Skip to main content
Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment logoLink to Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment
. 2019 Jun 7;15:1563–1571. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S203729

Prospective memory in non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis

Shi-Ze Lin 1,2, Yan-Kun Wu 2,3,4, Yun-Ai Su 2,3,4, Tian-Mei Si 2,3,4,
PMCID: PMC6565992  PMID: 31289442

Abstract

Prospective memory (PM) could be impaired in the non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia. This meta-analysis systematically examined the PM of non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia. Both Chinese and English databases were systematically searched for articles from the inception of the databases through November 13, 2018. Case-control studies of PM in non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia were included in the analyses. Confidence intervals (CIs) and standardized mean differences (SMDs) were calculated utilizing the random effects model. Four studies (n=268) that compared PM performance between non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia (n=136) and healthy controls (n=132) were included. Three studies were rated as “high quality”, while the quality of evidence of the three outcomes included in this meta-analysis was moderate. Compared with the healthy controls, the non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia showed impairments in overall PM (two studies, n=127; SMD: −0.46; 95% CI=−0.82, −0.11, P=0.01; I=0%), event-based PM (EBPM) (four studies, n=268; SMD: −0.56; 95% CI=−0.80, −0.31, P<0.00001; I=0%), and time-based PM (TBPM) (four studies, n=268; SMD: −0.66; 95% CI=−0.90, −0.41, P<0.00001; I=0%). This meta-analysis demonstrated that the overall PM, EBPM, and TBPM might be impaired in the non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia.

Keywords: schizophrenia, relatives, prospective memory, endophenotypes

Introduction

Schizophrenia is a recurrent, prolonged, and chronic mental illness with strong heritability,1 and the genetic risk of schizophrenia is estimated to be as high as 80%.2 Therefore, the risk of developing schizophrenia in non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia is higher than that of the general population.3 Schizophrenia is often associated with various cognitive dysfunctions,48 which may be related to the heritability of schizophrenia. Relevant studies have found that the non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia also exhibit cognitive defects that are similar but less severe than those of the patients.914In terms of memory impairment, one of the most extensively researched areas of cognitive impairment, verbal memory, has been found to be as deficient as working memory in non-onset biological relatives or healthy siblings of patients with schizophrenia.1519 Apparently, each of the studies have been restricted to retrospective memory, and there are few reports on prospective memory.

Prospective memory (PM) is defined as the capability of remembering to perform a planned action in the future.20 To complete a typical PM task, an individual first needs to form an intent (when and what needs to be done) and then begins to engage in an ongoing task. When performing ongoing tasks, individuals need to maintain focus and monitor PM cues. When a prospective memory cue appears and is detected, the individual should retrieve the intent and respond, which is of vital importance for everyday life, as research has shown that up to “50–80%” of daily activities are largely affected by PM.21 For example, remembering to attend a meeting tomorrow, remembering to sit for an exam next week, and remembering when to turn off the gas, etc., all require good PM function. We cannot imagine how severe and grave the consequences would be in our lives without PM. According to the nature of intention cues, PM falls into three categories: time-based prospective memory (TBPM), event-based prospective memory (EBPM), and activity-based prospective memory (ABPM).22,23 TBPM requires a person to remember to execute one intention after one specific time period (such as attending an academic conference next Saturday); EBPM requires a person to perform an intent when the prompt appears (such as purchasing a toothbrush when passing a supermarket, which is an external cue); and ABPM requires a person to do a planned action at the end of an activity (such as sending an email after a wedding).

We have developed an understanding of cognitive profiles of non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients suffering from schizophrenia both in working and verbal memory. In recent years, the PM function of the non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia has attracted increasing attention. To date, only a small number of case-control studies have been published, and these studies had small sample sizes and conflicting results.2427 By thoroughly searching the literature, no systematic review or meta-analysis can be located that specifically investigates the prospective memory of non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia, which provided an impetus for us to perform a comprehensive meta-analysis on the prospective memory of the non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia.

Methods

Search strategy and selection criteria

The inclusion criteria were identified by utilizing the PICOS acronym: Participants (P): non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia. Intervention (I): not applicable (NA). Comparison (C): healthy controls. Outcome (O): time-based PM, event-based PM, and overall PM. Study design (S): case-control studies exploring PM of non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia with the data required for the meta-analysis.

In accordance with the statement of the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA),28 two investigators (SZL, YKW) searched Chinese (China National Knowledge Infrastructure, WanFang and Chinese Biomedical databases) and English databases (Embase, PubMed, Cochrane Library, and PsycINFO databases) independently, from database inception through November 13, 2018. The following search keywords were adopted: (schizophrenia OR psychotic disorder) AND (relatives OR siblings OR parents OR children OR offspring OR family members) AND (prospective memory OR prospective memories OR prospective remembering OR delayed intention). In addition, hand searching of reference lists of the identified studies and related reviews was performed.

Data extraction

SZL and YKW checked, extracted, and analyzed data independently. The data is presented in forest plots. Any inconsistency during the collection and analysis of the data was attempted to be addressed by a consensus; if the inconsistency was not solved, a third reviewer (TMS) would get involved. The related variables were extracted from the articles using a pre-designed standard form. If the same data was reported in more than one study, only the study with complete data would be incorporated into the analysis. Moreover, the corresponding or first author was contacted to obtain more information if required.

Statistical approaches

In accordance with the recommendations from the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews,29 the meta-analysis was performed utilizing Review Manager Version 5.3 software. Due to heterogeneity within the characteristics of this study (like sampling and sample size), a random effects model was adopted for each meta-analysis outcome.30 In terms of continuous outcome data, the standard mean difference (SMD) and the 95% CI between the non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia and the controls were calculated using the inverse variance method. Notable heterogeneity of the meta-analysis results was defined when I2 values were >50% or P<0.1 within Q statistics.31 In terms of primary outcome, a subgroup or sensitivity analysis was performed to investigate the cause of the heterogeneity.

Egger’s intercept and funnel plots were used to assess the publication bias.32 Each analysis was two-tailed, with 0.05 set as the significance level.

Study quality assessment

Two investigators (SZL, YKW) evaluated the quality of all studies utilizing the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS).33Following previous studies, an NOS score ≥7 points was rated as high-quality.34,35 In addition, the grading of recommendation evaluation, development, and assessment (GRADE) system was utilized to assess the quality of evidence,36,37and the meta-analysis data results were rated as “high quality”, “moderate quality”, “low quality”, and “very low quality”.

Results

Search results

Figure 1 shows the PRISMA flow diagram demonstrating that 369 articles were discovered by searching the abovementioned databases. Three studies were excluded because of overlapping data with other studies.3840Four published English studies were finally included in the meta-analysis.2427

Figure 1.

Figure 1

PRISMA flow.

Studies, characteristics of the relatives, and PM measures

The four case-control studies (n=268) that compared the non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia (n=136) with healthy controls (n=132) were performed in Hong Kong, China, and India. The average age of the participants was between 11.33 and 50.02 years, and the average length of education of the participants was 11.71–14.94 years. In one study,26 the Cambridge Prospective Memory Test-Chinese version (C-CAMPROMPT) was used for assessing PM.41 In addition, one laboratory computerized dual-task paradigm was used for assessing PM in three studies (Table 1).4245

Table 1 Characteristics of the studies included in the meta-analysis

Table 1.

Characteristics of the studies included in the meta-analysis

Studies Country/area N Design, n Relative Healthy control PM measurement PM type NOS score
Age (years) Education (years) Age
(years)
Education (years)
Wang et al (2010)25 China 52 Case control 26/26 50.02 11.71 43.38 11.15 Computerized dual task paradigm Time, Event,
Activity, Overall
8
Lui et al (2011)24 Hong Kong 75 Case control 40/35 23.33 13.13 23.17 13.6 Computerized dual task paradigm Time, Event,
Activity, Overall
7
Zhou et al (2014)26 China 101 Case control 50/51 28 14 24.3 16.1 C-CAMPROMPT Time, Event 7
Saleem et al (2018)27 India 40 Case control 20/20 28.8 14.95 34.4 15.05 Computerized dual task paradigm Time, Event 6

Abrreviations: C-CAMPROMPT, the Cambridge Prospective Memory Test-Chinese version; NOS, Newcastle-Ottawa Scale; PM, prospective memory.

Assessment of quality

NOS scores of the four studies were between 6 and 8 (Table 2), and three studies were rated as “high quality” (NOS ≥7).2426The quality of evidence for the three meta-analysis outcomes was “moderate” according to the GRADE approach (Table 3).

Table 2.

Methodological quality assessment by the Newcastle-Ottawa scale

Studies Selection Comparability Exposure Total quality score
The case definition is adequate with independent validation Represent-ativeness of
cases
ommunity controls Controls with no history of disease Cases and controls with comparable education Cases and controls with comparable age Ascertainment of outcome Same method of ascertainment for cases and controls Similar non
response rate for both groups
Wang et al (2010)25 1 1 1 1 1 1 0 1 1 8
Lui et al (2011)24 1 1 0 1 1 1 0 1 1 7
Zhou et al (2014)26 1 1 1 1 0 1 0 1 1 7
Saleem et al (2018)27 1 1 0 1 1 0 0 1 1 6

Table 3.

GRADE analyses: prospective memory in non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia

Meta-analytic outcomes Studies (N) Risk of bias Inconsistency Indirectness Imprecision Publication bias Large effect Overall quality of evidencea
Overall PM 2 (127) No No No No Seriousb No +/+/±/;Moderate
Time-based PM 4 (268) No No No No Seriousb No +/+/±/;Moderate
Event-based PM
Activity-based PM
4 (268)
2 (127)
No
No
No
No
No
No
No
No
Seriousb
Seriousb
No
No
+/+/±/;Moderate
+/+/±/;Moderate

Notes: aGRADE Working Group grades of evidence: High quality=further research is very unlikely to change our confidence in the estimate of effect. Moderate quality=further research is likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and may change the estimate. Low quality=further research is very likely to have an important impact on our confidence in the estimate of effect and is likely to change the estimate. Very low quality=we are very uncertain about the estimate. bFor continuous outcomes, N<400.

Abbreviations: GRADE, grading of recommendations assessment, development, and evaluation; PM, prospective memory.

Table 2 Methodological quality assessment by the Newcastle-Ottawa scale

Table 3 GRADE analyses: prospective memory in non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia

Overall PM

In the two studies that had data available about overall performance of PM (n=127), the performance of non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia was poorer than that of healthy controls with a SMD of −0.46 (95% CI=−0.82, −0.11, P=0.01; I2= 0%, Figure 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Prospective memory in non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia: forest plot of overall PM, time-based PM, event-based PM, and activity-based PM.

Abbreviation: PM, prospective memory.

Time-based PM

In the four studies that had data available about time-based PM (n=268), the performance of non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia was poorer than that of healthy controls with a SMD of −0.66 (95% CI=−0.90, −0.41, P<0.00001; I2=0%, Figure 2).

Event-based PM

In the four studies that had data available about event-based PM (n=268), the performance of non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia was poorer compared to that of healthy controls with a SMD of −0.56 (95% CI=−0.80, −0.31, P<0.00001; I2=0%, Figure 2).

Activity-based PM

In the two studies that had data available about activity-based PM (n=127), the performance of non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia did not differ from healthy controls with a SMD of −0.11 (95% CI=−0.46, 0.24, P=0.55; I2=0%, Figure 2).

Publication bias

Because only four studies were incorporated in the meta-analysis, the publication bias of the meta-analysis outcomes could not be carried out using Egger’s test or a funnel plot.46

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first meta-analysis on the PM performance of first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia. The analysis results demonstrated that the overall prospective memory and the two categories of PM were markedly impaired with a medium effect size (overall PM: SMD=−0.46; EBPM: SMD=−0.56; TBPM: SMD=−0.66). These results are in line with the meta-analysis on working memory, verbal memory, and executive function in non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia.13,17 The performance of the overall PM, TBPM, and EBPM in first-degree relatives was markedly lower than that of normal controls, which was similar to that in schizophrenic patients.43,47,48 Although this impairment is less severe, this indicates that PM might be related to the heritability of schizophrenia. Additionally, the ABPM task was considered to be the simplest among the three memory tasks. Only two articles confirmed that the ABPM of first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia was not impaired in this task,24,25 suggesting that the first-degree relatives of patients suffering from schizophrenia had certain neurocognitive impairments, but their impairments were not as serious as those of patients with schizophrenia.

We discovered that the effect size of TBPM (SMD=−0.66) was larger than that of EBPM (SMD=−0.56), which showed a similar trend as that observed in any other neuropsychiatric disorder. For instance, one meta-analysis on the prospective memory of patients with schizophrenia revealed that TBPM impairment (Cohen’s d=−1.33) was more severe than EBPM impairment (Cohen’s d=−0.827).49 A larger effect size of TBPM than of EBPM was also reported in depression (SMD=−0.89 vs −0.44),50 bipolar disorder (SMD=−0.82 vs −0.51),51 Parkinson’s disease (Hedges’ g=−0.71 vs −0.55) and autism spectrum disorder (Hedges’ g=−0.87 vs −0.41) .52,53 This tendency can be explained by various TBPM and EBPM cognitive processes and their load on the prefrontal cortex.54,55TBPM has a more complicated cognitive process than EBPM, because there is not any specific external cue to trigger it. When performing tasks, much more effort is required to supervise time as well as self-initiate a prospective intention. In contrast, EBPM includes fewer demands for self-initiation, as it relies on one external cue as the trigger,22 leading to less demanding EBPM in the prefrontal cortex.55Therefore, TBPM is more demanding and is more likely to be compromised in patients suffering from pathological or other neuropsychiatric disorders, which includes first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia.22,55 This hypothesis was confirmed in the meta-analysis,49which suggested that the differences between TBPM and EBPM were heterogeneous.

Schizophrenia is an inherited disease,1but the results of conventional linkage and association studies on susceptibility loci are not consistent.56 Therefore, in recent years, researchers have adopted endophenotype approaches to investigate the genetic basis of schizophrenia.57,58 An endophenotype is closer to the function of gene action than the diagnosis of schizophrenia itself; thus, it is easier to locate the locus of the endophenotype than the locus of schizophrenia.57,59 There are a number of endophenotypes, and cognitive endophenotypes have been extensively studied. So far, some research has shown that the impairment of PM exists within schizophrenia patients, regardless of chronic,47,6063 first-episode, or even first-episode drug-naive status,6466 and meta-analyses and systematic reviews have been conducted.49,67 In this study, it was found that the non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia had defects in their prospective memory. Based on the above studies, we can determine that the characteristics of PM impairment of schizophrenia seem to conform to the disease association, state independence (regardless of first or chronic stages), family association, and the high-risk group of the endophenotype criterion proposed by Gottsman and Gould.57 In summary, it is reasonable to speculate that PM might be an endophenotype of schizophrenia.

Due to some methodological limitations, the meta-analysis results should be interpreted cautiously. First, the sample sizes involved in this study were comparatively small, and all meta-analysis results were rated only as “moderate quality” according to the GRADE score. Future studies may require larger samples and may be required to distinguish between parents, siblings, and offspring of the patients suffering from schizophrenia, since genetic studies have discovered that parents, siblings, and offspring of people with schizophrenia have different prevalence rates.3Second, only Chinese and English databases were searched, and only four studies were incorporated; therefore, the publication bias could not be tested. Third, different studies employed different PM measurement approaches, and the results were not the same. Therefore, we used a random effects model to provide conservative estimates.

Conclusion

The meta-analysis revealed that PM, especially TBPM, might be impaired in non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia. Further high-quality large sample studies are worthwhile and necessary. Furthermore, it is recommended that psychiatrists place more emphasis on early detection of PM deficits in first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia, and take into account the effective implementation of interventions to mitigate negative outcomes related to PM deficits.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 81630031), Beijing Municipal Science and Technology Project (Z171100000117016), and The Capital Foundation of Medicine Research and Development (2016-1-4111).

Disclosure

Dr Shi-Ze Lin reports grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China, Beijing Municipal Science and Technology Project, and The Capital Foundation of Medicine Research and Development during the conduct of the study; grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China, Beijing Municipal Science and Technology Project, and The Capital Foundation of Medicine Research and Development outside the submitted work. Dr Yan-Kun Wu reports grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China, Beijing Municipal Science and Technology Project, and The Capital Foundation of Medicine Research and Development during the conduct of the study; grants from The Capital Foundation of Medicine Research and Development, Beijing Municipal Science and Technology Project, and the National Natural Science Foundation of China, outside the submitted work. The authors report no other conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • 1.Kendler KS, Diehl SR. The genetics of schizophrenia: a current, genetic-epidemiologic perspective. Schizophr Bull. 1993;19(2):261–285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Cannon TD, Kaprio J, Lonnqvist J, Huttunen M, Koskenvuo M. The genetic epidemiology of schizophrenia in a Finnish twin cohort. A population-based modeling study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55(1):67–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Vogel F. Schizophrenia genesis: The origins of madness. Am J Hum Genet 1991;48(6):1218. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Fioravanti M, Carlone O, Vitale B, Cinti ME, Clare L. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in adults with a diagnosis of schizophrenia. Neuropsychol Rev. 2005;15(2):73–95. doi: 10.1007/s11065-005-6254-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Heinrichs RW. The primacy of cognition in schizophrenia. Am Psychol. 2005;60(3):229–242. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.3.229 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Green MF, Harvey PD. Cognition in schizophrenia: past, present, and future. Schizophr Res Cogn. 2014;1(1):e1–e9. doi: 10.1016/j.scog.2014.02.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Mesholam-Gately RI, Giuliano AJ, Goff KP, Faraone SV, Seidman LJ. Neurocognition in first-episode schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology. 2009;23(3):315–336. doi: 10.1037/a0014708 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Schulz SC, Murray A. Assessing cognitive impairment in patients with schizophrenia. J Clin Psychiatry. 2016;77(Suppl 2):3–7. doi: 10.4088/JCP.14074su1c.01 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Davalos DB, Compagnon N, Heinlein S, Ross RG. Neuropsychological deficits in children associated with increased familial risk for schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004;67(2–3):123–130. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00187-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Egan MF, Goldberg TE, Gscheidle T, et al. Relative risk for cognitive impairments in siblings of patients with schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50(2):98–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Fis NP, Cetin FC, Erturk M, Erdogan E, Dedeoglu C, Yazgan Y. Executive dysfunction in Turkish children at high risk for schizophrenia. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;17(7):424–431. doi: 10.1007/s00787-008-0684-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Groom MJ, Jackson GM, Calton TG, et al. Cognitive deficits in early-onset schizophrenia spectrum patients and their non-psychotic siblings: a comparison with ADHD. Schizophr Res. 2008;99(1–3):85–95. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.11.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Sitskoorn MM, Aleman A, Ebisch SJ, Appels MC, Kahn RS. Cognitive deficits in relatives of patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2004;71(2–3):285–295. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.03.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Wolf LE, Cornblatt BA, Roberts SA, Shapiro BM, Erlenmeyer-Kimling L. Wisconsin Card Sorting deficits in the offspring of schizophrenics in the New York high-risk project. Schizophr Res. 2002;57(2–3):173. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00301-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Conklin HM, Curtis CE, Katsanis J, Iacono WG. Verbal working memory impairment in schizophrenia patients and their first-degree relatives: evidence from the digit span task. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157(2):275–277. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.2.275 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Conklin HM, Curtis CE, Calkins ME, Iacono WG. Working memory functioning in schizophrenia patients and their first-degree relatives: cognitive functioning shedding light on etiology. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43(6):930–942. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.09.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Trandafir A, Meary A, Schurhoff F, Leboyer M, Szoke A. Memory tests in first-degree adult relatives of schizophrenic patients: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2006;81(2–3):217–226. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.09.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Whyte MC, McIntosh AM, Johnstone EC, Lawrie SM. Declarative memory in unaffected adult relatives of patients with schizophrenia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2005;78(1):13–26. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.05.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Wittorf A, Klingberg S, Wiedemann G. Secondary verbal memory: a potential endophenotype of schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res. 2004;38(6):601–612. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2004.03.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Brandimonte M, Einstein G, McDaniel M, eds. Prospective Memory. 1st ed. New York: Psychology Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kliegel M, Martin M. Prospective memory research: why is it relevant?. Int J Psychol. 2010;38(4):193–194. doi: 10.1080/00207590344000114 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Brandimonte M, Einstein GO, McDaniel MA. Varieties of intention: some distinctions and classifications In: Prospective Memory: Theory and Applications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 1996:23–51. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Einstein GO, McDaniel MA, Richardson SL, Guynn MJ, Cunfer AR. Aging and prospective memory: examining the influences of self-initiated retrieval processes. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 1995;21(4):996–1007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Lui SS, Wang Y, Liu AC, et al. Prospective memory in patients with first-onset schizophrenia and their non-psychotic siblings. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(8):2217–2224. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Wang Y, Chan RC, Cui J, et al. Prospective memory in non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2010;179(3):285–290. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2009.07.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Zhou FC, Hou WM, Wang CY, et al. Prospective memory performance in non-psychotic first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia: a controlled study. PLoS One. 2014;9(11):e111562. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0111562 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Saleem S, Kumar D, Venkatasubramanian G. Prospective memory in first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia. Clin Neuropsychol. 2018;32(5):993–1001. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2017.1406145 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. J Clin Epidemiol. 2009;62(10):1006–1012. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2009.06.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Higgins J, Green S. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. UK:Wiley ;2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.DerSimonian R, Laird N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Control Clin Trials. 1986;7(3):177¨C188. doi: 10.1016/0197-2456(86)90046-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Higgins JP, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat Med 2002;21(11):1539–1558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. Bmj. 1997;315(7109):629¨C634. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Connell O. D. The Newcastle-Ottawa scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of nonrandomized studies in meta-analyses. Appl Eng Agric. 2002;18(6):727–734. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Hernandez AV, Pasupuleti V, Benites-Zapata VA, Thota P, Deshpande A, Perez-Lopez FR. Insulin resistance and endometrial cancer risk: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur J Cancer. 2015;51(18):2747–2758. doi: 10.1016/j.ejca.2015.08.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Zhu J, Su X, Li G, Chen J, Tang B, Yang Y. The incidence of acute myocardial infarction in relation to overweight and obesity: a meta-analysis. Arch Med Sci. 2014;10(5):855–862. doi: 10.5114/aoms.2014.46206 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Atkins D, Best D, Briss PA, et al. Grading quality of evidence and strength of recommendations. Bmj. 2004;328(7454):1490. doi: 10.1136/bmj.328.7445.934 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Balshem H, Helfand M, Schunemann HJ, et al. GRADE guidelines: 3. Rating the quality of evidence. J Clin Epidemiol. 2011;64(4):401–406. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.07.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Lui SSY, Wang Y, Liu ACY, et al. Prospective memory in patients with early-stage schizophrenia and their unaffected first-degree siblings. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37:217. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Wang Y, Chan RCK, Cui J, et al. Prospective memory in schizophrenia and their non-psychotic first-degree relatives. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2010;44:A47–A48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Hou. Prospective Memory in Non-Psychotic First-Degree Relatives of Patients with Schizophrenia [master's thesis]. Beijing:Capital Medical University; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Lou Z, Dou Z, Zheng J, Man D The Study of The Chinese Version of Cambridge Prospective Memory Test (CAMPROMPT) for traumatic brain injury (unpublished Master thesis). Guangzhou, PR China, Sun Yat Sen University; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Chan RC, Wang Y, Ma Z, et al. Objective measures of prospective memory do not correlate with subjective complaints in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2008;103(1–3):229–239. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.02.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Wang Y, Chan RC, Hong X, et al. Prospective memory in schizophrenia: further clarification of nature of impairment. Schizophr Res. 2008;105(1–3):114–124. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Wang Y, Chan RC, Xin Y, Shi C, Cui J, Deng Y. Prospective memory deficits in subjects with schizophrenia spectrum disorders: a comparison study with schizophrenic subjects, psychometrically defined schizotypal subjects, and healthy controls. Schizophr Res. 2008;106(1):70–80. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.07.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Kumar D, Nizamie HS, Jahan M. Event-based prospective memory in schizophrenia. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2005;27(7):867–872. doi: 10.1080/13803390490919100 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Sterne JA, Sutton AJ, Ioannidis JP, et al. Recommendations for examining and interpreting funnel plot asymmetry in meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials. Bmj. 2011;343:d4002. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d4002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Shum D, Ungvari GS, Tang WK, Leung JP. Performance of schizophrenia patients on time-, event-, and activity-based prospective memory tasks. Schizophr Bull. 2004;30(4):693–701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Ungvari GS, Xiang YT, Tang WK, Shum D. Prospective memory and its correlates and predictors in schizophrenia: an extension of previous findings. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2008;23(5):613–622. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2008.06.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Wang Y, Cui J, Chan RC, et al. Meta-analysis of prospective memory in schizophrenia: nature, extent, and correlates. Schizophr Res. 2009;114(1–3):64–70. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.07.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Zhou FC, Wang YY, Zheng W, et al. Prospective memory deficits in patients with depression: A meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2017;220:79–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.05.042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Zhou FC, Wang YY, Zheng W, et al. Prospective memory in bipolar disorder: A meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 2018;259:184–190. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.09.073 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Ramanan S, Kumar D. Prospective memory in Parkinson’s disease: a meta-analysis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2013;19(10):1109–1118. doi: 10.1017/S1355617713001045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Landsiedel J, Williams DM, Abbot-Smith K. A meta-analysis and critical review of prospective memory in autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017;47(3):646–666. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2987-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Zhou FC, Xiang YT, Wang CY, et al. Characteristics and clinical correlates of prospective memory performance in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2012;135(1–3):34–39. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.12.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Einstein GO, McDaniel MA, Richardson SL, Guynn MJ, Cunfer AR. Aging and prospective memory: examining the influences of self-initiated retrieval processes. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1995;21(4):996–1007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Luo X, Huang L, Han L, et al. Systematic prioritization and integrative analysis of copy number variations in schizophrenia reveal key schizophrenia susceptibility genes. Schizophr Bull 2014;40(6):1285–1299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(4):636–645. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.636 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Gould TD, Gottesman II. Psychiatric endophenotypes and the development of valid animal models. Genes Brain Behav. 2006;5(2):113–119. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00186.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Gur RE, Calkins ME, Gur RC, et al. The consortium on the genetics of schizophrenia: neurocognitive endophenotypes. Schizophr Bull. 2007;33(1):49–68. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl055 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Au RW, Man D, Shum D, et al. Assessment of prospective memory in schizophrenia using the Chinese version of the Cambridge prospective memory test: a controlled study. Asia Pac Psychiatry. 2014;6(1):54–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1758-5872.2012.00217.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Au RW, Man D, Xiang YT, et al. Prospective memory predicts the level of community living skills in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2014;219(1):86–91. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.04.055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Au RWC, Xiang YT, Ungvari GS, et al. Prospective memory performance in persons with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder and healthy persons. Perspect Psychiatr Care. 2017;53(4):266–274. doi: 10.1111/ppc.12172 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Demeter G, Szendi I, Domjan N, et al. Preserved intention maintenance and impaired execution of prospective memory responses in schizophrenia: evidence from an event-based prospective memory study. Front Psychol. 2016;7:593. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00593 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Cheung EFC, Lui SSY, Wang Y, Yang TX, Shum DHK, Chan RCK. Time-based but not event-based prospective memory remains impaired one year after the onset of schizophrenia: a prospective study. Schizophr Res. 2015;169(1–3):147–152. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.09.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Liu D, Ji C, Zhuo K, et al. Impaired cue identification and intention retrieval underlie prospective memory deficits in patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2017;51(3):270–277. doi: 10.1177/0004867416640097 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Lui SS, Wang Y, Yang TX, et al. Problems in remembering to carry out future actions in first-episode schizophrenia: primary or secondary impairment? J Psychiatr Res. 2015;61:141–149. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.11.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Ordemann GJ, Opper J, Davalos D. Prospective memory in schizophrenia: a review. Schizophr Res. 2014;155(1–3):77–89. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.03.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment are provided here courtesy of Dove Press

RESOURCES