015; Rideout, 2016; Rideout, Foehr, & Roberts, 2010). TV and movie exposure to risk behaviors is prevalent among adolescents and is linked to involvement in risk-taking behaviors (Brown & Bobkowski, 2011; Strasburger, Jordan, & Donnerstein, 2010). These effects have been reported for alcohol use (Hanewinkel & Sargent, 2009; Wills et al., 2012), sexual risk-taking (Bleakley, Hennessy, Fishbein, & Jordan, 2008; Brown et al., 2006; Collins et al., 2004; Hennessy, Bleakley, Fishbein, & Jordan, 2009), and aggressive behaviors (Bushman & Huesmann, 2006). Although past studies have often assumed that the effect of media exposure on risk behaviors operates similarly for all adolescents, some adolescents may be more vulnerable than others (Valkenburg & Peter, 2013). By focusing on individual differences in impulsivity and sensation seeking (SS), this study examines the role of these personality dimensions in better understanding the media exposure-risk behavior relationship in adolescents.

Impulsivity, Sensation Seeking, and Adolescent Risk-Taking

Impulsivity and SS are robust predictors of adolescent risk behaviors. Impulsivity is typically defined as a tendency to act on the spur of the moment without adequate consideration of the consequences, and is assessed using self-report measures such as the Barratt (Patton, Stanford, & Barratt, 1995) or Eysenck (Eysenck, Easting, & Pearson, 1984) scales. SS on the other hand reflects a propensity towards novel or exciting behaviors that is considered to be evolutionarily adaptive (Spear, 2007) and is commonly assessed using self-report measures such as Zuckerman’s sensation seeking scale (Hoyle, Stephenson, Palmgreen, Lorch, & Donohew, 2002; Zuckerman, 2006). Developmental studies find that SS exhibits a universal peak during mid-late adolescence and declines thereafter (Collado, Felton, MacPherson, & Lejuez, 2014; Khurana, Romer, Betancourt, & Hurt, 2018). Impulsivity is a complex multidimensional construct (Whiteside & Lynam, 2001). Some dimensions of impulsivity, such as the one assessed in this study (i.e., acting without thinking), exhibit a peak during adolescence, but only for a sub-group of youth who have an underlying weakness in cognitive control (Khurana et al., 2018). These difficulties in impulse control can be identified at younger ages (Iacono, Malone, & McGue, 2008), and tend to get exacerbated during adolescence when there is a concomitant rise in reward sensitivity (Bjork & Pardini, 2015; Moffitt et al., 2011). Unlike impulsivity, SS does not reflect a lack of top-down control over behavioral urges. Instead, SS tends to be positively related to cognitive control measures like working memory (Khurana et al., 2018) and IQ (Raine, Reynolds, Venables, & Mednick, 2002). It is therefore not surprising that while SS is linked to experimentation with drugs and sexual behaviors, it is not associated with more adverse forms of risk-taking such as progressive drug use or dependence that reflect a loss of control (Khurana et al., 2015a; Khurana, Romer, Betancourt, & Hurt, 2017).

Guided by the “Uses and Gratifications” theory (Katz, Blumler, & Gurevitch, 1974; Rubin, 2009), several studies have linked SS and impulsivity to seeking out risky content in the media. Adolescents are not mere passive recipients but instead select media purposefully to gratify their needs. Such needs could be developmental (e.g. identity development), or to validate a particular behavior (e.g., validating aggressive tendencies by engaging with violent media). Studies have found that adolescents high in SS are more likely to be exposed to or seek out sexually explicit media content (Bleakley, Hennessy, & Fishbein, 2011; Brown & L’Engle, 2009; Kim et al., 2006; Slater, 2003). Individuals with high levels of impulsivity also report greater exposure to violent media (Chory & Goodboy, 2010; Krcmar & Kean, 2005). Thus, adolescents high in SS and impulsivity may be at greater risk for experiencing negative effects of media risk exposure (e.g., involvement in risk-taking behaviors) in part because of their higher exposure to risky content in the media (Anderson & Bushman, 2001; Strasburger et al., 2010).

Alternatively, it is also possible that adolescents high in impulsivity and SS are more susceptible to the negative effects of media exposure on risk behaviors. This idea is consistent with the Differential Susceptibility Model (Valkenburg & Peter, 2013) which posits that some youth experience greater susceptibility to media effects based on personality characteristics, moods, and cognitions. This model argues that personality dimensions such as impulsivity and SS may operate as moderators (as well as mediators) upon which effects of media would be conditional. Adolescents high in impulsivity and/or SS may be more likely to enact media depictions of risk behaviors in the real-world either due to the rewarding aspect or because of their relative inability to control behavioral urges and evaluate long term consequences. We hypothesized that the association between media risk exposure and risk behavior would be positive and stronger in magnitude among adolescents high in impulsivity and SS (Gibbons et al., 2016; Slater et al., 2004; Wills et al., 2010).

By focusing on three common adolescent risk behaviors, i.e., alcohol use, sexual risk, and violence, this study addressed the following research questions: (1) Are high levels of SS and impulsivity associated with greater media (TV and movie) exposure to alcohol use, sexual content, and violence, which in turn is linked to elevated risk for involvement in corresponding risk behaviors? (2) Do individual differences in SS and impulsivity moderate the relationship between media (TV and movie) risk exposure and real-world risk-taking for alcohol use, sexual behaviors and violence? (3) Does adolescent race moderate the pathways of influence described in RQ1 and RQ2, i.e., do the direct, mediated, or moderated effects vary for White and Black adolescents? Alcohol use, sexual risk, and violence were modeled as separate outcomes.

Methods

This study includes data from a content analysis of popular entertainment media titles and an online survey administered to a sample of 2,424 adolescents aged 14-17 years, recruited from online opt-in, volunteer panels through survey company GfK between November 13 and December 14, 2015. Most respondents (76%) were recruited through their parents; remaining were recruited directly. Potential respondents were screened for whether they were a teen aged 14-17, parent of a teen aged 14-17, or not eligible. Parents were asked to consent for their child to participate before being asked to bring their teen to complete the rest of the survey. All teens were given assent information before beginning the survey. The survey procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board of the sponsoring institution. The survey had a median length of 26 minutes. Respondents received an incentive for participating (e.g., panel points from survey firm). The purpose of the larger study was to examine Black and White differences in media exposure and its effects on risk behaviors, therefore Black adolescents were oversampled for a roughly equal sample of Black and White respondents. Given the study design, we selected the non-Hispanic Black (n=1000) and non-Hispanic White (n=990) participants to be included in the current sample (total n=1,990; Mean age = 15.6±1.1 years; 48.8% female).

Content analysis

Movie sample

The top 30-grossing movies of 2014 according to Variety magazine were selected as representing popular mainstream movies. Of the top 500 movies for 2013 and 2014 (1,000 total), according to www.boxofficemojo.com, 33 Black-oriented movies were selected based on following criteria: (1) Black actors comprised half or more of the main characters, and/or (2) the movie had a Black-oriented narrative (Allen, Dawson, & Brown, 1989; Schooler, Monique Ward, Merriwether, & Caruthers, 2004; Sheridan, 2006). Top-grossing movies of both 2013 and 2014 were included for Black-oriented movies in order to have a large enough sample for comparison purposes. The final sample included 29 mainstream movies and 34 Black-oriented movies (one film originally coded with mainstream movies (Ride Along) met the criterion for a predominately Black cast).

TV show sample

Nielsen statistics for adolescents aged 14-17 were used to determine the narrative television shows from the 2014-2015 season (from September 22, 2014 and June 28, 2015) that would be coded. The lists were separated by Black and non-Black adolescents because Black and non-Black adolescents tend to watch different kinds of shows (Ellithorpe & Bleakley, 2016) in that adolescents in general tend to gravitate toward content with more characters from their own groups. Thus, television shows popular with Black adolescents will have more Black characters than shows popular with non-Black adolescents. We created lists of the top 30 shows watched by Black adolescents and the top 30 shows watched by non-Black adolescents (including primarily White viewers, but also Hispanic, Asian, and others). Old shows airing repeat episodes and syndicated shows were also included, but only when the number of repeats aired were equivalent to at least of one season of that show (Bleakley et al., 2017).

Four shows appeared on both the top 30 for non-Black and top 30 for list for Blacks, thus 56 shows were coded. Episodes from the 2014-2015 season were coded, with the exception of a handful of shows in syndication or reruns (e.g., How I Met Your Mother) in which case the last season produced was coded. Either 3 or 5 episodes were randomly selected from each season for coding (Manganello, Franzini, & Jordan, 2008). If a show had ten or fewer episodes in the 2014-2015 season, 3 episodes were randomly selected (n=5, 8.9% of shows). If a show had more than ten episodes that season, 5 episodes were randomly selected (n=47, 83.9% of shows). If a show had a 15-minute runtime, six episodes were selected to equate the amount coded to a half-hour show (n=4, 7.1% of shows).

Content Coding Procedures

The television and movie content were coded by trained coders in five-minute segments using a directed, quantitative, previously-validated coding scheme (Bleakley, Jamieson, & Romer, 2012; Bleakley, Romer, & Jamieson, 2014; Jamieson & Romer, 2008). After multiple training sessions, the coders achieved inter-coder reliability for identifying the presence of each behavior as calculated by Krippendorff’s alpha (Hayes & Krippendorff, 2007) using a separately validated test sample of 59 segments. Each segment was coded for the portrayal of alcohol, sex, and/or violence. Alcohol portrayal was defined as a character being directly involved any activity related to alcohol, ranging from handling of alcohol bottles to observed consumption (α=0.94). Sexual behavior was defined as any type of sexual contact, from kissing on the lips to explicit intercourse (α=0.93). Violence was defined as initiated or received intentional acts to inflict injury or harm (α=0.94).

Online Survey

Media exposure to risk behaviors

Exposure to media content was calculated for alcohol, sex, and violence using content analysis of movies and TV shows and participants’ exposure to film titles and TV shows. Participants were presented with the list of coded films (randomly ordered) in a grid and asked to indicate whether they had never seen the film, seen the film once, or seen the film more than once. Similarly, they were presented with additional grids for the coded television show titles and asked to indicate how often they had watched each show. Content-specific exposure was operationalized by multiplying the proportion of segments for each film containing the risk behavior by each participant’s self-reported exposure to the film (indicated on the survey for whether they had seen each film: never (0), once (1), or more than once (2)) or television show (how often they watched each coded television show in the past year, on a scale from 0=never to 3=often). Using a previously tested approach (Bleakley et al., 2008; Bleakley, Hennessy, Fishbein, & Jordan, 2011; Hennessy et al., 2009), these scores were summed across all movie and all television titles, separately, to create a measure of exposure to each risk in movies and in television. The measures were then standardized.

Impulsivity

We used nine yes/no items adapted from the Junior Eysenck Impulsivity Scale (Eysenck et al., 1984) (e.g., Do you usually do and say things without stopping to think?). Scores were averaged to create a composite index (Range: 0-1; M= 0.47, SD =0.31, α = .80).

Sensation Seeking (SS)

We used four items (e.g., “I like to do frightening things”) representing each of the four dimensions (i.e., experience seeking, boredom susceptibility, thrill/adventure seeking, and disinhibition) of the Brief Sensation Seeking Scale (Hoyle et al., 2002). Responses were coded on a 5-point scale ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree, and averaged to create a composite score (Range: 1-5; M =3.29, SD=1.00), α = .87).

Alcohol use

Drinking frequency was assessed by asking about the number of times the respondent had had at least one drink of alcohol in the past six months (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2015). Range: never (0) to about every day (5) (M=0.58, SD=1.04).

Sexual activity

Progression in sexual activity was measured using an ordered (Guttman) scale of the following dichotomous items assessing activities engaged in the past six months: kissed, touched each other over clothes, touched breasts/had breasts touched, touched a partner’s private parts, saw a partner naked, was naked with partner, received oral sex, had vaginal sex, and gave oral sex (Hennessy, Bleakley, Fishbein, & Jordan, 2008). The items are listed by difficulty: the Loevinger’s H coefficient was 0.89 (van Schuur, 2003) and KR-20 for the scale was 0.94 (M=2.11, SD=3.01).

Violence

This variable was assessed by asking about involvement in a physical fight in the past six months (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2015). Response options ranged from 0 times (0) to 12 or more times (7). Given the skewed distribution, the highest 3 categories with low frequencies were collapsed, with the new variable range of 0 times (0) to 6-7 times or more (4) (M=0.44, SD=0.90).

Covariates

Age, gender (Male = 0, Female =1), perceived parental monitoring, maternal education, and daily TV time were included as covariates because of their relationship to media exposure and/or risk behaviors. In all models, race (White = 0; Black = 1) was examined as a moderator for direct, indirect, and moderated effects. If the moderated effect was non-significant, race was included as a control in the model. Perceived parental monitoring, or the extent to which an adolescent believes his or her parents knows about their whereabouts/activities, was measured by eight items ranging from never (1) to always (5) (α = 0.92, M=3.80, SD=0.94) (Kerr, Stattin, & Burk, 2010). Maternal education was assessed using 5 categories: 1 (some high school) - 5 (graduate degree). We calculated daily TV time by asking participants how many hours they spent watching television in three time periods the previous day: before noon, between noon and 6 pm, and after 6 pm. In order to have a maximum of 24 hours, responses greater than six hours for the time period between noon and 6 pm (n=41, 2.09%) were recoded as six hours, and responses greater than nine hours were recoded as nine for the other two time periods (before noon n=28, 1.42%; after 6 pm n=15, 0.08%). Responses were then summed (M=5.50, SD=4.61; Median=4.5).

Statistical Analysis

Descriptive analyses were conducted using t-tests or chi-square as appropriate, and correlations among the model variables were assessed. Separate linear regression models were used for the three behavioral outcomes, with robust estimation procedures to account for any violations of normality. Alcohol use was included as an additional control in models predicting sexual activity and violence. Descriptive analyses were conducted in STATA 14.0. Final models were estimated in Mplus v8, with mediated and moderated effects tested in the same model. Moderation effects were tested as two-way and three-way interactions by using product terms with mean centered variables (Jaccard & Turrisi, 2003). The interaction effects were tested individually, including main effects of predictor variables and covariates in the same model. The data presented here are unweighted.

Results

Correlations and means (SD) of study variables are presented in Table 1. The media exposure-risk behavior association was significant in case of sexual risk, alcohol use, and violence, for both TV and movie exposure. Impulsivity and SS were positively associated with TV and movie exposure to sex, alcohol use, and violence with involvement in all three risk behavior outcomes, with impulsivity having stronger associations than SS.

Table 1.

Correlation matrix of key study variables

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17
1 Sexual risk 1.00
2 Alcohol use 0.48 1.00
3 Violence 0.28 0.32 1.00
4 TV sex exposure 0.23 0.19 0.22 1.00
5 Movie sex exposure 0.25 0.20 0.24 0.69 1.00
6 TV alcohol exposure 0.25 0.21 0.20 0.96 0.64 1.00
7 Movie alcohol exposure 0.25 0.19 0.23 0.69 0.99 0.64 1.00
8 TV violence exposure 0.22 0.17 0.23 0.95 0.66 0.91 0.66 1.00
9 Movie violence exposure 0.18 0.13 0.18 0.60 0.83 0.58 0.83 0.63 1.00
10 Impulsivity 0.27 0.22 0.25 0.14 0.14 0.15 0.14 0.14 0.12 1.00
11 Sensation seeking 0.19 0.18 0.07 0.15 0.12 0.15 0.12 0.15 0.14 0.30 1.00
12 Parental monitoring −0.17 −0.17 −0.20 −0.01 −0.04 0.004 −0.03 −0.02 0.04 −0.18 −0.09 1.00
13 Gender (Female =1) −0.10 −0.05 −0.14 0.002 0.03 −0.07 0.02 −0.06 −0.07 −0.04 −0.01 0.07 1.00
14 Age 0.20 0.18 0.03 0.02 0.05 0.03 0.05 0.003 −0.02 −0.04 −0.04 −0.12 0.01 1.00
15 Daily TV time 0.09 0.09 0.05 0.29 0.28 0.27 0.28 0.29 0.27 0.11 0.08 −0.03 0.03 −0.02 1.00
16 Race (Black =1) 0.01 −0.11 0.08 0.24 0.31 0.19 0.31 0.25 0.19 0.003 −0.03 −0.22 0.03 −0.002 0.16 1.00
17 Maternal Education −0.04 −0.05 −0.10 −0.11 −0.08 −0.12 −0.06 −0.12 −0.06 −0.09 0.01 0.13 0.01 −0.01 −0.05 −0.03 1.00
Range; Mean (SD) 0-9; 2.11 (3.01) 0-5; 0.58 (1.04) 0-4; 0.44 (0.90) 0-19.88; 4.21 (3.56) 0-18.44; 3.30 (3.12) 0-27.14; 6.56 (5.24) 0-19.10; 3.45 (3.29) 0-39.07; 8.36 (6.93) 0-38.02; 10.20 (6.89) 0-1; 0.47 (0.31) 1-5; 3.29 (1.00) 1-5; 3.80 (0.94) 0/1 14-17; 15.59 (1.10) 0-24; 5.50 (4.61) 0/1 1-5; 3.12 (1.19)

Note. Shaded values denote coefficients not significant at p <0.05. Mean and SD reported for continuous variables only.

In the model predicting sexual activity, we found direct effects of media risk exposure and of SS and impulsivity. Some of the effect of impulsivity and SS on sexual activity was mediated by media exposure to sexual content. (See indirect effect estimates in Table 2). Adolescents with high levels of impulsivity and SS reported greater exposure to media sexual content, which in turn was associated with higher levels of sexual involvement. Impulsivity and SS did not moderate the direct or mediated effects. Further, no racial differences were observed in the direct or mediated effects, with the exception of SS’s effect on sexual behaviors which tended to be significant for White youth, B (SE) = 0.26 (0.09), p = 0.003, but not for Black youth, B (SE) = 0.06 (0.09), p = 0.51. However, this moderated effect (i.e., SS X race), B (SE) = −0.37 (0.13), p = 0.004, became non-significant, B (SE) = −0.20 (0.12), p = 0.09, controlling for the effect of alcohol use, and thus was not retained in the final model. The direct effect of TV sex exposure on sexual activity was also not significant at p <0.05, possibly due to its strong association with movie sex exposure (r = 0.69).

Table 2.

Unstandardized regression coefficients and standard errors for models predicting sexual risk, alcohol use, and violence.

Pathways of Influence Sexual Risk Alcohol Use Violence
B (SE) p value B (SE) p value B (SE) p value
Direct Effects: Predictors
TV exposure 0.05 (0.03) 0.08 0.02 (0.01) <0.001 0.01 (0.004) =0.01
Movie exposure 0.10 (0.03) =0.001 0.04 (0.01) <0.001 0.01 (0.004) =0.001
Impulsivity 1.36 (0.21) <0.001 0.43 (0.08) <0.001 0.58 (0.07) <0.001
Sensation seeking 0.16 (0.07) =0.02 0.20 (0.03) <0.001 0.01 (0.02) 0.72
Direct Effects: Covariates B (SE) p value B (SE) p value B (SE) p value
Age 0.36 (0.05) <0.001 0.14 (0.02) <0.001 −0.05 (0.02) =0.006
Gender (Female =1) −0.61 (0.11) <0.001 −0.05 (0.04) 0.28 −0.17 (0.04) <0.001
Race (Black = 1) 0.003 (0.13) =0.98 −0.39 (0.05) <0.001 0.08 (0.04) =0.06
Maternal Education 0.02 (0.05) =0.66 0.002 (0.02) 0.91 −0.04 (0.02) =0.04
Parental Monitoring −0.17 (0.07) =0.02 −0.15 (0.02) <0.001 −0.08 (0.03) =0.001
TV time 0.01 (0.02) 0.38 0.01 (0.01) 0. 13 0.001 (0.01) 0.84
Alcohol use 1.05 (0.07) <0.001 0.19 (0.03) <0.001
Indirect Effects B (SE) p value B (SE) p value B (SE) p value
Impulsivity 0.10 (0.04) =0.007 0.06 (0.02) <0.001 0.13 (0.03) <0.001
Sensation Seeking 0.05 (0.01) =0.001 0.03 (0.01) <0.001 0.05 (0.01) <0.001
Moderated Effects B (SE) p value B (SE) p value B (SE) p value
Impulsivity*TV exposure 0.04 (0.01) <0.001
Impulsivity*movie exposure 0.04 (0.01) <0.001
Sensation seeking*TV exposure 0.01 (0.004) 0.01 0.01 (0.003) =0.05
Sensation seeking*movie exposure 0.01 (0.01) =0.05 0.01 (0.003) =0.03
Race*Sensation seeking −0.17 (0.04) <0.001

Note. Interaction effects were retained in the model only when significant. Indirect effects of impulsivity and sensation seeking reflect the total indirect effect, involving mediated pathways through TV exposure and movie exposure. In the moderation models, the interactions of TV exposure and Movie exposure with impulsivity and sensation seeking were tested in separate models. Shaded boxes represent effects non-significant at p < 0.05.

In case of alcohol use, we observed direct positive effects of TV and movie alcohol exposure as well as of impulsivity and SS. Some of the effect of impulsivity and SS was channeled through the association with media exposure to alcohol use. Additionally, SS moderated the effect of TV and movie alcohol exposure on drinking (see Table 2). Specifically, the association between media alcohol exposure and self-reported drinking frequency was significant only at high levels of SS (i.e., values ≥3; variable range = 1-5) (See Figures 1a & b). There was also a significant interaction between SS and race. SS was significantly positively associated with drinking frequency only in case of White, B(SE) = 0.20 (0.03), p < 0.001, but not Black youth, B (SE) = 0.03 (0.03), p = 0.36. Race did not moderate any of the other direct or mediated effects.

Figures 1a & 1b.

Figures 1a & 1b.

Interaction effects of media (TV and movie) exposure and sensation seeking (SS) on alcohol use.

For violence, we observed direct effects of TV and movie violence exposure as well as direct effects of impulsivity and SS. Some of the effects of impulsivity and SS on violence were channeled through their association with media exposure to violence. Both impulsivity and SS also moderated the effects of media violence exposure on violence, such that the association between media violence exposure (TV and movies) on violent behavior outcome was stronger for youth with high levels of impulsivity and SS as compared to those with lower levels. Specifically, the relation between media (TV and movie) exposure to violence and involvement in physical fights was significant only at high levels of impulsivity (i.e., values ≥ 0.4; variable range = 0-1) and SS (i.e., values ≥3; variable range = 1-5). (See Figures 23). No race differences were observed.

Figures 2a & 2b.

Figures 2a & 2b.

Interaction effects of media (TV and movie) exposure and impulsivity (IMP) on violence.

Figures 3a & 3b.

Figures 3a & 3b.

Interaction effects of media (TV and movies) exposure and sensation seeking (SS) on violence.

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to examine the role of individual differences in impulsivity and SS in understanding the association between media risk exposure and adolescent risk behaviors. Specifically, we tested if adolescents high in impulsivity and SS reported greater exposure to TV and movie risk content, and whether that in turn was associated with greater involvement in risky behaviors. We found that both impulsivity and SS were positively associated with media risk exposure, which in turn was positively related to real-world risk-taking. We also tested if individual differences in impulsivity and SS moderated the media exposure-risk behavior link such that the association between media exposure and corresponding risk behavior would be stronger at higher levels of impulsivity and SS than at lower levels. We found evidence for moderation in case of alcohol use (for SS only) and violence (for both impulsivity and SS), but not for sexual behaviors. In terms of race differences, we found that the positive association between SS and alcohol use was significant only in case of White youth, and not for Black youth. Race failed to moderate the direct effect of SS on sexual behaviors when controlling for the effect of alcohol use. No other race differences were observed. Overall, our findings suggest that prevention efforts aimed at reducing negative effects of risky media on adolescent risk behaviors could benefit from targeting youth high in impulsivity and SS.

Consistent with past studies (Bleakley, Hennessy, & Fishbein, 2011; Brown & L’Engle, 2009; Kim et al., 2006; Slater, 2003), adolescents high in impulsivity and SS reported greater media risk exposure, which in turn was positively associated with involvement in risk behaviors. The effects of impulsivity on sexual activity and violence were more pronounced than SS. In case of alcohol use, the direct effects of impulsivity and SS were comparable, likely because experimenting with alcohol use is a more normative type of adolescent risk-taking that is often associated with social rewards (Balsa, Homer, French, & Norton, 2011). For sexual behaviors and violence, lack of impulse control is expected to play a larger role in predicting behavior than SS (Khurana et al., 2012, 2015b).

Both impulsivity and SS moderated the effect of media risk exposure in case of violence. But for alcohol use, only SS moderated the effect of TV alcohol exposure. It is possible that because experimentation with alcohol use is socially accepted and normative, adolescents high in SS may be more strongly influenced by media depictions of drinking behaviors and engage in these behaviors for the rewarding component. Violent behaviors on the other hand are not as normative and are more likely to be influenced by a lack of self-control (i.e., inability to control aggressive tendencies) and/or a rewarding component (e.g., deviant peer group conformity). Studies find that chronic exposure to violent forms of media (e.g., videogames) is associated with aggressive/violent tendencies, but consistent with our findings, these effects are stronger for individuals with poor impulse control (Kronenberger et al., 2005). Therefore, adolescents with impulse control difficulties and/or strong reward sensitivity may be more likely to act on aggressive tendencies fueled by media violence exposure.

In terms of race differences, the direct effect of SS on alcohol use was significant only in case of White youth. This may be because White youth are more likely to experiment with alcohol and have greater access to alcohol than their Black counterparts (Blum et al., 2000). This finding also highlights the fact that even though SS exhibits a universal peak during adolescence (Khurana et al., 2018), the social contexts of adolescents can play a significant role in determining which behaviors adolescents actually engage in to satisfy their exploratory drives, as well as the health outcomes associated with these behaviors.

Adolescence is a period of rapid brain development, providing a window of plasticity to train weaknesses in impulse control. Several cognitive training interventions have yielded promising results in improving executive functions related to top-down control over behavior (Jaeggi, Buschkuehl, Jonides, & Shah, 2011), and targeted drinking behaviors (Houben, Wiers, & Jansen, 2011), however, no study to date has examined effects of cognitive training on adolescent impulsivity. This is a ripe area for future research, as similar interventions with younger children have reported protective effects of cognitive training on self-control (Diamond & Lee, 2011). Other family-based interventions (Dishion & Stormshak, 2007) and cognitive behavioral approaches (Heller, Pollack, Ander, & Ludwig, 2013) have also been used successfully to reduce impulsivity in adolescents. Although these findings need to be replicated, these are promising strategies to support adolescents who may be more vulnerable to the detrimental effects of media.

Parental monitoring of media use and content may be another strategy to protect youth high in impulsivity and SS from being exposed to risky content in the media (Collier et al., 2016). Perceived parental monitoring, which is an indicator of the family climate and parental involvement, operated as a significant protective influence in all our models. Parents could play an important role by communicating with their child about media content and de-glamorizing it, so that youth high in SS and/or impulsivity are less likely to enact these behaviors. Policy-level changes can also help decrease risky media exposure for youth by having stricter and clearer guidelines. For example, a change in ratings systems (from age-based to content-based) could enable parents in reducing exposure to risky media.

Limitations

Our findings should be interpreted in light of the following limitations. First, cross-sectional data is not ideal for testing mediational processes that hypothesize specific temporal sequence of events (Maxwell & Cole, 2007). Future research should examine temporality of these relations and explore reciprocal effect models. Previous studies have found that SS and impulsivity can predict exposure to risky media, and not the reverse, with the exception of videogaming literature where there is some evidence of excessive exposure to violence resulting in more impulsive behaviors (Bushman & Huesmann, 2006). Second, we only examined effects of TV and movie risk exposure; other forms of interactive media, including videogames, internet, and social media could not be explored. Their appeal to youth high in impulsivity and SS, as well as their effects on risk behaviors can vary as compared to TV and movie risk exposure. Relatedly, the high correlations between the media risk exposure variables (e.g., TV alcohol exposure and TV sex exposure) made it difficult to isolate the unique association of content-specific media risk exposure and the corresponding behavioral outcome. Third, our findings are based on a convenience sample of 14-17 years old U.S. adolescents who were recruited online, and cannot be generalized to other populations. Finally, our findings based on self-report data are susceptible to issues of recall bias and single informant bias.

Our results provide support for both uses and gratifications paradigm and differential susceptibility hypothesis (for alcohol and violence). High impulsivity and SS can increase risk for problem behaviors through greater exposure to risky media, and by operating as moderators, although the influence of SS on some forms of risk-taking (e.g., experimentation with alcohol use) seemed to be stronger for White youth than Black youth. Understanding these processes longitudinally and in the context of environmental influences (e.g., parental and peer influences) will be an important direction for future research. Impulsivity and SS are important characteristics to consider when investigating how exposure to specific media content affects corresponding behavior, and they offer a possible point of intervention. For instance, by employing brief screeners for impulsivity and SS, health practitioners and educators could identify youth who may be at greater risk for negative outcomes associated with media risk exposure. This type of preventive approach would facilitate early identification and intervention support for youth most in need.

Acknowledgments

Funding source: This study was funded by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) (Grant Number 1R21HD079615). Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the NICHD.

Footnotes

Disclosure of potential conflicts of interest: The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval: All procedures involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. All procedures were approved by the University of Pennsylvania Institutional Review Board. This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by any of the authors.

Informed Consent: Informed consent was obtained from all participants included in the study.

References

  1. Allen RL, Dawson MC, & Brown RE (1989). A schema-based approach to modeling an African-American racial belief system. American Political Science Review, 83(2), 421–441. 10.2307/1962398 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson CA, & Bushman BJ (2001). Effects of violent video games on aggressive behavior, aggressive cognition, aggressive affect, physiological arousal, and prosocial behavior: A meta-analytic review of the scientific literature. Psychological Science, 12(5), 353–359. 10.1111/1467-9280.00366 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Balsa AI, Homer JF, French MT, & Norton EC (2011). Alcohol use and popularity: social payoffs from conforming to peers’ behavior. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21(3), 559–568. 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00704.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bjork JM, & Pardini DA (2015). Who are those “risk-taking adolescents”? Individual differences in developmental neuroimaging research. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 56–64. 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.07.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bleakley A, Ellithorpe ME, Hennessy M, Khurana A, Jamieson P, & Weitz I (2017). Alcohol, sex, and screens: Modeling media influence on adolescent alcohol and sex cooccurrence. The Journal of Sex Research, 0(0), 1–12. 10.1080/00224499.2017.1279585 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bleakley A, Fishbein M, Hennessy M, Jordan A, Chernin A, & Stevens R (2008). Developing respondent-based multi-media measures of exposure to sexual content. Communication Methods and Measures, 2(1–2), 43–64. 10.1080/19312450802063040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bleakley A, Hennessy M, & Fishbein M (2011). A model of adolescents’ seeking of sexual content in their media choices. The Journal of Sex Research, 48(4), 309–315. 10.1080/00224499.2010.497985 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bleakley A, Hennessy M, Fishbein M, & Jordan A (2008). It works both ways: The relationship between exposure to sexual content in the media and adolescent sexual behavior. Media Psychology, 11(4), 443–461. 10.1080/15213260802491986 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bleakley A, Hennessy M, Fishbein M, & Jordan A (2011). Using the integrative model to explain how exposure to sexual media content influences adolescent sexual behavior. Health Education & Behavior, 38(5), 530–540. 10.1177/1090198110385775 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bleakley A, Jamieson PE, & Romer D (2012). Trends of sexual and violent content by gender in top-grossing U.S. films, 1950–2006. Journal of Adolescent Health, 51(1), 73–79. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.02.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bleakley A, Romer D, & Jamieson PE (2014). Violent film characters’ portrayal of alcohol, sex, and tobacco-related behaviors. Pediatrics, 133(1), 71–77. 10.1542/peds.2013-1922 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Blum RW, Beuhring T, Shew ML, Bearinger LH, Sieving RE, & Resnick MD (2000). The effects of race/ethnicity, income, and family structure on adolescent risk behaviors. American Journal of Public Health, 90(12), 1879–1884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Brown JD, & Bobkowski PS (2011). Older and newer media: Patterns of use and effects on adolescents’ health and well-being. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21(1), 95–113. 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00717.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brown JD, & L’Engle KL (2009). X-rated: sexual attitudes and behaviors associated with u.s. early adolescents’ exposure to sexually explicit media. Communication Research, 36(1), 129–151. 10.1177/0093650208326465 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Brown JD, L’Engle KL, Pardun CJ, Guo G, Kenneavy K, & Jackson C (2006). Sexy media matter: Exposure to sexual content in music, movies, television, and magazines predicts black and white adolescents’ sexual behavior. Pediatrics, 117(4), 1018–1027. 10.1542/peds.2005-1406 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bushman BJ, & Huesmann LR (2006). Short-term and long-term effects of violent media on aggression in children and adults. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 160(4), 348–352. 10.1001/archpedi.160.4.348 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2015). 1991–2015 High School Youth Risk Behavior Survey Data. [Google Scholar]
  18. Chory RM, & Goodboy AK (2010). Is basic personality related to violent and non-violent video game play and preferences? Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(4), 191–198. 10.1089/cyber.2010.0076 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Collado A, Felton JW, MacPherson L, & Lejuez CW (2014). Longitudinal trajectories of sensation seeking, risk taking propensity, and impulsivity across early to middle adolescence. Addictive Behaviors, 39(11), 1580–1588. https://doi.org/10.1016Zj.addbeh.2014.01.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Collier KM, Coyne SM, Rasmussen EE, Hawkins AJ, Padilla-Walker LM, Erickson SE, & Memmott-Elison MK (2016). Does parental mediation of media influence child outcomes? A meta-analysis on media time, aggression, substance use, and sexual behavior. Developmental Psychology, 52(5), 798–812. 10.1037/dev0000108 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Collins RL, Elliott MN, Berry SH, Kanouse DE, Kunkel D, Hunter SB, & Miu A (2004). Watching sex on television predicts adolescent initiation of sexual behavior. Pediatrics, 114(3), e280–e289. 10.1542/peds.2003-1065-L [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Diamond A, & Lee K (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science, 333(6045), 959–964. 10.1126/science.1204529 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dishion TJ, & Stormshak EA (2007). The ecological family intervention and therapy model In Dishion TJ & Stormshak EA, Intervening in children’s lives: An ecological, family-centered approach to mental health care. (pp. 49–67). Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association; Retrieved from http://psycnet.apa.org/index.cfm?fa=search.displayrecord&uid=2006-11734-004 [Google Scholar]
  24. Ellithorpe ME, & Bleakley A (2016). Wanting to see people like me? Racial and gender diversity in popular adolescent television. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 45(7), 1426–1437. 10.1007/s10964-016-0415-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Eysenck SBG, Easting G, & Pearson PR (1984). Age norms for impulsiveness, venturesomeness and empathy in children. Personality and Individual Differences, 5(3), 315–321. 10.1016/0191-8869(84)90070-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gentile DA (2008). The rating systems for media products In Calvert SL & Wilson BJ (Eds.), Handbooks in communication and media. The handbook of children, media, and development (pp. 527–551). Malden: Blackwell Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  27. Gibbons FX, Kingsbury JH, Wills TA, Finneran SD, Dal Cin S, & Gerrard M (2016). Impulsivity moderates the effects of movie alcohol portrayals on adolescents’ willingness to drink. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 30(3), 325–334. 10.1037/adb0000178 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hanewinkel R, & Sargent JD (2009). Longitudinal study of exposure to entertainment media and alcohol use among german adolescents. Pediatrics, 123(3), 989–995. 10.1542/peds.2008-1465 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hayes AF, & Krippendorff K (2007). Answering the call for a standard reliability measure for coding data. Communication Methods and Measures, 1(1), 77–89. doi: 10.1080/19312450709336664 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Heller S, Pollack HA, Ander R, & Ludwig J (2013). Preventing youth violence and dropout: A randomizedfield experiment (Working Paper No. 19014). National Bureau of Economic Research; Retrieved from http://www.nber.org/papers/w19014 [Google Scholar]
  31. Hennessy M, Bleakley A, Fishbein M, & Jordan A (2008). Validating an index of adolescent sexual behavior using psychosocial theory and social trait correlates. AIDS and Behavior, 12(2), 321–331. 10.1007/s10461-007-9272-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hennessy M, Bleakley A, Fishbein M, & Jordan A (2009). Estimating the longitudinal association between adolescent sexual behavior and exposure to sexual media content. The Journal of Sex Research, 46(6), 586–596. 10.1080/00224490902898736 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Houben K, Wiers RW, & Jansen A (2011). Getting a grip on drinking behavior training working memory to reduce alcohol abuse. Psychological Science, 22(7), 968–975. 10.1177/0956797611412392 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hoyle RH, Stephenson MT, Palmgreen P, Lorch EP, & Donohew RL (2002). Reliability and validity of a brief measure of sensation seeking. Personality and Individual Differences, 32(3), 401–414. 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00032-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. Iacono WG, Malone SM, & McGue M (2008). Behavioral disinhibition and the development of early-onset addiction: Common and specific influences. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4(1), 325–348. 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.4.022007.141157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Jaccard J, & Turrisi R (2003). Interaction Effects in Multiple Regression. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [Google Scholar]
  37. Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, & Shah P (2011). Short- and long-term benefits of cognitive training. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 10.1073/pnas.1103228108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Jamieson P, & Romer D (2008). The Changing Portrayal of Adolescents in the Media Since 1950. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  39. Katz E, Blumler J, & Gurevitch M (1974). Utilization of mass communication by the individual In Blumler J & Katz E (Eds.), The uses of mass communication: current perspectives on gratifications research (pp. 19–32). Beverly Hills: Sage. [Google Scholar]
  40. Kerr M, Stattin H, & Burk WJ (2010). A reinterpretation of parental monitoring in longitudinal perspective. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 20(1), 39–64. 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2009.00623.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Khurana A, Romer D, Betancourt LM, Brodsky NL, Giannetta JM, & Hurt H (2015a). Experimentation versus progression in adolescent drug use: A test of an emerging neurobehavioral imbalance model. Development and Psychopathology, 27(3), 901–913. 10.1017/S0954579414000765 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Khurana A, Romer D, Betancourt LM, Brodsky NL, Giannetta JM, & Hurt H (2015b). Stronger working memory reduces sexual risk taking in adolescents, even after controlling for parental influences. Child Development, 86(4), 1125–1141. 10.1111/cdev.12383 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Khurana A, Romer D, Betancourt LM, & Hurt H (2017). Working memory ability and early drug use progression as predictors of adolescent substance use disorders. Addiction. 10.1111/add.13792 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Khurana A, Romer D, Betancourt L, & Hurt H (2018). Modeling trajectories of sensation seeking and impulsivity dimensions from early to late adolescence: Universal trends or distinct sub-groups? Journal of Youth and Adolescence [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Khurana A, Romer D, Hurt H, Betancourt L, Brodsky NL, & Giannetta JM (2012). Early adolescent sexual debut: The mediating role of working memory ability, sensation seeking and impulsivity. Developmental Psychology, 48(5), 1416–1428. 10.1037/a0027491 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kim JL, Collins RL, Kanouse DE, Elliott MN, Berry SH, Hunter SB, … Kunkel D (2006). Sexual readiness, household policies, and other predictors of adolescents’ exposure to sexual content in mainstream entertainment television. Media Psychology, 8(4), 449–471. 10.1207/s1532785xmep0804_6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. Krcmar M, & Kean LG (2005). Uses and gratifications of media violence: Personality correlates of viewing and liking violent genres. Media Psychology, 7(4), 399–420. 10.1207/S1532785XMEP0704_5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kronenberger WG, Mathews VP, Dunn DW, Wang Y, Wood EA, Giauque AL, … Li T-Q (2005). Media violence exposure and executive functioning in aggressive and control adolescents. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 61(6), 725–737. 10.1002/jclp.20022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Lenhart A (2015). Teens, Social Media & Technology Overview 2015. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center; Retrieved from http://www.pewinternet.org/2015/04/09/teens-social-media-technology-2015/ [Google Scholar]
  50. MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, & Williams J (2004). Confidence limits for the indirect effect: Distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39(1), 99–128. 10.1207/s15327906mbr3901_4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Maxwell SE, & Cole DA (2007). Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation. Psychological Methods, 12(1), 23–44. 10.1037/1082-989X.12.1.23 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky D, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington H, … Caspi A (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 2693–2698. 10.1073/pnas.1010076108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Patton JH, Stanford MS, & Barratt ES (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51(6), 768–774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Raine A, Reynolds C, Venables PH, & Mednick SA (2002). Stimulation seeking and intelligence: A prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(4), 663–674. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Rideout V (2016). Measuring time spent with media: The common sense census of media use by us 8- to 18-year-olds. Journal of Children and Media, 10(1), 138–144. 10.1080/17482798.2016.1129808 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  56. Rideout V, Foehr UG, & Roberts DF (2010). Generation M2: Media in the Lives of 8 to 18 Year Olds. Menlo Park, CA: Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation; Retrieved from http://kff.org/other/event/generation-m2-media-in-the-lives-of/ [Google Scholar]
  57. Romer D, Betancourt L, Giannetta JM, Brodsky NL, Farah M, & Hurt H (2011). Does adolescent risk-taking imply weak executive function? A prospective study of relations between working memory performance, impulsivity, and risk-taking in early adolescence. Developmental Science, 14(5), 1119–1133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Romer D, & Hennessy M (2007). A biosocial-affect model of adolescent sensation seeking: the role of affect evaluation and peer-group influence in adolescent drug use. Prevention Science, 8(2), 89–101. 10.1007/s11121-007-0064-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Rubin AM (2009). Uses and Gratifications: An evolving perspective of media effects In Nabi RL & Oliver MB (Eds.), The SAGE Handbook of Media Processes and Effects (pp. 147–160). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE. [Google Scholar]
  60. Schooler D, Monique Ward L, Merriwether A, & Caruthers A (2004). Who’s that girl: television’s role in the body image development of young white and black women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 28(1), 38–47. 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2004.00121.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  61. Sheridan E (2006). Conservative implications of the irrelevance of racism in contemporary african american cinema. Journal of Black Studies, 37(2), 177–192. [Google Scholar]
  62. Slater MD (2003). Alienation, aggression, and sensation seeking as predictors of adolescent use of violent film, computer, and website content. Journal of Communication, 53(1), 105–121. 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2003.tb03008.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  63. Slater MD, Henry KL, Swaim RC, & Cardador JM (2004). Vulnerable teens, vulnerable times: How sensation seeking, alienation, and victimization moderate the violent media content–aggressiveness relation. Communication Research, 31(6), 642–668. 10.1177/0093650204269265 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  64. Strasburger VC, Jordan AB, & Donnerstein E (2010). Health effects of media on children and adolescents. Pediatrics, 125(4), 756–767. 10.1542/peds.2009-2563 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Valkenburg PM, & Peter J (2013). The differential susceptibility to media effects model. Journal of Communication, 63(2), 221–243. 10.1111/jcom.12024 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  66. van Schuur WH (2003). Mokken scale analysis: Between the guttman scale and parametric item response theory. Political Analysis, 11(2), 139–163. [Google Scholar]
  67. Wills TA, Gibbons FX, Sargent JD, Gerrard M, Lee H-R, & Dal Cin S (2010). Good self-control moderates the effect of mass media on adolescent tobacco and alcohol use: Tests with studies of children and adolescents. Health Psychology, 29(5), 539–549. 10.1037/a0020818 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Wills TA, Sargent JD, Gibbons FX, Forbes M, Tappe M, Cin SD, & Knight R (2012). Movie alcohol exposure and adolescent alcohol use in a multiethnic sample in hawaii. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research, 36. [Google Scholar]
  69. Zuckerman M (2006). Sensation Seeking and Risky Behavior. APA Publications, Washington D.C. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES