Skip to main content
Ecology and Evolution logoLink to Ecology and Evolution
. 2019 May 21;9(11):6501–6507. doi: 10.1002/ece3.5227

Sex ratio at mating does not modulate age fitness effects in Drosophila melanogaster

Zahida Sultanova 1,, Pau Carazo 1
PMCID: PMC6580286  PMID: 31236239

Abstract

Understanding the effects of male and female age on reproductive success is vital to explain the evolution of life history traits and sex‐specific aging. A general prediction is that pre‐/postmeiotic aging processes will lead to a decline in the pre‐ and postcopulatory abilities of both males and females. However, in as much the sexes have different strategies to optimize their fitness, the decline of reproductive success late in life can be modulated by social context, such as sex ratio, in a sex‐specific manner. In this study, we used Drosophila melanogaster to investigate whether sex ratio at mating modulates age effects on male and female reproductive success. As expected, male and female age caused a decrease in reproductive success across male‐biased and female‐biased social contexts but, contrary to previous findings, social context did not modulate age‐related fitness decline in either of the two sexes. We discuss these results in the light of how sex ratio might modulate pre‐/postcopulatory abilities and the opportunity for inter‐ and intrasexual competition in D. melanogaster, and generally suggest that social context effects on these processes are likely to be species specific.

Keywords: aging, fitness, reproductive success, sex ratio, social context

1. INTRODUCTION

Exploring the effects of male and female age on reproductive success is vital to understand life history evolution and sex‐specific aging (Bonduriansky, Maklakov, Zajitschek, & Brooks, 2008; Flatt & Heyland, 2011; Maklakov & Lummaa, 2013; Zajitschek, Bonduriansky, Zajitschek, & Brooks, 2009). Both male and female reproductive success is expected to decrease with age due to a functional decline in pre‐ and postcopulatory reproductive processes and/or in other phenotypic traits with indirect effects on reproductive success, such as exploratory activity, locomotion, perception, or immunity (Monaghan, Charmantier, Nussey, & Ricklefs, 2008; Nussey, Froy, Lemaitre, Gaillard, & Austad, 2013; Pizzari, Dean, Pacey, Moore, & Bonsall, 2008; Reznick, Bryant, Roff, Ghalambor, & Ghalambor, 2004). Many studies have explored how female age can reduce fertility and fecundity (David, Cohet, & Fouillet, 1975; Deng, 2012; Holmes, Thomson, Wu, & Ottinger, 2003), or how male age can decrease copulation success, fertilizing ability, and sperm competition (Service & Fales, 1994; Economos, Miquel, Binnard, & Kessler, 1979; Kühnert & Nieschlag, 2004). More recent studies have further investigated how female and male age can decrease offspring viability by focusing on underlying pre‐ and postmeiotic aging processes (Firman, Young, Rowe, Duong, & Gasparini, 2015; Pizzari et al., 2008; Tan, Pizzari, & Wigby, 2013). In short, the effects of age on reproductive success have been relatively well explored, yet most available studies have overlooked how socio‐sexual factors such as density or sex ratio might modulate reproductive senescence across the sexes (Brengdahl, Kimber, Maguire‐Baxter, Malacrinò, & Friberg, 2018; Carazo, Molina‐Vila, & Font, 2011; Fricke, Green, Mills, & Chapman, 2013; Ruhmann, Koppik, Wolfner, & Fricke, 2018; Tan et al., 2013; Zhao, Xuan, Li, & Xi, 2008).

The social context (e.g., sex ratio and density) can modulate age effects on reproductive success in a sex‐specific way by influencing different factors such as mate encounter rate, mate choice, or intrasexual competition (Kokko & Rankin, 2006; Kvarnemo & Ahnesjo, 1996). For example, in the feral fowl, a species with strong male–male competition over female harems, Dean et al. (2010) found that old males had the potential to sire a relatively higher proportion of offspring in groups with a female‐biased (FB) sex ratio, compared to a male‐biased (MB) sex ratio. This was due to old males having a higher chance of being socially dominant in FB groups, where male–male competition was low (Dean et al., 2010). Unfortunately, this interesting result has not been followed up by similar studies in other organisms with different mating systems, nor with respect to female age.

In this study, we used Drosophila melanogaster to explore how male and female age affects the reproductive success of males and females in experimental mating patches with FB or MB sex ratios. In D. melanogaster, males have strong intrasexual competition over mating and allocate considerable time and effort to court available females, while females are able to re‐mate with multiple males (Dow & Schilcher, 1975; Markow, 2002; Pitnick, 1991). Although intrasexual competition is mostly observed in males, aggression between females is also present, mainly when food sources are scarce (Bath et al., 2017; Bath, Morimoto, & Wigby, 2018; Ueda & Kidokoro, 2002). Furthermore, both males and females of this species exhibit mate choice but the degree and direction of these choices can differ depending on the population of origin and the social environment (Byrne & Rice, 2006; Edward & Chapman, 2012, 2013; Gowaty, Steinichen, & Anderson, 2003; Monier, Nöbel, Isabel, & Danchin, 2018). Based on its mating system, we predicted that sex ratio would modulate age effects on the fitness of males and females differently. In males, intrasexual competition is believed to be stronger than intersexual selection (Gowaty et al., 2003). Hence, we predicted that male age would decrease reproductive success relatively more in a MB social context, because we expected old males to have a higher disadvantage under intense male–male competition. In contrast, in this species female intrasexual competition appears to be less important than intersexual selection (Gowaty et al., 2003), so we did not predict a similar outcome. Instead, D. melanogaster males exhibit a marked preference for young females (Cook & Cook, 1975; Lüpold, Manier, Ala‐Honkola, Belote, & Pitnick, 2011), so we predicted female age to decrease reproductive success more in a FB social context, where there is a potentially higher opportunity for males to choose young females over the old ones.

2. METHODS

2.1. Experimental populations

We used D. melanogaster flies from a laboratory‐adapted, wild‐type Dahomey stock population derived from an original population founded in 1970 (Partridge & Farquhar, 1983). Our population is maintained with overlapping generations at 25°C, ~50%–60% humidity and a 12 hr:12 hr Light:Dark cycle, and fed with a diet that contains yeast (10 g/L), sugar (50 g/L), soy flour (10 g/L), corn flour (60 g/L), nipagin (3 g/L) and 0.05% propionic acid. To obtain experimental flies, we collected eggs from our population cages using grape‐agar filled petri dishes with a smear of live yeast paste, at standardized density (Clancy & Kennington, 2001). We collected virgin adults emerging from these eggs within 7 hr of eclosion and generated old focal males and females by isolating them with excess food for 28 days prior to assays, during which time we flipped them into a new vial once a week. In contrast, young focal males and females were only kept in isolation for 3 days after their emergence and prior to assays. Young sparkling (spa) competitors/partners were kept in same‐sex groups of 10 for 3 days after their emergence and until the beginning of assays. Using spa flies as competitors, which are homozygous for the recessive spa allele and exhibit a rough‐eye phenotype, allowed us to differentiate between the offspring of wt and spa parents in competitive fitness assays (e.g., Fricke, Wigby, Hobbs, & Chapman, 2009).

2.2. Competitive fitness assays

In order to explore the effect of sex ratio and age on reproductive success, we studied the fitness of focal wt male and female flies when competing against spa rivals, in a factorial combination of sex ratio (i.e., MB—four males and two females—vs. FB—four females and two males–) and age (i.e., a young vs. old focal wt male/female competing against young spa rivals for young spa mating partners). Thus, within each vial, all flies except the focal experimental fly were spa (Figure 1). For all treatments, we allowed flies to interact and lay eggs for 2 days, after which time we discarded the males and allowed females to oviposit for three more days in a fresh vial. In order to control for larval density across treatments during this second period of oviposition, we separated the four females in FB sex ratio treatments in two vials containing two females each. After transferring/discarding females, we incubated vials from both the first and second period of oviposition for 16 days, froze the vials, and then proceeded to count the number of spa and wt offspring in each vial. In order to control for the potential effects of density on the development of larvae from eggs laid during the first oviposition period, we counted the number of pupae in these vials. The density of larvae per vial (number of pupae ± SEM = 44.7 ± 1.0) was, in all cases, comfortably below the threshold for which density effects have been described in D. melanogaster (Miller & Thomas, 1958)).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Scheme of the different treatments implemented to measure the reproductive success of young (y) and old (o) focal flies across different sex ratios

2.3. Statistical analysis

To determine the effect of aging and sex ratio on reproductive success in a way that is comparable across the two different sex ratio treatments (i.e., fixed density but which include a different amount of males and females), we first standardized data. We calculated the standardized reproductive success of each focal female by subtracting the average number of offspring that belong to competitors (spa) from the observed number of offspring that belong to the focal fly (wt) for each replicate:

#ofoffspringfromfocal-#ofoffspringfromcompetitors#ofcompetitors

We calculated the standardized reproductive success of each focal male using the same equation, but divided by the number of females that were present in the corresponding mating vial (two females in MB social context and four females in FB social context).

After standardization, we checked for heteroscedasticity and normality via graphical exploration, and subsequently validated all models. To explore the effect of age and sex ratio on reproductive success of each sex separately, we fitted a linear model including age, sex ratio, and their interactions as fixed factors. We then repeated this analysis using a restricted maximum likelihood LMMs (Linear Mixed Models) approach and introducing pupae density as a random intercept effect. In order to obtain minimum adequate models, we performed backward stepwise model selection based on Likelihood Ratio Tests (LRTs). All analyses were performed in R v. 3.3.2 (R Core Team, 2016).

3. RESULTS

When we did not control for pupae density, we did not find a significant age × sex ratio interaction (F 1,88 = 0.1027, p = 0.7494) or sex ratio effect (F 1,89 = 2.1731, p = 0.144) in male reproductive success. However, we found a significant main effect of age (F 1,89 = 19.2600, p < 0.001, Figure 2a). Similarly, in the case of female reproductive success, we did not find a significant age × sex ratio interaction (F 1,88 = 0.0967, p = 0.7566) or sex ratio effect (F 1,89 = 0.8208, p = 0.3674) but we did find a significant age effect (F 1,89 = 64.1757, p < 0.001, Figure 2b).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Standard reproductive success of (a) young/old focal males in male‐biased and female‐biased social contexts and (b) young/old focal females in male‐biased and female‐biased social contexts

Controlling for pupae density did not qualitatively change our results. For males, there was no significant age × sex ratio interaction (χ2 = 0.1073, df = 1, p = 0.7433) or sex ratio effect (F 1,89 = 2.1731, p = 0.144); whereas, we did find a significant age effect (F 1,89 = 18.9707, p < 0.001) on male reproductive success. Similarly, for females, we did not find a significant age × sex ratio interaction (χ2 = 0.101, df = 1, p = 0.7506) or sex ratio effect (F 1,89 = 0.8140, p = 0.3674). However, we found a significant main effect for age (F 1,89 = 64.8510, p < 0.001).

4. DISCUSSION

In this study, we investigated the potential role that sex ratio at mating might play in modulating the fitness effects of age in D. melanogaster males and females. We found that both male and female age caused a decline in reproductive success but, contrary to our expectations, this effect was not modulated by sex ratio at mating (i.e., was similar in both MB and FB social context).

In the case of males, we were expecting male age to decrease the reproductive success more in a MB social context, where intrasexual competition is high. Like in many other organisms, in D. melanogaster male–male competition is expected to increase drastically in MB social contexts (Wang & Anderson, 2010). In principle, this should lead to old males having relatively higher reproductive fitness in FB contexts, where male–male competition is low. Accordingly, Dean et al. (2010) showed that, in the feral fowl (Gallus gallus), the effects of age on the reproductive success of males were mitigated in FB (vs. MB) contexts. In this species, socially dominant males have privileged access to mating opportunities but females mate multiply, so sperm competition is intense (David Ligon & Zwartjes, 1995; Dean et al., 2010; Pizzari & Birkhead, 2000; Pizzari, Froman, & Birkhead, 2002). Dean et al. (2010) elegantly showed that despite old males having a lower sperm competition ability than young males, they had a relative advantage in FB (vs. MB) social groups, due to a higher possibility of being socially dominant when male–male intrasexual competition is low. Our failure to find similar effects in D. melanogaster may have to do with inherent differences in the mating system of these two species.

In fruit flies, male–male competition over access to females is generally high and, in the wild, males seem to exhibit a typical resource‐defense polygyny by defending pieces of decaying fruit where females feed (Markow, 1988). Recent evidence suggests that male–male aggression in this context also serves a mate‐guarding function (Baxter, Barnett, & Dukas, 2015), but flies do not live in stable social groups and hence males cannot monopolize access to females throughout their lifespan. Furthermore, laboratory populations like the one used in this study have been kept at high densities for thousands of generations, in conditions where mate monopolization is highly unlikely. As a result, the level of intrasexual competition in this species might not modulate age‐related fitness effects as it does in feral fowls (or might do so to a lesser extent). On the other hand, intersexual competition also seems to be quite important in D. melanogaster, and there is good evidence that both female and male mate choice are modulated by social context (Edward & Chapman, 2012, 2013; Monier et al., 2018). In particular, females prefer mating with young (or large) males that court more vigorously (Jagadeeshan, Shah, Chakrabarti, & Singh, 2015; Rezaei, Krishna, & Santhosh, 2015), and they appear to be less choosy when sex ratios are FB (Monier et al., 2018). Hence, old males might be expected to benefit in FB contexts due to females being less choosy in favor of young males. This makes it more striking that we did not find sex ratio to modulate age effects on male reproductive success.

It is possible that our results for males are partly explained by male mate choice effects. Under FB sex ratios, where males are expected to be choosier, young males may benefit by choosing high‐quality females while old males are left to mate with low‐quality females. In D. melanogaster, aging seems to diminish the ability of males to choose high‐quality females (Hu, Han, Wang, & Xue, 2014), so that old males may fail to be choosy despite ample opportunity for male mate choice in FB contexts, to the benefit of young “choosy” males. An intriguing possibility is that mate choice copying (Nöbel, Danchin, & Isabel, 2018) may have contributed to exacerbate male age effects in the FB sex ratio. In D. melanogaster, females prefer mating with young males that court more vigorously (Jagadeeshan et al., 2015; Rezaei et al., 2015) and, in our experiment, old and young focal males were always phenotypically distinguishable to their young rival sparkling flies (i.e. different eye‐color). Given recent findings showing that females tend to copy the mate choice of other females based on male color cues in fruit flies (Danchin et al., 2018), it is possible that the inherent advantage of young males over old males due to female choice may have been exacerbated in the FB context, where mate choice copying is more likely. In short, young males may hold a similar fitness advantage against old males irrespective of the sex ratio, but via different sexual selection mechanisms: via intrasexual competition and female mate choice, when the sex ratio is MB, and via male mate choice and female mate copying when the sex ratio is FB.

In the case of females, female reproductive success also decreased with age similarly in both FB and MB social contexts (Figure 2b). We might have expected that in a FB social context with a higher opportunity for males to be choosy, males (which are all young in this case) would prefer to mate with young (vs. old) focal females, which would thus have higher reproductive success. Several previous studies have reported the existence of both pre and postcopulatory male mate choice with respect to female age. For example, male courtship intensity decreases with female age (Cook & Cook, 1975) and males allocate less sperm to old females compared to their young counterparts (Lüpold et al., 2011). However, being attractive to males is not always beneficial for females. Mating and male harassment are known to decrease survival and reproductive success in female D. melanogaster (Chapman & Partridge, 1996; Partridge & Fowler, 1990; Partridge, Green, & Fowler, 1987; Wigby & Chapman, 2005). The fact that male preference for young females may have been more marked in the FB social context could have led males to be more harmful to these females, which in turn may have counterbalanced any benefits from male mate choice. As a matter of fact, Long, Pischedda, Stewart, and Rice (2009) showed that male harm is preferentially directed toward intrinsically higher‐fitness females and that, as a result, any fitness advantage that could be experienced by high condition females (young females in our design) might be compensated by the costs of being attractive in a FB social context (at least in simple environments such as the one used in this experiment; see Yun, Chen, Singh, Agrawal, & Rundle, 2017; MacPherson, Yun, Barrera, Agrawal, & Rundle, 2018). Similarly, relatively high mating costs in a MB social context might also contribute to explain why we did not observe a sex ratio × female age interaction. Although the opportunity for male mate choice is lower in this context and males might thus harm both young and old females, mating costs may be expected to be more pronounced in old females, which would tend to exacerbate age effects in a MB social context. Unfortunately, we currently have very little information about how social context changes intra‐ versus intersexual competition in males and females, in D. melanogaster or other species, which means the above possibilities remain to be explored.

Studies of reproductive senescence so far have focused on understanding the effect of male and female age on reproductive success (Flatt & Heyland, 2011; Williams, 1957), for example by studying male/female age effects on pre–post copulatory mating abilities, mate choice, and offspring viability (Carazo et al., 2011; Cook & Cook, 1975; Dunson, Baird, & Colombo, 2004; Lüpold et al., 2011; Maklakov et al., 2009; Tan et al., 2013; Velando, Noguera, Drummond, & Torres, 2011). Many studies have also investigated the interaction between social context and several fitness traits such as mating duration, reproductive success, survival, and lifespan (Adler & Bonduriansky, 2011; Bretman, Fricke, Hetherington, Stone, & Chapman, 2010; Bretman, Westmancoat, Gage, & Chapman, 2013; Costa, Mateus, Moura, & Machado, 2010; Iliadi, Iliadi, & Boulianne, 2009; Leech, Sait, & Bretman, 2017; Zajitschek, Zajitschek, Friberg, & Maklakov, 2013). In sharp contrast, how age effects on reproductive success may be modulated by the social context has so far been largely overlooked even though social context (such as sex ratio at mating) might play a crucial role in modulating sex‐specific age effects on reproductive success. We suggest future studies should aim to fill this gap in knowledge.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

None declared.

AUTHORS CONTRIBUTION

ZS and PC conceived and designed the study; ZS carried out the experiment, analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript with the revisions of PC; ZS and PC revised and approved the final form of the manuscript.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We would like to thank Miriam Ibañez Solaz and Roberto García‐Roa for kindly helping us during experiments. PC was supported by a Ramón y Cajal Fellowship (RYC‐2013‐12998) and a Plan Nacional I+D+i Excelencia Research Grant (CGL2014‐58722‐P). ZS was supported by the “Atracció de Talent” grant (Universitat de València).

Sultanova Z, Carazo P. Sex ratio at mating does not modulate age fitness effects in Drosophila melanogaster . Ecol Evol. 2019;9:6501–6507. 10.1002/ece3.5227

DATA ACCESSIBILITY

Data is archived in the DRYAD data repository, the doi for our data is https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.s67v7b1.

REFERENCES

  1. Adler, M. I. , & Bonduriansky, R. (2011). The dissimilar costs of love and war: Age‐specific mortality as a function of the operational sex ratio. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 24(6), 1169–1177. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2011.02250.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bath, E. , Bowden, S. , Peters, C. , Reddy, A. , Tobias, J. A. , Easton‐Calabria, E. , … Wigby, S. (2017). Sperm and sex peptide stimulate aggression in female Drosophila . Nature Ecology and Evolution, 1, 0154 10.1038/s41559-017-0154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bath, E. , Morimoto, J. , & Wigby, S. (2018). The developmental environment modulates mating‐induced aggression and fighting success in adult female Drosophila . Functional Ecology, 32, 2542–2552. 10.1111/1365-2435.13214 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baxter, C. M. , Barnett, R. , & Dukas, R. (2015). Aggression, mate guarding and fitness in male fruit flies. Animal Behaviour, 109, 235–241. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.08.023 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bonduriansky, R. , Maklakov, A. , Zajitschek, F. , & Brooks, R. (2008). Sexual selection, sexual conflict and the evolution of ageing and life span. Functional Ecology, 22, 443–453. 10.1111/j.1365-2435.2008.01417.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brengdahl, M. , Kimber, C. M. , Maguire‐Baxter, J. , Malacrinò, A. , & Friberg, U. (2018). Genetic quality affects the rate of male and female reproductive aging differently in Drosophila melanogaster . The American Naturalist, 192, 761–772. 10.1086/700117 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bretman, A. , Fricke, C. , Hetherington, P. , Stone, R. , & Chapman, T. (2010). Exposure to rivals and plastic responses to sperm competition in Drosophila melanogaster . Behavioral Ecology, 21(2), 317–321. 10.1093/beheco/arp189 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bretman, A. , Westmancoat, J. D. , Gage, M. J. , & Chapman, T. (2013). Costs and benefits of lifetime exposure to mating rivals in male Drosophila melanogaster . Evolution, 67(8), 2413–2422. 10.1111/evo.12125 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Byrne, P. G. , & Rice, W. R. (2006). Evidence for adaptive male mate choice in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster . Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 273, 917–922. 10.1098/rspb.2005.3372 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Carazo, P. , Molina‐Vila, P. , & Font, E. (2011). Male reproductive senescence as a potential source of sexual conflict in a beetle. Behavioral Ecology, 22, 192–198. 10.1093/beheco/arq187 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chapman, T. , & Partridge, L. (1996). Female fitness in Drosophila melanogaster: An interaction between the effect of nutrition and of encounter rate with males. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 263(1371), 755–759. 10.1098/rspb.1996.0113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Clancy, D. J. , & Kennington, W. (2001). A simple method to achieve consistent larval density in bottle cultures. Drosophila Information Services, 84, 168–169. 10.1126/science.aat1590 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cook, R. , & Cook, A. (1975). The attractiveness to males of female Drosophila melanogaster: Effects of mating, age and diet. Animal Behaviour, 23, 521–526. 10.1016/0003-3472(75)90129-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Costa, M. , Mateus, R. P. , Moura, M. O. , & Machado, L. P. D. B. (2010). Adult sex ratio effects on male survivorship of Drosophila melanogaster Meigen (Diptera, Drosophilidae). Revista Brasileira de Entomologia, 54(3), 446–449. 10.1590/S0085-56262010000300015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Danchin, E. , Nöbel, S. , Pocheville, A. , Dagaeff, A. C. , Demay, L. , Alphand, M. , … Allain, M. (2018). Cultural flies: Conformist social learning in fruitflies predicts long‐lasting mate‐choice traditions. Science, 362, 1025–1030. 10.1126/science.aat1590 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. David, J. , Cohet, Y. , & Fouillet, P. (1975). The variability between individuals as a measure of senescence: A study of the number of eggs laid and the percentage of hatched eggs in the case of Drosophila melanogaster . Experimental Gerontology, 10, 17–25. 10.1016/0531-5565(75)90011-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. David Ligon, J. , & Zwartjes, P. W. (1995). Female red junglefowl choose to mate with multiple males. Animal Behaviour, 49, 127–135. 10.1016/0003-3472(95)80160-X [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dean, R. , Cornwallis, C. K. , Løvlie, H. , Worley, K. , Richardson, D. S. , & Pizzari, T. (2010). Male reproductive senescence causes potential for sexual conflict over mating. Current Biology, 20, 1192–1196. 10.1016/j.cub.2010.04.059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Deng, M. (2012). Mechanisms of reproductive aging in the females. Science, China Life Sciences, 55, 653–658. 10.1007/s11427-012-4351-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dow, M. A. , & Schilcher, F. V. (1975). Aggression and mating success in Drosophila melanogaster . Nature, 254, 511–512. 10.1038/254511a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dunson, D. B. , Baird, D. D. , & Colombo, B. (2004). Increased infertility with age in men and women. Obstetrics and Gynecology, 103, 51–56. 10.1097/01.AOG.0000100153.24061.45 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Economos, A. C. , Miquel, J. , Binnard, R. , & Kessler, S. (1979). Quantitative analysis of mating behavior in aging male Drosophila melanogaster . Mechanisms of Ageing and Development, 10, 233–240. 10.1016/0047-6374(79)90037-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Edward, D. A. , & Chapman, T. (2012). Measuring the fitness benefits of male mate choice in Drosophila melanogaster . Evolution, 66, 2646–2653. 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2012.01648.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Edward, D. A. , & Chapman, T. (2013). Variation in male mate choice in Drosophila melanogaster . PLoS ONE, 8, e56299 10.1371/journal.pone.0056299 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Firman, R. C. , Young, F. J. , Rowe, D. C. , Duong, H. T. , & Gasparini, C. (2015). Sexual rest and post‐meiotic sperm ageing in house mice. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 28, 1373–1382. 10.1111/jeb.12661 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Flatt, T. , & Heyland, A. (2011). Mechanisms of life history evolution: The genetics and physiology of life history traits and trade‐offs. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  27. Fricke, C. , Green, D. , Mills, W. E. , & Chapman, T. (2013). Age‐dependent female responses to a male ejaculate signal alter demographic opportunities for selection. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 280, 20130428 10.1098/rspb.2013.0428 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fricke, C. , Wigby, S. , Hobbs, R. , & Chapman, T. (2009). The benefits of male ejaculate sex peptide transfer in Drosophila melanogaster . Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 22, 275–286. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01638.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Gowaty, P. A. , Steinichen, R. , & Anderson, W. W. (2003). Indiscriminate females and choosy males: Within‐ and between‐species variation in Drosophila . Evolution, 57, 2037–2045. 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2003.tb00383.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Holmes, D. J. , Thomson, S. L. , Wu, J. , & Ottinger, M. A. (2003). Reproductive aging in female birds. Experimental Gerontology, 38, 751–756. 10.1016/S0531-5565(03)00103-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hu, Y. , Han, Y. , Wang, X. , & Xue, L. (2014). Aging‐related neurodegeneration eliminates male courtship choice in Drosophila . Neurobiology of Aging, 35, 2174–2178. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.02.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Iliadi, K. G. , Iliadi, N. N. , & Boulianne, G. L. (2009). Regulation of Drosophila life‐span: Effect of genetic background, sex, mating and social status. Experimental Gerontology, 44(8), 546–553. 10.1016/j.exger.2009.05.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jagadeeshan, S. , Shah, U. , Chakrabarti, D. , & Singh, R. S. (2015). Female choice or male sex drive? The advantages of male body size during mating in Drosophila melanogaster . PLoS ONE, 10, e0144672 10.1371/journal.pone.0144672 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kokko, H. , & Rankin, D. J. (2006). Lonely hearts or sex in the city? Density‐dependent effects in mating systems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 361, 319–334. 10.1098/rstb.2005.1784 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kühnert, B. , & Nieschlag, E. (2004). Reproductive functions of the ageing male. Human Reproduction Update, 10, 327–339. 10.1093/humupd/dmh030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kvarnemo, C. , & Ahnesjo, I. (1996). The dynamics of operational sex ratios and competition for mates. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 11, 404–408. 10.1016/0169-5347(96)10056-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Leech, T. , Sait, S. M. , & Bretman, A. (2017). Sex‐specific effects of social isolation on ageing in Drosophila melanogaster . Journal of Insect Physiology, 102, 12–17. 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2017.08.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Long, T. A. , Pischedda, A. , Stewart, A. D. , & Rice, W. R. (2009). A cost of sexual attractiveness to high‐fitness females. PLoS Biology, 7(12), e1000254 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000254 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lüpold, S. , Manier, M. K. , Ala‐Honkola, O. , Belote, J. M. , & Pitnick, S. (2011). Male Drosophila melanogaster adjust ejaculate size based on female mating status, fecundity, and age. Behavioral Ecology, 22, 185–191. 10.1093/beheco/arq193 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. MacPherson, A. , Yun, L. , Barrera, T. S. , Agrawal, A. F. , & Rundle, H. D. (2018). The effects of male harm vary with female quality and environmental complexity in Drosophila melanogaster . Biology Letters, 14(8), 20180443 10.1098/rsbl.2018.0443 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Maklakov, A. A. , Hall, M. D. , Simpson, S. J. , Dessmann, J. , Clissold, F. J. , Zajitschek, F. , … Brooks, R. C. (2009). Sex differences in nutrient‐dependent reproductive ageing. Aging Cell, 8, 324–330. 10.1111/j.1474-9726.2009.00479.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Maklakov, A. A. , & Lummaa, V. (2013). Evolution of sex differences in lifespan and aging: Causes and constraints. BioEssays, 35, 717–724. 10.1002/bies.201300021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Markow, T. A. (1988). Reproductive behavior of Drosophila melanogaster and D. nigrospiracula in the field and in the laboratory. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 102, 169–173. 10.1037/0735-7036.102.2.169 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Markow, T. A. (2002). Perspective: Female remating, operational sex ratio, and the arena of sexual selection in Drosophila species. Evolution, 56, 1725–1734. 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2002.tb00186.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Miller, R. S. , & Thomas, J. L. (1958). The effects of larval crowding and body size on the longevity of adult Drosophila melanogaster . Ecology, 39, 118–125. 10.2307/1929973 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Monaghan, P. , Charmantier, A. , Nussey, D. H. , & Ricklefs, R. E. (2008). The evolutionary ecology of senescence. Functional Ecology, 22, 371–378. 10.1111/j.1365-2435.2008.01418.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. Monier, M. , Nöbel, S. , Isabel, G. , & Danchin, E. (2018). Effects of a sex ratio gradient on female mate‐copying and choosiness in Drosophila melanogaster . Current Zoology, 64, 251–258. 10.1093/cz/zoy014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Nöbel, S. , Danchin, E. , & Isabel, G. (2018). Mate‐copying for a costly variant in Drosophila melanogaster females. Behavioral Ecology, 29, 1150–1156. 10.1093/beheco/ary095 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  49. Nussey, D. H. , Froy, H. , Lemaitre, J. F. , Gaillard, J. M. , & Austad, S. N. (2013). Senescence in natural populations of animals: Widespread evidence and its implications for bio‐gerontology. Ageing Research Reviews, 12, 214–225. 10.1016/j.arr.2012.07.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Partridge, L. , & Farquhar, M. (1983). Lifetime mating success of male fruitflies (Drosophila melanogaster) is related to their size. Animal Behaviour, 31, 871–877. 10.1016/S0003-3472(83)80242-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Partridge, L. , & Fowler, K. (1990). Non‐mating costs of exposure to males in female Drosophila melanogaster . Journal of Insect Physiology, 36(6), 419–425. 10.1016/0022-1910(90)90059-O [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Partridge, L. , Green, A. , & Fowler, K. (1987). Effects of egg‐production and of exposure to males on female survival in Drosophila melanogaster . Journal of Insect Physiology, 33(10), 745–749. 10.1016/0022-1910(87)90060-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Pitnick, S. (1991). Male size influences mate fecundity and remating interval in Drosophila melanogaster . Animal Behaviour, 41, 735–745. 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80340-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Pizzari, T. , & Birkhead, T. R. (2000). Female feral fowl eject sperm of subdominant males. Nature, 405, 787–789. 10.1038/35015558 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Pizzari, T. , Dean, R. , Pacey, A. , Moore, H. , & Bonsall, M. B. (2008). The evolutionary ecology of pre‐ and post‐meiotic sperm senescence. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 23, 131–140. 10.1016/J.TREE.2007.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Pizzari, T. , Froman, D. P. , & Birkhead, T. R. (2002). Pre‐ and post‐insemination episodes of sexual selection in the fowl, Gallus g. domesticus . Heredity, 112–116, 10.1038/sj.hdy.6800014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. R Core Team (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; http://www.R-project.org/ [Google Scholar]
  58. Rezaei, A. , Krishna, M. S. , & Santhosh, H. T. (2015). Male age affects female mate preference, quantity of accessory gland proteins, and sperm traits and female fitness in D. melanogaster . Zoological Science, 32, 16–24. 10.2108/zs140121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Reznick, D. N. , Bryant, M. J. , Roff, D. , Ghalambor, C. K. , & Ghalambor, D. E. (2004). Effect of extrinsic mortality on the evolution of senescence in guppies. Nature, 431, 1095–1099. 10.1038/nature02936 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Ruhmann, H., Koppik, M., Wolfner, M. F., & Fricke, C. (2018). The impact of ageing on male reproductive success in Drosophila melanogaster . Experimental Gerontology, 103, 6501–10. 10.1016/j.exger.2017.12.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Service, P. M. , & Fales, A. J. (1994). Evolution of delayed reproductive senescence in male fruit flies: Sperm competition. Genetics and Evolution of Aging, 3, 130–144. 10.1007/BF01435992 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Tan, C. K. W. , Pizzari, T. , & Wigby, S. (2013). Parental age, gametic age, and inbreeding interact to modulate offspring viability in Drosophila melanogaster . Evolution, 67, 3043–3051. 10.1111/evo.12131 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Ueda, A. , & Kidokoro, Y. (2002). Aggressive behaviours of female Drosophila melanogaster are influenced by their social experience and food resources. Physiological Entomology, 27, 21–28. 10.1046/j.1365-3032.2002.00262.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  64. Velando, A. , Noguera, J. C. , Drummond, H. , & Torres, R. (2011). Senescent males carry premutagenic lesions in sperm. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 24, 693–697. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2010.02201.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Wang, L. , & Anderson, D. J. (2010). Identification of an aggression‐promoting pheromone and its receptor neurons in Drosophila . Nature, 463, 227–231. 10.1038/nature08678 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Wigby, S. , & Chapman, T. (2005). Sex peptide causes mating costs in female Drosophila melanogaster . Current Biology, 15(4), 316–321. 10.1016/j.cub.2005.01.051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Williams, G. C. (1957). Pleiotropy, natural selection, and the evolution of senescence. Evolution, 11, 398–411. 10.2307/2406060 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  68. Yun, L. , Chen, P. J. , Singh, A. , Agrawal, A. F. , & Rundle, H. D. (2017). The physical environment mediates male harm and its effect on selection in females. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 284(1858), 20170424 10.1098/rspb.2017.0424 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Zajitschek, F. , Bonduriansky, R. , Zajitschek, S. R. K. , & Brooks, R. C. (2009). Sexual dimorphism in life history: Age, survival, and reproduction in male and female field crickets Teleogryllus commodus under seminatural conditions. The American Naturalist, 173, 792–802. 10.1086/598486 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Zajitschek, F. , Zajitschek, S. R. , Friberg, U. , & Maklakov, A. A. (2013). Interactive effects of sex, social environment, dietary restriction, and methionine on survival and reproduction in fruit flies. Age, 35(4), 1193–1204. 10.1007/s11357-012-9445-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Zhao, R. , Xuan, Y. , Li, X. , & Xi, R. (2008). Age‐related changes of germline stem cell activity, niche signaling activity and egg production in Drosophila . Aging Cell, 7, 344–354. 10.1111/j.1474-9726.2008.00379.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

Data is archived in the DRYAD data repository, the doi for our data is https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.s67v7b1.


Articles from Ecology and Evolution are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES