Abstract
Objective:
We tested two hypotheses regarding age at onset within familial epilepsies: (1) family members with epilepsy tend to have similar ages at onset, independent of epilepsy syndrome; (2) age at onset is younger in successive generations after controlling for sampling bias.
Methods:
We analyzed clinical data collected by the Epi4K Consortium (303 multiplex families, 1,120 individuals). To test hypothesis 1, we used both linear mixed models commonly used for heritability analysis and Cox regression models with frailty terms to assess clustering of onset within families after controlling for other predictors. To test hypothesis 2, we used mixed effects models, pair-wise analyses, and survival analysis to address sampling-related bias that may mimic anticipation.
Results:
(1) Age at seizure onset was significantly heritable (ICC 0.17, p < 0.001) after adjusting for epilepsy type, sex, site, history of febrile seizure, and age at last observation. This finding remained significant after adjusting for epilepsy syndromes, and was robust across statistical methods in all families and in generalized families. (2) The mean age at onset decreased in successive generations (p < 0.001). After adjusting for age at last observation, this effect was not significant in mixed-effects models (p=0.14), but remained significant in pair-wise (p=0.0003) and survival analyses (p=0.02).
Interpretation:
Age at seizure onset is an independent familial trait, and may have genetic determinants distinct from the determinants of particular epilepsy syndromes. Younger onsets in successive generations can be explained in part by sampling bias, but the presence of genetic anticipation cannot be excluded.
INTRODUCTION
Epilepsy is a disorder with strong genetic contributions. Relatives of people with epilepsy have an increased incidence of epilepsy, even in families without Mendelian (monogenic) patterns of inheritance.1,2 Moreover, studies of twins and families have shown that specific features of epilepsy are themselves heritable traits, including specific epilepsy syndromes,3,4 seizure types and symptoms,5,6 and EEG patterns.7–9 These features may have genetic determinants that are distinct from the genetic determinants of epilepsy per se.
Age at epilepsy onset is variable across individuals, even those with the same epilepsy syndrome.10–12 Whether this variability in age at onset is (at least in part) genetically determined remains unknown. In this study, using data from a large cohort of multiplex families with epilepsy,13 we hypothesized that age at epilepsy onset is in part a heritable trait. This analysis must account for epilepsy syndromes, which often have characteristic ages of onset and which tend to aggregate in families. Thus, family members might have similar ages of onset because they share the same syndrome. If, however, family members have similar ages of onset after adjusting for epilepsy syndrome, this would suggest that age at onset is itself heritable.
In a separate analysis, we observed in these families that age at onset tends to decrease across successive generations. This effect may be due to anticipation, a known genetic phenomenon, or may be a spurious finding due to sampling-related bias.14–19 For example, early ages of onset may be over-represented in later generations because they are younger at the time of the study; conversely, older ages of onset may be over-represented in older generations because childhood epilepsies that have long-since resolved may be forgotten and not ascertained. We sought to determine whether these forms of sampling bias are present in our data and, if so, whether they completely account for the observed association between generation and age at epilepsy onset.
METHODS
Data Collection
Ascertainment methods and data collection have been described in detail.13 Briefly, each family contained three or more relatives with unprovoked seizures. Relatives with epilepsies of known acquired causes, such as structural brain lesions, were not included. Once a family was ascertained, data on every affected relative were obtained by direct interview, sourcing of available records or by interviewing reliably family informants. Ascertainment and data collection took place at 7 centers in North America, Europe, Australia and New Zealand, and has occurred gradually at each center over many years. For the Epi4K study, all data were systematically reviewed, entered into a detailed database, and cross-validated by study personnel at multiple sites. Families with known causal mutations were excluded.
During this review process, each individual was assigned to one of five epilepsy types (generalized, focal, combined generalized/focal, febrile seizures plus (FS+), or unknown) by an expert clinician. Those individuals with generalized or focal epilepsy were further assigned to epilepsy subtypes, including recognized epilepsy syndromes.20 The subtypes of generalized epilepsy were early onset absence epilepsy, childhood absence epilepsy, juvenile absence epilepsy, childhood/juvenile absence epilepsy indistinguishable, juvenile myoclonic epilepsy, generalized tonic-clonic seizures alone, “severe” generalized epilepsy (including myoclonic atonic epilepsy, absence epilepsy with eyelid myoclonia, myoclonic absence epilepsy, and one case of Lennox-Gastaut syndrome of unknown cause) and other generalized epilepsy. The subtypes of focal epilepsy were temporal lobe epilepsy, frontal lobe epilepsy, posterior quadrant epilepsy (including parietal and occipital lobes), self-limited focal epilepsy of childhood, and unlocalized focal epilepsy. After epilepsy types and subtypes were assigned to individuals, each family was assigned to one of four categories (generalized, focal, generalized epilepsy with febrile seizures plus (GEFS+), or mixed) as previously described.13
Age at epilepsy onset was defined as the age at first unprovoked seizure. Provoked seizures and febrile seizures were not included in this definition. Age at last observation was the age at the time of direct interview, informant interview, or last medical documentation available for review, whichever was last. For all family members, this reflects the end of an age window beyond which the subject could not have been observed to have onset of epilepsy. In a few cases where information about a subject was collected after his or her death, by interview of family members, we defined the last observation as the age of death. Generations within each pedigree were counted relative to the youngest generation, to account for pedigrees of various sizes across the cohort. Information about age was not collected for family members without epilepsy.
The Epi4K study was approved by the research ethics committee at each participating site and all subjects provided informed consent to participate. The current analysis used only deidentified data from that study, therefore separate ethics approval was not required.
Statistical Analysis
We conducted two sets of analyses, testing two distinct hypotheses: (1) that age at epilepsy onset correlates within families after accounting for other predictors, suggesting age at onset is a heritable trait; (2) that age at onset is younger in successive generations after controlling for sampling biases, suggesting that genetic anticipation may be present.
To test hypothesis 1, we constructed linear mixed effects models that modeled the known contributors as fixed effects and shared familial effects as random effects. To model random familial effects, we took the standard approach and assumed such effects followed a multivariate normal distribution with mean vector 0 and covariance matrix equivalent to twice the kinship matrix multiplied by the estimated variance due to polygenes. Fixed effects included sex, ascertainment site, history of febrile seizure, epilepsy type, and age at last observation. The fixed effect “epilepsy type” was first defined broadly for each individual (generalized, focal, combined, FS+, unknown). In separate models including only those families with generalized or focal epilepsies, we also included the applicable epilepsy subtypes or syndromes as fixed effects. Prior to analysis, we performed an inverse-normal transformation on age at onset to satisfy the assumptions of linear mixed effects models (repeating the analysis with untransformed age at onset values produced a qualitatively similar pattern of results).
Intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC) calculated from these mixed effects models were estimates of the proportion of variability in age at onset that is attributable to shared familial effects after first adjusting for the fixed effect variables. We tested the significance of the shared familial effects using a likelihood-ratio statistic, which compared the full model that included the random familial effect with the null model that excluded the random familial effect. Collinearity among independent variables was evaluated by variance inflation factor method and was not substantially present in the models.
As an alternative approach to testing hypothesis 1, we also performed a Cox proportional hazard (survival) analysis that accounted for right truncation of the distribution of onset ages, which must be less than age at last observation. We first reversed the time axis to convert right truncation into left truncation.21,22 We then constructed Cox proportional hazard models using the same fixed effects as the mixed-effects model above while modelling the random (shared) family effects using a frailty term following a gamma distribution. The significance of the shared-frailty term in these models tests the hypothesis that age at onset aggregates within families, while adjusting for the covariates and truncation bias.
To test hypothesis 2, we performed a series of analyses aimed at distinguishing the relative contributions of sampling bias versus genetic anticipation in producing younger onsets over successive generations within families. First, for the entire cohort, we constructed a mixed-effects model using age at onset as the outcome variable, generation as the independent variable (fixed effect), and family as a random effect. We then added age at last observation as an additional fixed-effect independent variable, and assessed the relative contributions of generation versus age at last observation as predictors of age at onset.
Next, following studies exploring anticipation versus sampling bias in other disorders,15–17 we identified affected parent-child pairs for further analysis. We included only one nuclear family per pedigree; following the procedure of Tsai et al16 we selected the largest nuclear family within each pedigree or, in case of ties, randomly selected one nuclear family per pedigree. To test whether children have earlier onset than parents while accounting for multiple affected parent-child pairs within families, we used a mixed-effect models to regress delta age at onset (i.e., Onsetparent – Onsetchild) on the model intercept, treating family as random effect. We repeated these pairwise analyses in the subset of “comparable” parent-child pairs,15,16 being those pairs in which both members’ age at last observation was older than both members’ age at onset. Conceptually, this excludes pairs in which the parent’s seizure onset occurs at an older age than the child was ascertained or last observed, because in that circumstance the child’s onset can only be earlier than the parent’s. This approach reduces bias due to right truncation, although it does so with some limitations and does not reduce other kinds of bias (see Discussion).
Finally, we repeated the Cox proportional hazards model with frailty terms in all families as described above, adding generation to the model as a covariate. The significance of this generation term in the model tests the association between generation and age at onset while adjusting for other fixed effects, familial aggregation, and truncation bias.
Analyses were performed using Stata and the R programming language. The R packages Kinship2 and FamAgg were used for pedigree analysis. Mixed-effects modeling was carried out in Stata and in R using the lmekin function within the coxme package. Survival analyses were performed in R using the survival package.
RESULTS
Clinical characteristics
The characteristics of this cohort have been described in detail.13 In summary, the cohort includes 1,120 individuals with epilepsy from 303 families. Families contained 3–8 affected individuals. Age at epilepsy onset was available for 1,064 subjects (Table 1), and complete data were available for 1,008 subjects from 300 families. The distribution of age at onset was skewed, with many childhood onset cases and relatively fewer adult onset cases, consistent with the known epidemiology of familial epilepsies.2 Focal epilepsy subtypes had more variability in age at onset compared to generalized epilepsy subtypes, as expected.
Table 1.
Age at onset (yrs) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Epilepsy Type/Subtype | n | Mean (±SD) | Median | Range |
Generalized | 492 | 9.5 (±6.3) | 9 | 0.1 – 40 |
- Early onset absence | 63 | 2.1 (±0.8) | 2 | 0.6 – 3 |
- Childhood absence (CAE)a | 106 | 6.0 (±1.8) | 6 | 1.5 – 8 |
- CAE/JAE indistinguishablea | 44 | 10.0 (±1.4) | 10 | 3 – 13 |
- Juvenile absence (JAE) | 36 | 13.8 (±2.0) | 13.5 | 10 – 18 |
- Juvenile myoclonic | 111 | 14.6 (±2.8) | 14 | 9 – 24 |
- Generalized tonic-clonic alone | 31 | 13.7 (±3.7) | 14 | 7 – 23 |
- Severe generalized | 24 | 2.9 (±2.1) | 2 | 0.8 – 8 |
- Other generalized | 77 | 11.2 (10.0) | 8 | 0.1 – 40 |
Focal | 313 | 13.8 (±11.7) | 11 | 0.1 – 84 |
- Temporal | 128 | 18.1 (±12.5) | 15 | 0.5 – 84 |
- Frontal | 27 | 9.6 (±8.5) | 6 | 0.5 – 37 |
- Posterior quadrant | 64 | 10.9 (±7.2) | 10.5 | 0.3 – 30 |
- Self-limited focal epilepsy | 43 | 6.3 (±2.8) | 6 | 1.3 – 15 |
- Unlocalized | 51 | 15.2 (15.4) | 12 | 0.1 – 68 |
Combined generalized/focal | 61 | 8.3 (±6.6) | 7 | 0.1 – 35 |
Febrile seizures plus | 16 | 4.9 (±3.3) | 5 | 0.1 – 7 |
Unknown epilepsy type | 182 | 15.9 (±12.2) | 14 | 0.1 – 80 |
Total | 1,064 | 11.7 (±9.7) | 10 | 0.1 – 84 |
A few participants with otherwise typical onset ages of CAE and CAE/JAE overlap had single unprovoked tonic-clonic seizures at younger ages and this initial seizure age was taken as age at onset.
Correlation of age at onset within families
Mixed effects models showed a significant correlation among family members’ ages of onset after adjusting for sex, ascertainment site, history of febrile seizure, epilepsy type, and age at last observation (Table 2). The results were similar when the analysis was carried out within strata defined by broad family epilepsy type (generalized families and focal families). When the stratified analysis was repeated with additional adjustment for the epilepsy subtypes and syndromes of the individual subjects (listed in Methods and in Table 1), the magnitude of the ICC decreased, suggesting that some of the familial correlation in age at onset is explained by familial clustering of these subtypes. Nonetheless, these models still demonstrated a significant family contribution to age at onset after adjusting for familial aggregation of epilepsy syndromes and subtypes.
Table 2.
We also performed shared-frailty survival analysis to account explicitly for right truncation of our observed distribution of age at onset. Testing the variance of the family-based frailty term, we continued to observe significant evidence of familial correlation of age of onset in all families (variance = 0.17, X2(102) = 183.6, p<0.001) and generalized families (variance = 0.24, X2(38) = 71.5, p<0.001) after adjustment for syndromes. However, after adjustment for syndrome, familial correlation in focal families did not reach significance (variance = 0.10, X2(11) = 16.6, p=0.11).
Earlier onset in successive generations
We observed in these data that younger generations had earlier age at onset compared to older generations (Figure 1A). To explore qualitatively the contribution of sampling bias to this observed association, we plotted age at last observation versus age at epilepsy onset (Figure 1B) which demonstrated evidence for two sources of bias. First, age at onset logically cannot be greater than age at last observation (creating the diagonal “ceiling” effect in the plot), so subjects with young ages at last observation must by definition have young ages at onset. Second, among subjects with older ages at last observation, young ages at onset are relatively under-represented. Because age at last observation is strongly related to generation (colors in Figure 1B), these two sampling-related effects contribute to the observed association between generation and age at onset.
We tested the association between generation and age at onset in a simple mixed-effects model, using family as a random effect to account for relatedness. Generation was significantly associated with age at onset (slope estimate 6.2, 95% CI 5.0, 7.4, p < 0.001). When age at last observation was added to the model as a second predictor of age at onset, generation was no longer significantly associated with age at onset (slope estimate −1.3, 95% CI −3.1, 0.3, p = 0.14), whereas age at last observation was significantly associated with age at onset (slope estimate 0.32, 95% CI 0.27, 0.36, p < 0.001). These slope estimates indicate change in age at onset (in years) per one unit change in the predictor variable.
Next, we performed pairwise analysis of affected parent-child pairs, using analyses that selected one nuclear family per pedigree and adjusted for multiple pairs within families (Table 3). In all eligible pairs the mean difference in age at onset between parent and child was 6.4 years (95% CI 4.6, 8.2). When we repeated these analyses in only “comparable” parent-child pairs, the mean difference in age at onset between parent and child was reduced to 2.3 years (95% CI 1.0, 3.5) but this difference was still significantly greater than zero (p = 0.0003).
Table 3.
Pair type | N of pairs | Δ onset age in yrs, mean (95% CI) | t-value | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
All pairs | 287 | 6.4 (4.6, 8.2) | 6.9 | <0.0001 |
“Comparable” pairs | 217 | 2.3 (1.0, 3.5) | 3.7 | 0.0003 |
Finally, we repeated the survival analysis in all families, adding generation as a covariate. The generation term was significantly associated with age at onset (X2(1) = 5.1, p = 0.02) despite adjusting for other covariates, familial aggregation, and truncation bias.
DISCUSSION
In this study, using data from a large cohort of families with common epilepsies, we tested two hypotheses related to age at epilepsy onset within families. First, we tested whether age at onset aggregates within families. The results indicated significant correlation of age at onset within families after controlling for multiple potential predictors of age at onset, including epilepsy types (e.g. generalized or focal) and more specific epilepsy subtypes and syndromes (e.g. absence epilepsies, juvenile myoclonic epilepsy). The findings suggest that age at onset has familial determinants, independent of epilepsy syndrome. Second, we tested whether age at onset decreases in successive generations. We found that much of this effect disappeared after controlling for sampling bias, but some evidence for declining age remained, suggesting a role for genetic anticipation in at least some families.
In testing the hypothesis that age at onset correlates within families, it was particularly important to account for epilepsy syndromes. Some syndromes have characteristic ages at onset (e.g. childhood absence epilepsy; juvenile myoclonic epilepsy), and these syndromes tend to aggregate within families, so correlation of age at onset within families could be partly explained by familial aggregation of these syndromes. We found that family membership explains a significant portion of the variability of age at onset after accounting for epilepsy syndromes. Conceptually, this implies that among families with a given syndrome (e.g. JME), some will tend to have onset at a younger age (e.g. 12 years) while others will tend to have onset at an older age (e.g. 18 years), and this age at onset is a heritable feature of each family’s epilepsy, independent of the syndrome they share. This finding was robust to multiple statistical approaches in the generalized epilepsies; in the focal epilepsies the effect was smaller, and in one statistical approach did not reach statistical significance. We note that decreasing age at onset in successive generations, while not incompatible with familial aggregation, tends to make family members’ onset ages less similar to one another. To the extent that this generational effect is an artifact of sampling bias (see below), our estimates of intrafamilial aggregation may underestimate the true heritability of age at onset.
Prior literature has hinted at the familial nature of age at epilepsy onset. For example, a study of twins with febrile convulsions found a correlation in their ages at onset.23 A study of families with genetic generalized epilepsy (formerly idiopathic generalized epilepsy) found that age at onset tends to cluster within families, but the analysis did not account for other predictors of age at onset and the authors acknowledged that shared epilepsy syndromes within families may have explained their observation.24 Age at onset is a heritable trait in other familial neurocognitive disorders, including major depressive disorder,26 essential tremor,27 and frontotemporal dementia,28 but this effect has not previously been fully explored in epilepsy.
Our findings suggest that age at seizure onset may be determined at least in part by genetic factors, and that these genetic determinants may be distinct from the genetic factors that lead to the development of epilepsy per se. The nature of these genetic determinants is presently unknown. One possibility is that the genetic factors that determine the time course and trajectory of neurodevelopment (including the maturation of neuronal networks, changes in the expression of different ion channel subunits, etc.) interact with the genetic factors that cause (or confer risk of) epilepsy. Under this model, age at seizure onset likely reflects the interaction of these overlapping but distinct processes and their respective genetic determinants. A less likely possibility is that age at seizure onset has its own set of genetic determinants, independent of the genetic determinants of epilepsy. Future studies will be needed to better understand the mechanisms of these complex processes.
These observations are relevant to syndrome diagnosis and definitions. Age at onset is considered a crucial piece of data in diagnosing some syndromes, particularly in childhood and adolescence, and in defining the nosological boundaries of those syndromes. For example, there are arguments as to whether one can diagnose juvenile myoclonic epilepsy in a nine-year-old or a 22-year-old, when the syndrome is classically regarded as beginning between 12 and 18 years.29,30 The distinction between childhood absence epilepsy (typical onset 4–8 years) and juvenile absence epilepsy (typical onset 12–18 years) can be blurred when patients present between these typical ages. Our finding that age at epilepsy onset is determined in part by familial factors independent of epilepsy syndromes sheds light on this debate, and argues for looser rather than tighter age-based definitions.
In our second analysis, we observed that successive generations within these families tended to have earlier ages at onset. This raises the possibility of anticipation, a well-known genetic phenomenon. Anticipation has previously been suggested in families with epilepsy.31 Repeat expansions, the best known mechanism of anticipation,32,33 are not common causes of epilepsy, but were recently identified as the mechanism of benign adult familial myoclonic epilepsy,34 and may be a more common mechanism of epilepsy than previously thought. Parental mosaicism, another potential cause of anticipation,35 is common in severe pediatric epilepsies36 and could also be present in milder forms of epilepsy. Additional mechanisms of anticipation may be yet undiscovered.
However, the appearance of anticipation may also be a spurious finding due to various forms of bias:14–19 (1) later onset ages will be underrepresented in the youngest generation, who are ascertained at young ages; (2) early onset epilepsies will be underrepresented among older generations, due to reduced reproductive fitness in severe cases or the resolution of mild childhood epilepsies; (3) ascertainment of multiplex families may be biased toward families with adult onset epilepsies in parents and childhood onset epilepsies in offspring, rather than the reverse (see Figure 2); (4) parents with subtle seizure types (e.g. absence) may recognize these seizures earlier in their children than they were recognized in the parents themselves.
We found evidence of bias within our data (see Figure 1 and accompanying text) and used several statistical approaches to address it. Analysis of “comparable” parent-child pairs15,16 reduced (but did not eliminate) a significant difference in onset ages between parents and children. Separately, a survival analysis adjusting for right truncation showed a significant association between generation and age at onset. These approaches address right truncation caused by ascertainment of children at younger ages than their parents, but they do not address other forms of bias. On balance, our interpretation is that the generation effect observed across this large cohort results largely from sampling-related bias, but the presence of anticipation in some families with epilepsy remains possible. The search for a possible mechanism, such as the recently shown repeat expansion in benign adult familial myoclonic epilepsy,34 may be worthwhile.
Our study has several limitations. Ascertainment of families occurred over time and retrospectively. Subjects’ memories of the age at which their epilepsy began may have been inaccurate. We attempted to collect medical records wherever possible, but these were not always available. Similarly, assignment of epilepsy types and subtypes was based on all available information about seizure types and EEG findings, but this information was sometimes limited and some patients could not be confidently classified. Retrospective ascertainment also creates the potential for biases that may lead to spurious findings.25 We have attempted to control for these biases in several ways: with mixed-effects models that adjust for age at last observation; with survival models that adjust for right truncation; and with pairwise analyses limited to comparable pairs. However, bias is best addressed through study design, and our findings should be confirmed by prospective studies that ascertain and follow both affected and at risk individuals over time, rather than through retrospective ascertainment of affected cases. Finally, while we cannot exclude the possibility that shared environmental (rather than genetic) factors contributed to similar ages at onset within families, this seems unlikely to be the major cause of our findings because there are no known environmental determinants of age at epilepsy onset and because the familial effect was present at every site of ascertainment.
In conclusion, our findings suggest that age at seizure onset is a familial trait, independent of epilepsy syndrome, and may have genetic determinants that are distinct from the genetic determinants of epilepsy per se. An apparent anticipation effect in these data is due largely to sampling bias, although anticipation may exist in some families with epilepsy. Future studies should confirm these findings and seek to clarify the underlying mechanisms that determine the age at which epilepsy occurs.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements:
We thank all individuals, including patients and their families, who participated in the study, as well as the clinicians and research staff at many institutions who contributed to the study.
The Epi4K family study was supported by an NINDS National Institute of Health grant (ID: U01NS077367). S.F.B. was supported by an NHMRC Program Grant (ID: 628952). R.O. was supported by NIH grants R01 NS078419 and P50 HG007257. M.P.E. was supported by NIH grant R01 GM117946. C.A.E. was supported in part by a Ruth L. Kirschstein National Research Service Award (NRSA) Institutional Research Training Grant, T32 NS091008–01. S.X.X. is supported by NIH grant R01-NS102324.
Footnotes
Potential Conflicts of Interest:
Nothing to report.
REFERENCES
- 1.Annegers JF, Hauser WA, Anderson VE, Kurland LT. The risks of seizure disorders among relatives of patients with childhood onset epilepsy. Neurology 1982;32(2):174–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Peljto AL, Barker-Cummings C, Vasoli VM, et al. Familial risk of epilepsy: a population-based study. Brain 2014;137(3):795–805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Berkovic SF, Howell RA, Hay DA, Hopper JL. Epilepsies in twins: Genetics of the major epilepsy syndromes. Ann. Neurol 1998;43(4):435–445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Vadlamudi L, Milne RL, Lawrence K, et al. Genetics of epilepsy: The testimony of twins in the molecular era. Neurology 2014;83(12):1042–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Winawer MR, Marini C, Grinton BE, et al. Familial clustering of seizure types within the idiopathic generalized epilepsies. Neurology 2005;65(4):523–528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Tobochnik S, Fahlstrom R, Shain C, et al. Familial aggregation of focal seizure semiology in the Epilepsy Phenome/Genome Project. Neurology 2017;89(1):22–28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Metrakos K, Metrakos JD. Genetics of convulsive disorders. II. Genetic and electroencephalographic studies in centrencephalic epilepsy. Neurology 1961;11:474–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.van Beijsterveldt CE, Molenaar PC, de Geus EJ, Boomsma DI. Heritability of human brain functioning as assessed by electroencephalography. Am. J. Hum. Genet 1996;58(3):562–73. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Waltz S, Stephani U. Inheritance of Photosensitivity. Neuropediatrics 2000;31(2):82–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Roger J, Bureau M, Dravet C, Genton P, Tassinari CA W P, editor. Epileptic syndromes in infancy, childhood and adolescence 4th Editio London: John Libbey Eurotext; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Jallon P, Latour P. Epidemiology of Idiopathic Generalized Epilepsies. Epilepsia 2005;46(s9):10–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Reichsoellner J, Larch J, Unterberger I, et al. Idiopathic generalised epilepsy of late onset: a separate nosological entity? J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2010;81(11):1218–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Epi4K Consortium. Phenotypic analysis of 303 multiplex families with common epilepsies. Brain 2017;140(8):2144–2156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hodge SE, Wickramaratne P. Statistical pitfalls in detecting age-of-onset anticipation: the role of correlation in studying anticipation and detecting ascertainment bias. Psychiatr. Genet 1995;5(1):43–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Rabinowitz D, Yang Q. Testing for age-at-onset anticipation with affected parent-child pairs. Biometrics 1999;55(3):834–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Tsai WY, Heiman GA, Hodge SE. New simple tests for age-at-onset anticipation: application to panic disorder. Genet. Epidemiol 2005;28(3):256–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Boonstra PS, Gruber SB, Raymond VM, et al. A review of statistical methods for testing genetic anticipation: looking for an answer in Lynch syndrome. Genet. Epidemiol 2010;34(7):756–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Fraser FC. Trinucleotide repeats not the only cause of anticipation. Lancet 1997;350(9076):459–460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Vieland VJ, Huang J. Statistical evaluation of age-at-onset anticipation: a new test and evaluation of its behavior in realistic applications. Am. J. Hum. Genet 1998;62(5):1212–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Scheffer IE, Berkovic S, Capovilla G, et al. ILAE classification of the epilepsies: Position paper of the ILAE Commission for Classification and Terminology. Epilepsia 2017;58(4):512–521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Klein JP, Moeschberger ML. Survival Analysis : Techniques for Censored and Truncated Data 2nd Edition New York: Springer; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Austin PC. A Tutorial on Multilevel Survival Analysis: Methods, Models and Applications. Int. Stat. Rev 2017;85(2):185–203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Tsuboi T, Endo S. Genetic studies of febrile convulsions: analysis of twin and family data. Epilepsy Res. Suppl 1991;4:119–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kinirons P, Rabinowitz D, Gravel M, et al. Phenotypic concordance in 70 families with IGE-implications for genetic studies of epilepsy. Epilepsy Res 2008;82(1):21–28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Minikel EV, Zerr I, Collins SJ, et al. Ascertainment bias causes false signal of anticipation in genetic prion disease. Am. J. Hum. Genet 2014;95(4):371–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Ferentinos P, Koukounari A, Power R, et al. Familiality and SNP heritability of age at onset and episodicity in major depressive disorder. Psychol. Med 2015;45(10):2215–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Louis ED, Hernandez N, Rabinowitz D, et al. Predicting age of onset in familial essential tremor: how much does age of onset run in families? Neuroepidemiology 2013;40(4):269–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Barbier M, Camuzat A, Houot M, et al. Factors influencing the age at onset in familial frontotemporal lobar dementia: Important weight of genetics. Neurol. Genet 2017;3(6):e203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Janz D Epilepsy with impulsive petit mal (juvenile myoclonic epilepsy). Acta Neurol. Scand 1985;72(5):449–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kasteleijn-Nolst Trenité DGA, Schmitz B, Janz D, et al. Consensus on diagnosis and management of JME: From founder’s observations to current trends. Epilepsy Behav 2013;28 Suppl 1:S87–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Cvetkovska E, Panov S. Possible genetic anticipation in families with idiopathic generalised epilepsy. Epileptic Disord 2011;13(2):150–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Sutherland GR, Haan EA, Kremer E, et al. Hereditary unstable DNA: a new explanation for some old genetic questions? Lancet 1991;338(8762):289–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Lindblad K, Schalling M. Expanded Repeat Sequences and Disease. Semin. Neurol 1999;19(03):289–299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ishiura H, Doi K, Mitsui J, et al. Expansions of intronic TTTCA and TTTTA repeats in benign adult familial myoclonic epilepsy. Nat. Genet 2018;50(4):581–590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Donkervoort S, Hu Y, Stojkovic T, et al. Mosaicism for dominant collagen 6 mutations as a cause for intrafamilial phenotypic variability. Hum. Mutat 2015;36(1):48–56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Myers CT, Hollingsworth G, Muir AM, et al. Parental Mosaicism in “De Novo” Epileptic Encephalopathies. N. Engl. J. Med 2018;378(17):1646–1648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.