Skip to main content
The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews logoLink to The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews
. 2019 Jun 21;2019(6):CD008223. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD008223.pub3

Social skills training for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children aged 5 to 18 years

Ole Jakob Storebø 1,2,3,, Mette Elmose Andersen 3, Maria Skoog 4, Signe Joost Hansen 2,3, Erik Simonsen 2,5, Nadia Pedersen 2, Britta Tendal 6,7, Henriette E Callesen 7, Erlend Faltinsen 2, Christian Gluud 8
Editor: Cochrane Developmental, Psychosocial and Learning Problems Group
PMCID: PMC6587063  PMID: 31222721

Abstract

Background

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children is associated with hyperactivity and impulsivity, attention problems, and difficulties with social interactions. Pharmacological treatment may alleviate the symptoms of ADHD but this rarely solves difficulties with social interactions. Children with ADHD may benefit from interventions designed to improve their social skills. We examined the benefits and harms of social skills training on social skills, emotional competencies, general behaviour, ADHD symptoms, performance in school of children with ADHD, and adverse events.

Objectives

To assess the beneficial and harmful effects of social skills training in children and adolescents with ADHD.

Search methods

In July 2018, we searched CENTRAL, MEDLINE, Embase, PsycINFO, 4 other databases and two trials registers.We also searched online conference abstracts, and contacted experts in the field for information about unpublished or ongoing randomised clinical trials. We did not limit our searches by language, year of publication, or type or status of publication, and we sought translation of the relevant sections of non‐English language articles.

Selection criteria

Randomised clinical trials investigating social skills training versus either no intervention or waiting‐list control, with or without pharmacological treatment of both comparison groups of children and adolescents with ADHD.

Data collection and analysis

We conducted the review in accordance with the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Intervention. We performed the analyses using Review Manager 5 software and Trial Sequential Analysis. We assessed bias according to domains for systematic errors. We assessed the certainty of the evidence with the GRADE approach.

Main results

We included 25 randomised clinical trials described in 45 reports. The trials included a total of 2690 participants aged between five and 17 years. In 17 trials, participants were also diagnosed with various comorbidities.

The social skills interventions were described as: 1) social skills training, 2) cognitive behavioural therapy, 3) multimodal behavioural/psychosocial therapy, 4) child life and attention skills treatment, 5) life skills training, 6) the "challenging horizon programme", 7) verbal self‐instruction, 8) meta‐cognitive training, 9) behavioural therapy, 10) behavioural and social skills treatment, and 11) psychosocial treatment. The control interventions were no intervention or waiting list.

The duration of the interventions ranged from five weeks to two years. We considered the content of the social skills interventions to be comparable and based on a cognitive‐behavioural model. Most of the trials compared child social skills training or parent training combined with medication versus medication alone. Some of the experimental interventions also included teacher consultations.

More than half of the trials were at high risk of bias for generation of the allocation sequence and allocation concealment. No trial reported on blinding of participants and personnel. Most of the trials did not report on differences between groups in medication for comorbid disorders. We used all eligible trials in the meta‐analyses, but downgraded the certainty of the evidence to low or very low.

We found no clinically relevant treatment effect of social skills interventions on the primary outcome measures: teacher‐rated social skills at end of treatment (standardised mean difference (SMD) 0.11, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.00 to 0.22; 11 trials, 1271 participants; I2 = 0%; P = 0.05); teacher‐rated emotional competencies at end of treatment (SMD −0.02, 95% CI −0.72 to 0.68; two trials, 129 participants; I2 = 74%; P = 0.96); or on teacher‐rated general behaviour (SMD −0.06 (negative value better), 95% CI −0.19 to 0.06; eight trials, 1002 participants; I2 = 0%; P = 0.33). The effect on the primary outcome, teacher‐rated social skills at end of treatment, corresponds to a MD of 1.22 points on the social skills rating system (SSRS) scale (95% CI 0.09 to 2.36). The minimal clinical relevant difference (10%) on the SSRS is 10.0 points (range 0 to 102 points on SSRS).

We found evidence in favour of social skills training on teacher‐rated core ADHD symptoms at end of treatment for all eligible trials (SMD −0.26, 95% CI −0.47 to −0.05; 14 trials, 1379 participants; I2= 69%; P = 0.02), but the finding is questionable due to lack of support from sensitivity analyses, high risk of bias, lack of clinical significance, high heterogeneity, and low certainty.

The studies did not report any serious or non‐serious adverse events.

Authors' conclusions

The review suggests that there is little evidence to support or refute social skills training for children and adolescents with ADHD. We may need more trials that are at low risk of bias and a sufficient number of participants to determine the efficacy of social skills training versus no training for ADHD. The evidence base regarding adolescents is especially weak.

Plain language summary

Social skills training for children aged between 5 and 18 with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)

Review question

What are the benefits and harms of social skills training for children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)?

Background

Children and adolescents with ADHD experience hyperactivity, impulsivity, attention problems, and difficulties with social interactions. Social skills training for ADHD seeks to improve and maintain social interaction and prevent interpersonal difficulties. Programs tend to focus on problem solving, control of emotions, and improving verbal and non‐verbal communication. We examined the benefits and harms of social skills training on the following outcomes: social skills, emotional competencies, general behaviour, ADHD symptoms, and performance in school.

Study characteristics

We found 25 randomised clinical trials (studies where participants with ADHD were randomly assigned to one of two or more groups) involving a total of 2690 participants. The trials lasted between five weeks and two years. The social skills training generally focused on teaching the children how to 'read' the subtle cues in social interaction, such as learning to wait for their turn, knowing when to shift topics during a conversation, and being able to recognise the emotional expressions of others. Social skills training often consists of role play, exercises and games, as well as homework. Children in the control groups either received no intervention or were placed on a waiting list.

Key results

We found no significant differences between social skills training versus controls on social skills, emotional competencies, and general behaviour as assessed by teachers. Compared with the children who had no social skills training, teachers rated those who had been in the social skills groups as having fewer ADHD symptoms at the end of treatment.. However, this finding was questionable because our other analyses did not support it. We found no indications of harmful effects.

All trials suffered from methodological problems such as overestimation of benefits and underestimation of harms. Many studies were also difficult to compare because they involved different interventions. The results from some trials were not very precise, which means it is difficult to be confident in the results. In seven trials, study authors were board members of pharmaceutical companies, had received funding from such companies, or had performed previous research on the topic.

Intepretation

We are unable to conclude whether social skills training is beneficial or not for children with ADHD. We need more randomised clinical trials on social skills training for children and adolescents with ADHD that have a sufficient number of participants and higher methodological quality.The evidence base regarding adolescents is especially weak. We found no adverse treatment effects.

Summary of findings

Summary of findings for the main comparison. Social skills training compared to no intervention.

Social skills training compared to no intervention
Patient or population: children aged five to 18 years with ADHD
 Settings: outpatient clinic; inpatient hospital wards; elementary schools; community mental health centre
 Intervention: social skills training
 Comparison: no intervention
Outcomes Illustrative comparative risks* (95% CI) Relative effect
 (95% CI) Number of participants (studies) Certainity of the evidence
 (GRADE) Comments
Assumed risk Corresponding risk
No intervention Social skills training
Teacher‐rated social skills
Measured by: Conners Behavior Rating Scale: Social Problems Index; Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire: Prosocial Behaviour Subscale (teacher‐rated); Social Skills Improvement System; Social Skills Rating Scale: Coorperation Subscale
Follow‐up: at end of treatment
The mean score for teacher‐rated social skills at end of treatment in the intervention groups was 0.11 standard deviations higher (0.00 lower to 0.22 higher)e 1271 (11 studies) ⊕⊝⊝⊝
 Very low
a,b,c
Social skills training may have no effect on teacher‐rated social skills
Parent‐rated social skills
Measured by: Social Skills Rating Scale; Weiss Functional Impairment Scale: Social Acitivities Domain (parent‐rated); Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire: Prosocial Behavior Subscale; Social Skills Improvement System
Follow‐up: at end of treatment
The mean score for parent‐rated social skills at end of treatment in the intervention groups was 0.19 standard deviations higher (0.06 higher to 0.32 higher) 1609 (15 studies) ⊕⊝⊝⊝
 Very low
a,b,c
Social skills training may have no effect on parent‐rated social skills
Teacher‐rated emotional competencies
Measured by: Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: Emotional Symptoms Subscale; Conners Behavior Rating Scale: Emotional Index Score
Follow‐up: at end of treatment
The mean score for teacher‐rated emotional competencies at end of treatment in the intervention groups was 0.02 standard deviations lower (0.72 lower to 0.68 higher) 129 (two studies) ⊕⊝⊝⊝
 Very low
a,b,c
Social skills training may have no effect on teacher‐rated emotional competencies
Teacher‐rated general behaviour
Measured by: Self‐Control Rating Scale; Conners Behavior Rating Scale: Aggressiveness Index; Disruptive Behavior Disorders Rating Scale; Conners Teacher Rating Scale: Conduct Problems Index; Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: Conduct Problems Subscale (teacher‐rated);
Child Symptom Inventory: ODD Scale (teacher‐rated); Child Behavior Checklist
Follow‐up: at end of treatment
The mean score for teacher‐rated general behaviour at end of treatment in the intervention groups was 0.06 standard deviations lower (0.19 lower to 0.06 higher) 1002 (eight studies) ⊕⊕⊝⊝
 Low
a,d
Social skills training may have no effect on teacher‐rated general behaviour
Parent‐rated general behaviour
Measured by: Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (parent‐rated; total scores); Conners Behavior Rating Scale: Aggressiveness Index; Disruptive Behavior Disorders Rating Scale; Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; SDQ: Conduct Problems Subscale (parent‐rated); Child Symptom Inventory; Child Behavior Checklist
Follow‐up: at end of treatment
The mean score for parent‐rated general behaviour at end of treatment in the intervention groups was 0.38 standard deviations lower (0.61 lower to 0.14 lower)   995 (eight studies) ⊕⊝⊝⊝
 Very low
a,b,c,d
Social skills training may slightly improve parent‐rated general behaviour
Teacher‐rated ADHD symptoms
Measured by: Disruptive Behavior Disorders Rating Scale; ADHD Rating Scales: Hyperactivity and Impulsivity Subscales (total scores); Conner Teacher Rating Scale: Hyperactivity Index; Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD Symptoms and Normal Behaviors; ADHD Symptom Checklist; Child Symptom Inventory (ADHD (inattention) scale score); SNAP‐IV (teacher rating scale)
Follow‐up: at end of treatment
The mean score for teacher‐rated ADHD symptoms at end of treatment in the intervention groups was 0.26 standard deviations lower (0.47 lower to 0.05 lower) 1379 (14 studies) ⊕⊝⊝⊝
 Very lowa,b,c Social skills training may slightly improve teacher‐rated ADHD symptoms
Parent‐rated ADHD symptoms
Measured by: Conners Parent Rating Scale: Hyperkinesis Index; Disruptive Behavior Disorders Rating Scale; Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD Symptoms and Normal Behaviors;
Sluggish Cognitive Tempo; ADHD Symptom Checklist; ADHD Rating Scales; Child Symptom Inventory: Inattention; SNAP‐IV (teacher rating scale); Child Attention Profile
Follow‐up: at end of treatment
The mean score for parent‐rated ADHD symptoms at end of treatment in the intervention groups was 0.54 standard deviations lower (0.81 lower to 0.26 lower) 1206 (11 studies) ⊕⊝⊝⊝
 Very lowa,b,c Social skills training may slightly improve parent‐rated ADHD symptoms
*The basis for the assumed risk (e.g. the median control group risk across studies) is provided in footnotes. The corresponding risk (and its 95% CI) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% Cl).
ADHD: Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; CI: Confidence interval; ODD: Oppositional defiant disorder; SNAP‐IV: Swanson, Nolan and Pelham rating scale ‐ Fourth Version.
GRADE Working Group grades of evidence
 High quality: we are very confident that the true effect lies close to that of the estimate of the effect.
 Moderate quality: we are moderately confident in the effect estimate; the true effect is likely to be close to the estimate of the effect, but there is a possibility that it is substantially different.
 Low quality: our confidence in the effect estimate is limited; the true effect may be substantially different from the estimate of the effect.
 Very low quality: we have very little confidence in the effect estimate; the true effect is likely to be substantially different from the estimate of effect.

aDowngraded one level due to high risk of bias (systematic errors leading to overestimation of benefits and underestimation of harms) in several 'Risk of bias' domains, including lack of sufficient blinding and selective outcome reporting (many of the included studies did not report on this outcome)
 bDowngraded one level due to inconsistency: moderate statistical heterogeneity (I2 = 30% to 50%)
 c Downgraded one level due to imprecision: wide CI
 dDowngraded one level due to indirectness (children's general behaviour was assessed by different types of rating scales, each with a different focus on behaviour)

eThe effect on the primary outcome, teacher‐rated social skills at end of treatment, corresponds to a MD of 1.22 points on the social skills rating system (SSRS) scale (95% CI 0.09 to 2.36). The minimal clinical relevant difference (10%) on the SSRS is 10.0 points (range 0 to 102 points on SSRS).

Background

Description of the condition

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) affects 3% to 5% of all children (Polanczyk 2007; Thomas 2015). The main symptoms of ADHD include problems with attention, impulsiveness, and hyperactivity (Sergeant 2003; Pasini 2007). Individuals with ADHD also present with difficulties in the domains of attentional and cognitive functions, such as problem‐solving, planning, orienting, flexibility, sustained attention, response inhibition, and working memory (Sergeant 2003; Pasini 2007). Other difficulties involve affective components such as motivation delay and mood regulation (Nigg 2005;Castellanos 2006;Schmidt 2009). These latter difficulties are closely related to the condition and are the fundamental basis for these children's problems with social skills (Whalen 1985; Landau 1991),

Prevalence estimates for ADHD vary across the international literature. A large survey in the UK found that 3.6% of boys aged five to 15 years had ADHD; for girls of the same age, this study reported a prevalence of 0.9% (Ford 2003). In one study from Columbia, the reported prevalence was considerably higher: 19.9% for boys and 12.3% for girls (Pineda 2003). A systematic review on the prevalence of ADHD reported a mean proportion of 5.3% children and adolescents having ADHD overall, and concluded that much of the variation is derived from differences in methods used to diagnose the condition (Polanczyk 2007). Among US children and adolescents, the estimated prevalence of diagnosed attention deficit hyperactivity disorder increased from 6.1% in 1997‐1998 to 10.2% in 2015‐2016 (Xu 2018).

The aetiology of ADHD involves genetic, environmental, and social factors that are not clearly understood. Family and twin studies have shown a high heritability and with no sex differences of heritability (Neale 2010; Franke 2012). Furthermore, genetic factors may be involved in determining the persistence of ADHD into adulthood (Faraone 2000; Franke 2012). Although family studies have shown high heritability, and there are many candidate genes that may be involved in the disorder (Neale 2010), genome‐wide studies have yet to find any clear associations. Environmental risk factors include prenatal substance exposures, heavy metal and chemical exposures, nutritional factors, and lifestyle/psychosocial factors (Froehlich 2011).

A diagnosis of ADHD is made through recognition of excessive inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity (according to the presence of 18 symptoms) in a child, before 12 years of age, that causes impairment to his/her functioning or development (DSM‐5; ICD‐10). The principal classification systems for diagnosing ADHD are: International Classification of Diseases ‐ 10th Version (ICD‐10); and the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) Fourth Edition (DSM‐IV), Fourth Edition ‐ Text Revision (DSM‐IV‐TR), andFifth Edition (DSM‐5).

In the DSM‐IV and DSM‐5, there are three different subdiagnoses, where particular symptoms are identified and classified: the 'predominantly inattentive type'; the 'predominantly hyperactive‐impulsive type'; and the 'combined' type, which presents with both hyperactive‐impulsive and inattentive symptoms (Willcut 2012).

Comorbid disorders, such as behavioural disorders (e.g. oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder), depression, anxiety, bipolar disorder, tics, motor skill development disturbance, learning difficulties, and verbal and cognitive difficulties are common in ADHD (Newcorn 2008; Schmidt 2009; Yoshimasu 2012; Czamara 2013; Perroud 2014).

ADHD is associated with negative social outcomes such as severe social incompetence, and displays of off‐task, disruptive and rule‐violating behaviour (Kolko 1990; Landau 1991), health problems such as abuse of drugs or alcohol, and criminality later in life (Barkley 2002; Dalsgaard 2002; Storebø 2014; Koisaari 2015).

ADHD is also associated with negative psychological outcomes such as an increased risk of developing personality disturbances and possibly psychotic conditions (Keshavan 2003; Storebø 2014).

Excessive weight and obesity are found in children with ADHD compared to children without ADHD (Cortese 2016). ADHD is associated with both early‐onset tobacco and alcohol use (Chang 2012). Similarly, ADHD comorbidity with conduct disorder can lead to adverse outcomes in academic achievement, failure to complete high school, criminality, substance use disorder, and unemployment (Erskine 2016).

ADHD seems to increase premature mortality by 50%, compared to individuals without ADHD, in a 24.9 million person‐years Danish cohort study (Dalsgaard 2015). A weakness of this study is that it did not include medical treatment of ADHD in the analysis as a possible confounder for the relationship between ADHD and mortality (Dalsgaard 2015). There have been some reports of sudden death in children and adults treated with stimulant treatment but it is unclear if these are related directly to methylphenidate (US FDA 2011). More research is being conducted on this topic.

Pharmacological management of ADHD

The drug most commonly used for the treatment of ADHD in children and adolescents is methylphenidate (a stimulant); atomoxetine, and dexamphetamine (another stimulant) are used less often (NICE 2009; NICE 2018). Storebø and colleagues conducted a comprehensive Cochrane Review investigating the short‐term benefits and harms of methylphenidate for children and adolescents. This review concluded that there is a possible small beneficial effect on ADHD symptoms, general behaviour, and quality of life, and that methylphenidate does not seem to increase the risk of serious adverse events in the short‐term but is associated with a relatively high risk of non‐serious adverse events in general (Storebø 2015). However, there were a number of limitations in the included trials such as lack of blinding in spite of placebo use, outcome reporting bias, and heterogeneity, which resulted in the evidence being rated as low to very low. The authors concluded that there is high need for long‐term randomised placebo tablet ('active placebo')‐controlled clinical trials, without risks of systematic errors, that investigate the effect of methylphenidate treatment for children and adolescents with ADHD (Storebø 2015). Whilst medication can help in the management of core behavioural symptoms, it is not designed to address skills deficits.

The most common adverse effects associated with methylphenidate are: headache, sleep problems, tiredness, and decreased appetite (Storebø 2018). Methylphenidate also affects the children's height and weight curves (Schachar 1997; Swanson 2007). Dexamphetamine seems to affect children's sleep and can result in dry mouth, thirst, weight loss, decreased appetite and stomach ache, and increase the risk of regressive, dependent behaviour and psychosis (Punja 2016). Atomoxetine is associated with pain, nausea and vomiting, decreased appetite with associated weight loss, dizziness, and slight increases in heart rate and blood pressure (Wolraich 2007).

Research on the neurochemical basis of ADHD has primarily focused on the neurotransmitters noradrenaline and dopamine, and their receptors in the central nervous system. Although the neurophysiological mechanism of the medications are not clearly known, it is presumed that their effects on symptoms of ADHD are explained primarily by the stimulant effects of dopaminergic and, to some extent, noradrenergic neurotransmission(Kadesjö 2002). Selective noradrenaline‐acting tricyclic medications and alpha‐2‐adrenergic agonists have also been observed to reduce symptoms of ADHD in children (Zametkin 1987).

Description of the intervention

Social skills training is developed with the characteristics of ADHD in mind in order to improve and maintain the individual’s social skills and prevent or alleviate social difficulties. Social skills are complex and involve different aspects of cognitions, emotions, and behaviour. Programmes vary in their focus on different aspects of social skills but tend to focus on problem‐solving, control of emotions, and verbal and non‐verbal communication. The training generally focuses on teaching the children how to 'read' the subtle cues in social interaction such as learning to wait for their turn, knowing when to shift topics during a conversation, and being able to recognise the emotional expressions of others (Fohlmann 2009a;Fohlmann 2009b [pers comm]). The children may be taught to practice how to adjust their verbal and non‐verbal behaviour in their social interactions. It may also include efforts to change the children’s cognitive assessment of the 'social world'. Social skills training also includes teaching social norms, social 'rules', and expectations of others (Liberman 1988).

Training may also focus on emotion regulation, such as the child's ability to deal with, manage, express, and control his or her emotions. An inability to regulate both positive and negative emotion has been associated with disorders such as ADHD and conduct disorder (Walcott 2004). Emotional self‐regulation is an important aspect of resilience. Children who have effective strategies for dealing with disappointment, loss, and other upsetting events are more likely to bounce back from adversity than those who do not. Managing positive emotion is also important. Success socially and at school depends on the ability to control exuberance, when appropriate.

Social skills training often consists of role play, exercises and games, as well as homework. Social skills training is often taught in groups and is a relatively short intervention typically lasting between eight and 12 weeks. The duration of each group session is usually 50 to 90 minutes. Treatment frequency can range from a couple of times per month to several times a week. Often the programme also involves parents or teachers. Parental groups are typically included to give the parents the opportunity to support the children's training in the social skills groups by understanding the nature of ADHD and the content of the treatment programme. Teachers are often included to facilitate learning objectives and to coordinate social skills training domains, such as homework.

How the intervention might work

The effect of the intervention may be measured by looking at social skills per se, or by looking at a more global assessment of psychological functioning such as the quality of peer relationships, emotional competences, and general behaviour. Social skills training includes procedures to identify problems and set goals in collaboration with the participant. Through role play, exercises and games, participants demonstrate the required skills, and positive or corrective feedback is given to them accordingly. By social modelling and behavioural practice, participants observe and repeat the skills until they become more generalised. Homework assignments are then given to motivate participants to implement these communications in real‐life situations (Almerie 2015).

Why it is important to do this review

Several randomised clinical trials suggest that social skills training may help children with ADHD (Pfiffner 1997; Antshel 2003; Pfiffner 2007). Social skills training may be effective alone, as an adjunct to medication, or both. However, the evidence on social skills training is unclear, and systematic reviews are necessary to evaluate its effectiveness and potential harms. It is always important to investigate the benefits and harms of interventions in order to not waste valuable resources in clinical practice.

Like medical treatments, the effects of social skills training do not always appear to endure. Some trials indicate that not all children benefit from social skills training, potentially due to lack of parental engagement during treatment (Kadesjö 2002). Some have argued that social skills training groups can have a negative effect on children with behavioural problems because the children’s aggressive and restless behaviour can limit their ability to learn social skills. This, paradoxically, can increase negative behaviour (Mager 2005).

This review is an update of our systematic review published in 2011, which, at the time, was the only high‐quality review on the topic (Storebø 2011). Many new trials have been conducted since 2011 and this update includes more than the double number of trials compared to our original review.

We have identified two meta‐analyses and one review investigating the efficacy of social skills or psychosocial training for children with ADHD. Two of these studies found a significant effect of social skills and psychosocial treatment (De Boo 2007;Majewicz‐Hefley 2007) and one did not find any significant effect (Van der Oord 2008). We also found a meta‐analysis which assessed the effectiveness of social skills training for students with behaviour disorders (Kavale 1997). This meta‐analysis also did not find any significant benefit from social skills training. The review and these meta‐analyses have serious methodological deficits. None of them were systematic reviews like ours and they all lacked a published protocol before they were conducted. Furthermore, none of them systematically evaluated systematic errors (bias) or random errors (play of chance), and therefore their results are questionable. A systematic review published in 2019 on stand‐alone social skills training for youth with ADHD concluded that social skills training implemented without additional treatment components like parent support, showed improvements on some areas of social functioning (Willis 2019). However, this review suffered from a very limited search strategy and did not evaluate systematic errors (bias) in the included trials. In our Cochrane review in 2011 on the topic, we were unable to demonstrate clear benefits or harms of social skills training (Storebø 2011).

Objectives

To present the available evidence on the beneficial and harmful effects of social skills training in children and adolescents with ADHD.

Methods

Criteria for considering studies for this review

Types of studies

Randomised clinical trials (RCTs) investigating social skills training alone or as an adjunct to pharmacological treatment compared to pharmacological treatment. The comparison groups were no intervention or waiting‐list control.

Types of participants

Children and adolescents between five and 18 years of age and diagnosed with ADHD according to the DSM‐IV, DSM‐IV‐TR or DSM‐5, or with hyperkinetic disorder according to the ICD‐10. The main term used in the DSM‐IV is 'ADHD 314', which is divided into three subdiagnoses: 'predominantly inattentive type' (314.00), 'predominantly hyperactive/impulsive type' (314.01), and 'combined type' (314.02). We also included trials that used the DSM‐IV diagnosis of 'ADHD unspecified' (314.9), as well as other diagnostic categories from earlier DSM systems (DSM‐III; DSM‐IV‐R), and 'hyperkinetic disorder' from the ICD‐9.

In addition, we included participants with a diagnosis of ADHD based on a cut‐off score from a validated diagnostic assessment instrument: for example, Conners’ Parent Rating Scales (Conners 1998). We also included participants with different kinds of comorbidity such as conduct or oppositional disorders, depression, attachment disorder, or anxiety disorders.

Types of interventions

We considered all forms of social skills training where training focused on behavioural and cognitive‐behavioural efforts to improve social skills and emotional competence. This included behavioural and cognitive treatments focusing on teaching the children how to 'read' the subtle cues in social interaction, such as learning to wait for their turn, knowing when to shift topics during a conversation, and being able to recognise the emotional expressions of others, as well as social 'rules', and expectations of others.

We included trials comparing social skills training versus either no intervention or waiting‐list control. We considered these control groups to be equal. Therefore, we did not distinguish between the control groups, but analysed the trials with relevant outcomes together in the same comparison. We also included trials with concurrent medical treatment if the medication was administered equally in both groups. In further updates of the review, we will include trials with social skills training versus placebo or sham intervention, as described in our protocol (Storebø 2010).

Types of outcome measures

Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills in school or at home, measured at post‐treatment and longest follow‐up, by well‐established and validated instruments such as the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS; Gresham 1990) or Conners' Behaviour Rating Scales (CBRS;Conners 2008a).

  2. Emotional competencies in school or at home, measured at post‐treatment and longest follow‐up, by well‐established and validated instruments such as the Emotion Regulation Checklist (ERC; Hannesdottir 2017).

  3. General behaviour in school or at home, measured at post‐treatment and longest follow‐up, by well‐established and validated instruments such as the Achenbach Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach 1991).

Secondary outcomes
  1. Core ADHD symptoms of inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity, measured at post‐treatment and longest follow‐up, by well‐established and validated instruments such as Conners' Parents’ Rating Scales (Conners 1998; Conners 2008b).

  2. Performance and grades in school, measured at post‐treatment and longest follow‐up, by well‐established and validated instruments.

  3. Participant or parent (or both) satisfaction with treatment, measured as continuous outcomes by psychometrically validated instruments such as the Client Satisfaction Questionnaire (Attkisson 1982).

  4. Adverse events. We included both severe and non‐severe adverse events. We defined serious adverse events as any event that led to death, was life‐threatening, required inpatient hospitalisation or prolongation of existing hospitalisation, resulted in persistent or significant disability, or any important medical event that may have jeopardised the participant's health or required intervention to prevent it (ICH 1996). We considered all other adverse events as non‐serious.

Search methods for identification of studies

We ran searches for the previous review up to March 2011, using the search strategies reported in Storebø 2011. For this update, we made some changes to the databases we searched (see Differences between protocol and review).

Electronic searches

For this update, we searched the following electronic databases up to July 2018.

  1. Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL; 2018, Issue 6), in the Cochrane Library (searched 11 July 2018).

  2. MEDLINE Ovid (1948 to 11 July 2018).

  3. Embase Ovid (1980 to 11 July 2018).

  4. ERIC EBSCOhost (Education Resources Information Center; 1966 to 11 July 2018).

  5. CINAHL EBSCOhost (Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature; 1980 to 11 July 2018).

  6. PsycINFO Ovid (1806 to 11 July 2018).

  7. Sociological Abstracts ProQuest (1952 to 11 July 2018).

  8. ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Global (searched 11 July 2018).

  9. ClinicalTrials.gov (clinicaltrials.gov; searched 12 July 2018).

  10. World Health Organization International Clinical Trials Registry Platform (WHO ICTRP; www.who.int/ictrp/en; searched 12 July 2018).

The search strategies for this update are shown in Appendix 1. We did not limit our searches by language, year of publication, or type or status of publication. We sought translation of the relevant sections of non‐English language articles.

Searching other resources

We searched the following online proceedings for potentially relevant conference abstracts.

  1. 2nd International Congress on ADHD: from childhood to adult disease; 2009 May 21 to 24; Vienna, Austria (International Congress on ADHD 2009).

  2. 3rd International Congress on ADHD: from childhood to adult disease; 2011 May 26 to 29; Berlin, Germany (World Congress on ADHD 2011).

  3. 4th World Congress on ADHD: from childhood to adult disease; 2013 June 6 to 9; Milan, Italy (World Congress on ADHD 2013).

  4. 5th World Congress on ADHD: from child to adult disorder; 2015 May 28 to 31; Glasgow, Scotland (World Congress on ADHD 2015).

  5. 6th World Congress on ADHD: from child to adult disorder; 2017 Apr 20 to 23; Vancouver, Canada (World Congress on ADHD 2017).

  6. Eunethydis 1st International ADHD Conference: from data to best clinical practice; 2010 May 26 to 28; Amsterdam, The Netherlands (Eunethydis 2010).

  7. Nordic ADHD Konference: livslange perspektiver og specielle behov [lifelong perspectives and special needs]; 2010 May 19 to 20; Aalborg, Denmark (Nordic ADHD konference 2010).

  8. InternationaI Society for Research in Child and Adolescent Psychopathology (ISRCAP) conference; 2009 June 17‐20; Seattle, Washington, USA.

  9. CADDRA: 14th Annual ADHD Conference; 2018 Nov 10 to 11; Calgary, Canada.

In addition, we contacted 176 experts in the field for information about possible unpublished or ongoing RCTs, and received responses from 15 (a list of those contacted is available from the review contact author).

Data collection and analysis

We conducted the review according to the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011). In the following section, we report only the methods that we were able to use in this update. Methods that we had planned to use as per our published protocol (Storebø 2010), but could not (e.g. cluster‐randomised trials), are reported in Table 2.

1. Methods not used in this update.

Section Protocol Review
Types of outcome measures We did not define what we meant by adverse events. We added a definition of adverse events according to the International Committee of Harmonization guidelines (ICH 1996), because many of the studies included pharmaceutical treatment and it is not known whether social skills training might have adverse events.
We stated that we would measure the three primary and the first two secondary outcomes at short‐term (up to six months), medium‐term (six to 12 months), and long‐term (more than 12 months) follow‐up. We changed this to end of treatment and at the longest follow‐up because we did not have data for the planned three time points.
We did not prespecify the most important comparisons for the 'Summary of findings' table. We reported a total of seven outcomes in the 'Summary of findings' table as per Cochrane recommendations; three primary outcomes (social skills, emotional competencies and general behaviour) and the first secondary outcome (ADHD symptoms).
Assessment of risk of bias in included studies We had not planned to evaluate blinding of participants and personnel. We assessed the blinding of participants and personnel, as this is also important to assess in trials investigating psychosocial interventions, even if it is very difficult to do in these types of trials.
We stated that we would only use studies at low risk (or lower risk) of bias in the meta‐analysis. We changed the decision to restrict the meta‐analysis to studies at comparable risk of bias (for example, all low risk of bias, all unclear risk of bias, or all high risk of bias), and performed sensitivity analyses accordingly. We decided to change this as there were very few trials at low risk of bias in this field.
We stated that we would assess 'baseline imbalance' and 'early stopping' as risk of bias domains. We did not assess these baseline domains. The randomisation procedure should give an even distribution of confounding factors and baseline imbalance.
Dealing with missing data We intended to assess the impact of missing dichotomous data in the results by applying procedures for 'intention‐to‐treat' and 'best‐case/worst‐case scenarios'. We were unable to perform this analysis as there were no dichotomous data.
Measures of treatment effect Dichotomous data
We planned to analyse dichotomous data as risk ratios and present these with 95% confidence intervals (CIs), and to calculate the risk difference and, where there was a significant effect with the intervention and reasonable homogeneity of studies (that is, clinical, methodological, or statistical heterogeneity was within reasonable limits), the number needed to treat for an additional beneficial outcome (Higgins 2011, Section 9.2).
We did not do this as there were no dichotomous data.
Unit of analysis issues Cluster‐randomised studies
We stated that we thought investigators would have presented their results after appropriately checking for clustering effects (robust standard errors or hierarchical linear models). We planed to contact the investigators for further information if this was unclear. Where appropriate checks were not used, we planned to request and re‐analyse individual participant data using multilevel models that check for clustering. Following this, we planned to analyse effect sizes and standard errors in RevMan 5 (Review Manager 2014), using the generic inverse method (Higgins 2011, Section 9.3.2). If there was insufficient information to check for clustering, we would have entered outcome data into RevMan 5 using individuals as the units of analysis, and then conducting a sensitivity analysis to assess the potential biasing effects of inadequately controlled clustered studies (Donner 2002). See 'Sensitivity analysis' below.
We did not find any cluster‐randomised trials.
Assessment of reporting biases We did not state that we would use Egger's test to test for small‐study effects. We performed Egger's statistical test for small‐study effects.
Subgroup analysis and investigation of heterogeneity We planned to perform subgroup analyses according to the following categories.
  1. Social skills training in a group setting compared to individual social skills training

  2. Children with ADHD plus depression, attachment disorder, or anxiety disorders compared to children with ADHD without these comorbidities

  3. Studies with low risk of bias compared to studies with high risk

We were not able to perform these subgroup analyses due to lack of sufficient data.
Sensitivity analysis We stated that we would repeat the analysis taking into consideration the different methods used to handle the missing data and the potential biasing effects of inadequately controlled clustered studies. We did not perform this analysis due to a lack of necessary data and, consequently, have analysed the data as reported.

ADHD: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.

Selection of studies

Eight reviewers (OJS, NP, EGF, MEA, BT, MS, HC and SJ) independently evaluated and selected trials for inclusion. Having removed duplicates, they assessed the titles and abstracts of all records generated by the search and excluded those that clearly did not meet the inclusion criteria: for example, non‐randomised trials or trials with participants outside the specified age range (Criteria for considering studies for this review). Next, they retrieved the full‐text reports for those trials deemed relevant or for which more information was needed to determine relevance and assessed them for eligibility. The review authors discussed differing interpretations regarding eligibility and consulted a third review author (ES) for those cases where they could not reach an agreement.

We have listed relevant RCTs that did not fulfil the inclusion criteria with reasons for exclusion in the Characteristics of excluded studies table. We recorded our selection process in a study flow diagram (Moher 2009).

Dealing with duplicate publication

We collected multiple reports of the same study to maximise data collection.

Data extraction and management

Working in pairs, eight review authors (OJS, MEA, EGF, BT, HC, SJH, NP, and MS) independently extracted data onto a data collection form (Appendix 2). We extracted data on participants, study design and methods, interventions, outcomes, and relevant data for 'Risk of bias' assessments. We resolved differences by discussion.

OJS entered the data into Review Manager 5 (RevMan 5) (Review Manager 2014).

In cases of lack of data, for the use of e.g. tables with sociodemographic data, 'risk of bias' assessment, or the analysis, or where data in the published study reports were unclear, we contacted the trial authors requesting them to clarify the missing information (see Dealing with missing data).

Assessment of risk of bias in included studies

For each included RCT, eight review authors (OJS, MEA, EGF, BT, HC, SJH, NP, and MS), working in pairs, independently evaluated the following 'Risk of bias' components: random sequence generation; allocation concealment; blinding of participants and personnel; blinding of outcome assessment; incomplete outcome data; selective outcome reporting; vested interest bias; and other sources of bias. We assigned trials to one of three categories (low risk of bias, uncertain risk of bias, and high risk of bias), according to guidelines in the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011, section 8.2.1) and from the Cochrane Hepato‐Biliary Group (Cochrane Hepato‐Biliary Group 2019) (see Appendix 3). We resolved disagreements by discussion. We used the results of the 'Risk of bias' assessment to inform the GRADE assessment.

Measures of treatment effect

Continuous data

We compared the mean score between the two intervention groups to give a mean difference (MD) and presented this with 95% confidence intervals (CIs). We wanted to use the overall MD, where possible, to compare the outcome measures from trials. However, because many of the included trials used different rating scales for measuring the same construct we used the standardised mean difference (SMD) in many analyses. For the primary outcome, teacher‐rated social skills at end of treatment, we transformed the MD and standard deviation (SD) from the different rating scales used in this analysis to the MD and SD of a commonly used scale, namely the Social Skills Rating Scale (SSRS). We reported this MD in the results section as well as in the abstract and we compared it to a plausible minimal relevant difference of 10% on this scale.

Unit of analysis issues

We did not encounter any unit of analysis issues. Our strategies for dealing with these can be found in Table 2 (see also Storebø 2010).

Dealing with missing data

We sought to retrieve any missing data from the trial authors. Overall, we wrote to 17 authors, eight of whom supplied us with missing sociodemographic data and missing information about methodology; some also supplied us with missing statistics. If data remained unavailable, we tried to estimate the missing data using the available information (e.g. if the standard deviation (SD) was missing, we estimated it from the standard error, if reported). When we were not able to obtain missing data, we conducted analyses using available (incomplete) data.

Assessment of heterogeneity

We assessed clinical heterogeneity by examining variability in the participants, interventions, and outcomes described in each included trial. We assessed methodological heterogeneity by inspecting variability in the designs of the trials, and statistical heterogeneity by assessing the difference in the trials' intervention effects. We assessed heterogeneity between trials by visual inspection of the forest plot for overlapping CIs, using the Chi2 test for homogeneity with a significance level of α (alpha) = 0.10, and the I2 statistic for quantifying inconsistency (estimating the percentage of variation in effect estimates due to heterogeneity rather than sampling error). We judged I2 values of 0% to 40% to indicate little heterogeneity; 30% to 60%, moderate heterogeneity; 50% to 90%, substantial heterogeneity; and 75% to 100%, considerable heterogeneity (Higgins 2011). Furthermore, we explored potential reasons for the heterogeneity by examining individual trial characteristics and conducting subgroup analyses (Subgroup analysis and investigation of heterogeneity).

Assessment of reporting biases

We handled different forms of reporting bias, especially publication bias and outcome reporting bias, according to the recommendations in the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011, Section 10.1). We drew funnel plots (estimated differences in treatment effects against their standard error), and we performed Egger's statistical test for small‐study effects (Egger 1997). There are several reasons for the asymmetry of a funnel plot; for example, true heterogeneity, poor methodological quality, or publication bias (Higgins 2011, section 10.4.1).

Data synthesis

We included and analysed trials undertaken in any setting; for instance, in groups, in the home, or at a centre. We summarised data in a meta‐analysis when they were available and if clinical heterogeneity was not excessive (for example, there was not too much variability in participants' characteristics). We performed statistical analysis in RevMan 5 (Review Manager 2014), according to recommendations in the latest version of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011, Section 9.4.1). We synthesised data by using final values and the inverse variance method in the meta‐analyses. We generally used the random‐effects model because we expected differences in the treatments. The fixed‐effect model is used when there is an assumption that the observed differences between the study results are just due to ‘play of chance’. When there is heterogeneity that cannot be explained as ‘play of chance’, it is common to use the random‐effects model. A random‐effects model has the assumption that apparent differences between study effects are random, but the estimated difference follows a normal distribution. This method gives more weight to small trials, whereas the fixed‐effect model gives more weight to large trials. We therefore conducted both fixed‐effect and random‐effects models, and checked for differences between these methods of analyses (Higgins 2011, Section 9.5.4). If both models gave the same results, we reported the results from the random‐effects model only. For some outcomes we were unable to conduct a meta‐analysis because the outcomes were reported only in a single study. For these outcomes, we provided a narrative description of the results.

Diversity‐adjusted required information size and Trial Sequential Analysis

Trial Sequential Analysis (TSA) is a tool for controlling risks of type I and type II errors in cumulative meta‐analyses, and gives a valuable overview of the number of participants needed to make a firm evaluation of a possible intervention effect (Brok 2008; Wetterslev 2008; Brok 2009; Thorlund 2009; Wetterslev 2009; Wetterslev 2017).

Comparable to the a priori sample size estimation in a single RCT, a meta‐analysis should include an information size (IS) at least as large as the sample size of an adequately powered single study to reduce the risks of random errors. The TSA provides the required information size (RIS) for a meta‐analysis, adjusting the significance level for sparse data and repetitive testing on accumulating data, to avoid the increased risk of random error (Wetterslev 2008; Wetterslev 2009; Wetterslev 2017).

Multiple analyses of accumulating data from new emergent trials leads to ‘repeated significant testing’, and use of the conventional P value is prone to exacerbate the risk of a type I random error (Lau 1995; Berkley 1996). Meta‐analyses that do not reach the RIS are analysed with trial sequential monitoring boundaries, which are analogous to interim monitoring boundaries in a single study (Wetterslev 2008; Wetterslev 2017). This approach is crucial in coming updates of this review.

We used an a priori assumption that the minimal relevant clinical intervention effect was 4.0 points. This is approximately ½ SD on the used scale, which can be used as a minimal clinical relevant difference (Norman 2003).

We calculated the diversity‐adjusted required information size (DARIS; that is the number of participants required to detect or reject a specific intervention effect in a meta‐analysis), and performed a TSA for the primary outcome (teacher‐rated social skills competences) at the end of treatment, based on the following a priori assumptions:

  1. the SD of the primary outcome of 9.5 points;

  2. an anticipated minimal relevant difference (MIREDIF) of 4.0 points;

  3. a maximum type I error of 2.5% (due to three primary outcomes; Jakobsen 2014);

  4. a maximum type II error of 10% (minimum 90% power; Castellini 2018); and

  5. the diversity observed in the meta‐analysis.

Subgroup analysis and investigation of heterogeneity

We conducted subgroup analysis both where we found statistically significant differences between intervention groups, and in other cases to make hypotheses about the subgroups mentioned below.

We performed the following subgroup analyses.

  1. Children aged five to 11 years compared to children aged 12 to 18 years;

  2. ADHD with comorbidity compared to ADHD without comorbidity;

  3. Social skills training only compared to social skills training supported by parent training;

  4. Social skills training, parent training and medication compared to social skills training and parent training without medication;

  5. No‐intervention control group compared to waiting‐list control group with possible minor active intervention components.

Sensitivity analysis

We assessed the robustness of the results by conducting sensitivity analyses in which we repeated the analysis:

  1. excluding the trial with longest treatment duration or the largest trial; and

  2. using different statistical models (fixed‐effect or random‐effects models) (Higgins 2011).

'Summary of findings' table

We constructed 'Summary of findings' tables using GRADE software (GRADEpro GDT 2015) for the comparison 'social skills training compared to no intervention'. We included three primary outcomes (social skills, emotional competencies and general behaviour) and one secondary outcome (ADHD core symptoms) assessed at end of treatment in the table.

We used the GRADE approach to assess the quality of the evidence associated with each of these outcomes (Guyatt 2008). The GRADE approach appraises the quality of a body of evidence based on the extent to which one can be confident that an estimate of effect or association reflects the item being assessed. Considerations are due to: within‐study risk of bias; directness of evidence; heterogeneity of the data; precision of effect estimates; and risk of publication bias (Balshem 2011; Guyatt 2011a; Guyatt 2011b; Guyatt 2011c; Guyatt 2011d; Guyatt 2011e; Guyatt 2011f; Guyatt 2011g; Andrews 2013a; Andrews 2013b; Brunetti 2013; Guyatt 2013a; Guyatt 2013b; Guyatt 2013c; Mustafa 2013).

Results

Description of studies

Results of the search

This updated review fully incorporates the results of searches conducted up to July 2018. We carried out electronic searches over five time periods: up to February 2009; February 2009 to June 2010; June 2010 to March 2011; March 2011 to May 2017; and May 2017 to July 2018. The number of unique records (i.e. number of records after duplicates were removed) generated by these searches were as follows.

  1. Up to February 2009 = 2500 (out of 3045);

  2. February 2009 until June 2010 = 200 (out of 643)

  3. June 2010 until March 2011 = 165 (out of 208)

  4. March 2011 until May 2017 = 1616 (out of 3229)

  5. May 2017 until July 2018 = 324 (out of 410)

To date, the electronic searches for this review have found 7535 records, plus an additional four records from searching other resources. Having removed duplicates, we screened 4805 records, and subsequently excluded 4492 as clearly irrelevant on the basis of title and abstract. We retrieved the full texts of the remaining 313 reports, which we assessed for eligibility against our selection criteria (Criteria for considering studies for this review). From these, we excluded 224 as irrelevant, formally excluded a further 39 with reasons (see Excluded studies), and included 25 trials (from 45 reports). We also identified four ongoing trials, and one which is awaiting classification (see Figure 1).

1.

1

Study flow diagram.

Included studies

This review includes 25 RCTs described in 45 reports. Of these 25 trials, one, Cohen 1981, did not have usable data to be included in the quantitative analysis (meta‐analysis). Another one of the trials had extreme values in some of the outcomes and we did not use the data on these outcomes (Tabaeian 2010).

See Characteristics of included studies tables for further details on each included study.

Setting

Thirteen trials were carried out in North America: 12 in the US (Bloomquist 1991; Pfiffner 1997; MTA 1999; Antshel 2003; Tutty 2003; Abikoff 2004; Pfiffner 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Pfiffner 2014; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Waxmonsky 2016). Of the remaining 12 trials, six were carried out in Asia; three in Iran (Tabaeian 2010; Azad 2014; Meftagh 2014a), two in China (Yuk‐chi 2005; Qian 2017), and one in South Korea (Choi 2015). Five trials were conducted in Europe; one apiece in Denmark (Storebø 2012), Iceland (Hannesdottir 2017), Germany (Schramm 2016), The Netherlands (Van der Oord 2007), and one which took place in both Belgium and The Netherlands (Bul 2016): One trial was conducted in Australia (Wilkes Gillan 2016), and one in Canada (Cohen 1981).

The majority of trials were conducted in an outpatient setting; six trials were carried out in a clinical setting (Pfiffner 1997; Tabaeian 2010; Storebø 2012; Azad 2014; Hannesdottir 2017; Qian 2017).

Participants

The 25 RCTs included a total of 2690 participants. The majority of trials included children between five and 13 years of age; a single trial included adolescents between 12 and 17 years of age (Schramm 2016). All participants were diagnosed with ADHD using tools that had been accepted for inclusion in this review. All of these diagnostic tools were based on the international DSM (DSM‐III; DSM‐IV; DSM‐IV‐R; DSM‐IV‐TR; DSM‐5) or ICD diagnostic systems ICD‐10), or a cut‐off score from the Conners' Rating Scale (Conners 1998; Conners 2008a; Conners 2008b).

Six trials did not specify intelligence (IQ) as inclusion or exclusion criteria (Pfiffner 1997; Tutty 2003; Tabaeian 2010; Meftagh 2014a; Azad 2014; Schramm 2016), and the remaining trials excluded children with low IQ (ranging from < 70 to < 90).

All but seven trials (Pfiffner 1997; Antshel 2003; Van der Oord 2007; Azad 2014; Meftagh 2014a; Choi 2015; Schramm 2016) excluded patients with one or more comorbid mental disorders – typically autism spectrum disorder, psychosis, or neurological disorder. Two trials used comorbidity as an exclusion criterion (Tutty 2003; Tabaeian 2010). Eighteen trials reported on different types of comorbidities, such as oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, and anxiety disorder for the children in addition to the ADHD diagnosis.

The distribution of boys to girls was almost equal in two trials (Pfiffner 2014; Choi 2015). In the remaining trials, boys outnumbered girls. The number of boys to girls was: 2:1 in seven trials (Bloomquist 1991; Pfiffner 1997; Pfiffner 2007; Storebø 2012; Evans 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Hannesdottir 2017); 3:1 in three trials (Antshel 2003; Tutty 2003; Pfiffner 2016); 4:1 in four trials (MTA 1999; Waxmonsky 2010; Bul 2016; Qian 2017); 6:1 in two trials (Schramm 2016; Wilkes Gillan 2016); 7:1 in one trial (Cohen 1981); 9:1 in one trial (Yuk‐chi 2005) and 14:1 in one trial (Abikoff 2004). The participants were all males in one trial (Tabaeian 2010), and three trials provided no information on the sex of the participants (Van der Oord 2007; Azad 2014; Meftagh 2014a).

Participants were between 80% and 100% Caucasian in six trials (Bloomquist 1991; Pfiffner 1997; Antshel 2003; Abikoff 2004; Van der Oord 2007; Waxmonsky 2010). Ethnicity was more mixed in seven other trials: 16% to 74% Caucasian; 3% to 75% Hispanic; 0% to 16% Asian; and 5% to 20% Afro‐American (MTA 1999; Tutty 2003; Pfiffner 2007; Pfiffner 2014; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Waxmonsky 2016). In four trials, ethnicity was stated with reference to the country of origin, with all or almost all being Canadian (Cohen 1981), Chinese (Yuk‐chi 2005), Iranian (Tabaeian 2010), or Australian (Wilkes Gillan 2016). Ethnicity was not explicitly described in eight trials (Storebø 2012; Azad 2014; Meftagh 2014a; Choi 2015; Bul 2016; Schramm 2016; Hannesdottir 2017; Qian 2017). Eleven trials included and controlled for a measure of socioeconomic status (Pfiffner 1997; Yuk‐chi 2005; Pfiffner 2014; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Schramm 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Tutty 2003; Van der Oord 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Wilkes Gillan 2016).

Sample size

There was considerable variation in sample sizes between trials. The number of participants randomised per study ranged from 24 to 576 participants in all trials. Only three trials reported a sample size calculation before the start of the study (MTA 1999; Storebø 2012; Bul 2016).

Interventions
Experimental

The 25 trials had different but comparable experimental interventions. The interventions named were: social skills training (Pfiffner 1997; Antshel 2003; Tabaeian 2010; Storebø 2012; Choi 2015; Hannesdottir 2017); cognitive behavioural intervention (Cohen 1981; Bloomquist 1991); meta‐cognitive training (Azad 2014); multimodal behavioural/psychosocial therapy (MTA 1999; Abikoff 2004; Van der Oord 2007); behavioural therapy/treatment (Pfiffner 2007; Waxmonsky 2010); behavioural and social skills treatment (Tutty 2003; Waxmonsky 2016); challenging horizon program (CHP; Evans 2016); children's verbal self‐instruction training (Meftagh 2014a); child life and attention skills treatment (CLAS; Pfiffner 2014; Pfiffner 2016), executive skills training (Qian 2017); learning skills training (Schramm 2016); different play or game‐based intervention (Bul 2016; Wilkes Gillan 2016); and psychosocial treatment (Yuk‐chi 2005). We considered that all these interventions were comparable and based on a cognitive behavioural model. Throughout the rest of the review, we referred to the experimental child interventions as 'child social skills training', in accordance with the Description of the intervention section.

The duration of the intervention varied between five and eight weeks in seven trials (Pfiffner 1997; Antshel 2003; Tutty 2003; Waxmonsky 2010; Storebø 2012; Azad 2014; Hannesdottir 2017), and between 10 and 16 weeks in 13 trials (Cohen 1981; Bloomquist 1991; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Tabaeian 2010; Meftagh 2014a; Pfiffner 2014; Choi 2015; Bul 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Wilkes Gillan 2016; Qian 2017). In two trials, the intervention lasted for 24 weeks (Yuk‐chi 2005; Schramm 2016), and, in one trial apiece, the intervention lasted for one year (Evans 2016), 14 months (MTA 1999), and two years (Abikoff 2004).

Five trials used social skills training for children plus parent training (Cohen 1981; Antshel 2003; Tutty 2003; Abikoff 2004; Waxmonsky 2010); one of these trials also administered academic organisational skills training and individual psychotherapy (Abikoff 2004). Seven trials used a combination of social skills training for children, parent training, and teacher consultations in the experimental group (Bloomquist 1991; Yuk‐chi 2005; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Pfiffner 2014; Pfiffner 2016; Schramm 2016). One trial used social skills training for children, parent training, teacher consultations, and classroom behavioural intervention in the experimental group (MTA 1999), and another used social skills training for children and parent training plus standard treatment in the experimental group (Storebø 2012). One trial used either social skills training for children or social skills training for children plus parent training (Pfiffner 1997).

One trial used social skills training only in the experimental group (Choi 2015). Another trial included the Challenging Horizons Program, which is designed to target different skills such as organisational and social skills (Evans 2016), whereas other trials used life skills (Pfiffner 2014) and group‐based social skills training as the experimental intervention (Hannesdottir 2017). One trial used verbal self‐instruction as the experimental programme (Meftagh 2014a), and another used a play‐based intervention (Wilkes Gillan 2016). Finally, one trial used a specific intervention targeting skills related to mood and behaviour (Waxmonsky 2016).

Two trials used meta‐cognitive training (Azad 2014; Qian 2017); one had a social game intervention, which targeted cooperation and planning skills among others (Bul 2016), and the other used behavioural and cognitive training (Schramm 2016).

Seven trials included concurrent medical treatment with ADHD medication in both the experimental and control groups (Cohen 1981; Antshel 2003; Tutty 2003; Abikoff 2004; Waxmonsky 2010; Tabaeian 2010; Storebø 2012).

Control

Eight trials used medications in the experimental group and as the only treatment in the control group (Cohen 1981; MTA 1999; Antshel 2003; Tutty 2003; Abikoff 2004; Yuk‐chi 2005; Van der Oord 2007; Waxmonsky 2010); one of these trials also included a no‐treatment control group (Cohen 1981). Two trials used standard treatment in the experimental group and as the only treatment in the control group (Storebø 2012; Bul 2016). Nine trials used a waiting‐list or no‐intervention control group without medication in any of the groups (Bloomquist 1991; Pfiffner 1997; Pfiffner 2007; Tabaeian 2010; Azad 2014; Choi 2015; Schramm 2016; Wilkes Gillan 2016; Hannesdottir 2017). One trial used parent training in the experimental group and as the only treatment in the control group (Pfiffner 2014). Four trials used a control group with some active intervention elements, however, the researchers did not provide any direct intervention to the individuals in this condition (Pfiffner 2014; Evans 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Qian 2017). One trial did not describe the control group (Meftagh 2014a).

Outcome measures

In the following section, we did not describe the measures used in one study, Tabaeian 2010, as we were not able to identify these reliably from the information provided in the translated report.

Social skills

See Table 3.

2. Measures of social skills from included studies.
Measures Description Number of studies Ratings
Teacher Parent Child Observer
Social Skills Rating Scale (SSRS) Three‐point Likert scale, ranging from zero (never) to two (often); higher scores indicate better social skills 9 Pfiffner 1997 Pfiffner 1997
MTA 1999 MTA 1999 MTA 1999
Pfiffner 2007
Antshel 2003 Antshel 2003
Abikoff 2004 Abikoff 2004
Van der Oord 2007
Waxmonsky 2010
Waxmonsky 2016
Hannesdottir 2017
SSRS: Cooperation Subscale Three‐point Likert scale, ranging from zero (never) to two (often); higher scores indicate better cooperation 1 Bul 2016 Bul 2016
Social Skills Improvement System (SSIS) Four‐point rating scale, ranging from zero (never) to three (almost always); higher scores indicate better social skills. 3 Pfiffner 2014 Pfiffner 2014
Evans 2016 Evans 2016
Pfiffner 2016 Pfiffner 2016
Teacher Report ‐ Walker‐McConnell Scale of Social Competence and School Adjustment Five‐point rating scale, ranging from one (never occurs) to five (frequently occurs); higher scores indicate better social skills 1 Bloomquist 1991
Weiss Functional Impairment Scale ‐ Parent Form (WFIRS‐P): Social Activities Subscale Four‐point rating scale, ranging from zero (never or not at all) to three (very often or very much); higher scores indicate better social skills 1 Qian 2017
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): Prosocial Behavior Subscale Three‐point rating scale, ranging from zero (not true) to two (certainly true); higher scores indicate better social skills. 1 Schramm 2016 Schramm 2016 Schramm 2016
Conners Behavior Rating Scale (CBRS): Social Problems Subscale Four‐point rating scale, ranging from zero (not true at all) to three (very much true); higher scores indicate better social skills 1 Storebø 2012
Social Interaction Observation Code Recording frequencies of positive, negative or neutral behaviour, including observations of negative behaviour 1 Abikoff 2004
Test of Social Skill Knowledge Scored from one (low knowledge) to 15 (high knowledge); higher scores indicate better social skills 1 Pfiffner 1997
Observation in Classrooms Observing children for three × eight‐minute periods during a one‐hour period for two categories of behaviour: play behaviour and social behaviour 1 Cohen 1981
Test of Playfulness: Skillfulness Four‐point rating scale, ranging from zero (unskilled) to three (highly skilled); higher scores indicate better social skills 1 Wilkes Gillan 2016

Nineteen trials measured social skills using a variety of scales (Cohen 1981; Bloomquist 1991; Pfiffner 1997; MTA 1999; Antshel 2003; Abikoff 2004; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Storebø 2012; Pfiffner 2014; Bul 2016; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Schramm 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Wilkes Gillan 2016; Hannesdottir 2017; Qian 2017). Ten trials used the Social Skills Rating Scale (Pfiffner 1997; MTA 1999; Antshel 2003; Abikoff 2004; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Bul 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Hannesdottir 2017); one trial used the Cooperation Subscale (Bul 2016), whereas the nine remaining trials used the full Social Skills Rating Scale. Three trials used the Social Skills Improvement System (Pfiffner 2014; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016). The remaining studies each used different measures to assess social skills.

Seven of the 19 trials used more than one informant to measure social skills: two used teacher, parent and observer ratings (Pfiffner 1997; Abikoff 2004); one used teacher, parent and child ratings (Schramm 2016); and four used teacher and parent ratings (MTA 1999; Antshel 2003; Pfiffner 2014; Bul 2016). Of the 12 remaining trials, nine used only teacher ratings (Bloomquist 1991; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Storebø 2012; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Hannesdottir 2017), two used only observer‐ratings (Cohen 1981; Wilkes Gillan 2016), and one used only parent ratings (Qian 2017).

Emotional competencies

See Table 4.

3. Measures of emotional competencies from included studies.
Measures Description Number of studies Ratings
Teacher Parent Child Observer
Emotion Expression Scale for Children Five‐point Likert scale, ranging from one (not at all) to five (extremely true); higher scores indicate poorer emotion awareness and greater reluctance to express emotion 1 Choi 2015
Emotion Regulation Checklist (ERC): Emotion Regulation Subscale Four‐point rating scale, ranging from one (never) to four (almost always); higher scores indicate better emotional regulation 1 Hannesdottir 2017
Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF): Emotion Control Subscale Three‐point rating scale, ranging from one (never) to three (often); lower scores indicate better emotional control. 1 Qian 2017
Conners Behavior Rating Scale (CBRS): Emotional Index Four‐point rating scale, ranging from zero (not true at all) to three (very much true); higher scores indicate better emotional competence 1 Storebø 2012
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): Emotional Symptoms Subscale Three‐point rating scale, ranging from zero (not true) to two (certainly true); higher scores indicate lower emotional competence 1 Schramm 2016 Schramm 2016 Schramm 2016
Richman‐Graham Scale Three‐point rating scale, ranging from zero (no difficulties) to two (occurs frequently). Higher scores indicate lower emotional competence. 1 Cohen 1981

Six trials measured emotional competencies, each using a different measure (Cohen 1981; Choi 2015; Storebø 2012; Schramm 2016; Hannesdottir 2017; Qian 2017). Of these, only one trial, Schramm 2016, used ratings from more than one type of informant: teacher, parent and child. The five remaining trials used only parent ratings (Cohen 1981; Hannesdottir 2017; Qian 2017), teacher ratings (Storebø 2012), or child ratings (Choi 2015). No trials used observer ratings.

General behaviour

See Table 5.

4. Measures of general behaviour from included studies.
Measures Description Number of studies Ratings
Teacher Parent Child Observer
Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) Three point rating scale, ranging from zero (not true) to two (often true); lower scores indicate better general behaviour 1 MTA 1999 MTA 1999
Clinical Global Impression (CGI) Scale Seven‐point rating scale, ranging from one (much worse) to seven (much improved); higher scores indicate improved general behaviour 2 Pfiffner 2007 Pfiffner 2007 Waxmonsky 2010
Disruptive Behavior Disorders Rating Scale: Oppositional Defiant Disorder index (DBDRS‐ODD) Four‐point Likert scale, ranging from zero (not at all) to three (very much); lower scores indicate better general behaviour 2 Evans 2016 Evans 2016
Waxmonsky 2016      
Child Symptom Inventory (CSI): Oppositional Defiant Disorder Subscale Four‐point rating scale, ranging from zero (never) to three (very often); lower scores indicate better general behaviour 1 Pfiffner 2016 Pfiffner 2016
Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) Three‐point rating scale, ranging from one (never) to three (often); lower scores indicate better general behaviour 1 Qian 2017
Conners Behavior Rating Scale (CBRS): Conduct Problem Subscale Four‐point rating scale, ranging from zero (not true at all) to three (very much true); lower scores indicate better general behaviour 1 Cohen 1981 Cohen 1981
CBRS: Aggressiveness Subscale Four‐point rating scale, ranging from zero (not true at all) to three (very much true); lower scores indicate better general behaviour 1 Storebø 2012
Conners Teacher Rating Scale (CTRS) Four‐point Likert scale, ranging from zero (not at all true) to three (very true); lower scores indicate better general behaviour 1 Abikoff 2004
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): Total Three‐point rating scale, ranging from zero (not true) to two (certainly true); lower scores indicate better general behaviour 1 Hannesdottir 2017
SDQ: Conduct Problems Subscale Three‐point rating scale, ranging from zero (not true) to two (certainly true); lower scores indicate better general behaviour 1 Schramm 2016 Schramm 2016 Schramm 2016
Social Skills Rating Scale (SSRS): Problem Behaviour Subscale Three‐point Likert scale, ranging from zero (never) to two (often); lower scores indicate better general behaviour 1 Waxmonsky 2016
Self‐Control Rating Scale Seven‐point continuum, ranging from one (indicating maximum level of self‐control) to seven (indicating maximum level of impulsivity; lower scores indicate better general behaviour 1 Bloomquist 1991

Thirteen trials measured general behaviour using a large variety of measures (Cohen 1981; Bloomquist 1991; MTA 1999; Abikoff 2004; Pfiffner 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Storebø 2012; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Schramm 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Hannesdottir 2017; Qian 2017). Of these 13 trials, six used more than one informant; one used teacher, parent and child ratings (Schramm 2016) and five trials used teacher and parent ratings (Cohen 1981; MTA 1999; Pfiffner 2007; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016). Of the seven remaining trials, three apiece used only teacher ratings (Bloomquist 1991; Abikoff 2004; Storebø 2012) or parent ratings (Waxmonsky 2016; Hannesdottir 2017; Qian 2017), and one trial used only observer ratings (Waxmonsky 2010).

Core ADHD symptoms

See Table 6.

5. Measures of ADHD symptoms from included studies.
Measures Description Number of studies Studies reporting ratings from:
Teacher Parent Child Observer
Disruptive Behavior Disorders Rating Scale (DBDRS) Four‐point Likert scale, ranging from zero (not at all) to three (very much); lower scores indicate fewer ADHD symptoms 4 Van der Oord 2007 Van der Oord 2007
Waxmonsky 2010 Waxmonsky 2010
Evans 2016 Evans 2016
Waxmonsky 2016 Waxmonsky 2016
ADHD Rating Scales (ADHD‐RS) Five‐point Likert scale, ranging from zero (never) to four (almost always); lower scores indicate fewer ADHD symptoms 2 Tutty 2003
Qian 2017
ADHD‐RS: Hyperactivity and Impulsivity Subscale Five‐point Likert scale, ranging from zero (never) to four (almost always); lower scores indicate fewer ADHD symptoms 1 Hannesdottir 2017
Child Symptom Inventory (CSI): Inattention Scale Four‐point rating scale, ranging from zero (never) to three (very often); lower scores indicate fewer ADHD symptoms 2 Pfiffner 2007 Pfiffner 2007
Pfiffner 2014 Pfiffner 2014
Child Symptom Inventory (CSI): ADHD Scale Four‐point scale (never, sometimes, often, very often); lower scores indicate fewer ADHD symptoms 1 Pfiffner 2016 Pfiffner 2016
Conners Teacher Rating Scale (CTRS) Four‐point Likert scale, ranging from zero (not at all true) to three (very true); lower scores indicate fewer ADHD symptoms. 2 Bloomquist 1991
Abikoff 2004 Abikoff 2004
Conners Parent Rating Scale (CPRS) Four‐point Likert scale, ranging from zero (not at all true) to three (very true); lower scores indicate fewer ADHD symptoms 2 Abikoff 2004
Azad 2014
Conners 3: Hyperactivity/impulsivity Scale Four‐point Likert scale, ranging from zero (not at all true) to three (very much true); lower scores indicate fewer ADHD symptoms 1 Storebø 2012
ADHD Symptom Checklist (Fremdbeurteilungsbogen für Hyperkinetische Störungen) Four‐point scale ranging from one (not at all) to three (very much); lower scores indicate fewer ADHD symptoms 1 Schramm 2016 Schramm 2016 Schramm 2016
Swanson, Nolan and Pelham Teacher Rating Scale (SNAP) Four‐point rating scale, ranging from zero (not at al) to three (very often); lower scores indicate fewer ADHD symptoms 1 MTA 1999 MTA 1999
Child Attention Profile (CAP) Three‐point rating scale (1 = not true, 2 = sometimes true, 3 = very often true); lower scores indicate fewer ADHD symptoms 1 Tutty 2003
Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD Symptoms and Normal Behaviors (SWAN) Seven‐point rating scale, including both positive and negative scores to reflect strengths and weaknesses, ranging from three (far below average) to minus three (far above average). Zero = normal/average 1 Yuk‐chi 2005 Yuk‐chi 2005
Structured Behavioural Observations Child behaviour coded as 'on task', 'off task', or 'off task/disruptive'; lower scores indicate fewer ADHD symptoms 1 Bloomquist 1991
Continuous Performance Test (CPT): Omission Errors CPT is a computerised test measuring impulse control and attention control based on the child's response to 150 stimuli, including 30 target stimuli. The omission errors reflect degree of inattention; higher score on omission errors indicate higher degree of inattention. 1 Meftagh 2014a

Eighteen trials measured ADHD symptoms using a variety of measures (Bloomquist 1991; Abikoff 2004; MTA 1999; Tutty 2003; Yuk‐chi 2005; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Storebø 2012; Azad 2014; Meftagh 2014a; Pfiffner 2014; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Schramm 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Hannesdottir 2017; Qian 2017). Of these 18, 10 trials used ratings from more than one type of informant; 10 trials used both teacher and parent ratings (MTA 1999; Tutty 2003; Yuk‐chi 2005; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Pfiffner 2014; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Waxmonsky 2016), and one trial apiece used teacher and observer ratings (Bloomquist 1991), parent and observer ratings (Abikoff 2004), or teachers, parents and child ratings (Schramm 2016). Of the five remaining trials, three used only parent ratings (Azad 2014; Hannesdottir 2017; Qian 2017) and one apiece used only teacher ratings (Storebø 2012) or observer ratings ( Meftagh 2014a).

Performance and grades in school

See Table 7.

6. Measures of performance in school from included studies.
Measure Description Numbder of studies Ratings
Teacher Parent Child Observer
Classroom Performance Survey (CPS) Five‐point Likert scale, ranging from one (always) to five (never); higher scores indicate lower performance in school 1 Evans 2016
Conners Behavior Rating Scale (CBRS): Academic Performance Index; Four‐point rating scale, ranging from zero (not true at all) to three (very much true); higher scores indicate better performance in school 1 Storebø 2012
Social Skills Improvement System (SSIS): Academic Competence Scale Four‐point scale, ranging from zero (never) to three (almost always); higher scores indicate better performance in school 1 Pfiffner 2016
Academic Performance Rating Scale (APRS) Five‐point Likert scale, ranging from one (never or poor) to five (very often or excellent); higher scores indicate better performance in school 1 Waxmonsky 2010
Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (WIAT) WIAT is a clinician‐administered performance test including 16 subtests divided between Oral Reading, Math Fluency and Early Reading Skills; higher scores indicate better performance 1 MTA 1999
German teacher‐rated questionnaire for learning and working behaviour (Arbeitsverhalten Lehrer) German teacher‐rated questionnaire for learning and working behaviour (Arbeitsverhalten Lehrer is a teacher‐rated scale) 1 Lauth 2004

Six trials measured performance and grades in school, each using a different measure (MTA 1999; Waxmonsky 2010; Storebø 2012; Evans 2016; Schramm 2016; Pfiffner 2016). Five of these trials used teacher ratings (Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Storebø 2012; Schramm 2016; Waxmonsky 2010) while one trial used observer ratings (MTA 1999).

Satisfaction with treatment

Ten trials reported on participants', parents', teachers' and/or mental health professionals' satisfaction with the treatment (Pfiffner 1997; MTA 1999; Yuk‐chi 2005; Pfiffner 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Storebø 2012; Pfiffner 2014; Bul 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Waxmonsky 2016). Four trials used the Consumer Satisfaction Questionnaire, which is rated on a seven‐point Likert scale (Pfiffner 1997; MTA 1999; Yuk‐chi 2005; Pfiffner 2007). One trial, Pfiffner 2016, developed a seven‐item measure (rated on a five‐point Likert scale) specifically for the study. The five remaining trials measured treatment satisfaction using a single‐item question that was rated on a five‐point Likert scale in two trials (Pfiffner 2014; Waxmonsky 2016), a seven‐point Likert scale in one trial (Waxmonsky 2010), and a 10‐point Likert scale in two trials (Storebø 2012; Bul 2016).

Adverse events

Only two trials reported data on adverse events (Storebø 2012; Bul 2016). They assessed adverse events as spontaneous reporting and reported no adverse events.

Funding

Fourteen studies reported funding sources. Two of these were funded by pharmacological companies (Waxmonsky 2010; Bul 2016) and the remaining 12 trials were funded by university national foundations (Cohen 1981; MTA 1999; Tutty 2003; Pfiffner 2007; Storebø 2012; Meftagh 2014a; Pfiffner 2014; Pfiffner 2016; Evans 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Wilkes Gillan 2016; Qian 2017). Two studies reported that they did not receive any funding for the trials (Choi 2015; Hannesdottir 2017) and we did not find any information on funding for the nine remaining trials (Bloomquist 1991; Pfiffner 1997; Antshel 2003; Abikoff 2004; Yuk‐chi 2005; Van der Oord 2007; Tabaeian 2010; Azad 2014; Schramm 2016).

Excluded studies

We excluded 263 full‐text reports in total. Of these, we excluded 224 clearly irrelevant reports. We formally excluded a further 39 full‐text reports, providing reasons for exclusion in the Characteristics of excluded studies tables. Of these 39 reports, we excluded 23 trials with ineligible interventions, 11 trials with ineligible patient populations, and five trials with ineligible comparators.

Ongoing studies

We included four ongoing trials (NCT01330849; Yang 2015; IRCT201609186834N11; NCT02937142). All trials used different methods to investigate the benefits of different types of social skills training or comparable cognitive behaviour training for children and adolescents with ADHD.

Studies awaiting classification

We included one study awaiting classification (NCT01019252).

Risk of bias in included studies

We assessed the risk of bias of each included trial using the Cochrane 'Risk of bias' tool (Higgins 2011). A summary of our assessment is provided below, and in Figure 2 and Figure 3. Further details can be found in the 'Risk of bias' tables (in the Characteristics of included studies tables). We also drew a funnel plot to visually assess whether the effect was associated with the size of the trial; it seemed to be symmetrical with no clinical significant effect. Eggers’ test, moreover, was not statistically significant (Egger’s regression intercept (bias) = 1.13 (two‐tailed P = 0.17)) in the conclusion of whether or not there was publication bias in the meta‐analysis on this outcome.

2.

2

Risk of bias summary: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item for each included study

3.

3

Risk of bias graph: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item presented as percentages across all included studies

Allocation

Generation of the allocation sequence

We considered the random sequence generation to be at low risk of bias in 14 trials that assigned allocation by computer‐generated random numbers derived from a table or by the coin‐toss method (MTA 1999; Antshel 2003; Tutty 2003; Abikoff 2004; Yuk‐chi 2005; Pfiffner 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Storebø 2012; Bul 2016; Evans 2016; Qian 2017; Waxmonsky 2016; Wilkes Gillan 2016; Hannesdottir 2017). We rated 11 trials that did not state the method used to generate the random sequence to be at unclear risk of bias (Cohen 1981; Bloomquist 1991; Pfiffner 1997; Van der Oord 2007; Tabaeian 2010; Azad 2014; Meftagh 2014a; Pfiffner 2014; Choi 2015; Pfiffner 2016; Schramm 2016). We rated no trials at high risk of bias on this domain.

Allocation concealment

We judged 10 trials to be at low risk of bias due to adequate concealment of the allocation (MTA 1999; Antshel 2003; Abikoff 2004; Pfiffner 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Storebø 2012; Evans 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Wilkes Gillan 2016; Qian 2017). Fourteen trials did not describe allocation concealment, so we considered them to be at unclear risk of bias (Bloomquist 1991; Pfiffner 1997; Tutty 2003; Yuk‐chi 2005; Van der Oord 2007; Tabaeian 2010; Azad 2014; Meftagh 2014a; Pfiffner 2014; Choi 2015; Bul 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Schramm 2016; Hannesdottir 2017). We rated one trial, Cohen 1981, to be at high risk of bias because four participants were moved between groups after randomisation due to adverse reactions.

Blinding

We do not believe it is possible to blind participants or personnel involved in the delivery of social skills interventions, and consequently, rated all trials at high risk of performance bias.

It is possible, however, to blind those that perform the ratings and observations. Two trials had blinded ratings and observations and we rated them to be at low risk of detection bias (Storebø 2012; Wilkes Gillan 2016). We rated two other trials to be at uncertain risk of detection bias because it was unclear if raters were blinded (Azad 2014; Pfiffner 2014). We rated the remaining 21 trials as having high risk of detection bias since none of them used blinded ratings and observations for all outcomes (Cohen 1981; Bloomquist 1991; Pfiffner 1997; Abikoff 2004; MTA 1999; Antshel 2003; Tutty 2003; Yuk‐chi 2005; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Tabaeian 2010; Meftagh 2014a; Choi 2015; Bul 2016; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Schramm 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Hannesdottir 2017; Qian 2017).

Five trials used blinding for at least one outcome, but we did not use these outcomes in our meta‐analyses as they were not outcomes prespecified for our review.

Incomplete outcome data

We rated 16 trials to be at low risk of attrition bias, as they adequately addressed incomplete outcome data (Pfiffner 1997; Antshel 2003; Tutty 2003; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Tabaeian 2010; Storebø 2012; Azad 2014; Meftagh 2014a; Pfiffner 2014; Bul 2016; Evans 2016; Schramm 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Wilkes Gillan 2016; Hannesdottir 2017). We assessed five trials to be at high risk of attrition bias (Cohen 1981; Bloomquist 1991; Abikoff 2004; Yuk‐chi 2005; Waxmonsky 2010). Of these five trials, one reported that 22 out of 103 children failed to complete the trial (Abikoff 2004); another permitted up to 50% missing items on indexes, and dropped participants when there were not enough data (Waxmonsky 2010); while the other three trials did not adequately address incomplete outcome data (Cohen 1981; Bloomquist 1991; Yuk‐chi 2005). We considered the remaining four trials to be at unclear of risk of attrition bias due to a lack of information (MTA 1999; Choi 2015; Pfiffner 2016; Qian 2017).

Selective reporting

We rated 13 trials (which had protocols published before the trial started, and reported on all protocol specified outcomes) to be at low risk of reporting bias (Cohen 1981; Bloomquist 1991; Antshel 2003; Tutty 2003; Yuk‐chi 2005; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Storebø 2012; Meftagh 2014a; Bul 2016; Schramm 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Hannesdottir 2017). We rated four trials to be at high risk of reporting bias (Pfiffner 1997; Pfiffner 2016; Qian 2017; Waxmonsky 2010). While most trials reported on all outcomes expected to be addressed as described in their published trial protocol, Pfiffner 1997 did not report on two important outcomes (the Swanson, Nolan and Pelham (SNAP) rating scale and the Conners, Loney, and Milich (CLAM) scale used in pre‐ and post‐treatment assessments) and there was an inconsistency between the published report and the description of the trial (protocol) on clinicaltrials.gov in Waxmonsky 2010, Pfiffner 2016 and Qian 2017. We rated the eight remaining trials to be at unclear risk of reporting bias due to a lack of information (MTA 1999; Abikoff 2004; Tabaeian 2010; Azad 2014; Pfiffner 2014; Choi 2015; Evans 2016; Wilkes Gillan 2016): we were unable to find reports on all prespecified outcomes in one trial (MTA 1999); another trial published reports on both the design of the trial and the results simultaneously (Abikoff 2004); another trial registered the protocol retrospectively (after participant enrolment), and did not report on all prespecified outcomes thus making it difficult to assess if there had been selective reporting (Wilkes Gillan 2016); and five trials had no published design report or trial registration and thus no information to assess this domain (Tabaeian 2010; Azad 2014; Pfiffner 2014; Choi 2015; Evans 2016).

Vested interest

We assessed seven trials to be at high risk of bias on this domain because the trial authors were board members in pharmaceutical companies, had received funding from pharmaceutical companies, or had performed previous research on the topic (Abikoff 2004; Pfiffner 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Choi 2015; Bul 2016; Schramm 2016; Waxmonsky 2016). We rated nine trials to be at unclear risk of bias because of a lack of information on vested interests (Bloomquist 1991; Antshel 2003; Yuk‐chi 2005; Van der Oord 2007; Tabaeian 2010; Azad 2014; Pfiffner 2014; Pfiffner 2016; Qian 2017). We rated the remaining nine trials to be at low risk of bias (Cohen 1981; Evans 2016; Hannesdottir 2017; Meftagh 2014a; MTA 1999; Pfiffner 1997; Storebø 2012; Tutty 2003; Wilkes Gillan 2016).

Other potential sources of bias

We rated 14 trials to be at low risk of other bias due to no other potential risk of bias (MTA 1999; Abikoff 2004; Yuk‐chi 2005; Tabaeian 2010; Azad 2014; Meftagh 2014a; Bul 2016; Evans 2016; Schramm 2016; Wilkes Gillan 2016; Hannesdottir 2017; Qian 2017).

We considered seven trials to be at high risk of other bias (Cohen 1981; Pfiffner 1997; Pfiffner 2007; Pfiffner 2014; Pfiffner 2016; Choi 2015; Waxmonsky 2016). In five of these trials (Pfiffner 1997; Pfiffner 2007; Pfiffner 2014; Pfiffner 2016; Waxmonsky 2016), the families and teachers were paid for doing the assessment at follow‐up, leading to potential bias from those who were prone to this incentive. In Pfiffner 1997, 44% of participants were medicated with stimulant medication, but the number of medicated children in the comparison group was not stated. One trial provided no information about the between‐group balance of stimulant medication (Cohen 1981), and there was no description of the participant selection procedure in another trial (Choi 2015).

With the exception of one trial where all kinds of medication were balanced between groups (MTA 1999), the remaining trials provided no information about any co‐medication for comorbid disorders.

We judged six trials to be at unclear risk of other bias due to a lack of information (Bloomquist 1991; Antshel 2003; Tutty 2003; Van der Oord 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Storebø 2012).

We assessed all trials to be at high risk of bias overall.

Effects of interventions

See: Table 1

We present the results for each of the three primary and four secondary outcomes below. We calculated and presented the effect sizes as SMD and, where possible, as MD. We considered a SMD effect size of: 0.15 or less to have no clinically meaningful effect; 0.15 to 0.40 to have a clinical meaningful but small effect ; 0.40 to 0.75 to have a moderate effect; and greater than 0.75 to have a large treatment effect (Thalheimer 2002). We only used the outcomes from included trials, which we had predefined in our protocol that we wanted to use in this review. For those trials for which we were unable to obtain the necessary data to calculate an effect size, or used outcomes that could not be included in the meta‐analyses, we reported the results in the same way as the original report as single study results. We contacted the authors of 17 trials with unclear or missing data and requested the necessary data (some of them several times) (Bloomquist 1991; Pfiffner 1997; MTA 1999; Antshel 2003; Tutty 2003; Abikoff 2004; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Tabaeian 2010; Waxmonsky 2010; Azad 2014; Choi 2015; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Wilkes Gillan 2016; Hannesdottir 2017; Qian 2017). We received information back from eight trial groups (Pfiffner 1997; Antshel 2003; Abikoff 2004; Pfiffner 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Evans 2016; Wilkes Gillan 2016; Hannesdottir 2017).

For 14 trials, we used all of their outcomes in meta‐analyses (Antshel 2003; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Tabaeian 2010; Storebø 2012; Meftagh 2014a; Pfiffner 2014; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Schramm 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Wilkes Gillan 2016; Qian 2017; Hannesdottir 2017). For seven trials, we reported some outcomes separately and used some in meta‐analyses (Bloomquist 1991; Pfiffner 1997; MTA 1999; Tutty 2003; Abikoff 2004; Yuk‐chi 2005; Waxmonsky 2010). Only Cohen 1981 had no outcomes included in a meta‐analysis; we reported all outcomes from this trial separately.

One of the trials did not report means and SD but P values connected to F values (Cohen 1981). We tried to transform these into SD, but this was not possible because we did not have the necessary between‐group values. For one trial, Pfiffner 2007, we received raw data on the SSRS parent‐ and teacher‐rated scores and used these for calculations.

Primary outcomes

Social skills

Twenty trials reported data on social skills (Bloomquist 1991; Pfiffner 1997; MTA 1999; Antshel 2003; Abikoff 2004; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Tabaeian 2010; Waxmonsky 2010; Storebø 2012; Pfiffner 2014; Choi 2015; Bul 2016; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Schramm 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Wilkes Gillan 2016; Hannesdottir 2017; Qian 2017).

Meta‐analysis results

We combined data from 11 eligible trials in a primary meta‐analysis of teacher‐rated social skills at end of treatment (Pfiffner 1997; MTA 1999; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Storebø 2012; Pfiffner 2014; Bul 2016; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Schramm 2016). We found no evidence of an effect of the intervention (SMD 0.11, 95% CI −0.00 to 0.22; 11 trials, 1271 participants; I2 = 0%; P = 0.05). We rated the certainty of the evidence as very low certainty due to high risk of bias, inconsistency, and imprecision. The primary outcome, teacher‐rated social skills at end of treatment, corresponds to a MD of 1.22 points on the SSRS scale (95% CI 0.09 to 2.36). The minimal clinical relevant difference (10%) on the SSRS is 10.2.

We tested the robustness of this result by conducting a sensitivity analysis in which we excluded the three trials with the longest treatment duration (MTA 1999; Pfiffner 2014; Evans 2016), and again found no evidence of an effect (SMD 0.11, 95% CI −0.05 to 0.27; eight trials, 620 participants; I2 = 0%; P = 0.17; Analysis 1.1).

1.1. Analysis.

1.1

Comparison 1 Social skills, Outcome 1 Primary meta‐analysis: Teacher‐rated social skills at end of treatment.

We conducted four further secondary meta‐analyses (Analysis 1.2), and found that, compared with no intervention, social skills training:

1.2. Analysis.

1.2

Comparison 1 Social skills, Outcome 2 Secondary meta‐analyses: Social skills.

  1. did not improve teacher‐rated social skills at longest follow‐up (SMD 0.06, 95% CI −0.22 to 0.35; three trials, 192 participants, I2 = 0%; P = 0.66; Pfiffner 1997; Storebø 2012; Pfiffner 2014);

  2. did improve parent‐rated social skills at end of treatment for all eligible trials (SMD 0.19, 95% CI 0.06 to 0.32; 15 trials, 1609 participants; I2 = 33% ; P = 0.003; very low‐certainty evidence; Pfiffner 1997; Abikoff 2004; MTA 1999; Antshel 2003; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Pfiffner 2014; Bul 2016; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Schramm 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Hannesdottir 2017; Qian 2017);

  3. did not improve parent‐rated social skills at longest follow‐up (SMD 0.13, 95% CI −0.35 to 0.62; two trials, 445 participants; I2 = 80% ; P = 0.59; Pfiffner 2014; Evans 2016); and

  4. did not improve participant‐rated social skills at end of treatment for all eligible trials (SMD 0.28, 95% CI −0.68 to 1.23; five trials, 344 participants; I2 = 92%; P = 0.57; Abikoff 2004; Antshel 2003; Tabaeian 2010; Choi 2015; Schramm 2016).

We conducted the analyses using a random‐effects model, and obtained similar results when repeating the analyses using a fixed‐effect model.

Single study results

Below, we present the data from six studies, which assessed social skills using different measures and thus could not be included in the aforementioned meta‐analyses (Cohen 1981; Bloomquist 1991; Pfiffner 1997; Abikoff 2004; Tabaeian 2010; Wilkes Gillan 2016).

The Cohen 1981 trial reported no significant group differences from observing children for three eight‐minute periods during a one‐hour period for two categories of behaviour in classrooms: play behaviour and social behaviour.

The Wilkes Gillan 2016 trial reported improved observer‐rated social skills at end of treatment for all eligible trials (SMD 2.88, 95% CI 1.80 to 3.96; one trial, 29 participants; P < 0.001; Analysis 1.2.4).

The Tabaeian 2010 trial reported a significant difference between the groups in favour of participant‐rated social skills at longest follow‐up (SMD 1.60, 95% CI 0.77 to 2.44; one trial, 30 participants; P=0.0002; Analysis 1.2.6).

The Bloomquist 1991 trial found no significant difference between the groups for social skills assessed using the teacher‐reported version of the Walker‐McConnell Scale of Social Competence and School Adjustment (five‐point scale ranging from 'never occurs' to 'frequently occurs'; higher scores indicate better social skills): MD 1.06 points, 95% CI −0.47 to 2.59; one trial, 46 participants; P = 0.18; Analysis 1.3.(fixed‐effects analysis).

1.3. Analysis.

1.3

Comparison 1 Social skills, Outcome 3 Teacher‐reported Walker‐McConnell Scale of Social Competence and School Adjustment.

The Pfiffner 1997 trial found evidence of a large treatment effect in favour of social skills training using the parent‐rated Social Skills Scale (UCI) UC Irvine Health Child Development School (higher scores indicate better social skills): MD 9.70 points, 95% CI 6.07 to 13.33; one trial, 18 participants; P < 0.001; Analysis 1.4. Pfiffner 1997 also found a significant difference between the groups when using the child‐rated Test of Social Skill Knowledge (scored by blinded raters; ranging from one (low knowledge) to 15 (high knowledge); higher scores indicate better social skills): MD 4.20 points, 95% CI 1.99 to 6.41; one trial, 18 participants; P < 0.001; Analysis 1.5.

1.4. Analysis.

1.4

Comparison 1 Social skills, Outcome 4 Parent‐rated Social Skills Scale (UCI).

1.5. Analysis.

1.5

Comparison 1 Social skills, Outcome 5 Child‐rated Test of Social Skill Knowledge.

Abikoff 2004 reported no significant difference between the groups in negative behaviour assessed by the Social Interaction Observation Code, which records the frequency of positive, negative, or neutral behaviour, including observations of negative behaviour (higher scores equate to more negative behaviour) (MD 0.20 points, 95% CI ‐0.11 to 0.51; one trial, 68 participants; P = 0.21; Analysis 1.6).

1.6. Analysis.

1.6

Comparison 1 Social skills, Outcome 6 Social Interaction Observation Code: Negative behaviour.

Emotional competencies

Five trials reported data on emotional competencies (Storebø 2012; Choi 2015; Schramm 2016; Hannesdottir 2017; Qian 2017).

Meta‐analysis results

We combined data from two trials in a meta‐analysis (Storebø 2012; Schramm 2016). We found no evidence of an effect of the intervention for the primary meta‐analysis of teacher‐rated emotional competencies at end of treatment (SMD −0.02, 95% CI −0.72 to 0.68; two trials, 129 participants; I2 = 74%; P = 0.96; Analysis 2.1). We rated the certainty of the evidence as very low due to high risk of bias, inconsistency, and imprecision.

2.1. Analysis.

2.1

Comparison 2 Emotional competencies, Outcome 1 Primary meta‐analysis: Teacher‐rated emotional competencies at end of treatment.

We also conducted two secondary meta‐analyses (Analysis 2.2), and found no evidence of an effect of the intervention on:

2.2. Analysis.

2.2

Comparison 2 Emotional competencies, Outcome 2 Secondary meta‐analyses: Emotional competencies.

  1. parent‐rated emotional competencies (SMD −0.27, 95% CI −0.59 to 0.05; three trials, 173 participants; I2 = 8%; P = 0.09; Schramm 2016; Hannesdottir 2017; Qian 2017);

  2. participant‐rated emotional competencies (SMD −0.27, 95% CI −0.62 to 0.09; two trials, 125 participants; I2 = 0% ; P = 0.14; Choi 2015; Schramm 2016).

We conducted the analyses using a random‐effects model, and obtained similar results when repeating the analyses using a fixed‐effect model.

Single study results

The Storebø 2012 trial reported parent‐rated emotional competencies at longest follow‐up (SMD 0.19, 95% CI −0.34 to 0.72; one trial, 56 participants; P = 0.49).

General behaviour

Eleven trials reported data on general behaviour (Bloomquist 1991; MTA 1999; Abikoff 2004; Pfiffner 2007; Storebø 2012; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Schramm 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Hannesdottir 2017; Qian 2017).

Meta‐analysis results

We combined data from eight trials in a meta‐analysis (Bloomquist 1991; MTA 1999; Abikoff 2004; Storebø 2012; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Schramm 2016; Waxmonsky 2016). We found no evidence of an effect for the primary meta‐analysis of teacher‐rated general behaviour at the end of treatment (SMD −0.06, 95% CI −0.19 to 0.06; eight trials, 1002 participants; I2 = 0%; P = 0.33; Analysis 3.1). We rated the quality of the evidence as low due to high risk of bias, inconsistency, and imprecision.

3.1. Analysis.

3.1

Comparison 3 General behaviour, Outcome 1 Primary meta‐analysis: Teacher‐rated general behaviour at end of treatment.

We tested the robustness of this result by conducting two sensitivity analyses, both of which found no evidence of an effect:

  1. sensitivity analysis excluding the two trials with the longest treatment duration (MTA 1999; Evans 2016): SMD −0.09, 95% CI −0.28 to 0.10; six trials, 422 participants; I2 = 0%; P = 0.36; and

  2. sensitivity analysis excluding the two largest trials (MTA 1999; Evans 2016): SMD −0.09, 95% CI −0.28 to 0.10; six trials, 422 participants; I2 = 0%; P = 0.36.

We also conducted two secondary meta‐analyses (Analysis 3.2), and found that, compared with no intervention, social skills training:

3.2. Analysis.

3.2

Comparison 3 General behaviour, Outcome 2 Secondary analyses: general behaviour.

  1. did not improve teacher‐rated general behaviour at longest follow‐up (SMD −0.10, 95% CI −0.27 to 0.07; four trials, 637 participants; I2 = 7% ; P = 0.24; Bloomquist 1991; MTA 1999; Storebø 2012; Evans 2016);

  2. did improve parent‐rated general behaviour at end of treatment (SMD −0.38, 95% CI −0.61 to −0.14; eight trials, 995 participants; I2 = 64%; P = 0.002; very low‐quality evidence; MTA 1999; Storebø 2012; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Schramm 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Hannesdottir 2017; Qian 2017).

We conducted the analyses using a random‐effects model, and obtained similar results when repeating the analyses using a fixed‐effect model.

Single study results

The Pfiffner 2007 trial measured general behaviour using parent and teacher ratings of the Clinical Global Impression Scale, and found that the intervention group showed significantly greater improvement than the control group (parents: F1, 51 = 28.46, P < 0.001; teachers: F1, 51 = 11.73, P = 0.001; one trial, 69 participants).

The Evans 2016 trial reported parent‐rated general behaviour at longest follow‐up (SMD −0.21, 95% CI 0.44 to 0.03; one trial; 326 participants; P = 0.08).

The Schramm 2016 trial reported participant‐rated general behaviour at end of treatment (SMD −0.07, 95% CI −0.52 to 0.38; one trial, 76 participants; P = 0.76).

Secondary outcomes

Core ADHD symptoms

Nineteen trials reported data on ADHD symptoms (Bloomquist 1991; Tutty 2003; Abikoff 2004; MTA 1999; Yuk‐chi 2005; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Tabaeian 2010; Waxmonsky 2010; Storebø 2012; Azad 2014; Meftagh 2014a; Pfiffner 2014; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Schramm 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Hannesdottir 2017; Qian 2017).

Meta‐analysis results

We combined data from 14 eligible trials in a meta‐analysis of the primary meta‐analysis of teacher‐rated ADHD symptoms at end of treatment (Bloomquist 1991; Abikoff 2004; MTA 1999; Yuk‐chi 2005; Van der Oord 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Storebø 2012; Pfiffner 2014; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Schramm 2016; Waxmonsky 2016; Hannesdottir 2017; Qian 2017). We found evidence of an effect in favour of the intervention (SMD −0.26, 95% CI −0.47 to −0.05; 14 trials, 1379 participants; I2 = 69%; P = 0.02; Analysis 4.1). We rated the quality of the evidence as very low due to high risk of bias, inconsistency, and imprecision.

4.1. Analysis.

4.1

Comparison 4 Core ADHD symptoms, Outcome 1 Primary meta‐analysis: Teacher‐rated ADHD symptoms at end of treatment.

We tested the robustness of this result by conducting two sensitivity analyses, both of which showed no evidence of an effect:

  1. sensitivity analysis excluding the three trials with the longest treatment duration (MTA 1999; Pfiffner 2014; Evans 2016): SMD −0.24, 95% CI −0.52 to 0.04; 11 trials, 677 participants, I2 = 69%; P = 0.10); and

  2. sensitivity analysis excluding the three largest trials (MTA 1999; Pfiffner 2014; Evans 2016): SMD −0.24, 95% CI −0.52 to 0.04; 11 trials, 677 participants; I2 = 69%; P = 0.10).

We also drew a funnel plot to visually assess whether the effect was associated with the size of the trial; it seemed to be symmetrical with no clinically significant effect. Eggers’ test, moreover was not statistically significant (Egger’s regression intercept (bias) = 0.40 (two‐tailed P = 0.78)) for the conclusion whether or not there was publication bias in the meta‐analysis for this outcome.

We conducted five further secondary meta‐analyses (Analysis 4.2), and found that, compared with no intervention, social skills training:

4.2. Analysis.

4.2

Comparison 4 Core ADHD symptoms, Outcome 2 Secondary meta‐analyses: ADHD symptoms.

  1. did not reduce teacher‐rated ADHD symptoms at longest follow‐up (SMD −0.11, 95% CI −0.28 to 0.06; five trials, 582 participants; I2 = 0%; P = 0.20; Bloomquist 1991; Yuk‐chi 2005; Storebø 2012; Pfiffner 2014; Evans 2016);

  2. did reduce parent‐rated ADHD symptoms at end of treatment for all eligible trials (SMD −0.54, 95% CI −0.81 to −0.26; 11 trials, 1206 participants; I2 = 79%; P < 0.001; very low‐quality evidence; Tutty 2003; Abikoff 2004; MTA 1999; Yuk‐chi 2005; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Azad 2014; Pfiffner 2014; Evans 2016; Schramm 2016);

  3. did reduce parent‐rated ADHD symptoms at longest follow‐up (SMD −1.36, 95% CI −2.48 to −0.25; three trials, 476 participants; I2 = 95%; P = 0.02; Azad 2014; Pfiffner 2014; Evans 2016);

  4. did not reduce participant‐rated ADHD symptoms at end of treatment (SMD −0.77, 95% CI −2.31 to 0.78; two trials, 106 participants; I2 = 91%; P = 0.33; Tabaeian 2010; Schramm 2016); and

  5. did not reduce observer‐rated ADHD symptoms at end of treatment for all eligible trials (SMD −3.15, 95% CI ‐9.88 to 3.57; two trials, 107 participants; I2 = 98%; P = 0.36; Meftagh 2014a; Schramm 2016).

We conducted the analyses using a random‐effects model. We obtained similar results when repeating the analyses using a fixed‐effect model, except for sensitivity analyses 4.1.2 and 4.1.3, both of which showed a statistical significant effect when analysed with fixed‐effect model. However, the random‐effects model is more appropriate because of the heterogeneity in these analyses.

Single study results

The Meftagh 2014a trial found a significant difference between groups for observer‐rated ADHD symptoms at longest follow‐up (SMD 3.95, 95% CI 2.66 to 5.23; one trial, 30 participants; P < 0.001).

The MTA 1999 trial found no significant difference between the groups on teacher‐rated ADHD symptoms (inattention) at end of treatment (SMD 0.01, 95% CI −0.23 to 0.26; one trial, 254 participants; P = 0.92).

The Pfiffner 2007 trial also found no significant difference between the groups on teacher‐rated ADHD symptoms (sluggish cognitive tempo) at end of treatment (SMD −0.29, 95% CI −0.78 to 0.20; one trial, 66 participants; P = 0.24).

The Tabaeian 2010 trial found a significant difference between groups on participant‐rated ADHD symptoms at longest follow‐up (SMD 1.62, 95% CI 0.78 to 2.46; one trial, 30 participants; P < 0.001).

Performance and grades in school

Five trials measured performance in school (MTA 1999; Waxmonsky 2010; Storebø 2012; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016).

Meta‐analysis results

We pooled the data in a meta‐analysis and found that social skills training did not improve teacher‐rated performance in school at end of treatment (SMD 0.15, 95% CI ‐0.01 to 0.31; five trials, 642 participants; I2= 0% ; P = 0.07; Analysis 5.1; Waxmonsky 2010; Storebø 2012; Evans 2016; Schramm 2016, Pfiffner 2016) or at longest follow‐up (SMD −0.01, 95% CI −0.22 to 0.20; two trials, 379 participants; I2 = 0%; P = 0.92; Analysis 5.2; Storebø 2012; Evans 2016).

5.1. Analysis.

5.1

Comparison 5 Teacher‐rated performance and grades in school, Outcome 1 At end of treatment.

5.2. Analysis.

5.2

Comparison 5 Teacher‐rated performance and grades in school, Outcome 2 At longest follow‐up.

We conducted the analyses using a random‐effects model, and obtained similar results when repeating the analyses using a fixed‐effect model.

Single study results

The MTA 1999 trial found no significant difference between groups for observer‐rated performance in school (MD 1.50 points, 95% CI −2.06 to 5.06; measured using Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (WIAT); (higher score indicates better performance); one trial, 260 participants; P = 0.41; Analysis 6.1).

6.1. Analysis.

6.1

Comparison 6 Observer‐rated performance and grades in school, Outcome 1 Wescheler Individual Achievement Test.

Participant or parent (or both) satisfaction with the treatment

Four trials (233 participants) measured satisfaction with treatment (Pfiffner 1997; Pfiffner 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Yuk‐chi 2005). Although satisfaction with treatment was high in all four trials, two trials found no significant difference between the intervention and controI groups (Pfiffner 1997; Waxmonsky 2010), and two trials did not report on differences between the groups (Pfiffner 2007; Yuk‐chi 2005).

Adverse events

Only two trials (226 participants) reported data on adverse events (Storebø 2012; Bul 2016).They assessed adverse events as spontaneous reporting and reported no adverse events in the trial.

Trial Sequential Analysis (TSA)

We conducted a TSA of the primary outcome, social skills rated by teachers at end of treatment, with data from four trials (Pfiffner 1997; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Waxmonsky 2010). Using an a priori assumption that the minimal relevant clinical intervention effect was 4.0 points, we found that the intervention effect almost reached the futility area (between the two widening dotted red lines), possibly signalling that the social skills intervention had no effect on teacher‐rated social skills at the end of treatment (MD 1.80, 95% CI ‐1.01 to 4.62; four trials, 185 participants; Analysis 7.1; Figure 4). We used a power of 90% in the analysis and this gives a maximum type II error of 10%, and therefore there is a 10% risk of overlooking a true effect.

7.1. Analysis.

7.1

Comparison 7 TSA, Outcome 1 Teacher‐rated social skills.

4.

4

Trial Sequential Analysis of teacher‐rated social skills ‐ SSRS

Footnotes

DARIS: diversity‐adjusted required information size
 MD: mean difference
 SSRS: Social Skills Rating Scale

We also conducted a post hoc TSA of social skills rated by teachers for all eligible trials; 11 trials in total provided data (Pfiffner 1997; MTA 1999; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Storebø 2012; Pfiffner 2014; Bul 2016; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016; Schramm 2016). To do this, we transformed the MD and SD from the different rating scales used in this analysis to the MD and SD of a commonly used scale, namely the SSRS, using the following formula: MD = SMD*SD (Thorlund 2011). In the TSA we found that the cumulative z scores (blue line) crossed into the areas of futility (in between the two widening dotted red lines) (SMD 0.11, 95% CI −0.00 to 0.22; 11 trials, 1271 participants; Analysis 7.1; Figure 5).

5.

5

Trial Sequential Analysis of teacher‐rated social skills ‐ all studies transformed to SSRS

Footnotes

MIREDIF: minimum relevant difference
 SD: standard deviation
 SSRS: Social Skills Rating Scale

Subgroup analyses

We performed five subgroup analyses, none of which showed significant differences in intervention effects.

  • Children aged five to 11 years compared to children aged 12 to 18 year:

Teacher‐rated social skills at end of treatment: children aged five to 11 years (10 trials, 1194 participants: Pfiffner 1997; MTA 1999; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Storebø 2012; Pfiffner 2014; Bul 2016; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016) compared to children aged 12 to 18 years (one trial, 77 participants: Schramm 2016). Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.06, df = 1 (P = 0.81), I2 = 0%; Analysis 8.1.

8.1. Analysis.

8.1

Comparison 8 Subgroup analysis 1: Children aged five to 11 years versus children aged 12 to 18 years, Outcome 1 Teacher‐rated social skills.

  • ADHD with comorbidity compared to ADHD without comorbidity:

Parent‐rated ADHD symptoms at end of treatment: ADHD with comorbidity (eight trials, 1003 participants: Abikoff 2004; MTA 1999; Yuk‐chi 2005; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Pfiffner 2014; Evans 2016) compared to ADHD without comorbidity (two trials, 173 participants: Tutty 2003; Schramm 2016). Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.10, df = 1 (P = 0.75), I2 = 0%; Analysis 9.1.

9.1. Analysis.

9.1

Comparison 9 Subgroup analysis 2: ADHD and comorbidity versus ADHD and no comorbidity, Outcome 1 Parent‐rated ADHD symptoms at end of treatment.

  • Social skills training only compared to social skills training supported by parent training:

Teacher‐rated social skills at end of treatment: social skills training only (four trials, 336 participants: Pfiffner 1997; Pfiffner 2014; Bul 2016; Evans 2016) compared to social skills training supported by parent training (four trials, 632 participants: Pfiffner 1997; Storebø 2012; Pfiffner 2014; Schramm 2016). Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.16, df = 1 (P = 0.69), I2 = 0%; Analysis 10.1.

10.1. Analysis.

10.1

Comparison 10 Subgroup analysis 3: Social skills training only versus social skills training supported by parent training, Outcome 1 Teacher‐rated social skills at end of treatment.

  • Social skills training, parent training and medication compared to social skills training and parent training without medication:

Parent‐rated social skills at end of treatment: social skills training, parent training and medication (four trials, 299 participants: Abikoff 2004; Antshel 2003; Waxmonsky 2010; Waxmonsky 2016) compared to social skills training and parent training without medication (four trials, 337 participants: Pfiffner 1997; Pfiffner 2007; Pfiffner 2014; Pfiffner 2016). Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 2.61, df = 1 (P = 0.11), I2 = 61.6%; Analysis 11.1.

11.1. Analysis.

11.1

Comparison 11 Subgroup analysis 4: Social skills training, parental training and medication versus social skills training and parental training without medication, Outcome 1 Parent‐rated social skills at end of treatment.

  • No‐intervention control group compared to waiting‐list control group with possible minor active intervention components:

Teacher‐rated social skills at end of treatment: no‐intervention control group (eight trials, 693 participants: Pfiffner 1997; MTA 1999; Pfiffner 2007; Van der Oord 2007; Waxmonsky 2010; Storebø 2012; Bul 2016; Schramm 2016) compared to waiting‐list control group with possible minor active intervention components (three trials, 578 participants: Pfiffner 2014; Evans 2016; Pfiffner 2016). Test for subgroup differences: Chi2 = 0.02, df = 1 (P = 0.89), I2 = 0%; Analysis 12.1.

12.1. Analysis.

12.1

Comparison 12 Subgroup analysis 5: No‐intervention control group versus waiting‐list control group with possible minor active intervention components, Outcome 1 Teacher‐rated social skills at end of treatment.

We conducted the analyses using a random‐effects model, and obtained similar results when repeating the analyses using a fixed‐effect model.

Single study result

One trial (576 participants) reported on subgroup analyses comparing children with ADHD only to children with ADHD and comorbid anxiety disorder (MTA 1999). This analysis found significant differences in teacher‐rated hyperactivity/impulsivity (F = 1.64, P = 0.04) and teacher‐rated social skills (F = 1.68, P = 0.03) between the two subgroups of children in connection with all four active treatments used in this trial. The subgroup with ADHD and comorbidity of anxiety disorder showed better results (MTA 1999).

Discussion

We conducted this systematic review to examine the effects of social skills training for children and adolescents with ADHD. We considered a total of 313 full‐text reports from which we included 25 trials published in 45 articles in this review. Of these 25 trials, we used the results from 24 in meta‐analyses.

Summary of main results

We included 25 trials published in 45 reports. Altogether these trials randomised 2690 participants. All trials included children aged between five and 13 years, except for a single study which included adolescents between 12 and 17 years of age. The duration of the trials ranged from five weeks to two years. Most were conducted in outpatient clinics in the USA, Asia, and Europe. We assessed all trials as having high risk of bias, which might lead to systematic errors: overestimation of benefits and underestimation of harms. We considered the parent‐rated findings to be more questionable than our primary analyses, which were based on teacher‐rated outcomes, due to high risk of systematic errors (lack of blinding) in the parent‐rated outcomes (Daley 2014b).

In accordance with the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011), we combined all relevant trials in a meta‐analysis to investigate common features of treatment effects. We found no significant differences between the group given social skills training and the groups given no intervention or assigned to a waiting list. There was a beneficial effect on some of the parent‐rated primary outcomes and the secondary outcome of teacher‐rated ADHD symptoms at end of treatment, but the finding was questionable due to lack of support from sensitivity analyses with low clinical significance and low‐certainty evidence. We found no indications of harmful effects.

We presented all results using the random‐effects model, thus giving more weight to smaller trials; however, the conclusions on the effect of intervention did not change when we applied a fixed‐effect model. Thus, we conclude that the observed statistical heterogeneity does not seem to be of importance for the results of the present review.

Overall completeness and applicability of evidence

We were able to include most of the data from the trials in our meta‐analyses, which provides a good basis for the evidence in this review. However, the interventions might be considered too heterogeneous to combine in a meta‐analysis and the multiplicity of different outcome measures might limit the external validity of this review. We found a small treatment effect for some of the outcomes, but all of the trials were at high risk of bias.

Components and duration of the interventions

All but three trials (Cohen 1981; Pfiffner 1997; Antshel 2003) used manual‐based interventions. The social skills interventions in the trials were, in general, cognitive behaviour‐based treatments but varied in form, content, and in the use of specific behaviour techniques. Most manuals were structured around specific themes, with most trials focusing on problem‐solving and emotion regulation and a few trials focusing more specifically on academic organisational skills (Evans 2016) or play skills (Wilkes Gillan 2016).

The duration of the treatment also differed greatly, from five weeks to 24 months. However, there were no differences in the results when we excluded the study with the longest intervention from the analyses of social skills and ADHD symptoms.

Parent training

Most trials included specific parent training in the social skills intervention. One study involved parents before the onset of training (Hannesdottir 2017) or as part of the first or last session (Qian 2017), in order that parents might understand the concept of the training and be able to support the children in home assignments or in applying what they learned at home. Other trials did not describe parental involvement in the training (Azad 2014; Meftagh 2014a; Choi 2015; Bul 2016; Evans 2016).

Teacher training

More than half of the trials included teacher training or teacher consultations as part of the social skills training. The inherent differences in the interventions accorded with this review's inclusion criteria (criteria for considering studies for this review), but were likely to produce heterogeneity in the analysis.

Treatment effects

Although the measurable beneficial effects of social skills training were small and questionable due to the low certainty of the evidence, participant, parent, and teacher satisfaction with the intervention was overall in the positive direction, as the level of satisfaction was rated as high in all of the included studies. However, in half of the trials measuring this outcome, there was no significant difference in the level of satisfaction when comparing the experimental and control group. The other half of the trials did not report on between‐group differences. This is a problem as participant satisfaction with treatment is often used as an argument for this kind of treatment.

In the MTA 1999 trial, the multimodal treatment had a superior treatment effect on children with ADHD and comorbid anxiety disorder compared to those without that comorbidity. This was an interesting subgroup finding and suggested that future trials on this topic should investigate these findings further by planning for subgroup analyses on children with and without comorbid anxiety disorder. Moreover, we know very little about the effects of social skills training in adolescents.

Limitations of the evidence

In all meta‐analyses that achieved significant findings with 95% confidence intervals, the findings could be due to bias and overestimation of beneficial intervention effects. We conducted TSA to control the risk of type I and type 2 errors and to estimate how far we were from obtaining the diversity adjusted required information size (DARIS) to detect or reject a certain plausible intervention effect. Moreover, the TSAs showed that the observed intervention effects could be due to type I errors. This highlights the need for more clinical research on this topic without risks of bias. Both the a priori and post hoc TSA showed that there is a need for more participants in order to reach a firm conclusion in the meta‐analyses.

A serious limitation of these types of trials was the lack of blinding or inability to blind. This introduced a high risk of bias in the assessment of outcomes (Schultz 1995; Kjaergard 2002; Wood 2008; Savovíc 2012; Savovic 2018). Statistical heterogeneity was low (0%) in most meta‐analyses, most likely due to inherent features of the trials leading to wide CIs of the estimates, and therefore was not mirroring the clinical and methodological heterogeneity.

RCTs are generally considered to be the highest level of evidence, but most of the trials included in this systematic review were at high risk of bias and the vast majority were at high risk of random errors due to their low sample sizes. Generally, we rated the certainty of the evidence as low or very low using the GRADE approach (GRADE Working Group), downgrading the certainty of the evidence by two or three levels due to high risk of bias, imprecision and inconsistency. Further research may change the estimates of the treatment effect, but such trials ought to be conducted without risk of systematic errors (bias), random errors (play of chance), and design errors (Keus 2010).

Quality of the evidence

Our review has some limitations. Our results were based on only 25 trials with a limited number of participants (n = 2690). Many of the trials were prone to selection bias due to unclear or inadequate generation of the allocation sequence or allocation concealment. All 25 trials had an overall assessment as having 'high risk of bias', so our results might not be robust and reliable (Figure 2).

Funnel Plots

We drew funnel plots of the following two outcomes for all eligible trials to visually assess whether effects were associated with the size of the study: 1) teacher‐rated social skills and 2) teacher‐rated ADHD symptoms. Both outcomes seemed to be symmetrical with no clinically significant effect. An Eggers’ test for both the outcomes was not statistically significant so we were unable to conclude whether or not there was publication bias in the meta‐analysis of these outcomes.

There is, therefore, currently insufficient evidence to draw any conclusions about any form of social skills training as having an effect on ADHD patients.

The important methodological limitations, which have been elaborated above, reduced the reliability of the results of most of the trials included in this review.

Potential biases in the review process

We sought to minimise potential biases in the review process in the following ways. We published a protocol before we embarked on the review itself. We conducted extensive searches of relevant databases. Two review authors, working independently, selected trials for inclusion and extracted data. Disagreements were resolved by discussion with team members. We assessed risk of bias in all trials according to the recommendations provided in Chapter 8 of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011). We recognise, however, that there are some limitations in the review process. In particular, we did not assess quality of life as an outcome and we defined serious adverse events as a secondary outcome when it should have been a primary outcome (though these were reported in the review). Furthermore, we believe that the outcomes of emotional competencies and general behaviour should have been secondary outcomes; we will change this in the next update of this review. Conduct disorder was used in two out of the 10 trials that were included in the analysis of general behaviour, and we took the decision to include this in a meta‐analysis of the impact of social skills training on behaviour more generally. Some might take issue with this, but we believe this was a sensible approach.

Agreements and disagreements with other studies or reviews

Four earlier meta‐analyses examined the effects of social skills training for children with ADHD. Two of these concluded that there was no effect of social skills training for children with ADHD (Kavale 1997; Van der Oord 2008); the other two concluded that there was a beneficial effect (De Boo 2007; Majewicz‐Hefley 2007). The obvious limitation of all four reviews is that all are at least 10 years old, and suffer from several methodological weaknesses; for example, none of these reviews evaluated systematic errors (bias) in the included trials. The conclusion in this update reinforces the conclusion in our original review where we wrote that "there is little evidence to support or refute social skills training for adolescents with ADHD (Storebø 2011). There is need for more trials, with low risk of bias and with a sufficient number of participants, investigating the efficacy of social skills training versus no training for both children and adolescents." (Storebø 2011). A new systematic review published in 2019 investigating the effectiveness of stand‐alone social skills training for youth with ADHD concluded that social skills training implemented without additional treatment components like parent support, showed improvements on some areas of social functioning (Willis 2019). However, this review suffered from a very limited search strategy and did not evaluate systematic errors (bias) in the included trials.

Authors' conclusions

Implications for practice.

We generally found no beneficial effects in favour of social skills training for children and adolescents with ADHD. In the primary analyses, we found no evidence of an effect on social skills, emotional competencies, and general behaviour. In the secondary analyses, we did find favourable effects on ADHD symptoms, indicating that social skills training could exert clinically useful effects in this domain, but we need more evidence to confirm this. Social skills training did not seem to affect child and adolescent school performance and was not associated with any adverse events. We cannot rule out that social skills training is associated with harms. On the basis of the evidence considered in this review, we are unable to conclude whether social skills training is beneficial for children with ADHD or whether it is a waste of scarce resources in clinical practice.

Implications for research.

We need more high‐quality trials that are at low risk of bias and have sufficient sample sizes to investigate social skills training versus no training for children and adolescents with ADHD. Such trials should include adolescents since only one study in this review assessed this group. Future trials should be based on pre‐published protocols to combat the problem of publication bias. Moreover, future trials should be designed according to the SPIRIT statement (Chan 2013), and reported according to the CONSORT statement (Schulz 2010). Social skills problems is an important area of intervention for children and adolescents with ADHD. Untreated social skills problems may decrease self‐confidence in these children due to limited positive social experiences.

Feedback

Comments on protocol by Peter Gøtzche, 16 February 2010

Summary

  1. The Background notes that drugs have a beneficial effect on major symptoms in about 80% of the patients treated. Such a statement is meaningless when we don't know what the effect was in groups treated with placebo. The authors need to rectify this so that the readers can understand what the effect is.

  2. Social skills training is the focus of the review and the authors state that "We have been unable to identify meta‐analyses or systematic reviews on the topic". This statement is a bit surprising. A quick and simple search on PubMed on "(attention deficit hyperactivity disorder children) AND training", limited to meta‐analysis, yielded 7 hits, of which one appears to be highly relevant for the authors' review, as they also want to review combination therapy: Majewicz‐Hefley A, Carlson JS. A meta‐analysis of combined treatments for children diagnosed with ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2007 Feb;10(3):239‐50.

  3. The following reference may also be relevant, particularly as the authors of the Cochrane protocol mention that training may increase negative behaviour, with reference to a single study. In contrast, based on its abstract, this reference seems to be to a meta‐analysis, and had different findings: Weiss B, Caron A, Ball S, Tapp J, Johnson M, Weisz JR. Latrogenic effects of group treatment for antisocial youths. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2005 Dec;73(6):1036‐44.

Reply

We thank Peter Gøtzsche for his interest in our review and for raising the comments.

Point 1

Peter Gøtzsche is correct that only giving the proportion of patients who respond to the active intervention and leaving out the response proportion among placebo‐treated patients does not inform the reader with regard to the relative risk reduction between the two. We will amend the protocol accordingly to make it explicit that the response proportion of response from ‘stimulant’ drugs is about 80% while the placebo response proportion is about 3% to 10%, leading to a relative risk reduction of at least 77%. We thus acknowledge Peter Gøtzsche's vigilance, and have now taken steps to correct the mistake.

Point 2

We would argue, regarding this point, that the truth may be more complex than the statement above. Six of the meta‐analyses identified by Dr Gøtzsche are not relevant to our review. The seventh, to which he makes particular reference, is potentially relevant. This is the meta‐analysis by Majewicz‐Hefley and Carlson (2007). The meta‐analysis includes a total of eight studies. The article divides the outcomes into five different categories of outcome variables. The Social Skills variable was based on four studies. Two of the four studies are not relevant for our review. One concerns behaviour therapy (and not social skills training); the other one is not a randomised clinical trial. That leaves two studies in the meta‐analysis of the social skills outcome variable, which we could have mentioned in the protocol, but chose not to. Both studies will of course be considered for the review, and be cited there.

Point 3

Our point in the protocol here was simply to show that we are aware of the possibility that group training can have adverse effects. We could have found articles (or meta‐analyses) that suggested the opposite, viz., that group training of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) or conduct disorder has positive effects, as this finding is more common, but this also would have not been pertinent. Furthermore, the article Peter Gøtzsche refers to concerns antisocial youths, and this population is not the same as that diagnosed with ADHD or conduct disorder.

Contributors

This feedback was prepared by Jane Dennis, feedback editor for CDPLPG, in consultation with the submitter, the authors, the CDPLPG Co‐ordinating Editor Geraldine Macdonald and the former CDPLPG Managing Editor Chris Champion.

What's new

Date Event Description
12 October 2018 New search has been performed We updated the review following a new search in July 2018.
12 October 2018 New citation required but conclusions have not changed We included 14 new studies.

History

Protocol first published: Issue 1, 2010
 Review first published: Issue 12, 2011

Date Event Description
14 April 2010 Amended US FDA reference corrected
16 March 2010 Feedback has been incorporated Feedback comments from Peter Gøtzsche incorporated

Notes

An administrative error was made in the first published version of the protocol and important information about the declaration of interest of the authors was not included in the publication. This has now been rectified.

Acknowledgements

Trine Lacoppidan Kæstel for her support in conducting the searches and editing the references. We thank Mathilde Holmskov for assessing the Storebø 2012 trial. We thank Psychiatric Research Unit, Region Zealand Psychiatry Denmark, and Institute for Psychology, University of Southern Denmark, Denmark. We also wish to thank Geraldine Macdonald (Co‐ordinating Editor), Joanne Duffield (Managing Editor) and Margareth Anderson (Information Specialist) of the Cochrane Developmental Psychosocial and Learning Problems Group for providing help and support. Finally, we would like to thank Professor M Fitzpatrick, Department of Psychiatry, Trinity College, Dublin 2, and Dr Donna Gillies for their feedback on an earlier version of this review.

Appendices

Appendix 1. Search strategies

Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL), in the Cochrane Library

#1 (intellect* disabl*):ti,ab,kw
 #2 MeSH descriptor Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity explode all trees
 #3 (adhd or addh):ti,ab,kw
 #4 (attention near/3 deficit):ti,ab,kw
 #5 (hyperactiv*):ti,ab,kw
 #6 (hyperkinesis*):ti,ab,kw
 #7 MeSH descriptor Hyperkinesis explode all trees
 #8 (minimal brain near/3 disorder*):ti,ab,kw
 #9 ((minimal brain near/3 dysfunction*) or (minimal brain near/3 damage*)):ti,ab,kw
 #10 (#1 OR #2 OR #3 OR #4 OR #5 OR #6 OR #7 OR #8 OR #9)
 #11 (social skill training):ti,ab,kw
 #12 (social skills education):ti,ab,kw
 #13 (social competen*):ti,ab,kw
 #14 ((behavior regulation) or (behaviour regulation)):ti,ab,kw
 #15 (social near/10 skills):ti,ab,kw
 #16 (learning near/25 social):ti,ab,kw
 #17 (role play*):ti,ab,kw
 #18 (psychosocial treatment):ti,ab,kw
 #19 (parent education):ti,ab,kw
 #20 (educat* near/10 parent*):ti,ab,kw
 #21 MeSH descriptor Psychotherapy, Group explode all trees
 #22 (behavior modification):ti,ab,kw
 #23 (behaviour modification):ti,ab,kw
 #24 (parent training):ti,ab,kw
 #25 (#11 OR #12 OR #13 OR #14 OR #15 OR #16 OR #17 OR #18 OR #19 OR #20 OR #21 OR #22 OR #23 OR #24)
 #26 (#10 AND #25)

Medline Ovid

1 exp Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/
 2 adhd.mp.
 3 addh.mp.
 4 (attention adj3 deficit).mp.
 5 hyperactiv$.mp.
 6 hyperkinesis$.mp.
 7 exp Hyperkinesis/
 8 (minimal adj brain adj3 disorder$).mp.
 9 (minimal adj brain adj3 dysfunction$).mp.
 10 (minimal adj brain adj3 damage$).mp.
 11 1 or 2 or 3 or 4 or 5 or 6 or 7 or 8 or 9 or 10
 12 social skills training.mp.
 13 social skills education.mp.
 14 social competenc$.mp.
 15 behavior regulation.mp.
 16 behaviour regulation.mp.
 17 (social adj10 skills).mp.
 18 (learning adj25 social).mp.
 19 role play$.mp.
 20 psychosocial treatment.mp.
 21 parent education.mp.
 22 (educat$ adj10 parent$).mp.
 23 exp psychotherapy, group/
 24 behavior modification.mp.
 25 behaviour modification.mp.
 26 12 or 13 or 14 or 15 or 16 or 17 or 18 or 19 or 20 or 21 or 22 or 23 or 24 or 25
 27 randomized controlled trial.pt.
 28 controlled clinical trial.pt.
 29 randomized controlled trials.mp.
 30 random allocation.mp.
 31 double blind method.mp.
 32 single blind method.mp.
 33 clinical trial.pt.
 34 (clin$ adj25 trial$).ti,ab.
 35 ((singl$ or doubl$ or tripl$ or trebl$) adj25 (blind$ or mask$ or dummy$)).mp.
 36 exp clinical trial/
 37 placebos.mp.
 38 placebo$.ti,ab.
 39 random$.ti,ab.
 40 comparative study.mp.
 41 evaluation studies as topic/
 42 exp clinical trials as topic/
 43 follow up studies.mp.
 44 prospective studies.mp.
 45 (control$ or prospectiv$ or volunteer$).ti,ab.
 46 27 or 28 or 29 or 30 or 31 or 32 or 33 or 34 or 35 or 36 or 37 or 38 or 39 or 40 or 41 or 42 or 43 or 44 or 45
 47 11 and 26 and 46

Embase Ovid

1 exp Attention Deficit Disorder/
 2 adhd.mp.
 3 addh.mp.
 4 exp Hyperactivity/
 5 Hyperkinesia/
 6 (attention adj3 deficit).mp.
 7 hyperactiv*.mp.
 8 hyperkinesis*.mp.
 9 (minimal adj brain adj3 disorder*).mp.
 10 (minimal adj brain adj3 dysfunction*).mp.
 11 (minimal adj brain adj3 damage*).mp.
 12 1 or 2 or 3 or 4 or 5 or 6 or 7 or 8 or 9 or 10 or 11
 13 social skills training.mp.
 14 social skills education.mp.
 15 social competence*.mp.
 16 behavior regulation.mp.
 17 behaviour regulation.mp.
 18 (learning adj25 social).mp.
 19 (social adj10 skills).mp.
 20 role play*.mp.
 21 psychosocial treatment.mp.
 22 parent training.mp.
 23 parent education.mp.
 24 (educat* adj10 parent*).mp.
 25 exp behavior modification/
 26 exp group therapy/
 27 13 or 14 or 15 or 16 or 17 or 18 or 19 or 20 or 21 or 22 or 23 or 24 or 25 or 26
 28 controlled study.de.
 29 clinical trial.de.
 30 major clinical study.de.
 31 randomized controlled trial.de.
 32 double blind procedure.de.
 33 clinical article.de.
 34 random$.mp.
 35 control$.mp.
 36 follow up.mp.
 37 ((singl$ or doubl$ or tripl$ or trebl$) adj (blind$ or mask$ or dummy)).mp.
 38 placebo$.mp.
 39 (clinic$ adj (trial$ or study or studies$)).mp.
 40 exp comparative study/
 41 28 or 29 or 30 or 31 or 32 or 33 or 34 or 35 or 36 or 37 or 38 or 39 or 40
 42 12 and 27 and 41

ERIC EBSCOhost

S1 DE attention deficit disorders
 S2 DE attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
 S3 DE hyperactivity
 S4 TX adhd or addh
 S5 TX attention within 3 deficit
 S6 TX attention N3 deficit
 S7 TX hyperkines*
 S8 TX minimal N3 brain N3 disorder*
 S9 TX minimal N3 brain N3 dysfunction*
 S10 TX minimal N3 brain N3 damage*
 S11 S1 OR S2 OR S3 OR S4 OR S5 OR S6 OR S7 OR S8 OR S9 OR S10
 S12 KW randomi*
 S13 AB random* N3 (allocat* or allot* or assign* or basis or divid* or order*)
 S14 AB random* N4 (trial* OR study OR studies)
 S15 AB (control* OR clinic* OR prospectiv*) N5 (trial* OR study OR studies)
 S16 AB allocat* OR allot* OR assign* OR divid* OR order*) N4 (compar* OR control* OR experiment* OR intervent* OR therap* OR treatment*) N4 (group* OR class*)
 S17 AB (singl* OR doubl* OR trebl* OR tripl*) N4 (blind* OR mask*)
 S18 KW placebo*
 S19 AB placebo*
 S20 AB (compar* N5 (trial* OR study OR studies)
 S21 AB (clinic* OR control*) N4 (trial* OR study OR studies)
 S22 S12 OR S13 OR S14 OR S15 OR S16 OR S17 OR S18 OR S19 OR S20 OR S21
 S23 DE "interpersonal competence" or "daily living skills" or "emotional intelligence" or "extraversion introversion" or "interpersonal communication" or "prosocial behavior" or "sharing behavior" or "sensitivity training" or "interpersonal relationship" or "board administrator relationship" or "caregiver child relationship" or "collegiality" or "counselor client relationship" or "dating social" or "employer employee relationship" or "family relationship" or "parent child relationship" or "parent student relationship" or "sibling relationship" or "friendship" or "group unity" or "helping relationship" or "interpersonal attraction" or "interprofessional relationship" or "supervisor supervisee relationship" or "teacher administrator relationship" or "marriage" or "teacher student relationship" or "parent caregiver relationship" or "peer relationship" or "physician patient relationship"
 S24 DE behavior modification
 S25 DE "group therapy" or "group counceling" or "sensitivity training"
 S26 DE "role playing" or "dramatic play"
 S27 DE "parent education"
 S28 KW social skills training
 S29 AB social skills training OR social skills education
 S30 AB social competenc*
 S31 AB behavior regulation
 S32 AB behaviour regulation
 S33 AB learning N5 social
 S34 AB social N5 skill*
 S35 AB psychosocial treatment
 S36 AB parent training
 S37 AB educat* N3 parent*
 S38 S23 OR S24 OR S25 OR S26 OR S27 OR S28 OR S29 OR S30 OR S31 OR S32 OR S33 OR S34 OR S35 OR S36 OR S37
 S39 S11 AND S22 AND S38

CINAHL EBSCOhost (Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature)

S42 S20 and S41
 S41 S22 or S23 or S24 or S25 or S26 or S27 or S28 or S29 or S30 or S31 or S32 or S33 or S34 or S35 or S36 or S37 or S38 or S39 or S40
 S40 TI behaviour modification or AB behaviour modification
 S39 TI behavior modification or AB behavior modification
 S38 TI learning N3 social or AB learning N3 social
 S37 TI social N3 skills or AB social N3 skills
 S36 TI educat* N2 parent* or AB educat* N2 parent*
 S35 (MH "Psychotherapy, Group")
 S34 (MH "Role Playing")
 S33 TX parent education
 S32 TX parent training
 S31 TX psychosocial treatment
 S30 TX behaviour regulation
 S29 TX behavior regulation
 S28 TX social competence*
 S27 TX social skills education
 S26 TX social skills training
 S25 (MH "Social Skills")
 S24 (MH "Social Behavior+/ED")
 S23 (MH "Interpersonal Relations+/ED")
 S22 (MH "Social Skills Training")
 S21 S9 and S19
 S20 S9 and S19
 S19 S10 or S11 or S12 or S13 or S14 or S15 or S16 or S17 or S18
 S18 TX ( singl* OR doubl* OR tripl* OR trebl* ) and TX ( blind* OR mask* OR dummy* )
 S17 TX clin* N25 trial*
 S16 (MH "Placebos")
 S15 TX placebo* OR random*
 S14 TX control* OR prospectiv* OR volunteer*
 S13 (MH "Evaluation Research+")
 S12 (MH "Prospective Studies+")
 S11 PT clinical trial
 S10 (MH "Clinical Trials+")
 S9 S1 or S2 or S3 or S4 or S5 or S6 or S7 or S8
 S8 TX minimal N1 brain N3 damage*
 S7 TX minimal N1 brain N3 dysfunction*
 S6 TX minimal N1 brain N3 disorder*
 S5 TX hyperkinesis*
 S4 TX hyperactiv*
 S3 TX attention N3 deficit
 S2 TX adhd or addh
 S1 (MH "Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder")

PsycINFO Ovid

1 exp attention deficit disorder/
 2 adhd.mp.
 3 addh.mp.
 4 (attention adj3 deficit).mp.
 5 hyperactiv$.mp.
 6 hyperkinesis$.mp.
 7 exp Hyperkinesis/
 8 (minimal adj brain adj3 disorder$).mp.
 9 (minimal adj brain adj3 dysfunction$).mp.
 10 (minimal adj brain adj3 damage$).mp.
 11 1 or 2 or 3 or 4 or 5 or 6 or 7 or 8 or 9 or 10
 12 exp Social Skills Training/
 13 exp Social Skills/
 14 Skill Learning/
 15 exp Human Relations Training/
 16 exp Parent Training/
 17 social skills training.mp.
 18 social skills education.mp.
 19 social competence$.mp.
 20 behavior regulation.mp.
 21 (social adj10 skills).mp.
 22 (learning adj25 social).mp.
 23 role play$.mp.
 24 exp Communication skills training/
 25 psychosocial treatment.mp.
 26 exp Assertiveness training/
 27 exp Behavior modification/
 28 behaviour regulation.mp.
 29 12 or 13 or 14 or 15 or 16 or 17 or 18 or 19 or 20 or 21 or 22 or 23 or 24 or 25 or 26 or 27 or 28
 30 random$.mp.
 31 ((singl$ or doubl$ or trebl$ or tripl$) adj25 (blind$ or dummy or mask$)).mp.
 32 placebo$.mp.
 33 crossover.mp.
 34 assign$.mp.
 35 allocat$.mp.
 36 ((clin$ or control$ or compar$ or evaluat$ or prospectiv$) adj25 (trial$ or studi$ or study)).mp.
 37 exp placebo/
 38 exp treatment effectiveness evaluation/
 39 exp mental health program evaluation/
 40 exp experimental design/
 41 versus.id.
 42 vs.id.
 43 30 or 31 or 32 or 33 or 34 or 35 or 36 or 37 or 38 or 39 or 40 or 41 or 42
 44 11 and 29 and 43

Sociological Abstracts ProQuest

SU.EXACT("Attention Deficit Disorder") OR ((add OR adds) OR (hyperactiv* OR hyperkines*) OR (minimal within 1 brain within 3 disorder) OR (minimal within 1 brain within 3 dysfunction*) OR (minimal within 1 brain within 3 damage) OR (attention within 3 deficit*)) AND if(random* NEAR/4 (trial OR study OR studies)) OR if(randomi*) OR if(random* NEAR/4 (allocat* OR assign* OR divid*)) OR if((control* OR clinic* OR divid*) WITHIN 5 (condition* OR experiment* OR treatment* OR control* OR group*)) OR if((singl* OR doubl*) NEAR/4 (blind* OR mask*)) OR if(placebo*) OR if((crossover OR cross over)) OR if((compar* WITHIN 5 (trial* OR study OR studies)))

ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global

su(adhd OR addh OR add OR attention deficit hyperactivity disorder OR add OR adds OR attention deficit disorder) AND ab(social skill* OR role play OR psychosocial treatment OR parent education OR group therapy OR behavior modification OR behaviour modfication OR behavior regulation OR behaviour regulation OR social competence)

Clinical Trials (https://clinicaltrials.gov/)

attention deficit hyperactivity disorder OR adhd OR attention OR attention deficit disorder OR hyperactivity OR hyperkin* | social skills training OR social skills education OR social competenc* OR behavior regulation OR social skill* OR role play* OR psychosocial treatment OR parent education OR group therapy OR behavior modification OR behaviour modification | Child |

WHO ICTRP (http://www.who.int/ictrp/en/)

attention deficit hyperactivity disorder OR adhd OR attention OR attention deficit disorder OR hyperactivity OR hyperkin* | social skills training OR social skills education OR social competenc% OR behavior regulation OR social skill* OR role play* OR psychosocial treatment OR parent education OR group therapy OR behavior modification OR behaviour modification | Child |

Appendix 2. Data extraction sheet

Version number: 1:1. 08‐10‐2010

For notes

:Source

Study ID
Report ID
Year of publication
Year of conduct
Review author(s)
Citation source
Authors

 Footnotes

ID: identifier

Eligibility

Confirm eligibility
Reasons for exclusion

Study

Design (e.g. randomised, blinded, placebo, etc.)  
 
Location (e.g. hospital, outpatient clinic)  
Duration of study  
Inclusion criteria
 
 
Exclusion criteria
 
 
Outcomes Primary  
Secondary  

Risk of bias

Domain Judgement (low/uncertain/high) Adequacy (yes/unclear/no) Descriptions
Generation of the allocation sequence     Quote:
Comment:
Allocation concealment     Quote:
Comment:
Blinding of participants and personnel     Quote:
Comment:
Blinding of outcome assessment     Quote:
Comment:
Incomplete outcome data     Quote:
Comment:
Selective outcome reporting     Quote:
Comment:
Vested interest bias     Quote:
Comment:
Other sources of bias: baseline imbalance     Quote:
Comment:
Other sources of bias: early stopping     Quote:
Comment:

Participants

Sample size or power calculation (yes/no): Quote:
Comment:
Total number (sample size): Pre‐randomisation:
Post‐randomisation:
Diagnostic criteria (e.g. ICD‐10 number, DSM‐IV number or by a cut‐off score from report)  
Age  
Sex  
Comorbidity    
Sociodemographics (e.g. double or single parent families, low, middle or upper class)  
Country/ethnicity  
Co‐medication  

Footnotes

DSM‐IV: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders ‐ Fourth Edition.
 ICD‐10: International Classification of Diseases ‐ Tenth Revision.

Interventions

Intervention groups
Number of participants allocated per group
Number of patients lost to follow‐up per group
Format and duration of the intervention (e.g. group base, individual, and setting)
Specific intervention (e.g. type of programme) and by whom (e.g. nurse, psychologist, teacher)
Content of the intervention
Treatment compliance (treatment to manual and participant to treatment)

Outcomes

Outcomes specified Reported (yes/no) Definition and unit of measurement Type of scale Summary statistic for each intervention group (short‐, medium‐ or long‐term)
         

Miscellaneous

Funding source
Key conclusions of the study authors
References to other relevant studies
Correspondence required
Miscellaneous comments from the study authors
Miscellaneous comments from the review authors

Appendix 3. Criteria for assigning 'Risk of bias' judgements

We assessed the 'Risk of bias' components as follows.

Generation of the allocation sequence

  • Low risk of bias: the method used was either adequate (for example, computer‐generated random numbers or table of random numbers), or was unlikely to have introduced selection bias

  • Uncertain risk of bias: there was not enough information to assess whether the method used could have caused bias

  • High risk of bias: the method used was improper and likely to have introduced bias

Allocation concealment

  • Low risk of bias: the method used (for example, central allocation) probably did not bias the observed intervention effect

  • Uncertain risk of bias: there was not enough information to assess whether the method used could have biased the estimate of effect

  • High risk of bias: the method (for example, open random allocation schedule) used probably biased the observed intervention effect

Blinding of participants and personnel

  • Low risk of bias: the method of blinding was described and blinding was carried out satisfactorily

  • Uncertain risk of bias: there was insufficient information to assess whether the type of blinding used could have biased the estimate of effect

  • High risk of bias: no blinding or incomplete blinding

Blinding of outcome assessors

  • Low risk of bias: the method of blinding was described and blinding was carried out satisfactorily

  • Uncertain risk of bias: there was insufficient information to assess whether the type of blinding used could have biased the estimate of effect

  • High risk of bias: no blinding or incomplete blinding

Incomplete outcome data

  • Low risk of bias: the underlying reasons for the missing data probably did not affect the outcome measurement of the effect of the study, or valid methods were used to handle the missing data

  • Uncertain risk of bias: there was not enough information to assess whether the missing data, or the method used to handle missing data, could have biased the estimate of effect

  • High risk of bias: the crude estimate of effect was definitely biased due to the underlying reasons for the missing data, or the methods used to handle missing data were unsatisfactory

Selective outcome reporting

  • Low risk of bias: the study protocol was available, or all prespecified outcomes that were of interest were reported

  • Uncertain risk of bias: there was not enough information to assess whether the direction and magnitude of the observed effect was related to selective outcome reporting

  • High risk of bias: not all of the prespecified primary outcomes were reported or participants were excluded after randomisation (selection bias)

Vested interest bias

  • Low risk of bias: the study's source(s) of funding did not come from any parties that might have had conflicts of interest (for example, a drug or a device manufacturer), or the study author(s) had not conducted previous studies addressing the same interventions

  • Uncertain risk of bias: the source of funding was not clear, or it was not clear if the study author(s) had conducted previous studies addressing the same interventions

  • High risk of bias: the study was funded by parties that might have conflicts of interest (e.g. a manufacture of a drug or a device manufacturer), or potential conflicts of interest were reported by study authors

Other sources of bias

  1. Low risk of bias: the study appeared to be free of other sources of bias

  2. Uncertain risk of bias: there was inadequate information and therefore it was not possible to assess other possible sources of bias

  3. High risk of bias: it is likely that potential sources of bias were present; for example, bias related to the specific design used, early termination due to some data‐dependent process, or lack of power calculation, or other bias risks

We defined overall low risk of bias studies as studies that had low risk of bias in all domains. We considered studies with one or more unclear or high risk of bias domains as studies with high risk of bias overall.

Appendix 4. Glossary

Attention Control Treatment ACT
Learning Skills Training for Adolescents with ADHD ADHS‐LeJA
ADHD plus Impairments in Mood AIM
Academic Performance Rating Scale APRS
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder ADHD
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder ‐ Inattentive subtype ADHD‐I
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder ‐ Rating Scales ADHD‐RS
Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function BRIEF
Child Behavior Checklist CBCL
[Connors] Comprehensive Behavior Rating Scales CBRS
Cognitive Behaviour Therapy CBT
Classroom Challenge CC
Conduct Disorder CD
Clinical Global Impression CGI
Challenging Horizons Program – After School version CHP‐AS
Challenging Horizons Program – Mentoring version CHP‐M
Confidence Interval CI
Collaborative Life skills CL
Clinical Linguistic and Auditory Milestone Scale CLAM
Child Life and Attention Skills [program] CLAS
Community Oriented Parent Education [program] COPE
Conners Parent Rating Scale CPRS
Classroom Performance Survey CPS
Continuous Performance Test CPT
Client Satisfaction Questionnaire CSQ
Conners Teacher Rating Scale CTRS
Comorbid disorder Additional condition co‐occurring with the primary condition
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders ‐ Third edition ‐ Revised DSM‐III‐R
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders ‐ Fourth edition DSM‐IV
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders ‐ Fourth edition ‐ Text Revision DSM‐IV‐ TR
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders ‐ Fifth edition DSM‐5
Disruptive Behavior Disorders Rating Scale DBDRS
Diversity‐Adjusted Required Information Size DARIS
Diagnostic Interview for Children & Adolescents ‐ Revised DIACA‐R
Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents ‐ Revised Parent version DICA‐R‐P
Parent interview with the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children DISC‐P2
Emotion Expression Scale for Children EESC
Emotion Management Training EMT
Emotion Regulation Checklist ERC
Fremdbeurteilungsbogen für Hyperkinetische Störungen FBB‐HKS
Full Scale Intelligence Quotient FSIQ
Information Size IS
Intelligence Quotient IQ
International Classification of Diseases ‐ 10th version ICD‐10
International Committee of Harmonization guidelines ICH
Impairment rating scale IRS
Intention‐To‐Treat ITT
Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia ‐ Present and Lifetime version K‐SADS
Mean Difference MD
Matching Familiar Figures Test MFFT
Multimodal Psychosocial treatment MPT
Multimodal Treatment of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder MTA
Number Needed to Treat NNT
Oppositional Defiant Disorder ODD
Primary Counsellor PC
Parent version of the Children’s Interview for Psychiatric Syndromes P‐ChIPS
Parent‐Focused Treatment PFT
Principal Investigator PI
Participant Intervention Comparison Outcome PICO
Progressive Muscle Relaxation PMR
Randomised Clinical Trial RCT
Required Information Size RIS
Risk Difference RD
Risk Ratio RR
Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitor SSRI
Self‐Control Rating Scale SCRS
Sluggish Cognitive Tempo SCT
Social Skills Improvement System SSIS
Social Skills Rating Scale SSRS
Social skills training SST
Standard Deviation SD
Standardized Mean Difference SMD
Social Skills Training Plus Parental Training Combined with Standard Treatment SOSTRA
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire SDQ
Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD Symptoms and Normal Behaviors SWAN
Swanson, Nolan and Pelham (‐ Revised) rating scale SNAP (‐R)
Treatment As Usual TAU
Trial Sequential Analysis TSA
Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence WASI
Wechsler Individual Achievement Test WIAT
WEISS Functional Impairment Scale ‐ Parent form WFIRS‐P
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children ‐ Revised edition WISC‐R
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children ‐ Third edition WISC‐III
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children ‐ Fourth edition WISC‐IV
Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence WIPPSI
World Health Organization WHO
Waiting List WL

Data and analyses

Comparison 1. Social skills.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 Primary meta‐analysis: Teacher‐rated social skills at end of treatment 11   Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) Subtotals only
1.1 All eligible trials 11 1271 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.11 [‐0.00, 0.22]
1.2 Sensitivity analysis excluding the 3 trials with longest treatment duration 8 620 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.11 [‐0.05, 0.27]
2 Secondary meta‐analyses: Social skills 19 2649 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.29 [0.11, 0.47]
2.1 Teacher‐rated social skills at longest follow‐up 3 192 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.06 [‐0.22, 0.35]
2.2 Parent‐rated social skills at end of treatment for all eligible trials 15 1609 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.19 [0.06, 0.32]
2.3 Parent‐rated social skills at longest follow‐up 2 445 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.13 [‐0.35, 0.62]
2.4 Observer‐rated social skills at end of treatment for all eligible trials 1 29 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 2.88 [1.80, 3.96]
2.5 Participants‐rated social skills at end of treatment for all eligible trials 5 344 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.28 [‐0.68, 1.23]
2.6 Participant‐rated social skills at longest follow‐up 1 30 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 1.60 [0.77, 2.44]
3 Teacher‐reported Walker‐McConnell Scale of Social Competence and School Adjustment 1 46 Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 1.06 [‐0.47, 2.59]
4 Parent‐rated Social Skills Scale (UCI) 1 18 Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 9.70 [6.07, 13.33]
5 Child‐rated Test of Social Skill Knowledge 1 18 Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 4.20 [1.99, 6.41]
6 Social Interaction Observation Code: Negative behaviour 1 68 Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.20 [‐0.11, 0.51]

Comparison 2. Emotional competencies.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 Primary meta‐analysis: Teacher‐rated emotional competencies at end of treatment 2 129 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.02 [‐0.72, 0.68]
2 Secondary meta‐analyses: Emotional competencies 5 353 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.20 [‐0.41, 0.01]
2.1 Parent‐rated emotional competencies 3 173 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.27 [‐0.59, 0.05]
2.2 Parent‐rated emotional competencies at longest follow‐up 1 55 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.19 [‐0.34, 0.72]
2.3 Participant‐rated emotional competencies 2 125 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.27 [‐0.62, 0.09]

Comparison 3. General behaviour.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 Primary meta‐analysis: Teacher‐rated general behaviour at end of treatment 8   Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) Subtotals only
1.1 All eligible trials 8 1002 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.06 [‐0.19, 0.06]
1.2 Sensitivity analysis excluding the 2 trials with the longest treatment duration 6 422 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.09 [‐0.28, 0.10]
1.3 Sensitivity analysis excluding the 2 largest trials 6 422 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.09 [‐0.28, 0.10]
2 Secondary analyses: general behaviour 9 2034 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.26 [‐0.40, ‐0.12]
2.1 Teacher‐rated general behaviour at longest follow‐up 4 637 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.10 [‐0.27, 0.07]
2.2 Parent‐rated general behaviour at end of treatment 8 995 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.38 [‐0.61, ‐0.14]
2.3 Parent‐rated general behaviour at longest follow‐up 1 326 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.21 [‐0.44, 0.03]
2.4 Participant‐rated general behaviour at end of treatment 1 76 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.07 [‐0.52, 0.38]

Comparison 4. Core ADHD symptoms.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 Primary meta‐analysis: Teacher‐rated ADHD symptoms at end of treatment 14   Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) Subtotals only
1.1 All eligible trials 14 1379 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.26 [‐0.47, ‐0.05]
1.2 Sensitivity analysis excluding the 3 trials with longest treatment duration 11 677 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.24 [‐0.52, 0.04]
1.3 Sensitivity analysis excluding the 3 largest trials 11 677 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.24 [‐0.52, 0.04]
2 Secondary meta‐analyses: ADHD symptoms 15 2857 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.39 [‐0.63, ‐0.15]
2.1 Teacher‐rated ADHD symptoms at longest follow‐up 5 582 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.11 [‐0.28, 0.06]
2.2 Parent‐rated ADHD symptoms at end of treatment for all eligible trials 11 1206 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.54 [‐0.81, ‐0.26]
2.3 Parent‐rated ADHD symptoms at longest follow‐up 3 476 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐1.36 [‐2.48, ‐0.25]
2.4 Participant‐rated ADHD symptoms at end of treatment 2 106 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.77 [‐2.31, 0.78]
2.5 Observer‐rated ADHD symptoms at end of treatment for all eligible trials 2 107 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐3.15 [‐9.88, 3.57]
2.6 Observer‐rated ADHD symptoms at longest follow for all eligible trials 1 30 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 3.95 [2.66, 5.23]
2.7 Single study result: Teacher‐rated ADHD symptoms (inattention) at end of treatment 1 254 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.01 [‐0.23, 0.26]
2.8 Single study result: Teacher‐rated ADHD symptoms (sluggish cognitive tempo) at end of treatment 1 66 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.29 [‐0.78, 0.20]
2.9 Single study result: Participant‐rated ADHD symptoms at longest follow‐up 1 30 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 1.62 [0.78, 2.46]

Comparison 5. Teacher‐rated performance and grades in school.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 At end of treatment 5 642 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.15 [‐0.01, 0.31]
2 At longest follow‐up 2 379 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.01 [‐0.22, 0.20]

Comparison 6. Observer‐rated performance and grades in school.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 Wescheler Individual Achievement Test 1 260 Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) 1.5 [‐2.06, 5.06]

Comparison 7. TSA.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 Teacher‐rated social skills 11   Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) Subtotals only
1.1 At end of treatment 4 185 Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 1.80 [‐1.01, 4.62]
1.2 All eligible trials 11 1271 Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 1.22 [0.09, 2.36]

Comparison 8. Subgroup analysis 1: Children aged five to 11 years versus children aged 12 to 18 years.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 Teacher‐rated social skills 11 1271 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.11 [‐0.00, 0.22]
1.1 Children aged 5 to 11 years 10 1194 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.11 [‐0.01, 0.22]
1.2 Children aged 12 to 18 years 1 77 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.16 [‐0.28, 0.61]

Comparison 9. Subgroup analysis 2: ADHD and comorbidity versus ADHD and no comorbidity.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 Parent‐rated ADHD symptoms at end of treatment 10   Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) Subtotals only
1.1 ADHD and comorbidity 8 1003 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.41 [‐0.67, ‐0.15]
1.2 ADHD and no comorbidity 2 173 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) ‐0.51 [‐1.05, 0.03]

Comparison 10. Subgroup analysis 3: Social skills training only versus social skills training supported by parent training.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 Teacher‐rated social skills at end of treatment 6 968 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.14 [‐0.00, 0.28]
1.1 Social skills training supported by parent training 4 336 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.22 [‐0.01, 0.45]
1.2 Social skills training only (i.e. without parent training) 4 632 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.15 [‐0.12, 0.41]

Comparison 11. Subgroup analysis 4: Social skills training, parental training and medication versus social skills training and parental training without medication.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 Parent‐rated social skills at end of treatment 8 636 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.26 [0.05, 0.48]
1.1 Social skills training, parent training and parent‐rated medication 4 299 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.10 [‐0.20, 0.39]
1.2 Social skills training, parent training without parent‐rated medication 4 337 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.43 [0.15, 0.70]

Comparison 12. Subgroup analysis 5: No‐intervention control group versus waiting‐list control group with possible minor active intervention components.

Outcome or subgroup title No. of studies No. of participants Statistical method Effect size
1 Teacher‐rated social skills at end of treatment 11 1271 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.11 [‐0.00, 0.22]
1.1 No‐intervention control group 8 693 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.12 [‐0.03, 0.27]
1.2 Waiting‐list control group with possible minor active intervention components 3 578 Std. Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) 0.14 [‐0.13, 0.42]

Characteristics of studies

Characteristics of included studies [ordered by year of study]

Cohen 1981.

Methods Design: RCT
Participants Country: ethnicity: 100 % Canadian, so Canada was assumed
Setting: not reported
Sample size calculation: not reported
Sample size: 24 children
Sex: 21 (87.5%) = boys, three (12.5%) = girls
Age: range = five‐six years
Ethnicity: Canadian
Socioeconomic status: not reported
IQ: IQ ≥ 80 on WPPSI
ADHD diagnosis: subtypes not reported
ADHD medication: not reported
Comorbidity: not reported
Medications for comorbid disorders: not reported
Inclusion criteria:
  1. scores ≥ 1.5 on CTRS

  2. IQ ≥ 80 on WPPSI

  3. no neurological damage or psychosis


Exclusion criteria: no neurological damage or psychosis
Baseline characteristics: no information about medication balance between groups
Interventions 24 children allocated to one of four groups
  1. Group one (n = 6): cognitive behaviour modification. Individual training to teach children to slow down, develop better problem‐solving ability, and evaluate his/her own performance. one‐hour × twice‐weekly sessions for 10 weeks (total of 20 sessions)

  2. Group two (n = 8): methylphenidate only. Drug dosage individually titrated and ranged from 10 mg to 30 mg methylphenidate per day

  3. Group three (n = 6): cognitive behaviour modification as described in group one + methylphenidate as administered in group two

  4. Group four (n = 4): no intervention


Attendance: not reported
Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills: observations in classrooms, observer‐rated

  2. Emotional competencies: Richman‐Graham scale, parent‐rated

  3. General behaviour: CBRS, teacher‐ and parent‐rated


Outcome assessment: end of treatment
Notes Study ID: not reported
Sponsorship source: grants from the Ontario Mental Health Foundation (Grant
 No. 701‐76/78) and The Hospital for Sick Children Foundation (Grant No. 77‐22)
Year conducted: not stated
Duration of the study: 15 months
Comments: none
Lead author's name: NJ Cohen
Institution: Psychiatric Research Unit, The Hospital for Sick Children
Email: not reported
Address: 555 University Avenue, Toronto, Ontario, Canada MSG 1X8.
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: unclear
Allocation concealment (selection bias) High risk Comment: allocation concealment not described, but four patients were moved between groups
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: no blinding
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: blinding on at least one of this review's primary outcomes, but no blinding for the rest of the outcomes
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: many tables different from text and no explanation provided. Lack of teacher responses
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: all apparent assessments were made.
Vested interest bias Low risk Comment: no other apparent biases, no previous research on the topic
Other sources of bias? High risk Comment: selection procedure of patients not stated in report

Bloomquist 1991.

Methods Design: RCT
Participants Country: USA
Setting: outpatient, 3 suburban elementary schools in the same school district
Sample size calculation: not reported
Sample size: 52 children
Sex: 36 (69%) = boys, 16 (31%) = girls
Age: range = eight‐nine years
Ethnicity: Caucasian = 95%
Socioeconomic status: not reported
IQ: mental retardation excluded
ADHD diagnosis: subtypes not reported
ADHD medication: not reported
Comorbidity: ODD = 18 (35%)
Medications for comorbid disorders: not reported
Inclusion criteria:
  1. T‐score ≥ 60 on the CBCL‐Teacher

  2. signed consent form

  3. T‐score ≥ 65 on the CBCL‐Parent

  4. ADHD diagnosis on the basis of DIACA‐R


Exclusion criteria:
  1. mental retardation

  2. epilepsy

  3. severe emotional disorder

  4. pervasive development disorder


Baseline characteristics: groups were highly comparable on descriptive and subjective identification measures; age, IQ, academic achievement, hyperactivity and self‐control behaviour, externalising and internalising behaviour at baseline.
Interventions 52 children with ADHD were allocated to one of three groups. Only group one and three were included in the analysis.
  1. Group one (n = 20): multicomponent CBT based on Braswell and Bloomquist (1991) and Bloomquist and Braswell's (Bloomquist 1991) CBT programme for ADHD children, which included coordinated child, parent, and teacher training components

    1. Child component: used a variety of cognitive‐ behavioural techniques such as didactic instructions, modelling, role‐play exercises and so on. It was led by a school psychologist and consisted of two × one‐hour group sessions each week over a 10‐week period (20 sessions).

    2. Parent component: targeted to teach the parents about ADHD, to establish a positive trusting atmosphere among the parents, and to teach them cognitive/behavioural principles identical to those addressed in the teacher training component. It was led by a therapist and consisted of seven × 90‐minute group sessions.

    3. Teacher training component: focused on, for example, problem‐solving in the classroom and on reinforcing appropriate behaviour and consequating disruptive behaviour. It was led by a consultant and consisted of one × two‐hour in service and six × 45 to 60‐minute consultations over a 10‐week period.

  2. Group two (n = 16): teacher intervention. Same teacher component as above but without the child only and the parent components

  3. Group three (n = 16): waiting list, consisting of no intervention


Attendance: almost 100% for child and teacher interventions
Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills: Teacher Report‐Walker‐McConnell Scale of Social Competance and School Adjustment, teacher‐rated

  2. General behaviour: Self‐Control Rating Scale, teacher‐rated


Secondary outcomes
  1. Core ADHD symptoms: CTRS, teacher‐rated


Outcome assessment: post‐intervention assessment
Notes Study ID: not reported
Sponsorship source: not reported
Year conducted: not stated
Duration of the study: 16 weeks
Comments: authors referred to unpublished paper with the treatment manual by Bloomquist and Braswell (Bloomquist 1991)
Lead author: Michael L. Bloomquist, PhD
Institution: Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
Email: not reported
Address: Box 95, University of Minnesota Hospital and Clinic, Harvard Street at East River Road, Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455
Year conducted: not reported
Duration of the study: not reported
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: no description of the randomisation method used
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: no description of the allocation method used
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: no blinding
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: observers were blinded to treatment assignment but teachers were not. No blinding on primary outcomes
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: 16 excluded data sets with much likelihood of biasing results
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: all measures of interest reported
Vested interest bias Unclear risk Comment: no funding sources stated
Other sources of bias? Unclear risk Comment: no test for compliance of intervention groups

Pfiffner 1997.

Methods Design: RCT
Participants Country: USA
Setting: university‐based behavioural paediatric clinic specialising in ADHD and related disorders. Participants were recruited from newspaper advertisement and from consecutive referrals.
Sample size calculation: not reported
Sample size: 27 children
Sex: 19 (70%) = boys, eight (30%) = girls
Age: range = eight‐10 years
Ethnicity: all Caucasian except for one boy, who was African American
Socioeconomic status: middle‐ to upper‐middle class; two children were from single‐parent families
IQ: not reported
ADHD diagnosis: following DSM‐III‐R, 25 children met criteria for ADHD and two met criteria for undifferentiated ADHD
ADHD medication: n = 12 (44%) received stimulant medication
Comorbidity: ODD = 19, CD = 3, separation anxiety disorder = four, overanxious disorder = five, dysthymic disorder = two
Medication for comorbid disorder: no information
Inclusion criteria:
  1. diagnosis of ADHD or undifferentiated ADHD following DSM‐III‐R criteria

  2. mean score at or above 1.5 on at least one of the parent‐completed subscales assessing ADHD. behaviour from the CLAM rating scale or the SNAP‐R, a T score of at least 60 on the Attention Problem subscale of the CBCL


Exclusion criteria: not reported. We attempted to get this information from the study investigators but have not succeeded in this attempt.
Baseline characteristics: at pretreatment, no significant difference in age, socioeconomic status, medication status and number of symptoms of ADHD, and comorbid disorders or on parent and teacher ratings of social skills and behaviour
Interventions 18 participants allocated to one of three groups (see below). Both treatment groups had a protocol and were led by psychologist and the same two therapists taught in the childrens' groups. The two treatment groups attended eight group sessions. Children in both treatment groups received 90‐minute group sessions during consecutive weeks. Assessment was at pre‐ and post‐treatment and follow‐up (3‐4 months post‐treatment).
  1. Group one (n = 9): social skills training (SST) for children, which covered six themes/modules: 1) good sportsmanship; 2) accepting consequences; 3) assertiveness; 4) ignoring provocations; 5) problem‐solving; and 6) recognising and dealing with feelings. Children were assigned homework to practice at home. The children received points for following the rules of the groups, participating, and attending the sessions. The points could be exchanged for child‐selected games and activities during the last 10 minutes for each group.

  2. Group two (n = 9): SST for children with parent‐mediated generalisation. Parents were used as a primary vehicle to programme generalisation of the social skills learned in the SST groups to home and school settings. The parents went through the same group themes or agendas as the children did. The parents met with their children's teacher and gave the teacher a template for the scorecard, also called the daily report card. The teacher scored the child on a four‐point scale and parents rewarded the child when the child scored high on the scale.

  3. Group three (n = 9): waiting list


Attendance: two families each missed one session
Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills: SSRS, parent‐, teacher and parent‐rated; Test of Social Skill Knowledge, observer‐rated (interviewers, scored by blinded raters)


Secondary outcomes
  1. Satisfaction with treatment: Consumer Satisfaction Questionnaire, parent‐rated


Outcome assessment: post‐intervention and follow‐up three to four months after post‐intervention assessment
Notes Study ID: not reported
Sponsorship source: not reported
Year conducted: not stated
Duration of the study: 8 months
Comments: none
Lead author: Linda J Pfiffner
Institution: Department of Psychiatry, The University of Chicago
Email: not reported
Address: 5841 South Maryland Avenue, MC 3077, Chicago, Illinois 60637
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: no information given in the article. We requested clarification from the study investigators and they reported in an email on 26 May 2011 that it was not possible to obtain these data at that time (Pfiffner 2011 [pers comm]).
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: no information given in the article. We requested clarification from the study investigators and they reported in an email 26 May 2011 that it was not possible to find these data at that time (Pfiffner 2011 [pers comm]).
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: no blinding
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: blinding on at least one of this study's primary outcomes; no blinding for the rest of the outcomes
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes Low risk Comment: three participants started medication after post‐intervention assessment but before follow‐up assessment. Individual scores for these participants were replaced by the grand mean of all other participants at follow‐up to avoid possible confounds associated with the medication treatment.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) High risk Comment: the author informed us in an email (Pfiffner 2011 [pers comm] that the CLAM and SNAP were used post‐treatment, but were not reported in the article. We were not able to get the data because they had been lost over time.
Vested interest bias Low risk Comment: no other apparent biases, no previous research on the topic
Other sources of bias? High risk Comment: Teachers were paid $10 for post‐intervention assessment and $25 for follow‐up assessment and families were paid $12 for follow‐up assessment.

Abikoff 2004.

Methods Design: RCT
Participants Country: USA and Canada
Setting: outpatient clinics in two large medical centres in New York and Montreal
Sample size calculation: not reported
Sample size: 103 children
Sex: 93% = boys, 7% = girls
Age: range = 7‐9.9 years
Ethnicity: white = 84%, African‐American = 13%, Hispanic = 2%, other = 1%
Socioeconomic status: 84 children (81.2%) lived with both parents, 13 (12.6%) with one parent, and six (5.8%) with their mother and stepfather
IQ: normal IQ (i.e. WISC‐R ≥ 85)
ADHD diagnosis: subtypes not reported
ADHD medication: all participants received psychostimulants
Comorbidity: ODD = 53.4%, CD = 30%, anxiety disorder = 16.5%
Medications for comorbid disorders: not reported
Inclusion criteria:
  1. diagnosis of ADHD based on the DISC‐P2 conducted by a clinical psychologist. The diagnosis had to be confirmed by a child psychiatrist based on a comprehensive clinical interview with the child and parent and teacher reports. The children had to, on two different occasions, receive a mean teacher rating of at least 1.5 on the hyperactivity factor or the Hyperactivity Index of the Conners Teachers Rating Scale (Conners 1998).

  2. children had to be medication‐free for at last two weeks before evaluation.

  3. normal IQ (i.e. WISC‐R ≥ 85)

  4. living with at least one parent, with telephone access

  5. positive response to methylphenidate


Exclusion criteria:
  1. children with diagnosable neurological disorders

  2. psychosis

  3. significant medical illness

  4. current physical or sexual abuse

  5. chronic tic disorder or Tourette's disorder

  6. DSM‐III‐R‐based developmental reading or arithmetic disorder, defined as a standard score in reading or mathematics on the Kaufmann Test of Educational Achievement of 85 or less

  7. children with a diagnosis of conduct disorder


Baseline characteristics: no between‐group differences except on socioeconomic status, where there were differences between the group given methylphenidate alone and the group given methylphenidate + attention control treatment
Interventions 103 participants allocated to one of three groups
  1. Group one (n = 34): medical manual and methylphenidate (effort was made to give each child a maximal dose of methylphenidate). Five‐week open methylphenidate titration study before randomisation

  2. Group two (n = 34): methylphenidate + Multimodal Psychosocial Treatment (MPT) comprising parent training/family therapy, academic organisational skills training, individualised academic assistance, reading remediation (when necessary), social skills training, and individual psychotherapy. All treatment modules were fully manual‐based and the manual was developed before the start of the study. Each component was delivered once a week in the first year and once a month during the second year. Parents received parent management training and counselling. Daily report cards were completed by teachers and formed the basis for a home‐based reinforcement programme for targeted school behaviour and academic performance.

  3. Group three (n = 35): methylphenidate + attention control treatment (ACT) consisting of components parallel to those in MPT but excluding the therapeutic content. Delivered once a week in the first year and once monthly during the second year


Attendance: 75% attendance required. 22 children (methylphenidate = 10, methylphenidate + MPT = 6, methylphenidate + ACT = 6)
Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills: SSRS, parent‐ and child‐rated; Social Interaction Observation Code

  2. General behaviour: CTRS, Conduct Problems Factor, teacher‐rated


Secondary outcomes
  1. Core ADHD symptoms: CPRS, Hyperkinesis Index, parent‐rated; CTRS, Hyperactivity Factor, teacher‐rated; CTRS Hyperkinesis Index, observer‐rated


Outcome assessment: end of treatment
Notes Study ID: not reported
Sponsorship source: not reported
Year conducted: not stated
Duration of the study: 2 years
Comments: none
Lead author's name: Howard Abikoff
Institution: NYU Child Study Center, New York University School of Medicine
Email: abikoh01@med.nyu.edu
Address: NYU Child Study Center, New York University School of Medicine, New York
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Low risk Comment: we requested clarification from one of the study investigators. Howard Abikoff informed us in an email on 28 January 2011 (Abikoff 2011 [pers comm]) that they had used a block randomisation scheme with blocks of four children. The groups were balanced for age, sex, ODD, and ethnicity.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Low risk Comment: we requested clarification from 1 of the study investigators. Howard Abikoff informed us in an email as above (Abikoff 2011 [pers comm]) that they had used sealed envelopes.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: no blinding
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: blinding on at least one of this review's primary outcomes but no blinding for the rest of the outcomes
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: 22 out of 103 children failed to complete the study.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Unclear risk Comment: no prior statement of assessment tools. Design article published at the same time as study article
Vested interest bias High risk Comment: the study was based in two large medical centres and the centres have extensive previous experience with research focused on ADHD and behavioural treatment. Dr Klein is a member of a pharmaceutical board.
Other sources of bias? Low risk Comment: no other sources identified

MTA 1999.

Methods Design: RCT
Participants Country: USA
Setting: six multisite outpatient clinics
Sample size calculation: 576 participants required
Sample size: 576 children
Sex: 465 (81%) = boys, 111 (19%) = girls
Age: range 7.0 to 9.9 years
Ethnicity: white = 61%, African‐American = 20%, Hispanic = 8%
Socioeconomic status: not reported
IQ: below 80 excluded
ADHD diagnosis:DSM‐IV, ADHD combined type
ADHD medication: 97% received methylphenidate
Comorbidity: anxiety disorder = 33.5%, conduct disorder = 14.3%, ODD = 39.9%, affective disorder = 3.8%, tic disorder = 10.9%, other = 2.2% such as bulimia, enuresis
Medications for comorbid disorders: balanced between groups.
Inclusion criteria:
  1. boys and girls

  2. 7‐9.9 years of age (1st‐4th grades)

  3. residing with primary caretakers for at least six months

  4. meet dimensional criteria for hyperactivity on the basis of parent and teacher rating scales and full diagnostic criteria for ADHD combined type


Exclusion criteria:
  1. currently in hospital (inability to obtain school assessments)

  2. currently in another treatment study (confounding of assessments and treatments)

  3. below 80 on WISC‐III Verbal IQ, Performance IQ or Full Scale IQ scores, and on Scales of Independent Behavior (insufficient ability to participate in psychosocial interventions)

  4. bipolar disorder, psychosis, pervasive developmental disorder, severe obsessive‐compulsive disorder (treatment may be incompatible with the study intervention)

  5. chronic, serious tics or Tourette's Disorder (possible contraindication for stimulant treatment)

  6. neuroleptic treatment in previous six months (may need resumption, which is incompatible with the study intervention)

  7. major neurological or medical illness that would interfere with study participation or require medications incompatible with the medications used in the study (inability to participate in the study intervention)

  8. history of intolerance to MTA medications (dangerous if participants assigned to arm involving medications)

  9. suicidal or homicidal (needs more intensive treatment than the study intervention provides)

  10. ongoing or previously undisclosed child abuse (risk of removal from home precludes parent intervention and consistent parent data)

  11. missed more than 25% of school days in previous two months (interference with teacher assessments and school intervention)

  12. another child in household already participating in the study intervention (cross‐arm contamination if two children in same household randomised to different arms)

  13. same classroom as child already participating in the study intervention (cross‐arm contamination if two pupils in same classroom are randomised to different arms)

  14. parental stimulant/cocaine abuse in past two years (possible co‐opting of child's medications)

  15. inability of parent to speak English (inability to participate in parent training)

  16. no telephone (inability to participate in telephone calls with therapists)


Baseline characteristics: no significant differences among study groups
Interventions 576 children allocated to one of four groups
  1. Group one (n = 144): medication. In total, 14 months of medical intervention implemented as follows: one month of blind titration with methylphenidate for best dose, if unsatisfactory, then open titration with d‐amphetamine, pemoline, imipramine, and others. Supplementary general advice and selected readings without systematic behavioural intervention. Monthly visits after the titration period, doses adjusted as indicated by monthly monitors

  2. Group two (n = 144): psychosocial. Intensive behavioural treatment consisting of three major components: 1) 27 group and eight individual sessions of parent training; 2) school intervention comprising teacher consultations (six to 20 sessions) and 12 weeks with classroom behavioural specialist for half the time in the classroom; and 3) a child treatment component anchored in an intensive 8‐week, full‐time, summer treatment programme. No medication

  3. Group 3 (n = 145): combined treatment. Integration of all treatment components in medication + psychosocial treatment groups and standard community care

  4. Group 4 (n =143): standard community care. Treatment of own choosing in the community; no treatment provided by the study group


Attendance: in group two and three, the families attended an average of 77.8% of the parent training sessions and 36.2 of 40 possible summer treatment programme days. In the school component, there was an average of 10.7 teacher consultation visits and 47.6 out of 60 possible days of work with a classroom aid
Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills: SSRS, parent‐, child‐ and teacher‐rated

  2. General behaviour: CBCL, parent‐ and teacher‐rated


Secondary outcomes
  1. Core ADHD symptoms: Swanson, Nolan and Pelham Rating Scale, teacher‐ and parent‐rated

  2. Performances and grades in school: Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (WIAT)


Outcome assessment: post‐intervention, end of treatment and follow‐up data
Notes Study ID:NCT00000388
Sponsorship source: The study was supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health (UO1 MH50461, U01 MH50447, U01 MH5044, Uo1 MH50453, U=1 MH 50454 and U01 MH50467).
Year conducted: not stated
Duration of the study: 38 months
Comments: The Multimodal Treatment Study (MTA study) is a cooperative study, performed by six independent research teams in collaboration with the National Institute of Mental Health, Rockville, MD, and the Office of Special Education Programs, US Department of Eduation, Washington, DC.
Lead author: MTA Cooperative Group. Corresponding author: Peter S Jensen
Institution: Department of Child Psychiatry, Unit 78, Center for the Advancement of Children's Mental Health, New York State Psychiatric Institute/Columbia University
Email: jensenp@child.cpmc.columbia.edu
Address: 1051 RiversideDr, New York, NY 10032
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Low risk Comment: adequate method used
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Low risk Comment: adequate method used
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: no blinding
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: blinded and unblinded raters
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes Unclear risk Comment: used imputation
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Unclear risk Comment: where is the consumer satisfaction and the CBCL data reported? We requested clarification from one of the study investigators, but had received no response when this review was finished.
Vested interest bias Low risk Comment: no vested interest
Other sources of bias? Low risk Comment: no other apparent sources of bias

Tutty 2003.

Methods Design: RCT
Participants Country: USA
Setting: outpatient clinic in Washington
Sample size calculation: not reported
Sample size: 100 children
Sex: 75 (75%) = boys, 25 (25%) = girls
Age: range = five‐12 years
Ethnicity: (group one: white = 49 (83%), African‐American = 4 (7%), Asian = 5 (8%), Hispanic = 1 (2%); group two: white = 38 (93%), African‐American = 2 (5%), Asian = 1 (2%), Hispanic = 0 (0%))
Socioeconomic status: parent education (group one: grades nine‐12 = nine (15%), grades 13‐16 = 18 (64%), < grade 16 = 12 (20%); group two: grades nine‐12 = 11 (27%), grades 13‐16 = 25 (61%), < Grade 16 = five (12%))
IQ: not reported
ADHD diagnosis: (group one: inattentive subtype = 25 (42%), combined subtype = 34 (58%); group two: inattentive subtype = 16 (39%), combined subtype = 25 (61%))
ADHD medication: a new prescription for stimulant medication had been filled in for all children
Comorbidity: not reported, but range of comorbid difficulties (see below) were part of exclusion criteria
Medications for comorbid disorders: allowed but not stated if it was balanced between groups
Inclusion criteria: diagnosis of ADHD (DSM‐IV), and filling in new prescription for stimulant medication (i.e. no stimulant medication use in past 120 days)
Exclusion criteria: comorbid CD, ODD, Tourette syndrome, affective disorder, active alcohol or other substance abuse during previously 90 days or chronic mental illness, if children had been enrolled in a child social skills training at the involved centre in the past
Baseline characteristics: mean baseline parented ADHD symptom scores were more symptomatic for the intervention group than for the control group, as well as the use of parent discipline practice. These between‐groups differences were adjusted before follow‐up analysis.
Interventions 100 participants allocated to one of two groups
  1. Group one (n = 59): child social skills training and parent training plus medical treatment. Medication regimen was stabilised for the participants during the first three to four weeks before the social skills training commenced. The social skills training consisted of eight 50‐minute group sessions that were delivered during 8 consecutive weeks. Each session was based on a structured session‐by‐session agenda and focused on the topics: listening, skills, expression of feelings, anger management, self control, conflict resolution, friendship skills, and self‐esteem. During the sessions, the group was divided into child‐only and parent‐only groups. The children were further divided into child groups based on age to minimise age differences within the child group. Seven therapists participated in the study and each therapist had at least two years of direct experience treating this population in individual and group formats, possessed a master's degree in social work, counselling, or educational psychology, and had participated in three 2‐hour preparation sessions with the study coordinator and a senior therapist to review session content, itinerary, and clinical/research protocols before facilitating the social skills training.

  2. Group two (n = 41) : medical treatment alone


Attendance: there was a 95% rate for completing all eight sessions in group one. Blinded follow‐up measures completed by 97% and 98% of parent or guardian participants at three and six months after enrolment, respectively. Follow‐up completion rates for teacher participants yielded 92% and 75% for three and six months after enrolment, respectively. Participants with missing data did not differ from participants with complete data sets across time or any clinical, functional, and demographic variables, according to the authors of the report. For the ADHD Rating Scale outcome, two children were lost to follow‐up. For the Child Attention Profile outcome, 24 children in total were lost to follow‐up (intervention = 16, control = eight).
Outcomes Secondary outcomes
  1. Core ADHD symptoms: ADHD Rating Scale, parent‐rated (telephone interviews); Child Attention Profile, teacher‐rated (telephone interviews)


Outcome assessment: at three (post‐intervention) and six months (follow‐up) after enrolment
Notes Study ID: not reported
Sponsorship source: the study was supported by the Group Health Cooperative (GHC)/Kaiser Permanente Community Foundation through a grant to the GHC, Center for Attention Deficit Disorders, located in Redmond, Washington
Year conducted: not stated
Duration of the study: 12 months
Comments: there was a third outcome used in this study, but it is not relevant for this review, because it measured the parents' discipline practice.
Lead author: Steve Tutty
Institution: Group Health Cooperative, Center for Health Studies
Email: tutty.s@ghc.org
Address: 1730 Minor Avenue, Suite 1600, Seattle, WA 98101
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Low risk Comment: study used coin toss method performed by research assistant, which is an adequate method of randomly generating the sequence, according to the Cochrane Handbook of Systematic Reviews of Interventions
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: no information reported. We requested clarification about the method of allocation concealment from the study investigators but received no information on this topic at the time of the original review.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: no blinding
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: outcome assessment by telephone interviews of parents and teacher, which were performed by a blinded research assistant. The parents, however, were not blinded, which is not an adequate method.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes Low risk Comment: ITT method used
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: all measures of interest reported. No protocol identified. Of the measures mentioned in the paper, all measures of interest for the review were reported.
Vested interest bias Low risk Comment: no apparent source of bias
Other sources of bias? Unclear risk Comment: co‐medication not specified

Antshel 2003.

Methods Design: RCT
Participants Country: USA
Setting: outpatient clinic in Kentucky. Participants were recruited from newspaper advertisements and from consecutive referrals to a university‐based behavioural paediatric clinic specialised in ADHD and related disorders.
Sample size calculation: not reported
Sample size: 120 children
Sex: 90 (75%) = boys, 30 (25%) = girls
Age: range = 8‐12 years
Ethnicity: Caucasian = 112, African‐American = six, Asian‐American = two
Socioeconomic status: not reported
IQ: IQ > 70
ADHD diagnosis:DSM‐IV (DICA‐R‐P); (inattentive type = 59, combined type = 61)
ADHD medication: stimulant medication = 110, SSRIs = 10
Comorbidity: ODD = 53, mood disorders = 29, anxiety disorders = 11, tic disorders = five
Medication for comorbid disorders: SSRI balanced between groups
Inclusion criteria: children scoring > 1 SD above the mean on the CBCL Attention subscale
Exclusion criteria:
  1. not having an ADHD diagnosis

  2. 8‐12 years of age

  3. children with significant cognitive delays (IQ < 70)

  4. Children with English as a second language (information received in an email from Kevin Antshel, 16 December 2010 Antshel 2010 [pers comm])


Baseline characteristics: no statistically significant between‐group differences in age, sex, or classroom placement, duration and severity of ADHD symptoms, or comorbid conditions. No statistically significant between‐group differences on medication type and dosage
Interventions 120 participants allocated to one of two groups
  1. Group one (n = 80): eight weeks of intervention with sessions for the child group and the parent group. All sessions were conducted by the same two therapists, a male doctoral student in psychology and a female master's student in social work, both of whom followed a manual. Sessions were videotaped to ensure treatment consistency.

    1. Child group: there were 90‐minute group sessions for the children during eight consecutive weeks. Sessions consisted of different methods to promote generalisation of social skills. There were six themes: 1) cooperation with peers; 2) learning how to take others' perspective; 3) problem‐solving; 4) recognising and controlling anger; 5) assertiveness; and 6) conversations (giving and receiving compliments)

    2. Parent group: parents attended three parent sessions in weeks one, four, and eight. The sessions consisted of information about the themes and content in the children's group and discussion of how to assess and monitor homework completion.

  2. Group two (n = 40): waiting list


Attendance: mean attendance at the eight sessions was 94% for the diagnostically homogeneous and 92% for the diagnostically heterogenous treatment groups.
Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills: SSRS, parent‐ and child‐rated


Outcome assessment: post‐treatment assessment eight weeks after pre‐test and follow‐up assessment three months after the post‐test
Notes Study ID: not reported
Sponsorship source: not reported
Year conducted: not stated
Duration of the study: 22 weeks
Comments: none
Lead author's name: Kevin M Antshel
Institution: Children’s Hospital, Boston University of Kentucky
Email: Antshelk@upstate.edu
Address: SUNY Upstate Medical University, Department of Psychiatry, 750 East Adams Street, Syracuse, NY 13210
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Low risk Comment: used a computer‐generated randomisation process. Information received from Kevin Antshel in an email 13 July 2011 (Antshel 2011 [pers comm])
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Low risk Comment: Allocation was concealed. Information received from Kevin Antshel in an email 13 July 2011 (Antshel 2011 [pers comm])
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: no blinding
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: no blinded outcome assessors
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes Low risk Comment: stated that there was 100% completion rate
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: all measures of interest reported
Vested interest bias Unclear risk Comment: no funding source reported
Other sources of bias? Unclear risk Comment: Referral of patients. The selection of ADHD‐I skewed the data.

Yuk‐chi 2005.

Methods Design: RCT
Participants Country: China
Setting: community mental health centre: outpatient clinic in Hong Kong
Sample size calculation: based on prior research, the assumption was made that effect sizes would be small to moderate, calculations showed that approximately 45 participants were required in each group.
Sample size: 90 children
Sex: 77 (90%) = boys, nine (10%) = girls (group one: 39 (87%) = boys, six (13%) = girls; group two: 38 (93%) = boys, three (7%) = girls)
Age: range = seven to 9.9 years, mean = eight years (SD = 0.95) (group one; mean = 7.87 years (SD = 0.77); group two: mean = 8.15 years (SD = 1.11))
Ethnicity: Chinese children
Socioeconomic status: mothers education: elementary or less = 17 (20%), junior high = 20 (23%), high school = 45 (50%), college = four (5%) (group one: elementary or less = six (13%), junior high = 10 (22%), high school = 28 (62%), college = one (2%), group two: elementary or less = 11 (27%), junior high = 10 (24%), high school = 15 (37%), college = three (7%))
IQ: Hong Kong WISC, short form, mean = 111.69 (SD = 13.5) (group one: mean = 111.2 (SD = 13.7); group two: mean = 112.25 (SD = 13.4))
ADHD diagnosis: ADHD combined type required for inclusion
ADHD medication: all participants received methylphenidate treatment
Comorbidity: anxiety = 29%, depression = 6%, ODD = 50%, CD = 6% (group one: anxiety = 27%, depression = 7%, ODD = 60%, CD = 9%, group two: anxiety = 32%, depression = 5%, ODD = 39%, CD = 2%)
Medications for comorbid disorders: not reported
Inclusion criteria:
  1. ADHD‐combined type based on DSM‐IV criteria

  2. seven to 9.9 years of age

  3. studying first to fourth grade

  4. living with a parent, who is the major caretaker

  5. IQ > 80

  6. no significant physical disability

  7. no stimulant medication (methylphenidate) use for more than two weeks previously

  8. parents willingness to accept stimulant medication and psychosocial intervention of this study

  9. parents willingness to accept random allocation

  10. no parent suffering from intellectual impairment or current psychosis


Exclusion criteria: refusal of group one intervention due to parental difficulties to apply leave from work
Baseline characteristics: no significant differences between the two treatment groups in demographic and socioeconomic status, comorbid conditions, and additional intervention received in the first six months of the treatment. No information about between group differences in the medical treatment
Interventions 90 children with ADHD allocated to one of two groups
  1. Group one (n = 45): methylphenidate + psychosocial treatment consisting of three components

    1. Child training*: which provided a rich direct contingency management environment, in which the training of problem‐solving's skills and anger control management was provided. All sessions were videotaped to check treatment integrity. Themes were, for example, feelings, games, problem‐solving, stop & think, role play school and home. 24 weekly sessions each lasting from one hour and 30 minutes to two hours

    2. Cognitive‐behavioural parent training*: themes were, for example, know yourself, attention rules, stress management, child mood management, and homework coaching. 18 weekly sessions in total, each lasting from one hour and 30 minutes to two hours

    3. School consultations: consisted of two telephone consultations in which the therapist in the child groups talked to the teachers about implementing classroom management strategies and reviewing the child's progress in school

  2. Group two (n = 41): methylphenidate treatment alone based on the standard management practice at the outpatient clinic where the study took place. This practice was not described in further detail.


Attendance: adherence in group two was defined as taking 80% of prescription without more than one month of discontinuation during school days. In group one, treatment adherence was defined as above in combination with at least 80% attendance in child and parent sessions.
*The child training and the parent training were developed to be implemented concomitantly. No protocol violations to the programme were detected in either the child or the parent intervention.
Outcomes Secondary outcomes
  1. Core ADHD: SWAN (the Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD symptoms and Normal behaviour rating scale), parent‐ and teacher‐rated; Matching Familiar Figures Test (MFFT), clinician‐rated

  2. Satisfaction with treatment: single item rating of experience with the intervention, parent‐rated


Outcome assessment: post‐intervention, six and 12‐month follow‐up
Notes Study ID: not reported
Sponsorship source: not reported
Year conducted: "this study was planned in 1999" (quote)
Duration of the study: 17.5 months
Comments: none
Lead author: Yuk‐chi So
Institution: Chinese University of Hong Kong
Email: not reported
Address: not reported
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Low risk Comment: table of random numbers, with block size of two
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: unclear
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: no blinding
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: no blinding on this review's primary outcome
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: type of imputation method used was unclear
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: low risk
Vested interest bias Unclear risk Comment: no information on funding
Other sources of bias? Low risk Comment: no other sources of bias identified

Van der Oord 2007.

Methods Design: RCT
Participants Country: The Netherlands
Setting: five different outpatient clinics
Sample size: 50 children*
Sample size calculation: not reported
Sex: group one =1 (4.2%), group two = 4 (19.4%), in the paper it was not specified which sex these numbers referred to, but it was assumed to be female sex
Age: range = eight to 12 years (group one: mean = 9.76 years (SD = 1.13), group two: mean = 9.96 years (SD = 1.31))
Ethnicity: Caucasian = 40 (89%), Caribbean = one (2%), mixed origin = four (9%)
Socioeconomic status: Educational level mother (%): low:20%, medium:40%, high:36%. Educational level father (%): low:24%, medium:42%, high:24%
IQ: total IQ of 75 or above required for inclusion
ADHD diagnosis: subtype not reported
ADHD medication: children with a history of methylphenidate treatment were excluded. All children received methylphenidate treatment as part of the study protocol.
Comorbidity: (group one: ODD/CD = 10 (41.7%), group two: ODD/CD = 13 (61.9%))
Medications for comorbid disorders: not reported
Inclusion criteria:
  1. ADHD diagnosis based on DSM‐IV established with the parent version of DISC‐IV

  2. total IQ of 75 or above based on the short version of WISC‐R

  3. parents had to give informed consent for their child to participate in the study


Exclusion criteria:
  1. inadequate mastering of the Dutch language by the child or both parents

  2. a history of methylphenidate use


Baseline characteristic: one‐way ANOVAs and Chi2 analyses showed no significant differences between the two conditions in terms of baseline demographic characteristics. Furthermore, one‐way ANOVAs showed no significant group differences. The treatment groups did not differ on dose of methylphenidate either at baseline or post‐treatment.
*baseline characteristics were reported and analysed for children participating in the pre‐intervention assessment, in total = 45, group one = 24, group two = 21.
Interventions 50 children allocated to one of two groups
  1. Group one (n = 27): methylphenidate (as described in group two) + multimodal behaviour therapy consisting of child cognitive‐behaviour therapy, parent behaviour therapy and teacher behavioural training, with manuals in all groups. To ensure treatment compliance, all therapists completed a treatment integrity checklist.

    1. Child cognitive‐behaviour therapy: used cognitive‐behaviour techniques and the programme for this group was adapted from Kendall and Braswell (Kendal 1982). Components included problem‐solving techniques, relaxation techniques, contingency management techniques, role playing, and guided practice. It consisted of 10 weekly, 75‐minute group sessions, provided by two therapists.

    2. Parent behaviour therapy: based on Barkley's training's manual "Defiant children: A clinicians manual for parent training (Barkley 1987). Components included, for example, psychoeducation on ADHD, structuring the environments, practicing positive attending skills, and contingency management skills. It consisted of 10 weekly sessions of 90 minutes of group therapy, provided by two therapists.

    3. Teacher behavioural training: based on the teacher training manual by Pelham (Pelham 1992) and consisted of a two‐hour workshop, which consisted of, for example, psychoeducation on ADHD, structuring the classroom environment, and a daily report card.

  2. Group two (n = 23): methylphenidate was administered following the procedures described in the MTA study MTA 1999. In this titration trial, 5, 10, and 20 mg of methylphenidate and placebo were administered in a pseudo random order twice daily at breakfast (around 7.30 a.m.) and at lunch (around 12.30 p.m.). All children weighed above 22 kg, thus the highest dose never exceeded 0.9 mg per kg of the body weight. Coding of doses was kept at a hospital pharmacy and in case of immediate side effects the pharmacy could be reached to unblind the coding.


Attendance: attendance at 75% was set as a criterion for intervention attendance in group one. Mean attendance in the combined condition was 88.6%. Of the 50 randomised children, one declined participation in group two (the methylphenidate‐only group) and two of the children in group one (combined intervention) discontinued the intervention. Furthermore, one child was lost to post‐test and follow‐up in the methylphenidate‐only intervention, and one was omitted from analysis in the combined intervention group as the criteria of 75% attendance was not met.
Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills: SSRS, parent‐ and teacher‐rated


Secondary outcomes
  1. Core ADHD symptoms: DBDRS, parent‐ and teacher‐rated


Outcome assessment: post‐intervention
Notes Study ID: not reported
Sponsorship source: not reported
Year conducted: not stated
Duration of the study: 10 weeks
Comments: none
Lead author: Saskia van der Oord
Institution: Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Amsterdam
Email: s.vanderoord@uva.nl
Address: Roeterstraat 15, 1018 WB Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: no information reported. We requested clarification about method of allocation concealment from the study investigators but received no information on this topic at the time of preparing the original review.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: no information reported. We requested clarification about the method of allocation concealment form the study investigators but received no information on this topic at the time of preparing the original review.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: no blinding
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: no blinding
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes Low risk Comment: stated numbers of all participants lost to follow‐up. Lost to follow‐up not believed to influence results
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: no
Vested interest bias Unclear risk Comment: funding source not reported
Other sources of bias? Unclear risk Comment: co‐medication not specified

Pfiffner 2007.

Methods Design: RCT
Participants Country: USA
Setting: outpatient clinic
Sample size calculation: not reported
Sample size: 69 children
Sex: 46 (67%) = boys, 23 (33%) = girls
Age: range = seven to 11 years
Ethnicity: white = 51%, Asian = 16%, Hispanic = 10%, Afro‐American = 6%, mixed = 17%
Socioeconomic status: income range: $21,000‐$150,000. Education level measured on scale from 1 (9th grade or less) to 6 (advanced graduate or professional degree), mothers = 5.0, fathers = 4.8
IQ: IQ > 80 on WASI
ADHD diagnosis: ADHD‐I required for inclusion
ADHD medication: not reported
Comorbidity: ODD = 23%, depression = 1%, anxiety = 12%
Medications for comorbid disorders: not reported
Inclusion criteria:
  1. DSM‐IV diagnosis of ADHD‐I

  2. IQ > 80 (based on WASI)

  3. living with at least one parent for the past year

  4. attending school full time

  5. the school consenting to participate in school‐based treatment


Exclusion criteria:
  1. families expecting to change medication status for their child during the study

  2. children with visual or hearing impairment

  3. severe language delay

  4. major neurological illness

  5. psychosis or pervasive development disorder

  6. a child being in the same classroom as another participant or having a sibling who was already enrolled


Baseline characteristics: no significant differences between groups regarding child age, sex, race, symptoms of hyperactivity/impulsivity, comorbid oppositional defiant disorder, anxiety or depression, IQ, or academic achievement.
Interventions 69 participants allocated to one of two groups
  1. Group one: (n = 36) Child Life and Attention Skills (CLAS) program. The treatment included three components, administrated concurrently over 12 weeks: child skills training; parent training; and teacher consultation (see below). All treatments were manual‐based, and some changes were made to the manuals to refine the interventions based on feedback from clinicians, participants, teachers, and parents.

    1. Child skills training: divided into modules focused on skills for independence and skills for social competence, and involved behavioural interventions (for example, a reward‐based contingency management programme) and cognitive‐behavioural interventions (for example, problem‐solving, the use of cues/verbal mediation strategies to stay on task and focused). Eight to 10 × 1½ hours a week groups with child skills training in the 12‐week period

    2. Parent training: modules in the child group were reviewed each week and parents were taught methods to promote and reinforce the child's use of skills at home. The parents were also taught methods for managing ADHD. Parents completed eight to 10 × 1½‐hour group sessions and four to five family sessions.

    3. Teacher consultations: a school‐home daily report card was designed and used (Classroom Challenge (CC)), and a special notebook was created for each child containing copies of CC. The consultations involved a ½ hour overview of behavioural interventions and classroom‐based accommodations for ADHD, followed by four to five × ½ hour meetings of teacher, child, and therapist over the 12‐week period.

  2. Group two (n = 33): no intervention


Attendance:
  1. Parents participated in more than 95% of the group meetings.

  2. Children lost to follow‐up: intervention = seven, control = eight

Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills: SSRS, parent‐ and teacher‐rated

  2. General behaviour: CGI, parent‐ and teacher‐rated


Secondary outcomes
  1. Core ADHD symptoms: CSI, parent‐ and teacher‐rated

  2. Satisfaction with treatment: Consumer Satisfaction Questionnaire, child‐, parent‐ and teacher‐rated


Outcome assessment: post‐intervention and follow‐up assessment. Time of follow‐up depended on what time of the school year the intervention ended.
Notes Study ID: not reported
Sponsorship source: supported by NIMH grant R21MH065927
Year conducted: not stated
Duration of the study: 33 weeks
Comments: none
Lead author: Linda J. Pfiffner
Institution: Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco
Email: lindap@lppi.ucsf.edu
Address: 401 Parnassus Avenue, Box 0984, San Francisco, CA 94143
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Low risk Comment: random table. Information received from Pfiffner in an email 25 May 2011 (Pfiffner 2011b [pers comm])
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Low risk Comment: sealed envelopes
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: no blinding
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: no blinding
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes Low risk Comment: used imputation
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: all apparent assessments made
Vested interest bias High risk Comment: had done previous studies
Other sources of bias? High risk Comment: changes in treatment protocol. Timing for follow‐up differed; same school year versus next school year (both approximately three months after treatment but summer break in between). Families and teachers were paid for each of the assessments (teachers = US$50, parents group one = US$30, group two = US$200) and teachers were paid for participating in meetings (US$50‐100).

Waxmonsky 2010.

Methods Design: RCT
Participants Country: USA
Setting: outpatient
Sample size calculation: not reported
Sample size: 56 children
Age: range = six to 12 years (group one: mean = 8.3 years (SD = 1.6), group two: mean = 8.9 years (SD = 1.5))
Sex: 45 (80%) = boys, 11 (20%) = girls (group one: 24 (82.8%) = boys, 5 (17.2%) = girls; group two: 21 (77.8%) = boys, 6 (22.2%) = girls)
Ethnicity: white = 80.4%, African‐American = 10.7%, mixed = 8.9%
Socioeconomic status: Nakao and Treas Socioeconomic Index (Nakao 1994): group one: mean = 61 (SD = 17), group two: mean = 53 (SD = 13)
IQ: WISC‐III IQ score > 75(group one: mean = 101 (SD = 16), group two: mean = 97 (SD = 13))
ADHD diagnosis:DSM‐IV‐TR; combined type = 48 (85.7%), inattentive type = seven (12.5%), hyperactive/impulsive type = one (1.8%) (group one: combined type = 24 (82.8%), inattentive type = four (13.8%), hyperactive/impulsive type = one (3.4%), group two: combined type = 24 (88.9%), inattentive type = three (11.1%), hyperactive/impulsive type = zero (0%))
ADHD medication: stimulant naive at baseline (group one = 13 (44.8%), group two = eight (29.6%)), all patients received psychostimulant medications as part of the study
Comorbidity: CD = 22 (39.3%), ODD = 24 (42.9%), no comorbidity = 10 (17.9%) (group one: CD = 11 (37.9%), ODD = 15 (51.7%), no comorbidity = three (10.3%), group two: CD = 11 (40.7%), ODD = nine (33.3%), no comorbidity = seven (25.9%))
Medications for comorbid disorders: not reported
Inclusion criteria: ADHD based on DSM‐IV
Exclusion criteria:
  1. current or past history of seizures (not including benign febrile seizures)

  2. other physical conditions that precluded administration of atomoxetine (for example, marked cardiac conduction delay)

  3. documented failed study of atomoxetine, defined as three weeks or more on treatment with at least 0.8 mg/kg/d, or a documented inability to tolerate this dose

  4. serious forms of psychopathology other than ADHD such as autism, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, or any other psychopathology requiring urgent treatment with psychotropic medication

  5. any history of major depression requiring treatment, or any past history of self‐harm or serious suicidal ideation

  6. an IQ of less than 75 (based on WISC‐III)

  7. no evidence of ADHD‐related impairment at school


Baseline characteristics: no significant between group differences in mean doses of atomoxetine
Interventions 56 participants allocated to one of two groups
  1. Group one (n = 29): medication (as described in group two) + behaviour therapy in eight‐week intervention with three components:

    1. Parent group: received two‐hour sessions once a week for eight weeks, following the manual of Community Oriented Parent Education Program (COPE). COPE uses the principles of social learning theory to help parents develop skills to target their children's behaviour and lack of impulse control. Group leaders were advanced graduate students or doctoral level clinicians.

    2. Child group: participated in a social skills training (SST) programme consisting of a two‐hour session once a week for eight weeks. Group leaders were graduate students in clinical psychology. It was unclear if the child group intervention also was based on a manual.

    3. Teacher group: completed a daily report card which was sent to the parent daily. Parents were taught to monitor the report cards and provide appropriate consequences for their child based on the report cards (i.e. reward for positive performance and loss of privileges for negative performance).

  2. Group two (n = 27): medication; all medication was dosed openly using atomoxetine. A weight‐based protocol similar to previous studies using atomoxetine was used.


Attendance: 62% of parents attended eight sessions, 62% attended six or more sessions. The children's attendance in the SST group was not reported; seven children (12.5%) discontinued the study (five from group one (medication + behaviour therapy) and two from group two (medication alone)).
Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills: SSRS, parent‐ and teacher‐rated

  2. General behaviour: CGI, observer‐rated


Secondary outcomes
  1. Core ADHD symptoms: DBDRS, parent‐ and teacher‐rated

  2. Academic performance: Academic Performance Rating Scale, teacher‐rated

  3. Satisfaction with treatment: treatment satisfaction, parent‐rated


Outcome assessment: post‐treatment
Notes Study ID:NCT00918567
Sponsorship source: investigator‐initiated trial funded entirely by Eli Lilly and Company
Year conducted: conducted between 2008 and 2013
Duration of the study: 11 weeks
Comments: none
Author name: James G Waxmonsky, MD
Institution: Center for Children and Families
Email: jgw@buffalo.edu
Address: 106 Diefendorf Hall, 3435 Main St., Bldg 20, Buffalo, NY 14214
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Low risk Comment: clarification requested from one of the study investigators and Dan Waschbusch informed us in an email on 22 June 2011 that they had used a computer‐generated randomisation process (Waschbusch 2011 [pers comm]).
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Low risk Comment: clarification requested from one of the study investigators and Dan Waschbusch informed us in an email on 22 June 2011 that the clinicians did not know the treatment assignment before it was assigned (Waschbusch 2011 [pers comm]).
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: no blinding
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: no blinding
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: We requested clarification from one of the study investigators and Dan Wascbusch informed us in an email that participants were dropped if there was not sufficient information. Scores in indexes were computed if at least 50% of the items in the index were answered; if not, they were counted as missing. Dan Wascbusch also informed us that they had essentially complete data at pre‐treatment and nearly complete data at post‐treatment. They had a lower response rate for teachers. They included whatever they had in the analyses and dropped participants when there was insufficient information, repeating this for each analysis (Waschbusch 2011 [pers comm]
Selective reporting (reporting bias) High risk Comment: protocol published in Clinicaltrials.gov after study had been conducted. Publication was not consistent with the report in Clinicaltrials.gov
Vested interest bias High risk Comment: funding from, and collaboration with, Eli‐Lilly
Other sources of bias? Unclear risk Comment: co‐medication not specified

Tabaeian 2010.

Methods Design: RCT, parallel group
Participants Country: Iran
Setting: outpatient clinics
Sample size calculation: not reported
Sample size: 45
Sex: 100% = boys
Age: not reported (based on the educational system in Iran, it should be eight to 10 years old)
Ethnicity: reported as Iranian without other information
Socioeconomic status: not reported
IQ: not reported
ADHD diagnosis: not reported
ADHD medication: all participants (100%) were taking methylphenidate during the study.
Comorbidity: children with comorbidity were excluded.
Medication of comorbid disorders: not relevant
Inclusion criteria:
  1. males

  2. school grade 3th‐4th

  3. problem with social skills

  4. normal or higher than normal intelligence level

  5. child had ADHD


Exclusion criteria: comorbidity
Baseline assessment: not reported
Interventions 45 participants allocated to one of three groups*. Number of participants in each group not reported
  1. Group one (n = 15*): social skills training, which involved directly training students in the following: eye contact skill, verbal communication, emotions, causes of expressing emotions, setting the emotions, problem‐solving. Consisted of 10 sessions, each lasting 90 minutes with 15 minutes rest and refreshments 40 minutes from start

  2. Group two (n = 15*): social skills training + parent training. Focus on understanding ADHD, listening to the child, eye contact, interaction, supporting expression of emotion, parent interaction and family interactions. Consisted of 10 sessions, each lasting 90 minutes and with a 15‐minute break

  3. Group three (n = 15*): no training


Attendance: not reported
*the number of participants in each group was not described explicitly but it is assumed that the participants were distributed equally
Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills*: Peer relationship subscale, child‐rated

  2. General behaviour*: "Accepted social behaviour" subscale, child‐rated


Secondary outcomes
  1. Core ADHD symptoms*: Impulsivity subscale, child‐rated


*the specific assessment instrument was not possible to confirm based on the description and references in the paper and extracted
Outcome assessment: post‐intervention
Notes Study ID: no information
Sponsorship source: no funding reported
Year conducted: 2010
Duration of the study: 10 weeks
Comments: this extraction was based on extraction made by Ghasaleh Aali based on the Persian paper.
Lead author's name: SR Tabaeian
Institution: University of Isfahan
Email: r.tabaeian@yahoo.com
Address:
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: 2 centres randomly selected and participants randomly assigned to groups. How this randomisation was done, however, was not stated.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: not described. We contacted the corresponding author for more information but did not receive a reply.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: not described but, based on the intervention, we judged that blinding was not possible
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: it was not clear what measure was used and if it was possible to blind the assessors. It seemed likely that it was a questionnaire, either for the child or the parents, and that it was not blinded.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes Low risk Comment: none reported. There were no dropouts.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Unclear risk Comment: no protocol available. Based on the translation, it was difficult to identify the measure used and judge the appropriateness of the reported outcomes.
Vested interest bias Unclear risk Comment: funding source not reported
Other sources of bias? Low risk Comment: none reported

Storebø 2012.

Methods Design: RCT, parallel group
Participants Country: Denmark
Setting: clinical
Sample size calculation: a sample size of 26 children in each group was needed based on a sample size calculation of 80% power in detecting a clinical relevant change of four points on the primary outcome measure of hyperactivity and impulsivity.
Sample size: 56 children*
Sex: 39 (71%) = boys, 16 (29%) = girls (group one: 19 (67.8%) = boys, nine (32.2%) = girls; group two: 20 (74%) = boys, seven (26%) = girls)
Age: (group one: mean = 10.6 years (SD = 1.29); group two: mean = 10.2 years (SD = 1.34))
Ethnicity: Danish 100%
Socioeconomic status: not reported
IQ: both verbal and nonverbal IQ > 80 (group one: WISC verbal mean = 93.9 (SD = 15.7), group two: WISC non‐verbal mean = 94.8 (SD = 19.0).
ADHD diagnosis: group one: inattentive = 10 (35.7%), hyperactive‐impulsive = 0 (0%), combined = 16 (57.2%), not otherwise specified = 2 (7.1%); group two: inattentive = 6 (22.2%), hyperactive‐impulsive = 2 (7.4%), combined = 16 (59.2%), not otherwise specified = 3 (11.1%)
ADHD medication: after assessment and confirmation, the family was offered medical treatment for the child following a medication protocol. The children had never previously received medical treatment for ADHD.
Comorbidity: (group one: ODD = four (33.3%), anxiety disorder = four (33.3%), depressive disorder = one (8.3%), tics and OCD = zero (0%), enuresis = two (20%), stuttering = one (5%); group two: ODD = four (40%), anxiety disorder = two (20%), depressive disorder = one (10%), tics and OCD = 1 (10%), enuresis = two (20%), stuttering = zero (0%))
Medication of comorbid disorders: not reported
Inclusion criteria:
  1. ADHD diagnosis according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM‐IV, 1994)

  2. eight to 12 years of age at the time of the start of assessment

  3. parents willing to take part in study and give consent for medical treatment of child and for child's participation in study


Exclusion criteria:
  1. schizophrenia or all the autism diagnoses according to DSM‐IV

  2. violent and criminal children

  3. both verbal and nonverbal (IQ) below 80

  4. previously medicated for ADHD

  5. resistance against participating


*baseline characteristics were reported for 55 children as data from the child that withdrew from group two was not allowed to be analysed.
Baseline characteristics: no significant difference in baseline demographics between the 2 groups
Interventions 56 participants were allocated to one of two groups
  1. Group one (28 participants): SOSTRA which consisted of social skills training plus parental training combined with standard treatment (medication). The children were offered 90‐minute, weekly social skills training sessions for a total of eight weeks, during which, the parents attended parental training. Social skills training aimed to improve and maintain the individual’s social skills. The children were taught how to adjust their verbal and nonverbal behaviour in their social interactions. The training also included efforts to change the child’s cognitive assessment of the ‘social world’ and generally focused on teaching the children to ‘read’ the subtle cues in social interaction such as learning to wait for their turn. The standard treatment offered encompassed the normal practice regarding ADHD patients at the Child Psychiatric Clinic in Holbaek: after assessment and confirmation of the ADHD diagnosis, the family was offered medical treatment for the child following a medication protocol; the children had never previously received medical treatment for ADHD.

  2. Group two (28 participants): normal practice, as reported above for the intervention group


Attendance: one participant in each group did not receive the allocated intervention and one participant in group two was lost to follow‐up.
Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills: CBRS, social problems subscale, teacher‐rated

  2. Emotional competencies: CBRS, emotional subscale, teacher‐rated

  3. General behaviour: CBRS, aggressiveness subscale, teacher‐rated


Secondary outcomes
  1. Core ADHD symptoms: Conners 3, hyperactivity‐impulsivity subscale, teacher‐rated

  2. Performance and grades in school: CBRS, academic score subscale, teacher‐rated


Outcome assessment: post‐intervention and follow‐up at three and six months after end of intervention
Notes Study ID:NCT00937469
Sponsorship source: the SOSTRA study was financially supported by Region’s Zealand University Hospital (RESUS), Region Zealand Research Foundation, and Psychiatric Research Unit, Region Zealand. Funding was also received from the Fru C. Hermansens Foundation, Slagtermester Max Wørzner and Inger Wøzners Foundation, and TrygFonden.
Year conducted: 2012
Duration of the study: 8 weeks
Comments: the study obtained approval from the Regional Ethics Committee of Zealand (SJ‐85), was registered at the Danish Data Protection Agency DO50892, and registered at clinical trials.gov/NCT00937469.
Lead author's name: Ole Jakob Storebø
Institution: Child Psychiatric Clinic, Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Department, Region Zealand, Denmark
Email: ojst@regionsjaelland.dk
Address: Birkevænget 3, 4300 Holbæk Denmark
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Low risk Comment: adequate method was used. Randomisation was conducted using computer‐generated, permuted randomisation sequences in blocks of four with an allocation ratio of 1:1 stratified for sex and comorbidity.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Low risk Comment: adequate method was used. All data that could be used to identify the allocation before data entry was hidden and block size was unknown to the investigators.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: it was not possible to ‘blind’ participants, parents, treating physicians, or personnel in the Child Psychiatric Clinic in Holbaek.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes Low risk Comment: the involved parties were instructed not to inform the teachers, who rated the primary and secondary outcome measures, of the intervention allocated. The outcome assessors were thus kept unaware of group allocation.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes Low risk Comment: one child from each group dropped out after the randomisation. Outcome assessment was still obtained from the child allocated to group two. Another child from group two was lost to follow‐up.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: matched study protocol. All outcome measures outlined in protocol were reported.
Vested interest bias Low risk Comment: no apparent source of bias. The funders of the study did not have a role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Other sources of bias? Unclear risk Comment: no other apparent sources of bias

Pfiffner 2014.

Methods Design: RCT, parallel group
Participants Country: USA
Setting: outpatient
Sample size calculation: not reported
Sample size: 125 children in the groups included in the analysis (199 children in the full study)
Sex: 58% = boys (group one: 51.4% = boys; group two: 58.8% = boys)
Age: mean = 8.6 years (range = seven to 11) (group one: mean = 8.8 years (SD = 1.15); group two: mean = 8.7 group two: mean = 8.4 years (SD = 1.13))
Ethnicity: Caucasian = 54%, Latino = 17%, Asian‐American = 8%, African‐American = 5%, self‐identified as mixed race = 17% (group one: 55.4% Caucasian, 12.2% Latino, 9.5% Asian‐American, 5.4% African‐American, 17.6% self‐identified as mixed race; group two: Caucasian (43.1%), Latino (25.5%), Asian‐American (3.9%), African‐American (3.9%), self‐identified as mixed race (23.5%))
Socioeconomic status: total household income: below US $50,000 = 14.1%, $50,000‐100,000 = 28.3%, $100,000‐150,000 = 28.8%, and more than $150,000 = 28.8% of families. 81.2% of the primary parents reported having graduated from college, 13% of participants were living in single‐parent families (group one: parent education = 83.6% college graduates, single‐parent household = 9.5%; group two: parent education = 78.4% college grads, single‐parent household = 11.8%)
IQ: group one: mean = 103.6 (SD = 11.0) on WISC FSIQ; group two: mean = 105.6 (SD = 11.6) on WISC FSIQ
ADHD diagnosis: only inattentive subtype
ADHD medication: stimulant medication = 4.5% (group one = 9.5%, group two = 2.0%). The small number of children taking stimulant medication (not otherwise specified) completed a one‐week wash‐out to assess behaviour and obtain ratings off‐medication.
Comorbidity: group one: anxiety = 6.8%, depression = 1.7%, ODD = 5.1%; group two: anxiety = 5.3%, depression = 2.6%, ODD = 5.3%
Medications for comorbid disorders: not reported
Inclusion criteria:
  1. primary DSM‐IV diagnosis of ADHD‐I (confirmed by the KSADS‐PL)

  2. IQ > 80 (confirmed with WISC‐IV, (Wechsler 2003)

  3. living with at least one parent for the past year

  4. child age between seven to 11 years (grades 2th‐5th)

  5. attending school full time in a regular classroom

  6. ability to participate in groups on the days scheduled

  7. school proximity within 45 minutes of study site to allow for the clinician to conduct school meetings

  8. teacher consents to participating in a school‐based treatment


Exclusion criteria:
  1. families of children who were taking nonstimulant psychoactive medication (because of difficulty withholding medication to confirm ADHD‐I symptoms)

  2. cases planning to initiate or change medication treatment (stimulant or otherwise) in the near term

  3. children with significant developmental disorders (e.g. pervasive developmental disorder) or neurological illnesses


Baseline characteristics: only medication status at randomisation differed across treatment groups (P = 0.04), with significantly more CLAS (Child Life and Attention Skills) children reporting medication use (9.5%) than PFT children (1.4%), but not compared to TAU children (2.0%).
Interventions 199 participants allocated to one of three groups. Only group one and group three were included in the analysis.
  1. Group one (n = 74): CLAS. This included three manualised coordinated components: (1) 10 × 90‐minute parent group meetings, along with up to six × 30‐minute family meetings (parent, child, and therapist); (2) 10 × 90‐minute child group meetings; and (3) a teacher consultation, which included one × 30‐minute orientation meeting involving the teacher and therapist and up to five subsequent 30‐minute meetings with the parent, child, teacher, and therapist and monthly booster sessions. Parent and child groups contained between five and eight families. Treatment occurred over a 10‐ to 13‐week period, with a follow‐up at five to seven months post‐treatment.

  2. Group two (n = 74): parent‐focused treatment consisting of the parent component described above under (1)

  3. Group three (n = 51): TAU. This included a written diagnostic report based on the assessment conducted at baseline. Families in the TAU condition also received a list of community treatment providers but were not given specific treatment recommendations.


Attendance:
  1. Teachers attended an average of 4.0 meetings (including the orientation).

  2. Participants in group one and group two did not differ significantly in the number of individual parent meetings attended; however, group two parents participated in slightly fewer group meetings (mean = 8.8) compared to group one parents (mean = 9.3), P = 0.02 (d = 0.38).

  3. Participation in the booster sessions varied across individuals, with a mean of 2.1 sessions for group one families (range = 0‐6) and a mean of 2.1 sessions for group two families (range = 0‐7).

  4. Clinicians met with teachers of 11/74 (15%) children in group one during the subsequent school year as an extension of treatment to the new classroom setting; nine met once, two met twice.

Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills: SSIS, teacher‐ and parent‐rated


Secondary outcomes
  1. Core ADHD symptoms: CSI: Inattentive symptoms, teacher‐ and parent‐rated


Outcome assessment: post‐intervention assessment and follow‐up assessment five to seven months after end of intervention
Notes Study ID: not reported
Sponsorship source: noncommercial. This research was supported by a grant from the National Institute of Mental Health MH077671.
Year conducted: 2014
Duration of the study: 4 years (2009‐2012)
Comments: none
Lead author's name: Linda J Pfiffner
Institution: Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco
Email: linda.pfiffner@ucsf.edu
Address: Department of Psychiatry, 401 Parnassus Ave., Box 0984, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA 94143
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: unclear how randomisation was done
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: not reported
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: participants not blinded
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes Unclear risk Comment: not reported
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes Low risk Comment: number of missing item values were reported as 0.8% at baseline, 3.3% at post‐intervention assessment, and 10.6% at follow‐up with most of the missing values at follow‐up being related to attrition.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Unclear risk Comment: no protocol, outcomes not prespecified
Vested interest bias Unclear risk Comment: study authors had conducted previous studies addressing same intervention.
Other sources of bias? High risk Comment: families were compensated for completion of post‐intervention (group one and two US$50, group three US$150) and follow‐up assessments (group one and two: US$100, group three: US$150); teachers were compensated at each time point (baseline assessment: US$50, post‐intervention and follow‐up: US$75). Further, the teachers received a total of US$100 for participating in the teacher consultation meetings.

Azad 2014.

Methods Design: RCT, parallel group
Participants Country: Iran
Setting: psychology and psychiatry clinic
Sample size calculation: not reported
Sample size: 30 children
Sex: not reported
Age: not reported; primary school students
Ethnicity: not reported
Socioeconomic status: not reported
IQ: not reported
ADHD diagnosis: subtypes not reported
ADHD medication: not reported
Comorbidity: not reported
Medication for comorbid disorders: not reported
Inclusion criteria: not reported
Exclusion criteria: not reported
Baseline characteristics: not reported
Interventions 30 participants allocated to one of two groups
  1. Group one, experimental group (n = 15): the intervention consisted of 16 sessions, three per week, each lasting 60 minutes.

    1. 1st session: pre‐intervention assessment and explaining the objective of the research to the parents

    2. 2nd and 3rd sessions: elaborating the application and importance of the research, explaining to the students the role of using certain methods in the improvement of their educational and non‐educational activities, and the fact that to have a better and more focused behaviour, they should take some steps

    3. 4th and 5th sessions: presenting the steps to the students, step one: the students imagine a new environment and a new behaviour, step two: the students interpret the new environment and the new behaviour, step 3: the students embed the appropriate behaviour in the new environment, step 4: the students think about the methods/strategies to express their behaviour, step 5: the students should guess the best method and choose it, step 6: the students should accurately appraise/review the cases

    4. 6th and 7th sessions: new cases were presented to the students and they were asked to follow the steps using a guide card.

    5. 8th and 9th sessions: transparent self‐guidance: the students should repeat the presented cases aloud and do the expected behaviour using the learned steps and the guide card. If required, minor verbal advices were given.

    6. 10th and 11th sessions: reductive self‐guidance: the objective of this session was to internalise the learned method. It was explained to the students that from now on they would practice the method that they had leaned internally. To do so, a new subjective environment was presented as a pattern and the proper reactive behaviours were suggested. The students were asked to act like this.

    7. 12th session: 6th session was repeated but the students were asked to minimise using a guide card.

    8. 13th and 14th sessions: presentation of proper and structured reactive behaviours without using guide card or presenting the steps

    9. 15th session: reviewing/appraising the precious sessions and reviewing the assignments

    10. 16th session: measuring the post‐intervention outcome

  2. Group two (n = 15): no training


Attendance: not reported
Outcomes Secondary outcomes
  1. Core ADHD symptoms: CPRS, parent‐rated


Outcome assessment: end of treatment and three month follow‐up
Notes Study ID: not reported
Sponsorship source: not reported
Year conducted: "This study enrolled the Isfahan primary school students afflicted with ADHD, during the educational years 2012‐2013." (quote)
Duration of the study: 5 weeks (15 sessions)
Comments: ethics approval not reported
Lead author's name: Azad Moslem Asli
Institution: Islamic Azad University, Science and Research Branch Esfahan, Esfaran, Iran
Email: Azzad2020@gmail.com
Address: not reported
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: participants were described to be randomly assigned to the groups but no further details were reported.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: no details reported and not possible to retrieve further information from the study authors
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: not reported, but probably not blinded
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes Unclear risk Comment: no details reported and it was not possible to retrieve further information from the study authors as the study authors did not respond to our email requests.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes Low risk Comment: no dropouts
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Unclear risk Comment: none known. No registered protocol available
Vested interest bias Unclear risk Comment: no other apparent biases
Other sources of bias? Low risk Comment: no other apparent biases

Meftagh 2014a.

Methods Design: RCT, parallel group
Participants Country: Iran
Setting: 15 male elementary schools from 4 educational areas in Shiraz
Sample size calculation: not reported
Sample size: 51
Sex: not reported
Age: mean = 8.98 years (SD = 0.77)
Ethnicity: not reported
Socioeconomic status: not reported
IQ: not reported
ADHD diagnosis: subtypes not reported
ADHD medication: not reported
Comorbidity: not reported
Medications for comorbid disorders: not reported
Inclusion criteria: ADHD diagnosis in clinical interview with children who had been identified with ADHD based on both teacher and parent rating on Child Symptom Inventory
Exclusion criteria: not reported
Baseline characteristics: no significant differences among study groups regarding age, family, and education
Interventions 51 patients allocated to one of three groups. Only group two and three were included in the analysis.
  1. Group one (n = 17): Behavioral Mother Training consisting of 10 educational sessions focusing e.g. on explaining ADHD, shaping positive behaviour, and minimising undesirable behaviours using training points to reinforce child behaviour. Each session lasting about 60 minutes, for 10 weeks

  2. Group two (n = 17): Verbal Self‐Instruction for the children, based on Meichenbaum 1978. The elements in this intervention were as follows: 1) directed discovery verbal self‐instruction that included identifying problems, determining the logical consequences and problems identification as the main causes of outcomes, identifying beneficial solutions, learning the self‐instruction sentences; 2) didactic verbal self‐instruction that included teaching five‐step problem‐solving strategy in verbal form; 3) faded rehearsal verbal self‐instruction that included task selection, cognitive modelling, overt external guidance, overt self‐guidance, modelling of faded overt self‐guidance, child’s practice of faded overt self‐guidance, modelling of covert self‐instruction, child practice of faded covert self‐instruction. Delivered in 10 educational sessions, each lasting about 60 minutes, for 10 weeks

  3. Group three (n = 17): format and duration of intervention provided to control group not reported


Attendance: 15 children in each group completed the study. Attendance rate was not reported.
Outcomes Secondary outcomes
  1. Core ADHD symptoms:continuous performance test (omission error), observer‐rated


Outcome assessment: post‐intervention and longest follow‐up two months after end of intervention.
Notes: data corresponded to change from pre‐test to post‐test
Notes Study ID: not reported
Sponsorship source: Research Committee of Shiraz University
Year conducted: 2014
Duration of the study: 10 weeks intervention plus 4 months follow‐up
Comments: none
Lead author's name: Sayyed Davood Meftagh
Institution: Department of Psychology, Payame Noor University, Iran
Email: a_najimi@hlth.mui.ac.ir
Address: Department of Health Education Health Promotion , School of Health Isfahan University of Medical Sciences, Isfahan, Iran
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: no details on sequence generation
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: no details
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: no description of blinding, probably not blinded
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: no description of blinding, probably not blinded
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes Low risk Comment: no flow chart and no explanation as to why some children did not complete the study
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: none detected
Vested interest bias Low risk Comment: the study authors declared no conflict of interest, no other sources of bias identified
Other sources of bias? Low risk Comment: no other apparent sources of bias

Choi 2015.

Methods Design: RCT, parallel group
Participants Country: Korea
Setting: outpatient
Sample size calculation: not reported
Sample size: 80 children
Sex: 32 (44%) = boys, 40 (56%) = girls
Age: mean = 11.2 years (SD = 0.93, range = 9‐13)
Ethnicity: not reported
Socioeconomic status: not reported
IQ: all IQ > 90
ADHD diagnosis: subtypes not reported
ADHD medication: all participants were under medication at the time of intervention, type of medication not reported
Comorbidity: not reported
Medications for comorbid disorders: not reported
Inclusion criteria
  1. meeting ADHD DSM‐IV criteria based on a structured interview by psychiatrist

  2. total Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) IQ score above 90, based on the full‐scale IQ (WISC, Revised Korean Version, Kwak, Park, Kim, 2001) (Kwak 2002)

  3. Behavior Problem Scale score on Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) within clinical range


Exclusion criteria: not reported
Baseline characteristics: no significant differences on study background variables or pretest measures
Interventions 80 participants allocated to one of three groups
  1. Group one (n = 25): Emotion Management Training (EMT), which is an emotion identification and expression treatment, and consists of four major components: (1) identification and labelling of emotional words; (2) emotional recognition and expression; (3) emotional understanding; and (4) emotional regulation in social situations. Each session began by discussing any problems or issues related to homework from the previous one, followed by exercises, and ending with an evaluation of the session. 50‐minute × once‐weekly sessions lasting 16 weeks.

  2. Group two (n = 28): social skills training (SST) programme (based on studies conducted by Elliot 1991 and Pfiffner 1997). SST is a form of behavioural training focused on teaching various social skills to children with ADHD to improve their interaction with peers and teachers. It uses various behavioural techniques such as prompts, role play, and reinforcement. Each session was focused on teaching a particular social skill such as listening skills, conversation skills, joining in, and reacting to rejection, negotiating, and reacting to being teased and criticised. Each session started with discussing homework, followed by exercises, and ending with a new homework assignment and an evaluation of the session.

  3. Group three (n = 27): waiting list. Children were later randomised into one of the two programs.


Attendance: at least 12 of 16 sessions of either EMT or SST. Mean number of sessions attended by the 75 programme completers was 14.9 (SD = 1.3), with an overall attendance rate of 90.5%. No group differences in the number of sessions attended
Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills: Peer Relational Skills Scale, child‐rated

  2. Emotional competencies: Emotion Expression Scale for Children, child‐rated


Outcome assessment: post‐intervention, one week after end of intervention
Notes Study ID: not reported
Sponsorship source: the author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Year conducted: 2015
Duration of the study: 16 weeks
Comments: ethics approval. The study was reviewed and approved by the Research Ethics Commitee of the university at which the experiment was conducted.
Lead author's name: Eun Sil Choi Woo Kyeong Lee
Institution: Kyungil University, Korea and Seoul Cyber University, Korea
Email: wisemind96@iscu.ac.kr
Address: 193‐15, Miadong Kangbuk‐gu, Seoul, 142‐700, Korea
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: from descriptions, it was unclear if all children had been included before randomisation. However, the sentence describing how WL children after 16 weeks were pooled with newly selected children indicated that randomisation was made progressively. It was unclear who did the sampling in blocks, how these blocks were generated, and if this process was to be considered random.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: it was unclear exactly how allocation concealment was done.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: parents, children and trainers were aware of group's status.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: the PICO measures of emotion expression and peer relational skills are both self‐report questionnaires.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes Unclear risk Comment: two children in the EMT group and three children in the SST group did not complete the study. The numbers of non‐completion were small and balanced. However, while it was stated that the reason for non‐completion was dropout during treatment, the reason for this dropout was not specified.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Unclear risk Comment: as we did not locate a study registration, it was unclear whether all planned measures had been reported accordingly. However, the measures presented in the paper were all described in the results section.
Vested interest bias High risk Comment: it was not specified if the therapist delivering the interventions was also one of the authors. The first author is the author of the manual used in one of the intervention arms. There may have been a bias given the first author's investment in the first study arm programme.
Other sources of bias? High risk Comment: no information on comorbid disorders. It was mentioned that all participants were under medication at the time of the intervention but it was not clear if this referred to ADHD medication or medication for comorbid disorders. It was not specified whether, for example, autism would be a reason for exclusion.

Bul 2016.

Methods Design: RCT, cross‐over
Participants Country: Netherlands, Belgium
Setting: outpatient
Sample size calculation: 78 participants per group required to detect differences of a medium effect size
Sample size: 170 children
Sex : 137 (80.6%) = boys, 33 (19.4%) = girls (group one: 70 (79.5%) = boys, 18 (20.5%) = girls, group two: 67 (81.7%) = boys, 15 (18.3%) = girls)
Age: mean = 9.85 years (SD = 1.26, range = 8‐12) (group one: mean = 9.89 years (SD = 1.28), group two: mean = 9.82 years (SD = 1.24))
Ethnicity: not reported
Socioeconomic status: not reported
IQ: mean = 106.18 (SD = 14.79) (group one: mean = 105.40 (SD = 14.46), group two: mean = 107.02 (SD = 15.18))
ADHD diagnosis: subtypes not reported
ADHD medication: n = 156 (91.8%) (group one: n = 80 (90.9%), group two: n = 76 (92.7%))
Comorbidity: ODD = 170 (149 (87.6%) = clinical, 21 (12.4%) = subclinical)) (group one: ODD = 88 (74 (84.1%) = clinical, 14 (15.9%) = subclinical), group two: ODD = 82 (75 (91.5%) = clinical, 7 (8.5%) = subclinical))
Medications for comorbid disorders: not reported
Inclusion criteria:
  1. DSM‐IV‐TR diagnosis of ADHD, confirmed by the K‐SADS

  2. eight to 12 years of age

  3. stable on pharmacological or psychological treatment (or both) for ADHD eight weeks before baseline (determined by healthcare professionals on the basis of medication data and behavioural observation)

  4. no initiation or change of pharmacological or psychological treatment (or both) for ADHD during the study period

  5. availability of a computer workstation at home with Internet and sound facilities

  6. sufficient understanding of the Dutch language by the child and by at least one of the parents


Exclusion criteria:
  1. estimated total IQ lower than 80 (determined by vocabulary and block design subtests of the WISC‐III)

  2. substance abuse problems (e.g. drugs, alcohol)

  3. conduct disorder, previously diagnosed by healthcare professionals

  4. autism spectrum disorder, previously diagnosed by healthcare professionals

  5. comorbid acute psychiatric disorder (e.g. depression, mania; confirmed by the K‐SADS)

  6. participation in a previous pilot study with a prototype of Plan‐It Commander

  7. children with a severe physical disability (e.g. blindness, deafness) or learning disability (e.g. dyslexia) according to child’s medical file and a standardised interview administered by phone to parents


Baseline characteristics: there was no significant difference in baseline demographics between the two groups.
Interventions Participants allocated to one of two groups and then crossed over
  1. Group one (n= 88): Plan‐IT commander, which is an internet‐based (online) mission‐guided adventure game designed to improve the following domains of daily life: 1) time management; 2) planning/organising; and 3) cooperation skills. Participants had access to the game environment and to a closed social community where it was possible to ask others for help or to help others using predefined messages. Player profiles were presented within the community and badges were awarded to the profile following achievements in the game. The game was played for a maximum of 65 minutes, three times per week (minimum time not provided). It was not possible to play more than 65 minutes in one × 24‐hour period.

  2. Group two (n= 82): TAU. Given TAU for first 10 weeks, then crossed over to the game intervention plus TAU for the subsequent 10 weeks.


Attendance: In group one, 77 % attended 20 weeks treatment. In group two, 89% attended 20 weeks treatment.
Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills: SSRS, teacher‐ and parent‐rated


Outcome assessment: end of treatment
Notes Study ID:ISRCTN62056259
Sponsorship source: Johnson Johnson was the funding source for game development and consultancy with regard to the design of the study. Flanders’ Care provided funding to perform the study (DEM2012‐02‐07) at the University Hospital Gasthuisberg (Belgium).
Year conducted: conducted from January to March 2013
Duration of the study: 20 weeks
Comments: none
Lead author's name: Kim CM Bul
Institution: Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Maastricht University
Email: k.bul@yulius.nl
Address: Yulius Academy, Yulius Mental Health Care Organization, Dennenhout 1, Barendrecht, 2994 GC, Netherlands
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Low risk Comment: 1:1 ratio and based on a prespecified computer‐generated randomisation list. Allocation was stratified by study site and gender, and arranged in permutated blocks.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: Group assignment performed online using the next available number on the randomisation list, which corresponded to the site and gender of the participant.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: not possible to blind participants to their treatment allocation. After screening and baseline assessment, parents received an email with notification of group allocation.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: parent and child self‐report. Detection bias due to child and parent knowledge of received intervention. Full blinding of researchers and teachers not guaranteed as participants could spontaneously talk about the game during the assessment or the study time.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes Low risk Comment: used intention‐to‐treat analyses and included all randomised participants. Used linear trend at point as an imputation method.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: all expected outcome measures reported. Only the satisfaction questionnaire was not mentioned in the protocol.
Vested interest bias High risk Comment: funders were Janssen‐Cilag (Netherlands) and Flanders Care (Netherlands). Funders may have had an interest as they are in the medical industry. It was not clearly stated if there was a commercial aspect to the computer game. Additionally, two of the authors were employees of Janssen Pharmaceuticals; statistical study experts Luc Janssens (MSc), who works for the Research and Development Department of Janssen Pharmaceuticals in Beerse (Belgium), and Franky de Cooman (MSc), who works for Art Deco. The study was conducted in collaboration with the following partners: Parent Association Centre ZitStil (Belgium), Focuz Treatment Centre for Children and Youth (Rotterdam), Mental Health Care Organization Mondriaan (Heerlen), and the University Hospital Gasthuisberg (Belgium). Johnson & Johnson was the funding source for game development and consultancy with regard to the design of the study. Flanders’ Care provided funding to perform the study (DEM2012‐02‐07) at the University Hospital Gasthuisberg (Belgium).
Other sources of bias? Low risk Comment: no other sources of bias identified

Waxmonsky 2016.

Methods Design: RCT, parallel group
Participants Country: USA
Setting: outpatient
Sample size calculation: not reported
Sample size: 68 children*
Sex: 39 = boys, 17 = girls (group one: 20/31 (65%) = boys; group two: 19/25 (76%) = boys)
Age: (group one: mean = 9.3 years (SD = 1.6), mean = 9.4 years (SD = 1.5))
Ethnicity: % defined as racial/ethnic minority: group one: = 12 (39%); group two = 9 (36%)
Socioeconomic status: group one: mean = 42.3 (SD = 15.2) on Socioeconomic Index**, group two: mean = 42.03 (SD = 12.8) both on Nakao and Treas Socioeconomic Index (Nakao 1994)
IQ: group one: mean = 100.6 (SD = 15.4); group two: mean = 100.7 (SD = 10.6)
ADHD diagnosis: not reported
ADHD medication: prior to therapy, phase psychostimulant doses were optimised for all participants. Thus, all participants received pharmacological treatment (group one: mean entry stimulant dose = 0.90 mg/kg/day (SD = 0.40); group two: mean entry stimulant dose = 0.90 mg/kg/day (SD = 0.43)) both in methylphenidate equivalents on a mg/kg/day.
Comorbidity: (group one: CD = 4 (13%), ODD = 29 (94%), anxiety/subthreshold anxiety = 9 (29%); group two: CD = 1 (4%), ODD = 24 (96%), anxiety/subthreshold anxiety = 11 (44%))
Medications for comorbid disorders: balanced between groups
Inclusion criteria:
  1. seven to 12 years of age

  2. combined subtype of ADHD and severe mood disorder

  3. ADHD evaluations based on the Disruptive Behavior Disorders Structured Parent Interview


Exclusion criteria:
  1. IQ below 80

  2. prominent traits of autism spectrum disorder

  3. use of any nonstimulant psychotropic

  4. bipolar I/II, or psychoses

  5. children with suicidal ideation

Interventions 68 participants allocated to one of two groups
  1. Group one (n = 35): medication + AIM (ADHD plus Impairments in Mood) consisting of 11 parallel‐group sessions for parents and children with parent and child group run in parallel. Each session lasted 105 minutes. Sesssions focused on: emotion recognition in self and others; connections between emotions and cognitions (e.g. problem‐solving when upset); application of coping tools and problem‐solving skills at school and home. A contingency management system was implemented with points that could be exchanged for gift cards.

  2. Group two (n = 33): medication + community psychosocial care. Encouraged to engaged with local psychosocial providers. Referrals but not treatment were provided from project staff. Use of community psychosocial care during the project period: 15 (60%) received other mental health services during the project period; of these two (8%) received school‐based counselling only, 13 (52%) received individual sessions (for mixture of behaviour problems, anger management, and social skills issues).


Attendance: % of attendance was required. Completers were participants attending at least six of the 11 sessions (n = 29). Mean attendance = 9.7 out of 11 group sessions. All but two participants attended at least half of the group or make‐up sessions.
Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills: Social Skills Rating Scale (SSRS), parent‐rated

  2. General behaviour: SSRS: Problem Behaviour subscale, teacher‐rated; DBDRS: ODD subscale ‐ teacher version, parent‐rated


Secondary outcomes
  1. Core ADHD symptoms: DBDRS: ADHD Symptoms subscale, teacher‐ and parent‐rated


Outcome assessment: assessment at following points (in weeks after baseline): mid‐intervention (6 weeks), post‐intervention (11 weeks) and group one at follow‐up assessment (17 weeks)
Notes Study ID:NCT00632619
Sponsorship source: National Institute of Mental Health (MH080791; Principal Investigator: Waxmonsky)
Year conducted: 2016
Duration of the study: 11 sessions
Comments: the study was approved by governing institutional review boards (IRBs) at both sites.
Lead author's name: James G. Waxmonsky
Institution: Pennsylvania State University, Department of Psychiatry
Email: jwaxmonsky@hmc.psu.edu
Address: Pennsylvania State University, Psychiatry, 500 University Dr. Dept of Psychiatry H073, Hershey Medical Center, Hershey, PA 17033
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Low risk Comment: randomised using a computer‐generated permutated blocking procedure. Randomisation occurred before the medication phase so parents would be aware of therapy status prior to making decisions about medication.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Low risk Comment: assumed that the use of a computer blocking procedure would conceal the allocation
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: blinding of participants and personnel not possible
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: the PICO measures included were rated by parents or teachers. Clinician‐rated assessments were completed by staff masked to therapy status, though this was not relevant for these measures.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes Low risk Comment: 3 dropped out of the CC group due to assignment. All dropout was well described and except the dropout due to assignment, the reasons did not indicate a bias.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: all primary and secondary outcome measures stated in protocol were reported in paper. SSRS was also reported in the paper but this measure was not described in the study registration.
Vested interest bias High risk Comment: potential vested interest due to relationships with pharmaceutical companies
Other sources of bias? High risk Comment: potential bias in the data collected. Selection of informants prone to monetary incentive. The referral may have increased the service received in the CC group during the intervention period and thus may have provided a potential bias. At study registration, the authors also mentioned as a limitation that the study therapy group had more contact with study staff, which may have impacted results. Additionally, the first author seemed to be involved in the development of the treatment programme used in the study and thus there might be bias of interests.

Wilkes Gillan 2016.

Methods Design: RCT, cross‐over
Participants Country: Australia
Setting: outpatient
Sample size calculation: not reported
Sample size: 31 children
Sex: 25 (86%) = boys, four (14%) = girls (group one: 13 (87%) = boys, 2 (13%) = girls; group two: 12 (86%) = boys, 2 (14%) = girls)
Age: group one: mean = 8.2 years (SD = 1.5); group two: mean = 8.5 years (SD = 1.7)
Ethnicity: 26/29 born in Australia
Socioeconomic status: (group one: parent with degree or diploma = 93%, occupation (requiring tertiary qualifications) = 60%; group two: parent with degree or diploma = 87%, occupation (requiring tertiary qualifications) = 57%)
IQ: not reported
ADHD diagnosis: (group one: predominantly inattentive = 5/15, predominantly hyperactive/impulsive = 1/15, combined subtype = 9/15; group two: predominantly inattentive = 6/14, predominantly hyperactive/impulsive = 0/14, combined subtype = 8/14))
ADHD medication: 20 of 29 (69%), specific type of medication not reported (group one = 60% , group two = 76%)
Comorbidity: subscale T‐scores on Connors Comprehensive Behaviour Rating scale, clinical cut‐off T‐score > 70 (group one: oppositional behaviour, mean = 75 (SD = 13.4), generalised anxiety disorder, mean = 71 (SD = 11.5); group two: oppositional behaviour, mean = 76 (SD = 13.0), generalised anxiety disorder, mean = 73 (SD = 12.9))
Medication for comorbid disorders: not reported
Inclusion criteria:
  1. five to 11 years

  2. a formal diagnosis of ADHD made by a paediatrician or psychiatrist using recognised diagnostic procedures such as the American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual 4th edition (DSM‐IV)

  3. playmate aged five to 11 years without ADHD or other DD diagnosis

  4. 1 parent attending clinic sessions and completing home activities

  5. Connors Comprehensive behavior Rating Scales (CCBRS) > 69 for child with ADHD, and 66 for playmate


Comorbid difficulties accepted (i.e. language difficulties, conduct disorder) and current medication was permitted but asked to be maintained in the study period. The therapist monitored the consistency of the medication use throughout the study period.
Exclusion criteria: diagnosed with other major developmental disorders (i.e. intellectual disability, autism spectrum disorder)
Baseline characteristics: no significant difference in baseline demographics between the two groups:
*baseline characteristics in this table is based on analysis of 29 children followed up in the outcome assessment (group one = 15; group two = 14)".
Interventions 31 participants allocated to one of two groups and then crossed over
  1. Group one (n = 16): play‐based intervention. The intervention lasted 10 weeks in total. Each child was included together with a playmate and both children participated in one‐hour play sessions in the clinic together with the child's parent and a therapist (in week 1‐3, 5, 7 and 10) and in home. Each session consisted of video feedback and play with adult modelling and support in the beginning of the intervention and without adult modelling and support in the end of the intervention. The parent was given one hour of training in week one and instructed to support play at home and facilitated weekly 40‐minute play dates at home where the playmate was invited home.

  2. Group two (n = 15): waiting list


Attendance: 98.3% (range 88‐100%), no specific % was required, one child in group one never started the intervention, and one child in group two did not finish the wait‐time.
Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills: Test of Playfulness, observer‐rated


Outcome assessment: post‐intervention
Notes Study ID:ACTRN12614000973617
Sponsorship source: Rotary Club of Mosman and the University of Sydney’s postgraduate research support scheme
Year conducted: 2016
Duration of the study: 10‐week intervention
Comments: none
Lead author's name: Sarah Wilkes‐Gillan
Institution: School of Allied Health, Australian Catholic University, North Sydney, NSW
Email: Sarah.WilkesGillan@acu.edu.au
Address: North Sydney, NSW, Australia
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Low risk Comment: simple randomisation used to assign one of each two children who entered to each group (1:1 ratio). Once two parents had booked a baseline assessment, a sealed envelope from each group and the times of the baseline assessments were taken to an academic staff member not involved in the research. The person shuffled the envelopes and used a coin toss to pick one of the two, writing it on the sealed envelopes. The researcher left the room while the academic staff member completed the procedure.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Low risk Comment: assessors were blinded to treatment allocation for all participants. While the researchers knew that children in the same family would receive the same allocation, treatment allocation was not revealed to them. The blinded raters were not aware of any familial relationships.
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: participants and personnel were blinded at baseline assessment, but participants were then informed of allocation.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes Low risk Comment: assessors were blind at baseline and outcome assessment. Blinded raters were used.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes Low risk Comment: reasons for dropout (death in family and change in sport schedule) unrelated to true outcome
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Unclear risk Comment: retrospectively registered; protocol submitted 2 September 2014 and approved 10 September 2014. Participants enrolled 1 June 2013 to 7 June 2014 and data collection was finished in October 2014. More measures described in protocol but not reported in paper (potentially published in other paper but we have not been able to identify other papers related to this study during the review process)
Vested interest bias Low risk Comment: funding organisation not found to have vested interests. A manual and DVD material were described which could indicate a potential commercial interest, however, we were unable to locate information on the material online.
Other sources of bias? Low risk Comment: no other sources of bias identified

Schramm 2016.

Methods Design: RCT, parallel group
Participants Country: Germany
Setting: outpatient
Sample size calculation: not reported
Sample size: 113 children
Sex: 85% = boys (group one: 34 (85%) = boys; group two: 32 (86.5%= = boys))
Age: mean = 13.99 years (SD = 1.43) (group one: mean = 14.10 years (SD = 1.42; group two: mean = 13.83 years (SD = 1.28))
Ethnicity: not reported
Socioeconomic status: not reported
IQ: not reported
ADHD diagnosis: not reported
ADHD medication: number of children not receiving medication, type not specified = 50% (group one = 16 (40%); group two = 17 (47%)
Comorbidity: not reported
Medications for comorbid disorders: not reported
Inclusion criteria:
  1. ADHD diagnosis based on DSM‐IV criteria established by a clinical interview and ADHD symptom criteria ratings DSM‐IV‐TR by parents and teachers based on Fremdbeurteilungsbogen für Hyperkinetische Störungen (FBB‐HKS)

  2. 12 to 17 years of age


Exclusion criteria: meeting criteria for severe comorbid disorders (e.g. psychotic episode)
Baseline characteristics: no significant difference found in baseline characteristics
Interventions 113 participants allocated to one of three groups. Only group one and group two were included in the review analysis.
  1. Group one (n = 40): social skills training ‐ Learning Skills Training for Adolescents with ADHD (ADHS‐LeJA). This was a manualised, multimodal intervention combining an adolescent‐directed training approach with a behavioural training component.

  2. Group two (n = 36): waiting list. Participants were invited twice for data collection with an average interval of 5.76 (SD= 51.65) months in between and expected to start intervention after post‐measurement, which was offered for ethical reasons.

  3. Group three (n = 37): active control. The active (i.e. therapeutic attention) intervention consisted of an adaptation of a Progressive Muscle Relaxation (PMR) training.


Attendance: rate not reported. Four dropped out (group one = two (one = familial difficulties, one = discontinued intervention), group two = two (no longer interested in participating))
Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills: SDQ, prosocial subscale, teacher‐, child‐ and parent‐rated

  2. Emotional competencies: SDQ, emotional symptoms subscale, teacher‐, parents‐ and child‐rated

  3. General behaviour: SDQ, conduct problems subscale, teacher‐, child‐ and parent‐rated


Secondary outcomes
  1. Core ADHD symptoms: Fremdbeurteilungsbogen für Hyperkinetische Störungen Hyperactivity/Impulsivity subscale, teacher‐, parent‐, child‐rated

  2. Performance and grades in school: the German teacher‐rated questionnaire for learning and working behaviour (Arbeitsverhalten Lehrer) (Lauth 2004), teacher‐rated


Outcome assessment: post‐intervention assessment
Notes Study ID: not reported
Sponsorship source: not reported
Year conducted: data were collected during the years of 2009‐2012
Duration of the study: 6 months
Comments: approved by the Research Board of the Department of Special Education and Rehabilitation, University of Oldenburg, Germany
Lead author: Satyam Antonio Schramm
Institution: University of Potsdam
Email: satyam.schramm@ifs.uni‐hannover.de
Address: University of Potsdam, Campus Golm, House 31, Space 2:12, Karl‐Liebknecht‐Strasse 24‐25, 14476 Potsdam, Germany
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: need more information. Trickle processing approach described as often being associated with corruption of assignment
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: need more information
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: trainers and children must have been aware of status, as training, active control or waiting list were blinded.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: parent and children were aware of group assignment. Teachers not described as effectively blinded
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes Low risk Comment: results for all measures noted in methods section reported
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: small (n = 2) loss to follow‐up in waiting‐list control group due to participants not wanting to wait anymore. One dropped out of the intervention group due to familial difficulties and one dropped out of the active control group due to an accident..
Vested interest bias High risk Comment: last study author was also the author of the published manual of the programme being investigated.
Other sources of bias? Low risk Comment: moderate level of missing data (6.4%), mostly due to incomplete teacher report (17.6%). Missing items were assumed to be missing at random and were imputed groupwise through expectation maximization algorithm.

Evans 2016.

Methods Design: RCT, parallel group
Participants Country: USA
Setting: outpatient
Sample size calculation: not reported
Sample size: 326 children (group one: 112, group two: 110, control :104)
Sex: 232 (71%) = boys, 94 (29%) = girls (group one: 79 (71%) = boys, 33 ( 29%) = girls, group two: 76 (69%) = boys, 34 (31%) = girls, group three: 77 (74%), 27 (26%) = girls)
Age: mean = 12.1 years (SD: 0.9‐1.0) (group one: mean = 12.1 years (SD = 0.9), group two: mean = 12.1 years (SD = 0.9), group three: mean = 12.1 years (SD = 1.0))
Ethnicity: group one: 7.1% African‐American, 74.1% white, 14.3% biracial, 4.5% other, 2.7% Hispanic, group two: 14.5% African‐American, 78.2% white, 5.5% biracial, 1.8% other, 5.5% Hispanic, group three: 14.4% African‐American, 79.8% white, 4.8% biracial, 1% other, 1% Hispanic
Socioeconomic status: (group one: mean 56.500, SD 45.200 in thousand $, group two: mean 61.500, SD 52.400 in thousand $, group three: mean 63.500, SD 55.500 in thousand $)
IQ: means: group one: mean = 100.3 (SD = 14.2), group two: mean = 99.2 (SD = 13.1), group three: mean = 101.4 (SD = 13.7))
ADHD diagnosis: children with combined subtype: group one = 55 (49.1%), group two = 55 (50%), group three = 49 (47.1%)
ADHD medication: group one: 49 (43.8%), group two: 57 (51.8%), group three 47 (45.4%)
Comorbidity: ODD or CD: 55.%, anxiety disorders: 27%, depressive disorders: 13% (group one: anxiety disorder = 19.6%, depression = 8%, mean ODD symptoms = 4.5 (SD = 2.3), mean CD symptoms = 2.1 (SD = 1.9), group two: anxiety disorder = 21.1%, depression = 10.9%, mean ODD symptoms = 4.7 (SD = 2.3), mean CD symptoms = 1.9 (SD = 1.3), group three: anxiety disorder = 17.3%, depression = 7.7%, mean ODD symptoms = 4.4 (SD = 2.2), mean CD symptoms = 1.7 (SD = 1.4)
Medication for comorbid disorders: group one: 49 (43.8%), group two: 57 (51.8%), group three: 47 (45.4%)
Inclusion criteria:
  1. attended one of the participating schools, grade six, seven, or eight

  2. met full DSM–IV–TR diagnostic criteria for either ADHD–predominantly inattentive type or ADHD–combined type, based on the Parent Children’s Interview for Psychiatric Syndromes (P‐ChIPS; (Weller 2010) or combined with teacher ratings on the Disruptive Behavior Disorders Rating Scale (Van Eck 2010)

  3. demonstrated impairment based on parent or teacher reports on the Impairment Rating Scale (IRS; score of 3 = impairment)

  4. IQ of 80 or above, estimated using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fourth Edition (Wechsler 2003)


Exclusion criteria:
  1. met diagnostic criteria for a pervasive developmental disorder or any of the following on the P‐ChIPS: bipolar disorder, psychosis, or obsessive–compulsive disorder


Baseline characteristics: there were no statistically significant differences between groups on any demographic variables.
Interventions 326 participants allocated to one of three groups
  1. Group one (n = 112): Challenging Horizons Program – After School version (CHP‐AS). Occurred twice weekly for two hours and 15 minutes per day and included organisation, social functioning, and academic study skills interventions. Further organisation and task progress was monitored daily. Six to 10 students were assigned to a group. One to two students were each assigned to a primary counsellor (PC). The CHP‐AS PCs were staffed by undergraduate students and a site supervisor (graduate student/postdoc fellow) who supervised the PCs and led group activities. All staff received nine hours of training prior to beginning the programme, and PCs received weekly supervision.

  2. Group two (n = 110): Challenging Horizons Program – Mentoring version (CHP‐M). Students were paired with a mentor (e.g. teacher) who was trained by a consultant to deliver a subset of the CHP‐AS interventions during school. Mentor meet weekly with student and biweekly with research staff.

  3. Group three (n = 104): community care/TAU (no direct intervention was provided in community care/TAU). A list of available resources in the specific community setting was collated and distributed to the participants randomised to this group.


Attendance:
  1. Group one (CHP‐AS):

    1. mean number of sessions offered in the group was 53.80 (range = 47‐68; median = 53.5).

    2. students attended a mean of 31.85 sessions (SD = 18.75, range = 0‐59; median = 36).

    3. of the 112 students assigned to the after‐school programme condition, 105 (94%) attended at least one session. Twenty‐two percent of the participants withdrew from treatment during the academic year. The average number of meetings attended by parents was 1.67 (SD = 1.23, range = 0 to 3; median = 2).

  2. Group two (CHP‐M):

    1. average number of consultant‐mentor meetings was 13.39 (SD = 3.65, range = 0‐22; median = 14).

    2. average consultant‐mentor meeting duration was 19.59 minutes (SD = 6.47, range = 8 to 44 minutes; median = 18.00).

    3. average number of mentor–student meetings (intervention sessions) was 25.17 (SD = 17.14; median = 22.5).

    4. average number of mentor–student feedback sessions completed was 1.84 (SD = 0.99; median = 2).

    5. average mentor–student intervention session duration was 12.12 minutes (SD = 7.17, range = 2 to 53 minutes; median = 10.33).

    6. mentor–student interventions involved organisational skills (75%), homework recording accuracy in assignment notebooks (53%), daily report cards (30%), missing assignment checks (20%), study skills (10%), or some other type of intervention (3%).

    7. mentor–student pairings involved one intervention (30%), two interventions (50%), three interventions (18%), and four interventions (2%).

    8. 3% of participants withdrew from treatment during the academic year (i.e. student discontinued meetings with mentor).

  3. Group three (TAU): three children withdrew from the study. No other information reported

Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills: SSIS, teacher‐ and parent‐rated

  2. General behaviour: DBDRS, ODD symptoms, teacher‐ and parent‐rated

  3. Core ADHD symptoms: DBDRS, Hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms subscale, teacher‐ and parent‐rated


Seconardy outcomes
  1. Performance and grades in school: Classroom Performance Survey (academic factor), teacher‐rated


Outcome assessment: post‐intervention and follow‐up six months after end of intervention
Notes Study ID: not identified
Sponsorship source: National Insitute of Mental Health (NIMH); R01MH082864, R01MH082865
Year conducted: 2016
Duration of the study: 1 academic year plus 6 months follow‐up
Comments: none
Lead author's name: Evans SW
Institution: Ohio University
Email: evanss3@ohio.edu
Address: Department of Psychology, Ohio University, Athens, OH45701
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Low risk Comment: comment from author (Evans 2017 [pers comm]): "Randomization was conducted after the recruitment of each of the three cohorts. Our statistician (who was not involved in recruitment) generated a string of random numbers that led to the assignment of participants to condition. The statistician sent the PIs at each site the condition allocation for our sites".
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Low risk Comment: comment from author (Evans 2017 [pers comm]): "The statistician sent the PIs at each site the condition allocation for our sites".
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: based on the type of intervention, it was not possible for participants and personnel to be blinded to group status. However, according to the study authors, parents had similar expectations for improvement in both active treatment conditions.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: the outcomes relevant for this review were parent and teacher ratings. The study author commented (Evans 2017 [pers comm]): "Teachers were minimally aware of condition as they completed assessments, but were not actively involved in any other activities for participants in any condition. Nevertheless, had they wanted to know the services a child was or was not receiving, the information was available to them. Thus, as most, if not all teachers remained unaware, they were not “blinded” to treatment condition".
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes Low risk Comment: used intention‐to‐treat. In total, 12 children were included in the study but were not included in the outcome assessment. Eight children did not start the intervention (seven from group one and one from group two) and four children withdrew from the study (one from group one and three from group three).
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Unclear risk Comment: no study registration found, so it was not possible to judge if all outcome data were reported. Grade point average data were collected, however mean and SD were not reported.
Vested interest bias Low risk Quote: the research was supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH; R01MH082864, R01MH082865), no sources of bias identified.
Other sources of bias? Low risk Comment: no other sources of bias identified

Pfiffner 2016.

Methods Design: Cluster‐RCT, parallel group
Participants Country: USA
Setting: outpatient
Sample size calculation: sample size was based on medium to large effect sizes previously found by the authors. For the sample in the study, the estimated detectable effect size was 0.48.
Sample size: 135 children
Sex: (group one: 54 (75 %) = boys, 18 (25%) = girls; group two: 42 (67%) = boys, 21 (33%) = girls)
Age: (group one: mean = 8.3 years (SD = 1.1); group two: mean = 8.5 years (SD = 1.1))
Ethnicity: (group one: white = 31%, African‐American = 8%, Asian = 22%, Hispanic/Latino = 21%, multiracial/multiethnic = 18%; group two: white = 22%, African‐American = 10%, Asian = 19%, Hispanic/Latino = 27%, multiracial/multiethnic = 22%)
Socioeconomic status: % college graduates (group one: 65%; group two 55%)
IQ: on WASI FSIQ (group one: mean = 103.0 (SD = 13.0); group two: mean = 101.0 (SD = 14.7))
ADHD diagnosis: group one: combined = 54%, inattentive = 40%, hyperactive‐impulsive = 6%; group two: combined = 62%, inattentive = 38%, hyperactive‐impulsive = none
ADHD medication: group one: 9.7%, group two: 7.9%
Comorbidity: group one: ODD = 43%, group two: ODD = 59%
Medication for comorbid disorders: (balanced between groups)
Inclusion criteria:
  1. high ratings of ADHD symptoms (i.e. equal or above six inattention symptoms and/or equal or above six hyperactive/impulsive symptoms) endorsed on the CSI by the parent or teacher as occurring often or very often

  2. cross‐situational impairment (home and school), documented as a score of at least three in at least one domain of functioning on parent and teacher Impairment rating scales

  3. full‐Scale IQ equivalent to higher than 79 on the WASI

  4. caretaker available to participate in treatment; and a primary classroom teacher who agreed to participate in the classroom component

  5. children taking medication providing their regimens were stable


Exclusion criteria: students with significant visual or hearing impairments,severe language delay, psychosis, or pervasive developmental disorder or who were in full‐day special day classrooms
Baseline characteristics: groups did not differ on demographics or medication use at baseline.
Interventions 135 participants allocated to one of two groups
  1. Group one (n = 72): Collaborative Life skills (CL). This is a 12‐week programme delivered by school‐based mental health providers. It is a psychosocial multicomponent treatment that integrates classroom interventions, parent training groups, and child skills groups. Social skills modules include, for example: good sportsmanship, accepting consequences, assertion, dealing with teasing, problem‐solving, self‐control, and friendship making. Independence modules include, for example: homework skills; completing chores and tasks independently; and establishing and following routines. Activities accommodated developmental needs (e.g. having older children take more of a leader/helper role in groups, providing age‐appropriate examples of skill use).

  2. Group 2 (n = 63): business as usual; i.e. usual school and community services


Attendance:
  1. Parent attendance at groups averaged above 79% (range 0–100%). More than 90% attended at least half the group sessions.

  2. Child attendance averaged above 92% (range 67–100%).

Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills: SSIS, teacher‐ and parent‐rated

  2. General behaviour: CSI, ODD subscale, teacher‐ and parent‐rated


Secondary outcomes
  1. Core ADHD symptoms: CSI: ADHD subscale, teacher‐ and parent‐rated

  2. Performance and grades in school: SSIS: academic competence subscale, teacher‐rated


Outcome assessment: post‐intervention assessment
Notes Study ID:NCT01686724
Sponsorship source: supported by a grant from the Institute of Education Sciences, US Department of Education to the University of California‐San Francisco (award number R324A120358)
Year conducted: 2016
Duration of the study: 12‐week programme
Comments: none
Lead author's name: Linda J Pfiffner
Institution: Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Francisco
Email: linda.pfiffner@ucsf.edu
Address: Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Francisco, 401 Parnassus Avenue, Box 0984, San Francisco, CA 94143
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: unclear how the randomisation was done
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: no details
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: not mentioned, but it did not seem possible to blind participants and personnel in the study
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: outcome measures were based on teacher and parent ratings and the authors commented that this might lead to rater bias.
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes Unclear risk Comment: not clear
Selective reporting (reporting bias) High risk Comment: protocol available. The primary and secondary outcomes, including the time frame reported in the study, did not match the protocol.
Vested interest bias Unclear risk Comment: the study was supported by a grant from the Institute of Education Sciences. Authors had conducted previous studies on the same intervention.
Other sources of bias? High risk Comment: at each assessment point, parents and teachers were paid US$50 for completing the measurements.

Qian 2017.

Methods Design: RCT, parallel group
Participants Country: China
Setting: clinical
Sample size calculation: not reported
Sample size: 86 children
Sex: 54 = boys, 14 = girls (group one: 32 (84.2%) = boys, 6 (15.8%) = girls; group two: 22 (73.3%) = boys, 8 (26.7%) = girls)
Age: 6‐12 years (group one: mean = 8.3 years (SD = 1.3); group two: mean = 7.8 years (SD = 1.2))
Ethnicity: not reported
Socioeconomic status: not reported
IQ: group one: mean = 105.7 (SD = 13.9); group two: mean = 101.8 (SD = 10.4)
ADHD diagnosis: group one: inattentive = 17 (44.7%), hyperactivity‐impulsivity = 0 (0%), combined = 21 (55.33%); group two: inattentive = 16 (53.3%), hyperactivity‐impulsivity = 1 (3.3%), combined = 13 (43.3%)
ADHD medication: group one: 10 participants had maintained steady dosage of medications for more than a half year and remained unchanged during the entire study. New medications could not be initiated during the study.
Comorbidity: group one: ODD = 7 (18.4%), learning disorder = 8 (21.1%), special phobia = 5 (13.2%); group two: ODD = 7 (23.3%), learning disorder = 4 (13.3%), special phobia = 2 (6.7%)
Medications for comorbid disorders: not reported
Inclusion criteria:
  1. diagnosis of ADHD: meeting DSM‐IV criteria based on parent ratings on ADHD‐RS‐IV confirmed by semi‐structured interview by experienced paediatric psychiatrist using the clinical diagnostic interview scale

  2. six to 12 years of age


Exclusion criteria:
  1. history of head injury

  2. diagnosis of other congenital or acquired neurological conditions

  3. estimated full‐scale IQ < 80

  4. diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder, psychosis, or an emergent psychiatric condition that needed immediate medication

  5. new medications could not be initiated during the study.


Baseline characteristics: no significant difference found in reported baseline characteristics
Interventions 86 participants allocated to one of two groups
  1. Group one (n = 44): executive skills training based on Dawson Guare's (Dawson 2010) training with the content adapted culturally to ensure acceptability to Chinese children. Groups of six to eight families received 12 weekly × one‐hour sessions in clinical setting. First and last sessions included parents; it was not clear whether the children participated. The first session focused on setting behavioural goals, action plans, environmental modifications and reward systems and how to help with homework. The last session was on how to continue to use the learned skills. Only children participated in sessions two to 11 and handbook‐specified homework between sessions.

  2. Group two (n = 42): waiting list


*Analysis, including baseline characteristics, was based on the following sample sizes: intervention = 38 participants, waiting‐list control = 30 participants
Attendance: 86.4% (38/44) of children in intervention group complied with the training, completing 10 or more sessions in the 12‐session period. All missed group sessions were administered to the trainee individually. The percentage of the number of sessions administered individually was not specified.
Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills: Weiss Functional Impairment Rating Scale, social activities subscale, parent‐rated

  2. Emotional competencies: BRIEF, emotional control subscale, parent‐rated

  3. General behaviour: BRIEF, total score, parent‐rated


Secondary outcomes
  1. Core ADHD symptoms: ADHD‐RS, fourth version, parent‐rated


Outcome assessment: post‐intervention assessment
Notes Study ID:NCT02327585
Sponsorship source: the study was supported by grants from the Beijing Municipal Science and Technology Commission (No. Z151100004015103), the Major State Basic Research Development Program of China (973 Program, No. 2014CB846100), National Key Research Plan of Ministry of Science and Technology of China (No. 2016YFC1306103), and the Capital Health Development Research Fund (No.2011‑4024‑04).
Year conducted: 2017
Duration of the study: 12 weeks
Comments: the study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Peking University Sixth Hospital.
Lead author's name: Ying Qian
Institution: Child Psychiatric Research Center, Peking University Sixth Hospital (Institute of Mental Health), National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders, Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Ministry of Health (Peing University)
Email: yangli_pkuimh@bjmu.edu.cn, corresponding author Dr Li Yang
Address: Beijing 100191, China
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Low risk Comment: randomised block design with participants randomised to a block that comprised a permutation of four participants, two for each group separately. The design was used to balance the individuals between the intervention and waiting‐list groups.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Low risk Comment: randomisation grouping concealed in envelopes and recruited participant notified of his or her group sequentially
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: blinding not possible
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: primarily used parent rating scales and parents were not blinded to group status. Unclear, however, if assessors of executive functioning performance tests were blinded to group status
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes Unclear risk Comment: 6/44 children in intervention group and 12/42 children in control (waiting‐list) group dropped out. No reasons given for dropout. Baseline data for the 18 children who dropped out not provided and no analysis of attrition
Selective reporting (reporting bias) High risk Comment: The study registration specified the following secondary outcomes, which were not mentioned or reported in the paper: Conners; and Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automatic Battery. The following measures were not mentioned in the study registration but were reported in the paper: BRIEF; WEISS Functional Impairment Scale‐Parents.
Vested interest bias Unclear risk Comment: funding source not reported
Other sources of bias? Low risk Comment: no other sources of bias identified

Hannesdottir 2017.

Methods Design: RCT, cross‐over
Participants Country: Iceland
Setting: clinical: Centre for Child Development and Behavior in Reykjavik
Sample size calculation: not reported
Sample size: 41
Sex: 29 (71%) = boys, 12 (29%) = girls
Age: mean = 9.2 years (SD = 0.62, range = 8‐10)
Ethnicity: not reported
Socioeconomic status: not reported
IQ: IQ > 70
Diagnosis: ADHD combined = 36 (88%), ADHD inattentive = four (10%), ADHD hyperactive‐impulsive = one (2%)
ADHD medication: intervention = 100%, control = 85.7%
Cormorbidity: not reported
Medications for comorbid disorders: not reported
Inclusion criteria:
  1. aged eight to 10 years

  2. previously diagnosed with ADHD by a licensed clinical psychologist and a medical doctor


Exclusion criteria:
  1. diagnosis of an autism spectrum disorder

  2. IQ below 70


Baseline characteristics: significant difference in number of days between assessment. Concern with regard to practice effect, but this was not the case for the waiting‐list control group, who had the fewest days between measurements.
Interventions 30 participants allocated to one of two groups and then crossed over
  1. Group one (n = 16): Participated in a social skills programme called “outSMARTers”. A group of six children, worked in smaller groups of three children in a predetermined order at multiple training stations with a reward system for completing assignments and following rules. The therapists led discussions among the children necessary to solve the tasks and to reinforce appropriate behaviours in the group with tokens. Tokens could be used at the end of the session to buy rewards such as trading cards or movie tickets. 10 afternoon sessions of two hours each over the duration of five weeks.

  2. Group 2 (n = 14): waiting list to receive the OutSMARTers programme later on


Attendance: no percentage of attendance reported as requirement. Participants in the intervention group attended more than 90% of the sessions and no child missed more than two sessions.
Outcomes Primary outcomes
  1. Social skills: SSRS, parent‐rated

  2. Emotional competencies: Emotion Regulation Checklist: Emotion Regulation subscale, parent‐rated

  3. General behaviour: SDQ, parent‐rated


Secondary outcomes
  1. Core ADHD symptoms: ADHD‐RS: Hyperactivity/impulsivity subscale, parent‐rated


Outcome assessment: post‐intervention and follow‐up three months after end of intervention
Notes Study ID: none found
Sponsorship source: no financial support for the study reported
Year conducted: 2014
Duration of the study: 5 weeks plus 3 months follow‐up
Comments: study was approved by the National Bioethics Committee in Iceland. We received additional information from authors on the randomisation procedure, allocation concealment, and assessment (Hannesdottir 2018 [pers comm]) and on the intervention and funding (Hannesdottir 2018b [pers comm]).
Lead author's name: Hannesdottir DK
Institution: Centre for Child Development and Behaviour for the Primary Health Care of the Capital Area
Email: dagmar.kristin@gmail.com
Address: Dagmar Kristin Hannesdottir, Throska‐ og hegdunarstod, Thonglabakka 1, 109 Reykjavik, Iceland
Risk of bias
Bias Authors' judgement Support for judgement
Random sequence generation (selection bias) Low risk Comment: Parents who contacted the coordinator for their child to participate in the OutSMARTers programme were allocated the next number available (e.g. fifth parent to contact the coordinator got the number five). A computer‐generated list with computer randomisation of numbers (1‐50) allocated participants to either the treatment group or the waiting‐list control group.
Allocation concealment (selection bias) Unclear risk Comment: allocation concealment not mentioned
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: it was not seen as possible to blind participants and personnel.
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) 
 All outcomes High risk Comment: outcomes were parent‐reported (who were not blinded)
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) 
 All outcomes Low risk Comment: only one child in intervention group one did not complete treatment, compared with two children in intervention group two.
Selective reporting (reporting bias) Low risk Comment: all measures described in methods section were reported in results tables. There was no study registration so it was not possible to judge whether any further measures had been included but not reported. It was not clear on what basis it was decided to use and report total scores or subscale scores for the included measures.
Vested interest bias Low risk Comment: the authors did not receive any funding for the study. The child centre is run by the government and is part of the primary healthcare service in Iceland. Thus, the government paid for the trainers/instructors and supplied the WISC subtests and other measures.
Other sources of bias? Low risk Comment: no other sources of bias identified

Characteristics of excluded studies [ordered by year of study]

Study Reason for exclusion
Wolraich 1978 Ineligible intervention
Rosén 1984 Ineligible intervention
Horn 1990 Ineligible comparator
Kolko 1990 Ineligible patient population
Klein 1997 Ineligible intervention
Miranda 2002 Ineligible intervention
Kolko 1999 Ineligible intervention
Gonzalez 2002 Ineligible intervention
Feinfield 2004 Ineligible patient population
Döpfner 2004 Ineligible intervention
Corkum 2005 Parent training programme only
Gol 2005 Ineligible comparator
Langberg 2008 Ineligible intervention
Molina 2008 Ineligible comparator
Grasmann 2011 Ineligible patient population
Dodge 2011 Ineligible patient population
Lessard 2011 Ineligible patient population
Pumpuang 2012 Ineligible intervention
Power 2012a Ineligible intervention
Cionek Szpak 2012 Ineligible intervention
Jans 2012 Ineligible intervention
ICBM 2012 Meeting Ineligible intervention
Malti 2012 Ineligible patient population
Langberg 2012 Ineligible intervention
Ostberg 2012 Ineligible intervention
ESCAP 2013 Ineligible intervention
Abikoff 2013 Ineligible intervention
Lim‐Ashworth 2013a Ineligible patient population
Cipolla 2013 Ineligible patient population
Sibley 2013 Ineligible intervention
Sibley 2013a Ineligible intervention
Vidal 2015 Adult population
Jans 2015 Ineligible intervention
Sibley 2016 Ineligible intervention
Bussing 2016 Ineligible patient population
Leung 2017 Ineligible patient population
NCT02574273 Ineligible comparator
Pfiffner 2018 Ineligible comparator
NCT03176108 Ineligible intervention

Characteristics of studies awaiting assessment [ordered by year of study]

NCT01019252.

Methods Design: RCT
Number of arms: two
Participants Country: USA
Sample size calculation: not reported
Target sample size: 66
Eligible age: age 14 to 18 years
Eligible sex: all
Inclusion criteria:
  1. age 14 to 18 years

  2. in high school

  3. principal diagnosis of ADHD

  4. stable prescription of medications for ADHD

  5. childhood onset of ADHD

  6. clinically significant ADHD symptoms


Exclusion criteria:
  1. comorbidity of: organic mental disorders; active substance abuse or dependence; diagnosis of conduct disorder; mental retardation or pervasive developmental disorder

  2. active suicidality

  3. other condition interfering with consent or participation

  4. previous history of CBT therapy in adolescence

Interventions
  1. Group one: behavioural: compensatory executive skills training, cognitive behavioural therapy. Participants provided with education about ADHD and instruction in organisational skills, reducing distractibility, and adaptive thinking. Twelve weekly treatment sessions

  2. Group two: no intervention. Cross‐over design with the participants on the waiting list receiving CBTafter the four‐month assessment

Outcomes
  1. ADHD symptoms: ADHD‐RS, child‐ and parent‐rated; CGI scale, clinician‐rated

  2. Secondary symptoms of ADHD (e.g. mood)


Time of assessment: before randomisation, and at follow‐up, four months and eight months after randomisation
Notes Study ID:NCT01019252
Sponsorship source: R34MH083063, US NIH Grant/Contract; DDTR B4‐TBI, National Institute of Mental Health
Study start date: October 2009
Study end date: August 2012
Status: completed
Declared conflict of interest: not reported
Comments: none
Registrant's name: Steven A Safren
Institution: Massachusetts General Hospital
Address: not reported
Email: not reported
Telephone: not reported

ADHD: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
 ADHD‐ RS: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder ‐ Rating Scal
 CBT: cognitive behavioural therapy
 CGI: Clincal Global Impressions 
 RCT: randomised controlled trial

Characteristics of ongoing studies [ordered by year of study]

NCT01330849.

Trial name or title Toolkit for school behavior modification in children with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)
Methods Design: RCT
Number of arms: two
Participants Country: Belgium
Sample size calculation: not reported
Target sample size: 100
Eligible age: five to 13 years old
Eligible sex: all
Inclusion criteria:
  1. teacher must rate student's ADHD symptoms ≧ 90.9th percentile on the inattention or hyperactivity/impulsivity subscale of the VvGK (a Dutch translation of the Disruptive Behaviour Disorders Rating Scale)

  2. maximum of two children per classroom may be included


Exclusion criteria:
  1. children with mental retardation (IQ < 70) automatically excluded from study, since study runs in schools

Interventions
  1. Group one: behavioural: ADHD Toolkit. For children allocated to the active intervention arm, teachers will be trained to apply the ADHD Toolkit. Teachers will use the behaviour modification tool for 3 months. They will be trained to select target behaviours causing impairment for the child and will apply a systematic approach of increased intensity of monitoring and feedback for the behaviour, including training of appropriate behaviour.

  2. Group two: waiting list control group. Children in the control group will receive no specific intervention, but are promised that their teachers will apply the school kit for them after the study.

Outcomes
  1. Disruptive Behavior Disorder (DBD) Rating Scale: ADHD subscale, teacher‐rated

  2. DBD Rating Scale: ADHD subscale, parent‐rated

  3. DBD Rating Scale: Conduct Disorder and Oppositional Defiant Disorder subscales, teacher‐ and parent‐rated

  4. Target Behaviour Improvement Rating Scale, teacher‐rated

  5. Teacher Report Form: Internalising Problems subscale, teacher‐rated

  6. Child Behaviour Check List: Internalising Problems subscale, child‐rated

  7. Impairment Rating Scale, parent‐ and teacher‐rated

  8. Perceived Competence Scale for Children, child‐rated

  9. Student‐Teacher Relationship Scale, rater not specified

  10. Teachers Beliefs and Attitudes towards ADHD Scale, teacher‐rated

  11. Feasibility, Acceptability and Usefulness Scale, teacher‐ and child‐rated


Time of assessment: evaluation after using the ADHD Toolkit for a three‐month period
Starting date Study start date: December 2010
Estimated study completion date: June 2011
Status: unknown. Registered, with June 2011 as the estimated final date of data collection. No changes since registration. First posted 7 April 2011
Contact information Registrant name: Marina Danckaerts
Institution: Universitaire Ziekenhuizen, Leuven
Address: Leuven, Vlaams‐Brabant, Belgium, 3000
Email:marina.danckaerts@uzleuven.be
Telephone: (+32)16343821
Notes Study ID:NCT01330849
Sponsorship source: Universitaire Ziekenhuizen Leuven
Declared conflict of interest: not reported
Comments: none

Yang 2015.

Trial name or title Efficacy of an integrated behavioural therapy for children with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a randomized controlled trial
Methods Design: RCT
Number of arms: two
Participants Country: China
Sample size calculation: not reported
Target sample size: 40
Eligible age: not reported
Eligible sex: not reported
Inclusion criteria: not reported
Exclusion criteria: not reported
Interventions
  1. Group one: integrated, behavioural, therapeutic programme for ADHD children. The integrated behavioural therapeutic programme was designed on the basis of Dawson and Guare’s (2010) executive skill training programme, integrated with behaviour modification and social skill training. Twelve weeks of group training, once a week, targeting time management, working memory, organisation, planning, and behaviour inhibition, etc.

  2. Group two: waiting list control group

Outcomes
  1. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder ‐ Rating Scale

  2. Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function


Time of assessment: pre‐ and post‐training
Starting date Start date: not reported
Estimated study completion date: not reported
Status: conference abstract but no publication identified
Contact information Lead author's name: L Yang
Institution: not reported
Address: Beijing, China
Email: not reported
Telephone: not reported
Notes Study ID: not reported
Sponsorship source: not reported
Declared conflict of interest: not reported
Comments: none

IRCT201609186834N11.

Trial name or title The effect of psychoeducational group therapy of emotional intelligence on enhancing of emotional intelligence of adolescents with ADHD
Methods Design: RCT
Number of arms: two
Participants Country: Iran
Sample size calculation: not reported
Target sample size: 76
Eligible age: 11 to 16 years old
Eligible sex: both
Inclusion criteria:
  • diagnosis of ADHD

  • age range of 11 to 16 years old

  • treated with methylphenidate

  • have no other physical illness or mental disorder according to the diagnosis of psychiatrist

  • have the ability to listen and sit in group therapy sessions and to respond to written questions


Exclusion criteria:
  • patients who discharge before the completion of group therapy sessions

  • patients with absence of more than two sessions of group therapy

Interventions
  1. Group one: intervention group will receive group therapy (10 sessions)

  2. Group two: control group will not receive any intervention

Outcomes
  1. Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire

  2. Revised Conner’s Parent Rating Scale


Time of assessment: before intervention, one week and three months after finishing the intervention
Starting date Study start date: recruitment expected to start on 22 September 2016
Estimated study completion date: recruitment expected to end 19 February 2017
Status: no updates made to the registration
Contact information Registrant name: Hossein Ebrahimi
Institution: Tabriz University of Medical Sciences
Address: Psychiatric Nursing Group, Tabriz Nursing and Midwifery Faculty, South Shariati Street
Email: ebrahimih@tbzmed.ac.ir
Telephone: +98 41 1475 1709
Notes Study ID:IRCT201609186834N11
Sponsorship source: Research Vice‐ Chancellor, Tabriz University of Medical Sciences
Declared conflict of interest: not reported
Comments: none

NCT02937142.

Trial name or title Cognitive behavior group therapy in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
Methods Design: RCT
Number of arms: two
Participants Country: Norway
Sample size calculation: not reported
Target sample size: 96
Eligible age: 14 to 18 years
Eligible sex: all
Inclusion criteria:
  • ADHD (ICD‐10)

  • score ≧ 3 on CGI scale

  • informed consent patient

  • informed consent parents

  • if on medication, dosage is stable since at least two months


Exclusion criteria:
  • mental retardation

  • behavioural problems

  • drug addiction

  • psychosis

  • suicidal

Interventions
  1. Group one: cognitive behaviour group therapy, in weekly, 1.5‐hour sessions during 12 weeks. Eight participants and two therapists per group, in addition to education on ADHD, and ADHD medication if indicated (TAU)

  2. Group two: education on ADHD, and ADHD medication if indicated (TAU)

Outcomes Primary outcomes:
  1. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder ‐ Rating Scale


Secondary outcomes:
  1. Clincal Global Impressions

  2. Weiss Functional Impairment Rating Scale

  3. Children's Global Assessment Scale

  4. Screen for Child Anxiety Related Disorders

  5. Mood and Feelings Questionnaire, Norwegian version

  6. Adolescent Sleep Wake Scale

  7. Rosenberg Self‐Esteem Scale

  8. General Perceived Self‐Efficacy Scale

  9. Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function

  10. Brief Problem Monitor


Time of assessment: at baseline, immediately after the last group therapy session (after 12 weeks) and at follow‐up (after nine months)
Starting date Study start date: January 2017
Estimated study completion date: August 2020
Status: last verified January 2019
Contact information Registrant name: Torunn Stene Nøvik, MD PhD
Institution: St Olavs Hospital; Norwegian University of Science and Technology
Address: not reported
Email:torunn.stene.novik@stolav.no
Telephone: not reported
Notes Study ID:NCT02937142
Sponsorship source: St Olavs Hospital
Declared conflict of interest: not reported
Comments: none

ADHD: attention deficit hyperactivity disorderCGI: Clincal Global Impressions
 DBD: Disruptive behavior disorder
 IQ: intelligence quotient
 RCT: randomised clinical trial
 TAU: treatment as usualVvGK: Dutch translation of the Disruptive Behaviour Disorders Rating Scale
 WFIRS: Weiss Functional Impairment Rating Scale

Differences between protocol and review

We were unable to use all of our methods as specified in our protocol, Storebø 2010, in this review update. We have archived these for use in future updates of this review in Table 2.

There are some changes in the number of authors between the protocol published in 2010 and the original review version published in 2012 as Mette Elmose Andersen, Signe Joost Hansen, Nadia Pedersen, Britta Tendal, Britta Tendal, and Erlend G Faltinsen now are new authors. Christian Gluud, Erik Simonsen, and Ole Jakob Storebø have been authors of both versions of the review and the protocol. Dorte Damm, and Per Hove Thomsen, who were both authors of the published protocol as well as the original version of the review, are not authors of this updated version of the review. The reason for this change is that Dorte Dam and Per Hove Thomsen did not have the time to participate in the work with this update and therefore the new authors were invited to help with the work.

We changed the databases that we planned to search in the protocol because of: lack of access [AMED] and because no unique relevant records were identified in the previous search [AMED].

Contributions of authors

Ole Jakob Storebø: development of protocol, study selection, data extraction, 'Risk of bias' assessment, data analysis, contact person, GRADE assessment, development of the final review ‐ original and update

Mette Elmose Andersen: study selection, data extraction, 'Risk of bias' assessment, development of the final review ‐ update

Maria Skoog: development of protocol, data extraction, 'Risk of bias' assessment, development of the final review ‐ original and update

Signe Joost Hansen: study selection , data extraction, 'Risk of bias' assessment, development of the final review ‐ update

Erik Simonsen: 'Risk of bias' assessment, development of the final review ‐ original and update

Nadia Pedersen: study selection, data extraction, 'Risk of bias' assessment, development of the final review ‐ update

Britta Tendal: study selection, data extraction, 'Risk of bias' assessment, development of the final review ‐ update

Henriette E Callesen: trial selection, data extraction, 'Risk of bias' assessment, development of the final review ‐ update

Erlend Faltinsen: trial selection, data extraction, 'Risk of bias' assessment, data analysis

Christian Gluud: development of protocol, advising on statistical methods and analysis, GRADE assessment, development of the final review ‐ original and update

All authors read and approved the final version of the review before submission.

Ole Jakob Storebø is the guarantor for the review.

Sources of support

Internal sources

  • None, Other.

External sources

  • CopenhagenTrial Unit, Denmark.

    Support with TSA analyses

  • Research Library, Psychiatric Research Unit, Region Zealand, Roskilde, Denmark.

    Support to develop search strategy and search databases

  • Department of Psychology, University of Southern Denmark, Denmark.

    Financial support

Declarations of interest

Ole Jakob Storebø is an Associate Editor with the Cochrane Developmental, Psycosocial and Learning Problems Group.
 Mette Elmose Andersen ‐ none known
 Maria Skoog ‐ none known
 Signe Joost Hansen ‐ none known
 Erik Simonsen ‐ none known
 Nadia Pedersen ‐ none known
 Britta Tendal ‐ none known
 Henriette E Callesen ‐ none known
 Erlend G Faltinsen ‐ none known
 Christian Gluud ‐ none known

Ole Jakob Storebø, Maria Skoog, Erik Simonsen, and Christian Gluud were involved in the Storebø 2012 trial, which is included in this review. This trial was assessed by Nadia Pedersen, Mette Elmose, Signe Joost, and Mathilde Holmsov. These authors independently assessed the eligibility of this study, extracted data from it, assessed the risk of bias within it and assessed the quality of the evidence provided by it.

New search for studies and content updated (no change to conclusions)

References

References to studies included in this review

Abikoff 2004 {published data only}

  1. Abikoff H, Hectman L, Klein RG, Gallagher R, Fleiss K, Etcovitch J, et al. Social functioning in children with ADHD treated with long‐term methylphenidate and multimodal psychosocial treatment. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2004;43(7):820‐9. [DOI: 10.1097/01.chi.0000128797.91601.1a; PUBMED: 15213583] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Abikoff H, Hectman L, Klein RG, Weiss G, Fleiss K, Etcovitch J, et al. Symptomatic improvement in children with ADHD treated with long‐term methylphenidate and multimodal psychosocial treatment. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2004;43(7):802‐11. [DOI: 10.1097/01.chi.0000128791.10014.ac; PUBMED: 15213581] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Hechtman L, Abikoff H, Klein RG, Weiss G, Respitz C, Kouri J, et al. Academic achievement and emotional status of children with ADHD treated with long‐term methylphenidate and multimodal psychosocial treatment. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2004;43(7):812‐9. [DOI: 10.1097/01.chi.0000128796.84202.eb; PUBMED: 15213582] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Klein RG, Abikoff H, Hechtman L, Weiss G. Design and rationale of controlled study of long‐term methylphenidate and multimodal psychosocial treatment in children with ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2004;43(7):792‐801. [DOI: 10.1097/01.chi.0000128798.91601.fe; PUBMED: 15213580] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Antshel 2003 {published data only}

  1. Antshel KM, Remer R. Social skills training in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a randomized‐controlled clinical trial. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology 2003;32(1):153‐65. [DOI: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3201_14; PUBMED: 12611031] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Azad 2014 {published data only}

  1. Azad MA, Faramarzi S, Arefi M, Farhadi T, Fakkar A. The effectiveness of meta‐cognitive knowledge on reduced hyperactivity and improved attention in children afflicted with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in the primary school. Journal of Advances in Cognitive Science 2014;16(1):49‐57. [icssjournal.ir/article‐1‐223‐en.html] [Google Scholar]

Bloomquist 1991 {published data only}

  1. Bloomquist ML, August GJ, Ostrander R. Effects of a school‐based cognitive‐behavioral intervention for ADHD children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 1991;19(5):591‐605. [PUBMED: 1770187] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bloomquist ML, Braswell L. Multicomponent cognitive‐behavior intervention for attention‐deficit hyperactivity disordered children. Data on file 1989.

Bul 2016 {published data only}

  1. Bul KC, Kato PM, Oord S, Danckaerts M, Vreeke LJ, Willems A, et al. Behavioral outcome effects of serious gaming as an adjunct to treatment for children with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Medical Internet Research 2016;18(2):e26. [DOI: 10.2196/jmir.5173; PMC4773597; PUBMED: 26883052] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Choi 2015 {published data only}

  1. Choi ES, Lee WK. Comparative effects of emotion management training and social skills training in Korean children with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders 2015;19(2):138‐46. [DOI: 10.1177/1087054713496460; PUBMED: 23929521] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Cohen 1981 {published data only}

  1. Cohen NJ, Sullivan J, Minde K, Novak C, Helwig C. Evaluation of the relative effectiveness of methylphenidate and cognitive behavior modification in the treatment of kindergarten‐aged hyperactive children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 1981;9(1):43‐54. [PUBMED: 7217537] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Evans 2016 {published data only}

  1. Evans SW, Langberg JM, Schultz BK, Vaughn A, Altaye M, Marshall SA, et al. Evaluation of a school‐based treatment program for young adolescents with ADHD. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 2016;84(1):15‐30. [DOI: 10.1037/ccp0000057; PUBMED: 26501496] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Langberg JM, Evans SW, Schultz BK, Becker SP, Altaye M, Girio‐Herrera E. Trajectories and predictors of response to the challenging horizons program for adolescents with ADHD. Behavior Therapy 2016;47(3):339‐54. [DOI: 10.1016/j.beth.2016.01.001; PUBMED: 27157028] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Schultz BK, Evans SW, Langberg JM, Schoemann AM. Outcomes for adolescents who comply with long‐term psychosocial treatment for ADHD. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 2017;85(3):250‐61. [DOI: 10.1037/ccp0000172; PMC5321617; PUBMED: 28221060] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Hannesdottir 2017 {published data only}

  1. Hannesdottir DK, Ingvarsdottir E, Bjornsson A. The OutSMARTers program for children with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders 2017;21(4):353‐64. [DOI: 10.1177/1087054713520617; PUBMED: 24505061] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Meftagh 2014a {published data only}

  1. Meftagh SD, Najimi A, Mohammadi N, Ghanizadeh A, Rahimi C, Amini MM. The most effective intervention for attention deficit‐hyperactivity disorder: using continuous performance test. Psychiatria Danubina 2014;26(2):165‐71. [PUBMED: 24909254] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

MTA 1999 {published data only}

  1. Arnold LE, Abikoff HB, Cantwell DP, Conners CK, Elliott GR, Greenhill LL, et al. NIMH collaborative multimodal treatment study of children with ADHD (MTA): design methodology, and protocol evolution. Journal of Attention Disorders 1997;2(3):141‐58. [DOI: 10.1177/108705479700200301] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arnold LE, Chuang S, Davies M, Abikoff HB, Conners CK, Elliot GR, et al. Nine months of multicomponent behavioral treatment for ADHD and effectiveness of MTA fading procedures. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 2004;32(1):39‐51. [PUBMED: 14998110] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Arnold LE, Elliot M, Sachs L, Bird H, Kraemer HC, Wells KC, et al. Effects of ethnicity on treatment attendance, stimulant response/dose, and 14‐month outcome in ADHD. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 2003;71(4):713‐27. [PUBMED: 12924677] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Hinshaw SP, March JS, Abikoff H, Arnold LE, Cantwell DP, Conners CK, et al. Comprehensive assessment of childhood attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder in the context of a multisite, multimodal clinical trial. Journal of Attention Disorder 1997;1(4):217‐34. [DOI: 10.1177/108705479700100403] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Jensen PS, Arnold LE, Swanson JM, Vitiello B, Abikoff HB, Greenhill LL, et al. 3‐year follow‐up of the NIMH MTA study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2007;46(8):989‐1002. [DOI: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3180686d48; PUBMED: 17667478] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Jensen PS, Hinshaw SP, Kraemer HC, Lenora N, Newcorn JH, Abikoff HB, et al. ADHD comorbidity findings from the MTA study: comparing comorbid subgroups. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2001;40(2):147‐58. [DOI: 10.1097/00004583-200102000-00009; PUBMED: 11211363] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. MTA Cooperative Group. A 14‐month randomized clinical trial of treatment strategies for attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Multimodal treatment study of children with ADHD. Archives of General Psychiatry 1999;56(12):1073‐86. [PUBMED: 10591283] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. MTA Cooperative Group. Moderators and mediators of treatment response for children with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder: the Multimodal Treatment study of children with Attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry 1999;56(12):1088‐96. [PUBMED: 10591284] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. MTA Cooperative Group. National Institute of Mental Health Multimodal Treatment Study of ADHD follow‐up: 24‐month outcomes of treatment strategies for attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics 2004;113(4):754‐61. [PUBMED: 15060224] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. MTA Cooperative Group. National Institute of Mental Health Multimodal Treatment Study of ADHD follow‐up: changes in effectiveness and growth after the end of treatment. Pediatrics 2004;113(4):762‐9. [PUBMED: 15060225] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Molina BSG, Hinshaw SP, Swanson JM, Arnold LE, Vitiello B, Jensen PS, et al. The MTA at 8 years: prospective follow‐up of children treated for combined‐type ADHD in a multisite study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2009;48(5):484‐500. [DOI: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31819c23d0; PMC3063150; PUBMED: 19318991] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Swanson J. The MTA follow‐up into adolescence: implications for personalized treatment. The International Society for Research in Child and Adolescent Psychopathology Conference; 2009 Jun 19; Seattle (WA). Seattle (WA): ISRCAP, 2009.
  13. Wells KC, Pelham WE, Kotkin RA, Hoza B, Abikoff HB, Abramowitz A, et al. Psychosocial treatment strategies in the MTA study: rationale, methods, and critical issues in design and implementation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 2000;28(6):483‐505. [PUBMED: 11104313] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Pfiffner 1997 {published data only}

  1. Pfiffner LJ, McBurnett K. Social skills training with parent generalization: treatment effects for children with attention deficit disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 1997;65(5):749‐57. [PUBMED: 9337494] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Pfiffner 2007 {published data only}

  1. Pfiffner LJ, Yee Mikami A, Huang‐Pollock C, Easterlin B, Zalecki C, McBurnett K. A randomized, controlled trial of integrated home‐school behavioral treatment for ADHD, predominantly inattentive type. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2007;46(8):1041‐50. [DOI: 10.1097/chi.0b013e318064675f; PUBMED: 17667482] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Pfiffner 2014 {published data only}

  1. Haack LM, Villodas M, McBurnett K, Hinshaw S, Pfiffner LJ. Parenting as a mechanism of change in psychosocial treatment for youth with ADHD, predominantly inattentive presentation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 2017;45(5):841‐55. [DOI: 10.1007/s10802-016-0199-8; PMC5352559; PUBMED: 27628742] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Pfiffner LJ, Hinshaw SP, Owens E, Zalecki C, Kaiser NM, Villodas M, et al. A two‐site randomized clinical trial of integrated psychosocial treatment for ADHD‐inattentive type. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 2014;82(6):1115‐27. [DOI: 10.1037/a0036887; PMC4244306; PUBMED: 24865871] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Pfiffner 2016 {published data only}

  1. Pfiffner LJ, Rooney M, Haack L, Villodas M, Delucchi K, McBurnett K. A randomized controlled trial of a school‐implemented school‐home intervention for attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and impairment. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2016;55(9):762‐70. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.05.023; PUBMED: 27566117] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Qian 2017 {published data only}

  1. Qian Y, Chen M, Shuai L, Cao QJ, Yang L, Wang YF. Effect of an ecological executive skill training program for school‐aged children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a randomized controlled clinical trial. Chinese Medical Journal 2017;130(13):1513‐20. [DOI: 10.4103/0366-6999.208236; PMC5494912; PUBMED: 28639564 ] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Schramm 2016 {published data only}

  1. Schramm SA, Hennig T, Linderkamp F. Training problem solving and organizational skills in adolescents with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology 2016;15(3):391‐411. [DOI: 10.1891/1945-8959.15.3.391] [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Storebø 2012 {published data only}

  1. Storebø OJ, Gluud C, Winkel P, Simonsen E. Social‐skills and parental training plus standard treatment versus standard treatment for children with ADHD ‐ the randomised SOSTRA trial. PLOS One 2012;7(6):e37280. [DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037280; PMC3380035 ; PUBMED: 22745657] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Storebø OJ, Pedersen J, Skoog M, Thomsen PH, Winkel P, Gluud C, et al. Randomised social‐skills training and parental training plus standard treatment versus standard treatment of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder ‐ the SOSTRA trial protocol. Trials 2011;12:18. [DOI: 10.1186/1745-6215-12-18; PMC3038941; PUBMED: 21255399] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Storebø OJ, Skoog M, Rasmussen PD, Winkel P, Gluud C, Pedersen J, et al. Attachment competences in children with ADHD during the social‐skills training and attachment (SOSTRA) randomized clinical trial. Journal of Attention Disorders 2015;19(10):865‐71. [DOI: 10.1177/1087054713520220; PUBMED: 24532801] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Tabaeian 2010 {published data only}

  1. Tabaeian SR, Kalantari M, Amiri S, Neshatdoost HT, Molavi H. Effectiveness of social skills training on social competence on primary school children with ADHD. Journal of Psychology 2010;13(4):362‐76. [Google Scholar]

Tutty 2003 {published data only}

  1. Tutty S, Gephart H, Wurzbacher K. Enhancing behavioral and social skill functioning in children newly diagnosed with attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder in a pediatric setting. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 2003;24(1):51‐7. [PUBMED: 12584485] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Van der Oord 2007 {published data only}

  1. Oord S, Prins PJM, Oosterlaan J, Emmelkamp PMG. Does brief, clinically based, intensive multimodal behaviour therapy enhance the effects of methylphenidate in children with ADHD?. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2007;16(1):48‐57. [DOI: 10.1007/s00787-006-0574-z; PUBMED: 16972117] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Waxmonsky 2010 {published data only}

  1. Waxmonsky JG, Waschbusch DA, Pelham WE, Draganac‐Cardona L, Rotella B, Ryan L. Effects of atomoxetine with and without behavior therapy on the school and home functioning of children with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 2010;71(11):1535‐51. [DOI: 10.4088/JCP.09m05496pur; PUBMED: 20673557 ] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Waxmonsky 2016 {published data only}

  1. Waxmonsky JG, Waschbusch DA, Belin P, Li T, Babocsai L, Humphery H, et al. A randomized clinical trial of an integrative group therapy for children with severe mood dysregulation. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2016;55(3):196‐207. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.12.011; PMC4764804; PUBMED: 26903253] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Wilkes Gillan 2016 {published data only}

  1. Wilkes‐Gillan S, Bundy A, Cordier R, Lincoln M, Chen YW. A randomised controlled trial of a play‐based intervention to improve the social play skills of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). PLOS One 2016;11(8):e0160558. [DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0160558; PMC4987013; PUBMED: 27529693] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Yuk‐chi 2005 {published data only}

  1. Yuk‐chi S. Effectiveness of Methylphenidate and Combined Treatment (Methylphenidate and Psychosocial Treatment) for Chinese Children with Attention‐Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in a Community Mental Health Center [PhD thesis]. Hong Kong (CN): The Chinese University of Hong Kong, 2005. [Google Scholar]

References to studies excluded from this review

Abikoff 2013 {published data only}

  1. Abikoff H, Gallagher R, Wells KC, Murray DW, Huang L, Lu F, et al. Remediating organizational functioning in children with ADHD: Immediate and long‐term effects from a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 2013;81(1):113‐28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Bussing 2016 {published data only}

  1. Bussing R, Nelson MM, Kurtz S. Parent‐child interaction therapy: treatment components and evidence‐base. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2016;55(10 Suppl. 1):S351. [Google Scholar]

Cionek Szpak 2012 {published data only}

  1. Cionek‐Szpak E, Holtmann M, Stadler C, Poustka F. EEG‐Neurobiofeedback in ADHD ‐ a treatment supplement or alternative‐rapport about the results of a controlled experimental trial. Neuropsychiatrie de l'Enfance et de l'Adolescence 2012;60(5 Suppl. 1):s267. [Google Scholar]

Cipolla 2013 {published data only}

  1. Cipolla DS. An investigation of the effects of a computer‐based intervention on the social skills of children with autism. Dissertation Abstracts International Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences 2013;73(11‐A(E)):No pagination specified. [Google Scholar]

Corkum 2005 {published data only}

  1. Corkum PV, McKinnon MM, Mullane JC. The effect of involving classroom teachers in a parent training program for families of children with ADHD. Child & Family Behavior Therapy 2005;27(4):29‐49. [Google Scholar]

Dodge 2011 {published data only}

  1. Dodge A. The effects of the fast track preventive intervention on the development of conduct disorder across childhood. Child Development 2011;82(1):331‐45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Döpfner 2004 {published data only}

  1. Döpfner M, Breuer D, Schürmann S, Metternich TW, Rademacher C, Lehmkuhl G. Effectiveness of an adaptive multimodal treatment in children with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder ‐ global outcome. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2004;13(Suppl. 1):117‐29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

ESCAP 2013 {published data only}

  1. No authorship indicated. 15th International Congress of ESCAP ‐ European Society for Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 2013 July 6‐10; Dublin (IRELAND). European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 2013;22(Suppl. 2):S81‐S323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Feinfield 2004 {published data only}

  1. Feinfield KA, Baker BL. Empirical support for a treatment program for families of young children with externalizing problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology 2004;33(1):182‐95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Gol 2005 {published data only}

  1. Gol D, Jarsus T. Effect of a social skills training group on everyday activities of children with attention deficit‐hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 2005;47(8):539‐45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Gonzalez 2002 {published data only}

  1. Gonzalez LO, Sellers EW. The effects of a stress‐management program on self‐concept, locus of control, and the acquisition of coping skills in school‐age children diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing 2002;15(1):5‐15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Grasmann 2011 {published data only}

  1. Grasmann D, Stadler C. VIA ‐ an intensive therapeutic treatment program for conduct disorders [VIA ‐ intensivtherapeutischer behandlungsansatz bei storungen des sozialverhaltens]. Zeitschrift fur Kinder und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie 2011;39(1):23‐31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Horn 1990 {published data only}

  1. Horn WF, Ialongo N, Greenberg G, Packard T, Smith‐Winberry C. Additive effects of behavioral parent training and self‐control therapy with attention deficit/ hyperactivity disordered children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 1990;19(2):98‐110. [Google Scholar]

ICBM 2012 Meeting {published data only}

  1. Anonymous. ICBM 2012 Meeting. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine 2012;19(Suppl. 1):1‐342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Jans 2012 {published data only}

  1. Jans T, Jacob C, Hennighausen K, Roesler M, Haenig S, Sobanski E, et al. Treatment outcome of behavioral parent‐child training in childhood ADHD as a function of the treatment of maternal ADHD. Neuropsychiatrie de l'Enfance et de l'Adolescence 2012;60(Suppl. 5):S51. [Google Scholar]

Jans 2015 {published data only}

  1. Jans T, Jacob C, Warnke A, Zwanzger U, Groß‐Lesch S, Matthies S, et al. Does intensive multimodal treatment for maternal ADHD improve the efficacy of parent training for children with ADHD? A randomized controlled multicenter trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines 2015;56(12):1298‐313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Klein 1997 {published data only}

  1. Klein RG, Abikoff H. Behavior therapy and methylphenidate in the treatment of children with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders 1997;2(2):89‐114. [Google Scholar]

Kolko 1990 {published data only}

  1. Kolko DJ, Loar LL, Sturnick D. Inpatient social‐cognitive skills training groups with conduct disordered and attention deficit disordered children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 1990;31(5):737‐48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Kolko 1999 {published data only}

  1. Kolko DJ, Bukstein OG, Barron J. Methylphenidate and behavior modification in children with ADHD and comorbid ODD or CD: main and incremental effects across settings. Journal of American Academic Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 1999;38(5):578‐86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Langberg 2008 {published data only}

  1. Langberg JM, Epstein JN. Efficacy of an organization skills intervention to Improve the academic functioning of students with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. School Psychology Quarterly 2008;23(3):407‐17. [Google Scholar]

Langberg 2012 {published data only}

  1. Langberg JM, Epstein JN, Becker SP, Girio‐Herrera E, Vaughn AJ. Evaluation of the Homework, Organization, and Planning Skills (HOPS) intervention for middle school students with ADHD as implemented by school mental health providers. School Psychology Review 2012;41(3):342‐64. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Lessard 2011 {published data only}

  1. Lessard L, Coutu S. Efficacy of a social skills program for children presenting with heterogeneous psychiatric diagnoses. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue Canadienne des Sciences du Comportement 2011;43(1):30‐9. [Google Scholar]

Leung 2017 {published data only}

  1. Leung C, Tsang S, Ng GSH, Choi SY. Efficacy of parent‐child interaction therapy with Chinese ADHD children: randomized controlled trial. Research on Social Work Practice 2017;27(1):36‐47. [Google Scholar]

Lim‐Ashworth 2013a {published data only}

  1. Lim‐Ashworth N, Ooi YP, Weng SJ, Lim CG, Fung DSS, Glenn A, et al. Preliminary findings on the effects of nutritional and social skills intervention among children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Annals of the Academy of Medicine Singapore 2013;42(9 Suppl. 1):S162. [Google Scholar]

Malti 2012 {published data only}

  1. Malti T, Ribeaud D, Eisner M. Effectiveness of a universal school‐based social competence program: the role of child characteristics and economic factors. International Journal of Conflict and Violence 2012;6(2):249‐59. [Google Scholar]

Miranda 2002 {published data only}

  1. Miranda A, Presentación J, Soriano M. Effectiveness of a school‐based multicomponent program for the treatment of children with ADHD. Journal of Learning Disabilities 2002;35(6):547‐63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Molina 2008 {published data only}

  1. Molina BSG, Flory K, Bukstein OG, Greiner AR, Baker JL, Krug V, et al. Feasibility and preliminary efficacy of an after‐school program for middle schoolers with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders 2008;12(3):207‐17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

NCT02574273 {published data only}

  1. NCT02574273. Pilot trial of a social skills group treatment (Secret Agent Society Program). clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT02574273 (first received 12 October 2015).

NCT03176108 {published data only}

  1. NCT03176108. Evaluation of a parent/child cognitive‐behaviorial therapy program in ADHD children with emotional dysregulation profile. clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT03176108 (first received 5 June 2017).

Ostberg 2012 {published data only}

  1. Ostberg M, Rydell AM. An efficacy study of a combined parent and teacher management training programme for children with ADHD. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry 2012;66(2):123‐30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Pfiffner 2018 {published data only}

  1. Pfiffner LJ, Rooney ME, Jiang Y, Haack LM, Beaulieu A, McBurnett K. Sustained effects of collaborative school‐home intervention for Attention‐Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder symptoms and impairment. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2018; Vol. 57, issue 4:245‐51. [DOI] [PubMed]

Power 2012a {published data only}

  1. Power TJ, Mautone JA, Soffer SL, Clarke AT, Marshall SA, Sharman J, et al. A family‐school intervention for children with ADHD: results of a randomized clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 2012;80(4):611‐23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Pumpuang 2012 {published data only}

  1. Pumpuang W, Phuphaibul R, Orathai P, Putdivarnichapong W. Effectiveness of a collaborative home‐school behavior management program for parents and teachers of children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Pacific RIM International Journal of Nursing Research 2012;16(2):138‐53. [Google Scholar]

Rosén 1984 {published data only}

  1. Rosén LA, O´Leary SG, Joyce AS, Conway G, Pfiffner L. The importance of prudent negative consequences for maintaining the appropriate behavior of hyperactive students. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 1984;12(4):581‐604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Sibley 2013 {published data only}

  1. Sibley MH, Pelham WE Jr, Derefinko KJ, Kuriyan AB, Sanchez F, Graziano PA. A pilot trial of Supporting Teens' Academic Needs Daily (STAND): a parent‐adolescent collaborative intervention for ADHD. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 2013;35(4):436‐49. [Google Scholar]

Sibley 2013a {published data only}

  1. Sibley MH. Supporting Teens' Academic Needs Daily (STAND): a parent‐adolescent collaborative intervention for ADHD. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering 2013;74(3‐B(E)):No pagination specified. [Google Scholar]

Sibley 2016 {published data only}

  1. Sibley MH, Graziano PA, Kuriyan AB, Coxe S, Pelham WE, Rodriguez L, et al. Parent‐teen behavior therapy + motivational interviewing for adolescents with ADHD. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 2016;84(8):699‐712. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Vidal 2015 {published data only}

  1. Vidal R, Castells J, Richarte V, Palomar G, Garcia M, Nicolau R, et al. Group therapy for adolescents with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2015;54(4):275‐82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Wolraich 1978 {published data only}

  1. Wolraich M, Drummond T, Salomon MK, O´Brian ML, Sivage C. Effects of methylphenidate alone and in combination with behavior modification procedures on the behavior and academic performance of hyperactive children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 1978;6(1):149‐61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

References to studies awaiting assessment

NCT01019252 {published data only}

  1. NCT01019252. Skills training for adolescents with ADHD [Compensatory executive functioning skills training in adolescents with ADHD]. clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT01019252 (first received 20 November 2009).

References to ongoing studies

IRCT201609186834N11 {published data only}

  1. IRCT201609186834N11. The effect of psycho‐educational group therapy of emotional intelligence on enhancing of emotional intelligence of adolescents with ADHD. www.irct.ir (first received 30 October 2016).

NCT01330849 {published data only}

  1. NCT01330849. Toolkit for school behavior modification in children with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [A randomized controlled trial of a behavior modification toolkit in primary school children with ADHD behaviors]. clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT01330849 (first received 5 April 2011).

NCT02937142 {published data only}

  1. NCT02937142. Cognitive behavior group therapy in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [Cognitive behavior group therapy in adolescents (14‐18 years) with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)]. clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT02937142 (first received 14 October 2016).

Yang 2015 {published data only}

  1. Yang L, Shuai L, Chen M, Qian Y, Wang Y. Efficacy of an integrated behavioural therapy for children with Attention‐Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: a randomized controlled trial. ADHD: Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders 2015;7(Suppl. 1):S94. [DOI: 10.1007/s12402-015-0169-y; P‐25‐0008] [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Additional references

Abikoff 2011 [pers comm]

  1. Abikoff H. Cochrane review: social skills training [personal communication]. Email to: OJ Storebø 28 January 2011.

Achenbach 1991

  1. Achenbach TM. Integrative Guide for the 1991 CBCL/4‐18, YSR, and TRF Profiles. Burlington (VT): University of Vermont, Department of Psychology, 1991. [Google Scholar]

Almerie 2015

  1. Almerie MQ, Okba Al, Marhi M, Jawoosh M, Alsabbagh M, Matar HE, et al. Social skills programmes for schizophrenia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2015, Issue 6. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD009006.pub2] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Andrews 2013a

  1. Andrews J, Guyatt G, Oxman AD, Alderson P, Dahm P, Falck‐Ytter Y, et al. GRADE guidelines: 14. Going from evidence to recommendations: the significance and presentation of recommendations. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2013;66(7):719‐25. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2012.03.013; PUBMED: 23312392] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Andrews 2013b

  1. Andrews JC, Schünemann HJ, Oxman AD, Pottie K, Meerpohl JJ, Coello PA, et al. GRADE guidelines: 15. Going from evidence to recommendation ‐ determinants of a recommendation's direction and strength. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2013;66(7):726‐35. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2013.02.003; PUBMED: 23570745] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Antshel 2010 [pers comm]

  1. Antshel K. [personal communication]. Email to: OJ Storebø 16 December 2010.

Antshel 2011 [pers comm]

  1. Antshel K. [personal communication]. Email to: OJ Storebø 13 July 2011.

Attkisson 1982

  1. Attkisson CC, Zwick R. The client satisfaction questionnaire. Psychometric properties and correlations with service utilization and psychotherapy outcome. Evaluation and Program Planning 1982;5(3):233‐7. [PUBMED: 10259963] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Balshem 2011

  1. Balshem H, Helfand M, Schünemann HJ, Oxman AD, Kunz R, Brozek J, et al. GRADE guidelines: 3. Rating the quality of evidence. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2011;64(4):401‐6. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.07.015; PUBMED: 21208779] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Barkley 1987

  1. Barkley RA. Defiant Children: A Clinician's Manual for Parent Training. 1st Edition. The Guilford Press, 1987. [Google Scholar]

Barkley 2002

  1. Barkely RA, Fischer M, Smallish L, Fletcher K. The persistence of attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder into young adulthood as a function of reporting source and definition of disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 2002;111(2):279‐89. [PUBMED: 12003449] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Berkley 1996

  1. Berkey CS, Mosteller F, Lau J, Antman EM. Uncertainty of the time of first significance in random effects cumulative meta‐analysis. Controlled Clinical Trials 1996;17(5):357‐71. [DOI: 10.1016/S0197-2456(96)00014-1] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Brok 2008

  1. Brok J, Thorlund K, Gluud C, Wetterslev J. Trial sequential analysis reveals insufficient information size and potentially false positive results in many meta‐analyses. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2008;61(8):763‐9. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2007.10.007; PUBMED: 18411040] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Brok 2009

  1. Brok J, Thorlund K, Wetterslev J, Gluud C. Apparently conclusive meta‐analyses may be inconclusive ‐ trial sequential analysis adjustment of random error risk due to repetitive testing of accumulating data in apparently conclusive neonatal meta‐analyses. International Journal of Epidemiology 2009;38(1):287‐98. [DOI: 10.1093/ije/dyn188; PUBMED: 18824466] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Brunetti 2013

  1. Brunetti M, Shemilt I, Pregno S, Vale L, Oxman AD, Lord J, et al. GRADE guidelines: 10. Considering resource use and rating the quality of economic evidence. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2013;66(2):140‐50. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2012.04.012; PUBMED: 22863410] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Castellanos 2006

  1. Castellanos FX, Sonuga‐Barke EJ, Milham MP, Tannock R. Characterizing cognition in ADHD: beyond executive dysfunction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2006;10(3):117‐23. [DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.011; PUBMED: 16460990] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Castellini 2018

  1. Castellini G, Bruschettini M, Gianola S, Gluud C, Moja L. Assessing imprecision in Cochrane systematic reviews: a comparison of GRADE and Trial Sequential Analysis. Systematic Reviews 2018;7:110. [DOI: 10.1186/s13643-018-0770-1] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Chan 2013

  1. Chan A‐W, Tetzlaff JM, Altman DG, Laupacis A, Gøtzsche PC, Krleža‐Jerić K, et al. SPIRIT 2013 statement: defining standard protocol items for clinical trials. Annals of Internal Medicine 2013;158(3):200‐7. [DOI: 10.7326/0003-4819-158-3-201302050-00583; PMC5114123; PUBMED: 23295957] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Chang 2012

  1. Chang Z, Lichtenstein P, Larsson H. The effects of childhood ADHD symptoms on early‐onset substance use: a Swedish twin study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 2012;40(3):425‐35. [DOI: 10.1007/s10802-011-9575-6; PUBMED: 21947618] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Cochrane Hepato‐Biliary Group 2019

  1. Cochrane Hepato‐Biliary Group. Information for authors. hbg.cochrane.org/information‐authors (accessed prior to 5 June 2019).

Conners 1998

  1. Conners KC, Sitarenios G, Parker JDA, Epstein JN. The Revised Conners´ Parent Rating Scale (CPRS‐R): factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 1998;26(4):257‐68. [PUBMED: 9700518] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Conners 2008a

  1. Conners KC. Conners Comprehensive Behavior Rating Scales (Conners CBRS). Toronto (ON): Multi‐Health Systems Inc, 2008. [Google Scholar]

Conners 2008b

  1. Conners KC. Conners 3. Toronto (ON): Multi‐Health Systems Inc, 2008. [Google Scholar]

Cortese 2016

  1. Cortese S, Moreira‐Maia CR, Fleur D, Morcillo‐Peñalver C, Rohde LA, Faraone SV. Association between ADHD and obesity: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry 2016;173(1):34‐43. [DOI: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15020266; PUBMED: 26315982] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Czamara 2013

  1. Czamara D, Tiesler CM, Kohlböck G, Berdel D, Hoffmann B, Bauer CP, et al. Children with ADHD symptoms have a higher risk for reading, spelling and math difficulties in the GINIplus and LISAplus cohort studies. PLOS ONE 2013;8(5):e63859. [DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0063859; PMC3664565; PUBMED: 23724008] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Daley 2014b

  1. Daley D, Oord S, Ferrin M, Danckaerts M, Doepfner M, Cortese S, et al. Behavioral interventions in attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta‐analysis of randomized controlled trials across multiple outcome domains. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2014;53(8):835‐47. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.05.013; PUBMED: 25062591] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Dalsgaard 2002

  1. Dalsgaard S, Mortensen PB, Frydenberg M, Thomsen PH. Conduct problems, gender and adult psychiatric outcome of children with attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry 2002;181(5):416‐21. [PUBMED: 12411268] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Dalsgaard 2015

  1. Dalsgaard S, Østergaard SD, Leckman JF, Mortensen PB, Pedersen MG. Mortality in children, adolescents, and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a nationwide cohort study. Lancet 2015;385(9983):2190‐6. [DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)61684-6] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Dawson 2010

  1. Dawson P, Guare R. Executive Skills in Children and Adolescents: A Practical Guide to Assessment and Intervention. 2nd Edition. New York: The Guilford Press, 2010. [Google Scholar]

De Boo 2007

  1. Boo GM, Prins PJM. Social incompetence in children with ADHD: possible moderators and mediators in social‐skills training. Clinical Psychology Review 2007;27(1):78‐97. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.03.006; PUBMED: 16814435] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Donner 2002

  1. Donner A, Klar N. Issues in the meta‐analysis of cluster randomised trials. Statistics in Medicine 2002;21(19):2971–80. [DOI: 10.1002/sim.1301; PUBMED: 12325113] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

DSM‐5

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM‐5). 5th Edition. Washington (DC): American Psychiatric Publishing, 2013. [Google Scholar]

DSM‐III

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM‐III). 3rd Edition. Washington (DC): American Psychiatric Association, 1980. [Google Scholar]

DSM‐IV

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM‐IV). 4th Edition. Washington (DC): American Psychiatric Association, 1994. [Google Scholar]

DSM‐IV‐R

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM‐III‐R). 3rd Edition. Washington (DC): American Psychiatric Association, 1987. [Google Scholar]

DSM‐IV‐TR

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM‐IV‐TR). 4th Edition. Washington (DC): American Psychiatric Association, 2000. [Google Scholar]

Egger 1997

  1. Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta‐analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 1997;315(7109):629‐34. [DOI: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629; PMC2127453; PUBMED: 9310563] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Elliot 1991

  1. Elliot SN, Gresham FM. Social Skills Intervention Guide: Practical Strategies for Social Skills Training. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, 1991. [Google Scholar]

Erskine 2016

  1. Erskine HE, Norman RE, Ferrari AJ, Chan GC, Copeland WE, Whiteford HA, et al. Long‐term outcomes of attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder and conduct disorder: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2016;55(10):841–50. [doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.06.016; PUBMED: 27663939] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Eunethydis 2010

  1. Eunethydis. Eunethydis 1st international ADHD conference: from data to best clinical practice. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 2010;19(Suppl. 1):5‐91. [DOI: 10.1007/s00787-010-0117-5] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Evans 2017 [pers comm]

  1. Evans S. Questions regarding your study [personal communication]. Email to: J Signe 29 December 2017.

Faraone 2000

  1. Faraone SV, Biederman J, Spencer T, Wilens T, Seidman LJ, Mick E, et al. Attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults: an overview. Biological Psychiatry 2000;48(1):9‐20. [PUBMED: 10913503] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Fohlmann 2009a

  1. Fohlmann AH. Social skills training [Social færdighedstræning]. In: Nordentoft M, Melau M, Iversen T, Kjær S editor(s). Psychosis in Adolescents. Symptoms, Treatment and the Future [Psykose hos Unge. Symptomer, Behandling og Fremtid]. Copenhagen (DK): Psykiatrifondens Forlag, 2009:161‐89. [Google Scholar]

Fohlmann 2009b [pers comm]

  1. Storebø OJ. Social Skills Training [personal communication]. Email to: AH Fohlmann 10 April 2009.

Ford 2003

  1. Ford T, Goodman R, Meltzer H. The British Child and Adolescent Mental Health Survey 1999: the prevalence of DSM‐IV disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2003;42(10):1203–11. [DOI: 10.1097/00004583-200310000-00011; PUBMED: 14560170] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Franke 2012

  1. Franke B, Faraone SV, Asherson P, Buitelaar J, Bau CHD, Ramos‐Quiroga JA, et al. The genetics of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults, a review. Molecular Psychiatry 2012;17(10):960‐87. [DOI: 10.1038/mp.2011.138; PMC3449233; PUBMED: 22105624] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Froehlich 2011

  1. Froehlich TE, Anixt JS, Loe IM, Chirdkiatgumchai V, Kuan L, Gilman RC. Update on environmental risk factors for attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports 2011;13(5):33‐44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

GRADE Working Group

  1. GRADE Working Group. GRADE. www.gradeworkinggroup.org (accessed 11 December 2018).

GRADEpro GDT 2015 [Computer program]

  1. McMaster University. GRADEpro GDT. Version accessed prior to 12 March 2018. Hamilton (ON): McMaster University, 2015.

Gresham 1990

  1. Gresham FM, Elliot SN. Social Skills Rating System. Circle Pines (MN): Assistance Service, 1990. [Google Scholar]

Guyatt 2011a

  1. Guyatt G, Oxman AD, Akl EA, Kunz R, Vist G, Brozek J, et al. GRADE guidelines: 1. Introduction ‐ GRADE evidence profiles and summary of findings tables. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2011;64(4):383–94. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.04.026; PUBMED: 21195583] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Guyatt 2011b

  1. Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Kunz R, Atkins D, Brozek J, Vist G, et al. GRADE guidelines: 2. Framing the question and deciding on important outcomes. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2011;64(4):395–400. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.09.012; PUBMED: 21194891] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Guyatt 2011c

  1. Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Vist G, Kunz R, Brozek J, Alonso‐Coello P, et al. GRADE guidelines: 4. Rating the quality of evidence ‐ study limitations (risk of bias). Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2011;64(4):407–15. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.07.017; PUBMED: 21247734] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Guyatt 2011d

  1. Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Kunz R, Brozek J, Alonso‐Coello P, Rind D, et al. GRADE guidelines 6. Rating the quality of evidence ‐ imprecision. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2011;64(12):1283–93. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2011.01.012; PUBMED: 21839614] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Guyatt 2011e

  1. Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Kunz R, Woodcock J, Brozek J, Helfand M, et al. GRADE guidelines: 7. Rating the quality of evidence ‐ inconsistency. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2011;64(12):1294–302. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2011.03.017; PUBMED: 21803546] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Guyatt 2011f

  1. Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Kunz R, Woodcock J, Brozek J, Helfand M, et al. GRADE guidelines: 8. Rating the quality of evidence ‐ indirectness. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2011;64(12):1303–10. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2011.04.014; PUBMED: 21802903] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Guyatt 2011g

  1. Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Sultan S, Glasziou P, Akl EA, Alonso‐Coello P, et al. GRADE guidelines: 9. Rating up the quality of evidence. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2011;64(12):1311–6. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2011.06.004; PUBMED: 21802902] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Guyatt 2013a

  1. Guyatt G, Oxman AD, Sultan S, Brozek J, Glasziou P, Alonso‐Coello P, et al. GRADE guidelines: 11. Making an overall rating of confidence in effect estimates for a single outcome and for all outcomes. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2013;66(2):151–7. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2012.01.006; PUBMED: 22542023] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Guyatt 2013b

  1. Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Santesso N, Helfand M, Vist G, Kunz R, et al. GRADE guidelines: 12. Preparing summary of findings tables ‐ binary outcomes. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2013;66(2):158–72. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2012.01.012; PUBMED: 22609141] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Guyatt 2013c

  1. Guyatt GH, Thorlund K, Oxman AD, Walter SD, Patrick D, Furukawa TA, et al. GRADE guidelines: 13. Preparing summary of findings tables and evidence profiles ‐ continuous outcomes. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2013;66(2):173–83. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2012.08.001; PUBMED: 23116689] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Hannesdottir 2018 [pers comm]

  1. Hannesdottir D. [personal communication]. Email to: S Joost 18 January 2018.

Hannesdottir 2018b [pers comm]

  1. Hannesdottir D. [personal communication]. Email to: S Joost 9 February 2018.

Higgins 2011

  1. Higgins JPT, Green S, editor(s). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0 (updated March 2011). The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011. Available from handbook.cochrane.org.

ICD‐10

  1. World Health Organization. The ICD‐10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders. Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva (CH): World Health Organization, 1992. [Google Scholar]

ICD‐9

  1. World Health Organization. International Classification of Diseases. 9th Edition. Vol. 1, Geneva (CH): World Health Organization, 1977. [Google Scholar]

ICH 1996

  1. International Conference on Harmonisation of Technical Requirements for Registration of Pharmaceuticals for Human Use. ICH Harmonised Tripartite Guideline. Guideline for Good Clinical Practice E6(R1). www.ich.org/fileadmin/Public_Web_Site/ICH_Products/Guidelines/Efficacy/E6/E6_R1_Guideline.pdf (accessed 6 May 2011).

International Congress on ADHD 2009

  1. International Congress on ADHD. 2nd International Congress on ADHD: from childhood to adult disease. www.adhd‐federation.org/fileadmin/user_upload/PDFs/ADHD_Final_Program.pdf (accessed prior to 29 May 2019).

Jakobsen 2014

  1. Jakobsen JC, Gluud C, Winkel P, Lange T, Wetterslev J. The thresholds for statistical and clinical significance – a five‐step procedure for evaluation of intervention effects in randomised clinical trials. BMC Medical Research Methodology. 2014;14:34. [DOI: 10.1186/1471-2288-14-34; PMC4015863; PUBMED: 24588900] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Kadesjö 2002

  1. Kadesjö B. ADHD in Children and Adults [ADHD hos Barn och Vuxna]. Stockholm (SE): Socialstyrelsen, 2002. [Google Scholar]

Kavale 1997

  1. Kavale KA, Mathur SR, Forness SR, Rutherford Jr RB, Quinn MM. Effectiveness of social skills training for students with behaviour disorders: a meta‐analysis. Advances in Learning and Behavioral Disabilities 1997;11:1‐26. [Google Scholar]

Kendal 1982

  1. Kendall PC, Braswell L. Cognitive‐behavioral self‐control therapy for children: a components analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 1982;50:672‐90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Keshavan 2003

  1. Keshavan MS, Sujata M, Mehra A, Montrose DM, Sweeney JA. Psychosis proneness and ADHD in young relatives of schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Research 2003;59(1):85‐92. [12413647] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Keus 2010

  1. Keus F, Wetterslev J, Gluud C, Laarhoven CJHM. Evidence at a glance: error matrix approach for overviewing available evidence. BMC Medical Research Methodology 2010;10:90. [DOI: 10.1186/1471-2288-10-90] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Kjaergard 2002

  1. Kjaergard LL, Gluud C. Citation bias of hepato‐biliary randomized clinical trials. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2002;55(4):407‐10. [PUBMED: 11927210] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Koisaari 2015

  1. Koisaari T, Michelsson K, Holopainen JM, Maksimainen R, Päivänsalo J, Rantala K, et al. Traffic and criminal behavior of adults with attention deficit‐hyperactivity with a prospective follow‐up from birth to the age of 40 years. Traffic Injury Prevention 2015;16(8):824‐30. [DOI: 10.1080/15389588.2015.1029068; PUBMED: 25837647] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Kwak 2002

  1. Kwak K, Park H, Kim C. The Manual for the Korean WISC‐III. Seoul: Special Education Publishing Co, 2002. [Google Scholar]

Landau 1991

  1. Landau S, Moore LA. Social skill deficits in children with attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder. School Psychology Review 1991;20(2):235‐51. [Google Scholar]

Lau 1995

  1. Lau J, Schmid CH, Chalmers TC. Cumulative meta‐analysis of clinical trials builds evidence for exemplary medical care. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 1995;48(1):45‐57. [DOI: 10.1016/0895-4356(94)00106-Z; PUBMED: 7853047] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Lauth 2004

  1. Lauth GW, Fellner C. Course of therapy and long‐term effects of a multimodal therapy program in ADHD. Single case studies [Therapieverlauf und langzeiteffekt eines multimodalen trainingsprogramms bei Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit‐/Hyperaktivitätsstörungen. Einzelfallstudien]. Kindheit und Entwicklung 2004;13(3):167‐79. [DOI: 10.1026/0942-5403.13.3.167] [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Liberman 1988

  1. Liberman RP. Social skills training. In: Liberman RP editor(s). Psychiatric Rehabilitation of Chronic Mental Patients. Washington (DC): American Psychiatric Press, 1988. [Google Scholar]

Mager 2005

  1. Mager W, Milich R, Harris MJ, Howard A. Intervention groups for adolescents with conduct problems: is aggression harmful or helpful?. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 2005;33(3):349‐62. [DOI: 10.1007/s10802-005-3572-6] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Majewicz‐Hefley 2007

  1. Majewicz‐Hefley A, Carlson JS. A meta‐analysis of combined treatments for children diagnosed with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders 2007;10(3):239‐50. [DOI: 10.1177/1087054706289934; PUBMED: 17242418] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Meichenbaum 1978

  1. Meichenbaum D. Cognitive‐Behavior Modification: An Integrative Approach. New York (NY): Plenum Press, 1978. [Google Scholar]

Moher 2009

  1. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta‐analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLOS Medicine 2009;6(7):e1000097. [DOI: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097; PMC2707599; PUBMED: 19621072] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Mustafa 2013

  1. Mustafa RA, Santesso N, Brozek J, Akl EA, Walter SD, Norman G, et al. The GRADE approach is reproducible in assessing the quality of evidence of quantitative evidence syntheses. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2013;66(7):736‐42. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2013.02.004; PUBMED: 23623694] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Nakao 1994

  1. Nakao K, Treas J. Updating occupational prestige and socioeconomic scores: how the new measures measure up. Sociological Methodology 1994;24:1‐72. [DOI: 10.2307/270978] [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Neale 2010

  1. Neale BM, Medland SE, Ripke S, Asherson P, Franke B, Lesch KP, et al. Meta‐analysis of genome‐wide association studies of attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2010;49(9):884‐97. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.06.008; PMC2928252; PUBMED: 20732625] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Newcorn 2008

  1. Newcorn JH. Co‐morbidity in adults with ADHD. CNS Spectrums 2008;13(Suppl. 12):12‐5. [DOI: 10.1017/S1092852900003199] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

NICE 2009

  1. National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health, commissioned by the National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Diagnosis and Management of ADHD in Children, Young People and Adults. National Clinical Practice Guideline Number 72. Leicester and London: The British Psychological Society and The Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2009. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

NICE 2018

  1. NICE. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: diagnosis and management. www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng87/resources/attention‐deficit‐hyperactivity‐disorder‐diagnosis‐and‐management‐pdf‐1837699732933 (accessed 12 July 2018).

Nigg 2005

  1. Nigg JT, Casey BJ. An integrative theory of attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder based on the cognitive and affective neurosciences. Development and Psychopathology 2005;17(3):785‐806. [DOI: 10.1017/S0954579405050376; PUBMED: 16262992] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Nordic ADHD konference 2010

  1. Nordic ADHD konference 2010. Nordic ADHD conference: lifelong perspectives of special needs [Nordic ADHD konference: livslange perspektiver of specielle behov]. www.bupnet.dk/media/ADHD‐konference_maj_2010‐_Program%5B1%5D.pdf (accessed prior to 29 May 2019).

Norman 2003

  1. Norman GR, Sloan JA, Wyrwich KW. Interpretation of changes in health‐related quality of life: the remarkable universality of half a standard deviation. Medical Care 2003;41(5):582‐92. [DOI: 10.1097/01.MLR.0000062554.74615.4C; PUBMED: 12719681] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Pasini 2007

  1. Pasini A, Paloscia C, Alessandrelli R, Porfirio MC, Curatolo P. Attention and executive functions profile in drug naive ADHD subtypes. Brain and Development 2007;29(7):400‐8. [DOI: 10.1016/j.braindev.2006.11.010; PUBMED: 17207595] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Pelham 1992

  1. Pelham WE Jr, Gnagny EM, Greenslade KE, Milich R. Teacher ratings of DSM‐III‐R symptoms for disruptive behaviour disorder.. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 1992;31(2):210‐8. [DOI: 10.1097/00004583-199203000-00006; PUBMED: 1564021] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Perroud 2014

  1. Perroud N, Cordera P, Zimmermann J, Michalopoulos G, Bancila V, Prada P, et al. Comorbidity between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and bipolar disorder in a specialized mood disorders outpatient clinic. Journal of Affective Disorders 2014;168:161‐6. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.06.053; PUBMED: 25051093] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Pfiffner 2011 [pers comm]

  1. Pfiffner L. Cochrane review: Social skills training [personal communication]. Email to: OJ Storebø 26 May 2011.

Pfiffner 2011b [pers comm]

  1. Pfiffner L. Cochrane review: Social skills training [personal communication]. Email to: OJ Storebø 25 May 2011.

Pineda 2003

  1. Pineda DA, Lopera F, Palacio JD, Ramirez D, Henao GC. Prevalence estimations of attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder: differential diagnoses and comorbidities in a Columbian sample. International Journal of Neuroscience 2003;113(1):49‐71. [PUBMED: 12691001] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Polanczyk 2007

  1. Polanczyk G, Rohde LA. Epidemiology of attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder across the lifespan. Current Opinion in Psychiatry 2007;20(4):386‐92. [DOI: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3281568d7a; PUBMED: 17551354] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Punja 2016

  1. Punja S, Shamseer L, Hartling L, Urichuk L, Vandermeer B, Nikles J, et al. Amphetamines for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children and adolescents. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2016, Issue 2. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD009996.pub2; PUBMED: 26844979] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Review Manager 2014 [Computer program]

  1. Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration. Review Manager 5 (RevMan 5). Version 5.3. Copenhagen: Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration, 2014.

Savovic 2018

  1. Savović J, Turner RM, Mawdsley D, Jones HE, Beynon R, Higgins JPT, et al. Association between risk‐of‐bias assessments and results of randomized trials in Cochrane Reviews: the ROBES Meta‐Epidemiologic Study. American Journal of Epidemiology 2018;187(5):1113‐22. [DOI: 10.1093/aje/kwx344; PMC5928453; PUBMED: 29126260] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Savovíc 2012

  1. Savović S, Jones HE, Altman DG, Harris RJ, Jüni P, Pildal J, et al. Influence of reported study design characteristics on intervention effect estimates from randomized, controlled trials. Annals of Internal Medicine 2012;157(6):429–38. [DOI: 10.7326/0003-4819-157-6-201209180-00537; PUBMED: 22945832] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Schachar 1997

  1. Schachar RJ, Tannock R, Cunningham C, Corkum PV. Behavioral, situational, and temporal effects of treatment of ADHD with methylphenidate. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 1997;36(6):754‐63. [DOI: 10.1097/00004583-199706000-00011; PUBMED: 9183129] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Schmidt 2009

  1. Schmidt S, Peterman F. Developmental psychopathology: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). BMC Psychiatry 2009;9:58. [DOI: 10.1186/1471-244X-9-58; PMC2751746 ; PUBMED: 19761584] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Schultz 1995

  1. Schultz KF, Chalmers I, Hayes RJ, Altman DG. Empirical evidence of bias. Dimensions of methodological quality associated with estimates of treatment effects in controlled trials. JAMA 1995;273(5):408‐12. [PUBMED: 7823387] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Schulz 2010

  1. Schulz KF, Altman DG, Moher D, CONSORT Group. CONSORT 2010 statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. BMJ 2010;340:c332. [DOI: 10.1136/bmj.c332] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Sergeant 2003

  1. Sergeant JA, Geurts H, Huijbregts S, Scheres A, Oosterlaan J. The top and the bottom of ADHD: a neuropsychological perspective. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 2003;27(7):583‐92. [PUBMED: 14624803] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Storebø 2014

  1. Storebø OJ, Simonsen E. Is ADHD an early stage in the development of borderline personality disorder?. Nord Journal of Psychiatry 2014;68(5):289–95. [DOI: 10.3109/08039488.2013.841992; 24117059] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Storebø 2015

  1. Storebø OJ, Ramstad E, Krogh HB, Nilausen TD, Skoog M, Holmskov M, et al. Methylphenidate for children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2015, Issue 11. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD009885.pub2] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Storebø 2018

  1. Storebø OJ, Pedersen N, Ramstad E, Kielsholm ML, Nielsen SS, Krogh HB, et al. Methylphenidate for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children and adolescents – assessment of adverse events in non‐randomised studies.. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2018, Issue 5. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD012069.pub2] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Swanson 2007

  1. Swanson JM, Hinshaw SP, Arnold LE, Gibbons RD, Marcus S, Hur K, et al. Secondary evaluations of MTA 36‐month outcomes: propensity score and growth mixture model analyses. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2007;46(8):1003‐14. [DOI: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3180686d63; PUBMED: 17667479] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Thalheimer 2002

  1. Thalheimer W, Cook S. How to calculate effect sizes from published research articles: a simplified methodology. www.bwgriffin.com/gsu/courses/edur9131/content/Effect_Sizes_pdf5.pdf (accessed 28 May 2011).

Thomas 2015

  1. Thomas R, Sanders S, Doust J, Beller E, Glasziou P. Prevalence of attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Pediatrics 2015;135(4):e994‐e1001. [DOI: 10.1542/peds.2014-3482; PUBMED: 25733754] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Thorlund 2009

  1. Thorlund K, Devereaux PJ, Wetterslev J, Guyatt G, Ioannidis JP, Thabane L, et al. Can trial sequential monitoring boundaries reduce spurious inferences from meta‐analyses?. International Journal of Epidemiology 2009;38(1):276‐86. [DOI: 10.1093/ije/dyn179; PUBMED: 18824467] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Thorlund 2011

  1. Thorlund K, Walter SD, Johnston BC, Furukawa TA, Guyatt GH. Pooling health‐related quality of life outcomes in meta‐analysis ‐ a tutorial and review of methods for enhancing interpretability. Research Synthesis Methods 2011;2:188‐203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

US FDA 2011

  1. US Food, Drug Administration. Communication about an ongoing safety review of stimulant medications used in children with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). www.fda.gov/Drugs/DrugSafety/PostmarketDrugSafetyInformationforPatientsandProviders/DrugSafetyInformationforHeathcareProfessionals/ucm165858.htm (accessed 6 May 2011).

Van der Oord 2008

  1. Oord S, Prins PJM, Oosterlaan J, Emmelkamp PMG. Efficacy of methylphenidate, psychosocial treatments and their combination in school‐aged children with ADHD: a meta‐analysis. Clinical Psychology Review 2008;28(5):783‐800. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.007; PUBMED: 18068284] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Van Eck 2010

  1. Eck K, Finney JS, Evans S. Parent report of ADHD symptoms of early adolescents: a confirmatory factor analysis of the Disruptive Behavior Disorders scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement 2010;70:1042‐59. [Google Scholar]

Walcott 2004

  1. Walcott CM, Landau S. The relation between disinhibition and emotion regulation in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology 2004;33(4):772‐82. [DOI: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3304_12; PUBMED: 15498744] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Waschbusch 2011 [pers comm]

  1. Waschbusch D. Cochrane review: Social skills training [personal communication]. Email to: OJ Storebø 22 June 2011.

Wechsler 2003

  1. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. 4th Edition. London (UK): Pearson, 2003. [Google Scholar]

Weller 2010

  1. Weller EB, Weller RA, Fristad MA, Rooney MT, Schecter BA. Children's Interview for Psychiatric Syndromes (ChIPS). Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 2000;39(1):76‐84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Wetterslev 2008

  1. Wetterslev J, Thorlund K, Brok J, Gluud C. Trial sequential analysis may establish when firm evidence is reached in cumulative meta‐analysis. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2008;61(1):64‐75. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2007.03.013; PUBMED: 18083463] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Wetterslev 2009

  1. Wetterslev J, Thorlund K, Brok J, Gluud C. Estimating required information size by quantifying diversity in random‐effects model meta‐analyses. BMC Medical Research Methodology 2009;9:86. [DOI: 10.1186/1471-2288-9-86; PMC2809074; PUBMED: 20042080] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Wetterslev 2017

  1. Wetterslev J, Jakobsen JC, Gluud C. Trial Sequential Analysis in systematic reviews with meta‐analysis. BMC Medical Research Methodology 2017;17:39. [DOI: 10.1186/s12874-017-0315-7; PMC5397700; PUBMED: 28264661] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Whalen 1985

  1. Whalen CK, Henker B. The social worlds of hyperactive (ADHD) children. Clinical Psychology Review 1985;5(5):447‐78. [DOI: 10.1016/0272-7358(85)90004-2] [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Willcut 2012

  1. Willcutt EG. The prevalence of DSM‐IV attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta‐analytic review. Neurotherapeutics 2012;9(3):490‐9. [DOI: 10.1007/s13311-012-0135-8; PMC3441936; PUBMED: 22976615] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Willis 2019

  1. Willis D, Siceloff ER, Morse M, Neger E, Flory K. Stand‐alone social skills training for youth with ADHD: a systematic review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review 2019 Feb 22 [Epub ahead of print]. [DOI: 10.1007/s10567-019-00291-3; PUBMED: 30796673] [DOI] [PubMed]

Wolraich 2007

  1. Wolraich ML, McGuinn L, Doffing M. Treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents: safety considerations. Drug Safety 2007;30(1):17‐26. [DOI: 10.2165/00002018-200730010-00003; PUBMED: 17194168] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Wood 2008

  1. Wood L, Egger M, Gluud LL, Schultz KF, Jüni P, Altman DG, et al. Empirical evidence of bias in treatment effect estimates in controlled trials with different interventions and outcomes: meta‐epidemiological study. BMJ 2008;336(7644):601‐5. [DOI: 10.1136/bmj.39465.451748.AD; PMC2267990; PUBMED: 18316340] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

World Congress on ADHD 2011

  1. World Congress on ADHD. 3rd World Congress on ADHD: from childhood to adult disease. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders 2011;3(2):77‐234. [DOI: 10.1007/s12402-011-0058-y] [DOI] [Google Scholar]

World Congress on ADHD 2013

  1. World Congress on ADHD. 4th World Congress on ADHD: from childhood to adult disease. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders 2013;5(2):111‐247. [DOI: 10.1007/s12402-013-0109-7] [DOI] [Google Scholar]

World Congress on ADHD 2015

  1. World Congress on ADHD. 5th World Congress on ADHD: from child to adult disorder. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders 2015;7(Suppl. 1):S1‐S119. [DOI: 10.1007/s12402-015-0169-y] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

World Congress on ADHD 2017

  1. World Congress on ADHD. 6th World Congress on ADHD: from child to adult disorder. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders 2017;9(Suppl. 1):S1‐S55. [10.1007/s12402‐017‐0224‐y] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Xu 2018

  1. Xu G, Strathearn L, Liu B, Yang B, Bao W. Twenty‐year trends in diagnosed Attention‐Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder among US children and adolescents, 1997‐2016. JAMA Network Open 2018;1(4):e181471. [DOI: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.1471] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Yoshimasu 2012

  1. Yoshimasu K, Barbaresi WJ, Colligan RC, Voigt RG, Killian JM, Weaver AL, et al. Childhood ADHD is strongly associated with a broad range of psychiatric disorders during adolescence: a population‐based birth cohort study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 2012;53(10):1036‐43. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02567.x; PMC3608464; PUBMED: 22647074] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Zametkin 1987

  1. Zametkin AJ, Rapoport JL. Neurobiology of attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity: where have we come in 50 years?. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 1987;26(5):676‐86. [DOI: 10.1097/00004583-198709000-00011; PUBMED: 2889717] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

References to other published versions of this review

Storebø 2010

  1. Storebø OJ, Skoog M, Damm D, Thomsen PH, Simonsen E, Gluud C. Social skills training for children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2010, Issue 5. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD008223] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Storebø 2011

  1. Storebø OJ, Skoog M, Damm D, Thomsen PH, Simonsen E, Gluud C. Social skills training for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children aged 5 to 18 years. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2011, Issue 12. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD008223.pub2; PUBMED: 22161422] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES