Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2019 May 17;8:e43194. doi: 10.7554/eLife.43194

Accelerated redevelopment of vocal skills is preceded by lasting reorganization of the song motor circuitry

Michiel Vellema 1,2,, Mariana Diales Rocha 1, Sabrina Bascones 3, Sándor Zsebők 4, Jes Dreier 5, Stefan Leitner 1, Annemie Van der Linden 2, Jonathan Brewer 5, Manfred Gahr 1
Editors: Richard B Ivry6, Richard B Ivry7
PMCID: PMC6570526  PMID: 31099755

Abstract

Complex motor skills take considerable time and practice to learn. Without continued practice the level of skill performance quickly degrades, posing a problem for the timely utilization of skilled motor behaviors. Here we quantified the recurring development of vocal motor skills and the accompanying changes in synaptic connectivity in the brain of a songbird, while manipulating skill performance by consecutively administrating and withdrawing testosterone. We demonstrate that a songbird with prior singing experience can significantly accelerate the re-acquisition of vocal performance. We further demonstrate that an increase in vocal performance is accompanied by a pronounced synaptic pruning in the forebrain vocal motor area HVC, a reduction that is not reversed when birds stop singing. These results provide evidence that lasting synaptic changes in the motor circuitry are associated with the savings of motor skills, enabling a rapid recovery of motor performance under environmental time constraints.

Research organism: Other

eLife digest

Developing a complex skill, for example learning how to play the violin, takes considerable time and effort. If you then abandon the violin for months or years, your ability to play will deteriorate over time. However, when you do pick up the violin again you will be able to recover your proficiency much faster than you did when learning for the first time.

It therefore seems that while first learning a complex motor skill, some sort of memory is formed and stored. Learning and memory in general rely on the patterns of connections, called synapses, among neurons in the brain. Early in development, neurons make too many of these connections. This network is then refined over time as unneeded connections are discarded. However, the structural changes to the network that make it easier to re-acquire a skill were not well understood.

Canaries are a useful example of this sort of learning. Young males learn complex songs that are critical for their ability to attract a mate, and can quickly re-acquire their songs at the beginning of each mating season. Female canaries do not normally sing but will develop songs if they are implanted with a testosterone-releasing device. When the implant is removed, they stop singing.

To investigate how songbirds re-acquire songs, Vellema et al. gave female canaries a testosterone implant, and the birds gradually developed songs. The implant was then removed, and for two and a half months the birds did not sing. Vellema et al. then gave the canaries a second testosterone treatment. The canaries rapidly began to sing songs that were strikingly similar to the ones they sang during the initial learning period.

Examining the brains of these canaries revealed that major structural changes in brain connections occurred while the canaries first developed their songs. After the initial period of testosterone exposure, the female canaries had fewer synapses in a brain region that is associated with learning and producing motor tasks. Importantly, this reorganization of the brain circuitry was irreversible.

These findings provide fundamental insights into how we learn and maintain new motor skills. Similar rapid re-learning phenomena are observed in other areas of neuroscience, and future research can explore whether the mechanism described here extends to these areas. Possibly, this mechanism could also illuminate how we can regain skills lost after trauma or injury.

Introduction

Complex motor skills, such as singing or playing an instrument, are not inherently determined but need to be acquired through repetitive practice. Many specialized skills are used only incidentally however, and skill performance degrades during intermittent periods of non-use. This means that skills need to be re-acquired each time the need arises. For example, trained human surgeons need to re-acquire their surgical proficiency after a period of absence (Crewther et al., 2016), Capuchin monkeys need to acquire manipulative foraging skills to retrieve difficult-to-access, seasonally available food items (Eadie, 2015), and songbirds need to re-develop high-quality songs to attract a mate after wintering or long-distance migration (Slagsvold, 1976). Whereas learning novel motor skills is generally a lengthy process, the short-term availability of suitable mates and food items imposes a considerable time pressure on the re-acquisition of such specialized skills.

Birdsong, an established model system for vocal motor learning (Brainard and Doupe, 2013), is characterized by a high degree of complex and rapid acoustic modulations. The fine motor skills that are necessary to produce such complex, high quality vocalizations are thought to advertise an individual’s quality through performance-related characteristics such as song complexity and/or production rate (Podos, 1997; Drăgănoiu et al., 2002; Holveck and Riebel, 2007; Byers et al., 2010). When juvenile songbirds are one to two months old they start singing noisy, unstructured ‘subsongs’ akin to human babbling (Doupe and Kuhl, 1999), which gradually develop into variable, but recognizable species-specific ‘plastic songs’ (Marler and Peters, 1982a; Nottebohm et al., 1986; Tchernichovski et al., 2001; Mori et al., 2018). With extensive vocal rehearsal the songs slowly consolidate into high performance ‘crystallized songs’, a process that can take up more than five months in some songbird species (Nottebohm et al., 1986). For adult songbirds that annually need to re-acquire their songs this process of juvenile song development greatly exceeds the time that is available to attract a mate during the early breeding season. Thus, adult songbirds must considerably speed-up song re-acquisition to quickly produce songs of adequate quality to impress conspecifics. It is currently unclear how songbirds cope with the seasonally imposed time pressure on song development, and what mechanisms may be involved to ensure reproductive success under such time constraints in nature.

The seasonal development and degradation in vocal performance are highly correlated with changes in testosterone levels (Nottebohm et al., 1987; Smith et al., 1997; Tramontin et al., 2000; Voigt and Leitner, 2008), and are often accompanied by gross anatomical and cytoarchitectural restructuring of the brain areas involved in song production (Nottebohm, 1981; Gahr, 1990; Kirn et al., 1994; Kafitz et al., 1999; Thompson and Brenowitz, 2005; Balthazart et al., 2008; Vellema et al., 2014). Thus testosterone-regulated adaptations of the songbird brain may play an important role in optimizing song performance. In this study we used adult female canaries (Serinus canaria) to investigate how a periodically acquired motor behavior attains a high performance level under developmental time pressure. Although female canaries rarely sing spontaneously, and never produce high quality songs under natural conditions (Pesch and Guttinger, 1985), the systemic application of testosterone leads to the development of high-performance song with male-typical features (Leonard, 1939; Shoemaker, 1939; Vallet et al., 1996). This trait provides us with a well-defined behavioral baseline, and allows us to manipulate in detail the onset and offset of song development through repeated testosterone treatments.

Here we demonstrate that during a protracted testosterone treatment, adult female canaries gradually develop stable, species-typical songs through a process of song crystallization similar to what is known for naturally-raised juvenile male canaries (Mori et al., 2018). Re-treatment with testosterone several months after birds have stopped singing, leads to a rapid recurrence of song performance with song features that strongly resemble those after the first treatment. We propose that once developed, vocal motor memories are retained for an extended period of time, enabling adult animals to recover song performance quickly at a later time, a process termed ‘savings’ (Ebbinghaus, 2013). We further demonstrate that neurons in the songbird’s premotor nucleus HVC (proper name), a central nucleus in the brain circuitry that controls song production (Nottebohm et al., 1976), irreversibly loses a significant number of dendritic spines during the initial testosterone-induced development of vocal skills. This state of reduced spine density is subsequently maintained over long periods in which testosterone levels are low and vocal skills are not used. The observed lasting synaptic pruning could play an important role in the formation of vocal skill savings, enabling birds to rapidly re-acquire song performance within a restricted time period.

Results

Testosterone-induced song development in female canaries

To study the development of vocal skills in adult female canaries we recorded and analyzed the entire song ontogeny in acoustically separated animals (5.6 ± 0.6 million song syllables per bird), while manipulating song output by consecutively implanting, removing, and re-implanting birds with testosterone implants.

Whereas no songs were observed prior to treatment, the systemic application of testosterone (T1+) stimulated birds to start singing their first songs after three days (3.22 ± 0.46 days). The phases of female song development were similar to those previously reported for male canaries (Figure 1) (Nottebohm et al., 1986; Mori et al., 2018; Weichel et al., 1986). The first subsongs were produced in an irregular fashion and consisted of unstructured syllables with few distinctive features. Songs gradually developed into plastic songs in which different phrases of repeated syllables could be clearly distinguished, albeit with variation between syllables of the same type. During the following six months, female canary songs continued to develop, gradually crystallizing into the species-typical stable songs. Although structurally similar to male canary songs, female songs had relatively small syllable repertoires (6.2 ± 1.1 syllable types), consistent with previous reports (Vallet et al., 1996; Fusani et al., 2003; Hartog et al., 2009).

Figure 1. Song in juvenile male and adult female canaries progresses through the same stages of development.

Figure 1.

(A) Schematic of natural song development in juvenile males. (B) Example spectral derivative spectrograms illustrating the different song developmental stages of a male canary. Subsong was recorded at 45 days of age, plastic song at 120 days of age and stable song at 1 year old. (C) Schematic of song development in adult female canaries during a first testosterone treatment (T1+), after removal of testosterone (T1-), and during a second testosterone treatment (T2+). (D) Example spectral derivative spectrograms from an adult female canary illustrating subsong after 3 days of testosterone treatment, plastic song after 30 days of treatment, and stable song after 200 days of treatment. Lowercase letters indicate different phrases of repeated syllables.

Re-application of testosterone triggers an accelerated re-acquisition of song performance

To investigate the birds’ abilities to re-acquire song performance after a period without vocal practice we withdrew testosterone (T1-) from the animals, completely abolishing singing behavior in approximately three days (3.14 ± 0.63 days). After 2½ months of no song production birds were treated for a second time with testosterone (T2+), inducing song output after three days (3.14 ± 0.74 days). No subsongs were observed after the second testosterone treatment, and all birds were able to produce plastic songs from the first day of singing.

We first compared the development of temporal song features during the first and second testosterone treatments (Figure 2, Figure 2—figure supplement 1, Figure 2—figure supplement 2). Particularly the speed at which subsequent syllables are repeated, the syllable repetition rate (SR), has been strongly associated with individual performance (Podos, 1997; Podos, 1996) and has been shown to increase the attractiveness of the song to conspecifics (Vallet et al., 1998; Vallet and Kreutzer, 1995). We observed that during the first two weeks of testosterone-induced female song development, all syllables were typically produced at a similar rate (SR: 10.3 ± 1.3 Hz). While practicing the song, different syllables were gradually sung at different rates, becoming more distinct towards song crystallization (Figure 2A). Once crystallized, syllables could be distinguished as fast syllables (SR: 21.7 ± 1.0 Hz), medium-speed syllables (SR: 11.9 ± 1.1 Hz), and slow syllables (SR: 4.9 ± 0.7 Hz). A similar distribution of syllable rates redeveloped during a 2nd testosterone treatment. The observed range of syllable rates was similar to what has previously been reported for wild-living male canaries (Leitner et al., 2001).

Figure 2. Syllable repetition rates develop faster in birds with prior singing experience.

(A) Syllable rate histogram from a female canary during two subsequent testosterone treatments. Color scales indicate the number of daily syllables produced for each discrete syllable rate. Curves were fitted through the most occurring syllable rates and are shown in red. (B) The correlation coefficient (CC) between the stabilized distribution of syllable rates at the end of the 1st testosterone treatment and each other day during the development and re-development of song in the same bird as shown in A. (C) The mean syllable rate correlation plot for all animals. Grey bars indicate the SEM. (D) Group statistics of all studied birds demonstrating that stable syllable rates were achieved more quickly during a 2nd testosterone treatment (red bars) than during the 1st treatment (blue bars). (E) The peak day-to-day increase in the syllable rate CC (dmax) was higher during a 2nd testosterone treatment than during the 1st treatment. (F) Example spectral derivative spectrograms from one bird and corresponding bar graphs illustrating syllable rates at 15, 70, and 200 days after a 1st testosterone treatment and 30 days after a 2nd treatment. Columns in D-E represent the mean ± SEM and open circles indicate individual data points (*p<0.05, ***p≤0.001, paired t-test, n = 6 animals). Columns in F represent the mean ± SEM (a,b,c: p≤0.001, ANOVA; n = 100 syllables). Source data for temporal song features are available in the Figure 2—source data 1.

Figure 2—source data 1. Source file for quantitative comparisons of temporal song features.
elife-43194-fig2-data1.xlsx (165.1KB, xlsx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.43194.007

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Differential re-development of temporal song features.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Mean syllable duration correlation plot for all animals showing a gradual development during a 1st testosterone treatment (T1+), followed by an immediate recovery of syllable duration during a 2nd treatment (T2+). (B) Group statistics demonstrating that stable syllable durations were achieved more quickly during a 2nd testosterone treatment (red bars) than during the 1st treatment (blue bars). (C) The peak day-to-day increase in the syllable duration CC (dmax) for T1+. Dmax could not be calculated for T2+, as we observed no developmental increase of this song feature during the 2nd testosterone treatment. (D) Mean correlation plot for the pause duration between subsequent song syllables. Pause durations stabilized relatively fast during a 1st testosterone treatment (T1+), and required a phase of re-development during a 2nd treatment (T2+). (E) Group statistics demonstrating that stable pause durations were achieved more quickly during a 2nd testosterone treatment (red bars) than during the 1st treatment (blue bars). (F) The peak day-to-day increase in the pause duration CC (dmax) was significantly higher for T2+ than for T1+. Grey bars in A and D indicate the SEM. Columns in B,C,E,F represent the mean ± SEM and open circles indicate individual data points (*p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p≤0.001, paired t-test, n = 6 animals).
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Development and re-development of temporal song features.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

Example song feature histograms from one individual illustrating the gradual acquisition during a 1st testosterone treatment (T1+) and the rapid recurrence during a 2nd treatment (T2+) for (A) syllable duration, and (B) pause duration. (C,D) Example spectral derivative spectrograms from two different birds illustrating a slower syllable rate with longer pause durations at the onset of song production during the 2nd (T2+ 3) testosterone treatment compared to stable songs during the 1st (T1+ 200) and 2nd (T2+ 30) treatment.

To determine how fast this syllable rate pattern emerged during subsequent testosterone treatments we took crystallized song from the last week of the first testosterone treatment as a reference pattern, and calculated the correlation coefficient (CC) between this reference and each day of song development and re-development (Figure 2B & C). While syllable repetition rates gradually developed and stabilized over the course of 160 ± 8 days during the first testosterone treatment, stable rates were obtained more than seven times faster, within 22 ± 2 days, during the second treatment (Figure 2D; paired t-test: p<0.001). In addition the maximum daily increase in similarity (dmax) was also significantly higher during song re-development than during initial song development (Figure 2E; dmax: 0.021 ± 0.005 for T1+, and 0.078 ± 0.012 for T2+; paired t-test: p<0.05), indicating a much accelerated development of song performance in birds with previous singing experience.

Since syllable rate is a compound feature that is determined by both syllable duration and the interval between subsequent syllables, we analyzed syllable durations and pause durations separately. Syllable durations developed slowly during the initial song acquisition (T1+), reaching stable values after 153 ± 19 days, while pause durations stabilized in 82 ± 13 days (Figure 2—figure supplement 1, Figure 2—figure supplement 2). Both temporal features re-emerged more quickly during song re-acquisition (T2+) than during the first song developmental phase (duration: 4.8 ± 1.3 days; paired t-test: p<0.001; pause: 10.7 ± 2.2 days; paired t-test: p<0.01; Figure 2—figure supplement 1).

To determine the bird’s ability to recover spectral song patterns we analyzed the recurring patterns of frequency modulation (FM), amplitude modulation (AM), bandwidth (BW), mean frequency (MF) and Wiener entropy (E) during song acquisition and re-acquisition. Similar to temporal song features, spectral features were more quickly established during song re-acquisition (T2+) than during the initial song development (T1+) (Figure 3, Figure 3—figure supplement 1, Figure 3—figure supplement 2). All spectral features developed gradually over more than 110 days before reaching stable values. After a period in which the birds did not produce any song, testosterone-induced song re-acquisition led to a rapid stabilization of spectral patterns within 16 days (Figure 3, Figure 3—figure supplement 1; paired t test: p<0.01). Thus both temporal and spectral song features were acquired significantly faster by experienced birds that had developed singing skills once before.

Figure 3. Differential re-development of song features.

(A) Mean correlation plot for all animals illustrating the development of the frequency modulation (FM) distribution in the song during the development (T1+) and re-development (T2+) of song. The correlation plot illustrates a gradual FM development during T1+, followed by a short phase of FM re-development during T2+. (B) Group statistics demonstrating that the stabilization of the FM distribution in the song took less time during a 2nd testosterone treatment (red bars) than during the 1st treatment (blue bars). (C) The peak day-to-day increase in the FM CC (dmax) was significantly higher during a 2nd testosterone treatment than during the 1st treatment. (D) Mean correlation plot for the syllable bandwidth showing a gradual development during a 1st testosterone treatment (T1+), followed by an immediate recovery of syllable bandwidth during a 2nd treatment (T2+). (E) Group statistics demonstrating that stable syllable bandwidths were achieved more quickly during a 2nd testosterone treatment (red bars) than during the 1st treatment (blue bars). (F) The peak day-to-day increase in the syllable bandwidth CC (dmax) for T1+. Dmax could not be calculated for T2+, as we observed no developmental increase of this song feature during the 2nd testosterone treatment. (G) Deterioration in the distribution patterns of all analyzed song features during absence of song production (T1-), and (H) subsequent recovery of song features during testosterone-induced re-development of song (T2+). Grey bars in A and D indicate the SEM. Columns in B,C and E-H represent the mean ± SEM and open circles indicate individual data points (*p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p≤0.001, paired t-test (B,C,E,F) and one-sample t-test (G,H), n = 6 animals). Source data for acoustic features are available in the Figure 3—source data 1.

Figure 3—source data 1. Source file for quantitative comparisons of spectral song features.
elife-43194-fig3-data1.xlsx (206.7KB, xlsx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.43194.011

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Differential re-development of spectral song features.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(A) Mean amplitude modulation (AM) correlation plot for all animals. The AM distribution demonstrated a gradual consolidation during the 1st testosterone treatment (T1+), and displayed a short phase of re-development during a 2nd testosterone treatment (T2+) (B,C). Both mean frequency (D–F) and wiener entropy (G–I) demonstrated a gradual development during the 1st testosterone treatment (T1+) followed by an immediate recovery during a 2nd treatment (T2+). Dmax could not be calculated for T2+ in F,I, as we observed no developmental increase of these song features during the 2nd testosterone treatment. Grey bars in A, D and G indicate the SEM. Columns in B,C, E,F, and H,I represent the mean ± SEM and open circles indicate individual data points (**p<0.01, ***p≤0.001, paired t-test, n = 6 animal).
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Development and re-development of spectral song features.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

Example song feature histograms illustrating the gradual acquisition during a 1st testosterone treatment (T1+) and the rapid recurrence during a 2nd treatment (T2+) for (A) frequency modulation, (B) amplitude modulation, (C) bandwidth, (D) mean frequency, and (E) Wiener entropy. (F,G) Example spectral derivative spectrograms from stable songs at 200 days after a 1st testosterone treatment (T1+ 200), plastic songs at 3 days after a 2nd testosterone treatment (T2+ 3), and stable songs 30 days after a 2nd treatment (T2+ 30).

The absence of song practice causes selective deterioration of song features

Whereas song re-acquisition in female canaries resulted in a rapid recurrence of stable temporal and spectral patterns, not all song features re-developed in the same way. Most notably, some song features demonstrated a clear deterioration in the absence of vocal practice, while other song features remained stable despite the absence of vocal practice (Figure 3G & H).

Of the studied temporal song parameters, syllable durations did not show any deterioration in the period that birds did not sing (T1-) and thus also did not need to be re-acquired (Figure 2—figure supplement 1A–C; Figure 2—figure supplement 2A). Both the syllable rate and the pause duration between syllables did deteriorate however, and a short phase of re-development was required to recover the originally developed patterns (Figure 2, Figure 2—figure supplement 1D–F, Figure 2—figure supplement 2B). The accelerated re-acquisition of syllable repetition rates thus appears to be driven by the need to re-optimize the timing between syllables rather than syllable lengths.

Of the spectral syllable features that were studied, the FM and AM patterns deteriorated when birds stopped singing (T1-), but were re-acquired during a short developmental phase after a 2nd testosterone treatment (T2+) (Figure 3A–C, Figure 3—figure supplement 1A–C, Figure 3—figure supplement 2A & B). BW, MF, and E did not need to be re-acquired, but maintained strong similarities with the initial acoustic patterns despite the absence of song production in the intermittent period (T1-) (Figure 3D–F, Figure 3—figure supplement 1D–I, Figure 3—figure supplement 2C–E). Thus song re-acquisition can be characterized by recalling a combination of sound features that immediately can be reproduced and sound features that require a short phase of redevelopment.

Song similarity during subsequent testosterone treatments

The songs that reappeared during the second testosterone treatment were strikingly similar to those that developed after the first treatment (Figure 4). Visual inspection of the song spectrograms revealed no differences in syllable repertoire when comparing crystallized songs during the first and second testosterone treatment (Figure 4A & B; 6.2 ± 1.1 syllable types for both treatments). In addition both temporal and spectral song features demonstrated a strong similarity in their distribution patterns between subsequent testosterone treatments (T1xT2), with correlation coefficients above 0.82 for all studied song features (Figure 4C). These values were not significantly different from the correlation coefficients obtained by cross-correlating distribution patterns from consecutive days during the first treatment period (T1xT1) for any of the measured song features (Figure 4C), indicating that the song patterns that developed during the first and second testosterone treatment are similar in structure.

Figure 4. Similarity of time and frequency parameters after subsequent testosterone treatments.

(A,B) Example spectral derivative spectrograms of stable song from two animals during the 1st (T1+) and 2nd (T2+) testosterone treatment illustrating a strong similarity in song structure. (C) Similarity analyses between stable song patterns from the 1st and 2nd testosterone treatment periods (T1xT2, red bars) demonstrated a high level of correlation of more than 80% for all analyzed song features. Correlation coefficients between songs from T1+ and T2+ were not significantly different from the CCs obtained when cross-correlating song patterns within the T1+ period (T1xT1, blue bars). Columns in C represent the mean ± SEM and open circles indicate individual data points (NS, paired t-test, n = 6 animals). Source data for similarity calculations are available in the Figure 4—source data 1.

Figure 4—source data 1. Source file for song similarity calculations between subsequent testosterone treatments.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.43194.015

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Syllable similarity during subsequent testosterone treatments.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

Euclidean distances are shown between syllables of the same type during a 1st testosterone treatment (T1xT1, blue bars), between syllables of the same type between the 1st and 2nd testosterone treatment (T1xT2, red bars), and between syllables of different types between the 1st and 2nd testosterone treatment (T1xT2ext, green bars). (A,B) Euclidean distances between individual song syllables and their means within and between testosterone treatments for the two birds shown in Figure 4. Lowercase letters correspond to the song phrases indicated in Figure 4A and B. (C) Group statistics demonstrating a strong similarity between syllables of the same type, but not between syllables of different types when comparing song syllables between the two testosterone treatments. Columns in A-C represent the mean ± SEM and open circles in C indicate individual data points (a,b: p<0.001, ANOVA, n = 6 animals).
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Similarity of song syntax during subsequent testosterone treatments.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

(A,B) Example probability distributions of song phrase transitions from two animals during the 1st (T1+, blue bars) and 2nd (T2+, red bars) testosterone treatment illustrating the strong similarity in phrase transitions between the two treatment periods. (C–E) Group statistics demonstrating stable consistency (C), linearity (D) and entropy (E) of phrase transitions during the two developmental phases. Columns in C-E represent the mean ± SEM and open circles indicate individual data points (NS, paired t-test, n = 6 animals).

To determine the similarity of syllables produced during the 1st and 2nd testosterone treatments we calculated for each bird the Euclidean distance across all eight analyzed temporal and spectral features within and between syllable types (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). The Euclidean distance between two syllables provides a measure of similarity where a syllable is represented as a point with coordinates that correspond to the eight analyzed sound features. The distance between syllables with similar sound features and thus similar coordinates is low, while the distance between syllables with differing sound features is high. The mean Euclidean distances between syllables of the same type across the 1st and 2nd testosterone treatment (T1xT2) were not different from the Euclidean distances between syllables from subsequent days during the 1st testosterone treatment (T1xT1: d = 0.73 ± 0.04, T1xT2: d = 0.76 ± 0.06; Tukey’s HSD: p=0.99). The mean Euclidean distances between syllables of different types across the two treatment periods were significantly larger than those between syllables of the same type (T1xT2ext: d = 2.12 ± 0.23; Tukey’s HSD: p<0.001). These results indicate that for a given syllable the sound features that redeveloped after the 2nd testosterone treatment fell within the range of variation observed during the 1st testosterone treatment for that same syllable type, but were clearly distinguishable from the sound features of other syllable types.

To investigate if the sequential structure of the song was different between the two testosterone treatments we analyzed the probability that specific phrase transitions occurred in the songs (Figure 4—figure supplement 2). Because the syllable repertoire is relatively small in testosterone treated female canaries, the number of possible phrase transitions is also small. Nevertheless, on average only 8.7% of all possible phrase transitions were used in the songs. Thus whereas the song sequences are not fixed as in for example zebra finches, the song sequences are not random either, following a limited subset of possible transitions similar to what has been observed in male canaries (Leitner et al., 2001; Markowitz et al., 2013). The percentage of phrase transitions that made up the songs during the 1st and 2nd testosterone treatments were not different (T1+: 9.2 ± 4.5%; T2+: 8.3 ± 4.2%; paired t-test: p=0.59), and the probability distribution of these transitions were visually similar between treatments (Figure 4—figure supplement 2A & B). The linearity, consistency, and entropy of song phrase transitions did not differ significantly between the songs from the 1st and 2nd treatment periods (Figure 4—figure supplement 2C).

Together these data strongly suggest that testosterone-induced song re-acquisition in canaries is driven towards the previously acquired song pattern.

Singing-related pruning of neuronal dendritic spines

The finding that the re-acquisition of song performance progresses considerably faster than the initial acquisition and leads to the production of highly similar song patterns suggests that singing skills are retained during intermittent silent periods. Motor learning and memory are generally considered to rely on the maturation and consolidation of the synaptic connections within the neural circuitry that drives the behavior in question (Changeux and Danchin, 1976; Yu and Zuo, 2011; Hoshiba et al., 2017). To investigate changes in synaptic connectivity during vocal motor development we quantified the number of neuronal dendritic spines in the forebrain motor nucleus HVC and the robust motor nucleus of the arcopallium (RA) before, during, and after the acquisition of vocal motor skills (Figure 5).

Figure 5. Lasting synaptic pruning in the forebrain motor nucleus HVC.

Figure 5.

(A) Photomicrographs of dendrite segments from non-singing control animals (C), singing female canaries sacrificed after five months of testosterone treatment (T1+), and non-singing individuals sacrificed 2.5 months after testosterone withdrawal (T1-). (B) Compared to naive control birds (C), spine densities were significantly reduced in testosterone-treated, singing birds (T1+), and remained significantly reduced up to 2.5 months after birds stopped singing by withdrawing testosterone (T1-). (C) No significant differences in spine densities were observed in RA between the experimental periods. (D–F) The probability distribution of dendrites with different spine densities in HVC demonstrated a shift towards more dendrites with fewer spines in testosterone treated (T1+), singing birds and testosterone removed (T1-), non-singing birds compared to non-singing control birds. Columns in B-F represent the mean ± SEM and open circles indicate individual data points (a,b: p<0.01, ANOVA, n = 6 animals). Scale bar = 100 µm. Source data for spine quantifications are available in the Figure 5—source data 1.

Figure 5—source data 1. Source file for spine quantification data.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.43194.017

Excitatory projection neurons in the HVC of testosterone-treated, singing female canaries displayed a more than 30% reduction in dendritic spine density compared to untreated, non-singing control birds (C: 0.95 µm−1, T1+: 0.64 µm−1; Dunnett’s test: p<0.01), indicating a significant synaptic pruning during the first acquisition of singing skills (Figure 5B). Compared to non-singing control birds, spine densities remained significantly lower in birds that stopped singing for 2.5 months after testosterone removal (C: 0.95 µm−1, T1-: 0.69 µm−1; Dunnett’s test: p<0.01). Thus, the synaptic pruning in HVC associated with testosterone-induced song acquisition is not reversed when birds stop singing, but instead the pruned state is maintained in periods in which the birds do not sing (Figure 5B). In contrast, we did not observe any changes in dendritic spine densities in RA across the different experimental periods (Figure 5C; ANOVA: p=0.857).

Strong differences were observed in the density of dendritic spines between different spinous neurites in HVC (range: 0.15 to 2.14 spines/µm). To investigate which neurite types were associated with the observed spine pruning we calculated the probability distribution of neuronal dendrites in HVC based on their spine density (Figure 5D–F). We observed a marked shift in the distribution of dendrites from more densely-spined dendrites in control birds towards less densely-spined dendrites in the testosterone treatment (T1+) and removal groups (T1-), reflecting a global decrease in spine density. The strongest reduction was detected in the range of dendrites with more than 0.8 spines per µm, which halved in number in both testosterone-treated, singing birds (T1+), and testosterone-removed, non-singing birds (T1-) compared to non-singing controls (C: 0.64 ± 0.06, T1+: 0.28 ± 0.05, T1-: 0.33 ± 0.06; ANOVA: p<0.01). The total density of both spinous and aspinous neurites in HVC did not significantly differ between the treatment groups (ANOVA: p>0.36), suggesting that changes in dendrite densities were not caused by specific cell death.

These data suggest that the observed vocal motor acquisition is accompanied by a lasting synaptic pruning specifically in nucleus HVC, providing a possible anatomical site for motor memory that could facilitate the observed rapid re-acquisition of previously established song patterns.

Discussion

The ability to learn new skills enables one to adapt to changes in the surrounding environment. Whereas learning new skills requires time and effort, the survival of many species relies on their capacity to utilize specialized skills in a timely fashion, capitalizing on the short-term availability of foods, mates, and other environmental factors (Prendergast et al., 2002). Such a capacity is biologically relevant for numerous skills in a multitude of species, ranging from periodic foraging skills in dolphins (Patterson et al., 2016), bats (Clarin et al., 2013), and monkeys (Boinski and Fragaszy, 1989) to courtship displays in crabs (Mowles et al., 2017) and songbirds (Ota et al., 2015). As the use of these specialized skills is interrupted over longer periods, for example during long-distance migration or hibernation (Slagsvold, 1976; Prendergast et al., 2002), such skills need to be re-acquired quickly to maximize survival and reproductive success. In this study we establish the repeated, hormone-driven acquisition of vocal motor performance in female canaries as a model system for studying motor savings. Using this model, we show that while the initial development of specialized motor skills can be a lengthy process, motor performance can quickly be re-acquired at a later time, even after extended periods of non-use (Figure 2 and 3). The accelerated song re-acquisition observed in this study indicates that acquiring a motor skill for the first time follows a different developmental trajectory than re-acquiring the same skill at a later time. This is both evident from the accelerated consolidation of song performance during song re-development (Figure 2 and 3), and the observation that birds skip the subsong phase of song development when re-acquiring their songs. Our data further suggest that songbirds have the ability to retain previously developed singing skills through a process of selective pruning of the neural circuitry (Figure 5), enabling them to rapidly recover songs of adequate quality to attract a mate before breeding opportunities disappear.

Several song parameters were maintained across a period in which birds did not sing, while other song features demonstrated clear deterioration (Figure 3G & H). Birds were able to recover those deteriorated song features with renewed vocal practice. At the peripheral level, song performance depends on the accurate control of the respiratory system, the sound producing organ (the syrinx), and the upper vocal tract. Interestingly, those song features that required a phase of re-development to be optimized seem primarily associated with motor constraints in syrinx function (Podos, 1997; Geberzahn and Aubin, 2014). Online modulation of frequency and amplitude requires active contractile modulation of syringeal muscles (Goller and Suthers, 1996; Elemans et al., 2008). Syllable repetition and the inter-syllabic interval also depend on how rapidly syringeal abductor and adductor muscles can open and close the syringeal aperture. The upper vocal tract may act as a tunable acoustic filter of the sounds produced by the syrinx (Nowicki, 1987; Beckers et al., 2003; Riede et al., 2006) and play a more important role in establishing the bandwidth, entropy and mean frequency of song syllables. Thus song parameters linked to vocal tract modulation may be preserved after birds stopped singing, while song parameters linked to contraction speed of syringeal muscles appear to deteriorate.

Whereas male canaries are known to annually re-organize their songs, even when deafened (Mori et al., 2018), no such modifications were observed in our study. It is not clear to what extent annual changes in male canary song structure are due to relearning or are driven by a selection process from a previously learned collection of syllable types during juvenile development (Nottebohm et al., 1986; Marler, 1997; Gardner et al., 2005; Belzner et al., 2009). Considering that the female canaries in our study were acoustically separated during the testosterone-induced development of song suggests that the emerging song patterns are either for a large part innate (Mori et al., 2018; Gardner et al., 2005), or are limited to a memorized collection of syllable types that they heard earlier in life (Belzner et al., 2009; Marler and Peters, 1981; Marler and Peters, 1982b; Prather et al., 2010; Kiefer et al., 2014). The initial song acquisition may thus rely on an innate or memorized auditory template, while the accelerated song re-acquisition may be driven by a motor memory trace of what the birds learned to produce during the first song acquisition phase. The syllable repertoires of our female canaries were much lower than what is common for males. Such a small vocabulary would limit the possibilities to re-arrange or replace syllables without compromising song complexity, and thus few modifications to female songs can be expected.

During juvenile brain development in humans, mammals, and birds, synaptic connections are initially overproduced, followed by protracted, activity-dependent synapse elimination during puberty (Huttenlocher, 1990; Petanjek et al., 2011; Rakic et al., 1986; Zuo et al., 2005; Nixdorf-Bergweiler et al., 1995). By selectively stabilizing functional synapses while pruning away inactive synapses, brain circuits are thought to reduce neuronal plasticity, consolidating newly learned information and reducing unnecessary redundancy (Changeux and Danchin, 1976). The lasting spine pruning observed in the forebrain song control area HVC (Figure 5) could reflect such a structural modification facilitating the long-term memorization of vocal patterns. Optical imaging studies have demonstrated a rapid accumulation and stabilization of neuronal dendritic spines in HVC during developmental song learning in zebra finches (Roberts et al., 2010). In the mouse cortex, morphological modifications to dendritic spines have been observed that lasted well beyond the learning experience that caused the spine modifications (Hofer et al., 2006; Yang et al., 2009; Xu et al., 2009; Hofer et al., 2009), strongly suggesting that long-term memory retention of previously acquired skills is facilitated by stably maintained synaptic connections. Despite such learning-related circuit modifications, the large majority of dendritic spines in the mammalian brain are stably maintained in adulthood (Petanjek et al., 2011; Rakic et al., 1986; Zuo et al., 2005; Yang et al., 2009; Xu et al., 2009; Grutzendler et al., 2002). In contrast we observed a strong spine reduction in fully adult birds (Figure 5), suggesting that the observed synaptic pruning in the adult canary HVC during song acquisition may reflect a developmentally delayed, activity-dependent consolidation of the neural motor circuitry.

A recent study in zebra finches has shown that singing prevention during juvenile development can also avert developmental spine pruning in the robust motor nucleus of the arcopallium (RA) (Hayase et al., 2018). In addition, auditory input during sensorimotor learning may play an important role in circuit consolidation, and auditory deprivation has been shown to delay the natural juvenile maturation of HVC in zebra finches (Huang et al., 2018). It is not known how long such developmental processes can be postponed, but we did not observe delayed synaptic pruning in RA upon song acquisition in adult female canaries (Figure 5C), suggesting that the time-window for large-scale spine pruning in RA may be more limited than in HVC. The developmentally delayed maturation of HVC (Figure 5B) is consistent with our observation that the delayed, testosterone-induced development of song performance in adult female canaries follows a juvenile male-like developmental trajectory (Figure 1). Thus, in the absence of singing behavior, parts of the vocal motor circuit may remain plastic in adult female canaries, only consolidating once neural activity within the circuitry increases to drive testosterone-induced song output.

Testosterone-induced song development in adult female canaries has previously been associated with changes in other neural attributes in HVC as well, including an increase in volume, neuron numbers (but not densities), increased vascularization, and changes in cellular structure (Vellema et al., 2014; Hartog et al., 2009; Gahr and Garcia-Segura, 1996; Nottebohm, 1980; Rasika et al., 1994; Louissaint et al., 2002). Many of the observed modifications to the song system follow an annually changing pattern in seasonally breeding male songbirds (Nottebohm et al., 1986; Smith et al., 1997; Nottebohm, 1981; Kirn et al., 1994), indicating that in males these modifications are not maintained outside the breeding season when birds no longer sing high performance song. In addition, withdrawing testosterone from male Gambel’s white-crowned sparrows, another well-studied seasonal singer, resulted in a rapid regression in HVC size, neuron number, and neuron size (Thompson et al., 2007), indicating that these neural modifications are reversible and unlikely to reflect long-term memorization of vocal patterns. The lasting synaptic pruning that we observed in HVC is to our knowledge the first report of a song-related modification to the adult songbird brain that persists after the behavior itself is no longer expressed.

HVC is at a central position between the motor circuitry that controls song output and a forebrain-basal ganglia circuit, analogous to the mammalian cortico-basal ganglia (CBG) circuit, which feeds back on the motor circuitry for song production (Brainard and Doupe, 2013; Nottebohm et al., 1976). Firing patterns in HVC neurons that project to RA are closely associated with millisecond precision of syllable timing (Yu and Margoliash, 1996; Hahnloser et al., 2002; Amador et al., 2013), suggesting that HVC is directly involved in maintaining time-related features of song performance. Burst patterns in RA neurons on the other hand are more closely associated with intrasyllabic structure (Yu and Margoliash, 1996; Chi and Margoliash, 2001). However, slowing down brain processes by local cooling of HVC has also been shown to change the intrasyllabic structure (Long and Fee, 2008; Alonso et al., 2015), indicating that both HVC and RA activity play a role in maintaining the fine temporal structure within syllables. The acquisition of song syllables in zebra finches is accompanied by increased timing accuracy of inhibitory neural firing within the HVC microcircuitry, suggesting that precisely-timed inhibition may play an important role in shaping song-related premotor sequences (Kosche et al., 2015; Vallentin et al., 2016). The lasting synaptic reorganization in HVC during song acquisition may be guided by such inhibitory activity and provide a neural scaffold for both intra and intersyllabic timing. RA-projecting HVC neurons are continuously replaced in the adult songbird brain (Alvarez-Buylla et al., 1990), providing a base for ongoing variability in syringeal muscle control. A permanently maintained HVC network may guide dynamically changing circuits in HVC and RA during subsequent periods of singing, quickly driving song features towards previously acquired patterns.

Neurons from HVC that project to the avian basal ganglia Area X are not replaced in adulthood, providing a permanent subpopulation of HVC neurons (Gahr, 1990; Alvarez-Buylla et al., 1988). Although we could not specifically distinguish between neuron types in our study, we observed the strongest reduction of 50% in the number of densely-spined dendrites in HVC after birds developed song. Previous studies have shown that those HVC neurons that project to Area X predominantly consist of neurons with densely-spined dendrites (Kornfeld et al., 2017; Nixdorf et al., 1989; Kubota and Taniguchi, 1998; Benton et al., 1998; Mooney, 2000; Mooney and Prather, 2005; Fortune and Margoliash, 1995). Thus our observed synaptic pruning may be related to a major reorganization within the HVC to Area X connection, a reorganization that could reflect the long-term memorization of vocal skills. CBG-like circuits have been strongly implicated in the learning and long-term retention of motor skills in humans (Albouy et al., 2008; Debas et al., 2010; Lehéricy et al., 2006; Poldrack et al., 2005; Coynel et al., 2010), mammals (Barnes et al., 2005; Pasupathy and Miller, 2005; Sheth et al., 2011; Yin et al., 2009), and birds (Bottjer et al., 1984; Scharff and Nottebohm, 1991; Brainard and Doupe, 2000; Kao et al., 2005; Olveczky et al., 2005; Aronov et al., 2008; Sober and Brainard, 2009; Andalman and Fee, 2009; Ölveczky et al., 2011; Charlesworth et al., 2012). Vocal exploration is driven by activity in the avian CBG circuit (Kao et al., 2005), which may guide synaptic modifications in plastic motor circuits (Mehaffey and Doupe, 2015), even in adulthood. Although selective elimination of X-projecting HVC neurons did not appear to alter the song structure of adult zebra finches (Scharff et al., 2000), altered expression of FoxP2 in Area X of adult male zebra finches has been associated with changes in vocal performance (Murugan et al., 2013; Thompson et al., 2013; Teramitsu and White, 2006). In addition neurotoxic lesions in Area X lead to changes in singing tempo and syllable sequencing in adult zebra finches (Kubikova et al., 2014), suggesting that the HVC to Area X connection continues to play a role in the long-term maintenance of vocal sequences.

The mechanisms behind learning-related, selective stabilization and elimination of dendritic spines remain largely unknown. Sex hormones may play an important role in the stabilization of such synaptic connections during the development of motor skills. Many of the brain areas that are involved in song learning and production express high numbers of androgen receptors (Arnold et al., 1976; Balthazart et al., 1992; Metzdorf et al., 1999; Nastiuk and Clayton, 1995; Gahr and Metzdorf, 1997), and X-projecting HVC neurons express estrogen receptors (Gahr, 1990) that can be activated after conversion of testosterone into estradiol. Furthermore, changes in neuronal dendritic growth and synaptic morphology in the songbird RA have been observed to accompany testosterone-induced song development in female canaries (DeVoogd and Nottebohm, 1981; Devoogd et al., 1985; Canady et al., 1988). In addition, Frankl-Vilches et al. (Frankl-Vilches et al., 2015) recently observed that 85% of the seasonally and testosterone upregulated genes in the canary HVC are related to neuron differentiation, axon, dendrite and synapse organization. In the brain of birds and mammals, sex hormones are known to upregulate the expression of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) (Dittrich et al., 1999; Rasika et al., 1999; Sohrabji et al., 1995), which in its turn contributes to the stabilization of synapses through the interaction with tyrosine receptor kinase B (TrkB) (Koleske, 2013). Furthermore, BDNF has been shown to be essential during testosterone-induced song development in female canaries (Hartog et al., 2009), providing a likely pathway by which sex hormones can consolidate motor skills by stabilizing synapses.

Testosterone levels and song consolidation are tightly linked, and it is difficult to determine if brain anatomical changes in our and other studies are caused by testosterone, singing activity or both. Interestingly, local intracerebral testosterone implants close to nucleus RA did not cause anatomical changes in this androgen receptor-rich brain area (Brenowitz and Lent, 2002), suggesting that modifications in RA are driven by singing-related, activity-dependent input from HVC. Whether or not testosterone, vocal practice, or both play a role in the observed synaptic pruning in HVC, the vocal circuitry demonstrated synaptic changes that persisted through several months in which testosterone levels were low and birds did not sing, potentially enabling the birds to quickly re-acquire their previously developed song patterns.

Sex hormones can be regulated by both environmental and social factors (Goymann et al., 2007; Oliveira, 2004; Wingfield et al., 1990), thereby providing a timed cue for the consolidation of behavioral output and the underlying brain circuitry. Such a cue could facilitate the timely formation and recall of long-term memories of relevant sensory and motor information. Our study suggests that lasting, hormone-driven synaptic pruning of related brain circuitries could form a basis for such long-term memories, enabling a quick recovery of previously acquired skills. The ability to respond quickly to environmental and social cues through such primed hormone-sensitive control mechanisms would constitute an important adaptation to ensure competitive proficiency and maximize reproductive success under environmental restrictions. The results obtained from this study may have implications not only across different animal groups, but may generally apply to different motor, sensory, and motivational systems that rely on a rapid recall of previously learned information.

Materials and methods

Subjects

For this study adult domesticated canaries (Serinus canaria) were either purchased from a local breeder in Antwerp or taken from the breeding colony of the Max Planck Institute for Ornithology, Seewiesen. All animals were raised and kept under local natural daylight conditions, until the day length reached 11 hr in early March. Birds were subsequently housed individually in sound attenuating chambers for song recordings. Experimental procedures were conducted according to the guidelines of the Federation of European Animal Science Associations (FELASA) and approved by the Ethical Committee on animal experiments of the University of Antwerp.

Hormone treatment

To stimulate song production in naive female canaries, birds were subcutaneously implanted (T1+) with 8 mm silastic tubes (Dow Corning, Midland, MI; ID: 1.47 mm) filled with crystalline testosterone (Sigma-Aldrich Co., St. Louis, MO). During the hormone treatments a light cycle of 11 hr light, and 13 hr darkness was maintained to exclude photoperiodic effects on song production. This light cycle was chosen because under natural conditions it corresponds to a phase of testosterone up-regulation and physiological sensitivity to hormone fluctuations in canaries (Nottebohm et al., 1987). After consolidation of song we removed the hormone implants (T1-) to end song production, and changed the daylight conditions to 8 hr light and 16 hr dark, inducing a molting phase of approximately 1.5 months. Once the molt was finished, the day length was gradually returned to 11 hr over the following month. Subsequently, the song-experienced birds received a second testosterone treatment (T2+), and were sacrificed 5 weeks after implantation.

Hormone analysis

Blood samples were collected from the birds’ right wing veins using heparinized haematocrit capillaries (Brand, Wertheim, Germany). Directly after blood collection, samples were centrifuged at 3000 RPM for 10 min to separate cells from plasma, and stored at −80°C for later analysis.

Testosterone concentrations in the blood plasma were determined by radioimmunoassay (RIA) after extraction and partial purification on diatomaceous earth (glycol) columns, following the procedures described previously (Goymann et al., 2001; Goymann et al., 2008).

Briefly, plasma samples were extracted with dichloromethane (DCM) after overnight equilibration of the plasma with 1500 dpm of 3H-labeled testosterone (Perkin-Elmer, Rodgau, Germany). After separation of the organic phase, the extracts were re-suspended in 2 × 250 µl 2% ethylacetate in isooctane and fractioned using columns of diatomaceous earth:propylene glycol:ethylene glycol (6:1.5:1.5, w:v:v). Collected testosterone fractions were dried, re-suspended in phosphate-buffered saline with 1% gelatin (PBSG), and left to equilibrate overnight at 4°C. Extraction of plasma testosterone with dichloromethane resulted in 86 ± 11% (mean ± sd) recovery. Hormone samples were incubated with antisera against testosterone (Esoterix Endocrinology, Calabasas Hills, CA), and after 30 min 3H-labeled steroids (13500 dpm) were added and left to incubate for 20 hr at 4°C. Free steroids were separated from the bound fractions by absorption on dextran-coated charcoal, centrifuged, and decanted into scintillation vials to be counted.

Testosterone concentrations in the plasma samples were measured in one assay. The lower detection limit of the RIA was 0.33 pg/tube, and all measured plasma levels were above the lower detection limit. The intra-assay variation of a chicken plasma pool as control sample at the beginning and end of the assay was 6.7%. Pooled plasma levels of testosterone for all birds during the different consecutive hormone treatments are shown in Table 1. Mean detected hormone levels were within range of physiological plasma levels in nest-building male canaries (range: 360–7970 pg/ml; n = 6) and previously reported values for other small passerines (Wingfield and Hahn, 1994; Kempenaers et al., 2008; Apfelbeck and Goymann, 2011).

Table 1. Plasma levels of testosterone (T) during different hormone treatments.

Treatment T levels (pg/ml) Significance*
control 238 ± 65
T1+ 6486 ± 488 p<0.001
T1- 138 ± 30 NS
T2+ 7279 ± 1249 p<0.01

* P-values of comparisons between different hormone treatments against control values (ANOVA with Dunnett’s t correction for multiple comparisons against one control).

Song recording

To monitor the entire song ontogeny during consecutive testosterone treatments randomly selected, naive female canaries (n = 6) were kept individually in sound attenuating chambers during testosterone treatment (T1+), testosterone removal (T1-) and testosterone re-treatment (T2+), while song output was continuously recorded. This approach resulted in a song database of 5.6 ± 0.6 million song syllables per bird, recorded over the course of approximately 1 year.

The vocal activity of each bird was recorded using Sound Analysis Pro 2.0 (Tchernichovski et al., 2000). Omnidirectional condenser microphones (TC-20; Earthworks, Milford, NH) connected to a multi-channel microphone preamplifier (UA-1000; Roland, Los Angeles, CA) were used to acquire and digitize all sounds produced within the sound attenuating boxes with a sampling frequency of 44.1 kHz. The incoming signal was filtered online using an amplitude and Wiener entropy threshold to exclude background noises, and saved in 60 s waveform audio files (16-bit PCM format).

Song analysis

Vocalizations were segmented into individual syllables with the fully automated Feature Batch module in Sound Analysis Pro by applying an amplitude threshold to the sound wave. Because the distance from perch to microphone ranged between 10 and 20 cm only, little variation in recorded amplitude levels can be expected. The amplitude threshold was selected once manually to assure reliable segmentation, and was kept constant during the analyses of all sound phrases within birds.

To filter out non-song vocalizations, syllables were only included when produced within a song bout of at least 750 ms. A song bout was defined as a sequence of sounds traversing the amplitude threshold with an interval of no more than 100 ms. We used Sound Analysis Pro to measure duration (d), pause duration (i), syllable rate (SR), frequency modulation (FM), amplitude modulation (AM), syllable bandwidth (BW), mean frequency (MF) and Wiener entropy (E) for each syllable and stored these syllable features in MySQL 5.1 tables (Oracle, Redwood Shores, CA). Bandwidth was calculated for each syllable by subtracting the minimum peak frequency from the maximum peak frequency. The syllable rate was defined for each syllable as: SRa = (da +ia)−1, where da is the duration of syllable ‘a’, and ia is the interval between the end of syllable ‘a’ and the beginning of the consecutive syllable within the same song bout, irrespective of syllable type. For a detailed computational description of how Sound Analysis Pro calculates the different sound features we refer to the accompanying publication (Tchernichovski et al., 2000) and the online manual (http://soundanalysispro.com).

To investigate the dynamic change of song features over the course of the experiment, daily histograms were obtained from the entire dataset by rounding individual values to the nearest integer, and plotting the number of times those integers occurred each day. To determine the SRs that were most commonly used at the onset and crystallization of song development a non-linear exponential curve (1) was fitted through the peak values of each SR histogram.

Fx=a+b1+ec-xd (1)

Further developmental correlation plots for all temporal and spectral song features were produced by calculating the Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient (CC) between a 7 day average of the histogram pattern at the time of song stabilization and all other recorded days. This calculation resulted in a value between −1 and 1 for each recorded day, and was used as a measure of similarity where one describes an absolute copy of the stable song pattern, and −1 describes the absolute inverse of the stable song pattern. A non-linear exponential curve (1) was fitted through the CC values and song parameters were considered stable as soon as the daily increase in similarity dropped below 0.001. For each song feature we used the fit curve to determine the duration between testosterone implantation and feature stabilization, and to determine the maximum daily increase in similarity (dmax). Dmax could only be calculated for birds and song features that demonstrated a developmental increase in similarity, and was only used for statistical purposes if three or more birds demonstrated such an increase. Mean CC plots combining all analyzed birds were calculated by averaging the raw (non-fitted) CC values that were calculated for each bird and for each day. We aligned song development in the different birds based on the first day where we detected song.

Spectrograms of the original sound files were visually inspected on syllable-like sound structures to determine the syllable repertoire of each bird and the first occurrence of song after testosterone treatments. To assess syllable similarity during consecutive testosterone treatments we calculated the Euclidean distance between each of 100 randomly selected syllables for each syllable type from stable songs during both the T1+ and T2+ periods. The Euclidean distance between two syllables ‘a’ and ‘b’ with coordinates c = SR, d, i, FM, AM, BW, MF, E was defined as:

da,b=c=1n(b1-a1)2

To give all analyzed temporal and spectral features equal weight in the analysis, each feature was normalized by dividing its value by its global median value from all syllables produced by all birds recorded in this study.

First, for each syllable type the mean Euclidean distance between 100 syllables of the same type during T1+ (T1xT1) was calculated to determine how much baseline variation existed between syllables of the same type. Lower values indicate a higher similarity between syllables. Secondly, we calculated the mean Euclidean distance between 100 syllables of the same type from the T1+ and T2+ (T1xT2) periods to determine if syllables produced during the 2nd testosterone treatment were similar to those produced during the 1st testosterone treatment. Finally, we calculated the mean Euclidean distance between 100 syllables of the same type from the T1 +period with each of 100 syllables from all other types of syllables that a bird produced (T1xT2ext) to provide a measure of dissimilarity between syllables of different types. All observed syllable types were included in this analysis with the exception of single transitional syllables connecting two song phrases.

To assess similarity of song syntax between subsequent testosterone treatments we determined the probability of song phrase transitions during the T1+ and T2+ periods for each bird. On average 794.3 phrase transitions per bird were used for the analysis. From the resulting transition probability distributions we calculated the sequence linearity, consistency and entropy as previously described (Scharff and Nottebohm, 1991; Daou et al., 2012). In short, sequence linearity is expressed as the number of syllable types divided by the number of transition types and addresses the way phrases are ordered in a song. Sequence consistency is expressed as the sum of common phrase transitions divided by the sum of total transitions and addresses how often the same sequences are produced. Common phrase transitions were defined as transitions that make up at least 5% of all phrase transitions. The entropy (S) of the transition probability distribution with transition type Ti is a measure of the spread of that distribution and was calculated as:

S=-i=1nTilog2(Ti)

Specimen preparation

To estimate spine densities and dendrite densities in motor nucleus HVC and RA, brains were collected from a control group of adult female canaries that did not receive any testosterone treatment (C: n = 6), a group of birds that was sacrificed five months after testosterone treatment (T1+: n = 8), and a group of birds that was sacrificed at least 2.5 months after testosterone withdrawal (T1-: n = 6). All experimental birds were raised and kept in group aviaries together with other male and female canaries throughout the experiment to assure similar social and auditory conditions. Birds were transferred to sound attenuating chambers for two weeks after testosterone treatment to assure that birds were singing, and prior to sacrifice. Individuals were randomly allocated to the different treatment groups. All birds were maintained on a light cycle of 11 hr light, and 13 hr darkness and were between 2.5 and 3 years old at the time of sacrifice. Brains were processed for Golgi-Cox staining using the FD Rapid GolgiStain kit (FD NeuroTechnologies, Columbia, MD).

After overnight fixation in a 4% formaldehyde solution in phosphate-buffered saline (PBS; 10 mM; pH 7.4), brains were stored in PBS with 0.05% sodium azide at 4°C until further processing.

Brain hemispheres were separated with a razor blade and left hemispheres were immersed for two weeks in an impregnation solution consisting of equal amounts of FD solutions A and B in total darkness at room temperature. Following three days of incubation in FD solution C, brains were cut into 30 µm sagittal sections using a sliding microtome (Leica Microsystems GmbH, Wetzlar, Germany), which were stored in PBS with 0.05% sodium azide at 4°C.

Sections with a regular interval of 180 µm were mounted on SuperFrost glass slides (Menzel GmbH, Brauschweig, Germany), developed in a solution of two parts distilled water, one part FD solutions D, and one part FD solution E for ten minutes, and rinsed in distilled water before embedding in CC/Mount tissue mounting medium (Sigma-Aldrich). Slides were incubated at 70°C until the mounting medium had hardened, cleared in xylene, and further embedded in Roti-Histokitt II (Carl Roth, Karlsruhe, Germany) before coverslipping.

Spine and Dendrite quantification

For each bird, z-stacks were obtained from three brain sections of nucleus HVC and two sections of nucleus RA with a Nikon Eclipse Ti microscope (Nikon, Tokyo, Japan), equipped with a 60x oil immersion lens (CFI Plan Apo VC 60x Oil). The outline of HVC and RA in each section was delineated in ImageJ (http://rsb.info.nih.gov/ij/), and 100 µm2 non-overlapping regions of interests (ROIs) were randomly placed within the boundaries of the target nucleus prior to the quantifications. Considering the relatively uniform distribution of X-projecting HVC neurons and RA-projecting HVC neurons (Fortune and Margoliash, 1995), this approach should result in a homogenous sampling across treatment groups.

Neuronal dendritic spine densities were estimated for each brain section by checking ROIs in random order until five ROIs were located that contained spinous dendrite segments (15 ROIs per animal for HVC and 10 ROIs for RA). The individual dendrite segments within the ROIs were traced in ImageJ to measure the length, and the number of visible spines that originated from the traced segment was manually counted. Spine densities were calculated as the number of visible spines per µm of dendrite. All visible spine types were included in the quantifications, including filopodia, long, thin, stubby, mushroom and branched spines (Risher et al., 2014). For branched spines, each spine head was counted separately.

To further obtain a density measure of spinous dendrites we counted the total number of spinous dendrite segments in all ROIs examined, and divided the number of observed dendrite segments with the number of ROIs, giving an estimation of the number of spinous dendrites per ROI. Neuronal dendrites were further binned into categories containing 0–0.2, 0.2–0.4, 0.4–0.6, 0.6–0.8, 0.8–1.0, 1.0–1.2, 1.2–1.4, 1.4–1.6, 1.6–1.8 spines per µm. The probability distribution of neuronal dendrite segments was then obtained by dividing the observed number of spinous dendrites per ROI in each bin with the total number of spinous dendrites per ROI.

To obtain a density measure of aspinous neurites in HVC, the number of visible spine-less neurite segments were counted in ten new non-overlapping ROIs that were randomly placed within the boundaries of HVC. We refer to these projections as neurites, because we were unable to distinguish between aspinous dendrites and axons in our tissues.

All quantifications were conducted by an experimenter that was blind to the experimental condition of the animals.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed in SAS 9.3 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC). No sample sizes were calculated prior to the experiments. A technical replication was defined as a repeated measurement of the same individual, and biological replications constitute the number of sampled individuals (n). Differences in spine and dendrite densities between treatment groups were tested with a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Dunnett’s correction for multiple comparisons against one control value (untreated birds). Changes in hormone levels were determined using a repeated one-way ANOVA with Dunnett’s correction. Differences in syllable similarity and syllable rates of individual syllable types were tested with one-way ANOVAs, and when significant, Tukey's HSD test was used to analyze differences between time points. Song pattern similarities between the 1st and 2nd hormone treatment were established by comparing the average CC from seven consecutive days of stable song during the 1st treatment with the average CC from seven consecutive days of stable song production during the 2nd treatment. Paired-samples t-tests were used to compare CC’s, sequence parameters, and developmental time parameters between subsequent hormone treatments. To determine song feature deterioration we calculated the difference between the average CC from the last 7 days of stable song production during the 1st testosterone treatment and the average CC from the first 2 days of song production during the 2nd treatment. Feature recovery was determined by subtracting the average CC from the first 2 days of song production from the average CC from the last 7 days of stable song production during the 2nd testosterone treatment. One-sample t-tests were used to determine if feature deterioration and recovery significantly exceeded zero. Measurement values in the text are given as means ± SEM, unless stated otherwise. No outliers or data were excluded from analysis.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Ingrid Schwabl, Monika Trappschuh and Wolfgang Goymann for performing and analyzing the hormone radioimmunoassays. We also thank Sébastien Derégnaucourt for providing male canary songs and Susan Urbanus and Lasse Jacobsen for constructive comments on the manuscript.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Michiel Vellema, Email: vellema@gmail.com.

Richard B Ivry, University of California, Berkeley, United States.

Richard B Ivry, University of California, Berkeley, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Horizon 2020 Framework Programme 701660 to Michiel Vellema.

  • National Research, Development and Innovation Office Hungary K-115970 to Sándor Zsebők.

  • Interuniversity Attraction Poles IUAP-NIMI-P6/38 to Annemie Van der Linden.

  • Max-Planck-Gesellschaft Open-access funding to Manfred Gahr.

  • National Research, Development and Innovation Office Hungary K-129215 to Sándor Zsebők.

  • National Research, Development and Innovation Office Hungary PD-115730 to Sándor Zsebők.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing—review and editing.

Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing—review and editing.

Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing—review and editing.

Resources, Data curation, Supervision, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: Experimental procedures were conducted according to the guidelines of the Federation of European Animal Science Associations (FELASA) and approved by the Ethical Committee on animal experiments of the University of Antwerp (protocol number: 2007-14).

Additional files

Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.43194.019

Data availability

SQL data of extracted song features for each individual have been deposited in Dryad. Further source data files have been provided for Figure 2, Figure 2—figure supplement 1, Figure 3, Figure 3—figure supplement 1, Figure 4, Figure 4—figure supplement 1, Figure 4—figure supplement 2, Figure 5.

The following dataset was generated:

Vellema M, Gahr M. 2018. Data from: Vocal motor experiences consolidate the vocal motor circuitry and accelerate future vocal skill development. Dryad Digital Repository.

References

  1. Albouy G, Sterpenich V, Balteau E, Vandewalle G, Desseilles M, Dang-Vu T, Darsaud A, Ruby P, Luppi PH, Degueldre C, Peigneux P, Luxen A, Maquet P. Both the hippocampus and striatum are involved in consolidation of motor sequence memory. Neuron. 2008;58:261–272. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alonso RG, Trevisan MA, Amador A, Goller F, Mindlin GB. A circular model for song motor control in Serinus canaria. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience. 2015;9:41. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2015.00041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Alvarez-Buylla A, Theelen M, Nottebohm F. Birth of projection neurons in the higher vocal center of the canary forebrain before, during, and after song learning. PNAS. 1988;85:8722–8726. doi: 10.1073/pnas.85.22.8722. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Alvarez-Buylla A, Kirn JR, Nottebohm F. Birth of projection neurons in adult avian brain may be related to perceptual or motor learning. Science. 1990;249:1444–1446. doi: 10.1126/science.1698312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Amador A, Perl YS, Mindlin GB, Margoliash D. Elemental gesture dynamics are encoded by song premotor cortical neurons. Nature. 2013;495:59–64. doi: 10.1038/nature11967. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Andalman AS, Fee MS. A basal ganglia-forebrain circuit in the songbird biases motor output to avoid vocal errors. PNAS. 2009;106:12518–12523. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0903214106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Apfelbeck B, Goymann W. Ignoring the challenge? Male black redstarts (Phoenicurus ochruros) do not increase testosterone levels during territorial conflicts but they do so in response to gonadotropin-releasing hormone. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2011;278:3233–3242. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.0098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Arnold AP, Nottebohm F, Pfaff DW. Hormone concentrating cells in vocal control and other areas of the brain of the zebra finch ( Poephila guttata) The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1976;165:487–511. doi: 10.1002/cne.901650406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Aronov D, Andalman AS, Fee MS. A specialized forebrain circuit for vocal babbling in the juvenile songbird. Science. 2008;320:630–634. doi: 10.1126/science.1155140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Balthazart J, Foidart A, Wilson EM, Ball GF. Immunocytochemical localization of androgen receptors in the male songbird and quail brain. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1992;317:407–420. doi: 10.1002/cne.903170407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Balthazart J, Boseret G, Konkle AT, Hurley LL, Ball GF. Doublecortin as a marker of adult neuroplasticity in the canary song control nucleus HVC. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;27:801–817. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06059.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Barnes TD, Kubota Y, Hu D, Jin DZ, Graybiel AM. Activity of striatal neurons reflects dynamic encoding and recoding of procedural memories. Nature. 2005;437:1158–1161. doi: 10.1038/nature04053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Beckers GJ, Suthers RA, ten Cate C. Pure-tone birdsong by resonance filtering of harmonic overtones. PNAS. 2003;100:7372–7376. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1232227100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Belzner S, Voigt C, Catchpole CK, Leitner S. Song learning in domesticated canaries in a restricted acoustic environment. Proceedings. Biological sciences. 2009;276:2881–2886. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.0669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Benton S, Cardin JA, DeVoogd TJ. Lucifer yellow filling of area X-projecting neurons in the high vocal center of female canaries. Brain Research. 1998;799:138–147. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(98)00485-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Boinski S, Fragaszy DM. The ontogeny of foraging in squirrel monkeys, saimiri oerstedi. Animal Behaviour. 1989;37:415–428. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(89)90089-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bottjer SW, Miesner EA, Arnold AP. Forebrain lesions disrupt development but not maintenance of song in passerine birds. Science. 1984;224:901–903. doi: 10.1126/science.6719123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Brainard MS, Doupe AJ. Interruption of a basal ganglia-forebrain circuit prevents plasticity of learned vocalizations. Nature. 2000;404:762–766. doi: 10.1038/35008083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Brainard MS, Doupe AJ. Translating birdsong: songbirds as a model for basic and applied medical research. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2013;36:489–517. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-152826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Brenowitz EA, Lent K. Act locally and think globally: intracerebral testosterone implants induce seasonal-like growth of adult avian song control circuits. PNAS. 2002;99:12421–12426. doi: 10.1073/pnas.192308799. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Byers J, Hebets E, Podos J. Female mate choice based upon male motor performance. Animal Behaviour. 2010;79:771–778. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.01.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Canady RA, Burd GD, DeVoogd TJ, Nottebohm F. Effect of testosterone on input received by an identified neuron type of the canary song system: a Golgi/electron microscopy/degeneration study. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1988;8:3770–3784. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-10-03770.1988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Changeux JP, Danchin A. Selective stabilisation of developing synapses as a mechanism for the specification of neuronal networks. Nature. 1976;264:705–712. doi: 10.1038/264705a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Charlesworth JD, Warren TL, Brainard MS. Covert skill learning in a cortical-basal ganglia circuit. Nature. 2012;486:251–255. doi: 10.1038/nature11078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Chi Z, Margoliash D. Temporal precision and temporal drift in brain and behavior of zebra finch song. Neuron. 2001;32:899–910. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00524-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Clarin TM, Ruczyński I, Page RA, Siemers BM. Foraging ecology predicts learning performance in insectivorous bats. PLOS ONE. 2013;8:e64823. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Coynel D, Marrelec G, Perlbarg V, Pélégrini-Issac M, Van de Moortele PF, Ugurbil K, Doyon J, Benali H, Lehéricy S. Dynamics of motor-related functional integration during motor sequence learning. NeuroImage. 2010;49:759–766. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Crewther BT, Shetty K, Jarchi D, Selvadurai S, Cook CJ, Leff DR, Darzi A, Yang GZ. Skill acquisition and stress adaptations following laparoscopic surgery training and detraining in novice surgeons. Surgical Endoscopy. 2016;30:2961–2968. doi: 10.1007/s00464-015-4584-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Daou A, Johnson F, Wu W, Bertram R. A computational tool for automated large-scale analysis and measurement of bird-song syntax. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2012;210:147–160. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2012.07.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Debas K, Carrier J, Orban P, Barakat M, Lungu O, Vandewalle G, Hadj Tahar A, Bellec P, Karni A, Ungerleider LG, Benali H, Doyon J. Brain plasticity related to the consolidation of motor sequence learning and motor adaptation. PNAS. 2010;107:17839–17844. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1013176107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Devoogd TJ, Nixdorf B, Nottebohm F. Synaptogenesis and changes in synaptic morphology related to acquisition of a new behavior. Brain Research. 1985;329:304–308. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(85)90539-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. DeVoogd T, Nottebohm F. Gonadal hormones induce dendritic growth in the adult avian brain. Science. 1981;214:202–204. doi: 10.1126/science.7280692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Dittrich F, Feng Y, Metzdorf R, Gahr M. Estrogen-inducible, sex-specific expression of brain-derived neurotrophic factor mRNA in a forebrain song control nucleus of the juvenile zebra finch. PNAS. 1999;96:8241–8246. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.14.8241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Doupe AJ, Kuhl PK. Birdsong and human speech: common themes and mechanisms. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1999;22:567–631. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.22.1.567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Drăgănoiu TI, Nagle L, Kreutzer M. Directional female preference for an exaggerated male trait in canary (Serinus canaria) song. Proceedings. Biological sciences. 2002;269:2525–2531. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Eadie EC. Ontogeny of foraging competence in capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus) for easy versus difficult to acquire fruits: a test of the needing to learn hypothesis. PLOS ONE. 2015;10:e0138001. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0138001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Ebbinghaus H. Memory: a contribution to experimental psychology. Annals of Neurosciences. 2013;20:155–156. doi: 10.5214/ans.0972.7531.200408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Elemans CP, Mead AF, Rome LC, Goller F. Superfast vocal muscles control song production in songbirds. PLOS ONE. 2008;3:e2581. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Fortune ES, Margoliash D. Parallel pathways and convergence onto HVc and adjacent neostriatum of adult zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1995;360:413–441. doi: 10.1002/cne.903600305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Frankl-Vilches C, Kuhl H, Werber M, Klages S, Kerick M, Bakker A, de Oliveira EH, Reusch C, Capuano F, Vowinckel J, Leitner S, Ralser M, Timmermann B, Gahr M. Using the canary genome to decipher the evolution of hormone-sensitive gene regulation in seasonal singing birds. Genome Biology. 2015;16:19. doi: 10.1186/s13059-014-0578-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Fusani L, Metzdorf R, Hutchison JB, Gahr M. Aromatase inhibition affects testosterone-induced masculinization of song and the neural song system in female canaries. Journal of Neurobiology. 2003;54:370–379. doi: 10.1002/neu.10141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gahr M. Delineation of a brain nucleus: comparisons of cytochemical, Hodological, and cytoarchitectural views of the song control nucleus HVc of the adult canary. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1990;294:30–36. doi: 10.1002/cne.902940104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Gahr M, Garcia-Segura LM. Testosterone-dependent increase of gap-junctions in HVC neurons of adult female canaries. Brain Research. 1996;712:69–73. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)01448-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Gahr M, Metzdorf R. Distribution and dynamics in the expression of androgen and estrogen receptors in vocal control systems of songbirds. Brain Research Bulletin. 1997;44:509–517. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(97)00233-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Gardner TJ, Naef F, Nottebohm F. Freedom and rules: the acquisition and reprogramming of a bird's learned song. Science. 2005;308:1046–1049. doi: 10.1126/science.1108214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Geberzahn N, Aubin T. Assessing vocal performance in complex birdsong: a novel approach. BMC Biology. 2014;12:58. doi: 10.1186/s12915-014-0058-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Goller F, Suthers RA. Role of syringeal muscles in controlling the phonology of bird song. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1996;76:287–300. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.1.287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Goymann W, East ML, Hofer H. Androgens and the role of female "hyperaggressiveness" in spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta) Hormones and Behavior. 2001;39:83–92. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2000.1634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Goymann W, Landys MM, Wingfield JC. Distinguishing seasonal androgen responses from male-male androgen responsiveness-revisiting the Challenge Hypothesis. Hormones and Behavior. 2007;51:463–476. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Goymann W, Wittenzellner A, Schwabl I, Makomba M. Progesterone modulates aggression in sex-role reversed female African black coucals. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2008;275:1053–1060. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2007.1707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Grutzendler J, Kasthuri N, Gan WB. Long-term dendritic spine stability in the adult cortex. Nature. 2002;420:812–816. doi: 10.1038/nature01276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Hahnloser RH, Kozhevnikov AA, Fee MS. An ultra-sparse code underlies the generation of neural sequences in a songbird. Nature. 2002;419:65–70. doi: 10.1038/nature00974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Hartog TE, Dittrich F, Pieneman AW, Jansen RF, Frankl-Vilches C, Lessmann V, Lilliehook C, Goldman SA, Gahr M. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor signaling in the HVC is required for testosterone-induced song of female canaries. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:15511–15519. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2564-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Hayase S, Wang H, Ohgushi E, Kobayashi M, Mori C, Horita H, Mineta K, Liu WC, Wada K. Vocal practice regulates singing activity-dependent genes underlying age-independent vocal learning in songbirds. PLOS Biology. 2018;16:e2006537. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2006537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Hofer SB, Mrsic-Flogel TD, Bonhoeffer T, Hübener M. Prior experience enhances plasticity in adult visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2006;9:127–132. doi: 10.1038/nn1610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Hofer SB, Mrsic-Flogel TD, Bonhoeffer T, Hübener M. Experience leaves a lasting structural trace in cortical circuits. Nature. 2009;457:313–317. doi: 10.1038/nature07487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Holveck M-J, Riebel K. Preferred songs predict preferred males: consistency and repeatability of zebra finch females across three test contexts. Animal Behaviour. 2007;74:297–309. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.08.016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  58. Hoshiba Y, Wada T, Hayashi-Takagi A. Synaptic ensemble underlying the selection and consolidation of neuronal circuits during learning. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2017;11:12. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2017.00012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Huang Z, Khaled HG, Kirschmann M, Gobes SM, Hahnloser RH. Excitatory and inhibitory synapse reorganization immediately after critical sensory experience in a vocal learner. eLife. 2018;7:e37571. doi: 10.7554/eLife.37571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Huttenlocher PR. Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development. Neuropsychologia. 1990;28:517–527. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(90)90031-I. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Kafitz KW, Güttinger HR, Müller CM. Seasonal changes in astrocytes parallel neuronal plasticity in the song control area HVc of the canary. Glia. 1999;27:88–100. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1136(199907)27:1&#x0003c;88::AID-GLIA9&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Kao MH, Doupe AJ, Brainard MS. Contributions of an avian basal ganglia-forebrain circuit to real-time modulation of song. Nature. 2005;433:638–643. doi: 10.1038/nature03127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Kempenaers B, Peters A, Foerster K. Sources of individual variation in plasma testosterone levels. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2008;363:1711–1723. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.0001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Kiefer S, Scharff C, Hultsch H, Kipper S, now Lit. Learn it now, sing it later? Field and laboratory studies on song repertoire acquisition and song use in nightingales. Die Naturwissenschaften. 2014;101:955–963. doi: 10.1007/s00114-014-1236-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Kirn J, O'Loughlin B, Kasparian S, Nottebohm F. Cell death and neuronal recruitment in the high vocal center of adult male canaries are temporally related to changes in song. PNAS. 1994;91:7844–7848. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.17.7844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Koleske AJ. Molecular mechanisms of dendrite stability. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2013;14:536–550. doi: 10.1038/nrn3486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Kornfeld J, Benezra SE, Narayanan RT, Svara F, Egger R, Oberlaender M, Denk W, Long MA. EM connectomics reveals axonal target variation in a sequence-generating network. eLife. 2017;6:e24364. doi: 10.7554/eLife.24364. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Kosche G, Vallentin D, Long MA. Interplay of inhibition and excitation shapes a premotor neural sequence. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:1217–1227. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4346-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Kubikova L, Bosikova E, Cvikova M, Lukacova K, Scharff C, Jarvis ED. Basal ganglia function, stuttering, sequencing, and repair in adult songbirds. Scientific Reports. 2014;4:6590. doi: 10.1038/srep06590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Kubota M, Taniguchi I. Electrophysiological characteristics of classes of neuron in the HVc of the zebra finch. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1998;80:914–923. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.2.914. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Lehéricy S, Bardinet E, Tremblay L, Van de Moortele PF, Pochon JB, Dormont D, Kim DS, Yelnik J, Ugurbil K. Motor control in basal ganglia circuits using fMRI and brain atlas approaches. Cerebral Cortex. 2006;16:149–161. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Leitner S, Voigt C, Gahr M. Seasonal changes in the song pattern of the Non-Domesticated island canary (Serinus canaria), a field study. Behaviour. 2001;138:885–904. [Google Scholar]
  73. Leonard SL. Induction of singing in female canaries by injections of male hormone. Experimental Biology and Medicine. 1939;41:229–230. doi: 10.3181/00379727-41-10631. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  74. Long MA, Fee MS. Using temperature to analyse temporal dynamics in the songbird motor pathway. Nature. 2008;456:189–194. doi: 10.1038/nature07448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Louissaint A, Rao S, Leventhal C, Goldman SA. Coordinated interaction of neurogenesis and angiogenesis in the adult songbird brain. Neuron. 2002;34:945–960. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00722-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Markowitz JE, Ivie E, Kligler L, Gardner TJ. Long-range order in canary song. PLOS Computational Biology. 2013;9:e1003052. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Marler P. Three models of song learning: evidence from behavior. Journal of Neurobiology. 1997;33:501–516. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4695(19971105)33:5&#x0003c;501::AID-NEU2&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Marler P, Peters S. Sparrows learn adult song and more from memory. Science. 1981;213:780–782. doi: 10.1126/science.213.4509.780. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Marler P, Peters S. Structural-Changes in song ontogeny in the swamp sparrow Melospiza-Georgiana. The Auk. 1982a;99:446–458. [Google Scholar]
  80. Marler P, Peters S. Long-term storage of learned birdsongs prior to production. Animal Behaviour. 1982b;30:479–482. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(82)80059-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  81. Mehaffey WH, Doupe AJ. Naturalistic stimulation drives opposing heterosynaptic plasticity at two inputs to songbird cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2015;18:1272–1280. doi: 10.1038/nn.4078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Metzdorf R, Gahr M, Fusani L, aromatase Dof. Distribution of aromatase, estrogen receptor, and androgen receptor mRNA in the forebrain of songbirds and nonsongbirds. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1999;407:115–129. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19990428)407:1&#x0003c;115::AID-CNE9&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-W. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Mooney R. Different subthreshold mechanisms underlie song selectivity in identified HVc neurons of the zebra finch. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20:5420–5436. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-14-05420.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Mooney R, Prather JF. The HVC microcircuit: the synaptic basis for interactions between song motor and vocal plasticity pathways. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:1952–1964. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3726-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Mori C, Liu WC, Wada K. Recurrent development of song idiosyncrasy without auditory inputs in the Canary, an open-ended vocal learner. Scientific Reports. 2018;8:8732. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-27046-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Mowles SL, Jennions M, Backwell PR. Multimodal communication in courting fiddler crabs reveals male performance capacities. Royal Society Open Science. 2017;4:161093. doi: 10.1098/rsos.161093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Murugan M, Harward S, Scharff C, Mooney R. Diminished FoxP2 levels affect dopaminergic modulation of corticostriatal signaling important to song variability. Neuron. 2013;80:1464–1476. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Nastiuk KL, Clayton DF. The canary androgen receptor mRNA is localized in the song control nuclei of the brain and is rapidly regulated by testosterone. Journal of Neurobiology. 1995;26:213–224. doi: 10.1002/neu.480260206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Nixdorf BE, Davis SS, DeVoogd TJ. Morphology of Golgi-impregnated neurons in Hyperstriatum Ventralis, pars caudalis in adult male and female canaries. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1989;284:337–349. doi: 10.1002/cne.902840302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Nixdorf-Bergweiler BE, Wallhäusser-Franke E, DeVoogd TJ. Regressive development in neuronal structure during song learning in birds. Journal of Neurobiology. 1995;27:204–215. doi: 10.1002/neu.480270207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Nottebohm F, Stokes TM, Leonard CM. Central control of song in the canary, serinus canarius. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1976;165:457–486. doi: 10.1002/cne.901650405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Nottebohm F. Testosterone triggers growth of brain vocal control nuclei in adult female canaries. Brain Research. 1980;189:429–436. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(80)90102-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Nottebohm F. A brain for all seasons: cyclical anatomical changes in song control nuclei of the canary brain. Science. 1981;214:1368–1370. doi: 10.1126/science.7313697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Nottebohm F, Nottebohm ME, Crane L. Developmental and seasonal changes in canary song and their relation to changes in the anatomy of song-control nuclei. Behavioral and Neural Biology. 1986;46:445–471. doi: 10.1016/S0163-1047(86)90485-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Nottebohm F, Nottebohm ME, Crane LA, Wingfield JC. Seasonal changes in gonadal hormone levels of adult male canaries and their relation to song. Behavioral and Neural Biology. 1987;47:197–211. doi: 10.1016/S0163-1047(87)90327-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Nowicki S. Vocal tract resonances in oscine bird sound production: evidence from birdsongs in a helium atmosphere. Nature. 1987;325:53–55. doi: 10.1038/325053a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Oliveira RF. Social modulation of androgens in vertebrates: mechanisms and function. Advances in the Study of Behavior. 2004;3434:165–239. [Google Scholar]
  98. Olveczky BP, Andalman AS, Fee MS. Vocal experimentation in the juvenile songbird requires a basal ganglia circuit. PLOS Biology. 2005;3:e153. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Ölveczky BP, Otchy TM, Goldberg JH, Aronov D, Fee MS. Changes in the neural control of a complex motor sequence during learning. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011;106:386–397. doi: 10.1152/jn.00018.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Ota N, Gahr M, Soma M. Tap dancing birds: the multimodal mutual courtship display of males and females in a socially monogamous songbird. Scientific Reports. 2015;5:16614. doi: 10.1038/srep16614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Pasupathy A, Miller EK. Different time courses of learning-related activity in the prefrontal cortex and striatum. Nature. 2005;433:873–876. doi: 10.1038/nature03287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Patterson EM, Krzyszczyk E, Mann J. Age-specific foraging performance and reproduction in tool-using wild bottlenose dolphins. Behavioral Ecology. 2016;27:401–410. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arv164. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  103. Pesch A, Guttinger HR. Song of female canary. Journal of Ornithology. 1985;126:108–110. [Google Scholar]
  104. Petanjek Z, Judaš M, Šimic G, Rasin MR, Uylings HB, Rakic P, Kostovic I. Extraordinary neoteny of synaptic spines in the human prefrontal cortex. PNAS. 2011;108:13281–13286. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1105108108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Podos J. Motor constraints on vocal development in a songbird. Animal Behaviour. 1996;51:1061–1070. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1996.0107. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  106. Podos J. A performance constraint on the evolution of trilled vocalizations in a songbird family (PASSERIFORMES: emberizidae) Evolution. 1997;51:537–551. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1997.tb02441.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Poldrack RA, Sabb FW, Foerde K, Tom SM, Asarnow RF, Bookheimer SY, Knowlton BJ. The neural correlates of motor skill automaticity. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:5356–5364. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3880-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Prather JF, Peters S, Nowicki S, Mooney R. Persistent representation of juvenile experience in the adult songbird brain. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:10586–10598. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6042-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Prendergast BJ, Nelson RJ, Zucker I. Mammalian seasonal rhythms: behavior and neuroendocrine substrates. In: Pfaff D. W, Arnold A. P, Fahrbach S. E, Etgen A. M, Rubin R. T, editors. Hormones, Brain and Behavior. San Diego, CA: Elsevier Science; 2002. pp. 93–156. [Google Scholar]
  110. Rakic P, Bourgeois JP, Eckenhoff MF, Zecevic N, Goldman-Rakic PS. Concurrent overproduction of synapses in diverse regions of the primate cerebral cortex. Science. 1986;232:232–235. doi: 10.1126/science.3952506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Rasika S, Nottebohm F, Alvarez-Buylla A. Testosterone increases the recruitment and/or survival of new high vocal center neurons in adult female canaries. PNAS. 1994;91:7854–7858. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.17.7854. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Rasika S, Alvarez-Buylla A, Nottebohm F. BDNF mediates the effects of testosterone on the survival of new neurons in an adult brain. Neuron. 1999;22:53–62. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80678-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Riede T, Suthers RA, Fletcher NH, Blevins WE. Songbirds tune their vocal tract to the fundamental frequency of their song. PNAS. 2006;103:5543–5548. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601262103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Risher WC, Ustunkaya T, Singh Alvarado J, Eroglu C. Rapid golgi analysis method for efficient and unbiased classification of dendritic spines. PLOS ONE. 2014;9:e107591. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0107591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Roberts TF, Tschida KA, Klein ME, Mooney R. Rapid spine stabilization and synaptic enhancement at the onset of behavioural learning. Nature. 2010;463:948–952. doi: 10.1038/nature08759. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Scharff C, Kirn JR, Grossman M, Macklis JD, Nottebohm F. Targeted neuronal death affects neuronal replacement and vocal behavior in adult songbirds. Neuron. 2000;25:481–492. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80910-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Scharff C, Nottebohm F. A comparative study of the behavioral deficits following lesions of various parts of the zebra finch song system: implications for vocal learning. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1991;11:2896–2913. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-09-02896.1991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Sheth SA, Abuelem T, Gale JT, Eskandar EN. Basal ganglia neurons dynamically facilitate exploration during associative learning. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:4878–4885. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3658-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Shoemaker HH. Effect of testosterone propionate on behavior of the female canary. Experimental Biology and Medicine. 1939;41:299–302. doi: 10.3181/00379727-41-10652. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  120. Slagsvold T. Annual and geographical variation in the time of breeding of the great tit parus major and the pied flycatcher ficedula hypoleuca in relation to environmental phenology and spring temperature. Ornis Scandinavica. 1976;7:127–145. doi: 10.2307/3676183. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  121. Smith GT, Brenowitz EA, Beecher MD, Wingfield JC. Seasonal changes in testosterone, neural attributes of song control nuclei, and song structure in wild songbirds. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;17:6001–6010. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-15-06001.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Sober SJ, Brainard MS. Adult birdsong is actively maintained by error correction. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12:927–931. doi: 10.1038/nn.2336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Sohrabji F, Miranda RC, Toran-Allerand CD. Identification of a putative estrogen response element in the gene encoding brain-derived neurotrophic factor. PNAS. 1995;92:11110–11114. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.24.11110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Tchernichovski O, Nottebohm F, Ho CE, Pesaran B, Mitra PP. A procedure for an automated measurement of song similarity. Animal Behaviour. 2000;59:1167–1176. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1999.1416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Tchernichovski O, Mitra PP, Lints T, Nottebohm F. Dynamics of the vocal imitation process: how a zebra finch learns its song. Science. 2001;291:2564–2569. doi: 10.1126/science.1058522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Teramitsu I, White SA. FoxP2 regulation during undirected singing in adult songbirds. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:7390–7394. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1662-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Thompson CK, Bentley GE, Brenowitz EA. Rapid seasonal-like regression of the adult avian song control system. PNAS. 2007;104:15520–15525. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707239104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Thompson CK, Schwabe F, Schoof A, Mendoza E, Gampe J, Rochefort C, Scharff C. Young and intense: FoxP2 immunoreactivity in Area X varies with age, song stereotypy, and singing in male zebra finches. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2013;7:24. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Thompson CK, Brenowitz EA. Seasonal change in neuron size and spacing but not neuronal recruitment in a basal ganglia nucleus in the avian song control system. The Journal of comparative neurology. 2005;481:276–283. doi: 10.1002/cne.20381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Tramontin AD, Hartman VN, Brenowitz EA. Breeding conditions induce rapid and sequential growth in adult avian song control circuits: a model of seasonal plasticity in the brain. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20:854–861. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-02-00854.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Vallentin D, Kosche G, Lipkind D, Long MA. Neural circuits. Inhibition protects acquired song segments during vocal learning in zebra finches. Science. 2016;351:267–271. doi: 10.1126/science.aad3023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Vallet E, Kreutzer M, Gahr M. Testosterone induces sexual release quality in the song of female canaries. Ethology. 1996;102:617–628. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1996.tb01153.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  133. Vallet E, Beme I, Kreutzer M. Two-note syllables in canary songs elicit high levels of sexual display. Animal Behaviour. 1998;55:291–297. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1997.0631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Vallet E, Kreutzer M. Female canaries are sexually responsive to special song phrases. Animal Behaviour. 1995;49:1603–1610. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(95)90082-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  135. Vellema M, Hertel M, Urbanus SL, Van der Linden A, Gahr M. Evaluating the predictive value of doublecortin as a marker for adult neurogenesis in canaries (Serinus canaria) Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2014;522:1299–1315. doi: 10.1002/cne.23476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Voigt C, Leitner S. Seasonality in song behaviour revisited: seasonal and annual variants and invariants in the song of the domesticated canary (Serinus canaria) Hormones and Behavior. 2008;54:373–378. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Weichel K, Schwager G, Heid P, Guttinger HR, Pesch A. Sex differences in plasma steroid concentrations and singing behaviour during ontogeny in canaries (Serinus canaria) Ethology. 1986;73:281–294. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1986.tb00810.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  138. Wingfield JC, Hegner RE, Dufty, AM, Ball GF. The "Challenge Hypothesis": Theoretical Implications for Patterns of Testosterone Secretion, Mating Systems, and Breeding Strategies. The American Naturalist. 1990;136:829–846. doi: 10.1086/285134. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  139. Wingfield JC, Hahn TP. Testosterone and territorial behaviour in sedentary and migratory sparrows. Animal Behaviour. 1994;47:77–89. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1994.1009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  140. Xu T, Yu X, Perlik AJ, Tobin WF, Zweig JA, Tennant K, Jones T, Zuo Y. Rapid formation and selective stabilization of synapses for enduring motor memories. Nature. 2009;462:915–919. doi: 10.1038/nature08389. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Yang G, Pan F, Gan WB. Stably maintained dendritic spines are associated with lifelong memories. Nature. 2009;462:920–924. doi: 10.1038/nature08577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Yin HH, Mulcare SP, Hilário MR, Clouse E, Holloway T, Davis MI, Hansson AC, Lovinger DM, Costa RM. Dynamic reorganization of striatal circuits during the acquisition and consolidation of a skill. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12:333–341. doi: 10.1038/nn.2261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Yu AC, Margoliash D. Temporal hierarchical control of singing in birds. Science. 1996;273:1871–1875. doi: 10.1126/science.273.5283.1871. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Yu X, Zuo Y. Spine plasticity in the motor cortex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2011;21:169–174. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.07.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Zuo Y, Lin A, Chang P, Gan WB. Development of long-term dendritic spine stability in diverse regions of cerebral cortex. Neuron. 2005;46:181–189. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Richard B Ivry1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Vocal motor experiences consolidate the vocal motor circuitry and accelerate future vocal skill development" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by Richard Ivry, serving as Reviewing Editor and Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

Testosterone treated female canaries are proposed as a model system to study the behavioral trajectories of seasonal song re-acquisition, taking advantage of the fact that female canaries can produce male-like song following systemic application of testosterone. The authors use this artificially-induced fast song acquisition process to study the fast re-acquisition of a previously learned skill. The authors quantified the behavioral changes during the first acquisition of a fine motor behavior, singing, to the behavioral progresses observed upon re-acquisition of the same skill after a period of absence of practice. In parallel, they investigated the changes in synaptic dendritic spine density in HVC that accompany these different phases of recurring motor development. Building on the female canaries' sensitivity to testosterone, the approach allows the experimenters to elicit on command (by adding/removing testosterone implants) the first acquisition of a song, impose a period of no practice (implant removal) and finally trigger re-acquisition of singing (new testosterone implant).

The results make a strong case that the song, upon reacquisition is similar to that acquired during the first phase, and that the behavioral course for first song acquisition is different than for re-acquisition, where only acoustic parameters linked to time (syllable rate, FM, AM, inter-syllable intervals) need to be reacquired. The stability of the song following reacquisition is particularly interesting in multiple regards: a) Unlike in male canaries (where song changes across seasons) the female song remains the same and b) The re-acquisition process of the same motor skill can be studied. The spine density results show that after the first acquisition of the song, the spine density diminished by 30% and this persisted after removal of the testosterone implant and during the absence of singing behavior. This finding is in line with the idea that synaptic pruning is responsible for improving and consolidating complex skilled movements. Although, the relationship between performance changes (from a slow first time learning process to fast relearning of a motor skill) and structural changes (pruning of spines in HVC) is purely correlational, the study represents a very creative approach to study the neuronal underpinnings of fast re-learning of already learned skilled motor behaviors.

Primary Issues for revision:

1) Since the song similarity claim is essential for the impact of this study (Figure 4), it is important to make this comparison as rigorously as possible, analyzing the similarity between the songs produced at the end phase of treatment 1 and the songs 2 produced during treatment 2. For example, it would be good to develop some sort of similarity score. Some ideas here include looking at the occurrence of same syllable sequences for a given bird compared to when comparing the productions of two birds. Or looking at the cross correlation between two conspecific testosterone treated female canaries. One important analysis is the quantification of syllable duration across treatment periods. Syllable duration did not deteriorate which strongly hints towards the re-acquisition of the same song. This finding might provide some evidence that single syllables are consolidated as one unit.

2) One interesting aspect of song, especially in the canary (as compared to the zebra finch), is that the transitions from one syllable to another obey certain probabilistic rules that are most likely dictated by HVC. While this is the case in male natural singing behavior, are females treated with testosterone singing a variable song with some transition rules or a very stereotyped song with strict rules? Were the transition rules affected by the testosterone treatment?

3) When looking at spine density only, can you differentiate dendritic pruning from apoptosis of part of the cell population? If the total number of cells is not stable, then the number of synapses should also change? Can the authors control for the cell density in HVC (or use the constant total number of neurites as a proxy)?

4) Need for more quantification of the synaptic changes undergone by the network (both HVC and RA) in all of the groups (including the +T2 group) will increase significantly the interest of this study for a broad readership. As it is, the timing coincidence between synaptic pruning in HVC and behavioral changes is an interesting piece of evidence for circuit reorganization but its link to behavior is relatively week. The extension of the quantification (what is happening in RA, analysis of the T2+ birds) would help make the case that synaptic changes may be linked to the behavior shown here. While we're probably not expecting a second pruning event, if the hypothesis is that the first pruning would be some sort of a motor memory trace of the song acquired under T+1, it could be worth checking that the re-acquisition of the song does not engage a second event of pruning.

5) With the experimental design, the decrease in spine number could also be explained by the application of testosterone alone and, thus, being independent of motor skill consolidation. How can the authors disentangle the effect of the treatment versus relearning?

6) The link between synaptic pruning in HVC and the fast re-learning process is a bit loose. Structural changes in HVC are linked to vocal learning in songbirds, but the specific pruning in HVC following song acquisition shown here has not been described previously. It is however difficult to parse out a possible effect of the different auditory experience (due to auditory feedback during singing) and the specific contribution of sensorimotor learning. Moreover, changes in HVC happening during the initial acquisition period may not be linked to the capacity of birds to reacquire the task rapidly. Indeed, some song parameters that are rapidly re-acquired are thought to depend more on the neural activity of nucleus RA (frequency and amplitude modulation) as they do not correlate with HVC activity in finches. These issues should be considered in revision.

7) Please provide more information about the auditory experience of the birds used for histology.

Secondary comments:

Some ideas for future work that came up in the discussion and you may wish to comment on:

1) This model could be used to investigate the difference between the motor memory trace of the song and the auditory memory trace, if the females were to match a template they heard before. Did the author control for the songs that the females might have heard from birth until Testosterone treatment? Are there any indications of song copying behavior?

2) Could it be possible to manipulate the dendritic growth during the resting period (implant removal) and see whether the re-acquisition of the song follows, after a second implant, a first-time learning trajectory or, even, the same first-time learning trajectory?

In males songbirds, it is known that with seasonal testosterone increases, there is an increase in the number of HVC cells (for instance Brenowitz and Larson, 2015). Is there anything known for females treated with testosterone?

Figure 2F: example spectrographs -> spectrograms

Figure 3—figure supplement 1: "could not be calculated for T2+ in F, I, as we observed no developmental increase of this song feature" -> "of these song features".

Figure 5: Dendrite ratio is unclear here. Do you mean probability or percentage?

Even looking at the method in the third paragraph of the subsection “Spine and dendrite quantification”, I don't quite get this ratio. By likelihood I assume you mean probability. Are these probabilities calculated per ROI over the total number of segments in a given ROI and then averaged over ROIs? Are you effectively calculating the probability of a density of spine given the probability of observing a spinous dendrite in an ROI?

Subsection “Song analysis”, fourth paragraph: Time = duration or delay to stabilization?

Subsection “Song analysis”, last paragraph: spectrographs -> spectrograms.

The title should be shorter and more concise.

Abstract: Skilled motor behaviors are not necessarily a response to another action but can be executed independent of prior inputs.

Introduction, first paragraph: What is meant by 'manipulative skills'? Retrieving the memory of food cashes is not a motor skill.

Introduction, second paragraph: Is it re-acquisition of the same song or learning new songs? Can the correlation of structural changes within HVC be explained if the birds learn a new song? Why should existing structures facilitate learning a new motor skill? What is the impact of the development of physical characteristics, constraints due to external factors e.g. muscle constraints or internal factors e.g. genetic background? This topic is discussed at the end but it might be useful to mention it during the Introduction.

Discussion, first paragraph: Overview of motor savings should be mentioned before own findings.

Discussion, second paragraph: This statement is out of context because the question was not addressed within this study. The authors should discuss the known difference of acquisition periods between fine versus gross motor skills.

Discussion, sixth paragraph: The authors argue that vocal maintenance can be explained by 'structural consolidation of […] X-projecting HVC neurons'. In zebra finches it has been shown that the deletion of X-projecting HVC neurons does not lead to an impairment of stable song production. How does this finding fit in?

Figure 1A It should be indicated that the depicted spectrogram is the spectral derivative or a regular spectrogram presentation can be used.

Figure 1B Recording of the same bird or different birds?

Figure 1D When were the songs recorded? Days post treatment? Is it true that testosterone treated females sing faster (more syllables per sec) compared to male canaries? Can the authors correlate singing behavior (amount of singing/ quality of song) with testosterone levels?

Supplementary Figure 1 is missing?

Figure 2: Can the authors compare the song learning of male canaries with the testosterone treated females in respect to syllable production rate? In the second testosterone treatment phase the three syllable speeds reemerge instantaneously. How do the authors explain that the birds in T2+ sing the slow and the medium syllables reliably more often than during phase T1? Doesn't this argue against the idea that the animals have to re-acquire the motor skill but remain proficient without practice?

Figure 2B: What is the indication of a negative cross correlation of syllable rate during the first 40 days post treatment? Is this example bird an outlier? As far as I understand the data analysis the cross-correlation was calculated based on the histogram pattern. How can a negative correlation value occur? General request: For reproducibility and the understanding of the study it is crucial that the reader can retrace whether the effect depends on single individuals or if it is an effect that is reflected in the entire group. Mean value presentations should be substituted by individual data points.

Figure 5A: When where the example photomicrographs taken (time point in relation to testosterone implant)? Figure 5B: What is the meaning of 'a' and 'b'?

Are the behavioral results displayed as mean over subjects or mean over all song syllable analyzed? This is important to make this point explicit since one bird may contribute disproportionally to the entire data set in term of number of syllables produced.

Subsection “Singing-related pruning of neuronal dendritic spines”, first paragraph: this sentence summarizing the current knowledge of neuronal processes linked to song learning is a bit minimal. It would be important to highlight the known contributions of HVC and RA to song control and the evidence for plasticity at various sites of the song control network.

In Figure 4C, it is unclear whether the correlation coefficients for the T2+ columns are computed among syllables produced in the T2+ condition or between syllables from the T1+ and T2+ conditions. As written, I understand that it's among T2+ syllables. If true, this measures the similarity of syllables within a condition, while the sentence 'both temporal and spectral features demonstrated a strong similarity in their distribution patterns between subsequent testosterone treatments' rather suggests that the authors want to point to the similarity between T1+ and T2+. Either the analysis or the phrasing should be changed.

Figure 5C-E: I do not understand the statement made concerning this figure: 'Instead of an equal reduction in spines across all neurite types, we observed a shift in the dendrite ratio from densely packed dendrites towards less dense dendrites in the testosterone treatment and removal groups (Figure 5C-E)". The distribution of spine density across neurites appears shifted to the left, reflecting a global decrease in spine density in my opinion. If not, the authors should explain why.

Statistical comments/presentation of data:

It would be nice if the authors could show the distribution of raw data along with mean and SEM in their graphs instead of bar plots of mean and SEM.

More information should be given as of how the acoustic parameters were measured. Did the authors used Sound Analysis Pro for instance for these as well? Does the FM correspond to the CV of the fundamental frequency? etc.

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "Accelerated redevelopment of vocal skills is preceded by lasting reorganization of the song motor circuitry" for further consideration at eLife. Your revised article has been favorably evaluated by Richard Ivry (Senior and Reviewing Editor) and three reviewers.

We are quite pleased with the revision. There are some issues that we would like you to address, but as you will see, these are really suggestions about how to improve the presentation of the material and conceptual issues you may wish to raise in the Discussion. I do not see any of these as really "major".

Conceptual/clarification issues for revision:

1) Clarify how FM can be lost but bandwidth maintained at the beginning of T2. Seems that these two parameters should go hand in hand given the pure tone types of syllables. Adding supplementary figures that show the distributions for every parameter akin to Figure 2A and along with some spectrograms examples of these deteriorations or preservations would help better picture the phenomenon.

2) What is mean by declarative features? Declarative memory? Is the idea that this is about the auditory template/memory of (a) tutor song(s).

3) You responded that the spine density was calculated '….of randomly selected spinous dendrite segments [of excitatory neurons in HVC…'. The study by Kornfeld et al., 2017, demonstrates that spine density of HVC-X projecting neurons is significantly larger than HVC-RA projecting neurons. How did you achieve a random selection of spinous segments in order to ensure that HVC-X and HVC-RA projecting neurons were sampled uniformly? Would it be possible to only sample HVC-X or HVC-RA projecting neurons?

4) Since this study is focused on the changes of spines of excitatory neurons, you may wish to address the importance of testosterone and/or vocal practice on changes in inhibition in the Discussion. It has been shown that inhibition, especially in HVC, is one of the crucial factors that is guiding the song learning process in zebra finches. Could a robust inhibitory network be the mechanism that allows seasonal birds (or in this case testosterone treated females) to re-learn their song more efficiently?

5) The second paragraph of the Discussion section is unclear. As defined (difference between max and min peak frequency) and given the sound produced by this species and papers by Beckers, Suthers and ten Cate, 2003, and Riede et al., 2006 that showed that several bird species filter out harmonics with their vocal tract to only leave the fundamental, bandwidth likely corresponds to the range of fundamental frequencies the bird is engaging for each syllable type. So bandwidth would be determined by the contraction frequency of the syrinx. Mean frequency and entropy, however, could be determined by the shape of the vocal tract more than by the syrinx contraction frequency. Could there be a statement here about aspects of the songs that are linked to the vocal tract modulation and that are preserved vs parameters linked to the speed of contraction of certain syrinx muscles that need to be reacquired? And/or add references and discussion about vocal production mechanisms to push such an argument.

6) In the third paragraph of the Discussion, does "learned" mean "memorized" here or did females produce these type of syllables (even rarely) before the first testosterone treatment? This may be an important distinction between the two mechanisms of song acquisitions. If females never sung such song syllables before the first treatment, females can potentially only rely on an auditory memory template of songs they heard (not demonstrated here but that could be another future investigation) or driven by some internal auditory template of what a male canary song should sound like (the implication/necessity of the auditory feedback for song production in females under T should by the way be tested by deafening or something similar) while during the second treatment, females can rely on a motor memory/trace of what they learned to produce during the first phase.

eLife. 2019 May 17;8:e43194. doi: 10.7554/eLife.43194.024

Author response


Primary Issues for revision:

1) Since the song similarity claim is essential for the impact of this study (Figure 4), it is important to make this comparison as rigorously as possible, analyzing the similarity between the songs produced at the end phase of treatment 1 and the songs 2 produced during treatment 2. For example, it would be good to develop some sort of similarity score. Some ideas here include looking at the occurrence of same syllable sequences for a given bird compared to when comparing the productions of two birds. Or looking at the cross correlation between two conspecific testosterone treated female canaries. One important analysis is the quantification of syllable duration across treatment periods. Syllable duration did not deteriorate which strongly hints towards the re-acquisition of the same song. This finding might provide some evidence that single syllables are consolidated as one unit.

One of the strengths of our proposed model system is the recurrence of highly similar song patterns, and we agree that it is important to rigorously demonstrate this similarity. The data shown in Figures 2, 3 and 4 all include correlations between the songs produced at the end phase of treatment 1 and the songs produced during treatment 2, providing a measure of song similarity between the two treatments. To support this data we have included two additional song analyses: (1) For each bird we calculated the Euclidean distance between pairs of syllables of the same type from the consecutive treatment periods, providing us with a measure of similarity between groups of syllables. This analysis shows that syllables from the second treatment phase are structurally not different from syllables from the first treatment (Figure 4—figure supplement 1; subsection “Song similarity during subsequent testosterone treatments”, second paragraph of the Results and subsection “Song analysis”, sixth paragraph of the Materials and methods). (2) We calculated the probability distribution of syllable phrase transitions, which provides information on how syllable phrases are ordered in the song. This analysis shows that song sequence parameters are highly similar between the two treatment periods (Figure 4—figure supplement 2; subsection “Song similarity during subsequent testosterone treatments”, third paragraph).

2) One interesting aspect of song, especially in the canary (as compared to the zebra finch), is that the transitions from one syllable to another obey certain probabilistic rules that are most likely dictated by HVC. While this is the case in male natural singing behavior, are females treated with testosterone singing a variable song with some transition rules or a very stereotyped song with strict rules? Were the transition rules affected by the testosterone treatment?

Testosterone-treated female canaries generally sang a variable song with some transition rules, similar to what has been shown for male canaries. Songs are not random, but also not very stereotyped. Not surprisingly, birds with larger syllable repertoires tended to have more variation in how syllable phrases were sequenced than birds with smaller repertoires. The number and probability of phrase transitions in the songs did not differ between treatment 1 and treatment 2, and we did not observe significant changes in song sequence parameters, suggesting that the rules governing these phrase transitions did not change between the two treatment periods. This data has been included in Figure 4—figure supplement 2, subsection “Song similarity during subsequent testosterone treatments, third paragraph,” of the Results and subsection “Song similarity during subsequent testosterone treatments”, last paragraph, of the Materials and methods.

3) When looking at spine density only, can you differentiate dendritic pruning from apoptosis of part of the cell population? If the total number of cells is not stable, then the number of synapses should also change? Can the authors control for the cell density in HVC (or use the constant total number of neurites as a proxy)?

A distinction has to be made here between how we calculated spine densities and how we calculated dendrite densities. (1) Spine densities (Figure 5B and C) were calculated as the number of spines per µm of dendrite for a number of randomly selected spinous dendrite segments, and not per volume unit of HVC and RA. By itself this analysis is independent of how many neurons or dendrites were present in the target brain region, and apoptosis or other changes in cell density therefore do not affect this calculation. (2) Dendrite densities are represented as the probability to detect a spinous dendrite within the volume of HVC (Figure 5D-F), and are therefore susceptible to changes in cell density. We did not observe a significant difference in the density of the total number of spiny dendrites in HVC between treatments however, indicating that the shift in the probability distribution shown in Figure 5D-F is not likely to be caused by a reduction in neuronal dendrites. Taking both pieces of evidence into account, these findings suggest a reduction of synapses in HVC through selective pruning of dendritic spines, without reducing the total number of dendrites in HVC. This point has been clarified in the second paragraph of the Results subsection “Singing-related pruning of neuronal dendritic spines”.

4) Need for more quantification of the synaptic changes undergone by the network (both HVC and RA) in all of the groups (including the +T2 group) will increase significantly the interest of this study for a broad readership. As it is, the timing coincidence between synaptic pruning in HVC and behavioral changes is an interesting piece of evidence for circuit reorganization but its link to behavior is relatively week. The extension of the quantification (what is happening in RA, analysis of the T2+ birds) would help make the case that synaptic changes may be linked to the behavior shown here. While we're probably not expecting a second pruning event, if the hypothesis is that the first pruning would be some sort of a motor memory trace of the song acquired under T+1, it could be worth checking that the re-acquisition of the song does not engage a second event of pruning.

We have included an additional analysis of spine densities in nucleus RA demonstrating that this area does not undergo specific spine pruning during the acquisition and loss of song in adult female canaries, and suggests that the delayed maturation of the motor circuitry may be specific for HVC. This data has been included in Figure 5C and subsection “Singing-related pruning of neuronal dendritic spines”, second paragraph. Unfortunately we have no brain material available to test if a second pruning event would take place during a second testosterone treatment. The experiment was specifically designed to test if we could find an anatomical ‘memory’ trace that would last through a period in which the birds did not sing and could explain the rapid re-acquisition of a previously learned song pattern. Considering the extent of pruning that took place in HVC during song acquisition it seems highly unlikely that a pruning event of such magnitude would repeat itself during a second testosterone treatment in females, or occur on an annual basis in male canaries.

5) With the experimental design, the decrease in spine number could also be explained by the application of testosterone alone and, thus, being independent of motor skill consolidation. How can the authors disentangle the effect of the treatment versus relearning?

The reviewer raises an important question on the mechanisms behind selective stabilization and elimination of dendritic spines, mechanisms that are largely unknown. In songbirds testosterone levels are inseparably linked to the development and consolidation of vocal performance, both in our study as well as in nature. From our study we cannot determine if vocal practice, testosterone levels, or a combination of both cause the observed spine pruning in HVC. Singing prevention in zebra finches delays the developmental pruning of dendritic spines in RA at least to some extent (Hayase S, et al., 2018), suggesting that vocal practice induces spine pruning in this brain region. The authors did not control for testosterone levels however, and mechanisms of spine pruning in RA may differ from those in HVC (Figure 5B and C). Independent of the role that testosterone and vocal practice play in consolidating the song system, the reduction in dendritic spines was not reversed during a long period in which both testosterone levels were low and vocal practice was absent, providing a possible anatomical basis for the rapid recurrence of song patterns. We discuss this issue in detail in the ninth and tenth paragraphs of the Discussion.

6) The link between synaptic pruning in HVC and the fast re-learning process is a bit loose. Structural changes in HVC are linked to vocal learning in songbirds, but the specific pruning in HVC following song acquisition shown here has not been described previously. It is however difficult to parse out a possible effect of the different auditory experience (due to auditory feedback during singing) and the specific contribution of sensorimotor learning. Moreover, changes in HVC happening during the initial acquisition period may not be linked to the capacity of birds to reacquire the task rapidly. Indeed, some song parameters that are rapidly re-acquired are thought to depend more on the neural activity of nucleus RA (frequency and amplitude modulation) as they do not correlate with HVC activity in finches. These issues should be considered in revision.

In our study we associate lasting structural changes in HVC with the development and re-acquisition of vocal motor performance. Our study does not directly address sensorimotor integration, and it is currently unclear if adult canaries can learn song from an external song template. Auditory deprivation during juvenile development leads to aberrant songs in canaries (e.g. Lehongre, C. et al., 2006 Anim. Behav. 72: 1319-1327) indicating the importance of audition for vocal learning. Testosterone-treated adult female canaries on the other hand develop species-typical songs even when acoustically isolated during the time they start singing for the first time, suggesting that adult song development does not necessarily depend on the same processes as juvenile song learning. In our study we cannot directly distinguish between the effects of vocal motor practice and auditory feedback on motor circuit consolidation. We consider these issues together with the potential roles that HVC and RA may play in the formation of motor memories in the fifth paragraph of the Discussion.

7) Please provide more information about the auditory experience of the birds used for histology.

All birds used for histology were raised and kept in group aviaries together with other male and female canaries throughout the experiment to assure similar social and auditory conditions. This information has been added the first paragraph of the subsection “Specimen preparation”.

Secondary comments:

Some ideas for future work that came up in the discussion and you may wish to comment on:

1) This model could be used to investigate the difference between the motor memory trace of the song and the auditory memory trace, if the females were to match a template they heard before. Did the author control for the songs that the females might have heard from birth until Testosterone treatment? Are there any indications of song copying behavior?

Therefore we did not control for the auditory background of the birds during the first year of their lives prior to the experiment. We agree that our model would be well suited to investigate differences between procedural and declarative memory formation. We would speculate that declarative song memory, or the so called song template, would primarily be formed during juvenile development and is dependent on auditory input during that time, since our birds were able to develop species-typical songs in adulthood without being exposed to external auditory input. Unpublished song playback trials have not revealed clear song copying in adult female canaries, but it is currently unknown if adult female canary songs are composed of song patterns that the birds have memorized during juvenile development. This is partly discussed in the third and fifth paragraphs of the Discussion.

2) Could it be possible to manipulate the dendritic growth during the resting period (implant removal) and see whether the re-acquisition of the song follows, after a second implant, a first-time learning trajectory or, even, the same first-time learning trajectory?

We agree that this would be a very interesting approach to demonstrate causality between dendritic spine pruning and the observed rapid re-acquisition of previously learned song patterns. The mechanisms behind spine pruning or their possible regrowth are not clear, and to our knowledge no molecular techniques currently exist to induce specific regrowth of previously-pruned dendritic spines.

In males songbirds, it is known that with seasonal testosterone increases, there is an increase in the number of HVC cells (for instance Brenowitz and Larson, 2015). Is there anything known for females treated with testosterone?

Testosterone-treated female canaries demonstrate a similar increase in HVC size and HVC neuron number (e.g. Hartog et al., 2009), without changing neuron density. We included a discussion on this topic in the sixth paragraph of the Discussion.

Figure 2F: example spectrographs -> spectrograms

Corrected.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1: "could not be calculated for T2+ in F, I, as we observed no developmental increase of this song feature" -> "of these song features"

Corrected.

Figure 5: Dendrite ratio is unclear here. Do you mean probability or percentage?

Even looking at the method in the third paragraph of the subsection “Spine and dendrite quantification”, I don't quite get this ratio. By likelihood I assume you mean probability. Are these probabilities calculated per ROI over the total number of segments in a given ROI and then averaged over ROIs? Are you effectively calculating the probability of a density of spine given the probability of observing a spinous dendrite in an ROI?

Figure 5D-F shows the probability distribution of dendrites with different spine densities. We calculated the probability distribution by dividing the number of spinous dendrites in each bin with the total number of spinous dendrites. This has been clarified in Figure 5 and in the third paragraph of the Materials and methods subsection “Spine and dendrite quantification”.

Subsection “Song analysis”, fourth paragraph: Time = duration or delay to stabilization?

‘Time’ has been replaced by ‘duration’.

Subsection “Song analysis”, last paragraph: spectrographs -> spectrograms.

Corrected.

The title should be shorter and more concise.

To convey a more concise message we changed the title to: ‘Accelerated redevelopment of vocal skills is preceded by lasting reorganization of the song motor circuitry’.

Abstract: Skilled motor behaviors are not necessarily a response to another action but can be executed independent of prior inputs.

‘Response’ has been replaced by ‘behaviors’.

Introduction, first paragraph: What is meant by 'manipulative skills'? Retrieving the memory of food cashes is not a motor skill.

With manipulative skills we mean the motor movements from e.g. paws to retrieve hidden food items. To avoid confusion with spatial memory retrieval we replaced the example with a less ambiguous example of motor skill acquisition. ‘Capuchin monkeys need to acquire manipulative foraging skills to retrieve difficult-to-access, seasonally available food items’.

Introduction, second paragraph: Is it re-acquisition of the same song or learning new songs? Can the correlation of structural changes within HVC be explained if the birds learn a new song? Why should existing structures facilitate learning a new motor skill? What is the impact of the development of physical characteristics, constraints due to external factors e.g. muscle constraints or internal factors e.g. genetic background? This topic is discussed at the end but it might be useful to mention it during the Introduction.

This sentence refers globally to seasonal song re-acquisition. Different bird species may adopt different strategies, but for adult male canaries it is factually not known if they relearn new songs each year, or if they re-develop song patterns that they have learned during juvenile development. Increasing evidence, including this study, suggests that in canaries the latter is the case. As such we correlate the structural changes that we observe in HVC with the re-acquisition of a previously learned vocal pattern. Since this is an extensive topic that depends for a part on the findings from this study, we prefer to discuss this issue in detail in the third paragraph of the Discussion.

Discussion, first paragraph: Overview of motor savings should be mentioned before own findings.

We moved the examples describing motor savings up to the first paragraph of the Discussion.

Discussion, second paragraph: This statement is out of context because the question was not addressed within this study. The authors should discuss the known difference of acquisition periods between fine versus gross motor skills.

We restructured the Discussion considerably. Because songbirds are often used as a model for sensorimotor learning, we thought it useful to specifically mention that it is not known if adult canaries can learn through sensorimotor integration. This point is more generally discussed in the third paragraph of the Discussion.

Discussion, sixth paragraph: The authors argue that vocal maintenance can be explained by 'structural consolidation of […] X-projecting HVC neurons'. In zebra finches it has been shown that the deletion of X-projecting HVC neurons does not lead to an impairment of stable song production. How does this finding fit in?

We have included a discussion on this finding in context to similar studies that argue for a role of the cortico-basal ganglia circuit in the long-term maintenance of song (Discussion, eighth paragraph).

Figure 1A It should be indicated that the depicted spectrogram is the spectral derivative or a regular spectrogram presentation can be used.

We have included this information in the legends of Figure 1, 2 and 4.

Figure 1B Recording of the same bird or different birds?

The shown spectrograms are recordings from the same individual at different times during development. This information has been clarified in the figure legend of Figure 1.

Figure 1D When were the songs recorded? Days post treatment? Is it true that testosterone treated females sing faster (more syllables per sec) compared to male canaries? Can the authors correlate singing behavior (amount of singing/ quality of song) with testosterone levels?

Recording dates have been included in the legend of Figure 1. We have no evidence that testosterone treated females can sing faster than males. The range of syllable rates observed in this study falls within the natural range of syllable rates that has been observed in male canaries living in the wild. This information has been included in the second paragraph of the subsection “Re-application of testosterone triggers an accelerated re-acquisition of song performance”. We do not have a sufficient spread of testosterone levels within and between birds to provide a meaningful quantitative correlation of song behavior with testosterone levels, except for the fact that birds do not sing when testosterone levels are very low, but sing on average ~30.000 syllables per day when testosterone levels are high.

Supplementary Figure 1 is missing?

Figure supplements are provided for main Figures 2, 3, and 4.

Figure 2: Can the authors compare the song learning of male canaries with the testosterone treated females in respect to syllable production rate?

Syllable rates of stable song from testosterone treated females fell within the natural range of syllable rates that have been observed in male canaries living in the wild (Leitner, S. et al., 2001). This information has been included in the second paragraph of the subsection “Re-application of testosterone triggers an accelerated re-acquisition of song performance”. We don’t have longitudinal song data from sufficient juvenile males to quantitatively compare syllable rate development in males and females.

In the second testosterone treatment phase the three syllable speeds reemerge instantaneously. How do the authors explain that the birds in T2+ sing the slow and the medium syllables reliably more often than during phase T1? Doesn't this argue against the idea that the animals have to re-acquire the motor skill but remain proficient without practice?

The three syllable rates did not reemerge instantaneously but took 26 days to stabilize in the individual shown in Figure 2A. This can be seen by the red fit curves in the histogram that do not start at their final value for any of the three distinguishable syllable rates. In addition, Figure 2B shows for the same individual how much the SR pattern during treatment 2 correlates with the SR pattern from treatment 1 illustrating a clear phase in which the SR pattern from treatment 2 redevelops and increases in similarity in respect to the SR pattern from treatment 1. On average across all birds SR patterns stabilized in 22 days during treatment 2, which was significantly faster than during treatment 1 (160 days). This information is quantitatively shown for all animals in Figure 2C-E and described in the third paragraph of the subsection “Re-application of testosterone triggers an accelerated re-acquisition of song performance”.

Figure 2B: What is the indication of a negative cross correlation of syllable rate during the first 40 days post treatment? Is this example bird an outlier? As far as I understand the data analysis the cross-correlation was calculated based on the histogram pattern. How can a negative correlation value occur? General request: For reproducibility and the understanding of the study it is crucial that the reader can retrace whether the effect depends on single individuals or if it is an effect that is reflected in the entire group. Mean value presentations should be substituted by individual data points.

We calculated Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficient based on the histogram pattern to determine similarity between treatment periods. Pearson’s CC has a value between -1 and 1, where 1 describes two patterns that are identical, and -1 describes two patterns that are completely opposite to each other (and thus are negatively correlated). Values around zero indicate no correlation. This is described in detail in the fourth paragraph of the Materials and methods subsection “Song analysis”. We have included raw data point for all behavioral bar graphs, and further provide this information in the accompanying source data for each figure.

Figure 5A: When where the example photomicrographs taken (time point in relation to testosterone implant)? Figure 5B: What is the meaning of 'a' and 'b'?

The time of sacrifice of the birds shown in Figure 5A has been included in the figure legend and is further described in the first paragraph of the Materials and methods subsection “Specimen preparation”. ‘a’ and ‘b’ in Figure 5B illustrate if a significant difference was observed between treatment groups. This is indicated in the legend of Figure 5.

Are the behavioral results displayed as mean over subjects or mean over all song syllable analyzed? This is important to make this point explicit since one bird may contribute disproportionally to the entire data set in term of number of syllables produced.

All means in the text and figures describe the mean over subjects, with the exception of Figure 2F which illustrates mean values of syllable rates for one syllable type in one individual. This has been clarified in all figure legends and is also described in the Materials and methods subsection “Statistical analysis”. In addition we have included raw data points in bar graphs to illustrate the spread between individuals.

Subsection “Singing-related pruning of neuronal dendritic spines”, first paragraph: this sentence summarizing the current knowledge of neuronal processes linked to song learning is a bit minimal. It would be important to highlight the known contributions of HVC and RA to song control and the evidence for plasticity at various sites of the song control network.

This sentence refers to motor learning and memory in general with the purpose of providing a rationale of why we decided to look specifically at spine pruning in relation to the observed vocal motor savings. We agree that the known contributions of HVC and RA to song control and evidence for plasticity in the song system are important items to discuss, but introducing these topics in this part of the Results section would overshadow the results. We have included an extensive discussion of the roles of HVC and RA during song control and testosterone-related plasticity in the song system in the sixth and seventh paragraphs of the Discussion.

In Figure 4C, it is unclear whether the correlation coefficients for the T2+ columns are computed among syllables produced in the T2+ condition or between syllables from the T1+ and T2+ conditions. As written, I understand that it's among T2+ syllables. If true, this measures the similarity of syllables within a condition, while the sentence 'both temporal and spectral features demonstrated a strong similarity in their distribution patterns between subsequent testosterone treatments' rather suggests that the authors want to point to the similarity between T1+ and T2+. Either the analysis or the phrasing should be changed.

The CC’s presented in the T2+ columns are computed between syllables from the T1+ and T2+ conditions. The aim of the Figure 4C is to illustrate that the correlations between T1+ and T2+ patterns are no different from the correlations between T1+ and T1+ patterns, and thus the same song patterns are produced during T1+ and T2+. We realize that the figure labels for these comparisons may have caused confusion. To clarify the comparisons we changed the color legends in the figure with more intuitive labels (T1xT1 vs. T1xT2). In addition we clarified the wording of the figure legend of Figure 4C and the first paragraph of the Results subsection “Song similarity during subsequent testosterone treatments”.

Figure 5C-E: I do not understand the statement made concerning this figure: “Instead of an equal reduction in spines across all neurite types, we observed a shift in the dendrite ratio from densely packed dendrites towards less dense dendrites in the testosterone treatment and removal groups (Figure 5C-E)". The distribution of spine density across neurites appears shifted to the left, reflecting a global decrease in spine density in my opinion. If not, the authors should explain why.

We fully agree with the reviewer that the change in the dendrite distribution reflects a global decrease in spine density. With ‘unequal reduction’ we attempted to refer to the changed shape of the distribution (which would keep the same shape if the quantified number of dendrites in each bin would be reduced by an ‘equal’ amount). Instead the distribution of dendrites shifts to the left from more densely-spined dendrites towards less densely-spined dendrites and supports our finding that spine densities reduce during the first testosterone treatment period. We have modified this sentence to make this message more clear (subsection “Singing-related pruning of neuronal dendritic spines”, second paragraph).

Statistical comments/presentation of data:

It would be nice if the authors could show the distribution of raw data along with mean and SEM in their graphs instead of bar plots of mean and SEM.

We have included the raw data point in all bar graphs that represent behavioral data, and provide source data for each figure.

More information should be given as of how the acoustic parameters were measured. Did the authors used Sound Analysis Pro for instance for these as well? Does the FM correspond to the CV of the fundamental frequency? etc.

We used Sound Analysis Pro to calculate acoustic parameters for each syllable. We included this information in the second paragraph of the Materials and methods subsection “Song analysis”, and refer to the accompanying publication and online manual for the detailed mathematical descriptions of each sound feature. Sound Analysis Pro calculates FM as the angular component of squared time and frequency derivatives. Effectively FM describes the angle of frequency traces in a syllable and is expressed in degrees.

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

We are quite pleased with the revision. There are some issues that we would like you to address, but as you will see, these are really suggestions about how to improve the presentation of the material and conceptual issues you may wish to raise in the Discussion. I do not see any of these as really "major".

Conceptual/clarification issues for revision:

1) Clarify how FM can be lost but bandwidth maintained at the beginning of T2. Seems that these two parameters should go hand in hand given the pure tone types of syllables. Adding supplementary figures that show the distributions for every parameter akin to Figure 2A and along with some spectrograms examples of these deteriorations or preservations would help better picture the phenomenon.

Bandwidth (defined here as the difference between the max and min peak frequency) describes the range of peak frequencies of a syllable while FM is calculated in Sound Analysis Pro as the mean angular component of all frequency contours within a syllable (and not only of the peak frequency). The mean FM of a syllable thus increases with an increased up and down modulation within the duration of the syllable, but bandwidth does not necessarily change as long as these increased modulations in frequency occur within the original range of peak frequencies.

To further illustrate the detailed song changes, we have included 2 supplementary figures (Figure 2—figure supplement 2 and Figure 3—figure supplement 2) with example histograms of each song feature and included several spectrogram examples of song syllables from the end of T1+, beginning of T2+, and end of T2+.

2) What is mean by declarative features? Declarative memory? Is the idea that this is about the auditory template/memory of (a) tutor song(s).

This term is no longer used here, as we have changed the focus of this section from a discussion on motor constraints in general to the contribution of the syrinx and vocal tract in shaping and re-shaping song output (Discussion, second paragraph).

3) You responded that the spine density was calculated '….of randomly selected spinous dendrite segments [of excitatory neurons in HVC…'. The study by Kornfeld et al., 2017, demonstrates that spine density of HVC-X projecting neurons is significantly larger than HVC-RA projecting neurons. How did you achieve a random selection of spinous segments in order to ensure that HVC-X and HVC-RA projecting neurons were sampled uniformly? Would it be possible to only sample HVC-X or HVC-RA projecting neurons?

The sampling areas that we used for quantifications were spatially randomly distributed across HVC. Considering that HVCX and HVCRA neurons are relatively uniformly distributed throughout HVC (see e.g. Fortune and Margoliash, 1995) our sampling method likely captures equal ratios of both neuron types between the different treatment groups. This has been clarified in the third paragraph of the subsection “Spine and dendrite quantification”. The actual spine density of each segment does not influence the sampling frequency.

4) Since this study is focused on the changes of spines of excitatory neurons, you may wish to address the importance of testosterone and/or vocal practice on changes in inhibition in the Discussion. It has been shown that inhibition, especially in HVC, is one of the crucial factors that is guiding the song learning process in zebra finches. Could a robust inhibitory network be the mechanism that allows seasonal birds (or in this case testosterone treated females) to re-learn their song more efficiently?

We have included a discussion on the potential role of inhibitory activity during song acquisition and re-acquisition in the eighth paragraph of the Discussion.

5) The second paragraph of the Discussion section is unclear. As defined (difference between max and min peak frequency) and given the sound produced by this species and papers by Beckers, Suthers and ten Cate, 2003, and Riede et al., 2006, that showed that several bird species filter out harmonics with their vocal tract to only leave the fundamental, bandwidth likely corresponds to the range of fundamental frequencies the bird is engaging for each syllable type. So bandwidth would be determined by the contraction frequency of the syrinx. Mean frequency and entropy, however, could be determined by the shape of the vocal tract more than by the syrinx contraction frequency. Could there be a statement here about aspects of the songs that are linked to the vocal tract modulation and that are preserved vs parameters linked to the speed of contraction of certain syrinx muscles that need to be reacquired? And/or add references and discussion about vocal production mechanisms to push such an argument.

We have changed the focus of this section to discuss the potential role of the syrinx and vocal tract during song acquisition and re-acquisition, and included a statement on how they may relate to the preservation and/or deterioration of song features (Discussion, second paragraph).

6) In the third paragraph of the Discussion, does "learned" mean "memorized" here or did females produce these type of syllables (even rarely) before the first testosterone treatment? This may be an important distinction between the two mechanisms of song acquisitions. If females never sung such song syllables before the first treatment, females can potentially only rely on an auditory memory template of songs they heard (not demonstrated here but that could be another future investigation) or driven by some internal auditory template of what a male canary song should sound like (the implication/necessity of the auditory feedback for song production in females under T should by the way be tested by deafening or something similar) while during the second treatment, females can rely on a motor memory/trace of what they learned to produce during the first phase.

The female canaries in this study were not observed to produce any song syllables prior to the first testosterone treatment (subsection “Testosterone-induced song development in female canaries”). ‘Memorized’ may be a better description here and we have changed the term accordingly in the fourth paragraph of the Discussion.

We completely agree with the reviewer that the observed differences in song acquisition during the first and second treatments may depend on the absence (first treatment) and presence (secondtreatment) of a motor memory trace of the song. This idea is consistent with our view that large-scale synaptic pruning occurred in motor area HVC during the first testosterone treatment, a modification that may reflect the formation of such a motor memory trace. We have emphasized this idea in the aforementioned paragraph.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Vellema M, Gahr M. 2018. Data from: Vocal motor experiences consolidate the vocal motor circuitry and accelerate future vocal skill development. Dryad Digital Repository. [DOI]

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 2—source data 1. Source file for quantitative comparisons of temporal song features.
    elife-43194-fig2-data1.xlsx (165.1KB, xlsx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.43194.007
    Figure 3—source data 1. Source file for quantitative comparisons of spectral song features.
    elife-43194-fig3-data1.xlsx (206.7KB, xlsx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.43194.011
    Figure 4—source data 1. Source file for song similarity calculations between subsequent testosterone treatments.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.43194.015
    Figure 5—source data 1. Source file for spine quantification data.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.43194.017
    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.43194.019

    Data Availability Statement

    SQL data of extracted song features for each individual have been deposited in Dryad. Further source data files have been provided for Figure 2, Figure 2—figure supplement 1, Figure 3, Figure 3—figure supplement 1, Figure 4, Figure 4—figure supplement 1, Figure 4—figure supplement 2, Figure 5.

    The following dataset was generated:

    Vellema M, Gahr M. 2018. Data from: Vocal motor experiences consolidate the vocal motor circuitry and accelerate future vocal skill development. Dryad Digital Repository.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES