Abstract
Many learning tasks that children encounter necessitate the ability to direct and sustain attention to key aspects of the environment while simultaneously tuning out irrelevant features. This is challenging for at least two reasons: (a) The ability to regulate and sustain attention follows a protracted developmental time course, and (b) children spend much of their time in environments not optimized for learning—homes and schools are often chaotic, cluttered, and noisy. Research on these issues is often siloed; that is, researchers tend to examine the relationship among attention, distraction, and learning in only the auditory or the visual domain, but not both together. We provide examples in which auditory and visual aspects of learning each have strong implications for the other. Research examining how visual information and auditory information are distracting can benefit from cross-fertilization. Integrating across research silos informs our understanding of attention and learning, yielding more efficacious guidance for caregivers, educators, developers, and policymakers.
Keywords: visual attention, auditory attention, selective sustained attention, distraction, learning, noise, clutter
Children need to acquire knowledge from both informal and formal settings, but the world around them is filled with different sounds and sights all vying for attention. Research suggests that successful learning depends in part on the ability to selectively focus attention (Erickson, Thiessen, Godwin, Dickerson, & Fisher, 2015; Oakes, Kannass, & Shaddy, 2002), and this capacity develops slowly during childhood (Colombo, 2001). A growing body of research is focused on investigating the nature of children’s auditory and visual environments and how these features impact learning (Fisher, Godwin, & Seltman, 2014; Pereira, Smith, & Yu, 2014; Tomalski et al., 2017).
Children have difficulty learning in chaotic environments containing visual or auditory distractors (Barr, Shuck, Salerno, Atkinson, & Linebarger, 2010; Chiong & DeLoache, 2013; Flack & Horst, 2017; Godwin et al., 2016; Parish-Morris, Mahajan, Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff, & Collins, 2013). Research on these issues tends to be siloed: Relatively little cross talk occurs among researchers with expertise in auditory and language development and researchers focusing on visual attention and learning.1 We argue that each domain has important implications for the other, and considering visual and auditory distractions jointly may lead to new insights and recommendations for best practices for caregivers, educators, developers, and policymakers. Here, we provide an overview of attention, discuss areas of overlap between auditory and visual domains, and conclude by presenting relevant research on learning, focusing on how auditory and visual distraction may be fundamentally intertwined.
Attention, Auditory Processing, and Visual Processing
The environment contains many distinct sources of visual and auditory information; however, only a subset of this information may be relevant for a particular learning task. Thus, to learn, children must selectively attend to relevant features of the environment at the expense of others. Imagine a child sitting at the kitchen table listening to a caregiver read a story. This task might require the child to visually attend to the illustrations and carefully listen to the story. Simultaneously, the child needs to ignore the sights and sounds of a busy household that are irrelevant to the task at hand (e.g., an intricate tablecloth, the dishwasher humming, the dog walking past). We refer to this ability to attend to task-relevant information, inhibit irrelevant information, and maintain this state over time as selective sustained attention.
Attention regulation can be automatic, meaning that attention is captured by salient aspects of the environment— such as loud sounds, bright colors, and motion—or top- down and voluntary, based on an individual’s goals and interests (Colombo & Cheatham, 2006; Ruff & Rothbart, 2001). Early in development, selective sustained attention is largely driven by stimulus properties such as brightness, contrast, and novelty (Bornstein, 1990; Ruff & Rothbart, 2001). As brain regions such as the prefrontal cortex mature, children acquire increasing ability to deploy attention voluntarily (Oakes et al., 2002). This dual model of attention regulation is a common framework in cognitive psychology; however, debate exists over precisely how to define attention, its components, and functions. A comprehensive treatment of the nature of attention is beyond the scope of the present article (for reviews, see Colombo, 2001; Ruff & Rothbart, 2001); nevertheless, there is general agreement that (a) attention is multifaceted, with distinct functions, and (b) selective sustained attention is critically important for complex processing.
Prior work has examined the relationship among attention, task performance, learning, and academic achievement (Choudhury & Gorman, 2000; Commodari, 2012; Dixon & Salley, 2007; Duncan et al., 2007). Although selective sustained attention is hypothesized to be critical for learning in both the auditory and visual domains, a disproportionate amount of research on the development of attention has focused on visual attention. Additionally, research with infants and toddlers tends to focus on attention that is regulated automatically. These asymmetries are likely due to difficulties in measuring voluntary attention in children who are too young to understand or follow directions and because attention is often measured via behavioral indices such as eye gaze, which is more tightly linked to visual attention—although it is possible to shift the focus of attention without changing gaze (Duc, Bays, & Husain, 2008; Hoffman & Subramaniam, 1995).
Key Differences and Areas of Overlap
Many differences exist between the auditory and visual domains. For example, although sounds may be sustained over time, decay and transience are among their fundamental properties. Thus, auditory processing often involves making sense of rapidly changing or disappearing signals. In contrast, visual input is more stable and less likely to suddenly disappear or to disappear as quickly. Consequently, visual processing may be less fundamentally linked to temporal principles governing auditory processing. Research suggests that temporal dynamics favoring learning in the visual domain often differ from those of similar learning tasks presented in the auditory domain (Conway & Christiansen, 2009). In comparison, language processing occurs in both the auditory and visual domains (recognizing words within a speech stream and reading respectively); however, the transient nature of the auditory domain makes the task inherently different.
In the same way that temporal dynamics are critical to auditory processing, spatial factors are of greater importance to visual processing. To attend to a target among distracting objects, a viewer must localize it in space. At a physiological level, visual information is spatially distributed, such that information from different objects is processed by different receptors in the eye. In audition, all sounds are funneled down the ear canal to the tympanic membrane or eardrum; collective vibrations are transmitted to the cochlea and auditory receptors. The brain must reseparate the target and distracting signals prior to making sense of an attended signal (Bregman, 1990). In some respects, visual distractions may be easier to ignore than auditory distractions, especially if an individual can physically orient away from the distractions. For instance, desk dividers can shield against visual distractions and focus attention on instructional materials. In contrast, a physical shield (e.g., earplugs) can dampen all sound but cannot typically selectively reduce distracting sounds.
Another important difference relates to phenomena associated with increasing the number of auditory and visual objects. In vision, increasing the number of objects can produce clutter, possibly increasing the difficulty of maintaining attention to a target (Dixon & Salley, 2007; Fisher, Thiessen, Godwin, Kloos, & Dickerson, 2013). In contrast, increasing the number of auditory signals can fuse signals into a single noise that is more intense but also less variable and thus less likely to cause distraction; this is particularly true of voices (Brungart & Simpson, 2002). Consequentially, attentional effects based on the number of objects likely differ by domain. There are also differences in modality dominance: Infants and toddlers rely more on auditory information in contexts in which auditory and visual information compete. Around age 4, this preference evens out, and eventually visual information begins to dominate (Robinson & Sloutsky, 2004). The protracted developmental trajectory of attention, as well as differences in the perceptual impact of the environment across development, point to the importance of considering how acoustically and visually optimized learning environments will likely differ with age.
There are also similarities between the auditory and visual domains. For example, background noise can impair processing of a target through energetic or informational masking (Brungart & Simpson, 2002). In energetic masking, energy from one signal interferes with another; this can occur when a distractor at the same frequency as the target makes the target inaudible or when the auditory representations of two signals interfere as a result of spread of excitation on the basilar membrane. In vision, spatial occlusion of one object by another can be thought of as analogous to energetic masking. Informational masking refers to cases in which a potential distractor causes confusion, making the listener uncertain of which sounds belong to which signal; here, the target signal typically remains partially or even fully audible but can still be difficult to distinguish from background noise. In the visual domain, an analogous scenario occurs when a target object is fully visible but presented with other objects. The proximity of dis- tractor objects may increase the difficulty of distinguishing the target from distractors or generally reduce attention to the target.
In both the visual and auditory domains, distractors can be simple and static, such as the relatively constant sounds of the air conditioning humming, or plain and unadorned stationary objects. Distractors can also be complex and variable—for example, speech sounds changing in frequency, pitch, or volume or objects or displays that are bright, moving, or patterned. Regardless of domain, individuals may find it more difficult to habituate or ignore variable and complex stimuli (see Kavšek, 2013, for a review). Moreover, the level of concentration required for a given learning task may influence susceptibility to distraction as well as the level of impairment the distractor (or distractors) may cause.
Finally, the intensity of auditory and visual information can cause frustration and stress, and in some cases physical damage; for example, very loud sounds and bright lights can damage sensory receptors (Andley, 1987; Ising & Kruppa, 2004; Shield & Dockrell, 2003). Tolerance for extraneous noise and clutter may also vary. Children with autism or hearing loss may be disproportionately affected by extraneous information in the environment because of heightened sensitivity to noise and susceptibility to visual distractions (Dunn, Myles, & Orr, 2002; Guardino & Anita, 2012; Hanley et al., 2017). Similarly, bilingual children seem to be more susceptible to auditory noise (Mayo, Florentine, & Buus, 1997).
Implications for Learning
Background noise can be detrimental to children’s speech comprehension and learning (Barr et al., 2010), which is important given that noise levels in day-care centers and schools are frequently higher than recommended levels (American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, or ASHA, 1995; Erickson & Newman, 2017). Hygge, Evans, and Bullinger (2002) reported a variety of negative effects on cognitive performance measures in elementary school students exposed to aircraft noise. Learning costs related to more pleasant background noise have also been shown, as instrumental music can impair learning from television among infants (Barr et al., 2010). Similarly, background speech can disrupt the acquisition of new labels: McMillan and Saffran (2016) found that toddlers struggled to learn new labels unless they were substantially louder than background speech. Although the cause of such difficulties in listening and learning when noise is present is uncertain, early maturation of the auditory system may implicate attentional difficulties (Erickson & Newman, 2017). Background noise that varies in content or volume over time may automatically capture attention, resulting in divided attention. Thus, background noise may increase the burden on children’s attentional systems as they strive to attend to a learning activity.
Findings such as these have led to classroom design recommendations to help improve acoustics by adding drop ceilings, acoustical ceiling tiles, carpeting, and noise-absorbing surfaces (Crandell & Smaldino, 2001; Woolworth & Phinney, 2015). Specific recommendations regarding this latter acoustical modification include incorporating noise-absorbing materials, such as cork bulletin boards, and hanging quilts, flags, and student work from classroom walls (ASHA, n.d.; Crandell & Smaldino, 2001; Manlove, Frank, & Vernon- Feagans, 2001). However, such recommendations should be tempered considering how these design elements interact with children’s visual attention: A growing literature has found greater inattention and reduced learning outcomes in environments containing visual distractions such as educational posters and artwork, compared with visually streamlined environments (Fisher et al., 2014; Hanley et al., 2017). Similarly, classroom complexity and color are negatively related to student achievement (Barrett, Zhang, Moffat, & Kobbacy, 2013); however, Barrett, Davies, Zhang, and Barrett (2015) recently found evidence of a curvilinear relationship suggesting that moderate amounts of visual stimulation may be optimal for learning. Given this evidence, hanging bulletin boards or soft materials may reduce noise but may inadvertently decrease attention and learning by directing attention to features of the visual environment.
As discussed above, visual clutter can be detrimental for school-age children (Barrett et al., 2015; Barrett et al., 2013; Fisher et al., 2014; Hanley et al., 2017), but it can also serve as a distraction and impair learning in early childhood. For example, visual clutter can impede vocabulary acquisition: Pereira et al. (2014) found that toddlers’ acquisition of novel labels was enhanced when the target was centrally positioned in the toddlers’ view, with few or no distractors, compared with cases in which the target was less central or among more distractors (see also Horst, Scott, & Pollard, 2010). However, here again, the relationship may be curvilinear, as label acquisition may be enhanced by the presence of a single distractor compared with conditions under which all visual clutter is omitted (Zosh, Brinster, & Halberda, 2013). Complexity of visual stimuli or overloading also affects preschoolers’ ability to learn new words. Three-year-olds struggled to learn new words from books when they contained multiple illustrations per page compared with visually streamlined books containing a single illustration per page (Flack & Horst, 2017). Additionally, young children show diminished learning outcomes when learning novel words or content from books containing pop-ups or manipulative features compared with standard picture books (Tare, Chiong, Ganea, & DeLoache, 2010). Efforts to increase attention and engagement while reading have resulted in electronic books filled with animations and sound effects (for a discussion, see Moody, 2010). However, recent research suggests that extraneous auditory features can also reduce toddlers’ story comprehension (Parish-Morris et al., 2013).
These examples highlight the importance of integrating across disciplines, and they underscore the significance of considering both auditory and visual properties and their potential for distraction when creating learning environments and instructional materials that support early learning. For example, if quilts are used to muffle distracting sounds, solid and neutral-color materials may be less visually distracting than colorful or patterned fabric. As discussed above, highly decorated learning environments, even those containing educational content, can increase inattention and decrease learning (Fisher et al., 2014). Educational practitioners can help mitigate these negative effects by reducing the amount of visual material displayed in the classroom. Instead of decorating the classroom itself, educators can create exhibits showcasing student work in hallways or the cafeteria. With advancements in technology, classrooms can become adaptive places where only materials relevant for the current lesson are projected, reducing attentional competition between the visual environment and learning activity—a possibility we are currently investigating. One could easily extend these ideas to other formats, including educational applications, games, books, and television programing. More generally, integrating knowledge across disciplines can lead to new insights and yield more visually and acoustically optimized educational materials and environments.
Conclusions
In this article, we provide an overview of selective, sustained attention as it relates to infants’ and young children’s formal and informal learning by considering how auditory and visual distractions are interconnected. Work on both auditory and visual attention can be brought to bear to guide interventions and inform design of optimal instructional materials and learning environments. More research is needed, as much remains unknown about how auditory and visual complexity interact in the context of attention and learning. Furthermore, assuming that perception is not amodal, it is unlikely that auditory and visual distractions work exactly the same way. Thus, considering these domains jointly could also have important theoretical implications. In conclusion, joint consideration of visual and auditory attention can generate new insights and enhance the efficacy of recommendations for research-based practices for caregivers, educators, developers, and policymakers.
Recommended Reading
Colombo, J. (2001). (See References). A thorough review of the development of visual attention in infants.
Colombo, J., & Cheatham, C. L. (2006). (See References). An overview of the development of endogenous attention in early development, as well as the neurological underpinnings.
Fisher, A. V., Godwin, K. E., & Seltman, H. (2014). (See References). An empirical examination of the relationship between attention allocation and learning that highlights the importance of considering children’s developing attentional system in classroom design.
Ruff, H. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2001). (See References). A comprehensive overview of the development of attention as well as factors contributing to individual differences in the development of attention.
Werner, L. A. (2007). Issues in human auditory development. Journal of Communication Disorders, 40, 275–283. A discussion of the stages of, as well as the role of attention in, auditory development.
Acknowledgments
Thanks to Anna Fisher, Paulo Carvalho, and three anonymous reviewers for their insightful comments and suggestions for revision.
Funding
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant 5R01HD081127-02.
Footnotes
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author(s) declared that there were no conflicts of interest with respect to the authorship or the publication of this article.
We focus on attention with a specific emphasis on how auditory and visual distractions influence learning. Multimodal perception is a related but separate line of inquiry. We refer interested readers to Bremner, Lewkowicz, and Spence (2012) for more information on this important research area.
References
- American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (1995). Acoustics in educational settings. Subcommittee on Acoustics in Educational Settings of the Bioacoustics Standards and Noise Standards Committee American Speech-Language- Hearing Association. ASHA, 37(Suppl. 14), 15–19. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (n.d.). Tips for creating a good listening environment in the classroom. Retrieved from https://www.asha.org/public/hearing/Creating-a-Good-Listening-Environment-in-the-Classroom/
- Andley UP (1987). Photodamage to the eye. Photochemistry and Photobiology, 46, 1057–1066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barr R, Shuck L, Salerno K, Atkinson E, & Linebarger DL (2010). Music interferes with learning from television during infancy. Infant and Child Development, 19, 313–331. [Google Scholar]
- Barrett P, Davies F, Zhang Y, & Barrett L (2015). The impact of classroom design on pupils’ learning: Final results of a holistic, multi-level analysis. Building & Environment, 89, 118–133. doi: 10.1016/j.buildenv.2015.02.013 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett P, Zhang Y, Moffat J, & Kobbacy K (2013). A holistic, multi-level analysis identifying the impact of classroom design on pupil’s learning. Building and Environment, 59, 678–689. [Google Scholar]
- Bornstein MH (1990). Attention in infancy and the prediction of cognitive capacities in childhood In Enns JT (Ed.), Advances in Psychology: Vol. 69. Development of attention: Research and theory (pp. 3–20). New York, NY: Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
- Bregman AS (1990). Auditory scene analysis: The perceptual organization of sound. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Bremner AJ, Lewkowicz DJ, & Spence C (Eds.). (2012). Multisensory development. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Brungart DS, & Simpson BD (2002). Within-ear and acrossear interference in a cocktail-party listening task. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 112, 2985–2995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiong C, & DeLoache JS (2013). Learning the ABCs: What kinds of picture books facilitate young children’s learning? Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 13, 225–241. [Google Scholar]
- Choudhury N, & Gorman KS (2000). The relationship between sustained attention and cognitive performance in 17–24-month old toddlers. Infant and Child Development, 9, 127–146. [Google Scholar]
- Colombo J (2001). The development of visual attention in infancy. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 337–367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colombo J, & Cheatham CL (2006). The emergence and basis of endogenous attention in infancy and early childhood In Kail RV (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 34, pp. 283–322). New York, NY: Elsevier. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Commodari E (2012). Attention skills and risk of developing learning difficulties. Current Psychology, 31, 17–34. [Google Scholar]
- Conway CM, & Christiansen MH (2009). Seeing and hearing in space and time: Effects of modality and presentation rate on implicit statistical learning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21, 561–580. [Google Scholar]
- Crandell CC, & Smaldino JJ (2001). Acoustical modifications for the classroom. Volta Review, 101(5), 33–46. [Google Scholar]
- Dixon WE, & Salley BJ (2007). “Shh! We’re tryin’ to concentrate”: Attention and environmental distracters in novel word learning. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 167, 393–414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duc AH, Bays P, & Husain M (2008). Eye movements as a probe of attention. Progress in Brain Research, 171, 403–411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duncan GJ, Dowsett CJ, Claessens A, Magnuson K, Huston AC, Klebanov P, & Sexton H (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1428–1446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunn W, Myles BS, & Orr S (2002). Sensory processing issues associated with Asperger syndrome: A preliminary investigation. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 56, 97–102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erickson LC, & Newman RS (2017). Influences of background noise on infants and children. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26, 451–457. doi: 10.1177/0963721417709087 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erickson LC, Thiessen ED, Godwin KE, Dickerson JP, & Fisher AV (2015). Endogenously and exogenously driven selective sustained attention: Contributions to learning in kindergarten children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 138, 126–134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher A, Thiessen E, Godwin K, Kloos H, & Dickerson J (2013). Assessing selective sustained attention in 3- to 5-year-old children: Evidence from a new paradigm. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 275–294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher AV, Godwin KE, & Seltman H (2014). Visual environment, attention allocation, and learning in young children when too much of a good thing may be bad. Psychological Science, 25, 1362–1370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flack ZM, & Horst JS (2017). Two sides to every story: Children learn words better from one storybook page at a time. Infant and Child Development, 27(1), Article e2047. doi: 10.1002/icd.2047 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Godwin KE, Almeda MV, Seltman H, Kai S, Skerbetz MD, Baker RS, & Fisher AV (2016). Off-task behavior in elementary school children. Learning and Instruction, 44, 128–143. [Google Scholar]
- Guardino C, & Anita S (2012). Modifying the classroom environment to increase engagement and decrease disruption with students who are deaf or hard of hearing. The Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 17, 518–533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanley M, Khairat M, Taylor K, Wilson R, Cole-Fletcher R, & Riby DM (2017). Classroom displays—Attraction or distraction? Evidence of impact on attention and learning from children with and without autism. Developmental Psychology, 53, 1265–1275. doi: 10.1037/dev0000271 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman JE, & Subramaniam B (1995). The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 57, 787–795. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horst JS, Scott EJ, & Pollard JA (2010). The role of competition in word learning via referent selection. Developmental Science, 13, 706–713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hygge S, Evans GW, & Bullinger M (2002). A prospective study of some effects of aircraft noise on cognitive performance in schoolchildren. Psychological Science, 13, 469–474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ising H, & Kruppa B (2004). Health effects caused by noise: Evidence in the literature from the past 25 years. Noise Health, 6(22), 5–13. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kavšek M (2013). The comparator model of infant visual habituation and dishabituation: Recent insights. Developmental Psychobiology, 55, 793–808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manlove EE, Frank T, & Vernon-Feagans L (2001). Why should we care about noise in classrooms and child care settings? Child and Youth Care Forum, 30, 55–64. doi: 10.1023/A:1016663520205 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mayo LH, Florentine M, & Buus S (1997). Age of second- language acquisition and perception of speech in noise. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 40, 686–693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McMillan B, & Saffran JR (2016). Learning in complex environments: The effects of background speech on early word learning. Child Development, 87, 1841–1855. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moody AK (2010). Using electronic books in the classroom to enhance emergent literacy skills in young children. Journal of Literacy and Technology, 11(4), 22–52. [Google Scholar]
- Oakes LM, Kannass KN, & Shaddy DJ (2002). Developmental changes in endogenous control of attention: The role of target familiarity on infants’ distraction latency. Child Development, 73, 1644–1655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parish-Morris J, Mahajan N, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM, & Collins MF (2013). Once upon a time: Parent–child dialogue and storybook reading in the electronic era. Mind, Brain, and Education, 7, 200–211. [Google Scholar]
- Pereira AF, Smith LB, & Yu C (2014). A bottom-up view of toddler word learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 178–185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson CW, & Sloutsky VM (2004). Auditory dominance and its change in the course of development. Child Development, 75, 1387–1401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruff HA, & Rothbart MK (2001). Attention in early development: Themes and variations. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Shield BM, & Dockrell JE (2003). The effects of noise on children at school: A review. Building Acoustics, 10, 97–116. [Google Scholar]
- Tare M, Chiong C, Ganea P, & DeLoache J (2010). Less is more: How manipulative features affect children’s learning from picture books. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31, 395–400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomalski P, Marczuk K, Pisula E, Malinowska A, Kawa R, & Niedźwiecka A (2017). Chaotic home environment is associated with reduced infant processing speed under high task demands. Infant Behavior and Development, 48, 124–133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolworth D, & Phinney P (2015). Classroom acoustics for architects: A companion booklet for ANSI/ASA S12.60 parts 1 and 2. Retrieved from https://acousticalsociety.org/wp-content/uploads/2018/02/Classroom_Acoustics_for_Architects_4_18_15.pdf
- Zosh JM, Brinster M, & Halberda J (2013). Optimal contrast: Competition between two referents improves word learning. Applied Developmental Science, 17, 20–28. [Google Scholar]