Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Neuroimaging. 2019 Feb 3;29(3):335–343. doi: 10.1111/jon.12600

Adaptive Identification of Cortical and Subcortical Imaging Markers of Early Life Stress and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder

Lauren E Salminen 1, Rajendra A Morey 1, Brandalyn C Riedel 1, Neda Jahanshad 1, Emily L Dennis 1, Paul M Thompson 1
PMCID: PMC6571150  NIHMSID: NIHMS1010301  PMID: 30714246

Abstract

BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE:

Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a heterogeneous condition associated with a range of brain imaging abnormalities. Early life stress (ELS) contributes to this heterogeneity, but we do not know how a history of ELS influences traditionally defined brain signatures of PTSD. Here, we used a novel machine learning method – evolving partitions to improve classification (EPIC) – to identify shared and unique structural neuroimaging markers of ELS and PTSD in 97 combat-exposed military veterans.

METHODS:

We used EPIC with repeated cross-validation (CV) to determine how combinations of cortical thickness, surface area, and subcortical brain volumes could contribute to classification of PTSD (n = 40) versus controls (n = 57), and classification of ELS within the PTSD (ELS+ n = 16; ELS n = 24) and control groups (ELS+ n = 16; ELS n = 41). Additional inputs included intracranial volume, age, sex, adult trauma, and depression.

RESULTS:

On average, EPIC classified PTSD with 69% accuracy (SD = 5%), and ELS with 64% accuracy in the PTSD group (SD = 10%), and 62% accuracy in controls (SD = 6%). EPIC selected unique sets of individual features that classified each group with 75–85% accuracy in post hoc analyses; combinations of regions marginally improved classification from the individual atlas-defined brain regions. Across analyses, surface area in the right posterior cingulate was the only variable that was repeatedly selected as an important feature for classification of PTSD and ELS.

CONCLUSIONS:

EPIC revealed unique patterns of features that distinguished PTSD and ELS in this sample of combat-exposed military veterans, which may represent distinct biotypes of stress-related neuropathology.

Keywords: early life stress, machine learning, neuroimaging, PTSD

Introduction

Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a serious mental health condition in which patients often show brain abnormalities in regions involved in memory, fear, and emotional processing – namely, the hippocampus and amygdala – and regions of the prefrontal and cingulate cortex.1 However, the etiology of PTSD is complicated by individual differences in predisposing factors that may also affect brain structure and symptoms.2,3 There is interest in identifying disease mechanisms and risk factors that influence brain outcomes among individuals with PTSD (PTSD+); early life stress (ELS) is one key predisposing factor that affects brain abnormalities in this population.4,5

ELS affects cortical and subcortical brain structures implicated in PTSD,6 but ELS and PTSD do not impact brain structure identically.7,8 In a study of military veterans,9 PTSD symptom severity was related to cortical thickness (CT) in the posterior cingulate (PCC)/paracentral area, but the direction of this association depended on ELS. Specifically, CT in these regions was positively associated with PTSD symptom severity in ELS-exposed (ELS+) veterans, but negatively associated with symptom severity in ELS-unexposed (ELS) veterans. Further, associations of symptom severity with amygdala and hippocampal volumes were significant only in the ELS+ group. ELS may therefore have a regionally selective influence on the brain that leads to different profiles of abnormalities in trauma exposed veterans with and without PTSD. We do not fully understand how PTSD affects cortical surface area (SA), but lower SA has been reported in maltreated children and may have implications for PTSD+ patients with a history of ELS.10

We recently developed a new supervised machine learning tool, evolving partitions to improve classification (EPIC), that adaptively selects and merges sets of brain measures to improve group classification.11 EPIC is based on the idea that certain features may not relate to a grouping variable when examined independently, but when integrated with others, may better discriminate subjects from different classes. This top-down approach reduces the dimension of a set of imaging predictors by repartitioning the cortex into regions and “super-regions” (the result of merging 2 or more regions) to boost group classification. EPIC was initially used to denote “evolving partitions to improve connectomics”11 – it was used to cluster and simplify connectivity matrices to achieve better classification. In general, the adaptive merging process can be applied to improve classification based on clusters of any type of spatial data, including the structural data used here, so here we use the term to refer to “evolving partitions to improve classification.” Our group has previously shown that EPIC improves the accuracy of disease classification in Alzheimer’s disease11 and traumatic brain injury.12 Here, we used EPIC to determine if unique combinations of SA, CT, and subcortical volumes (VL) could discriminate individuals with PTSD and ELS. We aimed to identify new patterns of neuroimaging signatures that may represent unique phenotypes of PTSD or ELS and could be tested in subsequent studies in both military and civilian samples.

Method

Participants

Data were collected from 97 combat-exposed Operation Enduring Freedom and Operation Iraqi Freedom (OEF/OIF) veterans (PTSD n = 40, controls n = 57) who took part in the military conflicts in Iraq and Afghanistan following the September 11, 2001, terrorist attacks. Participants were aged 23–65 (male, n = 80; female, n = 17) and recruited from the Durham VA and Duke University Medical Centers. Exclusion criteria consisted of any Axis I diagnosis other than PTSD or major depressive disorder, substance dependence (other than nicotine), high risk for suicide, history of learning disability or developmental delay, history of head injury with loss of consciousness > 5 minutes, neurological disorders, major medical conditions, and contraindications for MRI (eg, claustrophobia). The same exclusion criteria applied to controls, except that control subjects could not meet criteria for any Axis I diagnosis. We did not exclude individuals who were taking medication for depression, anxiety, and/or sleep disturbances as these conditions are prevalent in this population. All participants provided informed consent; procedures were approved by the local IRBs.

Clinical Assessment

A diagnosis of PTSD was determined using the Clinician Administered PTSD scale for DSM-IV (CAPS-4). Adult trauma and ELS exposure were evaluated using the Traumatic Life Events Questionnaire (TLEQ).13 Adult trauma was quantified as a continuous variable reflecting exposure severity (ie, total number of exposures). Participants indicating exposure to any traumatic event before age 18 were identified as ELS+, whereas participants indicating no trauma exposure before age 18 were identified as ELS (unexposed).14 Current depression was measured using the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI).15

Neuroimaging Acquisition and Processing

MRI scanning was completed at two sites using a 3T GE MR750 (93% of sample) and 3T GE Signa EXCITE (7% of sample) scanner, each equipped with an 8-channel head coil. χ2 analyses revealed no significant differences between sites and target groups (PTSD by site, x2(1) = .79, P = .375; ELS by site, x2(1) = .33, P = .564). Independent of the target groups, we did not observe site differences in the distribution of sex (x2(1) = 1.6, P = .205), age (t(95) −.86, P = .393), BDI scores (t(95) = −1.24, P = .216), or adult trauma exposure (t(95) −.33, P = .743). T1-weighted axial brain images were obtained with 1-mm isotropic voxels using the following parameters: GE Signa EXCITE: TR/TE/flip = 8.208-ms/3.22-ms/12°, GE MR750: TR/TE/flip = 7.484-ms/2.984-ms/12°; all images were captured with FOV = 256 × 256 mm, 1-mm slice thickness. T1-weighted images were processed using FreeSurfer version 5.3. Cortical and subcortical extractions were performed and checked for quality control using standardized protocols from the ENIGMA consortium, yielding CT and SA for 34 bilateral cortical regions of interest (ROIs), and bilateral VL for 8 subcortical structures.

Design

Primary analyses were completed to determine if EPIC could distinguish PTSD+ veterans from trauma-exposed controls using 152 neuroimaging inputs (68 CT, 68 SA, and 16 VL). EPIC was also used to determine the role of ELS within each group (PTSD: N=40, ELS+ (n=16)/ELS (n=24); Controls: N=57, ELS+ (n = 16)/ELS (n = 41)). All analyses included age, sex, and intracranial volume (ICV) as input variables. Preliminary analyses revealed no significant differences in age, sex, BDI scores, TLEQ scores, PTSD, and ELS by site, and therefore we did not include site/scanner as input features. Total score on the BDI was included as an input feature when depression differed significantly between the target groups in order to model depression symptoms as an important feature of PTSD and ELS.

Classification with EPIC

The workflow of EPIC is shown in Figure 1. Here, input variables included CT, SA, and VL measurements, and the respective covariates for each analysis. Structural MRI inputs were used to generate merged composite cortical partitions through iterative ROI sampling over the space of possible ROI groupings. We applied a 70/30 (training/testing) stratified split of the data, in which a linear support vector machine (SVM) learned the optimal partitions for group classification in the training data, and then evaluated the partitions in the test data using stratified k-fold cross-validation (CV). After splitting the data, 67 participants were used for k-fold CV and 30 participants were held out for testing. In line with original group distributions, we maintained 40% cases and 60% controls for each analysis, resulting in 12 cases/18 controls for hold-out, and 27 cases/40 controls for training. For the primary analysis of PTSD versus controls, we applied 5-fold CV to the training data, resulting in approximately 5 PTSD cases and 8 controls per fold. For secondary analyses of ELS within the PTSD and control groups, we used 2-fold CV to account for lower cell sizes in the target subgroups. We then rebased the stratified CV rates according to 40% of ELS+ participants and 60% of ELS participants within each subgroup. For the analysis of ELS in PTSD patients, the rebased CV rate provided a total test set of 6 ELS+ and 14 ELS PTSD patients, divided by 2 (folds), equaling 3 ELS+ and 7 ELS patients per fold. For the analysis of ELS in controls, the rebased cross-validation rate provided a total test set of 6 ELS+ and 25 ELS controls, divided by 2 (folds), equaling 3 ELS+ and 13 ELS controls per fold. k-Fold CV was repeated 10 times for all analyses. Simulated annealing was used to better identify optimal partitions, or ways of merging structural measures.11 The EPIC algorithm was written in Python using an in-house modification of scikit learn.16 The code is available from the authors upon request.

Fig 1.

Fig 1.

Diagram of the combinatorial support vector machine (SVM) approach used by EPIC. Labels depict the partitions used for classifying PTSD from trauma-exposed controls in the main analysis.

We evaluated EPIC’s performance using balanced accuracy ([(true positive (TP)/(TP + false negative (FN)) + true negative (TN)/(TN + false positive (FP))]/2), sensitivity (TP/TP+FN), specificity (TN/FP+TN), and the positive predictive value (PPV; TP/TP+FP) averaged across the 10 repeats. We also report performance metrics for the top repeats of EPIC, which we defined as classification accuracy ≥ 65%, to demonstrate the importance of repeated CV in machine learning studies. Balanced accuracy was used as our primary index of performance to account for modest sample size imbalances between the target groups. We include three additional metrics of classifier performance for each analysis in Table 1, to allow for comparison to others that have been reported in the literature. To determine whether EPIC’s combinatorial approach improved classification of the target groups, we report classifier performance for the SVM using the original FreeSurfer brain measures and covariates modeled individually. The same optimization parameters were applied to both sets of models. Using a thresholding feature elimination procedure, individual regions and super regions with absolute SVM coefficient weights (w) < .05 on training data were considered noise and removed from the primary analysis to enhance power. We set a threshold of .003 for the secondary analyses, which was determined from the final accuracy outputs from the test data.

Table 1.

Average Performance Results Across 10 Repeats

Classification with Features Modeled Individually
Model Acc bAcc* Sens* Spec* PPV* F1 Gmean
PTSD vs. Controls 68% 67% 56% 77% 65% 0.60 66%
ELS+ vs. ELS, PTSD 64% 63% 56% 70% 58% 0.54 63%
ELS+ vs. ELS, Controls 68% 57% 30% 84% 42% 0.33 50%
Classification with EPIC
Model Acc bAcc* Sens* Spec* PPV* F1 Gmean
PTSD vs. Controls 71% 69% 58% 81% 69% 0.62 69%
ELS+ vs. ELS, PTSD 66% 64% 53% 75% 60% 0.54 63%
ELS+ vs. ELS, Controls 70% 62% 43% 81% 48% 0.44 59%

Abbreviations and formulas: Acc = Accuracy, true positive (TP)+true negative (TN)/TP+TN+false positive (FP)+false negative (FN); bAcc = balanced accuracy, [(TP/(TP+FN)+TN/(TN+FP)]/2; F1 = harmonic mean of precision and recall, 2*(Recall * Precision)/(Recall + Precision); Gmean = geometric mean, √TPrate*TNrate; *reported in main text; PPV = positive predictive value (precision), TP/TP+FP; Sens = sensitivity (recall), TP rate, TP/TP+FN; Spec = specificity, TN rate, TN/FP +TN.

Results

PTSD versus Trauma-Exposed Controls

PTSD+ veterans scored higher (M = 15.9, SD = 11.2) on the BDI compared to controls (M = 4.2, SD = 5.4, P < .001),, so the total score was included as an input feature. EPIC classified PTSD with 69% accuracy (SD = 5%) on average, with 58% sensitivity, 81% specificity, and 69% PPV. Best performance with EPIC classified PTSD from trauma-exposed controls with 77% accuracy (63% sensitivity, 91% specificity, 83% PPV, Fig 2C); seven out of 10 repeats classified PTSD with greater than 65% accuracy. Across these repeats, EPIC identified the BDI as the strongest predictor of PTSD compared to all imaging variables. Super regions were identified as important features across 5 out of the 7 top repeats but did not contribute to accuracy for the best individual repeat with EPIC (Fig 2). In Figure 2H, we report the most frequently selected features across the top repeats of EPIC. After the BDI, the strongest predictors of PTSD were CT in the left temporal pole (TP), and SA in the left post central gyrus, and left inferior temporal gyrus (ITG). Compared to average performance using the individual input features, EPIC modestly improved classification accuracy by 2%, on average (Table 1).

Fig 2.

Fig 2.

Panels A-G. The top features for the strongest repeats of EPIC (≥65% classification accuracy) are plotted in rank order according the absolute value of the SVM coefficient. We limited the number of features shown to the top 15 in each feature list, but most repeats revealed less than 15 features that contributed to final classification accuracy. The most important features are reported (Panel H) in rank order according to the frequency by which each variable was identified as a top predictor of PTSD across the top repeats of EPIC. Variables that were selected the same number of times across repeats were ranked by their relative position and coefficient weight across feature lists of each individual repeat. Most MRI features were selected more than once across repeats (blue); features selected in one repeat are displayed in yellow. Super regions (red) consisted of 3 or less merged regions in this analysis. Nonbrain regions (green) were selected in each repeat, with the BDI selected as the most important feature in each repeat. Acronyms (in descending order from left to right): SA (surface area), CT (cortical thickness), VL (subcortical volumes), L (left hemisphere), R (right hemisphere), BDI (score on the Beck Depression Inventory), ITG (inferior temporal gyrus), FP (frontal pole), SPG (superior parietal gyrus), TP (temporal pole), SMG (supramarginal gyrus), ParsOp (pars opercularis), PCAL (pericalcarine), PCC (posterior cingulate), IPL (inferior parietal lobule), STG (superior temporal gyrus), ParsOrb (pars orbitalis), ParsTri (pars triangularis), PHG (parahippocampal gyrus), hippo (hippocampus), TrTG (transverse temporal gyrus), ACC (anterior cingulate), MFG (middle frontal gyrus).

As a post hoc analysis, we retested the classifier using input features that were residualized for the total BDI score of each participant to determine whether EPIC or the original FreeSurfer segmentation could distinguish PTSD from the control group after regressing out the effects of depression on the predictor variables. Neither the original FreeSurfer segmentation, nor the EPIC classifier, could distinguish PTSD groups from controls, with the same low discriminatory power between methods (38% accuracy, on average). This finding confirmed the importance of the BDI as an input feature when scores differed between the target groups. Given this low classification accuracy, we did not run subsequent models without BDI scores in subsequent analyses where total scores differed significantly between the target groups.

PTSD+ Veterans with and without ELS

PTSD+ groups did not differ significantly on BDI scores (ELS+:M = 16.4, SD = 12.2; ELS: M = 15.5, SD = 10.7) or adult trauma exposure (ELS+: M = 13.6, SD = 8.5; ELS: M = 10, SD = 10.1) indexed with the TLEQ, so these variables were not used here as input features. On average, EPIC classified ELS in the PTSD group with 64% accuracy (SD = 10%), 56% sensitivity, 70% specificity, and 60% PPV. Best performance with EPIC classified ELS with 77% accuracy (63% sensitivity, 92% specificity, 83% PPV; Fig 3C); five of the 10 repeats classified ELS with greater than 65% accuracy. EPIC repeatedly selected VL in the right putamen, and CT in the left caudal anterior cingulate (ACC) and right parahippocampal gyrus (PHG) as the strongest predictors of ELS (Fig 3F). Super regions were not selected as important features for distinguishing ELS across the top repeats. In comparison to the SVM that modeled features individually, EPIC marginally improved classification accuracy by 2% (Table 1).

Fig 3.

Fig 3.

Panels A-E. The top features for the strongest repeats of EPIC (≥65% classification accuracy) are plotted in rank order according the absolute value of the SVM coefficient. We limited the number of features shown to the top 15 in each feature list. The most important features selected across the top repeats of EPIC are reported in rank order (Panel F) according to the frequency by which each variable was identified as a top predictor of ELS in the PTSD group. Variables that were selected the same number of times across repeats were ranked by their relative position and coefficient weight across feature lists. Most MRI features were selected more than once across repeats (blue); features selected in one repeat are displayed in yellow. Nonbrain metrics (green) were not important for ELS classification in this analysis. Although super regions were identified in the complete feature list of each repeat, they were among the weakest predictors and not plotted here. Nonbrain regions (green) were selected in each repeat, with the BDI selected as the most important feature in each repeat. Acronyms (in descending order from left to right): SA (surface area), CT (cortical thickness), VL (subcortical volumes), L (left hemisphere), R (right hemisphere), MTG (middle temporal gyrus), ACC (anterior cingulate), PHG (parahippocampal), ParsOrb (pars orbitalis), PCC (posterior cingulate), MFG (middle frontal gyrus), SPG (superior parietal gyrus), TP (temporal pole), FP (frontal pole), SFG (superior frontal gyrus), hippo (hippocampus), TrTG (transverse temporal gyrus), PCAL (pericalcarine), STG (superior temporal gyrus), IPL (inferior parietal lobule), ParsTri (pars triangularis), ParsOp (pars opercularis), OFC (orbitofrontal cortex), LatVent (lateral ventricle).

Trauma-Exposed Controls with and without ELS

BDI scores and adult trauma exposure differed significantly between ELS+ (BDI, M = 6.9, SD = 7.1; TLEQ, M = 13.1, SD = 5.8) and ELS controls (BDI, M = 3.1, SD = 4.2, P = .016; TLEQ, M = 13.1, SD = 5.8, P = .001), so these variables were included as input features in this analysis. On average, EPIC classified ELS with 62% accuracy (SD 6%), 43% sensitivity, 81% specificity, and 48% PPV. Three out=of 10 repeats achieved accuracy ≥ 65%, and the best performance with EPIC classified participants with ELS with 71% accuracy (63% sensitivity, 80% specificity, 67% PPV; Fig 4A). One super region of CT was identified as a top feature in the 9th repeat of EPIC (Fig 4B), but sensitivity and PPV here were low (50%). Adult trauma indexed on the TLEQ was the only input feature that was repeatedly selected as an important predictor of ELS across the top three repeats. Several features were selected in two of the three repeats (Fig 4D), including CT in the right isthmus cingulate (ICC) and pericalcarine, SA in the right supramarginal gyrus (SMG) and PCC, and total scores on the BDI. EPIC improved classification accuracy from the original FreeSurfer segmentation by 4% (Table 1).

Fig 4.

Fig 4.

Panels A-C. The top features for the strongest repeats of EPIC (≥65% classification accuracy) are plotted in rank order according the absolute value of the SVM coefficient. Only three repeats achieved accuracy ≥ 65%. We limited the number of shown features to the top 15 in each repeat. The most important features selected across the top repeats of EPIC are reported in rank order (Panel D), according to the frequency by which each variable was identified as a top predictor of ELS in the control group. Variables that were selected the same number of times across repeats were ranked by their relative position and coefficient weight across feature lists. Most MRI features were selected once across repeats (yellow); features selected more than once are displayed in blue. Nonbrain metrics (green) were identified as important features in each repeat, with adult trauma exposure collectively selected as the most important feature for ELS classification in this analysis. One super region (red) was identified as a top feature in the 9th repeat of EPIC (Panel B). It consisted of merged regions of cortical thickness (CT). Acronyms (in descending order from left to right): SA (surface area), L (left hemisphere), R (right hemisphere), TLEQ (score on the Traumatic Life Events Questionnaire-adult), BDI (score on the Beck Depression Inventory), PCC (posterior cingulate), ICC (isthmus cingulate), MTG (middle temporal gyrus), SMG (supramarginal), PHG (parahippocampal gyrus), OFC (orbitofrontal cortex), TP (temporal pole), FP (frontal pole), banks STS (banks of superior temporal sulcus), MFG (middle frontal gyrus), ICV (intracranial volume), PCAL (pericalcarine), ParsOp (pars opercularis), ITG (inferior temporal gyrus), ParsTri (pars triangularis), lateral OCC (lateral occipital cortex), ACC (anterior cingulate).

Observed Feature Patterns across the Analyses

In Table 2, we provide a ranked list of unique and shared features of the target groups based on the repeated selection of variables that were among the top discriminating predictors in each analysis. SA in the right PCC was the only feature that was repeatedly selected as an important predictor for each analysis. As a post hoc analysis, we used logistic regression to test whether the unique feature patterns identified in each analysis would explain a significant portion of variance between the target groups. The PCC was included as an independent variable in all three analyses. For the main analysis, EPIC’s feature pattern classified participants with PTSD at 74% accuracy (Nagelkerke R2 = .36); the strongest individual predictors were greater SA in the left pars opercularis (P = .007, β = .89) and left lingual gyrus (P = .002, β = 1.2). In the PTSD group, EPIC’s feature pattern classified participants with ELS at 85% accuracy (Nagelkerke R2 = .78). No individual predictors were significant at the .05 alpha level, but lower CT in the rostral ACC (P = .056, β = −3.4) and PCC (P = .069, β = −4.4), and greater CT=in the caudal ACC (P = .062, β = −4.03) trending toward significance. Finally, the feature pattern in controls classified participants with ELS at 83% accuracy (Nagelkerke R2 = .57), with greater adult trauma exposure (P = .007, β = 1.7) and lower CT in the right medial OFC (P = .015, β = −1.5) as the strongest predictors in the model.

Table 2.

Unique and Shared Input Features by Analysis

Pattern 1: PTSD Pattern 2: ELS+PTSD Pattern 3: ELS Shared Features
L postcentral (SA) R putamen (VL) TLEQ (adult) R PCC (SA)
L ITG (SA) L caudal ACC (CT/SA) R ICC (CT)
L ParsOp (SA) R PHG (CT) R medial OFC (CT/SA)
R postcentral (CT) L MTG (SA) R insula (CT/SA)
L lingual (SA) R caudal ACC (SA)
R IPL (SA) R entorhinal (CT)
R TrTG (CT) L putamen (VL)
Age L rostral ACC (CT)
L pallidum (VL)
L PHG (SA)
R FP (CT)
R SFG (CT/SA)
R rostral ACC (CT/SA)

Variables in the first three columns are features repeatedly selected as important predictors of the target group in each column; they were not repeatedly selected in other analyses. The last column indicates variables selected as important features across the analyses. Acronyms (in descending order from left to right): L (left hemisphere), R (right hemisphere), SA (surface area), ITG (inferior temporal gyrus), ParsOp (pars opercularis), CT (cortical thickness), PCC (posterior cingulate), IPL (inferior parietal lobule), TrTG (transverse temporal gyrus), VL (subcortical volumes), ACC (anterior cingulate), PHG (parahippocampal gyrus), MTG (middle temporal gyrus), FP (frontal pole), SFG (superior frontal gyrus), TLEQ (traumatic life events questionnaire), ICC (isthmus cingulate), OFC (orbitofrontal cortex).

Discussion

Our machine learning method, EPIC, revealed preliminary neuroimaging signatures of PTSD and ELS that classified participants with greater than 60% accuracy, on average, across analyses. BDI scores were identified as an important feature for distinguishing PTSD in the whole sample and ELS in trauma exposed controls, suggesting a critical role of trauma-related emotional dysregulation that occurs independent of a PTSD diagnosis. Comparison of the learned features that were repeatedly selected in each analysis revealed three unique patterns of neuroimaging features, with common involvement of the PCC – an important brain region for processing emotionally salient stimuli.17,18 These patterns may represent underlying “biotypes” of childhood and adult trauma, which we discuss in detail below.

Classification was strongest when distinguishing PTSD from trauma-exposed controls (69% on average), with total scores on the BDI repeatedly selected as the most important feature across all repeats of EPIC. The most important neuroimaging features included metrics of cortical regions involved in somatosensory function, social cognition, and emotional processing of sensory input. These regions distinguished individuals with PTSD after total BDI scores, suggesting that traditionally defined PTSD brain abnormalities in the hippocampus and amygdala may be due to comorbid depression rather than features unique to PTSD (eg, hypervigilance). Features that were unique to PTSD have been described as constituents of the extra-striate ventral visual cortex.19 When we examined the relative importance of these features as predictors of PTSD, we found larger SA in the lingual gyrus and pars opercularis – regions that have shown abnormal imaging results in relation to trauma and psychosis-spectrum symptoms across psychiatric diagnoses.2022 The metric-specificity of these results may implicate a developmental risk for PTSD – SA expands throughout childhood and adolescence and thus is vulnerable to early environmental influence.23 Recent work using resting state fMRI shows distinct functional specialization of cortical regions within the extra-striate ventral visual cortex in human newborns, including unique functional associations between the cortical regions that repeatedly classified PTSD from controls.19 Replication efforts are needed to determine the reliability of a corticolimbic-somatosensory imaging pattern in relation to PTSD and its specific symptom clusters. Twin studies will be helpful for elucidating a potential risk for PTSD that may be tied to this learned neural profile.

The top predictors of ELS in the PTSD+ group included brain regions that tap emotion regulation, reward sensitivity, and executive control – functional domains that tend to be abnormal in PTSD.24 Among these regions was the rostral and caudal ACC, a structure that is commonly disrupted in ELS+ populations.6 ELS may prime brain regions that subserve these functions to exhibit a chronic and exaggerated threat response that disrupts brain structure and increases risk for PTSD following adult trauma exposure. The interpretation of directional differences between the caudal and rostral ACC in our post hoc analysis is unclear but may reflect a nuisance result of the multivariate design. This is an interesting topic for future work given the large body of literature implicating the ACC in stress-related phenotypes.

Adult trauma exposure was the most important feature for classifying ELS in trauma-exposed controls. This is consistent with evidence that ELS is linked with high risk for subsequent trauma exposure in adults,25 which may be due to increased risk-taking behaviors in ELS+ individuals.26 Additional work shows significantly higher prevalence of ELS in military samples, which may reflect an escape from adversity among those who voluntarily enlist.27 The observation that depression was an important predictor of ELS also is consistent with studies that show higher symptoms of depression and emotional dysregula tion among otherwise healthy individuals with a history of ELS compared to unexposed controls.28,29 The unique neuroimaging pattern that distinguished ELS involved the ICC, medial OFC, and insula, with the strongest post hoc predictive value in the medial OFC. This cluster of regions has been previously associated with negative affect and self-referential processing,30 which is consistent with a higher degree of emotional dysregulation observed in the ELS+ group.

The observation that the BDI was repeatedly selected as an important feature for discerning PTSD from trauma-exposed controls deserves discussion. There is controversy in the literature regarding the true independence of PTSD and depression given the high prevalence of comorbidity.31,32 Neuroimaging studies of PTSD often covary for depression with the idea that it is a core component of PTSD and is an exercise in tautology to include it in statistical models comparing PTSD patients and controls. The observation that PTSD and control groups were poorly discriminated when the input features were residualized for BDI scores aligns with this idea. However, our data may also suggest that PTSD and depression are related but distinct phenotypes, as only 42% of the PTSD group met criteria for clinically significant depression (BDI cutoff score = 17).15 Further post hoc tests revealed only small to moderate correlations between depression severity indexed with the BDI, and PTSD symptom severity indexed with the CAPS (r = .38). It is possible that the importance of the BDI for PTSD classification, despite low agreement between PTSD and depression severity, may be explained by high levels of general distress. Recent work by Byllesby et al. showed that the relationship between PTSD and depression was driven by nonspecific underlying distress rather than any single PTSD symptom cluster.33 This study also showed that negative alterations in cognition and mood (“emotional numbing”) predicted depression when PTSD was decomposed into distinct factors. The authors concluded that general distress underlies the overarching construct of PTSD, but not its individual counterparts, and may explain common PTSD and depression comorbidity. This conclusion is consistent with earlier work showing moderate correlations between the BDI and CAPS factor scores of emotional numbing.34 The idea of divergent outcomes when considering generalized distress versus traumatic stress also aligns with studies showing divergent effects of trauma and depression on the dexamethasone (DEX) suppression test – an index of endogenous cortisol. Specifically, trauma-exposed individuals show abnormal cortical suppression on the DEX test, whereas trauma-unexposed individuals with depression show abnormal nonsuppression of cortisol; DEX outcomes are comparable in trauma-exposed individuals with comorbid depression.35,36 These studies and others point to a unique biological phenotype of PTSD and comorbid depression,31 and may explain the relevance of the BDI as a discriminant predictor of PTSD in our study.

Several limitations should be acknowledged. (1) EPIC combined neuroimaging features to boost classification accuracy in each analysis, but super regions showed low feature importance relative to the individual regions. Combining neuroimaging metrics into super regions may reduce noise in the individual measures that are most relevant to group classification, thereby improving accuracy relative to using each feature individually.11 (2) Sensitivity was low in all analyses, ranging from 43% to 58%, on average. This is not necessarily surprising because all participants in this study were exposed to military combat. Lifetime trauma exposure in military veterans may yield a unique phenotype that is independent of PTSD and ELS, limiting the detection rate of either “condition.” Sensitivity was lowest for detection of ELS in the control group (43% accuracy, on average), likely because the control group consists of nonclinical participants who should not have any gross brain abnormalities. Of note, PPVs generally revealed better performance than sensitivity outcomes across analyses, indicating a higher probability that individuals who screened positive for the target group truly belonged to that group. The high specificity observed in controls with ELS also suggests that EPIC can distinguish participants with subtle brain differences within the normal range of variance that would be expected in a nonclinical sample. (3) Although our sample size is consistent with typical machine learning studies in neuroimaging and psychiatry, numbers were small when considering the subdivisions of cases and controls used for training and testing; this may have limited our ability to detect more robust effects across the target groups. However, recent work from the ENIGMA consortium reveals comparable rates of classification accuracy for bipolar disorder in over 3,000 individuals,37 suggesting that sample size may not be an issue in our study. Using the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) approach to guide future machine learning studies may significantly improve classification and prediction of complex psychiatric diseases;38 this is currently a goal of several Working Groups within the ENIGMA consortium. (4) We did not examine the ability of EPIC to distinguish ELS+ from ELS controls independent of PTSD as multiple projects within the ENIGMA consortium are pursuing this research topic in larger samples with complex psychiatric histories.39 Thus, we cannot determine from the information presented whether PTSD is easier to detect and classify than ELS. This is an important research question to address using clinically diverse datasets to provide a more powerful depiction of ELS phenotypes independent of psychiatric illness. (5) This cohort was exposed to military combat and results may not generalize to civilians. However, the most salient predictive features of PTSD and ELS are consistent with civilian studies, so they may represent the larger PTSD+ and ELS+ populations.

EPIC adapts regions of interest to improve classification, and a similar approach could be implemented for functional imaging data. Here, we focused on structural MRI features to compare classifier results to the large body of literature showing structural brain disruptions among individuals with PTSD and ELS. In functional connectivity analyses, however, the seed regions that act as nodes of the network could be adaptively refined to improve classification of the target groups, and some regions could be merged or split to adapt the set of predictors. This is a current goal of the PGC-ENIGMA PTSD Working Group – the larger data source from which this work stems.

In sum, our results show region-specific distinctions in the neuroimaging profiles of PTSD and ELS in military veterans. An important strength of this study is the use of repeated CV, where we show notable variance across 10 repeats of EPIC for each analysis. This is an important element of machine learning designs that should be considered in future studies, as it provides a reliability check that a feature selected as “important” is not a spurious result from one individual repeat of the classifier. We also report a specific function of self-reported depression and adult trauma exposure as important nonimaging markers that may distinguish people with PTSD and ELS in future data-driven designs. Further work will determine the generalizability of these findings in other cohorts with additional sources of clinical heterogeneity that are characteristic of PTSD populations.

Acknowledgments and Disclosure:

This work was supported by the Mid-Atlantic (VISN 6) Mental Illness Research, Education and Clinical Center (MIRECC) and VA Merit awards (1I01RX000389–01, 1I01CX000748) from the U.S. Department of Veteran Affairs, and grants from the National Institutes of Health (R01 NS086885, R01 MH111671, U54 EB020403 (BD2K), P41 EB015922, K23 MH073091). The authors have no conflicts of interest.

References

  • 1.Admon R, Milad MR, Hendler T. A causal model of post-traumatic stress disorder: disentangling predisposed from acquired neural abnormalities. Trends Cogn Sci 2013;17:337–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Fani N, King TZ, Shin J, et al. Structural and functional connectivity in posttraumatic stress disorder: associations with FKBP5. Depress Anxiety 2016;33:300–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Philip NS, Carpenter SL, Sweet LH. Developing neuroimaging phenotypes of the default mode network in PTSD: integrating the resting state, working memory, and structural connectivity. J Vis Exp 2014;89:e51651. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Fonzo GA, Huemer J, Etkin A. History of childhood maltreatment augments dorsolateral prefrontal processing of emotional valence in PTSD. J Psychiatr Res 2016;74:45–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Morey RA, Haswell CC, Hooper SR, et al. Amygdala, hippocampus, and ventral medial prefrontal cortex volumes differ in mal-treated youth with and without chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 2015;43:791–801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Cohen RA, Grieve S, Hoth KF, et al. Early life stress and morphometry of the adult anterior cingulate cortex and caudate nuclei. Biol Psychiat 2006;59:975–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Paquola C, Bennett MR, Lagopoulos J. Understanding heterogeneity in grey matter research of adults with childhood maltreatment—a meta-analysis and review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2016; 69:299–312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Woon FL, Hedges DW. Hippocampal and amygdala volumes in children and adults with childhood maltreatment-related post-traumatic stress disorder: a meta-analysis. Hippocampus 2008;18: 729–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Corbo V, Salat DH, Amick MM, et al. Reduced cortical thickness in veterans exposed to early life trauma. Psychiat Res 2014;223: 53–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Kelly PA, Viding E, Wallace GL, et al. Cortical thickness, surface area, and gyrification abnormalities in children exposed to maltreatment: neural markers of vulnerability? Biol Psychiat 2013;74:845–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Prasad G, Joshi SH, Thompson PM. Optimizing brain connectivity networks for disease classification using EPIC. Proc IEEE Int Symp Biomed Imaging 2014;2014:834–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Dennis EL, Prasad G, Babikian T, et al. Adaptive algorithms to map how brain trauma affects anatomical connectivity in children. Proceedings of the SPIE (abstract) 2015;9681:B7. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kubany ES, Haynes SN, Leisen MB, et al. Development and preliminary validation of a brief broad-spectrum measure of trauma exposure: the traumatic life events questionnaire. Psychol Assess 2000;12:210–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Clancy CP, Graybeal A, Thompson WP, et al. Lifetime trauma exposure in veterans with military-related posttraumatic stress disorder: association with current symptomatology. J Clin Psychiat 2006;67:1346–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio 1996;78,490–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Pedregosa F, Varoquaux G, Gramfort A, et al. Scikit-learn: machine learning in Python. J Mach Learn Res 2011;12:2825–30. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Philip NS, Sweet LH, Tyrka AR, et al. Early life stress is associated with greater default network deactivation during working memory in healthy controls: a preliminary report. Brain Imaging Behav 2013;7:204–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Williams LM. Precision psychiatry: a neural circuit taxonomy for depression and anxiety. Lancet Psychiat 2016;3:472–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Barttfeld P, Abboud S, Lagercrantz H, et al. A lateral-to-mesial organization of human ventral visual cortex at birth. Brain Struct Funct 2018. [Epub ahead of print]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Kaczkurkin AN, Moore TM, Calkins ME, et al. Common and dissociable regional cerebral blood flow differences associate with dimensions of psychopathology across categorical diagnoses. Mol Psychiatry 2017;23:1981–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Yu T, Li Y, Fan F, et al. Decreased gray matter volume of cuneus and lingual gyrus in schizophrenia patients with tardive dyskinesia is associated with abnormal involuntary movement. Sci Rep-UK 2018;8:12884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Harricharan S, Rabellino D, Frewen PA, et al. fMRI functional connectivity of the periaqueductal gray in PTSD and its dissociative subtype. Brain Behav 2016;6:e00579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Walhovd KB, Fjell AM, Giedd J, et al. Through thick and thin: a need to reconcile contradictory results on trajectories in human cortical development. Cereb Cortex 2017;27:1472–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Morey R, Brown VM. Neural systems for cognitive and emotional processing in posttraumatic stress disorder. Front Psychol 2012;3:449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.McLaughlin KA, Conron KJ, Koenen KC, et al. Childhood adversity, adult stressful life events, and risk of past-year psychiatric disorder: a test of the stress sensitization hypothesis in a population-based sample of adults. Psychol Med 2010;40:1647–58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Lovallo WR, Farag NH, Sorocco KH, et al. Early life adversity contributes to impaired cognition and impulsive behavior: studies from the Oklahoma family health patterns project. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2013;37:616–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Blosnich JR, Dichter ME, Cerulli C, et al. Disparities in adverse childhood experiences among individuals with a history of military service. JAMA Psychiatry 2014;71:1041–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Cohen RA, Hitsman BL, Paul RH, et al. Early life stress and adult emotional experience: an international perspective. Int J Psychiat Med 2006;36:35–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Pechtel P, Pizzagalli DA. Effects of early life stress on cognitive and affective function: an integrated review of human literature. Psychopharmacology 2011;214:55–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Wang T, Wang K, Qu H, et al. Disorganized cortical thickness covariance network in major depressive disorder implicated by aberrant hubs in large-scale networks. Sci Rep-UK 2016;6:27964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Flory JD, Yehuda R. Comorbidity between post-traumatic stress disorder and major depressive disorder: alternative explanations and treatment considerations. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 2015;17:141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Stander VA, Thomsen CJ, Highfill-McRoy RM. Etiology of depression comorbidity in combat-related PTSD: a review of the literature. Clin Psychol Rev 2014;34:87–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Byllesby BM, Elhai JD, Tamburrino M, et al. General distress is more important than PTSD’s cognition and mood alterations factor in accounting for PTSD and depression’s comorbidity. J Affect Disorders 2017;211:118–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.King DW, Leskin GA, King LA, et al. Confirmatory factor analysis of the clinician-administered PTSD scale: Evidence for the dimensionality of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychol Assess 1998;10:90. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Savic D, Knezevic G, Damjanovic S, et al. Is there a biological difference between trauma-related depression and PTSD? DST says ‘NO’. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2012;37:1516–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Yehuda R, Halligan SL, Golier JA, et al. Effects of trauma exposure on the cortisol response to dexamethasone administration in PTSD and major depressive disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2004;29:389–404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Nunes A, Schnack HG, Ching CR, et al. Using structural MRI to identify bipolar disorders–13 site machine learning study in 3020 individuals from the ENIGMA Bipolar Disorders Working Group. Mol Psychiatr 2018. [Epub ahead of print]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Galatzer-Levy IR, Ruggles KV, Chen Z. Data science in the Research Domain Criteria era: relevance of machine learning to the study of stress pathology, recovery, and resilience. Chronic Stress (Thousand Oaks) 2018;2 [Epub ahead of print]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Thompson P Large-scale machine learning and neuroimaging in psychiatry. Biol Psychiat 2018;83:S51. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES