Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: Med Hypotheses. 2019 Apr 29;128:13–16. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2019.04.020

FRONTOLIMBIC AFFECTIVE BIAS AND FALSE NARRATIVES FROM BRAIN DISEASE

Mario F Mendez 1
PMCID: PMC6581201  NIHMSID: NIHMS1528740  PMID: 31203901

Abstract

Since the nineteenth century, clinicians and investigators have systematically evaluated the origin of delusions and psychotic thinking. One major clue to understanding the neurobiological underpinnings of delusions is the emergence of false narratives from brain disease. In addition to delusions themselves, there are a range of other false narratives not due to deliberate lying and resulting from neurological disorders, including provoked confabulations, fantastic confabulations, false memories, magical thinking, dream delusions, and “fantastic thinking”. A comparison of their characteristics, similarities, and differences suggest a hypothesis: despite different sources for their false narrative experiences, such as unusual thoughts or perceptions, all false narratives from brain disease involve erroneous or mismatched “affective biases” applied to the experiences. Affective labels usually signal the sense of rightness, sense of familiarity, and the external vs. internal origin of an experience, and they can be altered by limbic neuropathology. The location and involvement of neuropathology that facilitates false narratives involves frontolimbic regions and their connections, particularly on the right. Future investigations can focus on frontolimbic mechanisms involved in the provision of the intrinsically-linked affective biases, which indicate the nature and external/internal origin of experiences.

INTRODUCTION

Delusions are a primary symptom of psychosis. There are many classifications of delusions, but they all share the concept of false narratives that do not correspond to real events of personal history and are not deliberate deceptions. Despite 130 years or more of discussion as to the nature of delusions (1), clinicians and investigators do not understand their mechanism and origin in the brain. Current knowledge of the different types of false narratives from neurological disorders may clarify the origin of delusions, and, thereby, shed light on the nature of psychosis. A broad characterization of false narratives reported from brain disorders include delusions, provoked or fantastic confabulations, false memories, magical thinking, dream delusions, magical thinking, and even the phenomena of “fantastic thinking” (2). A comparison of the similarities and differences of these different false narratives, and the neuropathological localization leading to these reports, suggests a hypothetical common mechanism underlying delusions and, perhaps, psychosis in general.

HYPOTHESIS/THEORY

False narratives result from a mismatch, or absence, of the appropriate, frontally-mediated affective bias as applied to different sources of experience. Although not entirely new (3), compared to other novel formulations (4), this hypothesis gives priority to the altered feeling states that motivate erroneous interpretations, rather than to alterations in the content of the experiences themselves. Brain disorders alter the affective labels for experiences such as cognitive/analytic (thoughts), perceptual, imaginative events, or abnormally reconstructed memories. “Affective bias” is used here as broadly representing values, attitudes, or judgments prompted by automatic feelings (5, 6), which are often unconscious and unrecognized, that underlie experiences, such as the feelings of familiarity or strangeness that accompany perceptions, memories, or internal vs. external events. Linked intrinsic feelings are fundamentally frontolimbic, originating in mesial frontal regions, including ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), frontal-insular cortex (FIC), and their frontal-subcortical connections. These neuroanatomical areas participate in an unconscious evaluation that labels thoughts, perceptions, or other experiences (7). Ultimately, this hypothesis, of an erroneous affective bias for experiences, could lead to a new conceptualization on the management of delusions and greater emphasis on targeting “emotional” dysfunction.

EVIDENCE FROM BRAIN DISEASE

This hypothesis is informed from recurrent reports of the type of neurological patients and lesions that result in delusions and other false narratives (2, 8). A delusion is a fixed belief that is false but firmly held with absolute conviction and despite all evidence to the contrary (9). Delusional beliefs are unshakable and often implausible to others; a perverted view of reality that departs from consensual reality (10). The content of delusions vary from the persecutory to the grandiose, but all seem to involve the misinterpretation of an experience, either internally generated or externally perceived, as having an altered emotional meaning. For example, content specific delusion of Capgras, De Clérambault, or Cotard syndromes indicate disturbed feelings of person familiarity and trust, false beliefs of requited affection, or personal feelings of degeneration or dying, respectively. Two neurological conditions are illustrative. After schizophrenia, Alzheimer’s disease (AD) may be the second most common causes of delusions. Content specific delusions based on impaired memory, such as the delusion of theft or of the “phantom boarder syndrome” are common in AD as the disorder spreads to the right frontal lobe and couples feelings of apprehension and distrust with the perception of lost or moved items (11). In addition, an interictal psychosis may occur among epileptics who have frequent and long-standing temporal lobe epilepsy with frequent epileptic activity in limbic structures (12). Temporal limbic areas are intimately connecting with mesiofrontal areas in a broader limbic region. A further clue is the epileptic emergence of déjà vu, jamais vu, déjà vecu, and other abnormal feelings of familiarity associated with experiences (13).

In contrast to delusions, confabulation are false recollections that create a “coherent self- narrative” of oneself in the presence of amnesia or in the context of an imagined experience (14). Although confabulations have similarities to delusions, they are usually quite distinct (15, 16) (See Table 1). Simple provoked or “momentary” confabulations are memory-related misstatements resulting from attempts to fill-in memory gaps often in response to questions (8, 15, 17). They occur in amnestic disorders such as Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome, herpes or limbic encephalitis, ruptured communication artery aneurysms especially anterior, strategic diencephalic strokes, traumatic brain injury, nicotinic acid deficiency, multiple sclerosis, hypoxic-ischemic injury such as from attempted hanging, normal pressure hydrocephalus, frontotemporal dementia (FTD), and focal frontal lesions (8, 18). These confabulations differ from delusions in the usual involvement of memory impairment, a fleeting or variable nature, and in the absence of systematized beliefs held with certainty and against evidence to the contrary (19, 20). They are motivated by a momentary familiarity for past overtly similar events misapplied to explain current non-remembered experiences.

TABLE 1:

Characteristics of False Narratives

DELUSIONS PROVOKED CONFABULATION FANTASTIC CONFABULATION FALSE MEMORY MAGICAL THINKING DREAM DELUSION FANTASTIC THINKING
Certainty or absolute conviction of an external world origin + + + + +

Incorrigibility against all evidence + ± + + + ±

Implausibility or highly improbable ± + + ± +

Factor-Dependent. eg provocations, REM breakthrough + +

Persistent Ongoing vs past memory deficit + + + ± ±

Short-term Consistency + + + +

Multiple False Narratives possible ± + + ± ± +

Frontal Dysfunction often present + + + + + + +

In addition to provoked confabulations, there are spontaneous confabulations, which are often “fantastic” and can be difficult to distinguish from delusions, suggesting overlap between these two types of false narratives (21). Spontaneous, “fantastic” confabulations are rarer grandiose or impossible statements that are internally-generated but persist as externally true despite a wavering of certainty when challenged (2225). They tend to be pleasant and self-enhancing and can be associated with a wish fulfillment, vivid imagination or daydreams, and proneness to fantasy and stories or embellishment (2, 22, 23). These patients cannot emotionally label their internal daydreams with the appropriate feelings of doubt as to its internal source and have an attenuation of the intuitive “feeling of rightness” that something is true (15, 26, 27). Fantastic confabulations are usually associated with significant VMPFC and other prefrontal pathology (23, 2628). Spontaneous confabulations have some important similarities with delusions (15, 16, 29). In FTD, the occurrence of both delusions and fantastic confabulations emphasizes the spectrum of false narratives from frontal lobe disturbances in this disorder, specifically erroneous affective bias indicating doubt of external truth for thoughts or internal daydreams (7).

Both forms of confabulations are internally-generated external-world attributions and associated frontal-executive deficits (21,23, 30). Among patients with either form of confabulations, there is a bias towards identifying imagined and often wish fulfilling events as occurring externally (23, 26, 31), resulting in inability to tell real memories or events from internally-generated thoughts (15, 23, 27). These observations seem to indicate a disturbed affective bias that provides an intuitive, automatic, and unconscious “feeling of rightness” for experiences (7). Some studies show a positive bias for false recognition among those with confabulations (32). Affective bias regarding an experience or the absence of uncertainty regarding an experience, may be incorrectly linked to it, resulting in a false narrative (7, 21).

False memories are another form of false narratives that can occur from brain disease (33). In 1886, Emil Kraepelin described “pseudoreminiscences”, or hallucinations of memory, that arise spontaneously in the mind and appear as false narratives (1, 34). False memories overlap with confabulations and may occur in the same patients. Memory is modified with each reconsolidation so that it can lead to the retrieval of distorted information (35, 36). Memory is a process or active modification and reconstruction with each retrieval, and parts can be erroneously retrieved or be illusory if there is an affective bias in the reconstruction or reconsolidation(35), including from free-floating or irrelevant memory fragments and fuzzy-traces with dilapidation of details (36, 37), or an abnormal attribution of internal vs. external source or memory trace (27). Memories may be biased because of emotionally-facilitated or “free-floating” memory fragments or an attention to a “verbatim” level of detail while retaining the basic “gist” of memory (37). Studies show a systematic affective bias for more pleasant places when there are false beliefs about place (38). Other studies implicate the prefrontal grey matter, particularly VMPFC, with false recollections and recognition (3941), as well as the occurrence of frontal-executive dysfunction with disturbed organization for retrieval and ordering of retrieved memories (15, 42).

Four other rarer false narrative types occur in brain disease. First, magical thinking refers to erroneous beliefs of a relationship between one’s thoughts or actions and resultant but unconnected actions or events in the real world. One example would be the belief that wishing or thinking about someone’s demise could precipitate their death. Magical thinking can occur not only in patients with schizophrenia or other mental illness, but also in those with prefrontal lesions, especially if they are predisposed because of religious or spiritual beliefs to altered feelings of causality (43). Second, dream delusions are false narratives that emerge from persistent dream memories, most commonly in narcolepsy, and possibly in dementia with Lewy bodies (44). Difficulty distinguishing dreams from reality may be related to frequent sleep-wake transitions, the intrusion of aspects of rapid eye movement sleep into waking, or to the abnormal encoding of dream content into long-term memory (9, 44). Dream delusions are associated with heightened feeling of reality of fantastic intrusions from sleep (9). Third, the phenomena of fantastic thinking occurs in some patients with frontal disease. It is distinguished from spontaneous confabulations in that these patients know and acknowledge that their narratives are in their head and originate internally, sometimes from daydreams and often with wish fulfillment (24). In fact, fantastic thinking usually emerges as the product of vivid daydreaming or dreams, and may be facilitated by a predisposition to imagination. FTD is a cause of fantastic thinking where “thinking may be as good as doing it” (2), and many false narratives among patients with FTD are actually fantastic thinking (2, 45). Finally, a last consideration is a particularly emotionally overvalued idea that is pursued by the patient beyond bounds of reason and dominating his/her life. Overvalued ideas may apply to some of the compulsive-like behaviors of FTD and Parkinson’s disease.

NEUROPATHOLOGY

Disease in the mesial frontolimbic regions is implicated in patients with false narratives. Patients with delusions and confabulations and brain disorders usually have frontal dysfunction with impairment in accurately evaluating the truth of retrieved or generated information (21,27, 30). When patients accept false narratives as real, this frontal dysfunction may involve impairment in self-monitoring mediated by frontolimbic medial and orbital frontal regions. Studies show decreased prefrontal cortex gray matter density correlates with false memories or recognition (40). Some patients with frontal hypometabolism have unfettered imaginations characterized by wish-fulfillment, which may be likened to persistent dreaming. Delusions have been associated with frontal lobe abnormalities, particularly on the right (11), and, patients with Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome, a common source of provoked confabulations, have frontal-executive dysfunction (18).

Confabulations and other false narratives appear to originate from prefrontal disease particularly involving VMPFC and the adjacent orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) (31, 46, 47). Neuroimaging studies indicate that VMPFC is involved in confabulations and false memory (39), source monitoring and external origin of experience, and with identifying whether imagined words were previously seen (28). The VMPFC is associated with correct retrieval of a narrative with its feeling of rightness (42), decoupling in false belief attribution (48), and source monitoring with temporal context (47), and all these attributes can be disturbed with pathology (7, 21, 42, 48, 49). The adjacent OFC (posterior, medial) may also have a role in producing false narratives as this region affects suppression of interference from thoughts, prior anticipations, and irrelevant memories traces (15, 17, 21,5052). Disease in the posterior medial OFC, or “orbitofrontal reality filter”, is associated with behaviorally spontaneous confabulation (53, 54). The combination of VMPFC and OFC damage may be necessary for fantastic or spontaneous confabulations (15). Finally, the right hemisphere may be involved more than the left. The right PFC is mainly activated with false recollections (39), and the right VMPFC/OFC may be responsible for adding the emotional label to experiences (51).

Other neuropathological localizations have resulted in false narratives. Delusions have been associated with lesions in the heads of the caudate nuclei (CN) (55), and in schizophrenia, studies associate delusions of reference and negative symptoms with decreased gray matter density in the CN (56, 57). Right CN strokes can present with content-specific delusions and reduced metabolism in the inferior prefrontal cortex (55); however, left CN head strokes can also present with recurrent delusions, disturbed sleep, and decreased cerebral blood flow in the frontal lobes (58). Investigators speculate that frontal hypoperfusion or hypometabolism associated with CN infarctions are associated with delusions and other false narratives, presumably from disruption of fronto-subcortical circuits traversing the heads of the CN (55, 5861). In a review of neuroimaging studies in schizophrenia, the greatest gray matter reductions over time occurred in medial frontal gyrus, ACC, and insula (62), and psychosis may be linked to dysfunction of a dorsal fronto-striato-thalamic circuit (63). Moreover, a review of longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging studies suggests that treatment of schizophrenia induces changes in prefrontal cortex and ACC (64).

CONSEQUENCES AND DISCUSSION

Patients with medial frontal (“frontolimbic”) disease may develop false narratives from disturbed or absent affective biases or labeling of specific experiences. False narratives differ in the targeted experience, which can be thoughts and beliefs, perceptions, memories, imaginations, or breakthrough dreams. These experiences become delusions, confabulations, false memories, and other false narratives when there is an erroneous mental feeling towards the source, or an incorrect affective bias. This erroneous intrinsic label alters the interpretation of experiences, thought, or action, independent of his or her conscious awareness of the presence or absence of affective bias (5). This hypothesis is in accord with the emotional and motivational aspects of the frontolimbic role in motivating complex human behavior. Future research may explore this model of delusions and false narratives derived from disturbances observed among patients with brain disorders. Specifically, investigators can test this hypothesis with studies of unconscious affective bias among patients with false narratives.

Acknowledgement

This work is supported in part by U.S. National Institute on Aging (NIA) grant 1RF1AG050967-01 (MFMendez PI)

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST STATEMENT:

The author does not have any financial and personal relationships with other people or organizations that could inappropriately influence (bias) this work. The author has no conflicts of interest to declare.

The author (MfMendez) has no conflicts of interest to declare.

REFERENCES

  • [1].Kraepelin E, Ueber Erinnerungsfälschungen, Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten Bd. 17 (1886), pp. 830–840,. [Google Scholar]
  • [2].Kremen SA, Solis OE, Shapira JS, Vinters HV and Mendez MF, “Fantastic thinking” in pathologically proven Pick disease, Cogn Behav Neurol 23 (2010), pp. 130–134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [3].Roberts G, The origins of delusion, Br J Psychiatry 161 (1992), pp. 298–308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [4].Park JS and Kang UG, How delusion is formed?, Med Hypotheses 87 (2016), pp. 61–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [5].Berridge K and Winkielman P, What is an unconscious emotion?(The case for unconscious “liking”), Cogn Emot 17 (2003), pp. 181–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [6].Winkielman P, Berridge KC and Wilbarger JL, Unconscious affective reactions to masked happy versus angry faces influence consumption behavior and judgments of value, Pers Soc Psychol Bull 31 (2005), pp. 121–135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [7].Turner M and Coltheart M, Confabulation and delusion: a common monitoring framework, Cogn Neuropsychiatry 15 (2010), pp. 346–376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [8].Berlyne N, Confabulation, Br J Psychiatry 120 (1972), pp. 31–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [9].Corlett PR, Canavan SV, Nahum L, Appah F and Morgan PT, Dreams, reality and memory: confabulations in lucid dreamers implicate reality-monitoring dysfunction in dream consciousness, Cogn Neuropsychiatry 19 (2014), pp. 540–553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [10].Kendler KS, Glazer WM and Morgenstern H, Dimensions of delusional experience, Am J Psychiatry 140 (1983), pp. 466–469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [11].Sultzer DL, Brown CV, Mandelkern MA, et al. , Delusional thoughts and regional frontal/temporal cortex metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease, Am J Psychiatry 160 (2003), pp. 341–349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [12].Elliott B, Joyce E and Shorvon S, Delusions, illusions and hallucinations in epilepsy: 2. Complex phenomena and psychosis, Epilepsy Res 85 (2009), pp. 172–186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [13].Bosnjak Pasic M, Horvat Velic E, Fotak L, et al. , Many Faces of Deja Vu: a Narrative Review, Psychiatr Danub 30 (2018), pp. 21–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [14].Fotopoulou A, False selves in neuropsychological rehabilitation: the challenge of confabulation, Neuropsychol Rehabil 18 (2008), pp. 541–565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [15].Kopelman MD, Varieties of confabulation and delusion, Cogn Neuropsychiatry 15 (2010), pp. 14–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [16].Langdon R and Turner M, Delusion and confabulation: overlapping or distinct distortions of reality?, Cogn Neuropsychiatry 15 (2010), pp. 1–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [17].Nahum L, Ptak R, Leemann B and Schnider A, Disorientation, confabulation, and extinction capacity: clues on how the brain creates reality, Biol Psychiatry 65 (2009), pp. 966–972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [18].Borsutzky S, Fujiwara E, Brand M and Markowitsch HJ, Confabulations in alcoholic Korsakoff patients, Neuropsychologia 46 (2008), pp. 3133–3143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [19].Engelborghs S, Maertens K, Nagels G, et al. , Neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia: cross-sectional analysis from a prospective, longitudinal Belgian study, Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 20 (2005), pp. 1028–1037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [20].Gilboa A and Verfaellie M, Telling it like it isn’t: the cognitive neuroscience of confabulation, J Int Neuropsychol Soc 16 (2010), pp. 961–966. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [21].Gilboa A, Alain C, Stuss DT, Melo B, Miller S and Moscovitch M, Mechanisms of spontaneous confabulations: a strategic retrieval account, Brain 129 (2006), pp. 1399–1414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [22].Fotopoulou A, Conway MA, Tyrer S, Birchall D, Griffiths P and Solms M, Is the content of confabulation positive? An experimental study, Cortex 44 (2008), pp. 764–772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [23].Johnson MK, O’Connor M and Cantor J, Confabulation, memory deficits, and frontal dysfunction, Brain Cogn 34 (1997), pp. 189–206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [24].Glowinski R, Payman V and Frencham K, Confabulation: a spontaneous and fantastic review, Aust N Z J Psychiatry 42 (2008), pp. 932–940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [25].Gundogar D and Demirci S, [Confabulation: a symptom which is intriguing but not adequately known], Turk Psikiyatri Derg 18 (2007), pp. 172–178. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [26].Turner MS, Cipolotti L and Shallice T, Spontaneous confabulation, temporal context confusion and reality monitoring: a study of three patients with anterior communicating artery aneurysms, J Int Neuropsychol Soc 16 (2010), pp. 984–994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [27].Johnson MK, Hashtroudi S and Lindsay DS, Source monitoring, Psychol Bull 114 (1993), pp. 3–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [28].Turner MS, Simons JS, Gilbert SJ, Frith CD and Burgess PW, Distinct roles for lateral and medial rostral prefrontal cortex in source monitoring of perceived and imagined events, Neuropsychologia 46 (2008), pp. 1442–1453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [29].Langdon R and Bayne T, Delusion and confabulation: mistakes of perceiving, remembering and believing, Cogn Neuropsychiatry 15 (2010), pp. 319–345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [30].Kessels RP, Kortrijk HE, Wester AJ and Nys GM, Confabulation behavior and false memories in Korsakoff’s syndrome: role of source memory and executive functioning, Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 62 (2008), pp. 220–225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [31].Turner MS, Cipolotti L, Yousry TA and Shallice T, Confabulation: damage to a specific inferior medial prefrontal system, Cortex 44 (2008), pp. 637–648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [32].Alkathiri NH, Morris RG and Kopelman MD, Is there a positive bias in false recognition? Evidence from confabulating amnesia patients, Neuropsychologia 77 (2015), pp. 201–210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [33].Mendez MF and Fras IA, The false memory syndrome: experimental studies and comparison to confabulations, Med Hypotheses 76 (2011), pp. 492–496. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [34].Burnham WH, Memory, Historically and Experimentally Considered, The American Journal of Psychology 2 (1888), pp. 431–464. [Google Scholar]
  • [35].Rosenbaum RS, Gilboa A, Levine B, Winocur G and Moscovitch M, Amnesia as an impairment of detail generation and binding: evidence from personal, fictional, and semantic narratives in K.C, Neuropsychologia 47 (2009), pp. 2181–2187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [36].Nader K and Einarsson EO, Memory reconsolidation: an update, Ann N Y Acad Sci 1191 (2010), pp. 27–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [37].Reyna VF and Brainerd CJ, Fuzzy-trace theory and false memory: new frontiers, J Exp Child Psychol 71 (1998), pp. 194–209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [38].Turnbull OH, Berry H and Evans CE, A positive emotional bias in confabulatory false beliefs about place, Brain Cogn 55 (2004), pp. 490–494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [39].Marchewka A, Brechmann A, Nowicka A, Jednorog K, Scheich H and Grabowska A, False recognition of emotional stimuli is lateralised in the brain: An fMRI study, Neurobiol Learn Mem 90 (2008), pp. 280–284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [40].Marchewka A, Jednorog K, Nowicka A, Brechmann A and Grabowska A, Grey-matter differences related to true and false recognition of emotionally charged stimuli - a voxel based morphometry study, Neurobiol Learn Mem 92 (2009), pp. 99–105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [41].Schacter DL and Dodson CS, Misattribution, false recognition and the sins of memory, Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 356 (2001), pp. 1385–1393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [42].Moscovitch M and Melo B, Strategic retrieval and the frontal lobes: evidence from confabulation and amnesia, Neuropsychologia 35 (1997), pp. 1017–1034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [43].Zhong W, Krueger F, Wilson M, Bulbulia J and Grafman J, Prefrontal brain lesions reveal magical ideation arises from enhanced religious experiences, Peace Confl 24 (2018), pp. 245–249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [44].Wamsley E, Donjacour CE, Scammell TE, Lammers GJ and Stickgold R, Delusional confusion of dreaming and reality in narcolepsy, Sleep 37 (2014), pp. 419–422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [45].Nedjam Z, Devouche E and Dalla Barba G, Confabulation, but not executive dysfunction discriminate AD from frontotemporal dementia, Eur J Neurol 11 (2004), pp. 728–733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [46].Benson DF, Djenderedjian A, Miller BL, et al. , Neural basis of confabulation, Neurology 46 (1996), pp. 1239–1243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [47].Gilboa A, Alain C, He Y, Stuss DT and Moscovitch M, Ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions produce early functional alterations during remote memory retrieval, J Neurosci 29 (2009), pp. 4871–4881. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [48].Abraham A, Rakoczy H, Werning M, von Cramon DY and Schubotz RI, Matching mind to world and vice versa: Functional dissociations between belief and desire mental state processing, Soc Neurosci 5 (2010), pp. 1–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [49].Ferchiou A, Schurhoff F, Bulzacka E, Mahbouli M, Leboyer M and Szoke A, [Source monitoring: general presentation and review of literature in schizophrenia], Encephale 36 (2010), pp. 326–333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [50].Schnider A and Ptak R, Spontaneous confabulators fail to suppress currently irrelevant memory traces, Nat Neurosci 2 (1999), pp. 677–681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [51].Schnider A, von Daniken C and Gutbrod K, The mechanisms of spontaneous and provoked confabulations, Brain 119 ( Pt 4) (1996), pp. 1365–1375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [52].Garoff-Eaton RJ, Kensinger EA and Schacter DL, The neural correlates of conceptual and perceptual false recognition, Learn Mem 14 (2007), pp. 684–692. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [53].Schnider A, Nahum L and Ptak R, What does extinction have to do with confabulation?, Cortex 87 (2017), pp. 5–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [54].Schnider A, Orbitofrontal reality filtering, Front Behav Neurosci 7 (2013), p. 67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [55].McMurtray AM, Sultzer DL, Monserratt L, Yeo T and Mendez MF, Content-specific delusions from right caudate lacunar stroke: association with prefrontal hypometabolism, J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 20 (2008), pp. 62–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [56].Tao H, Wong GH, Zhang H, et al. , Grey matter morphological anomalies in the caudate head in first-episode psychosis patients with delusions of reference, Psychiatry Res 233 (2015), pp. 57–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [57].Li Y, Li WX, Xie DJ, Wang Y, Cheung EFC and Chan RCK, Grey matter reduction in the caudate nucleus in patients with persistent negative symptoms: An ALE meta-analysis, Schizophr Res 192 (2018), pp. 9–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [58].Meguro K, Meguro M and Akanuma K, Recurrent delusional ideas due to left caudate head infarction, without dementia, Psychogeriatrics 12 (2012), pp. 58–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [59].Mendez MF, Adams NL and Lewandowski KS, Neurobehavioral changes associated with caudate lesions, Neurology 39 (1989), pp. 349–354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [60].Narumoto J, Matsushima N, Oka S, et al. , Neurobehavioral changes associated with bilateral caudate nucleus infarctions, Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 59 (2005), pp. 109–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [61].Santos S, Alberti O, Corbalan T and Cortina MT, [Stroke-psychosis. Description of two cases], Actas Esp Psiquiatr 37 (2009), pp. 240–242. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [62].Shepherd AM, Laurens KR, Matheson SL, Carr VJ and Green MJ, Systematic meta-review and quality assessment of the structural brain alterations in schizophrenia, Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36 (2012), pp. 1342–1356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [63].Dandash O, Pantelis C and Fornito A, Dopamine, fronto-striato-thalamic circuits and risk for psychosis, Schizophr Res 180 (2017), pp. 48–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [64].Kani AS, Shinn AK, Lewandowski KE and Ongur D, Converging effects of diverse treatment modalities on frontal cortex in schizophrenia: A review of longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging studies, J Psychiatr Res 84 (2017), pp. 256–276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES