Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Opin Psychol. 2018 Dec 13;28:120–125. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.12.004

Mindfulness, acceptance, and emotion regulation: Perspectives from Monitor and Acceptance Theory (MAT)

Emily K Lindsay 1,*, J David Creswell 2
PMCID: PMC6565510  NIHMSID: NIHMS1518366  PMID: 30639835

Abstract

Experiential acceptance—an orientation of receptivity and noninterference with present-moment experiences—is described as central to mindfulness interventions, yet little experimental work has tested acceptance as a mechanism for mindfulness intervention effects. Guided by Monitor and Acceptance Theory (MAT), this review situates acceptance as an emotion regulation mechanism and reviews self-report mindfulness literature showing that attention monitoring skills are only associated with beneficial mental and physical health outcomes when accompanied by acceptance skills. New experimental dismantling work shows that removing acceptance training from mindfulness interventions reduces their efficacy for improving stress, positive emotion, and social relationship outcomes. Overall, converging evidence demonstrates that acceptance is a critical emotion regulation mechanism of mindfulness interventions. This work advances basic research, has translational value, and offers opportunities for future research.

Keywords: mindfulness, acceptance, emotion regulation, mechanisms, interventions


Experiential acceptance and equanimity skills are commonly described as central to many contemplative practices and third-wave therapeutic interventions (1), yet relatively little experimental work has explored the mechanistic role of acceptance skills training in mindfulness interventions. Contemporary mindfulness interventions instruct two specific mindfulness skills: (a) how to use attention to monitor present-moment experiences and (b) how to orient toward these experiences with acceptance, openness, and equanimity (2). We use ‘acceptance’ as an umbrella term to describe an orientation of receptivity and noninterference with present-moment experiences (3,4), which contrasts with tendencies to suppress, avoid, alter, prolong, or fixate on certain stimuli. Rather than striving for pleasant experiences (craving) and avoiding negative experiences (aversion), acceptance and equanimity break the typical association between desire (i.e., wanting and not wanting) and the hedonic tone (i.e., pleasant and unpleasant) of experiences (5). In practice, all momentary experiences are allowed to arise, unfold, and pass regardless of their valence. We recently developed a new theoretical model, Monitor and Acceptance Theory (MAT), which conceptually explains how monitoring and acceptance skills interact to drive mindfulness and mindfulness intervention effects (6). Specifically, MAT posits that (a) skills in monitoring the present moment may enhance the vividness of experience, thus intensifying affective reactivity, but that (b) bringing an attitude of acceptance toward monitored experiences is a key emotion regulation mechanism for the effects of mindfulness interventions on affective, stress, social relationship, and health outcomes.

This selective review first situates acceptance as an emotion regulation mechanism and reviews emerging literature testing MAT’s acceptance predictions in the context of trait mindfulness. We then focus on new work that dismantles acceptance from mindfulness interventions to experimentally test these predictions. We conclude by describing how new research has advanced MAT and suggest directions for future research.

MAT: Acceptance as an emotion regulation mechanism

Prominent theoretical models identify emotion regulation as a central mechanism for the effects of mindfulness on mental and physical health outcomes, acting to reduce affective and physiological reactivity and promote recovery (7). And indeed, meta-analytic evidence shows that emotion regulation processes mediate the effects of mindfulness interventions on improvements in mental health (8). In contrast, ineffective emotion regulation is a core feature of psychopathology (9), and a growing body of evidence relates non-acceptance—including experiential avoidance (10,11), emotional non-acceptance (12), and distress intolerance (13)—with clinical symptoms. Moreover, the emotion regulation literature shows that acceptance is an effective strategy (14), and acceptance training is considered a key therapeutic element of many clinical interventions targeting anxiety, depressive, and other disorders characterized by experiential avoidance (e.g., (15,16)). Building on this rich tradition, MAT posits that acceptance skills are a critical feature of mindfulness interventions that drive emotion regulation and subsequent improvements in mental and physical health outcomes.

Importantly, present-focused awareness (‘monitoring’) is a core feature of mindfulness (which is canonically described as a state of clear awareness (17)), and as such, MAT focuses on the synergistic effects of monitoring and acceptance. Acceptance is thought to transform how momentary experiences are observed and processed, facilitating engagement (i.e., welcoming in) and subsequent disengagement (i.e., letting go) with emotional stimuli (1820), and thus enriching experience while also reducing emotional reactivity. Studies of trait mindfulness components in non-meditating samples generally support MAT’s predictions that monitoring alone can increase affective reactivity, whereas monitoring and acceptance skills together promote emotion regulation and favorable affective, stress, and health outcomes (21). Specifically, recent work shows that, when unaccompanied by self-reported acceptance skills, the self-reported tendency to monitor one’s present moment experiences has been linked with outcomes reflecting emotion dysregulation, including affective symptoms (e.g., higher depressive, anxiety, and other clinical symptoms (2225)), poor social relationship functioning (26), and poor health behaviors and outcomes (e.g., (27,28)). In contrast, in these same studies, people who report high levels of both monitoring and acceptance skills show evidence of successful emotion regulation, including lower negative affectivity, adaptive social relationship functioning, and salutary health behaviors and outcomes.

On the other hand, trait monitoring skills, independent of acceptance skills, have also been associated with enhanced positive affectivity (e.g., life satisfaction and purpose (25,29)). Moreover, EMA studies have shown that monitoring present-moment experience—assessed in the moment—is related to greater concurrent positive affect (30,31). As such, the trait mindfulness literature suggests that monitoring skills alone may be sufficient to heighten positive experiences (32).

Overall, correlational evidence supports MAT’s predictions that monitoring skills may intensify both negative and positive affect, but when paired with acceptance skills, monitoring and acceptance together facilitate emotion regulation and foster mental and physical health. However, these studies do not test the development of monitoring and acceptance skills through mindfulness training. One important question is whether the correlations observed in self-report mindfulness studies generalize to the mechanisms underlying mindfulness interventions.

Testing MAT: Dismantling acceptance from mindfulness interventions

To test whether learning acceptance skills is a critical emotion regulation mechanism of mindfulness interventions for improving affective, stress, and social relationship outcomes, recent studies have employed intervention dismantling designs. Dismantling designs have been recommended and used to identify active treatment elements of multi-component mindfulness interventions (33,34). We have adopted this experimental approach to test whether removing acceptance skills training from mindfulness interventions reduces or eliminates treatment-related improvements in emotion regulation-relevant outcomes.

Early iterations of this dismantling approach provide initial support for the importance of acceptance instruction in mindfulness interventions for regulating emotion. For example, in the context of recalling and writing about a traumatic experience, instruction to adopt an accepting, nonjudgmental stance buffered against the negative affect associated with monitoring the contents of present-moment awareness (35). Similarly, three sessions of mindfulness meditation with both monitoring and acceptance instruction reduced mind-wandering on a frustrating sustained attention task relative to mindfulness training without acceptance instruction (36); acceptance practice may have facilitated disengagement with emotionally-charged thoughts, given that the self-reported “stickiness” of thoughts can impair performance on this task (37).

More recently, we conducted two dismantling RCTs to compare the effects of (a) standard mindfulness training in both monitoring and acceptance (Monitor+Accept) vs. (b) structurally-matched mindfulness training only in monitoring present-moment experiences (Monitor Only). These parallel trials recruited stressed community adults and tested for post-intervention differences in acute stress reactivity and changes in daily life stress, affect, and social relationship outcomes. In both trials, all active interventions were framed as stress management programs and produced equivalent treatment expectancies, ruling out placebo effects. The trials differed in intervention length (2-week vs. 8-week), delivery format (individually-delivered smartphone-based vs. group-based Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR)), and control group (active control intervention vs. no treatment). Together, the trials allowed us to isolate the unique contributions of acceptance training from other treatment components.

Building on the idea that acceptance skills foster emotion regulation, a primary MAT prediction is that acceptance training is a necessary component of mindfulness interventions for reducing stress. First, in our 14-lesson smartphone-based intervention trial, Monitor+Accept mindfulness training decreased cortisol and blood pressure reactivity to acute stress compared to Monitor Only and active control trainings (38). However, our 8-week dismantling trial found no condition differences in biological stress reactivity (unpublished data). Mindfulness intervention effects on biological stress reactivity outcomes are not all consistent (e.g., (39)), and it’s possible that a pre-stress booster meditation session (implemented in our 2-week trial) or more sustained mindfulness practice may be necessary to reliably impact biological reactivity on powerful acute stress tasks. Second, in the context of daily life subjective stress, our two trials suggest dosage effects; medium-sized decreases in daily life stress were observed following 2-week Monitor+Accept training (d=.45) and large decreases following 8-week Monitor+Accept training (d=.72). Significant advantages of Monitor+Accept over Monitor Only training emerged after 8week training (d=.38) (40), with a smaller nonsignificant advantage after 2-week training (d=.16) (unpublished data). Together, these findings demonstrate a clear role of acceptance training in mindfulness interventions for stress reduction effects, and suggest that improvements in daily life stress build with continued practice.

In addition to regulating stress responding, acceptance appears to be a critical component of mindfulness interventions for boosting positive psychosocial outcomes. In both of our recent trials, Monitor+Accept training was more effective for increasing positive emotions in daily life compared to Monitor Only training and control groups across different training lengths and delivery methods (41). Orienting toward momentary experiences with receptivity, openness, and acceptance may broaden awareness and afford people greater access to positive stimuli that might otherwise go unnoticed in daily life. Our 2-week trial also showed that acceptance is an active component of mindfulness for reducing loneliness and increasing social contact in daily life (42). These findings suggest that acceptance skills drive emotion regulation in ways that improve social relationships (43,44); bringing equanimity to feelings of loneliness and social threat may allow them to dissipate, and bringing openness and curiosity to social situations may encourage greater engagement with others in daily life. A growing body of evidence shows benefits of mindfulness interventions for boosting positive emotions and improving social relationships. In addition to promoting health via stress and symptom reduction (45), mindfulness interventions may also boost positive resilience factors with potential implications for improving long-term health outcomes (e.g., (4648)).

All together, these dismantling trials offer promising evidence that acceptance is a critical emotion regulation mechanism for mindfulness intervention effects. By experimentally dismantling mindfulness interventions and conceptually replicating findings across different lengths of training and delivery formats, these studies underscore the importance of acceptance training for regulating emotional reactivity, stress, and loneliness and for opening awareness in ways that boost positive affect and social engagement.

MAT insights, revisions, and future directions

We reviewed evidence that trait acceptance skills moderate the link between present-focused attention monitoring and poor emotional, social, and health functioning, and that removing acceptance training from mindfulness interventions reduces their efficacy for improving stress, positive emotion, and social relationship outcomes. Together, recent evidence suggests that acceptance is a critical emotion regulation mechanism of mindfulness interventions.

Yet further work is needed to directly test mechanistic pathways linking mindfulness training, acceptance skill development, and changes in emotion regulation processes. As an emotion regulation skill, acceptance may influence the deployment of attention toward emotional stimuli and facilitate more flexible use of multiple regulatory strategies (49). First, acceptance strategies have been shown to enhance attention to novel emotional stimuli (50) while also dampening physiological reactivity (5153), reflecting a nonreactive attentional engagement with emotional experiences. Second, although acceptance does not involve intentionally changing emotional experience (54), early emotion processing and regulation may facilitate the use of reappraisal, distraction, and other strategies in appropriate contexts (55,56). Clarifying how acceptance acts to regulate emotion is an important direction for future mindfulness intervention research. Study designs that dismantle acceptance from mindfulness interventions and those that test acceptance as a mediating mechanism of mindfulness interventions can both contribute to this question, with ambulatory assessment approaches providing a promising way to test for intervention-related changes in acceptance and emotion regulation in daily life (57).

There are also many opportunities to test MAT predictions in other domains; mindfulness interventions show a broad range of effects across cognitive, affective, and health domains. For example, although Monitor+Accept interventions improved health-relevant outcomes (e.g., stress; stress biology (45)), further work is needed to test acceptance as a mechanism for improving long-term markers of health in at-risk populations. The cognitive domain is also underexplored, and MAT predicts that monitoring alone is sufficient for boosting affectively-neutral cognitive outcomes (6).

The intervention dismantling work we have conducted so far in stress and affective domains provides insights that suggest potential revisions to MAT. MAT’s predictions were developed from patterns observed in correlational self-report mindfulness studies, which consistently relate monitoring skills with heightened affective reactivity in predominantly meditation-naïve samples. Although Monitor Only mindfulness interventions were not effective for reducing stress, neither did they intensify negative or positive affectivity. Our experimental findings suggest that systematic monitoring practice may improve attentional control in ways that promote emotional clarity, which could promote emotion regulation over time (58). However, at least in the early stages of meditation practice, developing greater awareness by itself does not provide an efficient means for reducing stress. Also in contrast with the self-report mindfulness literature, Monitor Only interventions were not sufficient for enhancing positive emotions; acceptance may be necessary to open awareness to the many positive stimuli that are available to be monitored and appreciated. Overall, this new experimental work highlights some unique effects of developing mindfulness skills through formal practice compared to naturally occurring trait mindfulness tendencies, and leaves room for further research to establish the time course of monitoring and acceptance skill development and maintenance of training effects.

Broadly, recent findings from dismantling trials have translational value for improving the quality and efficiency of mindfulness interventions. Mindfulness-specific improvements in stress, emotion, and social relationship domains may be maximized by emphasizing concrete acceptance and equanimity training techniques. To further amplify intervention efficiency, intervention dismantling approaches hold promise for identifying target populations who may benefit more from certain skills (e.g., people low in trait acceptance may benefit more from mindfulness interventions that emphasize acceptance training (59)) and developing tailored interventions for these population subsets. Finally, this dismantling work provides an opportunity to find common ground between behavioral interventions, with the possibility that acceptance is a common emotion regulation mechanism underlying treatment effects across a variety of interventions (e.g., Acceptance and Commitment Therapy, expressive writing (1)).

Conclusions

Accumulating evidence shows that experiential acceptance is a critical component of mindfulness interventions for improving affective, stress, and social relationship outcomes. This work advances basic research on the mechanisms of mindfulness, has translational value for maximizing the efficiency and impact of mindfulness interventions, and offers exciting opportunities for future research.

Highlights.

  • Experiential acceptance is described as a central feature of mindfulness interventions.

  • Based on theory and correlational research, MAT posits that acceptance is a key emotion regulation mechanism.

  • Dismantling trials show that acceptance is a key ingredient of mindfulness interventions.

  • Emphasizing acceptance skills may maximize the efficacy of mindfulness interventions.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by grants from the National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health (NCCIH) of the National Institutes of Health (F32AT009508, R21AT008493, and R01AT008685). We thank Anna Marsland for helpful comments on an early draft.

Footnotes

Conflict of interest

We declare no conflicts of interest.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Contributor Information

Emily K. Lindsay, University of Pittsburgh.

J. David Creswell, Carnegie Mellon University.

References

Papers of particular interest have been highlighted as:

* of special interest

** of outstanding interest

  • 1.Mennin DS, Ellard KK, Fresco DM, Gross JJ. United We Stand: Emphasizing Commonalities Across Cognitive-Behavioral Therapies. Behav Ther. 2013. June;44(2):234–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Bishop SR, Lau M, Shapiro S, Carlson L, Anderson ND, Carmody J, et al. Mindfulness: A Proposed Operational Definition. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 2004. September 1;11(3):230–41. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Desbordes G, Gard T, Hoge EA, Hölzel BK, Kerr C, Lazar SW, et al. Moving Beyond Mindfulness: Defining Equanimity as an Outcome Measure in Meditation and Contemplative Research. Mindfulness. 2015;6(2):356–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.**.Young S What Is Mindfulness? A Contemplative Perspective In: Schonert-Reichl KA, Roeser RW, editors. Handbook of Mindfulness in Education [Internet]. Springer; New York; 2016. [cited 2016 Nov 25]. p. 29–45. (Mindfulness in Behavioral Health). Available from: http://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-1-4939-3506-2_3 Deconstructs the basic components of mindfulness practice and provides a clear illustration of acceptance and equanimity skills through traditional contemplative philosophy, practice examples, and metaphor. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.*.Hadash Y, Segev N, Tanay G, Goldstein P, Bernstein A. The decoupling model of equanimity: theory, measurement, and test in a mindfulness intervention. Mindfulness. 2016;7(5):1214–1226. This work introduces the Decoupling Model of Equanimity, a conceptual model that links Buddhist perspectives of equanimity with contemporary psychological constructs. Factor analyses show that equanimity is manifested as acceptance and nonreactivity toward experience independent of hedonic tone. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.**.Lindsay EK, Creswell JD. Mechanisms of mindfulness training: Monitor and Acceptance Theory (MAT). Clin Psychol Rev. 2017. February;51:48–59. Details the conceptual framework for Monitor and Acceptance Theory (MAT), describes MAT’s testable tenets, and reviews initial correlational evidence to support MAT. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Hölzel BK, Lazar SW, Gard T, Schuman-Olivier Z, Vago DR, Ott U. How Does Mindfulness Meditation Work? Proposing Mechanisms of Action From a Conceptual and Neural Perspective. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2011. November 1;6(6):537–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Gu J, Strauss C, Bond R, Cavanagh K. How do mindfulness-based cognitive therapy and mindfulness-based stress reduction improve mental health and wellbeing? A systematic review and meta-analysis of mediation studies. Clin Psychol Rev. 2015. April;37:1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schweizer S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30(2):217–237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Chawla N, Ostafin B. Experiential avoidance as a functional dimensional approach to psychopathology: An empirical review. J Clin Psychol. 2007;63(9):871–890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kashdan TB, Barrios V, Forsyth JP, Steger MF. Experiential avoidance as a generalized psychological vulnerability: Comparisons with coping and emotion regulation strategies. Behav Res Ther. 2006. September;44(9):1301–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Yoon S, Dang V, Mertz J, Rottenberg J. Are attitudes towards emotions associated with depression? A Conceptual and meta-analytic review. J Affect Disord. 2018. May;232:329–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Leyro TM, Zvolensky MJ, Bernstein A. Distress tolerance and psychopathological symptoms and disorders: A review of the empirical literature among adults. Psychol Bull. 2010;136(4):576–600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kohl A, Rief W, Glombiewski JA. How effective are acceptance strategies? A meta-analytic review of experimental results. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2012. December;43(4):988–1001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Roemer L, Orsillo SM. Expanding our conceptualization of and treatment for generalized anxiety disorder: Integrating mindfulness/acceptance-based approaches with existing cognitive-behavioral models. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 2002;9(1):54–68. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hayes SC. Acceptance and commitment therapy, relational frame theory, and the third wave of behavioral and cognitive therapies. Behav Ther. 2004;35(4):639–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Quaglia JT, Brown KW, Lindsay EK, Creswell JD, Goodman RJ. From Conceptualization to Operationalization of Mindfulness In: Handbook of mindfulness: Theory, research, and practice. New York: Guilford; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Quaglia JT, Goodman RJ, Brown KW. Trait mindfulness predicts efficient top-down attention to and discrimination of facial expressions. J Pers. 2016;84(3):393–404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Teper R, Inzlicht M. Meditation, mindfulness and executive control: the importance of emotional acceptance and brain-based performance monitoring. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2013. January;8(1):85–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Vago DR, Nakamura Y. Selective Attentional Bias Towards Pain-Related Threat in Fibromyalgia: Preliminary Evidence for Effects of Mindfulness Meditation Training. Cogn Ther Res. 2011. September 18;35(6):581–94. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.*.Curtiss J, Klemanski DH, Andrews L, Ito M, Hofmann SG. The conditional process model of mindfulness and emotion regulation: An empirical test. J Affect Disord. 2017. April;212:93–100. Describes a complementary model to MAT. Confirmed through path analyses, the conditional process model shows that self-reported acceptance (‘nonreactivity’) moderates the relationship between self-reported monitoring (‘observing’) and clinical symptoms through an emotion regulation pathway, such that monitoring is only beneficial when accompanied by acceptance. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Bravo AJ, Boothe LG, Pearson MR. Getting Personal with Mindfulness: a Latent Profile Analysis of Mindfulness and Psychological Outcomes. Mindfulness. 2016. April;7(2):420–32. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Bravo AJ, Pearson MR, Kelley ML. Mindfulness and Psychological Health Outcomes: a Latent Profile Analysis Among Military Personnel and College Students. Mindfulness. 2018;9(1):258–270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Lam KFY, Lim HA, Kua EH, Griva K, Mahendran R. Mindfulness and Cancer Patients’ Emotional States: a Latent Profile Analysis Among Newly Diagnosed Cancer Patients. Mindfulness. 2018. April;9(2):521–33. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Sahdra BK, Ciarrochi J, Parker PD, Basarkod G, Bradshaw EL, Baer R. Are people mindful in different ways? Disentangling the quantity and quality of mindfulness in latent profiles and exploring their links to mental health and life effectiveness. Eur J Personal. 2017;31(4):347–365. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Kimmes JG, Durtschi JA, Fincham FD. Perception in Romantic Relationships: a Latent Profile Analysis of Trait Mindfulness in Relation to Attachment and Attributions. Mindfulness. 2017. October;8(5):1328–38. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Lau WK, Leung M-K, Wing Y-K, Lee TM. Potential Mechanisms of Mindfulness in Improving Sleep and Distress. Mindfulness. 2018;9(2):547–555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Manigault AW, Figueroa WS, Hollenbeck CR, Mendlein AE, Woody A, Sinegar SE, et al. A Test of the Association Between Mindfulness Subcomponents and Diurnal Cortisol Patterns. Mindfulness. 2018;897–904. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Iani L, Lauriola M, Cafaro V, Didonna F. Dimensions of mindfulness and their relations with psychological well-being and neuroticism. Mindfulness. 2017;8(3):664–676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Blanke ES, Riediger M, Brose A. Pathways to happiness are multidirectional: Associations between state mindfulness and everyday affective experience. 2017; [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 31.Felsman P, Verduyn P, Ayduk O, Kross E. Being present: Focusing on the present predicts improvements in life satisfaction but not happiness. Emotion. 2017;17(7):1047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Lindsay EK, Creswell JD. Back to the Basics: How Attention Monitoring and Acceptance Stimulate Positive Growth. Psychol Inq. 2015. October 2;26(4):343–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Britton WB, Davis JH, Loucks EB, Barnes P, Cullen BH, Reuter L, et al. Dismantling Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy: Creation and validation of 8-week focused attention and open monitoring interventions within a 3-armed randomized controlled trial. Behav Res Ther. 2018;101:92–107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Williams JMG, Crane C, Barnhofer T, Brennan K, Duggan DS, Fennell MJ, et al. Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for preventing relapse in recurrent depression: a randomized dismantling trial. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2014;82(2):275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Valdez CE, Sherrill AM, Lilly M. Present moment contact and nonjudgment: Pilot data on dismantling mindful awareness in trauma-related symptomatology. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2016;38(4):572–581. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Rahl HA, Lindsay EK, Pacilio LE, Brown KW, Creswell JD. Brief Mindfulness Meditation Training Reduces Mind Wandering: the Critical Role of Acceptance Training. Emotion. 2017;17(4):224–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.van Vugt MK, Broers N. Self-Reported Stickiness of Mind-Wandering Affects Task Performance. Front Psychol [Internet]. 2016. May 18 [cited 2018 Aug 25];7 Available from: http://journal.frontiersin.org/Article/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00732/abstract [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.**.Lindsay EK, Young S, Smyth JM, Brown KW, Creswell JD. Acceptance lowers stress reactivity: Dismantling mindfulness training in a randomized controlled trial. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2018. January;87:63–73. This paper provides the first experimental evidence supporting MAT’s predictions in the context of stress reactivity. Mindfulness training in both monitoring and acceptance lowered biological stress reactivity, with no differences between mindfulness training in monitoring only and active control training. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Nyklíček I, Mommersteeg PMC, Van Beugen S, Ramakers C, Van Boxtel GJ. Mindfulness-based stress reduction and physiological activity during acute stress: A randomized controlled trial. Health Psychol. 2013;32(10):1110–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Chin B, Lindsay EK, Greco CM, Brown KW, Wright AG, Smyth JM, et al. Acceptance Skills Drive Stress Resilience in a Mindfulness Training Randomized Controlled Trial. under review; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 41.*.Lindsay EK, Chin B, Greco CM, Young S, Brown KW, Wright AG, et al. How mindfulness training promotes positive emotions: Dismantling acceptance skills training in two randomized controlled trials. J Pers Soc Psychol. in press; This paper provides new insight to MAT in the context of positive emotions, showing across two dismantling RCTs that acceptance is a critical component of mindfulness interventions for boosting positive affect in daily life. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Lindsay EK, Young S, Brown KW, Smyth JM, Creswell JD. Acceptance skills training reduces loneliness and increases social contact: A randomized controlled trial of mindfulness training. under review; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 43.Adair KC, Fredrickson BL, Castro-Schilo L, Kim S, Sidberry S. Present with You: Does Cultivated Mindfulness Predict Greater Social Connection Through Gains in Decentering and Reductions in Negative Emotions? Mindfulness. 2017;1–13. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Quaglia JT, Goodman RJ, Brown KW. From mindful attention to social connection: The key role of emotion regulation. Cogn Emot. 2015;29(8):1466–1474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Creswell JD, Lindsay EK. How Does Mindfulness Training Affect Health? A Mindfulness Stress Buffering Account. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2014;23(6):401–7. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Wichers M, Peeters F, Geschwind N, Jacobs N, Simons CJP, Derom C, et al. Unveiling patterns of affective responses in daily life may improve outcome prediction in depression: A momentary assessment study. J Affect Disord. 2010. July;124(1–2):191–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Steptoe A, Wardle J. Positive affect measured using ecological momentary assessment and survival in older men and women. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2011. November 8;108(45):18244–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Baker M, Harris T, Stephenson D. Loneliness and Social Isolation as Risk Factors for Mortality A Meta-Analytic Review. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015. March 1;10(2):227–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Slutsky J, Rahl HA, Lindsay EK, Creswell JD. Mindfulness, Emotion Regulation, and Social Threat In: Papies EK, Karremans J, editors. Mindfulness in Social Psychology. New York: Routledge; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Uusberg H, Uusberg A, Talpsep T, Paaver M. Mechanisms of mindfulness: The dynamics of affective adaptation during open monitoring. Biol Psychol. 2016;118:94–106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Dan-Glauser ES, Gross JJ. The temporal dynamics of emotional acceptance: Experience, expression, and physiology. Biol Psychol. 2015. May;108:1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Troy AS, Shallcross AJ, Brunner A, Friedman R, Jones MC. Cognitive reappraisal and acceptance: Effects on emotion, physiology, and perceived cognitive costs. Emotion. 2018;18(1):58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Wang S-M, Lee H-K, Kweon Y-S, Lee CT, Chae J-H, Kim J-J, et al. Effect of emotion regulation training in patients with panic disorder: Evidenced by heart rate variability measures. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2016;40:68–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.*.Ford BQ, Lam P, John OP, Mauss IB. The Psychological Health Benefits of Accepting Negative Emotions and Thoughts: Laboratory, Diary, and Longitudinal Evidence. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2017. July 13;This theoretically-driven work provides systematic cross-sectional, experimental, and longitudinal evidence to show that habitual emotional acceptance is associated with psychological health in part by lessening affective reactivity to stress. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Alkoby A, Pliskin R, Halperin E, Levit-Binnun N. An eight-week mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) workshop increases regulatory choice flexibility. Emot; Wash DC: 2018; [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Garland EL, Farb NA, Goldin PR, Fredrickson BL. Mindfulness Broadens Awareness and Builds Eudaimonic Meaning: A Process Model of Mindful Positive Emotion Regulation. Psychol Inq. 2015. October 2;26(4):293–314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Shoham A, Hadash Y, Bernstein A. Examining the decoupling model of equanimity in mindfulness training: an intensive experience sampling study. Clin Psychol Sci. 2018;6(5):704–720. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.O’Bryan EM, Kraemer KM, Johnson AL, McLeish AC, McLaughlin LE. Examining the role of attentional control in terms of specific emotion regulation difficulties. Personal Individ Differ. 2017;108:158–163. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Gawrysiak MJ, Grassetti SN, Greeson JM, Shorey RC, Pohlig R, Baime MJ. The many facets of mindfulness and the prediction of change following mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR). J Clin Psychol. 2018;74(4):523–535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES